Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

  • Upload
    coie8t

  • View
    214

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    1/17

    Spring 2009 Volume 1 Number 1, Edited by Elin Lobel, Ph.D.

    Official Publication of the Laban/Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies (LIMS)

    Journal ofLaban Movement Studies

    Inside this Issue

    Applications of Laban Movement Analysis:

    An Integrated Approach to Voice, Speech, and

    Movement Training for Actors.

    Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors:

    Revealing the Equivalence Between Movement

    and the Sounds of Music.

    Inter-relationships Between Laban MovementAnalysis and Psychoanalysis: A Qualitative

    Research Project.

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    2/17

    Volume 1, Number 1, 2009 Laban/Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies-LIMS, New York, New York

    Journal of

    Laban Movement StudiesOfficial Publication of theLaban/Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies-LIMS

    EditorElin Lobel, Ph.D., CMA

    Department of KinesiologyTowson University, Towson, MD

    Assistant EditorsLinda Almar Caldwell, Ph.D., CMA

    Department of Dance

    Texas Womens University, Denton, TX

    Kate Jobe, M.A., CMA, Dipl. P.O. Psych., RSMT/EProcess Work Institute

    Portland, OR

    Editorial BoardThomas Casciero, Ph.D., CMA

    Karen Studd, MS, CMA, RSMT & RSMEBarbara Nordstrom-Loeb, MFA, CMA, ADTR, MAMFT

    Ann Axtman, Ph.D., CMAMartie Barylick, CMA

    External ReviewKaren Bradley, M.A. CMA

    Julie A. Brodie, MFA

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    3/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies (JLMS)is a publication of the Laban/Bartenieff Institute of Move-ment Studies (LIMS). It publishes scholarly articles, book reviews, and reports of scholarly conferences,archives, and other projects of interest to the field. We accept submissions for publication consideration fromboth members and nonmembers of LIMS. Scholarly articles are peer reviewed.

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies (JLMS) considers for publication original manuscripts on topics relatedto the theoretical, methodological, historical, and practical aspects of Rudolf Labans movement studies andtheir applications. Articles may present current research, scholarly reviews, or an application to a practice,research, or teaching methodology. All manuscript submissions should follow theJLMSauthor guidelines.

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies (JLIMS) is a refereed publication, using a blind review process. TheEditor and at least one outside reader independently review all articles before an editorial decision is made.Authors are advised of the editorial decision as soon as it is available.

    Call For Papers

    Laban Movement Studies in the World of Theatre (secondissue)

    TheJournal of Laban Movement Studies (JLMS) is requesting submis-

    sions in the area of Laban Movement Studies (Laban MovementAnalysis and Bartenieff Fundamentals) and its relationship to thediscipline of Theatre. Particular areas of interest at this timeinclude, but are not limited to, research, historical perspective,applications to theatre practices, and the development and execu-tion of pedagogy or training methodology.

    There is great potential for Laban Movement Studies (LMS) in afield as diverse and expressive as Theatre. The Guest Editor envi-sions and encourages a wide range of possible topics the role ofLMS in acting and actor training, alignment, clown, Commedia,cirque training, directing, devised theatre, historical contexts,mask work, physical characterization, set design, stage combat,tension release, voice pedagogy (general or specific), Viewpointstraining, and more.

    Direct all manuscripts and inquiries for the Theatre issue to:Tom Casciero, Ph.D., Guest Editor,JLMSTheatre DepartmentTowson University8000 York RoadTowson, MD 21252Tel: 410-704-3503, Fax: 410-704-3914Email: [email protected]

    Acknowledgments

    Thanks to all who participated in making this journal possible by donating technical, personal and editorialskills. Some of those people are:Ann Axtman, Karen Bradley, Martie Barylick, Linda Almar Caldwell,Thomas Casciero, Kate Jobe, Elin E. Lobel, Alyson Nehren, Barbara Nordstrom-Loeb, and Karen Studd.

    Journal and cover design:Kate Jobe

    Laban/Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies, LIMS - Board of Directors: Executive Committee, VirginiaReed, President of LIMS. Regina Miranda, LIMS Chair of the Board and Chief Executive Officer, Karen Brad-

    ley, LIMS Vice-Chair and Director of Research. Board Members: Bob Bejan, Jane Bonbright, Nanette Burst-ein, Luis Cancel, Marcia Feuerstein, Marjorie Hamilton, Lucinda Lavelli, Bala Sarasvati, Keith Sedlacek, JanWhitener.

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies is published semi annually by the Laban/Bartenieff Institute, LIMS.520 Eighth Avenue, Suite 304. New York, NY 10018. Tel: 1.212.643.8888; Fax: 1.212.643.8388. LIMS website:www.limsonline.org. Contact LIMS at: [email protected]

    Copyright 2009,Laban/Bartenieff Institute for Movement Studies (LIMS). No part of this publicationmay be reproduced (except brief excerpts for the purpose of review or citation and except where otherwisenoted) by any means, electronic or mechanical, without the written permission of the publisher. The opinionsexpressed in The Journal of Laban Movement Analysisare those of the authors and are not necessarily endorsed bythe Laban/Bartenieff Institute of Movement Studies (LIMS).

    All correspondance regarding the current issue should be directed to: Elin Lobel, Ph.D., Editor,Journal ofLaban Movement Studies, Department of Kinesiology, Towson University, 8000 York Road,

    Bartenieff Fundamentals (thirdissue)

    TheJournal of Laban Movement Studies(JLMS) is requesting submissions in thearea of Bartenieff Fundamentals.

    Direct all manuscripts and inquiries forthe Theatre issue to:

    Elin Lobel, Ph.D., Editor,Journal ofLaban Movement Studies,Department of Kinesiology,Towson University,8000 York Road,Towson, MD 21252-0001,Fax: 410-704-3912 ,[email protected].

    Deadline: June 9, 2009 is the

    deadline for inclusion in thissecond and third issue ofJLMS.

    For complete submission guidelines

    please refer to www.limsonline.org

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    4/17

    Journal of

    Laban

    MovementStudiesVolume 1, Number 1, 2009

    Editorial

    Documenting Laban Movement Analysis in the 21stCentury: The First

    Issue of the Journal of Laban Movement StudiesElin E. Lobel, PhD, CMA

    Feature Articles

    Applications of Laban Movement Analysis: An Integrated Approach to

    Voice, Speech,and Movement Training for ActorsBarbara Adrian, MFA, CMA

    Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors: Revealing the Equivalence

    Between Movement and MusicCharles Gambetta, DMA

    Interrelationships Between Movement Analysis and Psychoanalysis: A

    Qualitative Research ProjectKatya Bloom, PhD, CMA

    5

    6

    21

    35

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    5/17

    20

    Laban Movement Analysis for

    Conductors: Revealing the Equivalence

    Between Movement and Music

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    Abstract

    Most modes of live musical perfor-

    mance require the synchronized participa-

    tion of mind and body. Some certainly

    require greater physical activity than others,

    and all traditional instrumental and vocal

    music performance endeavors share the

    same direct link between the movements

    required to generate sounds and the sounds

    themselves. Upon producing sounds, musi-

    cians receive simultaneous aural feedback

    that informs them of the level of congru-

    ency between their imagined interior per-

    formance and the external, aurally

    experienced performance. Continuous

    comparison of that external flow of musical

    events with the interior flow of audiation or

    music thinking enables performers to

    make adjustments to their movements

    (including the breath) and to the physical

    relationships with their instruments in

    order to maintain control over all elements

    of their performance. Such adjustments

    might include fingering changes, embou-

    chure adjustments, adding or ceasing a

    vibrato, postural shifts and changing the

    qualities of bow strokes or tonguings.

    Conductors, unlike their collaborators

    in the orchestra or choir who wed move-

    ment to sound through the context of aninstrument, have no direct physical contact

    with an instrument that produces musical

    sounds. Yet highly skilled conductors know

    the precise musical effects their movements

    will produce before they execute them in

    spite of this apparent absence of such physi-

    cal contact. Therefore conductors, more

    than all other musical performers, must cul-

    tivate within themselves the confluence of

    musical and physical expression to such a

    degree that they are able to audiate sound

    and movement together as a single gestalt.

    The resulting condition of mind and body

    suggests a consciously induced state of Syn-

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    6/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies Spring 2009 Volume 1, Number 1

    21

    esthesia wherein an individual seems to hear

    movements and touch sounds. Conductors

    who have so merged their kinesthetic and

    musical abilities have found the genuineequivalence between music and movement.

    The application of Laban Movement Analy-

    sis (LMA) presented in this essay introduces

    an approach to conducting that augments

    traditional emphasis on beating in repetitive

    patterns with Effort/Shape instruction.

    Exposure to and experience with these trans-

    formative qualities of movement present

    conductors with the opportunity to discover

    the single common source of their musical

    interpretations and expressive movements.

    The Problem

    Conducting technique is a highly spe-

    cialized, extremely rich form of nonverbal

    communication through gesture. Conduc-

    tor Hermann Scherchen (1891-1966) identi-

    fies gesture as the conductors one and only

    medium during performance. He further

    maintains that conducting gesture must

    indicate perfectly clearly the metrical course

    of the work; and at the same time, it must

    convey in unequivocal fashion the varying

    expression and general shape of the work.1

    In other words, the conductors gestures

    should convey tempo, rhythm, articulation,dynamics, character, style, breath and

    shapeevery nuance the orchestra needs to

    complete a mutually satisfying musical per-

    formance.2Consummation of this marriage

    of music to movement comes only when the

    conductor has established a deep and

    unbreakable physical connection between

    his gestures and his fully formed abstract

    representation of the sounds of music. To

    effectively communicate musical intentionsto an ensemble, conducting gestures must

    demonstrate an understanding of the funda-

    mental equivalence between movement and

    music. Laban described this relationship on

    several occasions including his 1958 address

    to the Annual Conference of the Laban Art

    of Movement Guild.

    Many people will associate this world,in their minds, with the realm of

    music. This is quite right so far as one

    considers the branch of the art of

    human movement resulting in the pro-

    duction of works which become audi-

    ble to the ear. Few people realize that

    all music, vocal or instrumental, is pro-

    duced by movements of the body.3

    The means to experience and fully

    exploit this equivalence continues to eludemany conductors (save those fortunate few

    blessed with a natural inner kinesthetic

    sense) because they lack the direct physical

    connection to the sounds of music that con-

    tact with an instrument provides. Yet effec-

    tive conductors, like the musicians under

    their direction, know the precise musical

    effects their movements will produce before

    they execute them. My study of Laban Move-

    ment Analysis (LMA) led me to develop an

    approach to conducting gesture that sup-

    plies every conductor with the tools and ter-

    minology he needs to discover and traverse

    his own personal path towards the conver-

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    7/17

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    22

    gence of music thinking (audiation) and

    movement thinking that is the essence of

    the art of conducting.4

    Related Research

    Thirty years ago, Neale King Bartee

    (1977) introduced conductors to Labans

    theoretical framework with his groundbreak-

    ing thesis, citing the limited familiarity that

    conductors have with the possibilities of

    movement as the most significant motiva-

    tion for his investigation.5Foremost among

    Bartees proposals was his assertion thatthorough grounding in the practice of

    Effort/Shape sequences can help conduc-

    tors create gestures that better convey the

    mood and feelings reflected in their inter-

    pretations of the music. Paraphrasing

    Laban, Bartee maintains:

    [The] performance of sequences of

    Effort and Shape structures can pro-

    duce moods and feelings which corre-spond to [these sequences]. This

    concept implies that certain move-

    ments express certain effects and that

    the conductor can learn which pat-

    terns better express the mood he is try-

    ing to achieve in the music.6

    While he stops short of describing a

    specific method for using LMA to unite ges-

    ture with musical intent, Bartee extends thistheoretical application from moods to

    expression, observing that Labans link

    between movement and inner feeling pat-

    terns gives the conductor a basis for develop-

    ing expressive movements corresponding to

    his conception of the expressive line of the

    music.7 After adding a lengthy list of spe-

    cific implications to support these two over-

    arching themes, Bartee sums up his

    exploratory thesis by concluding that theconductor can improve his ability to use

    expressive gesture by studying movement as

    it is practiced in the movement arts.8

    Interest in LMA training for conduc-

    tors has since waxed and waned in cycles

    that seem to follow the publication of new

    research studies and the introduction of

    LMA workshops and seminars at academic

    and professional conferences. Researchers

    Hibbard (1994) and Benge (1996) arranged

    for Certified Movement Analysts to observe

    experienced conductors in rehearsal and/or

    performance. Results from their analyses

    revealed a number of possible connections

    between the conductors gestures and the

    expressive qualities of the music they were

    conducting.9Several additional studies usedvarious research modelssome experimental

    and some descriptiveto investigate the

    effects of LMA training on conductors or

    ensemble members.

    Billingham (2001) used the eight Basic

    Effort Actions (BEAs) as the basis for a

    vocabulary of conducting gestures that

    proved to be partially successful.10

    After pre-paring one choir with LMA training and the

    other with traditional rehearsal technique,

    choral conductor Holt (1992) found that the

    LMA-trained choir performed better than

    the traditionally prepared choir.11 Miller

    (1988) split two populations of conducting

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    8/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies Spring 2009 Volume 1, Number 1

    23

    students into two treatment groups and two

    control groups respectively. The treatment

    groups were given a combination of LMA

    training and traditional instruction whilethe control groups received only traditional

    training (beat patterns, expressive gestures,

    preparations, releases, fermatas, dynamics,

    etc.). The results showed that the treatment

    groups scores were consistently higher than

    those of the control groups.12Yontzs (2001)

    repeat of Millers study added some impor-

    tant refinements and produced results that

    clarified and confirmed Millers findings.13

    Neidlinger (2003) combined pedagogical

    and observational approaches to study the

    effects of LMA training on young conduc-

    tors ability to perceive expressiveness in

    movement. Although her findings were

    somewhat inconsistent, she was still able to

    conclude that LMA training for young con-

    ductors did help them improve their obser-vational skills.14

    Currently, a growing but still surpris-

    ingly small cadre of conductors and teachers

    integrate the principles of LMA into their

    performance and teaching activities, but the

    discipline has yet to gain widespread accep-

    tance. Some resistance may stem from con-

    tinuing mistaken perceptions of LMA as a

    specialized field for dancers only. Additional

    skepticism flows from confusion created by

    flawed research and from the belief that the

    validity of Labans theoretical framework

    has yet to be confirmed outside, as well as

    inside, the field of music. In any case, virtu-

    ally all investigations of LMA training for

    conductors have thus far been limited to

    narrow applications of the eight Basic Effort

    Actions within the confines of traditionalconducting patterns.

    Looking for Answers

    In 1994 I realized my conducting had

    reached a plateau that I was unable to sur-

    pass. My teachers were not able to help me

    break through this barrier because they were

    also products of the same pedagogical model

    that had served me for nearly twenty years:

    the organization of conducting gesture

    according to metrically based beat patterns.

    Adherents to the beat pattern school of

    conducting argue that these repetitive move-

    ments are the best means to convey the

    meter or number of rhythmic pulses in a

    measure of music (i.e. two, three, four or

    more to a bar). I also agree that certain con-

    ditions may favor strict adherence to thesemetrical representationsextremely com-

    plex or intricate rhythmic figures, for exam-

    ple. However, maintaining the integrity of

    any beat pattern for the sake of metric clarity

    frequently leads to uninspired, pedestrian

    performances since the shape and progres-

    sion of the music often supersedes or con-

    tradicts the regularity of meter. Simply put,

    it is sometimes impossible to concurrently

    represent the Effortor inner impulse behind

    the sounds of music in the context of pre-

    scriptive movement patterns.

    I was introduced to Laban Movement

    Analysis quite by chance at a two-hour work-

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    9/17

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    24

    shop presented in conjunction with the

    Conductors Guild Annual Conference in

    January, 1995 at Columbia University. I was

    immediately attracted to Labans theoriesand specific language because I recognized

    their potential as the means to help me to

    evolve from beating the music to moving it.

    I wasnt sure how to make use of the knowl-

    edge as I had no teacher at the time, but as

    the saying goes: When the student is ready,

    the teacher will appear. While registering

    that fall for classes at Teachers College, I was

    excited to find an Introduction to Laban

    Movement Analysis course taught by Jackie

    Hand in the schedule. By semesters end, I

    was entirely convinced that LMA would give

    me the answers I had been searching for

    even though I was still unsure how to inte-

    grate this new knowledge into my conduct-

    ing practice. My final project for that class,

    an LMA novices analysis of his personalconducting practice, ignited eight years of

    experimentation and investigation that

    would eventually serve as the basis for my

    doctoral thesis. The product of that contin-

    ued research led to a fresh approach to the

    practice and physicality of conducting ges-

    ture based on the principles of Laban Move-

    ment Analysis.

    An Introduction to Effort for Conductors

    The common thread that unites a con-

    ductors body and mind with his musical and

    artistic intentions should be an awareness of

    the paired oppositional forces or qualities that

    permeate both movement and music. Laban

    uses the concept of bi-polar opposites to elabo-

    rate his Effort theory. He identifies eight

    Effort elements that arise from inner attitudes

    of indulging/accepting or condensing/resist-ing the four Motion Factors: Space, Weight,

    Time and Flow.15 Conductors who have

    honed their capacities to experience and

    observe Effort can make conscious use of

    these oppositional forces to create gestures

    that accurately reflect the audiatedinner atti-

    tudes towards corresponding pairs of

    opposed musical qualities (i.e. loud/soft,

    high/low, etc.). These elements of musical expres-

    sioncoalesce with equivalent qualities of move-

    ment to produce the desired gestural

    representation of the composers notation in

    the score.16A list of these paired musical ele-

    ments is easy to begin but difficult to com-

    plete because the various qualities are so

    numerous, and because they will differ from

    one piece to the next and from one individualto the next. A generic list might include:

    loud/soft, fast/slow, long/short, high/low,

    sound/silence, thick/thin, firm/gentle, rhyth-

    mic/melodic, connected/detached, heavy/

    light, small/large, agitated/calm, consonant/

    dissonant, tension/resolution, complex/sim-

    ple, expressive/plain, tender/brutal, etc. A

    short character piece may exhibit only a few

    paired qualities while a large extended work

    might include a much broader range of expres-

    sion. Borrowing from Laban and Lamb, I have

    assembled and organized an introductory set

    of affinities between the Efforts and elements

    of musical expression (see Table 1).

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    10/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies Spring 2009 Volume 1, Number 1

    25

    The table is self-explanatory, but a brief

    description of its organization scheme will

    enable readers to establish and understand

    connections more quickly. The Efforts are

    listed in pairs in the first column, and six

    among the many elements of musical expression

    extend across the top of the table.

    Tendencies for each Effort are listed

    across its corresponding row underneath

    each musical term so readers can choose to

    consider the affinities in relation to any given

    Effort or musical characteristic. For example,

    Light Weight tends to increase tempo and

    decrease dynamic intensity, but Strong

    Weight tends to decrease tempo and increase

    dynamic intensity. For the purposes of this

    demonstration, the table is limited to six ele-

    ments of music that can be expressed as con-

    trasting pairs. Cues, holds, releases and

    processes that occur over an extended period

    of time such as crescendos, diminuendos,

    accelerandos, ritardandos, etc. were purpose-

    fully left off the list. These types of events

    cannot be considered or properly represented

    in the context of a single Effort because, by

    definition, they normally appear together

    with one or more additional elements of

    musical expression. Since nearly all musical

    events require at least two Efforts, these more

    complex events are beyond the scope of thisintroductory presentation. The resulting list,

    although far from complete, helps conduc-

    tors establish rudimentary connections

    between their powers of audiation and move-

    ment thinking.17 With practice and contin-

    ued study these connections can ultimately

    lead to a genuine experience of the equiva-

    lence between movement and music.

    Examining the Efforts and their rela-

    tionships with the six categories across the

    top of the table, a curious yet logical pattern

    emerges. Each Motion Factor is active in

    four of the six categories and neutral for

    the remaining two. This neutrality applies

    Table 1: The Effort/Conducting Affinities

    Efforts Elements of Musical Expression

    Tempo Dynamics Articulations Character Precision Phrasing

    Light Weight Increase Decrease Less Intensity Light Neutral Neutral

    Strong Weight Decrease Increase More Intensity Forceful Neutral Neutral

    Flexible Space Neutral Neutral Longer Broad Less Pliant

    Direct Space Neutral Neutral Shorter Focused More Strict

    Sustained Time Decrease Neutral Less Accented Calm Neutral Stretched

    Quick Time Increase Neutral More Accented Hurried Neutral Condensed

    Free Flow Neutral Increase Neutral Carefree Less Fluent

    Bound Flow Neutral Decrease Neutral Restrained More Controlled

    http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    11/17

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    26

    only when the Motion Factors are consid-

    ered singly. Its explanation helps illustrate

    how the Efforts combine to emphasize spe-

    cific qualities of musical expression.18

    Thetendency for any single Motion Factor to

    affect any of the six categories of musical

    expression is neutral if: 1) it exerts little or

    no obvious influence upon the considered

    musical quality, or 2) it is equally capable of

    communicating the effects at either end of

    the spectrum. For example, because Flow by

    itself suggests little or no sense of pulse, it is

    difficult to imagine the representation of

    tempo with Flow alone. Simply put, Free or

    Bound Flow may be applied to slow or fast

    tempos. Once Flow combines with Time or

    Weight (or both) the pulse emerges and

    tempo can be established. Space Effort is

    also neutral when considering tempo

    because ones attitude towards space alone

    does not affect tempo. Conversely, even

    though it may be difficult to conceive a

    tempo with Time Effort alone, it is equally

    obvious that accelerating or quick move-

    ments favor faster tempos while sustained,

    drawn out movements favor slower tempos.

    The affinities are justified because the ten-

    dency for each Time Effort is clear.

    Effort/Space and Effort/Shape Affinities

    As Laban developed his Effort theory,

    he also noticed that the body and its limbs

    are able to execute certain dynamic nuances

    in movements towards certain areas in space

    better than towards others.19 He observed

    that lightness favors upward movement

    while strong movements correlate to down-

    ward directions. Movements across the body

    exhibit an affinity with the quality of direct-ness or narrow focus, and roundabout, indi-

    rect movements favor flexible directions that

    open outward. Quick or sudden movements

    relate to backwards directions while sus-

    tained movements tend to favor reaching

    forward. Further experimentation with

    these six Effort/Space affinities in groups of

    three and the continued refinement of hisEffort theory eventually helped Laban

    develop the eight Basic Effort Actions and

    the Effort Cube.20 Lamb validated Labans

    discovery of the connections between the

    Effort elements and specific spatial tenden-

    cies and adopted them as the basis for his

    more detailed system of Effort/Shape affini-

    ties. After pointing out the correspondence

    between Space and the horizontal dimen-

    sion, Weight and the vertical and Time and

    the sagittal dimension, he used these three

    Effort/Space affinities as the organizing

    principle for his Effort/Shape affinities

    [Table 2].21

    These relationships provide conductors

    with the bond that joins the qualities of

    their gestures (their Effort choices) to the

    shapes their limbs and bodies create as they

    carve pathways through space Affinities fre-

    quently reinforce musical events like the

    downward stroke that may accompany a

    strong, direct, quick accent. Disaffinities

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    12/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies Spring 2009 Volume 1, Number 1

    27

    movements that contradict natural tenden-

    cies) often support some kind of musical

    tension, and they can appear simultaneously

    with additional affinities or disaffinities.

    An extended diminuendo, for example,

    might require a light, direct, sustained ges-

    ture with the left arm that sinks (a disaffin-

    ity), encloses (an affinity) and retreats (a

    second disaffinity).

    Confirmation through Research

    LMA had clearly produced positive

    results for me, but I needed to know if my

    approach would help others as well. To find

    out I conducted a study of four student con-

    ductors at the University of North Carolina

    at Greensboro School of Music. Following a

    pretest conducting performance of the open-

    ing 154 measures of Beethovens (1770-1827)

    Overture to Coriolanus, Op. 62 (1808), the

    four participating conductors completed five

    hours of introductory LMA training basedon my ten years of study and experimenta-

    tion. This mini-course included a brief

    exploration of BESS but focused on the

    material presented in this articleEffort, the

    Effort/Space and Effort/Shape Affinities

    and their specific application to musical

    expression. I provided movement instruc-

    tion, some of which was related to conduct-

    ing, but I did not teach the course as a

    conducting class. No specific applications of

    the course material to the study repertoire

    were prescribed, and the student conductors

    received no counsel from instructors outside

    the limits of the study. Participants were

    asked to use homework assignments and in-

    class participatory exercises as the means to

    incorporate newly acquired skills into their

    conducting. Upon completion of the course,

    the student conductors performed the same

    repertoire with the same community orches-

    tra in a posttest. Both the pretest and post-

    test performances were documented with a

    Samsung model SD23 MiniDV camcorder

    and single-point stereo microphone placed

    in front of the orchestra. The resulting video

    recordings were edited without any change

    in content and transferred to DVD. Expert

    panels of two conductors and two Certified

    Movement Analysts analyzed the partici-

    pants pretest and posttest performances,

    and a third channel of data was collected

    through post-study interviews with each par-

    ticipant.

    Table 2: The Effort/Shape Affinities

    Motion Factor

    Accepting

    Effort

    Element

    Shape Affinity

    Resisting

    Effort

    Element

    ShapeAffinity

    Space Flexible Spreading Direct Enclosing

    Weight Light Rising Strong Sinking

    Time Sustained Advancing Quick Retreatin

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    13/17

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    28

    The LMA panel was able to confirm

    significant changes in movement choices

    and an expanded range of movement possi-

    bilities for all four participants that could beattributed to LMA training. Focusing on the

    participants overall presentation, one of the

    CMA panelists noticed: In general the par-

    ticipants were more grounded (i.e. con-

    nected to their own bodies and aware of

    their potential power to communicate

    through movement) in the post-tests than

    they were in the pre-tests. Further elaborat-

    ing on these changes, she continued:

    Each participant in the beginning of

    his/her posttest, standing in prepara-

    tion to begin the music, had an aware-

    ness of his/her ability to communicate

    through the choices (s)he made in his/

    her inner attitude toward Space,

    Weight, Time and Flow Effort. This

    awareness of how those choices would

    affect the music produced by the

    orchestra was not as apparent . . . inthe pretest.22

    The other CMA observed: All conduc-

    tors in the study seemed to be able to use

    shape change affinities to help crystallize

    Efforts better in the posttest than the pre-

    test. She explained:

    In the pretest many were working in a

    different plane of motin, which oftenseemed to counteract the chosen

    Effort: e.g. sinking with quickness or

    advancing with strength in the pretest

    versus retreating or advancing with

    quickness, and sinking with strength

    in the posttest. 23

    The conductor panel was also able to

    concur that the changes they observed con-

    stituted a positive development for all partic-

    ipants. One found that overall, all four

    participants general performance improvedsignificantly in the posttest compared to the

    pretest. All four used a wider range of

    motion and involved different gestural solu-

    tions to inherent problems in the score. He

    also reported that the participants were

    more relaxed in the posttest. As a result,

    the ensemble was more relaxed and

    responded with overall better execution of

    the piece.24 The second expert conductor

    noted: In general, the four conductors

    exhibited greater control of body language

    in their posttest performances as compared

    to the pretests. They seemed more poised

    and relaxed physically and more self-confi-

    dent both physically and mentally. Zeroing

    in on changes in the participants gestures,

    he added:

    Their gestures were more focused and

    more purposeful with fewer extraneous

    motions in their arms or bodies. Con-

    trasts of dynamics were more vividly

    delineated, as were certain, but not all,

    changes of character. Shaping of

    phrases also benefited from the greater

    physical focus, and, in some cases, eye

    contact with the players improved.25

    Lastly, the participants themselves

    expressed unanimous approval regarding the

    quality of the course content and instruc-

    tion, and all agreed that they would eagerly

    recommend LMA training to their col-

    leagues and peers. Participant 1 reported: I

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    14/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies Spring 2009 Volume 1, Number 1

    29

    find that I am much more confident with

    my movement choices, and he recognized

    that musicians react a lot more to a conduc-

    tors gestures than most of us realize.26

    Thecourse has helped participant 2 develop a

    better understanding of the connection

    between what I do and the sound I get and

    exactly how to describe that connection.

    She plans to continue to use the skills she

    acquired during the course because LMA

    has, for her, become a very useful frame-

    work to analyze [her] movements, analyze

    what other people are doing and what works

    versus what doesnt.27 Before taking the

    course, participant 3 didnt think of con-

    ducting in terms of gesture. Through the

    LMA training she has developed much

    more appreciation for the art and the diffi-

    culty of conducting as well as the ability to

    convey a lot of things that, previously, I just

    couldnt figure out. When asked how thecourse had improved the connection

    between her gestures and elements of musi-

    cal expression, she replied:

    At the most basic level, I now have

    choices to make. Before the class I felt

    like I was lost in the music with no

    tools or system to help me connect my

    gestures to the music. After the course

    I am able to look at a score and knowthat I can quickly find a solution to

    almost any problem or challenge I

    encounter. 28

    After the study, participant 4 realized:

    many of the things I already believed have

    been reinforced, and he added that the LMA

    training had provided him with some very

    useful terminology to describe movements [he

    is] already using. He reported that since the

    course, he is definitely watching conductorsmore closely to try to see if their Effort choices

    match their musical intentions. When asked

    whether or not he would encourage friends

    and colleagues to add LMA to their studies he

    answered: Im convinced that LMA training

    should almost be a requirement for conduc-

    tors.29

    The expert panels evaluations, the par-

    ticipants observations and my personal

    experience combine to provide convincing

    evidence that conductors who choose to

    master the principles of Laban Movement

    Analysis have at their disposal a comprehen-

    sive set of tools for conceiving and executing

    potent, persuasive gestures that display genu-

    ine equivalence with the sounds of music.

    The Motion Factors, Effort elements and

    Effort/Shape affinities are the raw ingredi-

    ents conductors combine to create recipes

    for gestures that perfectly reflect both the

    conductors own personal movement style

    and the musical and technical demands

    present in the score. Such a conductors

    movements illumine the music and inspire

    the performers under his direction because

    he has firmly, irrevocably fused his musical

    intentions with his force of will and body by

    grounding himself at the point of conver-

    gence between his powers of audiation and

    movement thinking. Lamb describes Effort

    and Shape as the two processes from which

    http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-http://-/?-
  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    15/17

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    30

    movement is created.30From the evidence

    presented in this essay, it also seems abun-

    dantly clear that Effort and Shape together

    with audiation are the three processes fromwhich music and, most specifically, effective,

    compelling conducting gestures are created.

    Bibliography

    Bartee, Neale King. 1977. The development of a the-

    oretical position on conducting using princi-

    ples of body movement as explicated by Rudolf

    Laban. Ph.D. diss., University of Illinois at

    Urbana-Champaign.

    Benge, Timothy John. 1996. Movements utilized by

    conductors in the stimulation of expression

    and musicianship. D.M.A. diss., University of

    Southern California.

    Billingham, Lisa Adalade. 2001. The development of

    a gestural vocabulary for choral conductors

    based on the movement theory of Rudolf

    Laban. D.M.A. diss., University of Arizona.

    Gambetta, Charles. 2005. Creating a fresh approach

    to conducting gesture through the principles

    of Laban Movement Analysis. D.M.A. diss.,

    University of North Carolina at Greensboro.

    Gordon, Edwin E. 1980. Learning sequences in music:

    Skill, content and patterns. Chicago: GIA Publi-

    cations, Inc.

    Hibbard, Therees Tkach. 1994. The use of move-

    ment as an instructional technique in choral

    rehearsals. D.M.A. diss., University of Oregon.

    Holt, Michele Menard. 1992. The application to

    conducting and choral rehearsal pedagogy of

    Laban Effort/Shape and its comparative effect

    upon style in choral performance. D.M.A

    diss., University of Hartford.

    Laban, Rudolf. 1988.The mastery of movement, 4thed.

    Revised by Lisa Ullmann. Plymouth: North-

    cote House. Original edition, London: Mac-donald and Evans, 1980.

    _____. 1974. The language of movement: A guidebook to

    choreutics. Edited by Lisa Ullman. Boston:

    Plays, Inc.

    _____. 1971. Rudolf Laban speaks about movement and

    dance. Edited by Lisa Ullmann. Addlestone,

    Surrey: Laban Art of Movement Centre.

    Lamb, Warren. 1965. Posture and gesture. London:Gerald Duckworth and Company.

    Lamb, Warren and Elizabeth Watson. 1979. Body

    code: The meaning in movement. London: Rout-

    ledge and Kegan Paul.

    Maletic, Vera. 1987. Body - space - expression: The devel-

    opment of Rudolf Laban's movement and dance

    concepts. Approaches to Semiotics 75. Berlin

    and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Maletic, Vera. 2004. Dance dynamics effort and phras-

    ing workbook. Columbus: Grade A Notes.

    Miller Stephen W. 1988. The effect of Laban move-ment training on the ability of student conduc-

    tors to communicate musical interpretation

    through gesture. Ph.D. diss., University of

    Wisconsin-Madison.

    Neidlinger, Erica Jean. 2003. The effect of Laban

    effort-shape instruction on young conductors'

    perception of expressiveness across arts disci-

    plines. Ph.D. diss., University of Minnesota.

    Preston-Dunlop, Valerie and Charlotte Purkis. 1989.

    Rudolf Laban The making of modern dance:

    The seminal years in Munich 1910-1914. Dance

    Theatre Journal7, no. 3 (winter): 10-13.Scherchen, Hermann. 1966. Handbook of conducting,

    10th ed. Translated by M. D. Calvocoressi.

    London:

    Oxford University Press.

    Yontz, Timothy Gene. 2001. The effectiveness of

    Laban-based principles of movement and pre-

    vious musical training on undergraduate

    beginning conducting students ability to con-

    vey intended musical content. Ph.D. diss.,

    University of Nebraska.

    Endnotes

    1. Hermann Scherchen, Handbook of Conducting, 10th

    ed., trans. M. D. Calvocoressi (London: Oxford

    University Press, 1966), 14.

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    16/17

    The Journal of Laban Movement Studies Fall 2008 Volume 1, Number 1

    31

    2. The term gesture as used by Hermann Scherchen and the present author refers to a broad range of conduct-

    ing movement possibilities that, in Laban terminology, may include gestures, postures and posture-gesture

    mergers. Because the primary audience for this research consisted of conductors and musicians, distinc-

    tions between these categories were not made.

    3. Rudolf Laban, Rudolf Laban Speaks about Movement and Dance, ed. Lisa Ullmann (Addlestone: Laban Art of

    Movement Centre, 1971), 40.

    4. Charles Gambetta, Creating a Fresh Approach to Conducting Gesture Through the Principles of Laban

    Movement Analysis (D.M.A. diss., University of North Carolina at Greensboro, 2005), 100-23.

    5. Bartee, The Development of a Theoretical Position on Conducting Using Principles of Body Movement as

    Explicated By Rudolf Laban (Ph.D. diss., University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, 1977), 2, 17-8.

    6. Ibid., 160.

    7. Ibid., 161.

    8. Ibid., 192-200.

    9. Therees Tkach Hibbard, The Use of Movement as an Instructional Technique in Choral Rehearsals

    (D.M.A. diss., University of Oregon, 1994), 226-37. Timothy John Benge, Movements Utilized by Con-

    ductors in the Stimulation of Expression and Musicianship (D.M.A. diss., University of Southern Califor-

    nia, 1996), 59-61.

    10. Lisa Adalade Billingham, The Development of a Gestural Vocabulary for Choral Conductors Based on

    the Movement Theory of Rudolf Laban (D.M.A. diss., University of Arizona, 2001), 65-72.

    11. Michele Menard Holt, The Application to Conducting and Choral Rehearsal Pedagogy of Laban Effort/

    Shape and its Comparative Effect upon Style in Choral Performance (D.M.A diss., University of Hart-

    ford, 1992), 9, 64.

    12. Stephen W Miller, The Effect of Laban Movement Training on the Ability of Student Conductors to

    Communicate Musical Interpretation Through Gesture (Ph.D. diss., University of Wisconsin-Madison,

    1988), 105-6.

    13. Timothy Gene Yontz, The Effectiveness of Laban-based Principles of Movement and Previous MusicalTraining on Undergraduate Beginning Conducting Students Ability to Convey Intended Musical Con-

    tent (Ph.D. diss., University of Nebraska, 2001), 102-3.

    14. Erica Jean Neidlinger, The Effect of Laban Effort-Shape Instruction on Young Conductors' Perception of

    Expressiveness across Arts Disciplines (Ph.D. diss., University of Minnesota, 2003), 126-32.

    15. Though far beyond the limits of the present article, a more thorough examination of Labans theories

    would lead to an understanding of his fundamental views of movement as a dynamic process on a contin-

    uum between polarities. Maletic, Body - Space - Expression, 52. Apart from Effort, for example, Laban orga-

    nized his concept of bodily rhythms . . . in terms of polarities. Preston-Dunlop and Perkins explain: He

    had developed two sources, Ionian opposites (hot/cold, right/left, big/small, etc.) and the colour contrast

    theory described in Kandinskys work. Valerie Preston-Dunlop and Charlotte Perkins, Rudolf Laban

    The Making of Modern Dance: The Seminal Years in Munich 1910-1914 Dance Theatre Journal7, no. 3

    (winter 1989): 10-13.16. Music educator Edwin Gordon defines audiation as a cognitive process by which the brain gives mean-

    ing to music. Edwin E. Gordon, Learning Sequences in Music: Skill, Content and Patterns(Chicago: GIA Pub-

    lications, Inc., 1980), 4-5. It is the convergence of this inward hearing that Gordon refers to as audiation

    and Labans concept of thinking in terms of movement allows me to find genuine equivalence between my

    gestures and the sounds of music.

    17. The author has been experimenting and refining this approach for nearly ten years and has expanded the

    list of affinities to include combinations of two and three Efforts as well as Effort/Shape and Effort/Space

    affinities.

  • 7/24/2019 Laban Movement Analysis for Conductors R

    17/17

    Charles Gambetta, DMA

    32

    18. While all four Motion Factors are not visible in States and Drives, the absent Efforts in such cases result

    from conscious or unconscious choices made by the mover. In contrast, the neutrality associated with

    Effort/Conducting affinities is not the result of choice. Just as an Effort/Shape affinity exists between

    lightness and rising but not lightness and retreating, an Effort/Conducting affinity exists between Weight

    and dynamics but not Weight and precision.

    19. Rudolf Laban, The Language of Movement: A Guidebook to Choreutics, ed. Lisa Ullman (Boston: Plays, Inc.,1974), 30-2.

    20. Vera Maletic, Dance Dynamics Effort and Phrasing Workbook, 37.

    21. Warren Lamb, Posture and Gesture, (London: Gerald Duckworth and Company, 1965), 63-4.

    22. Gambetta, 269-70.

    23. Ibid., 280.

    24. Ibid., 306.

    25. Gambetta, 332.

    26. Ibid., 336-7.

    27. Ibid., 341-2.

    28. Gambetta., 346.

    29. Ibid., 351-3.

    30.Warren Lamb and Elizabeth Watson, Body Code: The Meaning in Movement(London: Routledge and Kegan

    Paul, 1979), 81.

    Conductor/composer Charles Gambettaholds a Doctor of Musical Arts degree in conducting from the University of

    North Carolina at Greensboro. He is currently on the music faculties of Greensboro College, Guilford College and Winston-

    Salem State University in North Carolina. A student of LMA since 1995 when he studied for a semester with Jackie Hand

    at Teachers College, Columbia University, Charles addressed the 2006 Laban and the Performing Arts conference in Brat-

    islava, and he will speak at the 2008 Laban International Conference in London. Maestro Gambetta has served as Music

    Director of the Young American Symphony Orchestra, the Greensboro Symphony Youth Orchestra and Assistant Conductor

    of the Greensboro Symphony, the Fayetteville Symphony and the Philharmonia of Greensboro. In 2008 he accepted an

    appointment to the faculty of the prestigious International Institute for Conductors.