1
www.postersession.com Mitigating Implicit Bias among Criminal Court Jurors: Intervention through Instruction Hannah Bolotin and Steven Stemler, PhD Department of Psychology, Wesleyan University Bracketing Implicit bias exists across environments and toward a wide variety of individuals and social groups 4 . Implicit racial bias can influence criminal justice procedure and outcomes 5,6,7 . While some district courts have attempted to mitigate the effect of implicit bias on jurors’ decisions through special instructions 8 , the efficacy of such instructions remains understudied 9 . Participants Participants (N = 217) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and compensated $1.00 for participating. Participants did not find Black defendants guilty more often than White defendants, nor were there significant effects of defendant race for any of the additional measures. Error bars represent standard deviations. * Implicit bias instruction excerpted from “Duty of Jury” preliminary instruction to be given before opening statements, Model Ninth Circuit Criminal Instruction 1.1, Western District of Washington State. **Sentencing options were proposed by two prosecuting attorneys working in state criminal courts. The sentencing options were converted into a 6-point numerical scale (1 = least severe option; 6 = most severe option) for analysis. Implicit bias instruction did not influence rate at which participants’ found Black defendants guilty, nor did instruction influence ratings on any of the additional measures, but we could not fully assess the effect of the instruction given the aforementioned lack of baseline bias. Error bars represent standard deviations. Battery case: χ2 = 2.15e-31, p = 1; Robbery case: χ2 = 1.00e-31, p = 1; Shoplifting case: χ2 = 0.79, p = .374 All additional outcome measures, across all three cases: p > .05 Battery case: χ2 = 1.02e -30 , p = 1 ; Robbery case: χ2 = 0.00, p = 1; Shoplifting case: χ2 = .045, p = .831 All additional outcome measures, across all three cases: p > .05 Fig. 3: How strong is the case in favor of the defense? Fig. 4: How responsible is the defendant for the crime? Fig. 5: Please rate the defendant on the extent to which he possesses each of the following characteristics. Unlike previous studies investigating juror race bias 10,11 , the present study also investigated baseline differences in judgments depending on participant (i.e., mock juror) race, independent of defendant race and instruction condition. There was a strong pattern of differences between White and Black participants’ evaluations of case materials, such as: Black participants rated the strength of the defense’s case higher than White participants across all three cases (Fig. 3). White participants rated the defendants as more responsible for the crime than Black participants in the battery and robbery cases while Black participants rated the defendants as more responsible than White participants in the shoplifting case (Fig. 4). White participants rated the defendants as more culpable for the crimes than Black participants, based on a defendant characteristics scale score, in the battery and robbery cases (Fig. 5). Despite the lack of studies on this topic, it is reasonable to think that Black and White individuals could have “different race-related motivations when they serve as jurors,” such as Black jurors evaluating cases with more concern for institutional bias in mind 12 . Ultimately, if there are in fact baseline racial differences in juror decision-making (i.e., if future studies focusing on juror race differences can replicate the present finding), then that offers an additional avenue for consideration and intervention, such as substantiating an argument for racial diversity in jury panels. Battery: t = 4.28, p < .001 Robbery: t = 5.15, p < .001 Shoplifting: t = 3.67, p = .001 Error bars represent standard deviations. Battery: t = -2.30, p = .03 Robbery: t = -2.14, p = .036 Shoplifting: t = 2.17, p = .032 Error bars represent standard deviations. Battery: t = -1.85, p = .003; Robbery: t = -3.31, p = .002; Shoplifting: t = -1.04, p > .05 Error bars represent standard deviations. Lifetime Likelihood of Imprisonment by Race 3 In the United States, African Americans represent… 38 Results – Planned Analyses Methods Participants did not show baseline bias against which to assess the effect of the implicit bias jury instruction . This may be due to: A lack of ambiguity in defendant guilt A lack of mundane realism (i.e., the study was not sufficiently similar to a real courtroom trial for juror bias to present itself) Necessity of reinforcement through multiple instructions 38% of Prison Population 2 References Participants were asked to evaluate 3 separate criminal cases (Battery; Robbery; Shoplifting) Jury Instruction Manipulation (Control v. Implicit Bias Instruction*) Defendant Race Manipulation (Black v. White Defendant) Verdict decision (”Guilty” or “Not Guilty”) Sentencing Recommendation** Defendant Culpability Ratings Between-subjects design (Instruction Condition X Defendant Race) Case Strength Ratings Defendant Characteristics Scale 1 Nellis, A. (2016). The Color of Justice: Racial and Ethnic Disparity in State Prisons. Retrieved from The Sentencing Project website: https://www.sentencingproject.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/06/The-Color-of-Justice-Racial-and-Ethnic- Disparity-in-State-Prisons.pdf. 2 U.S. Census Bureau. (2018). Retrieved April 13, 2019, from United States Census Bureau website: https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/US/PST045218. 3 Bonczar, T. P. (2003). Prevalence of Imprisonment in the U.S. Population, 1974-2001. Bureau of Justice. 4 Kahler 2007 4 Greenwald, A. G. & Krieger, L. H. (2006). Implicit bias: Scientific foundations. California Law Review, 94(4). 5 Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2018). Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(4), 393–401. 6 James, L., James, S. M., & Vila, B. J. (2016). The Reverse Racism Effect. Criminology & Public Policy, 15(2), 457–479. 7 Radelet, M. L., & Pierce, G. L. (1985). Race and prosecutorial discretion in homicide cases. Law & Society Review, 19(4), 587–621. 8 Doyle, J. T. (2017). U.S. district court produces video, drafts jury instructions on implicit bias. King County Bar Association Bulletin, 2. 9 Elek, J. K., & Hannaford-Agor, P. (2014). Can explicit instructions reduce expressions of implicit bias? New questions following a test of a specialized jury instruction. SSRN Electronic Journal. 10 Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2000). Race in the courtroom: Perceptions of guilt and dispositional attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 1367–1379. 11 Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2001). White juror bias: An investigation of prejudice against Black defendants in the American courtroom. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 201–229. 12 Sommers, S. R. (2007). Race and the decision making of juries. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 12(2), 171-187. 13% of General Population 1 Research Question: Is an implicit bias jury instruction sufficient to reduce mock jurors’ presentation of bias in their judicial decisions? Reshape the current study before concluding that implicit juror bias does not exist. The present study’s inability to replicate previously established implicit juror racial bias 10,11 should not be viewed as justification that such bias has vanished. Statistics such as those presented in the Introduction continue to suggest bias interferes with colorblind justice. Rethink bias interventions. Intervention during jury deliberation. Courts can instruct jurors to engage in a “checks and balances” system for bias mitigation during deliberation, such that jurors “watch out” for evidence of implicit bias in other jurors’ decision-making. During jury selection. Questioning potential jurors about their racial attitudes can “serve to remind Whites of their egalitarian values, suppressing the eventual expression of racial bias” 16 . During the trial. Studies have shown that an attorney’s description of the “role race played in [a given] incident was enough to lead White jurors to render nonprejudiced judgments of a Black defendant” 16 . I would like to thank the Wesleyan Feldman Fund for their support. For further information regarding this project, please contact Hannah Bolotin ([email protected]). Fig. 2: Does the implicit bias instruction influence the rate at which the Black defendant is found guilty? Fig. 1: Are Black defendants rated as guilty more often than White defendants? 1. 3. 2a. 4. 5. 6. 7. 2b. Conclusions and Future Directions Results – Exploratory Analyses Exploratory Analyses: Participant Race Effects Introduction

Mitigating Implicit Bias among Criminal Court Jurors: Intervention …spsp.org/sites/default/files/Hannah-Bolotin.pdf · 2020-03-10 · Mitigating Implicit Bias among Criminal Court

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    6

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Mitigating Implicit Bias among Criminal Court Jurors: Intervention …spsp.org/sites/default/files/Hannah-Bolotin.pdf · 2020-03-10 · Mitigating Implicit Bias among Criminal Court

www.postersession.com

Mitigating Implicit Bias among Criminal Court Jurors: Intervention through Instruction

Hannah Bolotin and Steven Stemler, PhDDepartment of Psychology, Wesleyan University

Bracketing

• Implicit bias exists across environments and toward a wide variety of individuals and social groups4.• Implicit racial bias can influence criminal justice procedure and outcomes5,6,7.• While some district courts have attempted to mitigate the effect of implicit bias on jurors’ decisions

through special instructions8, the efficacy of such instructions remains understudied9.

ParticipantsParticipants (N = 217) were recruited from Amazon’s Mechanical Turk and compensated $1.00 for participating.

Participants did not find Black defendants guilty more often than White defendants, nor were there significant effects of defendant race for any of the additional measures.

Error bars represent standard deviations.

* Implicit bias instruction excerpted from “Duty of Jury” preliminary instruction to be given before opening statements, Model Ninth Circuit CriminalInstruction 1.1, Western District of Washington State.

**Sentencing options were proposed by two prosecuting attorneys working in state criminal courts. The sentencing options were converted into a 6-pointnumerical scale (1 = least severe option; 6 = most severe option) for analysis.

Implicit bias instruction did notinfluence rate at which participants’ found Black defendants guilty, nor did instruction influence ratings on any of the additional measures, but we could not fully assess the effect of the instruction given the aforementioned lack of baseline bias.

Error bars represent standard deviations.

Battery case: χ2 = 2.15e-31, p = 1; Robbery case: χ2 = 1.00e-31, p = 1; Shoplifting case: χ2 = 0.79, p = .374All additional outcome measures, across all three cases: p > .05

Battery case: χ2 = 1.02e-30, p = 1; Robbery case: χ2 = 0.00, p = 1; Shoplifting case: χ2 = .045, p = .831All additional outcome measures, across all three cases: p > .05

Fig. 3: How strong is the case in favor of the defense?

Fig. 4: How responsible is the defendant for the crime?

Fig. 5: Please rate the defendant on the extent to which he possesses each of the following characteristics.

Unlike previous studies investigating juror race bias10,11, the present study also investigated baselinedifferences in judgments depending on participant (i.e., mock juror) race, independent of defendant raceand instruction condition.

There was a strong pattern of differences between White and Black participants’ evaluations of casematerials, such as:

• Black participants rated the strength of the defense’s case higher than White participants across all threecases (Fig. 3).

• White participants rated the defendants as more responsible for the crime than Black participants in thebattery and robbery cases while Black participants rated the defendants as more responsible than Whiteparticipants in the shoplifting case (Fig. 4).

• White participants rated the defendants as more culpable for the crimes than Black participants, basedon a defendant characteristics scale score, in the battery and robbery cases (Fig. 5).

Despite the lack of studies on this topic, it is reasonable to think that Black and White individuals couldhave “different race-related motivations when they serve as jurors,” such as Black jurors evaluating caseswith more concern for institutional bias in mind12. Ultimately, if there are in fact baseline racialdifferences in juror decision-making (i.e., if future studies focusing on juror race differences can replicatethe present finding), then that offers an additional avenue for consideration and intervention, such assubstantiating an argument for racial diversity in jury panels.

Battery: t = 4.28, p < .001 Robbery: t = 5.15, p < .001 Shoplifting: t = 3.67, p = .001Error bars represent standard deviations.

Battery: t = -2.30, p = .03Robbery: t = -2.14, p = .036Shoplifting: t = 2.17, p = .032Error bars represent standard deviations.

Battery: t = -1.85, p = .003; Robbery: t = -3.31, p = .002; Shoplifting: t = -1.04, p > .05Error bars represent standard deviations.

Lifetime Likelihood of Imprisonment by Race3

In the United States,African Americans represent…

38

Results – Planned Analyses

Methods

Participants did not show baseline bias against which to assess the effect of the implicit bias juryinstruction.

This may be due to:• A lack of ambiguity in defendant guilt• A lack of mundane realism (i.e., the study was not sufficiently similar to a real courtroom trial for

juror bias to present itself)• Necessity of reinforcement through multiple instructions

38% of Prison Population2

References

Participants were asked to evaluate 3 separate criminal cases

(Battery; Robbery; Shoplifting)

Jury Instruction Manipulation

(Control v. Implicit Bias Instruction*)

Defendant Race Manipulation

(Black v. White Defendant)

Verdict decision (”Guilty” or “Not Guilty”)

Sentencing Recommendation**

Defendant Culpability Ratings

Between-subjects design (Instruction Condition X Defendant Race)

Case Strength Ratings

Defendant Characteristics Scale

1Nellis, A. (2016). The Color of Justice: Racial and Ethnic Disparity in State Prisons. Retrieved from The Sentencing Project website: https://www.sentencingproject.org/wp-content/uploads/2016/06/The-Color-of-Justice-Racial-and-Ethnic-Disparity-in-State-Prisons.pdf.2U.S. Census Bureau. (2018). Retrieved April 13, 2019, from United States Census Bureau website: https://www.census.gov/quickfacts/fact/table/US/PST045218.3Bonczar, T. P. (2003). Prevalence of Imprisonment in the U.S. Population, 1974-2001. Bureau of Justice.4Kahler 20074Greenwald, A. G. & Krieger, L. H. (2006). Implicit bias: Scientific foundations. California Law Review, 94(4).5Hehman, E., Flake, J. K., & Calanchini, J. (2018). Disproportionate use of lethal force in policing is associated with regional racial biases of residents. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 9(4), 393–401. 6James, L., James, S. M., & Vila, B. J. (2016). The Reverse Racism Effect. Criminology & Public Policy, 15(2), 457–479. 7Radelet, M. L., & Pierce, G. L. (1985). Race and prosecutorial discretion in homicide cases. Law & Society Review, 19(4), 587–621. 8Doyle, J. T. (2017). U.S. district court produces video, drafts jury instructions on implicit bias. King County Bar Association Bulletin, 2.9Elek, J. K., & Hannaford-Agor, P. (2014). Can explicit instructions reduce expressions of implicit bias? New questions following a test of a specialized jury instruction. SSRN Electronic Journal. 10Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2000). Race in the courtroom: Perceptions of guilt and dispositional attributions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26(11), 1367–1379. 11Sommers, S. R., & Ellsworth, P. C. (2001). White juror bias: An investigation of prejudice against Black defendants in the American courtroom. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 7(1), 201–229. 12Sommers, S. R. (2007). Race and the decision making of juries. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 12(2), 171-187.

13% of General Population1

Research Question: Is an implicit bias jury instruction sufficient to reduce mock jurors’ presentation of bias in their judicial decisions?

Reshape the current study before concluding that implicit juror bias does not exist.

The present study’s inability to replicate previously established implicit juror racial bias10,11 should not beviewed as justification that such bias has vanished. Statistics such as those presented in the Introductioncontinue to suggest bias interferes with colorblind justice.

Rethink bias interventions.

Intervention during jury deliberation. Courts can instruct jurors to engage in a “checks and balances”system for bias mitigation during deliberation, such that jurors “watch out” for evidence of implicit biasin other jurors’ decision-making.

During jury selection. Questioning potential jurors about their racial attitudes can “serve to remindWhites of their egalitarian values, suppressing the eventual expression of racial bias”16.

During the trial. Studies have shown that an attorney’s description of the “role race played in [a given] incident was enough to lead White jurors to render nonprejudiced judgments of a Black defendant”16.

I would like to thank the Wesleyan Feldman Fund for their support.

For further information regarding this project, please contact Hannah Bolotin ([email protected]).

Fig. 2: Does the implicit bias instruction influence the rate at which the Black defendant is found guilty?

Fig. 1: Are Black defendants rated as guilty more often than White defendants?

1.

3.

2a.

4.

5.

6.

7.

2b.

Conclusions and Future Directions

Results – Exploratory AnalysesExploratory Analyses: Participant Race Effects

Introduction