25
http://ecl.sagepub.com/ Journal of Early Childhood Literacy http://ecl.sagepub.com/content/10/2/159 The online version of this article can be found at: DOI: 10.1177/1468798410363754 2010 10: 159 Journal of Early Childhood Literacy Emma Hepburn, Bridget Egan and Naomi Flynn Vocabulary acquisition in young children: The role of the story Published by: http://www.sagepublications.com can be found at: Journal of Early Childhood Literacy Additional services and information for http://ecl.sagepub.com/cgi/alerts Email Alerts: http://ecl.sagepub.com/subscriptions Subscriptions: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.nav Reprints: http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Permissions: http://ecl.sagepub.com/content/10/2/159.refs.html Citations: What is This? - May 20, 2010 Version of Record >> at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012 ecl.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Journal of Early Childhood Literacy ... · Additional services and information for Journal of Early Childhood Literacy can ... Downloaded from ecl.sagepub.com at ... especially in

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

http://ecl.sagepub.com/Journal of Early Childhood Literacy

http://ecl.sagepub.com/content/10/2/159The online version of this article can be found at:

 DOI: 10.1177/1468798410363754

2010 10: 159Journal of Early Childhood LiteracyEmma Hepburn, Bridget Egan and Naomi Flynn

Vocabulary acquisition in young children: The role of the story  

Published by:

http://www.sagepublications.com

can be found at:Journal of Early Childhood LiteracyAdditional services and information for    

  http://ecl.sagepub.com/cgi/alertsEmail Alerts:

 

http://ecl.sagepub.com/subscriptionsSubscriptions:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsReprints.navReprints:  

http://www.sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.navPermissions:  

http://ecl.sagepub.com/content/10/2/159.refs.htmlCitations:  

What is This? 

- May 20, 2010Version of Record >>

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Vocabulary acquisition inyoung children: The role of the story

EMMA HEPBURN University of Winchester, UK

BRIDGET EGAN University of Winchester, UK

NAOMI FLYNN University of Winchester, UK

Abstract Sharing storybooks with babies increases their futureachievements in literacy, especially in reading (Hall, 2001; Moore andWade, 1997, 2003; Scarborough et al., 1991; Wade and Moore, 1998;Wells, 1985). This study, focusing on case studies of two 20-month-old children, attempts to identify the role the storybookplays in children’s vocabulary acquisition. Their mothers adopted aregime of daily reading of specific picture books over a six-weekperiod, and recorded the children’s acquisition of new vocabulary, inorder to explore what specific contribution these texts made to thechildren’s speech. The findings demonstrate that storybooks form onesource of children’s newly-acquired vocabulary. Factors that mightaccount for this were more difficult to determine through a study ofthis scale.

Keywords babies; vocabulary acquisition; storybooks

The focus of this article is the role played by storybooks in the early stagesof young children’s vocabulary acquisition. The use of storybooks withbabies is well established as contributing to their future achievements inliteracy, especially in reading (Hall, 2001; Moore and Wade, 1997, 2003;Scarborough et al., 1991; Wade and Moore, 1998; Wells, 1985). However,there seems to be less research into the potential effects of sharing story-books on young children’s language acquisition. This study attempts toidentify the role the storybook plays in children’s acquisition of vocabulary.It focuses on two male children aged between 20 and 22 months in theearly stages of language production, and their development of vocabularyover a 6-week period when sharing storybooks with their mothers. The

a rt i c l e

159

Journal of EarlyChildhood Literacy

© The Author(s), 2010.Reprints and permissions:

http://www.sagepub.co.uk/journalsPermissions.nav

vol 10(2) 159–182DOI: 10.1177/1468798410363754

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 159

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

potential for early vocabulary development through the use of story isdiscussed.

The subject of first language acquisition has been explored for manyyears, with varying outcomes. Many researchers believe that language is aninstinctive response and that as long as children are exposed to speechthey will learn to talk regardless of the quality of input they are given(Aitchison, 1997; Chomsky, 2000; Lenneberg, 1967; Pinker, 1994).Bruner’s (1983: 119) position on language acquisition, building on Vygotskyan theory, differs from this in that he believes that speech developsthrough shared communication. Further studies support this need forcontext and social interaction in developing oracy in young children(Landau and Gleitman, 1985: 208; Otomo, 2001: 50).

There are several models of first language development in youngchildren. Some believe that all children go through various stages oflanguage acquisition at set times of their childhood (Lenneberg, 1967:105). Others offer a more flexible interpretation of stages of languageacquisition (Dionne et al., 2003: 395).

Lenneberg’s (1967) theoretical outlook on first language acquisitionshared some common ground with Chomsky (2000: 7), who believes thata predisposition for language acquisition is innate; that it ‘happens’ to achild rather than being a conscious act. Aitchison’s (1997: 47) view is thatthere is a predetermined sequence of development. Sheridan (1997: 53)argues that ‘there are wide variations among normal children in the rate oflanguage acquisition’. Chomsky (2000), Pinker (1994), and Cook andNewson (1996) appear to support Sheridan’s view that all children learnlanguage at varying rates. More generally, Jarvis and Lamb (2001: 135) state‘individual differences between children, their personalities, characteristicsand abilities all play a part in development . . .’. Wells (1985: 12) alsofavours the latter view, and found that individual difference in temperamentcould account for the extent to which young children were active inenhancing their own speech development. Most authors, however, agreethat there is a period of very rapid development, beginning in childrenfrom approximately eighteen to twenty-four-months-old, during which an‘explosion’ of new vocabulary occurs, and preceding the emergence ofrecognisable grammatical structures.

Fletcher et al. (2005) emphasise some of the benefits that using bookswith young children may have on children’s language acquisition. This is aview shared by others (Moore and Wade, 2003; Wells, 1985; Whitehead,1999, 2002), and is supported by research conducted by Locke (1996:251), who found ‘physical proximity and vocal interaction between mother[sic] and infant have been shown to aid language learning’. Odean (1998)

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

160

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 160

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

and Clark (2004) suggest that reading aloud may nurture language development through an early and playful introduction to vocabulary.Furthermore, Fletcher et al. (2005: 65) found, during their research intoyoung children’s responsiveness and attention during picture book reading,that children’s responses might nurture future positive responses to picturebooks.

Tomasello and Farrar (1986: 1454) considered the effects of joint atten-tional episodes, which they describe as mother [sic] and child talking abouta common focus, on children’s language acquisition. They found that ‘theamount of time spent in joint attentional episodes was positively related tothe child’s vocabulary size’ (Tomasello and Farrar, 1986: 1454). Storybookreading could be considered as an example of a joint attentional episode asboth adult and child are concentrating their focus on the book (Fletcher etal., 2005: 66). Fletcher et al. (2005: 75) also found a correlation betweenchildren being read to and vocabulary size. This is perhaps because theinteraction between adult and child while book reading is likely to beconversational and involve features of dialogue such as turn taking,question and response (Ninio and Bruner, 1978: 6).

Much research identifies positive effects that reading to babies andtoddlers has on their future literacy success (Hall, 2001; Moore and Wade,1997, 2003; Scarborough et al., 1991; Wade and Moore, 1998; Wells,1985). Meek (1991: 13) believes that children have an innate desire to joinin with speech, in order to communicate, as soon as possible. This reflectsthe views of Bruner (1983), suggesting that children who are immersed inconversation may begin to join in those conversations sooner.

A range of studies have discovered a correlation between the amount oflanguage input from parents and the size of children’s early vocabulary (Liuet al., 2003: F1; Messer, 1995: 222; Wells, 1985: 52). It could be arguedthat sharing a book with a toddler provides parents with a quantity oflanguage that may not otherwise have been available to them, or may nothave been shared with their child. This is consistent with Taylor and Strickland’s (1986: 47) view that listening to storybooks presents childrenwith language not usually associated with everyday speech. Justice and Ezellfound further opportunities for exposure to speech in parental praise andresponse to children’s utterances during shared reading (2000: 266). Wells(1985: 7) shares their view that a positive response from parents duringstorybook reading is likely to increase the speed of early language development.

Jack (2005) proclaims ‘books provide the perfect opportunity for[infants] to learn about speech patterns and how to make sounds’(Internet). If storybooks are shared with infants in the ways suggested by

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

161

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 161

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Justice and Ezell (2000), it is possible that, during the shared reading,infants can be exposed to both the types of vocal input described by Mennand Stoel-Gammon (1996: 339) and by Jack (2005). This would allowthem to practise their own vocalisations, which Menn and Stoel-Gammon(1996: 339) believe is an important prerequisite for word production.Taylor and Strickland (1986: 50) also advocate the use of storybooks toassist children in learning to make sounds.

It is worth remembering that storybooks for infants and young childrenhave not always been in existence. William Godwin, in the late 18th andearly 19th century, was one of the earliest educators to advocate the use ofstorybooks designed specifically to entertain children (Bottoms, 2004). Theproduction of illustrated books aimed at children flowered and developedthroughout the 19th century, but it was the development of inexpensivecolour printing techniques in the 20th century that made possible theproduction of picture storybooks for a mass market. Prior to this, storieswere told to children orally and the relationship that exists between wordsand pictures, and the printed and spoken word would have been hidden.

Menn and Stoel-Gammon (1996) also describe how the development ofphonological awareness links directly to language acquisition. Many story-books for young children contain rhyme and alliteration, and thereforecould enhance phonological awareness, in turn assisting language acquisi-tion. Foy and Mann (2003: 60) found that children with a less developedsense of rhyme were likely to have a less mature pattern of articulation.

It is thus widely agreed that sharing books with infants is beneficial totheir language development. However, there are those who believe that thetype of language experiences children encounter are of little importance totheir language development (Aitchison, 1997; Chomsky, 2000: Lenneberg,1967; Pinker, 1994). If this is true, then while sharing books with infantsmay be enjoyable it is unlikely to aid their acquisition of language. However,there are those who advocate the use of books to encourage language playas part of first language acquisition (Clark, 2004; Jack, 2005; Odean, 1998;Taylor and Strickland, 1986).

The aim of this study was to explore the relationship between the sharingof storybooks with one-year-olds and their early language development;specifically it looked at their rates of vocabulary acquisition. Questionswere: Can the sharing of specific books be traced in the child’s acquisitionof new vocabulary? And, are there particular aspects of such sharing or ofthe books, which promote vocabulary acquisition?

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

162

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 162

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

MethodTwo children were selected for case study. Although the non-generalisabilityof case study may be seen to be problematic (Gomm et al., 2000: 98), itcan establish a basis for ‘naturalistic generalization’ (Lincoln and Guba,2000: 36; Stake, 2000: 23), which Gomm et al. (2000) suggest is aroutine part of professional reflection. Donmoyer (2000: 52) also pointsout ‘in fields such as education . . . in which there is concern with indi-viduals . . . all research findings are tentative’, therefore suggesting thatthe case study method is as effective as any other. Case study was also asuitable approach for ensuring the children could remain in a natural,familiar setting, following their normal routine, as advocated by Langstonet al. (2004: 153).

The research concentrated on two male children in their second year. Thechildren shared similar backgrounds. Both were single children living intwo parent families. The mother of Joseph was a trainee teacher, and bothparents of Aaron were teachers (‘Joseph’ and ‘Aaron’ are pseudonyms). Tobe able to discover what role storybooks played in the children’s earlyvocabulary acquisition, it was important to observe children for longperiods, in order to identify their acquisition and use of new vocabulary,and their ‘reading’ behaviours throughout the day during the period of datacollection. It was important that these children be in a familiar setting sothat their language use was as natural as possible. The mothers shared one‘set book’ per week with the children on a daily basis, and recorded theirresponses to that book. The children’s use of new words was recorded daily,and the mothers also noted titles and frequency of any other books thechildren had asked to have read or ‘read’ for themselves.

Both children were used to sharing storybooks with adults, so theaddition of another story would not have seemed unusual. Studying thesetwo children also meant that the majority of the research could take placein the home. The mothers of both children kept a running record of thechildren’s vocabulary prior to the study, and at the outset of the datacollection period these two children each had a vocabulary of about 400words.

Data collection was undertaken by the mothers of the two children (oneof whom is a co-author of this article). In both cases, the active consent ofthe fathers was also sought, to ensure that a less directly involved parentwas included in the consent process. Both mothers were observing thechildren while reading to them and keeping closely to a normal dailyroutine. For this research there were benefits of being a parent researcherdespite the obvious risk of subjectivity. The children being researched were

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

163

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 163

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

familiar and comfortable with the data collector/parent, who was thusconstantly ‘on site’, and observations could be recorded during the child’ssleep time in the day, or in the evening once the child was in bed.

The research was conducted over a six-week period. Prior to this, a one-week pilot study was conducted to ensure a match in the way that eachmother was collecting and recording data.

Both children were exposed to the same ‘set books’ each week to seewhether specific texts elicited similar words from both children. These werebooks that were previously unknown to them, so that much of the vocab-ulary was new to them. As both children already had a large vocabulary fortheir ages, and were accustomed to book sharing, the books chosen werethose recommended for a slightly older age range. This resulted in one ofthe texts being longer than the books usually shared with the children, butwith quite fast-paced text and repeated rhyming. This was kept for the finalweek when it was thought that increased attention spans might allow fora longer story.

The pilot week used a simple rhyming book with actions. The book wasread three times a day, plus any other times the child requested it, and theirresponses recorded in notebooks; notes included whether they enjoyed thebook, any words they joined in, or used themselves, other books readtogether and books they had ‘read’ independently. At the start of the weekboth children thoroughly enjoyed the book, however by the end of the pilotweek both seemed reluctant to settle to read it. It seemed that reading thebook to them three times a day was excessive, and for the following six-week study once a day would be sufficient.

The six-week study began shortly after, ensuring that all new vocabularyin between the pilot week and the start of the study was recorded. Readingevents between adult and child included discussion with the child, of bothtext and pictures, and opportunities for the child to join in, completesentences, volunteer ideas, and comment on text and pictures. The weekly‘set book’ was shared with the children once a day and their responseslogged as before (Figure 1).

After two weeks, it was felt that much of the information was beingrepeated daily, and recording it in longhand was becoming excessive. Arecord sheet for each book was devised which took the form of a typedcopy of each page of the book with space to circle any words said, and spacefor notes under each section to log discussions the children and carer had(Figure 2).

The form also included a yes/no response for if the children enjoyed thebook with space for comments, space for personal feelings, for example ifthere was anything that may have affected the way in which the book was

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

164

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 164

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

enjoyed, a sheet for any new vocabulary, and another for books read bothindependently and together.

However, there may be some limitations to the approach taken that needto be considered. Both mothers had commitments outside the home, which

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

165

Book: Here Come the BabiesRead mid-afternoonLooked at first page of pictures – pointed to Baby Bunting and said ‘asleep’

‘bed’ ‘zip’ (pointing to cover – relating back to his own sleeping bag) Pointedto moon and cats

p. 1 ‘meow’ with cat and repeated ‘bats’p. 2 ‘bed’ – ‘frt’ (zip) ‘This one has a funny smell’ ‘what do you think he smells of?’ ‘Poo’p. 3 said ‘piyo’ (pillow) but did not point to the pillows. Asked him where

at the elephant was and he replied ‘eclump’ and also pointed out ‘bear’p. 5 wriggled his own fingers and held up toes. ‘meow’ with cat, ‘Daddy’

(daddy’s socks)p. 6 pointed to moon and said ‘moon’, pointed to cat and said ‘meow’,

pointed to sheep and said ‘baa’p. 8 asked him where the washing was and he replied ‘washing’ p. 9 thought yellow towel was the moonp. 11 (five babies) pointed to the sitting down baby and said ‘uhoh’ Told

him the baby was just sitting down and was OK!p. 12 pointed to the moon and said ‘moon’ and cat ‘meow’

Figure 1 A field note sheet from week 2Note: Adult’s words in italics.

Figure 2 Sample of notes sheet for The Very Hungry CatepillarNotes: Shaded words are those which the child said as they were being read. Italicised notes includechild’s other utterances.

Set book: The Very Hungry Caterpillar

Date: 16th June Day: 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

In the light of the moon a little egg lay on a leaf

Pointed to the moon and its face

One Sunday morning the warm sun came up and – pop! – out of the egg came a tinyand very hungry caterpillar

On Monday he ate through one apple. But he was still hungry

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 165

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

meant the children spent a lot of time with grandparents during the week,and Aaron attended a day nursery part time. At these times the people caringfor the child were asked to log the child’s new vocabulary, and that infor-mation was passed on to the parents at the end of the day. It is possible thatduring these times the vocabulary of the children may not have been loggedas thoroughly, therefore new words may have gone unnoticed for longer.

FindingsDuring the collection of data, the mothers noted how many times bookswere shared with the children, meaning an adult read the book with thechild, and how many times the child ‘read’ independently. The use of theword ‘read’ here indicates that the children freely chose books that theyenjoyed to ‘read’ to themselves or their teddy bears. In both cases thisinvolved the child sitting alone, or with a favoured toy, demonstratingreading habits such as page turning, and concentrating on a page. Aaronalso said some of the words from the story to his toys during the ‘reading’.As the main focus was on the sharing of storybooks, it was important toseparate the number of books each child ‘read’ independently from the totalnumber of books read. This independent ‘reading’ has been considered inthe findings as it gave an insight into the child’s enjoyment of a book, sincethey generally chose favoured books to ‘read’ independently.

Figures 3 and 4 show the total number of stories each child interactedwith each week, how many were ‘read’ independently and how manydifferent stories made up the total. They also show the number of wordsacquired on a weekly basis and break down the total number of new wordsinto those related to stories, and those unrelated. For example, in week oneJoseph had 78 readings of 34 different stories, 6 of which he ‘read’ in -dependently. He acquired a total of 52 new words, 14 of which were relatedto stories, 38 were unrelated.

As the numbers of books read, and words acquired, over the six-weekperiod enter the hundreds, we present these data as percentages. Figures 5and 6 show how the data break down into percentage shared and percent-age ‘read’ independently. These figures make it clear that the large majorityof the books the children encountered were shared with a carer.

Figures 7 and 8 show the percentage of new vocabulary related and un -related to storybooks. They also show that over one third of each child’snew vocabulary was related to storybooks.

Data emerging from the findings raised the question of whether thenumber of storybooks shared over each week influenced the rate of vocab-ulary acquired. Figures 9 and 10 show the number of books shared each

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

166

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 166

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

167

7879

86

102

8182

6

19

7

1612

10

34

21

34

51

41

35

52

7676

47

55

48

14

36

25

18

25

17

3840

51

2930

31

0102030405060708090100

110

12

34

56

Wee

k

Tota

l num

ber

of s

tori

es r

ead

Num

ber

of d

iffe

rent

sto

ries

rea

d

Num

ber

of w

ords

rel

ated

to

stor

ies

Num

ber

of w

hich

wer

e ‘re

ad’ i

ndep

ende

ntly

Tota

l num

ber

of w

ords

lear

nt

Num

ber

of w

ords

unr

elat

ed t

o st

orie

s

Tota

ls o

ver

Stor

ies

shar

ed 4

38in

depe

nden

tly ‘r

ead’

70

six

wee

ks:

Voca

bula

ryre

late

d to

sto

rybo

oks

135

unre

late

d 21

9

Figu

re 3

Brea

kdow

n of

boo

ks r

ead

and

wor

ds a

cqui

red:

Jos

eph

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 167

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

168

57

52

5655

48

68

8

1616

13

9

27

14

2121

25

1417

2926

68

44

65

72

1214

12

2023

26

17

12

56

24

42

46

01020304050607080

12

34

56

Wee

k

Tota

l num

ber

of s

tori

es r

ead

Num

ber

of d

iffe

rent

sto

ries

rea

d

Num

ber

of w

ords

rel

ated

to

stor

ies

Num

ber

of w

hich

wer

e ‘re

ad’ i

ndep

ende

ntly

Tota

l num

ber

of w

ords

lear

nt

Num

ber

of w

ords

unr

elat

ed t

o st

orie

s

Tota

ls o

ver

Stor

ies

shar

ed 2

47in

depe

nden

tly ‘r

ead’

89

six

wee

ks:

Voca

bula

ryre

late

d to

sto

rybo

oks

107

unre

late

d 19

7

Figu

re 4

Brea

kdow

n of

boo

ks r

ead

and

wor

ds a

cqui

red:

Aar

on

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 168

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

week over a six-week period against the number of new words the childrenlearnt related to storybooks in the corresponding weeks. These are plottedfrom high to low and show that no trend occurred. The number of booksthe children ‘read’ independently is shown beneath the number of booksshared. This shows a possible relationship between the child’s independent‘reading’ and the number of new words acquired. The bottom row

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

169

Number ‘read’ independently

14%

Number shared86%

Figure 5 Percentage of books shared and ‘read’ independently: Joseph

Number ‘read’ independently

26%

Number shared74%

Figure 6 Percentage of books shared and ‘read’ independently: Aaron

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 169

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

indicates the most frequent sharing of a book chosen by the child duringthat week. We have included these data as they suggest a possible connec-tion between the high frequency of sharing a book chosen by the child andthe number of new words acquired.

The books that the children were interacting with each week includedone ‘set book’. This book was shared with the child at least once a day, moreif the child requested it, and was kept with the child’s other books so thatit was available for them to ‘read’ independently during its ‘set week’. Figure11 shows the frequency with which the ‘set book’ was shared and ‘read’independently. On all but one occasion the number of times the ‘set book’was shared during its ‘set week’ was equal to or greater than 10, with additional independent ‘reading’ occurring often.

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

170

Number of words unrelated to storybooks

62%

Number of words related to storybooks

38%

Figure 7 Percentage of new vocabulary related to storybooks: Joseph

Number of words related to storybooks

35%Number of words

unrelated to storybooks

65%

Figure 8 Percentage of new vocabulary related to storybooks: Aaron

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 170

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

On all but one occasion the frequency with which the ‘set book’ wasshared is greater than the sharing of any other book during the week.Figures 12 and 13 show that the frequency with which most other bookswere shared during a week was typically once or twice, accounting for 78per cent for Joseph and 76 per cent for Aaron. Only 10 per cent of all

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

171

18

25

17

14

25

35

0

10

20

30

40

86 79 72 72 69 60

16 7 10 6 12 19

7 9 9 5 4 14

Books shared (top row)Books ‘read’ independently (middle row)

Most frequent sharing of a book chosen by the child (bottom row)

Num

ber

of n

ew w

ords

rel

ated

to

book

s

12 12

20

26

23

14

0

10

20

30

49 43 42 41 39 36

8 13 13 27 9 16

12 6 3 7 5 4

Books shared (top row)Books 'read' independently (middle row)

Most frequent sharing of a book chosen by the child (bottom row)

Num

ber

of n

ew w

ords

rel

ated

to

book

s

Figure 9 Number of books shared v story related vocabulary acquired: Joseph

Figure 10 Number of books shared v story related vocabulary acquired: Aaron

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 171

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

172

Joseph Aaron

Week Shared Alone Total Shared Alone Total

1 13 2 15 14 1 152 10 0 10 13 0 133 11 0 11 10 2 124 14 1 15 17 3 205 12 1 13 17 3 206 9 0 9 11 1 12

Figure 11 Frequency of use of ‘set book’ during its ‘set week’

223%

312%

4+10%

155%

221%

37%

4+17%

155%

Figure 12 Frequency with which different books were read (excluding ‘set books’):Joseph

Figure 13 Frequency with which different books were read (excluding ‘set books’):Aaron

Frequencybook shared No. of books

Once 120Twice 51Three times 27Four times + 22

Frequencybook shared No. of books

Once 58Twice 23Three times 8Four times + 18

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 172

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Joseph’s books, and 17 per cent of all Aaron’s books were read four timesor more during one week.

As the frequency with which the ‘set books’ were shared greatlyoutweighed that of the majority of other books, it was relevant to ask howmany of the newly acquired story related words were associated with thesebooks alone. Did the frequency with which these books were shared influence the relating number of words acquired? Figures 14 and 15 clearlyshow that three-fifths of Joseph’s, and almost three-quarters of Aaron’snewly acquired story related vocabulary was directly linked to the ‘setbooks’ shared with them.

Figures 16 and 17 show the frequency with which the ‘set books’ wereshared with each child over the six-week period, from high to low, and thenumber of words acquired from each book. The number of independent‘readings’ is shown beneath the number of times the book was shared. This

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

173

Words relating to ‘set books’

60%

Words relating to other books

40%

Words relating to ‘set books’ 81Words relating to other books 53Total 134

Words relating to ‘set books’

71%

Words relating to other books

29%

Words relating to ‘set books’ 76Words relating to other books 31Total 107

Figure 15 Words learnt over six-week period relating to storybooks: Aaron

Figure 14 Words learnt over six-week period relating to storybooks: Joseph

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 173

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

highlights that the order of frequency with which the books were read wasunchanged by independent ‘reading’.

As the majority of story related words originated from the ‘set books’, itseemed prudent to compare the words each child had learnt from these sixbooks. There were at least two of the same words from each book that bothchildren acquired during the research period. The highest number camefrom Each Peach Pear Plum (Ahlberg and Ahlberg, 1989), which elicited sevenof the same words, three of which were rhyming character names. The

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

174

15

9

29

14

7 7

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

16 16 13 12 11 9

2 Each Peach PearPlum

1 Very HungryCaterpillar

0 Here Come theBabies

1 Pirate Pete 0 Rosie’s Walk 0 Bumpus JumpusDinosaurumpus

Frequency ‘set book’ shared (top row)Frequency ‘set book’ ‘read’ independently (bottom row)

Num

ber

of n

ew w

ords

acq

uire

d

Figure 16 Frequency 'set book’ shared and related vocabulary acquired: Joseph

12

21

1412

16

8

0

5

10

15

20

25

28 22 18 17 11 10

1 Each Peach PearPlum

3 Very HungryCaterpillar

3 Pirate Pete 0 Here Come theBabies

1 Bumpus JumpusDinosaurumpus

2 Rosie’s Walk

Frequency ‘set book’ shared (top row)Frequency ‘set book’ ‘read’ independently (bottom row)

Num

ber

of n

ew w

ords

acq

uire

d

Figure 17 Frequency 'set book’ shared and related vocabulary acquired: Aaron

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 174

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

majority of both children’s other words acquired from Each Peach Pear Plumwere also rhyming words.

Next was Pirate Pete (Sharratt, 2003), from which the children acquiredsix of the same words. With both of these books, the majority of the wordsthat the children acquired were nouns. Looking more closely at the typesof words the children had absorbed from the books they each acquired themost new vocabulary from, it could be seen that Joseph, who acquired themost new vocabulary from Here Come the Babies (Anholt and Anholt, 1993)had learnt words describing familiar items and actions, such as ‘blocks’ and‘crawl’. He also picked up on many of the words presented alliteratively,such as ‘hide’, ‘hug’ and ‘hold’. Aaron acquired the highest number of newwords, many of which were nouns, from The Very Hungry Caterpillar (Carle,1974). Aaron also learnt many new words from Bumpus Jumpus Dinosaurumpus(Mitton, 2003); looking in more detail at the types of words acquired fromthis book, he appeared most receptive to many of the onomatopoeic words,such as ‘bomp’, ‘donk’ and ‘thwack’.

AnalysisOver the six-week period Joseph participated in 172 more reading eventsand 191 more shared reading events than Aaron. Of their new vocabulary,Joseph acquired three per cent more words relating to storybooks thanAaron. These findings suggest that the higher number of stories beingshared with Joseph had some influence on the vocabulary he was acquir-ing. If this were the case, then plotting the number of shared readings fromhigh to low should have shown a downward trend in the number of newwords related to storybooks. Figures 9 and 10 indicate that this trend wasnot established, and the frequency of shared readings did not directly relateto the amount of new vocabulary acquired.

Joseph acquired the highest number of story related words in the weekthat he had the least amount of shared reading, and Aaron in the fourthlowest week. Tomasello and Farrar (1986: 1454) suggest a positive corre-lation between the amount of time spent in joint-attentional activities anda child’s vocabulary size. What our study did not record was how much timewas spent reading. It is possible that even though fewer books were sharedin these weeks, the amount of time dedicated to each book may have beengreater. There may well have been lengthier discussion between adult andchild about the story or illustrations, and the child may have been givenmore opportunities to respond. This would suggest a tentative link to thefindings of Liu et al. (2003), Justice and Ezell (2000) and Wells (1985) allof who found that the quality of shared book reading was more important

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

175

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 175

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

than quantity. However, such a link would be based on a presumption thatmore time was spent on fewer books.

In the week he acquired the most story related vocabulary, Joseph experienced 14 shared readings of a self-chosen book. This was the highestoccurrence of shared reading of a book of his choice from the six-weekperiod. Aaron experienced seven shared readings each of two different self-chosen books, the second highest occurrence during the six-weeks, and theonly week that more than one book of his choice was shared over five times.It is reasonable to suggest that the children were more focused on the booksin these weeks because they were receiving more joint attention on at leastone book that they had selected to be read to them. This corresponds withthe findings of Tomasello and Farrar (1986) on child-focused joint atten-tional episodes. However, these findings are not entirely conclusive as bothchildren acquired a high number of new words in the weeks they receivedthe fewest shared readings of a book of their choice.

Both children acquired the most story related vocabulary in the weeks thatthey did the most independent ‘reading’, and the least story related vocab-ulary in the weeks that they did the least independent ‘reading’. However,the weeks in between do not follow this trend. It may be coincidence thatthis happened with both children, but it raises the question of whether thechildren were using the independent ‘reading’ time to consolidate internal -ised vocabulary knowledge acquired through shared reading before attempt-ing vocalisations. There is tentative support of this theory from Pinker(1994: 268), who states ‘there is more going on in children’s minds thanwhat comes out of their mouths . . .’. This is neither an area that was suffi-ciently researched in this study, nor have we found it researched elsewhere.

The ‘set books’, those texts chosen specifically for the research and reada minimum of once a day, were read with far more frequency than themajority of other books. Both children acquired a majority of their storyrelated vocabulary from these ‘set books’. This suggests that the more oftena story is shared with a child, the more vocabulary they will acquire fromit. However, the findings do not support this suggestion further. Whenplotting the frequency with which each ‘set book’ was shared, from highto low, the graphs show no particular trend for either child. If the frequencyof reading were influencing the number of words learnt, the graphs wouldshow a downward trend. Therefore, it would appear that the frequency withwhich a book was shared with the children did not affect the number ofwords learnt from that book. Neither child learnt the most words from thebook shared the most frequently. This raises the questions of whether thechild’s enjoyment, and also the adult reader’s enjoyment, of a book had aneffect on their word acquisition?

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

176

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 176

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

I will first consider the adult’s enjoyment of a book. From the six ‘setbooks’ some appealed to the adult readers more than others. It is thereforepossible that when reading these books the adult reader spent more timetalking about the text and pictures, or used more varied tone of voice tomake the reading more exciting and enjoyable. This could have in turninfluenced the child’s perception of that book. This again refers back to thefindings of Liu et al. (2003), Justice and Ezell (2000) and Wells (1985)regarding quality over quantity. This is apparent in Aaron’s case. Hismother’s favourite of the ‘set books’ was The Very Hungry Caterpillar, and thiswas in turn the book from which Aaron acquired the most vocabulary. Theaccompanying notes also suggest that Aaron thoroughly enjoyed this bookand it became a favourite. Once again, however, this was not the casethroughout. Both mothers enjoyed reading Bumpus Jumpus Dinosaurumpus,because its alliterative and rhyming text, good rhythm, and onomatopoeicwords made the text fun to read. However, both children quickly tired ofthis book, and would not always participate in the reading.

The other question was whether the child’s enjoyment of the book playedany role in their acquisition of vocabulary related to it. It seems reasonableto assume that the more a child enjoyed a book, the more likely they wouldbe to concentrate on that book, and therefore acquire more vocabularyfrom it. It is important to remember that the ‘set books’ were only ‘set’ forseven readings; any further readings were at the child’s request. It shouldalso be remembered that Each Peach Pear Plum was the ‘set book’ for week one,so was available for the whole research period, whereas Bumpus JumpusDinosaurumpus was ‘set’ in week six, so was only available for one week. Rosie’sWalk (Hutchins, 2001), which was available for four of the six weeks, wasthe second to least read book for Joseph, and the least read book for Aaron,suggesting that neither child enjoyed this book to any great extent. Inaddition, both children learnt the fewest, or joint fewest, words from thisbook. This appears to support the above assumption. Neither child tookmuch enjoyment from Bumpus Jumpus Dinosaurumpus, and this was the bookfrom which Joseph learnt the joint fewest words, again supporting theearlier assumption. However Aaron, who frequently refused to listen to thestory, acquired the second highest number of new words from this book.Similarly, Joseph acquired the most new vocabulary from Here Come the Babies,a book which was less favoured than The Very Hungry Caterpillar and Each PeachPear Plum, both of which he chose to have read more frequently and ‘read’independently. These findings seem to suggest that the frequency and enjoy-ment of specific books are not the strongest influencing factors in aidingvocabulary acquisition.

Of the words acquired from all six ‘set books’, the vast majority were

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

177

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 177

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

nouns. Aitchison (1997: 44) idenitifies ‘naming insight’ as a precursor tothe ‘naming explosion’. It would appear that the children were experienc-ing this ‘naming explosion’ and were using the texts to acquire the namesof both new and familiar items. Aaron acquired the highest number ofwords from The Very Hungry Caterpillar; these words were mainly nouns, andsix of them directly related to food. Joseph acquired the highest number ofwords from Here Come the Babies, of which many, but not all, were nouns. Theywere also words of familiar items and actions. It appears that both of thesechildren were acquiring some words from the storybooks that would aidthem in communicating their needs and desires aside from the storybookreadings. This suggests that the children were focusing their attention onvocabulary that would help them to progress in their conversations, andnot simply acquiring random words, reflecting a Brunerian view of sociallydriven language acquisition (Bruner, 1983).

Both children learnt some of the same words from every ‘set book’. Thebook that had the highest frequency of commonly learnt words was EachPeach Pear Plum, a rhyming book that they each shared the most often. Fourof the seven words that they commonly acquired, and many of their indi-vidually acquired words, were rhyming words. This would suggest that therhyme was aiding their acquisition, thus echoing findings of Foy and Mann(2003). The words Joseph acquired from Here Come the Babies were oftenpresented alliteratively. The notes accompanying the data indicate that hehad a fondness for the page of alliterative text in this book, and would oftenturn to it during the shared reading. As Joseph acquired the highest numberof new words from this book, it is probable that this alliteration was aidinghis vocabulary acquisition, reflecting the findings of other studies (Justiceet al., 2002; Menn and Stoel-Gammon, 1996; Tsao et al., 2004). Aaronacquired a large number of words from Bumpus Jumpus Dinosaurumpus. Themajority of the words he acquired from this book were onomatopoeic,some of which also rhymed, word types that are core features of this text.This suggests that in addition to the rhyme aiding his vocabulary growth,Aaron was enjoying the language play that onomatopoeia allowed andsubsequently gained vocabulary from it (Taylor and Strickland, 1986).

Since each child gained over one third of his new vocabulary from story-books during the research period, it is fair to conclude that storybookscertainly did play a part in supporting the vocabulary acquisition of thesetwo toddlers. However, the frequency with which a book was shared didnot seem to bear any direct relation to how much new vocabulary wasacquired. The findings were inconclusive regarding the effect the adult andchild’s enjoyment of the book had on the acquisition of vocabulary fromit. What appears to be of more influence is the type of text encountered.

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

178

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 178

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Those books with rhyme and alliteration elicited more vocabulary, andmore specifically the children acquired a large majority of nouns. At theirstage of language acquisition it seemed important to the children to be ableto give names to new and familiar items, and further their ability at participating in conversation (Meek, 1991).

ConclusionThe findings from this study indicate that sharing storybooks with childrenin their second year of life does play a role in aiding their vocabulary acqui-sition. It should be remembered, however, that while both childrenacquired over one third of their new words from storybooks, they gainedalmost two thirds from other experiences, conversations and activities.Nevertheless, the findings do not imply that the children’s vocabularieswould necessarily have been more restricted without the use of storybooks.While it seems that the sharing of storybooks does play an important rolein vocabulary acquisition, for the benefit of the child, it is only one part ofa range of language related experiences.

The outcomes of this study have identified possible areas for furtherresearch. First, did the amount of time spent reading to the children affecttheir acquisition of vocabulary? On some occasions was a longer period oftime spent reading fewer books? Conducting similar research with moreemphasis placed on the amount of time spent reading, could help tosubstantiate claims that the quality of shared storybook reading is moreimportant in aiding vocabulary acquisition than the quantity of booksshared. Second, what effect, if any, was the children’s independent ‘reading’having on their vocabulary acquisition? Were they using this time to consol-idate learning before attempting vocalisations? Carrying out detailed obser-vations of children’s independent ‘reading’ might help provide answers asto the influence this was having on their acquisition of vocabulary.However, careful thought would have to be given to how this could be donewithout interrupting or influencing the child. Also the children in thisstudy acquired a large number of naming words. It would be interesting toknow if the same would be true of older children. It may be beneficial toconduct similar research with older children to discover if texts containingrhyme, alliteration and onomatopoeia continue to elicit new vocabulary.

A limitation of this research is that only two children of mothers with asimilar educational background were involved. While this was appropriatefor the scale of the study, it would be worthwhile conducting furtherresearch with a larger and more varied sample to attempt to clarify thefindings and to obtain a broader range of observations.

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

179

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 179

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

ReferencesAhlberg, A. and Ahlberg, J. (1989) Each Peach Pear Plum. London: Penguin.Aitchison, J. (1997) The Language Web: The Power and Problem of Words. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.Anholt, C. and Anholt, L. (1993) Here Come the Babies. London: Walker.Bottoms, J. (2004) ‘“Awakening the Mind”: The Educational Philosophy of William

Godwin’, History of Education 33(3): 267–82.Bruner, J. (1983) Child’s Talk: Learning to Use Language. Oxford: Open University Press.Carle, E. (1974) The Very Hungry Caterpillar. London: Puffin.Chomsky, N. (2000) New Horizons in the Study of Language and Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge

University Press.Clark, R. (2004) ‘Reading the Small Print’, Community Practitioner 77(8): 289–90.Cook, V.J. and Newson, M. (1996) Chomsky’s Universal Grammar: An Introduction. Oxford:

Blackwell.Dionne, G., Dale, P., Boivin, M. and Plomin, R. (2003) ‘Genetic Evidence for Bi -

directional Effects of Early Lexical and Grammatical Development’, Child Development74(2): 394–412.

Donmoyer, R. (2000) ‘Generalizability and the Single-Case Study’, in R. Gomm,M. Hammersley and P. Foster (eds) Case Study Method, pp. 45–68. London: SAGE.

Fletcher, K., Perez, A., Hooper, C. and Claussen, A. (2005) ‘Responsiveness and Attention during Picture-book Reading in 18-month-old to 24-month-old Toddlersat Risk’, Early Child Development and Care 175(1): 63–83.

Foy, J. and Mann, V. (2003) ‘Home Literacy Environment and Phonological Awarenessin Preschool Children: Differential Effects for Rhyme and Phoneme Awareness’,Applied Psycholinguistics 24: 59–88.

Gomm, R., Hammersley, M. and Foster, P. (2000) ‘Case Study and Generalization’, inR. Gomm, M. Hammersley and P. Foster (eds) Case Study Method, pp. 98–115. London:SAGE.

Hall, E. (2001) ‘Babies, Books and “Impact”: Problems and Possibilities in the Evalua-tion of a Bookstart Project’, Educational Review 53(1): 57–64.

Hutchins, P. (2001) Rosie’s Walk. London: Red Fox.Jack, K. (2005) ‘Why Babies need Books: They Coo, They Cuddle . . . But Can They

Really Enjoy Books?’, URL (consulted March 2005): http://www.scholastic.com/familymatters/read/ageb_2/needbooks.htm

Jarvis, J. and Lamb, S. (2001) ‘Interaction and the Development of Communication inthe Under Twos: Issues for Practitioners Working with Young Children in Groups’,Early Years 21(2): 129–38.

Justice, L. and Ezell, H. (2000) ‘Enhancing Children’s Print and Word Awarenessthrough Home-Based Parent Intervention’, American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology9: 257–69.

Justice, L., Weber, S., Ezell, H. and Bakeman, R. (2002) ‘A Sequential Analysis ofChildren’s Responsiveness to Parental Print References During Shared Book-ReadingInteractions’, American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11: 30–40.

Landau,B.and Gleitman,L.(1985)‘Language and Experience’, in B.Lust and C.Foley (eds)(2004) First Language Acquisition: The Essential Readings, pp. 208–24. Oxford: Blackwell.

Langston, A., Abbott, L., Lewis, V. and Kellett, M. (2004) ‘Early Childhood’, in S. Fraser,V. Lewis, S. Ding, M. Kellett and C. Robinson (eds) Doing Research with Children and YoungPeople, pp. 147–60. London: SAGE.

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

180

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 180

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lenneberg, E. (1967) ‘Language in the Context of Growth and Maturation’, in B. Lustand C. Foley (eds) First Language Acquisition: The Essential Readings, pp. 103–8 Oxford: Blackwell.

Lincoln, Y. and Guba, E. (2000) ‘The Only Generalization is: There is No Generaliza-tion’, in R. Gomm, M. Hammersley and P. Foster (eds) Case Study Method, pp. 27–44.London: SAGE.

Liu, H., Kuhl, P. and Tsao, F. (2003) ‘An Association between Mothers’ Speech Clarityand Infants’ Speech Discrimination Skills’, Developmental Science 6(3): F1–F10.

Locke, J.L. (1996) ‘Why do Infants Begin to Talk?: Language as an Unintended Consequence’, Journal of Child Language 23: 251–68.

Meek, M. (1991) On Being Literate. London: Random House.Menn, L. and Stoel-Gammon, C. (1996) ‘Phonological Development’, in P. Fletcher,

and B. MacWhinney (eds) The Handbook of Child Language, pp. 335–59. Oxford: Blackwell.

Messer, D. (1995) The Development of Communication: From Social Interaction to Language. Chichester: Wiley.

Mitton, T. (2003) Bumpus Jumpus Dinosaurumpus. London: Orchard Books.Moore, M. and Wade, B. (1997) ‘Parents and Children Sharing Books: An Observational

Study’, Signal (September): 203–14.Moore, M. and Wade, B. (2003) ‘Bookstart: A Qualitative Evaluation’, Educational Review

55(1): 3–13.Ninio, A. and Bruner, J. (1978) ‘The Achievement and Antecedents of Labelling’, Journal

of Child Language 5(1): 1–15.Odean, K. (1998) ‘Why Babies Need to Read’, URL (consulted March 2005):

http://www.bookmagazine.com/archive/issue1/nevertoosoon.shtmlOtomo, K. (2001) ‘Maternal Responses to Word Approximations in Japanese Children’s

Transition to Language’, Journal of Child Language 28: 29–57.Pinker, S. (1994) The Language Instinct. London: Penguin.Scarborough, H., Dobrich, W. and Hager, M. (1991) ‘Preschool Literacy Experience and

Later Reading Achievement’, Journal of Learning Disabilities 24(8): 208–511.Sharratt, N. (2003) Pirate Pete. London: Walker Books.Sheridan, M. (1997) From Birth to Five Years: Children’s Developmental Progress. London:

Routledge (revised and updated by M. Frost and A. Sharma).Stake, R. (2000) ‘The Case Study Method in Social Inquiry’, in R. Gomm, M. Hammer-

sley, and P. Foster (eds) Case Study Method, pp. 19–26. London: SAGE.Taylor, D. and Strickland, D. (1986) Family Storybook Reading. London: Heinemann.Tomasello, M. and Farrar, M. (1986) ‘Joint Attention and Early Language’, Child Develop-

ment 57(6): 1454–63.Tsao, F., Liu, H. and Kuhl, P. (2004) ‘Speech Perception in Infancy Predicts Language

Development in the Second Year of Life: A Longitudinal Study’, Child Development75(4): 1067–84.

Wade, B. and Moore, M. (1998) ‘An Early Start with Books: Literacy and MathematicalEvidence from a Longitudinal Study’, Educational Review 50(2): 135–45.

Wells, G. (1985) Language, Learning and Education. Slough: NFER-Nelson.Whitehead, M. (1999) Supporting Language and Literacy Development in the Early Years.

Buckingham: Open University Press.Whitehead, M. (2002) ‘Dylan’s Routes to Literacy: The First Three Years with Picture

Books’, Journal of Early Childhood Literacy 2(3): 269–89.

hepburn et al . : vocabulary acquis it ion in children

181

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 181

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Correspondence to:bridget a. egan, Faculty of Education, University of Winchester, Winchester, SO22

4NR, UK. [email: [email protected]]

journal of early childhood l iteracy 10(2)

182

159-182 ECL363754 Egan_Article 156 x 234mm 28/04/2010 13:33 Page 182

at SAGE Publications on November 28, 2012ecl.sagepub.comDownloaded from