Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    1/22

    A Model of Language Development Based on Self-Organisation ofGestalts and Metaphor

    David Rail, MD; FRACP

    Neurologist

    Campbelltown, Sydney, Australia 2560.

    [email protected]

    Abstract

    Modeling language development has recently seen a shift towards studies of the

    interdependence of language and perceptual reality. Cognitive linguistics has revived

    the Gestalt approach where things and relations constitute wholes: relations emerge

    with the objects through a process of segmentation and transformation. According to

    this view the continuous and dynamic form of the external, phenomenal world

    motivates sentential semantic structures as an expression of the unity between

    perception and language. These structures also represent the progressive self-organisation of image schemata where meaning emerges through metaphor.

    Metaphor involves double scope blending where structures emerge from the

    interaction between incongruent conceptual frames. That process is recursive leading

    to creative structures. Although language development is based on gestalt metaphor

    self-organisation no model has concentrated on this fundamental approach. To

    produce such a model we conceive metaphor in its rhetorical structural form, as

    coordination of the master tropes, or a tropology. We rationalize the role of the

    tropology and show how it functions recursively throughout the forebrain. To

    understand the recursive form of the tropology we show how it functions in a gestaltmanner and that perception is a tropological process. The latter stems from

    perception and the tropology becoming self similar (isomorphic) as the language

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    2/22

    system self organises in ontogeny. To corroborate the isomorphism we show that

    the Gestalt principles and the master tropes are homologous. This finding enables us

    to determine the structure of perception. We indicate how semantic sentential

    structures are generated from self-organisation of the tropology. With self-

    organisation tropological function incorporates spatiotemporal scaling (fractal time).

    We indicate other important aspects of the model, in particular metalinguisticdevelopment.

    Keywords: Brain function; cognitive linguistics; concept formation; double scope

    blending; emergence; fractal time; gestalts; gestalt principles; image schemata; irony;

    language development; master tropes; metaphor; metastability; microgenesis;

    morphogenesis; perception; self organisation; semiotics; topology.

    Introduction

    Modeling language development has recently seen a shift towards studies of the

    interdependence of language and perceptual reality. Cognitive linguistics proposes

    that language is a cognitive phenomenon where conceptual structures stem from

    perception and embodiment (Hampe, 2005; Lakoff & Johnson, 1980; Taylor, 1989;

    Skoldberg, 2002). Language is imaginatively embodied where metaphor is central to

    the origins of meaning (Danesi, 2004). Modeling the development of language has

    focused on meaning rather than syntax. This leads to the important question: how are

    events in the physical world transformed into semantic notions? This question

    confronts the aporia or discrepancy between the analog world we live in and the

    discrete or digital nature of language in terms of categories and symbols. To

    overcome this difficulty Thom proposed that we need to preserve a priori forms ofspace and time by generating dynamic structures or morphologies (Thom, 1972,

    1990).

    These structures are dynamic, morphological and gestalt based. In this approach

    early morphodynamic models from Ren Thom have received wide support, both

    from cognitive linguists such as Talmy (2000) and Langacker (1987; 1990), and from

    psychology through the study of metaphor by Lakoff and Johnson (1980) and Lakoff

    (1993). Morphodynamic theories have proposed that there are syntactico-semantic

    infrastructures of a topological and dynamic nature, which form universals (seeManjali, 1997 (a) and (b)). These underlie a morphological emergent level of reality

    where "surface structures" (gestalts) emerge from physical "deep structures".

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    3/22

    Petitot calls the extraction of semantic structures or invariants in spatiotemporal

    transformations the morphogenesis of meaning (Petitot, 1995 and 2003). Based on

    this work cognitive linguistics has revived the Gestalt approach calling into

    question the traditional roles assigned to perception as a faculty only dealing with

    relations between objects. In the Gestalt or mereological conception things andrelations constitute wholes: relations are not given for granted but emerge together

    with the objects through a process of segmentation and transformation (Doursat &

    Petitot, 2005 (a) and (b)).

    Two major examples of the morphogenesis of meaning are found in our

    development of spatial (prepositions) and spatiotemporal meaning (verbs).

    Understanding prepositions, e.g. in, above, across amounts to the brain

    forming morphodynamic transforms, where transforms create a morphology that

    evolves temporally (Doursat & Petitot, 2005 (a) and (b)). Transformation routines perform a drastic, yet targeted simplification of the geometric data relating the

    relevant items in perception. By erasing details they create virtual structures or

    singularities that govern the development of geometric relationships between the

    interacting morphologies. For example, what characterises the semantic

    development of the concept of in is invariance across all the perceived instances.

    The relationships that emerge with the transformations represent structural

    invariance across an infinite range of real world instances or topologies. What

    remains invariant is the Gestalt, the preservation of similar structural relationships

    that have developed dynamically.

    For verbs (Thom, 1972 and 1990) geometrico-topological analysis associates

    combinatorial invariants with spatio-temporal process in the physical world. This

    primordial schematism governs the linguistic organisation of our Gestaltic vision of

    the world, where these invariants form the basis for verbal process. Actantial graphs

    encompass Tesnieres concepts of little dramas (Manjali, 1997 (a) and (b)) where

    the verb organises or structures sentential meaning, binding objects and situations to

    form dynamic gestalts. These gestalts are organic and binding principles of brain

    organisation.

    Based on these ideas understanding language development centers on how

    topological and dynamic information (morphodynamics) provided by perception

    can be iconically encoded in image schemata and processed by the semantics of

    natural language (Nuessel, 1996; Petitot, 1995, 2003). Schemata capture the

    structural contours of sensorimotor experience integrating information from

    multiple modalities (Grady, 2005; Hampe, 2005; Rohrer, 2005). Image schemata

    organise knowledge and reasoning about the world. They function somewhat like

    the abstract structure of an image, and thereby connect up a vast range of different

    experiences that manifest this same recurring structure (Johnson, 1987). Byrepeatedly activating a set of properties in a particular way individuals form top-

    down frames for organising different aspects of perception via metaphor.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    4/22

    Metaphor preserves the topological contours of perceptual experience (Invariance

    principle) (Lakoff, 1993), where perception is structured by the Gestalt principles:

    emergence, reification, invariance and multistability (Lehar, 2003).

    Image schemata are condensed redescriptions of perceptual experience for the purpose of mapping spatial structure on to conceptual structure. Therefore

    modeling language development in terms of image schemata function must respect

    the view that has emerged in cognitive linguistics that the continuous and dynamic

    form of the external, phenomenal world motivates sentential semantic structures

    (Manjali, 1997 (a) and (b)). The continuous plane of content has its source in

    perception, as it is through perception that the human organism establishes contact

    with the world. The elements of the perceptually rooted linguistic schemas produce

    a 'dynamic gestalt' by means of which semantic comprehension of sentences can

    take place.

    Modeling language development

    Based on the above we propose that modeling language development can be based

    on the idea that the continuous and dynamic form of the external, phenomenal

    world motivates dynamic Gestalts. These sentential semantic structures are an

    expression of the unity between perception and language. These structures are a

    product of the self-organisation of image schemata in ontogeny, where gestalts

    acquire meaning via metaphor.

    Metaphor needs to be considered in terms of the many space conceptual integration

    model that is based on double-scope blending (Fauconnier & Turner, 1998, 2003,

    2008). Blending is dynamic where we construct meaning by actively reinterpreting

    the unknown (percepts, ideas) in terms of the known or idealised concepts. It

    involves the complex interaction between the contingencies of the source (unknown)

    considered in terms of the preconceived ideals of the known (target). The dynamics

    give rise to emergent properties representing reality from a certain perspective.Double-scope blending creates vast conceptual networks with elaborate relations

    running across the networkrelations of time, space, cause-effect, representation,

    analogy and disanalogy, change, identity, uniqueness, and so on. Despite the vast

    scales involved nonetheless concepts are anchored in scenes that are at human scale.

    Integration networks consisting of conventional parts, conventionally structured

    parts, and novel mappings and compressions represent reality from a certain

    perspective.

    Blending is recursive: packed, human-scale blends become inputs to new networks(Fauconnier & Turner, 1998, 2003, 2008). Emergent structures can be incorporated

    into more complex ones. Human scale blends contained in the network provide a

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    5/22

    platform, a scaffold, a cognitively congenial basis from which to reach out, manage,

    manipulate, transform, develop, and handle the network. Human thought anchoring

    vast network scales in human scale enables us to bring the distant past and future

    together in the here-and-now. The individual can become aware of identity and

    existence in subjective time that extends from the past through the present to the

    future. Blending involves a major personal and emotional input so that the semanticproduct is highly personal and unpredictable. It also accommodates the central role

    of the ego as being both the agent and also changed in the blending process. It

    countenances paradox and anomalies.

    Despite the importance of self-organisation between gestalt and metaphor in language

    development no model has concentrated on this fundamental approach. We contend

    that such a model needs to incorporate a number of aspects or constraints on its form.

    These are as follows: rhetorical, where language stems from a continual questioningof the nature of perception and thought; microgenetic (Brown, 1988, 1998), where

    language is an actualisation (Aktualgenese)of a cognition over "layers" in mind and

    brain that retrace growth patterns in phyloontogeny. In this way of thinking, the

    momentary actualisation of the organism, its becoming, is the fundamental note from

    which the melody of development is composed; ontological, where the conceptual

    units underlying the dynamics can interpret reality; ontogenic, in that the units should

    recognise the maturational sequence that characterises ontogeny; structural, where the

    units self organise into a structure that transforms dynamic gestalt structures into

    sentential semantic structures; recursivity, where the same dynamic should be evidentat all levels of the neuraxis, from perception to frontal planning; gestaltic, the

    dynamics should reflect the gestalt basis of language development; incorporate

    double scope blending concepts of emergence, metaphor as anchoring,

    spatiotemporal scaling, compression, completion, paradox resolution and ego

    formation; and finally self-organisation, which incorporates notions of metastability,

    metalinguistic development, fractal time and contextual dissociation.

    Metaphor as coordination of the master tropes

    In order to incorporate all these constraints into our model we propose that we need

    to consider metaphor in terms of its constituent structure, coordination of the master

    tropes (metaphor, metonymy, synecdoche and irony). To trope means to change

    meaning. Of what? Of words? Yes, but more specifically they are the source of

    changing what or how we mean. Each trope represents the stages consciousness must

    pass on the way to abstract thought (D'Angelo, 1987). Metaphor presents perceptual

    equivalence, representing an intuitive grasp of the whole, a primordial functionalunity of sensory, affective, imagistic and linguistic elements. Metonymy

    differentiates into parts, transductively leading over the mind from one thing to

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    6/22

    another. Synecdoche is an inductive movement where a part is put for the whole, and

    vice versa. Irony is a self-conscious process that interprets the whole process.

    The coordinated function of the four tropes as a system is considered essential for our

    conceptual adaptation. From the 17th

    Century when Vico (Danesi, 2003; Vico, 1944)

    first recognised the master tropes until the early 1980s the tropology waschampioned as a primitive semiotic unit (Burke, 1969; DAngelo, 1992; Kellner,

    1981; Oswick et al, 2004). Since then the tropology has been relatively neglected as

    interest has focused on metaphor and conceptual blending theory (Fauconnier &

    Turner, 1998, 2003, 2008; Lakoff, 1987; Lakoff, (1993); Lakoff & Johnson, 1980;

    Taylor, 1989).

    We propose that a tropological approach offers fresh insights into language

    development. We will now rationalize use of the tropology in terms of the

    constraints we iterated above.

    Considering the rhetorical constraint, metaphor is a cybernetic, homeostatic

    process that continually questions the nature of perceived events and

    simultaneously our attempt to represent these events through language. Metaphor

    formation involves a top-down approach organising perception, synthesising

    information into a suitably conceptualisable form. The tropology constitutes the

    rhetorical basis for concept formation.

    Concerning the next three constraints, microgenetic, ontological and ontogenic we

    note that the master tropes were originally considered to be merely figures of

    speech, but we now realise that their function is more fundamental (Burke, 1969;

    DAngelo, 1987 and 1992; Kellner, 1981; Oswick et al, 2004). The development

    of language stems from images and tropes. The tropes represent the capacity of

    man for direct sensation and imaginative perception (D'Angelo, 1987; Danesi,

    2004). They underlie creative process based on the use of analogical reasoning.

    They are considered the basis for much of our understanding in everyday life

    (Culler, 1981). Tropes constitute a system by which the mind comes to grasp the

    world conceptually in language. They reflect our fundamentally relationalunderstanding of reality, a reality framed within systems of analogy (Chandler,

    2002). Analogy is a reflection of our sensitivity to ontological form, which is

    rooted in the perception of patterned resonance in the world (Zwicky, 2003).

    Tropes shape thought so enabling our minds to echo our world.

    Tropes are ontological concepts essential for our interpretation of reality. They

    symbolise relationships within phenomena, where each trope represents a specific

    strategy for presenting the perceived experience (White, 1985). They are modelsof the different directions thought might take to offer meaning to areas of

    experience not cognitively secured. Functioning together the tropological approach

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    7/22

    reflects ontogeny. The order of the master tropes is not ad hoc but reflects

    cognitive and semantic development (Kellner, 1981; DAngelo, 1992). The order

    is strictly and logically entailed. The tropology represents a form of knowing, of

    grasping a concept. The tropology accords, whether loosely or strictly, with many

    theoretical systems of knowing or coming to be known, as in Vico, Kant, Hegel,

    Marx and Goethe (Kellner, 1981; also see Burke, 1969). The tropology alsorepresents the stages of cognitive development which encompass both the

    phylogeny and ontogeny of cognitive systems (DAngelo, 1992; Kellner, 1981;

    Werner, 1944; Piaget, 1969; Vygotsky, 1962). Foucoult (1970) and Vico (1944)

    have shown that the logic of the tropology (poetic logic) underlies the general

    stages of development of Western thought.

    The next constraint is structural. We have previously raised the question underlying

    the cognitive linguistic approach to language formation: how is the physical world

    transformed into semantic notions? The answer was a structuralist approach, wherestructures are dynamic, morphological and gestalt based. The major tropes provide a

    dynamic, structural basis for concept formation.

    The next section will develop the recursive form of the tropology that is also central

    to the microgenic approach (Brown, 1988) where language formation consists of a

    cascade of whole/part shifts over evolutionary growth planes in the brain leading

    from a core in upper brainstem through limbic formations to the neocortical rim.

    Then in the following two sections weindicate the gestalt form of the tropology that

    is the foundation of the model. We show first how the tropology functions in a

    gestalt manner and then how perception functions tropologically. We propose that

    the latter is based on self-organisation in ontogeny where perception and the

    tropology become self similar (isomorphic). To corroborate the isomorphism we

    demonstrate homologies between the Gestalt principles and the master tropes. These

    findings reveal our recursive model of language development as inherently

    tropological and gestaltic. In the next stage we discuss metalinguistic development

    as the tropology self organises in ontogeny to function in fractal time. The

    synchronic form of the tropology collapses past present and future into one

    expression. These findings reflect on the nature of double scope blending in terms

    outlined above.

    The extension of the tropology through the forebrain

    The recursive pattern of the tropology is expressed in different ways through variouslevels of the forebrain. We will first briefly outline the extent in neurological terms

    (Brown, 1988) and then develop the tropological core. In neurological terms

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    8/22

    language can be conceived as levels in a "vertical" structure, not as centers in a two-

    dimensional network. These levels constitute a stratified system of phyletic growth

    planes. Language maps onto this structure. Language is the outcome of a multitiered

    system distributed over levels in the evolution of brain and behavior. The structure as

    a whole develops out of medial and paraventricular formations through several

    growth planes of limbic and paralimbic (transitional) cortex to a stage of generalized("association," "integration") cortex.

    Perceptual semantic transformation originates from linking polymodal sensorimotor

    circuits. At the most primitive level of language perceptual data is organised by the

    Gestalt principles. These dynamics involve the coordination of the sensorimotor

    activity with the right temporoparietal lobes. These early stages of language

    formulation integrate incoming topological information as Gestaltic primitives, e.g.

    as preverbal and prepositional structures. They define the morphemes, the most

    primitive semantic units. The right and left temporoparietal regions combine toreinterpret the morphemes and formulate speech production. Finally at the highest

    level of integration frontal lobe planning continually modulates the lower levels.

    In tropological terms language development stems from the slow maturation and

    eventually full coordination of the tropes. The tropes mature in the fixed order:

    metaphor; metonymy; synecdoche and finally, irony. The expression of the tropology

    differs from perception through to frontal planning (also see Kellner, 1981 for the

    following). Metalinguistic function ensues when ironic self-awareness develops

    leading to narrativity and the development of fluent speech.

    As the tropes gradually mature in function they unfold upon themselves. In their

    most primitive form, perception is interpreted in terms of a simple double binary

    system of categories, defining same - other (metaphor / metonymy) and part whole

    (synecdoche). This matrix forms bisociations that are the most thorough way of

    encompassing the diversity among the things that make up reality (States, 1998).

    With the development of ironic awareness the tropology becomes complete and

    functions in a cyclical manner. Maturation beyond the categorical level leads to

    projection upon a syntagmatic axis that represents self-explanation through speech.This is the second level of tropological explication, leading the tropes conceptually

    through the stages of linguistic cooperation above the morpheme. The lexical process

    of creating whole-whole correspondences to the concepts and words that are thus

    joined is a metaphoric transfer. The reductive categorisation of these words governed

    by grammatical rules of combination creates cause-effect entailments, which is

    metonymic. Synecdochal organisation at this level refers to the organic status of

    sentences, where the rules of syntax are the cohesive force. Finally, irony provides

    meaning from above the level of the sentence.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    9/22

    The highest level of tropological expansion is the level of thought and language

    planning in the frontal regions. The intentional state establishes an obligatory,

    recurrent and unidirectional configuration on the percept / thought interpretation

    process. Intentionality involves the selection of broad semantic units from the

    meaning potential, the equivalent of the paradigmatic or metaphoric axis of language.

    These selections invoke specific structural relationships depending on the contextualdemands needed for expression. The necessary entailments broadly select for the

    specificities of syntactic structure that will be realised in syntagms. These expressions

    underlie metonymy at this level. Synecdoche concerns the formulation of symbolic

    relationships needed to match the coevolving paradigmatic / syntagmatic aspect of

    expression. Symbols are selected relative to idealised relations and contexts in

    worlds of the same type, or possibly any imaginable type.

    Finally, the ironic stance overviews the planning processes. Irony represents a self-

    conscious process of interpretation of the process so that it foregrounds theinadequacy of words to things, of appearance to essence. Irony signals dissatisfaction

    with representation as such and motivates recourse to the tropological lexicon from

    which a new and more responsive formulation may be sought. Full tropological

    process leads to restructuring, an explication of the implicate order, momentary

    adaptation via a Darwinian based exploration of the fabric of embodiment.

    Tropology function in gestalt terms

    The gestalt function of the tropology is as follows. Metaphor is an emergent process

    where the individual creates a new perspective or meaning. To do that intentional

    needs highlight specific relationships within the emerging whole, the metaphoric

    representation of the real world or perceptual scene. Metonymy determines the

    contingent aspects of that specific intentional state. The representation of figural

    relationships emerges from the whole in context. Metonymy differentiates or reducesthe overall situation in a figure ground manner (see Koch, 1999; Talmy, 1988), where

    contiguity is the salient relation that exists between the sub frame elements of a

    conceptual frame or between the frame as a whole and its elements. That is, the

    salient links between elements of a given frame as constituting a prototypical

    conceptual gestalt are contiguity relations. From a Gestalt perspective perceptual

    saliency of stimuli critically depends on the surrounding context. Metonymy

    preserves the perceived relationships in contextual form as a contingent structure.

    Simultaneously, synecdoche compares and contrasts the developing contingencies

    with previously determined idealised structural relationships, representations of a

    general kind. For example, see Doursat & Petitot, 2005 (a) and (b) model and

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    10/22

    discussion of the development of simple spatial concepts such as in. In other words

    intelligence constructs a world first by turning outwards to objectise itself through

    metonymy only to turn inside, to come to know self in the image via synecdoche.

    Here the part-whole relationship is inverted; the part is broadly reinterpreted in terms

    of what remains invariant despite the contingencies of the real world. Synecdoche

    evolves to represent a higher level of interpretation, of the actual within the many possible worlds available to the imagination. Synecdoche recontextualises the

    specific within the personal.

    The tropology reconciles or blends the contrasting worlds so that the contingency of

    metonymy forms a best fit with the ideal or invariant one developed through

    synecdoche. With synecdoche the specificities of the moment may be placed in a

    broader context. Irony feeds back on both the actual and the ideal demands for

    expression. Irony questions the reality value of metonymy and of representation in

    general i.e. using words to express the essence of things. The metastable systemcontinually cycles through the tropes as it adapts to the moment. With each turn of

    the cycle the emergent metaphoric level of resolution leads to a novel perspective.

    Metaphor represents the now as an attempt to resolve the dynamics concerning:

    present contingencies (metonymy); past idealised contentions (synecdoche); and the

    future via irony.

    The tropological function of perception

    We have proposed that our language development model is based on the progressive

    self-organisation of the percept / metaphor interaction in ontogeny. Image schemata

    form top-down frames for organising perception via tropological function. We

    conclude that in ontogeny when the language system self-organises to criticality the

    function of both the tropology and perception becomes self similar, 1/f or isomorphic

    (Anderson & Mandell, (1996); Kello &.Van Orden (2009). We postulate that with

    self-organisation the perceptual system functions in a tropological manner where the

    components of the tropology and perception are homologous.

    We now propose and substantiate these homologies between each major trope and

    Gestalt principal: emergence, reification, invariance and multistability (Lehar, 2003).

    Metaphor is the analog of emergence. The unifying function of metaphoric

    resemblance accounts for the emergence of new meaning in language. It involves an

    act of perceptual and semantic restructuring where a sense of imagination operates to

    draw meaning from the comparison made (Ricoeur, 1975; 1991). The process

    includes an iconic moment or image that acts as a gathering of emergent meanings

    that underlies our ability to see reality other than that received. This is rooted in the

    imagination to construct an image from many diverse semantic fields.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    11/22

    In Gestalt theory, emergence refers to the formation of the macro structure, the

    Gestalt or meaningful form. The whole is greater than the parts. Emergence is

    unpredictable as the total structure remains beyond the sum of the instances. The

    dynamic aspect of emergence is reflected in the fact that the final global state is not

    computed in a single pass, but continuously, like a relaxation to equilibrium in a

    dynamic system model.

    Secondly, metonymy is analogous to reification. As indicated above metonymy is a

    figureground effect (see Koch, 1999; Talmy, 1988) where from a Gestalt

    perspective perceptual saliency of stimuli critically depends on the surrounding

    context. Metonymy conveys some incorporeal or intangible state in terms of the

    corporeal or tangible. It conserves perception of the worlds of objects, reflects their

    quiddity, their particular precisions (Hejinian, 1996). Metonymy underlies the

    gappy nature of thought, where looking through the gaps between the disparate

    contiguous parts imagines meaning. The gaps (relationships) point to a missingwhole, a structure that unites the parts.

    Reification refers to the perceptual state where specific virtual structures or

    singularities emerge in context. The gestalt is created through a constructive or

    generative process so that the part reified is defined by its contingent context. With

    reification more explicit information develops than is immediately obvious or present

    in the specific scene. As with metonymy meaning arises by looking through the

    gaps between the disparate contiguous parts.

    Synecdoche is equivalent to invariance. Synecdoche creates an explicit hierarchy by

    situating one thing as part of another. It is holistic and imagistic, presupposing some

    constitutive quality that can unify its relation to the world and symbolise this unity.

    Synecdoche underlies the categorical tendency that is the psychological (but not

    strictly formal) sense of invariance.

    Invariance is of great importance in Gestalt perception (Hochberg & McAllister,

    1953; Hoffman, 1992). The preferred interpretation of a stimulus is the one with the

    simplest code. That is a code that enables a reconstruction of the stimulus using a

    minimum number of descriptive parameters. Such a code is obtained by capturing amaximum amount of regularity and yields a hierarchical organization of the stimulus

    in terms of wholes and parts. Simplicity is often based on symmetry criteria. The

    gestalt is of great importance for the semantic development of the concept. The

    gestalt, is what remains invariant across all percepts, the preservation of similar

    structural relationships that have developed dynamically.

    Irony is analogous to multistability. Irony foregrounds the inadequacy of words to

    things, of appearance to essence. Irony underlines the discontinuity between what is

    said and what is meant, what is planned and what occurs. It takes the dialecticbetween external reality and language to a new level that is essential for resolution of

    the paradox.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    12/22

    Multistability indicates the tendency of ambiguous experiences to oscillate unstably

    between alternative interpretations. Interpretation of scenes can vary form moment to

    moment merely depending on what constitutes figure and context. Multistability

    underlies symmetry breaking and phase transformation in self-organisation. For

    example, with embedded figures metastability and phase transitions arise in the self-organising decisional process (Kelso and Tognoli, 2007). In general, symmetry

    breaking in cognition requires a creative answer to overcome the paradox.

    Language development based on dynamics within the tropology

    We have sought to establish the recursive nature of the tropology underlying

    language development through all levels of the forebrain. To do that we first showed

    that the tropology functions in a gestalt manner. Then we proposed that when the

    tropology and perception self-organise perception is governed by a tropological

    sequence: emergence, reification, invariance and multistability.

    Based on these findings we will now look at the model from two perspectives. In

    general terms we see language development as a progressive increase in the ability to

    extract, consolidate and manipulate gestalts within the extended forebrain system.Gestalts are the invariant structures determined by realising the universality aspects

    of each specific percept; that is, despite the differences between the infinite number

    of scenes the child meets (see Doursat & Petitot, 2005(a); Breidbach, & Jost, 2006).

    The dialectical tension between the specific and universal governs the development.

    Gestalts are formed through reification and then given initial significance through

    invariance, where they are correlated with preconceived transformations of a similar

    type. In ontogeny gestalts become more and more integrated as they are consolidated

    within the micro (perceptual level) and macro (extending to the forebrain) systems.

    The tropology stabilises and reinterprets the Gestalt through the interaction between

    metonymy and synecdoche. The process becomes one of progressive embedding

    through transformations. Objects and situations embed in verbs or morphogenic

    transformations. These are in turn further embedded through metonymy and

    synecdochal blending. From recursive tropological extension the gestalts become

    sentential semantic structures. They gain an increasing sense of meaning and

    permanence as they become consolidated (embedded) in the process. We see the

    primary function of the language system is to consolidate meaning potential through

    gestalt metaphor self organisation. The development and further use or the semantic

    sentential structures enables in to adapt from moment to moment. Within each

    moment as it updates it restructures self to best represent what we mean. Thisbecomes and is the essence of Pragnanz, our meaning as self-realisation.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    13/22

    In more specific terms language development is based on the gradual development of

    the ability to recognise objects and realise their meaning through contextual

    relationships. In the earliest development of perception the child differentiates

    aspects of the whole, experiencing objects apart from self. The semantic task is to

    stabilise perception. That is performed by transforming percepts into gestalts ofincreasing stability by increasing the state space of synecdochal or invariant forms.

    Stability is also conferred by more widely distributing the gestalt, making it a

    function of widespread coordinated forebrain activity.

    Language development is characterised by an increasing ability to manipulate gestalts

    by recontextualising them through tropal interaction. The gestalt is defined and

    structured by its context. Tropal dynamics modify gestalts so that the figure-ground

    relationship in perception becomes reinterpreted through the interaction or blending

    between metonymy and synecdoche and irony. In this blending manner the primitive perceptual syntagms become sentential semantic structures, the basic units of

    language.

    Metalinguistic development through self-organisation of the

    language system

    The development of the language system is based on increasing coordination of

    patterns through all levels of the forebrain. The embodied, situated nature of

    cognition is founded on the ability of these patterns spanning multiple time scales to

    organize in space and time (Kello & Van Orden, 2009; Kelso & Tognoli, 2007).

    Systems self organise to metastability (Bressler & Kelso, 2001; Kelso, 2002) which

    produces a range of brain behaviours where numerous patterns of activity co-exist as

    latent potentials (Kello et al., 2008). Systems become more flexible and metastable

    as their capacity to concurrently hold many distinct latent patterns increases.

    Fractal dynamics or 1/f scaling behaviour is pervasive throughout the nervous

    system. Fractal time means that the extended tropological processes become

    coordinated vertically (Anderson & Mandell, 1996). In systems at criticality

    behaviour becomes correlated across levels. In our model we propose that the three

    different temporal processes within the tropology become synchronous: irony relates

    to future time of planning; metonymy to the contingency of the present; synecdoche

    reflects the past, invariance gained through experience. Metaphor is emergent

    leading to the formation of new structural relations as each cycle of the tropology

    tries to resolve the particular intention. Thought and language stem from recurrent

    metaphoric anchoring of vast network scales in the human scale. This processbrings the past and future together in the here-and-now (Fauconnier & Turner, 2008).

    The synchronic coordination of the tropology is the basis for language. Language is a

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    14/22

    diachronic transformation and via self-reference feeds back to the planning and

    perceptual centers.

    As mentioned semantic growth is characterised by the increasing ability to

    recontextualise. In the early stages of development the child is governed by

    ambience so figure-ground (context or structure) are inseparable. The child cannotdissociate the figure from its context and language remains literal (mimetic, gestural).

    However, through self-organisation and developing ironic awareness the tropology

    turns back on itself. Self-awareness leads to major changes in the dynamics of the

    gestalt / tropology relationship. The development of self-reference means that

    reference to self means self and other, or subject and object. Self now becomes the

    structure within which the gestaltic object coevolves through self-reference.

    However, the self / object relation where self is context and object is figure are of

    different logical types. Now the object is dissociated from the self; figure can also be

    separated from its ground. This leads to the increasing autonomy of language and thedevelopment of metalanguage.

    The ability to dissociate figure from ground, or context, is essential for conceptual

    fluidity, metaphor and language development. The individual realises that figure and

    ground can be interchanged, or even changed completely. Recontextualisation occurs

    at will. The ability to realise figures in different contexts underlies the growth of

    imagination and meaning. All meaning refers back to self-interpretation so that

    eventually we can say and even become anything we wish.

    Further semantic aspects of the model

    We have proposed that self-organisation between gestalts and the tropology can serve

    as a basis to model semantic development. We conclude by further justifying the

    importance of a tropological approach. The first is what Piaget called significant

    implication (Scholnook & Cookson, 1994). Meaning is the understanding of how anaction changes (transforms) the world. Significant implication constructs the action-

    result regularities that image schemata represent. In order to understand the logic of

    action, the child must first build contingencies based on the inference that certain

    actions rather than others produce specific results. The tropology is an analog of

    Piagets schemata depicting the general stages of cognitive development (DAngelo,

    1992). The tropology provides a basis for the logic inherent in action perception

    schemata. This same logic can now be seen to pervade the coordinated activity

    necessary for language development.

    Second, we have seen that narrativity develops as the tropology matures to form

    recursive cyclical activity. Narrativity is of great psychological importance because

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    15/22

    it organises the structure of human experience. Narrativity typifies the child after 3

    or 4 where they begin to remember their autobiographical experience (Young &

    Saver, 2001). Narrativity constructs our notion of reality and asserts that the

    experience of life takes on meaning through allegorical inflation.

    Thirdly, tropological process leads to development of the sense of continuity of self.This process is characterised by transitions between stable and unstable phase

    synchronisation as seen in metastability. The tropes of transition and closure

    underlie the instability (see Grossman, 1998). Metaphor and synecdoche are the

    brakes of the tropological machine; metonymy and irony are its engines. Metaphor

    and synecdoche represent the identity of versions of self differing over time.

    Metaphor affirms the identity between things and their inner meaning. Synecdoche

    represents the sublation of difference in the perceived homogeneity of subject and

    object. Metonymy and irony on the other hand represent transitions in the search for

    identity. Metonymy denies identity. However, with irony there is a simultaneous

    negation and preservation of identity that advances the dialectic to a new phase. The

    tropology oscillates between cycles of tension and resolution, where the

    confrontation of the tropes informs the progress of internal narrative innovation that

    underlies identity as continuity within change.

    Finally, in addition to our comments on irony we further highlight the important role

    of irony in modeling language development (Colbrook, 2003; White, 1985). Irony is

    essential for developing meaning; how knowledge or gnosis arises from language.

    Gnosis is not an algorithmic process but rather a pragmatic need to understand theworld. This process is better understood as diagnosis, literally knowing through, a

    knowledge gained through ironic self-awareness, the self-reflective process of the

    tropology (Kellner, 1981). Here knowledge comes from non-understanding. That is,

    not by man extending his mind and taking the world in. Rather he makes things out

    of himself and becomes them by transforming himself into them.

    Discussion

    We have developed a dynamic model of language development based on a unifying

    concept: tropes shape thought so enabling our minds to mould our world. In the

    language model the forebrain functions as a complex adaptive system subserved by

    the metastable tropology dynamically reconstructing self from moment to moment.

    This is a gestalt notion, where the tropes coordinate to generate semantic sentential

    structures that can serve as the primary currency of semantic exchange at all levels ofthe neuraxis. These enable us to conceive inputs or ideas in contextual terms and

    then recontextualise them so that we can become them. In becoming we are

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    16/22

    restructured through self-explication and ultimately self-explanation.

    The thesis provides a systematic link between gestalt and metaphor. We have

    asserted many reasons for the continuing relevance of the tropology in language

    development. In particular the model reflects the way in which four-dimensional

    space-time becomes internalised as fractal space-time. That transformation is the keyto semantics. We see the tropology as central to that conversion because it functions

    in a gestalt manner and perception is tropological. We contend that these

    groundbreaking ideas are pivotal to understanding the unity of forebrain function.

    The model has several other important aspects. It introduces a novel basis for

    studying language development in terms of seven self-organising parameters. Four

    of these relate to transformations in ontogeny between the gestalt principles and the

    tropes: reification and metonymy; invariance and synecdoche; multistability and

    irony; and emergence of perception and metaphor. The fifth axis concerns therelationship between local multistability and the global coordinative concept,

    metastability. The sixth axis is the development of metalinguistic capability through

    to self-organisation. Finally, we provide a fresh way to understand Pragnanz as the

    semantic goal of the tropological gestalt system. In ontogeny we come to perceive

    objects as a function of the extended tropology. As we perceive with intention we

    project downwards (from our past) so that objects are discovered anew. That fresh

    outlook stems from each language act where we reinvent objects through the

    emergent process based on the recursive process of blending, irony and metaphor.

    The object becomes meaningful as we relate to it by extending fractal time into

    reflexic fractal space-time. Gestalt production restructures self through Pragnanz.

    The model enables us to consider the importance of time constraints in language

    development. Communication is governed by conflicting constraints. It must

    maximise meaning in minimal time. Gestalts, iconic or imagistic forms have rich

    relational semantics but they are ambiguous (Wilden, 1980). On the other hand,

    although discursive language has a powerful syntax that makes it unambiguous, it

    takes time. The model proposes that the optimal resolution of the conflict between

    these two forms of communication lead to development of a gestalt system thatgenerates semantic structures with a sentential form. These semantic sentential

    structures are produced from and represent brain function at points of instability, at

    criticality and metastability, where brain adaptation is optimised. We propose that

    his type of max / min optimizing process leads to Zipfs law where there is a 1/f

    relationship between word length and usage. It also helps to understand the

    importance of implication in metonymy and gestalt formation through synecdoche

    and metaphor.

    Furthermore, the brain functions at metastability in order to compress its infiniteoptions to completion (in a semantic sense) in the least time. This mechanism

    requires a strong constraining force, limiting language and maximising semantics.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    17/22

    We propose that irony evolved to subserve that function. Irony resolves the conflict

    between the actual and the ideal frames. It also enables us to adopt a metalinguistic

    stance. Irony questioning both actualisation (metonymy) and representation

    (synecdoche). Irony may acts as an optimiser in the max / min dynamic mentioned

    above.

    We propose that the model also provides fresh insights into the mechanisms

    underlying microgenesis (Brown, 1988, 1998). We will detail the close relationship

    between the two models more comprehensively in a later paper. At this stage we

    note that microgenesis is couched in terms of neurological process and process

    metaphysics. It is concerned with the concept of time, change and the actualisation

    (becoming) over phases in the brain in the momentary development of a cognition.

    Becoming creates the novelty as well as the duration through which the entity

    momentarily exists. Each novel moment is a constituent of an imaginative series over

    which the entity endures. Our model is also based on the creation of time (timecollapse, fractal time) and imagination. These issues will serve as a major link

    between the two theories.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    18/22

    References

    Anderson, C.M.. & Mandell, A. J. (1996). Fractals of brain, fractals of mind: in

    search of a symmetry bond in E.R. Mac Cormac, M. Stamenov,.Advances inconsciousness research. Editors: J. Benjamins Publishing. Amsterdam. 75- 127.

    Breidbach, O. & Jost, J. (2006) On the gestalt concept. Theory in Biosciences. 125,

    19-36.

    Bressler, S.L. & Kelso, J.A.S. (2001). Cortical coordination dynamics and cognition,

    Trends in Cognitive Science, 5, 26-36.

    Brown, J. W. (1988). The life of the mind. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. London.

    Brown, J. W. (1998). Foundations of cognitive metaphysics. Process Studies, Center

    for Process Studies 21:1-2, 79-92.

    Burke, K. (1969). A grammar of motives. Berkeley: University of California.

    Chandler, D. (2002). Semiotics: The basics. London: Routledge.

    Colebrook, C. (2003) Irony in the work of philosophy. University of Nebraska Press.

    Culler, J. (1981). The pursuit of signs: Semiotics, literature, deconstruction. London:

    Routledge & Kegan Paul.

    Danesi, M. (1993). Vico, metaphor and the origin of language. Indiana University

    Press.

    Danesi, M. (2004). Poetic logic. The role of metaphor in thought, language and

    culture. Madison: Atwood Publishing.

    D'Angelo, F.J. (1987). Prolegomena to a rhetoric of tropes. Rhetoric Review, 6, 32-

    40.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    19/22

    D'Angelo, F.J. (1992). The four master tropes: Analogues of development. Rhetoric

    Review, 11, 91-107.

    Doursat, R. & Petitot, J. (2005a) Bridging the gap between vision and language: A

    morphodynamical model of spatial categories. Proceedings of the International Joint

    Conference on Neural Networks.

    Doursat, R. & Petitot, J. (2005b) Dynamical systems and cognitive linguistics:

    Toward an active morphodynamical semantics. Neural Networks. 18: 628-638.

    Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (1998). "Conceptual Integration Networks." Cognitive

    Science, 22(2): 133-187.

    Fauconnier, G. & Turner M. (2003). The Way We Think. New York: Basic Books.

    Fauconnier, G. & Turner, M. (2008) Rethinking Metaphor. In Ray Gibbs, ed.

    Cambridge Handbook of Metaphor and Thought. Cambridge University Press. New

    York: Cambridge University Press, 53-66.

    Foucault, M. (1970). The order of things. Tavistock London.

    Grady, J.E. (2005). Image schemas and perception: Refining a definition. In: B.

    Hampe, J. Grady (Ed.) From perception to meaning. Image schemas in cognitive

    linguistics, Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Grossman, M. (1998) The story of all things: Writing the self in English renaissance

    narrative poetry. Duke University Press.

    Hampe, B. (2005). Image schemas in cognitive linguistics. In B. Hampe & J. Grady

    (Eds.), From perception to meaning. Berlin:Mouton de Gruyter. 1-15.

    Hejinian, L. (1996). The rejection of closure. Onward. Contemporary poetry and

    poetics, New York: Peter Lang.

    Hochberg, J.E., & McAlister, E. (1953). A quantitative approach to figural goodness,

    Journal of Experimental Psychology, 46, 361-364.

    Hoffman, W.C. (1992). Symmetry and geometric psychology. Symmetry: Culture &

    Science, 3, 305-310.

    Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind. The bodily basis of meaning, imagination,

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    20/22

    and reason. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Kellner, H. (1981). The inflatable trope as narrative theory: Structure or allegory?

    Diacritics, 11, 14-28.

    Kello, C.T. &.Van Orden, G.C., (2009). Soft assembly of sensorimotor function.

    Nonlinear Dynamics, Psychology and Life Sciences, 13, 57-78.

    Kelso, J.A.S. & Tognoli, E. (2007) Towards a complementary neuroscience:

    Metastable coordination dynamics of the brain. In: Ed. L. I. Perlovsky, I.; R. Kozma,

    Neurodynamics of cognition and consciousness. Springer Berlin / Heidelberg, 39-61.

    Kelso, J.A.S. (2002). The complementary nature of coordination dynamics: Self-organization and agency. Nonlinear Phenomena in Complex Systems, 5, 364-371.

    Koch P. (1999) Frame and contiguity: on the cognitive basis of metonymy and

    certain types of word formation. In K.-U. Panther and G., Radden (Eds.) Metonymy

    in language and thought. (pp 139-169). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Lakoff, G. (1993) The contemporary theory of metaphor. Metaphor and Thought.

    Ed. A. Ortony. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp 202-251.

    Lakoff, G. & Johnson, M. (1980). Metaphors we live by. Chicago: University of

    Chicago Press.

    Langacker, R. (1987). Foundations of Cognitive Grammar. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford

    University Press.

    Langacker, R. W. (1990). Concept, image, and symbol. The cognitive basis of

    grammar. Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Lehar, S. (1980). The world in your head: A gestalt view of the mechanism of

    conscious experience. Mahwah, New Jersey and London: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Manjali, F. (1997): (a) Catastrophe Theoretic Semantics: Towards a Physics of

    Meaning. Dynamical Models in Semiotics/Semantics. Semiotics Institute Online.:(b) Metaphors in Grammar. Dynamical Models in

    Semiotics/Semantics. Semiotics Institute Online.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    21/22

    Nuessel, F. (1996). Introducing semiotics. Semiotica. Volume 110, Issue 1-2, 145

    196

    Oswick, C., Putnam, L., & Keenoy, T. (2004). Tropes, discourse and organising. In:D.Grant, C.Oswick, L.Putnam.The Sage handbook of organizational discourse. Sage

    Publications. 105-129.

    Petitot, J. (1995). Morphodynamics and attractor syntax. In T. van Gelder & R. Port

    (Eds.), Mind as Motion. MIT Press.

    Petitot, J. (2003). Morphogenesis of Meaning. Peter Lang.

    Piaget, J. & Inhelder, B. (1969). The psychology of the child. New York. Basis.

    Ricoeur, P. (1975). The rule of metaphor. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Ricoeur, P. (1991). Metaphor and the main problem of hermeneutics. A Ricoeur

    Reader: Reflection and Imagination. Toronto: University of Toronto Press.

    Rohrer, T. (2005). Image schemata in the brain. In B. Hampe and J. Grady (Eds.),

    From perception to meaning. (pp 165-199). Berlin:Mouton de Gruyter.

    Scholnick, E.K. & Cookson, K. (1994) A developmental analysis of cognitive

    semantics: What is the role of metaphor in the construction of knowledge and

    reasoning? In W. F. Overton, D. S. Palermo (Ed.) The nature and ontogenesis of

    meaning: image schemas in cognitive linguistic. New Jersey, U.K.: Lawrence

    Erlbaum Associates. Pp 109-128.

    Skldberg, K. (2002). The poetic logic of administration: Styles and changes of style

    in the art of organizing. Routledge. London. Spangenberg.

    States, B.O. (1998) Of paradoxes and tautologies. American Scholar, 67, 51-66.

    Talmy, L. (1988). The relation of grammar to cognition. In Rudzka- Ostyn, (ed.)

    Topics in cognitive linguistics. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. 165-205.

  • 8/9/2019 Modelling Language Development Based on Self Organisation Between Metaphor and Gestalts

    22/22

    Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a Cognitive Semantics. Volume I: Concept Structuring

    Systems. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Taylor, J.R. (1989). Linguistic categorisation: prototypes in linguistic theory.

    Oxford: Claredon Express.

    Thom, R. (1972). Structural stability and morphogenesis. New York: Benjamin,

    Paris: Ediscience.

    Thom, R. (1990). Eng. trans. by V. Meyer, as Semio Physics: A Sketch. Aristotelian

    Physics and Catastrophe Theory, Addison-Wesley.

    Vico, G. (1944). The new science of Giambattista Vico. Trans. T. Bergin, M. Fisch.

    Ithaca. Cornell UP.

    Vygotsky, L.S. (1962). Thought and language. Ed. E.Hanfmann, G.Vakar.

    Cambridge MA: MIT P.

    Werner, H. (1948). Comparative psychology of mental development. Ed. P.Follett,

    Chicago.

    White, H. (1985). Metahistory: The historical imagination in nineteenth-centuryEurope. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Young, K. & Saver, J.L. (2001) The neurology of narrative. SubStance, 30, 72-84.

    Zwicky, J. (2003) Wisdom and metaphor. Gaspereau Press.