45
Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Lecture 21

communication and collaboration models

Page 2: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Today’s Lecture Introduction Face-to-face communication Conversation

Grounding Text-based communication Group working

Page 3: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Overview All computer systems, single user or multi-

user, interact with the work-groups and organizations in which they are used.

Need to understand normal human-human communication face-to-face communication involves eyes,

face and bodyconversation can be analysed to establish its

detailed structure

Page 4: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Overview This can then be applied to text-based

conversation, which has Reduced feedback for confirmation Less context to disambiguate expression Slower pace of interaction But is more easily reviewed

Group working is more complex than that of a single person Influenced by the physical environment Experiments are more difficult to control and record Field studies must take into account the social situation

Page 5: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Social nature of humans Humans are inherently social creatures We live together, work together, learn

together, play together, etc. Therefore, we need to develop interactive

systems that support and extend these kinds of social interactions

Communication and collaboration

Page 6: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Face-to-face communication Most primitive and most subtle form of

communication Often seen as the paradigm

for computer mediated communication

Page 7: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Face-to-face communication Transfer effects

Carry expectations into electronic media People are adaptable

e.g. “the use of ‘over’ for turn-taking when using a walkie-talkie”

But also expect they can use existing norms (e.g. cultural)

Page 8: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Face-to-face communicationSometimes with disastrous results

The rules of face-to-face conversation are not conscious, so, when they are broken, we do not always recognize the true problem.

May interpret failure as rudeness of colleague e.g., Personal space

video may destroy mutual impression of distance happily the “glass wall” effect helps

Often the ‘glass wall’ afforded by the video screen makes the precise distance less important, which couldhave a positive effect during cross-cultural meetings.

Page 9: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Personal Space

Page 10: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Eye contact To convey interest and establish social

presence Video may spoil direct eye contact But poor quality video better than audio

onlyEstablishing context – focus of the

conversation

Page 11: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Gestures and body language Much of our communication is through our

bodies Gesture (and eye gaze) used for deictic

reference Deictic – “directly pointing out” (oxford dictionary)

Head and shoulders video loses this So: close focus for eye contact or wide focus for body

language?

Page 12: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Back channels Alison:

do you fancy that film . . . er. . . ‘The green’ um . . . it starts at eight.

Brian: great!

Not just the words! Back channel responses from Brian at 1 and 2

Quizzical at 1 Affirmative at 2

Back channels include: Nods and grimaces Shrugs of the shoulders Grunts and raised eyebrows Utterance begins vague then sharpens up just enough

Page 13: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Back channels II Restricting media restricts back channels

Video … loss of body language Audio … loss of facial expression Half Duplex … lose most voice back channel responses Text Based … nothing left!

Back channels used for turn-taking: Speaker offers the floor (fraction of a second gap) Listener requests the floor (facial expression, small noise) Grunts, ‘um's and ‘ah's, can be used by the:

listener to claim the floor speaker to hold the floor

But often too quiet for half-duplex channels

Trans-continental conferences - special problems lag can exceed the turn taking gap

leads to a monologue!

Page 14: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Basic conversational structure Alison: Do you fancy that film Brian: the uh (500 ms) with the black cat –”The Green

whatsit” Alison: yeah, go at uh (looks at watch 1.2 s) twenty to? Brian: sure

Smallest unit is the utterance Turn taking … utterances usually alternate

Page 15: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Basic conversational structure Simplest structure - adjacency pair Adjacency pairs may nest;

Brian: Do you want some gateau? (X) Alison: is it very fattening? (Y) Brian: yes, very (Y) Alison: and lots of chocolate? (Z) Brian: masses (Z)

Alison: I'll have a big slice then. (X)

Structure is: B-x, A-y, B-y, A-z, B-z, A-x Inner pairs often for clarification But, simple pairing is not always possible or useful

Page 16: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Context in conversation

Utterances are highly ambiguous We use context to disambiguate

Brian: (points) that post is leaning a bitAlison: that's the one you put in

Page 17: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Context in conversation

Two types of context:external context

reference to the environment e.g., Brian's “that” = the thing pointed to [deictic

reference]

internal context reference to the previous conversation e.g., Alison's “that” = the last thing spoken of

Page 18: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Context in conversation Often contextual utterances involve

indexicals: that, this, he, she, it

These may be used for internal or external context

Also descriptive phrases may be used:external: “the corner post is leaning a bit” internal: “the post you mentioned”

Page 19: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Common Ground Resolving context depends on meaning

participants must share meaning so must have shared knowledge

Conversation constantly negotiates meaning process called grounding

Alison: So, you turn right beside the river. Brian: past the hotel. Alison: yeah -

Each utterance is assumed to be: relevant - furthers the current topic helpful - comprehensible to listener

Page 20: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Focus and breakdown Context resolved relative to current dialogue focus

Alison: Oh, look at your roses – Brian: mmm, but I've had trouble with green fly. Alison: they're the symbol of the English summer. Brian: green fly? Alison: no roses silly!

Tracing topics is one way to analyse conversation. Alison begins - topic is roses Brian shifts topic to green fly Alison misses shift in focus = breakdown

Page 21: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Focus and breakdown

You can classify utterances by the task they perform in the conversation Substantive

– directly relevant to the development of the conversation

Annotative – points of clarification, elaboration etc

Procedural – talking about the process of collaboration itself

Page 22: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Focus and breakdown

Alison is giving Brian directions, using a whiteboard

Alison: you go along this road until you get to the river Brian: do you stop before the river or after you cross it? Alison: before Brian: draw the river in blue and the road in black Alison: So, you turn right beside the river Brian: past the hotel Alison: yeah … is there another black pen? This one is

running dry.

substantive

annotative

procedural

substantive

substantive

procedural

annotative

NB: The final utterance is “procedural technical” and indicates that the system has become apparent to the participants

Page 23: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Breakdown Breakdown happens at all levels:

Topic, indexicals, gesture

Breakdowns are frequent, but:

Redundancy makes detection easy(brian cannot interpret “they're the symbol of the english summer”)

People very good at repair(brain and alison quickly restore shared focus)

Electronic media may lose some redundancy= breakdown more severe

Page 24: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

breakdown Alison: Isn’t that beautiful

Points to a large male deer (stag) standing next to a tree Brian: the symmetry of the branches

He thinks she pointed to the tree Alison: how some people can dislike them I cannot understand! Brian: Yes – the park rangers should shoot all those damn deer

before they kill the trees off for good! Alison: (silence)

NOTE: Brian’s reference to symmetrical branches MAY have sounded to Alison like a reference to the stag’s antlers!

Page 25: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Speech-Act Theory A specific form of conversational analysis Utterances characterised by what they do,

they’re actse.g., “I'm hungry”

propositional meaning – hunger intended effect – “get me some food”

Classic example: “I now pronounce you man & wife”

Page 26: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Speech-Act Theory Basic conversational acts (illocutionary

points):PromisesRequestsDeclarationsAssertionsCountersRenegesWithdrawals

Page 27: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Speech-Act Theory

Speech acts need not be spokene.g., silence often interpreted as

acceptance

Page 28: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Speech-Act Theory Generic patterns of acts can be identified:

Conversation for action (CfA) Seeks to obtain a specific request

Conversation for clarification (CfC) Usually embedded in CfA - to clarify the requested

actionConversation for possibilities (CfP)

Looking towards future actionsConversation for Orientation (CfO)

Building a shared understanding

Page 29: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Conversations for action

Circles represent ‘states’ in the conversation Arcs represent utterances (speech acts)

Simplest route 1-2-3-4-5: Alison: have you got the market survey on chocolate? [request] Brian: sure [promise] Brian: there you are [assert] Alison: thanks [declare]

More complex routes possible, e.g., 1-2-6-3 Alison: have you got – [request] Brian: I've only got the summary figures [counter] Alison: that'll do [accept]

Page 30: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Text based communication Most common media for asynchronous

groupwareexceptions: voice mail, answer phone

Familiar medium, similar to paper lettersbut, electronic text may act as speech

substitute!

Page 31: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Text based communication Types of electronic text:

Discrete: directed messages, no structureLinear: messages added (in temporal order)Non-Linear: hypertext linkagesSpatial: two dimensional arrangement

Page 32: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Text based communication Most obvious loss, no facial expression or

body languageweak back-channels, so it is difficult to

convey: affective state - happy, sad, angry humorous illocutionary force - urgent, important, deferential

Participants compensate by flaming and smilies ;-)

Page 33: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Grounding constraints Establishing common ground depends on

grounding constraintsCo-Temporality: - instant feedthroughSimultaneity: - speaking togetherSequence: - utterances ordered

33

Page 34: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Grounding constraints These constraints are often weaker in text

based communication than in face-to-face conversatione.g., loss of sequence in linear text:

network delays or coarse granularity = overlap

Page 35: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Grounding constraints

1. Bethan: how many should be in the group?2. Rowena: maybe this could be one of the 4 strongest reasons3. Rowena: please clarify what you mean4. Bethan: I agree5. Rowena: hang on6. Rowena: Bethan what did you mean?

Message pairs 1&2 and 3&4 composed simultaneously

i.e., lack of common experience

Rowena: 2 1 3 4 5 6 Bethan: 1 2 4 3 5 6

Above shows breakdown of turn-taking result of poor back channels

Page 36: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Maintaining context

Recall context was essential for disambiguation

Text loses external context, hence deixis (cf: deictic) linking to shared objects can help

1. Alison: Brian's got some lovely roses

2. Brian: I'm afraid they're covered in green fly

3. Clarise: I've seen them, they're beautiful

Both (2) and (3) are responses to (1) but the transcript suggests green

fly are beautiful Hypertext can maintain ‘parallel’

conversations

Page 37: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Pace and Granularity

Pace of conversation - the rate of turn taking face-to-face - every few seconds telephone - half a minuteemail - hours or days

face-to-face conversation is highly interactive If initial utterance is vague feedback gives cues for

comprehension lower pace = less feedback = less interactive

Page 38: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Pace and Granularity

Coping strategies attempt to increase granularity:eagerness - looking ahead in the

conversation game Brian: Like a cup of tea? Milk or lemon?

multiplexing - several topics in one utterance Alison: No thanks. I love your roses.

Page 39: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

The Conversation Game

Conversation is like a game Linear text follows one path through it Participants choose the path by their utterances Hypertext can follow several paths at once

Page 40: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Group dynamics

Workgroups constantly change: in structure in size

Several groupware systems have explicit roles But roles depend on context and time

e.g., M.D. down a mine is under the authority of the foreman e.g., a General can be under a Private during an Int. Briefing

and may not reflect duties e.g., subject of biography, author, but now writer

Page 41: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Group dynamics

Social structure may change: democratic, autocratic,and group may fragment into sub-groups

Groupware systems rarely achieve this flexibility Groups also change in composition

new members must be able to ‘catch up’

Page 42: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Physical environment

Face-to-face working radically affected by layout of workplacee.g., meeting rooms:recessed terminals reduce visual impact inward facing to encourage eye contactdifferent social-power positions

Traditional cognitive psychology is all in the head

Page 43: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Physical environment

Distributed cognition suggests we look to the world

Thinking takes place in interaction with other people and the physical environment

Implications for group work: importance of mediating representationsgroup knowledge greater than sum of partsdesign focus on external representation

Page 44: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Summary Face-to-face communication is extremely

complex. People maintain precise distances, which can

be disrupted through video links. At a higher level, the structure of

conversation can be seen as a sequence of turns, usually alternating between the participants.

Page 45: Lecture 21 communication and collaboration models

Summary Context is important in disambiguating

utterances, especially when deictic reference is also used.

Text-based communication loses most of the low-level feedback of face-to-face conversation.

Group dynamics make it very difficult to predict how a particular group willbehave.