1
Before exposure, the experimental group (Fig. 1a) showed 25% accuracy (s.d. 11%), which is not significantly different from 33% chance (binomial from chance: P40.9). Exposure for 21 days doubled the androstenone-detection accuracy in both the exposed nostril (from 25% to 55%, change from baseline: t (11)43.3, P*0.007; binomial from chance: P*0.002) and the unexposed nostril (from 25% to 49%, change from baseline: t (11)42.3, P*0.04; binomial from chance: P*0.02). There was no significant difference in the extent of improvement between the exposed and unexposed nostrils (t (11)40.4, P40.7). We screened a further 50 subjects to obtain 12 more non-detectors (9 female) for a control study (Fig. 1b) in which we tested for confounding possibilities: first, subjects were initially selected for non- detection, and thus their performance could either remain the same or improve, but not deteriorate (a ‘floor effect’); second, even a minute leak in the block may have enabled the blocked nostril to learn; third, participation in olfactory testing may improve performance over time. The con- trol study was identical to the experimental, except that both nostrils were blocked. Control subjects (Fig. 1b) did not differ from the experimental group at baseline (mean accuracy at baseline: experimental group, 25%, s.d. 11; control group, 27%, s.d. 11, t (22)40.44, P40.7), and remained at chance detection after 21 days of exposure (change from baseline: t (11)41.6, P40.14; binomial from chance: P40.4). The differ- ence between the two groups after exposure (mean accuracy after 21 days, average for both nostrils) was 52% (s.d. 20) in the experimental group and 35% in the control group (s.d. 14; t (22)42.3, P*0.03), negat- ing possible confounding factors. We exploited the paired anatomy of the olfactory system 5,6 to demonstrate that the plasticity that underpins the emergence of androstenone detection originates in the central components of the olfactory system. These components may be likened to pattern recognition, which occurs at the olfactory bulbs or in primary olfactory cortex — a substrate that shares information from both nostrils 7 and is optimized for olfactory learn- ing 8–10 . We do not rule out a contribution to plasticity from the peripheral components of the olfactory system 11,12 — peripheral recep- tors may be induced in the unexposed nostril in response to a central signal (direct or hormonal, for example). It remains to be determined how central and peripheral mechanisms could interact to maximize plasticity in the olfactory system. Joel D. Mainland*, Elizabeth A. Bremner†, Natasha Young*†, Brad N. Johnson ‡, Rehan M. Khan*, Moustafa Bensafi*, Noam Sobel*†‡ *Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, Department of Psychology, and Program in Bioengineering, University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720, USA e-mail: [email protected] 1. Wysocki, C. J. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 86, 7976–7978 (1989). 2. Wang, H. W., Wysocki, C. J. & Gold, G. H. Science 260, 998–1000 (1987). 3. Dalton, P., Doolittle, N. & Breslin, P. A. Nature Neurosci. 5, 199–200 (2002). 4. Gilbert, A. N. & Wysocki, C. J. Natl Geogr. Mag. 172, 514–525 (1987). 5. Cain, W. S. Nature 268, 50–52 (1977). 6. Sobel, N. et al. Nature 392, 282–286 (1998). 7. Wilson, D. A. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 160–169 (1997). 8. Haberly, L. B. & Bower, J. M. Trends Neurosci. 12, 258–264 (1989). 9. Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A. & Leon, M. J. Neurosci. 9, 3998–4006 (1989). 10.Brennan, P. A. & Keverne, E. B. Progr. Neurobiol. 51, 457–481 (1997). 11. Nevitt, G. A. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 91, 4288–4292 (1994). 12.Yee, K. K. & Wysocki, C. J. Physiol. Behav. 72, 705–711 (2001). Competing financial interests: declared none. COMMUNICATIONS ARISING Light microscopy Beyond the diffraction limit I n a comment on resolution in light microscopy 1 , Stelzer makes some mislead- ing, if not erroneous, points. His claim to have himself demonstrated the relationship between Abbe’s diffraction limit and Heisen- berg’s uncertainty principle is surprising: this relationship is readily derived from the Fourier theory. The resolution criterion that he seeks to convey is more perplexing: in his description of Dyba and Hell’s work 2 , Stelzer ignores the fact that the resolution of any recording system, whether optical or of any other type, is determined by the span of signal frequencies transferred. The higher the transferred frequencies, the finer are the details and the better is the resolution. A standard way to determine these fre- quencies and the resolution of a micro- scope — or of any other signal-recording instrument — is to record the instrument’s responses to point- or step-like objects. The responses observed by Dyba and Hell 2 are clearly of the subdiffraction type, with fre- quencies beyond the diffraction barrier; they are object-independent by definition. In contrast to Stelzer’s view, Dyba and Hell’s images, including that of Bacillus megaterium, do not imply a priori informa- tion about the sample. The distance between the bacterial membranes is there- fore not relevant for proving that the dif- fraction barrier has been broken. Pekka Hänninen Department of Medical Physics, University of Turku, Turku, Finland e-mail: [email protected] 1. Stelzer, E. H. K. Nature 417, 806–807 (2002). 2. Dyba, M. & Hell, S. W. Phys. Rev. Lett. 88, 163901 (2002). 802 NATURE | VOL 419 | 24 0CTOBER 2002 | www.nature.com/nature Olfactory plasticity One nostril knows what the other learns A bout 30% of the adult human popula- tion does not perceive an odour when sniffing the steroid androstenone (5- a-androst-16-en-3-one), but will become sensitive to its smell after repeated exposure to the compound 1–3 . Here we investigate the origin of the plasticity that governs this acquired ability by repeatedly exposing one nostril of non-detecting subjects to androstenone and then testing the unex- posed nostril. We find that the exposed nostril and the naive nostril can both learn to recognize the smell, effectively doubling detection accuracy. As the two olfactory epithelia are not connected at the peripher- al level, our results indicate that learning occurs in the brain by a mechanism that shares information from both nostrils. We screened 42 subjects for their ability to detect androstenone by using a four-trial, three-alternative forced-choice paradigm. As expected, this screen yielded 29% non-detec- tors 4 (12 out of 42, of whom 7 were female), who were then exposed to this odour for 10 min daily for 21 days. One nostril was blocked by insertion of an inflatable plug, and heated humidified air was injected through the plug at 5 litres min 11 to prevent androstenone from entering the occluded nostril by reverse flow (retronasal olfaction). brief communications 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 Accuracy (%) Baseline Exposed nostril (day 21) Unexposed nostril (day 21) Baseline Right nostril (day 21) Left nostril (day 21) a b Figure 1 Detection accuracy of androstenone odour by individual nostrils of initially insensitive subjects before (baseline) and after 21 days of exposure. a, Experimental group; b, control group; dotted lines, chance. Odorants were presented in 60-ml glass bottles to blindfolded subjects. To prevent odour contamination, all testing and exposure was done in a room coated with stainless steel and equipped with high-efficiency particulate air filtration and high-rate carbon filtration. An adjustable vacuum hood was placed over each subject’s head. To avoid experimenter-generated cues, all commands (such as “sniff now”), questions (such as “which jar contains the odorant?”) and replies were generated by a computer-controlled digitized voice (further details at http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~borp/supp.htm). © 2002 Nature Publishing Group

Light microscopy (communication arising): Beyond the diffraction limit

  • Upload
    pekka

  • View
    213

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Before exposure, the experimental group(Fig. 1a) showed 25% accuracy (s.d. 11%),which is not significantly different from33% chance (binomial from chance:P40.9). Exposure for 21 days doubled theandrostenone-detection accuracy in boththe exposed nostril (from 25% to 55%,change from baseline: t(11)43.3, P*0.007;binomial from chance: P*0.002) and theunexposed nostril (from 25% to 49%,change from baseline: t(11)42.3, P*0.04;binomial from chance: P*0.02). There wasno significant difference in the extent ofimprovement between the exposed andunexposed nostrils (t(11)40.4, P40.7).

We screened a further 50 subjects toobtain 12 more non-detectors (9 female)for a control study (Fig. 1b) in which wetested for confounding possibilities: first,subjects were initially selected for non-detection, and thus their performancecould either remain the same or improve,but not deteriorate (a ‘floor effect’); second,even a minute leak in the block may haveenabled the blocked nostril to learn; third,participation in olfactory testing mayimprove performance over time. The con-trol study was identical to the experimental,except that both nostrils were blocked.

Control subjects (Fig. 1b) did not differfrom the experimental group at baseline(mean accuracy at baseline: experimentalgroup, 25%, s.d. 11; control group, 27%, s.d.11, t(22)40.44, P40.7), and remained atchance detection after 21 days of exposure(change from baseline: t(11)41.6, P40.14;binomial from chance: P40.4). The differ-ence between the two groups after exposure(mean accuracy after 21 days, average forboth nostrils) was 52% (s.d. 20) in theexperimental group and 35% in the controlgroup (s.d. 14; t(22)42.3, P*0.03), negat-ing possible confounding factors.

We exploited the paired anatomy of theolfactory system5,6 to demonstrate that theplasticity that underpins the emergence ofandrostenone detection originates in thecentral components of the olfactory system.These components may be likened to patternrecognition, which occurs at the olfactorybulbs or in primary olfactory cortex — asubstrate that shares information from bothnostrils7 and is optimized for olfactory learn-ing8–10. We do not rule out a contribution toplasticity from the peripheral components ofthe olfactory system11,12 — peripheral recep-tors may be induced in the unexposed nostrilin response to a central signal (direct or hormonal, for example). It remains to bedetermined how central and peripheralmechanisms could interact to maximizeplasticity in the olfactory system.Joel D. Mainland*, Elizabeth A. Bremner†,Natasha Young*†, Brad N. Johnson ‡, Rehan M. Khan*, Moustafa Bensafi*, Noam Sobel*†‡*Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute, †Department

of Psychology, and ‡Program in Bioengineering,University of California at Berkeley, Berkeley,California 94720, USAe-mail: [email protected]

1. Wysocki, C. J. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 86,

7976–7978 (1989).

2. Wang, H. W., Wysocki, C. J. & Gold, G. H. Science 260,

998–1000 (1987).

3. Dalton, P., Doolittle, N. & Breslin, P. A. Nature Neurosci. 5,

199–200 (2002).

4. Gilbert, A. N. & Wysocki, C. J. Natl Geogr. Mag. 172,

514–525 (1987).

5. Cain, W. S. Nature 268, 50–52 (1977).

6. Sobel, N. et al. Nature 392, 282–286 (1998).

7. Wilson, D. A. J. Neurophysiol. 78, 160–169 (1997).

8. Haberly, L. B. & Bower, J. M. Trends Neurosci. 12, 258–264 (1989).

9. Sullivan, R. M., Wilson, D. A. & Leon, M. J. Neurosci. 9,

3998–4006 (1989).

10.Brennan, P. A. & Keverne, E. B. Progr. Neurobiol. 51,

457–481 (1997).

11.Nevitt, G. A. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 91,

4288–4292 (1994).

12.Yee, K. K. & Wysocki, C. J. Physiol. Behav. 72, 705–711 (2001).

Competing financial interests: declared none.

COMMUNICATIONS ARISING

Light microscopy

Beyond the diffraction limit

In a comment on resolution in lightmicroscopy1, Stelzer makes some mislead-ing, if not erroneous, points. His claim to

have himself demonstrated the relationshipbetween Abbe’s diffraction limit and Heisen-berg’s uncertainty principle is surprising:this relationship is readily derived from theFourier theory. The resolution criterion thathe seeks to convey is more perplexing: in hisdescription of Dyba and Hell’s work2, Stelzerignores the fact that the resolution of anyrecording system, whether optical or of anyother type, is determined by the span of signal frequencies transferred. The higherthe transferred frequencies, the finer are the details and the better is the resolution.

A standard way to determine these fre-quencies and the resolution of a micro-scope — or of any other signal-recordinginstrument — is to record the instrument’sresponses to point- or step-like objects. Theresponses observed by Dyba and Hell2 areclearly of the subdiffraction type, with fre-quencies beyond the diffraction barrier;they are object-independent by definition.In contrast to Stelzer’s view, Dyba andHell’s images, including that of Bacillusmegaterium, do not imply a priori informa-tion about the sample. The distancebetween the bacterial membranes is there-fore not relevant for proving that the dif-fraction barrier has been broken.Pekka HänninenDepartment of Medical Physics, University ofTurku, Turku, Finlande-mail: [email protected]

1. Stelzer, E. H. K. Nature 417, 806–807 (2002).2. Dyba, M. & Hell, S. W. Phys. Rev. Lett. 88, 163901 (2002).

802 NATURE | VOL 419 | 24 0CTOBER 2002 | www.nature.com/nature

Olfactory plasticity

One nostril knows what the other learns

About 30% of the adult human popula-tion does not perceive an odour whensniffing the steroid androstenone (5-

a-androst-16-en-3-one), but will becomesensitive to its smell after repeated exposureto the compound1–3. Here we investigate theorigin of the plasticity that governs thisacquired ability by repeatedly exposing one nostril of non-detecting subjects toandrostenone and then testing the unex-posed nostril. We find that the exposed nostril and the naive nostril can both learnto recognize the smell, effectively doublingdetection accuracy. As the two olfactoryepithelia are not connected at the peripher-al level, our results indicate that learningoccurs in the brain by a mechanism thatshares information from both nostrils.

We screened 42 subjects for their abilityto detect androstenone by using a four-trial,three-alternative forced-choice paradigm. Asexpected, this screen yielded 29% non-detec-tors4 (12 out of 42, of whom 7 were female),who were then exposed to this odour for 10 min daily for 21 days. One nostril wasblocked by insertion of an inflatable plug,and heated humidified air was injectedthrough the plug at 5 litres min11 to preventandrostenone from entering the occludednostril by reverse flow (retronasal olfaction).

brief communications

70605040302010

0

70605040302010

0

Acc

urac

y (%

)

Baseline Exposednostril

(day 21)

Unexposednostril

(day 21)

Baseline Rightnostril

(day 21)

Leftnostril

(day 21)

a

b

Figure 1 Detection accuracy of androstenone odour by individual

nostrils of initially insensitive subjects before (baseline) and after

21 days of exposure. a, Experimental group; b, control group;

dotted lines, chance. Odorants were presented in 60-ml glass

bottles to blindfolded subjects. To prevent odour contamination, all

testing and exposure was done in a room coated with stainless

steel and equipped with high-efficiency particulate air filtration

and high-rate carbon filtration. An adjustable vacuum hood was

placed over each subject’s head. To avoid experimenter-generated

cues, all commands (such as “sniff now”), questions (such as

“which jar contains the odorant?”) and replies were generated by

a computer-controlled digitized voice (further details at

http://socrates.berkeley.edu/~borp/supp.htm).

© 2002 Nature Publishing Group