41
8/16/2002 Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from Individual Level Data Julie Agnew* The College of William and Mary *The College of William and Mary, School of Business Administration, P.O. Box 8795, Williamsburg, Virginia 23187. Tel. (757) 221-2672. E-mail: [email protected]. The author thanks CitiStreet for providing the 401(k) plan data and Stefan Bokor for his immense help in organizing the data. The author thanks her dissertation committee, Pierluigi Balduzzi (Chair), Alicia Munnell, Eric Jacquier and Peter Gottschalk, for their careful comments and guidance. In addition, the author is grateful to Shlomo Benartzi for his comments and insight. Finally, the author thanks conference participants from the Retirement Research Consortium Fourth Annual Conference and the Frank Batten Young Scholars Conference. She gratefully acknowledges financial support from a dissertation fellowship from the Center for Retirement Research. Any errors are my own. The research reported herein was performed pursuant to a grant from the U.S. Social Security Administration (SSA) funded as part of the Retirement Research Consortium. The opinions and conclusions are solely those of the author and should not be construed as representing the opinions or policy of SSA or any agency of the Federal Government. 1

Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

8/16/2002

Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from

Individual Level Data

Julie Agnew*

The College of William and Mary

*The College of William and Mary, School of Business Administration, P.O. Box 8795, Williamsburg, Virginia 23187. Tel. (757) 221-2672. E-mail: [email protected]. The author thanks CitiStreet for providing the 401(k) plan data and Stefan Bokor for his immense help in organizing the data. The author thanks her dissertation committee, Pierluigi Balduzzi (Chair), Alicia Munnell, Eric Jacquier and Peter Gottschalk, for their careful comments and guidance. In addition, the author is grateful to Shlomo Benartzi for his comments and insight. Finally, the author thanks conference participants from the Retirement Research Consortium Fourth Annual Conference and the Frank Batten Young Scholars Conference. She gratefully acknowledges financial support from a dissertation fellowship from the Center for Retirement Research. Any errors are my own. The research reported herein was performed pursuant to a grant from the U.S. Social Security Administration (SSA) funded as part of the Retirement Research Consortium. The opinions and conclusions are solely those of the author and should not be construed as representing the opinions or policy of SSA or any agency of the Federal Government.

1

Page 2: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from

Individual Level Data

Abstract: This paper investigates how individual characteristics, such as age, salary, job tenure, and gender, influence an individual’s decision to over-invest in company stock and follow naïve diversification rules. Using a new and unique data set from one 401(k) plan with over 73,000 eligible employees, the results suggest that individual characteristics do influence company stock holdings. Ordered probit regression results indicate that the probability of over-investing in company stock for the average participant is greater for males, decreases with salary, increases with past company stock performance and is related to the participant’s division of employment. In addition, the paper investigates if individuals tend to follow a simple diversification rule, the 1/n heuristic. According to this rule, some 401(k) investors will choose to divide their contributions evenly among the n options available regardless of the type of investment vehicles offered. While the percentage of individuals who follow the 1/n heuristic in this study is lower than that found in previous studies, it still represents 5% of the sample. Probit regression analysis results suggest that the probability of following the 1/n heuristic decreases with increases in job tenure and salary. Finally, consistent with the mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat company stock as a separate “account”. The evidence from this study indicates that over-investing in company stock and practicing naïve diversification strategies may be occurring frequently in 401(k) plans and that certain individuals are more prone to make these decisions than others. Given the pivotal role 401(k) savings play in individuals’ retirements, this research may help plan sponsors design plans and train participants to make better-informed decisions.

2

Page 3: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Introduction

401(k) holdings are projected to become the largest asset an individual owns, with

the possible exception of his/her home (Sahadi (2001)). As a result, the financial security

of most individuals’ retirements will depend on how well their 401(k) portfolio has

performed. Thus, the importance of an individual’s asset allocation choices cannot be

overstated. As the debate over Social Security moves towards private accounts, these

allocation decisions take on even greater significance. Unfortunately, empirical research

focusing on the quality of asset allocation choices has been difficult to conduct in the past

because of a lack of detailed individual level data. This paper overcomes this obstacle by

taking advantage of a newly available database from one 401(k) plan with over 73,000

eligible participants. The fine level of detail in these data provides a unique opportunity

to analyze two potentially inefficient and commonly made retirement choices in 401(k)

plans: over-investing in company stock and following naïve diversification strategies.

While the influence of plan design on these decisions has already been documented, this

research contributes to the literature by highlighting how individual characteristics, such

as age, salary, job tenure and gender, relate to the efficiency of allocation decisions. It is

the first study to examine these two issues in one paper.

The perils of over-investing in company stock are well known as a result of several

recent high profile cases, including Enron, Lucent and WorldCom, where many

individuals lost most of their 401(k) nest eggs by following this strategy. Given the

benefits of diversification, it has long been a puzzle why so many individuals invest a

substantial amount of their retirement funds in the stock of their own company. Not only

are they concentrating their assets in a single stock, which is more risky than a well-

3

Page 4: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

diversified portfolio, but they are investing in a security that is highly correlated with

their own human capital. The risks of allocating a large portion of an individual’s

portfolio to company stock or even more simply to just one stock are so great that

legislation limits such investments in defined benefit plans and mutual funds.1 Yet

despite these laws and evidence that individuals are prone to over-invest in company

stock, the majority of 401(k) plans (80 percent) that offer company stock place no

restrictions at all on company stock allocations (Dugas (2000)).

Given the absence of restrictions, understanding what factors might lead an investor

to over-invest in company stock is important. The influence of past company stock

performance and plan design on company stock holding is already well documented in

the literature. Benartzi (2001) finds that company stock holdings are higher for

companies with relatively strong long-run stock performance and for companies with

employer stock only matches. However, until now, no one has tested for an additional

link between individual characteristics and company stock allocations. This paper fills

this gap. It is the first paper to jointly test the significance of past company stock

performance and demographic factors on company stock allocations.

In addition to investigating the determinants of company stock holdings, this paper

also examines the practice of naïve diversification strategies. The psychology literature

predicts that naïve diversification strategies will result when individuals are faced with

complicated decisions. The complexities of these decisions cause them to fall back on

simple rules of thumb. This paper investigates one particular strategy, the “1/n heuristic”,

studied by Bernatzi and Thaler (2001). According to this strategy, some 401(k) investors

overwhelmed by their investment choices will choose to simply divide their contributions

4

Page 5: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

evenly among the n investment options offered. They do so regardless of the type of

investment options they are given. While it can be argued that this rule of thumb will

often lead to a diversified portfolio, Benartzi and Thaler (2001) show that it can also lead

to large ex ante welfare losses when the portfolio chosen does not correspond to the

individual’s risk preferences.2 This paper investigates the extent to which individuals

follow this rule in this plan and if individual characteristics matter in this decision.

Finally, this paper investigates whether individuals treat company stock as a separate

asset class from other equities. Benartzi and Thaler (2001) find evidence supporting this

practice using aggregate 401(k) plan data. As a result when company stock is an option in

a 401(k) plan, Benartzi and Thaler (2001) predict that participants will choose to divide

their non-company stock contributions evenly among the non-company stock options. In

this paper, I call this the modified 1/n heuristic.

Benartzi and Thaler (2001) test their prediction using a sample of 103 401(k) plans

offering company stock and 67 plans that do not offer company stock. They find that the

mean allocations of the asset balances for the plans not offering company stock are split

approximately 50/50 between equities and fixed income. In contrast, they find that the

mean allocation to equities is over 71 percent when company stock is an option. Looking

closer, they find an average allocation of 42 percent to company stock. The remainder is

divided almost evenly between non-company stock equities (29 percent) and fixed

income (28 percent). This supports the modified 1/n heuristic.

Treating company stock as a separate account does have theoretical foundations.

This investment behavior is consistent with Shefrin and Statman’s (2000) multi-account

behavioral portfolio theory.3 According to their theory, individuals have difficulty

5

Page 6: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

processing covariances and other properties related to joint probability distributions so

they simplify their decision making by separating their assets into different mental

“accounts”. They ignore any correlation between these accounts when making their

portfolio decisions. In the case of 401(k) plans with company stock, it appears that

individuals are ignoring the correlation between their “company stock account” and their

“other financial investment account”.

The detail of the individual level data in this paper allows for a stronger test of the

modified 1/n heuristic. In addition, the data in this study avoid problems associated with

“allocation drift” of asset balances because the allocations are based on contribution

allocations.

Three main results emerge from the analysis of company stock investments. First, the

company stock investment patterns of individuals tend to be multimodal, with 78 percent

of the sample centered on allocations of 0, 25, 50, 75 and 100 percent. Second, over 75

percent of the individuals invest more in company stock than the maximum limit of 10

percent permitted by law in defined benefit plans. Third, results from ordered probit

regressions that control for past company stock performance suggest that the probability

of over-investing in company stock for the average participant is greater for males,

decreases with salary and is lower for corporate division workers.

The results of the analysis of the 1/n heuristic indicate that some individuals do

appear to follow the 1/n heuristic. However, the percentage is not as large as that found in

Benartzi and Thaler’s (2001) survey results. Kernel densities suggest that individuals are

treating company stock investment as a separate investment from other equities

confirming Benartzi and Thaler’s work. Finally, regression analysis results suggest that

6

Page 7: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

the probability of following the 1/n heuristic decreases with increases in job tenure and

salary/compensation status. Interestingly, individuals earning relatively higher salaries

tend to make more efficient decision related to both their company stock allocations and

diversification strategies than others.

The remainder of the paper is organized as follows. Section I summarizes the data set.

Section II describes the plan design and asset allocation choices. Section III discusses the

participation level in this plan and provides an overview of the possible reasons why it is

relatively low. Section IV summarizes the demographic and employment characteristics

of the actively contributing participants. Section V and VI present the empirical results

associated with company stock holdings and naïve diversification, respectively. Section

VII concludes.

I. Data

This paper uses a detailed database supplied by CitiStreet. The cross-sectional data

are from one large 401(k) plan with over 73,000 eligible employees.4 The plan is

sponsored by a global consumer product company. During the first two weeks of August

1998, 28,809 of the eligible participants made contributions. For the purpose of this

study, these participants are considered “active” participants. The data set includes each

active participant’s contribution allocations and, for most participants, the actual date this

allocation was chosen.

CitiStreet took over administration of this 401(k) plan in 1992. As a result, some data

are missing for participants who entered the plan prior to the administration change. In

particular, the actual date the participant made their allocation decision is unavailable for

5,814 participants who were employed prior to 1992 and did not change their

7

Page 8: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

contribution allocations during State Street’s tenure. An indicator variable identifies these

individuals so that the later analysis can control for the possible effects of administration

style on asset allocation decisions. For these individuals, the date they chose their

contribution allocation is estimated as either the date of employment or the date the plan

began in 1983, whichever is later. Since these individuals did not change their

allocations between 1992-1998, it seems reasonable to assume that they did not actively

change their allocations prior to 1992. Therefore, their current allocations are most likely

unchanged from the initial allocation decision they made when they first entered the plan.

For each allocation decision date, the past company stock returns over several buy and

hold periods are calculated.

One of the important features of these data is that detailed demographic information

is available for each eligible participant in the plan including each individual’s

participation status, salary, birthdate, date of employment, compensation status, and

gender. For the empirical analysis, age and time employed are calculated based on the

allocation decision date.

This data set has three noteworthy features. First, the asset allocations of the

contributions are broken down at the individual level. Aggregate contribution plan data

can blur the results if high contributing participants invest differently than low

contributing participants. The effect of large contribution levels is analogous to the

influence of large market capitalization stocks on a value-weighted index. Second, the

data are from one plan. While multiple plan data are appropriate for studying across-plan

variation, they can create a potential for omitted variable bias related to plan design or

plan educational efforts. Analyzing one plan eliminates this concern. A final advantage of

8

Page 9: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

the data is that allocations are based on contributions not asset balances. Asset

performance can move asset allocations based on asset balances away from the

participant’s intended allocation. Contribution allocations do not suffer from this

potential bias.

A disadvantage of these data is that information regarding participants’ assets outside

of the plan is not available. However, the evidence suggests that investors tend to invest

their retirement savings in the same fashion as their non-retirement assets

(Uccello(2000)). Another drawback of the data set is that it is missing some variables

that have been shown to impact asset allocation decisions: such as marital status,

education and financial literacy (eg. Agnew, Balduzzi and Sunden (2001), Sunden and

Surrette (1998), Dwyer, Gilkeson and List (2000)).

II. Plan Design and Asset Choices

In this plan, each participant may allocate his/her retirement fund contributions

among four different investment vehicles: an equity income fund, an S&P 500 index

fund, a guaranteed income contract fund (GIC), and company stock. Participants have the

option to change their contribution allocations daily. The company offers no financial

incentive for investing in company stock nor do they offer an employer match. The

absence of an employer match is an advantage because it eliminates any confounding

effects caused by the match design.

III. Plan Participation

Of the 73,721 eligible participants, 39 percent made at least one contribution

during the first two weeks of August 1998. This participation rate is low compared to

other studies.5 The low participation rate observed in this plan might be a result of several

9

Page 10: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

factors shown to decrease participation in the academic literature. One of the main

reasons might be that the plan does not offer an employer match (Munnell, Sunden, and

Taylor (2000), Papke and Poterba (1995)). Another is that the company offers a defined

benefit plan. Many studies (e.g. Andrews (1992), Bernheim and Garrett (1996)) have

shown that employees tend to participate less in their 401(k) plan if their company offers

a pension plan. Another possible factor is the relatively large size of this plan. This plan

is considered part of the large plan market (over 25,000 participants) and Clark and

Schieber (1998) find that the probability of participation decreases with the size of the

company. On the other hand, the plan sponsor did offer educational services, including

seminars and literature. These forms of corporate communication and education have

been shown to increase levels of participation (Munnell, Sunden and Taylor (2000),

Clark and Schieber (1998), Bernheim and Garrett (1996)). It is unclear how these

educational efforts affected participation in this plan. Finally, the definition of active

participant in this study is fairly restrictive because it limits participants to those who

made a contribution during the first two weeks of August 1998. Other studies use

different definitions. For example, Clark and Schieber (1998) define an active participant

as a person who made at least one contribution in the year 1994.

IV. Demographic and Employment Characteristics of Active Participants

Panel A of Table I describes the demographic and employment characteristics of

the active participants. Age and time employed are measured as of August 1998, while

salary is the 1997 annual salary. Individuals in this data sample are predominately male

(78 percent) with an average age of 39 years old. It is noteworthy that the participants

have relatively long average job tenures (10 years), which may indicate strong company

10

Page 11: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

loyalty. Interestingly, the median time employed is approximately eight and half years

and is over double the 1996 national median of nearly four years (CPS (1997)). In the

plan, nine percent of the sample are considered highly compensated individuals. This is a

legal designation based on several factors including salary. This status affects how much

a participant can contribute but does not restrict their allocation decisions.

Participants in the company work in one of four different divisions with the

majority of the participants (99 percent) working in two large consumer product

manufacturing divisions, “Division 1” and “Division 2”. The “Corporate Division”

employs 1% of the 401(k) participants and under 100 employees work for the “Other

Division”.

Participants earned mean 1997 salaries of approximately $46,000. Panel B of Table I

compares the plan’s median salary by age group to the median salary of the U.S.

population. The table shows that participants in this plan earn more than the general

population. However, the relationship between salary and age is similar between the two

groups with the exception of the 65+ age group. The discrepancy in the 65+ group may

be due to the limited number of participants (29) in this age group in this data set.

Table II describes the demographic characteristics by division. The main difference

between the four divisions appears to be the salary distributions. Employees of the two

smallest divisions make significantly higher salaries than the other divisions. The

Corporate Division’s mean salary is approximately $107,000, while the Other Division’s

mean salary is nearly $140,000. These salaries compare to approximately $48,000 and

$44,000 earned in Division 1 and 2, respectively. Employees in the two small divisions

also earn significantly more in the tenth and ninetieth percentiles of their sample and they

11

Page 12: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

are more likely to be highly compensated individuals. Except for the Corporate

Division, the divisions are predominately male. The groups do not differ significantly in

terms of average age or time employed.

V. Company Stock Allocations

A. General Findings

Consistent with anecdotal evidence, participants in this 401(k) plan show a tendency

to over-invest in company stock. The overall mean allocation to company stock holdings

in this plan is quite high (49 percent) compared to the 10 percent legal maximum defined

benefit plans may hold. The large average allocation might be partially explained by the

above normal price performance of the plan’s company stock. In this study, the company

stock had an annualized stock price return of 20.6 percent over the 10-year period ending

on December 31, 1997, compared to a S&P 500’s annual return of 14.7 percent over the

same time period. Benartzi (2001) shows that firms with relatively high returns over the

previous ten years have higher company stock allocations than poor performing firms.

This finding motivates why past company stock performance is controlled for in the later

regression analysis.

The general patterns of company stock allocations also deserve mention. One

interesting feature of the data is that despite the absence of restrictions on the

participants’ allocations, 78 percent of the allocations are clustered within one percentage

point of zero, 25, 50, 75 and 100 percent. Furthermore, there is a clear tendency for

many of the participants (52 percent) to invest either all or none of their contributions to

company stock.

12

Page 13: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

B. Nonparametric Analysis

Some interesting trends in company stock allocations emerge when these allocations

are summarized based on the participant’s demographic characteristics. This is not

surprising because demographic characteristics can proxy for several factors that can

affect a participant’s company stock holdings including financial knowledge, non-

retirement company stock holdings, risk preferences, company loyalty, and perceived

influence and knowledge of the company. This section will describe in detail why these

demographic characteristics could proxy for these factors. It presents a “nonparametric”

analysis of the data that will complement the regression analysis to follow.

Table III reports the company stock allocations based on demographic characteristics.

The non-normal distribution of the company stock holdings makes standard summary

statistics, such as means and standard deviations, less meaningful descriptors of the data.

Therefore, in addition to these statistics, Table III reports the proportion of each

demographic category that invests in six different investment ranges: zero percent, 1-25

percent, 26-50 percent, 51-75 percent, 76-99 percent, and 100 percent. A simple test of

proportions within each demographic category and investment range is used to test

whether a statistically significant difference exists. If demographic characteristics do not

matter, then a statistically significant difference in proportions should not be found. For

example, under the null hypothesis gender does not matter. Therefore, the proportion of

women investing 100 percent of their contributions to company stock should not be

statistically different than the proportion of men investing 100 percent of their

contribution to company stock. Notice that within each demographic category in Table

III the top row is bolded. This row is considered the base category. For each

13

Page 14: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

demographic group this base category is used in each test of proportions. Table III reports

the results of the test of proportions. Two (one) stars beside the proportions denote a

statistically significant difference from the base category at the one (five) percent level.

The first demographic category tested is gender. Empirical evidence suggests that

gender may proxy for financial education or risk tolerance. For example, research shows

that when a measure of financial education is not available, gender may serve as an

effective proxy for it. Dwyer, Gilkeson and List (2000) find that women typically have

less financial knowledge than men and that the educational disparities can substantially

explain the gender differences they find in risky mutual fund allocations.

Indeed, there is broad evidence suggesting that individuals overall lack a general

understanding of the risks associated with company stock investment and that education

may explain much of the variation in financial aptitude. A recent John Hancock

Financial Services’ survey highlights how individuals misread the risks of the market. In

the survey, respondents on average thought that a diversified stock fund was more risky

than investment in company stock. Benartzi (2001) reports an equally disturbing result

that 84% of respondents to a Morningstar survey believe that the overall stock market is

riskier than company stock. When this sample is limited to individuals with high school

education or less, this number increases to 94%. Therefore if gender proxies for

differences in education, men might be expected to invest less in company stock than

women. On the other hand, empirical research has found that men are more likely to

invest in riskier assets than women leading to the opposite conclusion (for example,

Sunden and Surette (1998), Hinz, McCarthy and Turner (1997), Agnew, Balduzzi and

Sunden (2001), Barber and Odean (2001)).

14

Page 15: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

The tests of proportions support the latter. In all but the 51-75% range, there is a

statistically significant (albeit economically small) difference in the proportion of men

investing in each investment range than women. The most pronounced difference

between the proportion of women and men investing in company stock is at the 100

percent investment range. Observe that 29 percent of the men allocate their entire

contribution to company stock compared to 26 percent of the women and that this

difference is significant at the one percent level. Furthermore, the mean allocation to

company stock by men is 50% compared to 47% for women. Interestingly, this gender

difference is smaller than that found by Clark, Goodfellow, Shieber and Warwick (2000).

In their study of several 401(k) plans, men invested an average 41% to company stock

compared to 27% for women. However, these gender differences could be a result of

different plan designs or varied long run company stock performance across plans.

The next two sections of the table demonstrate the influence of compensation level,

either salary or compensation status, on company stock investment. Compensation level

is another potential proxy for financial knowledge. Generally, financial knowledge is

considered positively related to compensation. This leads to the hypothesis that

employees who earn relatively high salaries or are considered highly compensated should

hold less in company stock.

Alternatively, compensation may be proxying for an employee’s opportunities for

stock based compensation. Generally, greater opportunities exist for higher salaried

employees to receive stock based compensation than for their lower wage counterparts.

This is the case in this plan where three stock option plans are offered. One plan is open

to all full-time employees and the number of options available is based on earnings. The

15

Page 16: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

second and third plans are targeted at middle and senior management. The options in

these plans are based on reaching performance goals. Thus, higher salaried and middle

and upper management employees have more opportunities to earn stock options than

lower salaried employees. Research shows that highly paid executives are concerned

about diversifying their company stock holdings but are often reluctant to sell their stock

based compensation. As a result they are finding sophisticated ways to hedge their

holdings. Results from one recent paper suggest that executives diversify their company

stock holdings through the use of zero cost collars and equity swaps (Ofek and Yermack

(2000)). Additional research shows that executives with high stock ownership negate

much of the impact from their stock compensation by selling previously owned shares

(Bettis, Bizjak and Lemmon (2000)). Given the demonstrated lengths that these

employees go to diversify their holdings, one might expect that these employees will hold

smaller amounts of company stock in their 401(k) accounts.

The results support both theories. Table III shows a decrease from the lowest wage

category to the highest wage category in the proportion of individuals allocating their

entire contribution to company stock that is significant at the one percent level. Twenty

eight percent of the under $25,000 category invest their whole contribution to company

stock compared to 24 percent of the $100,000 plus category. The reverse trend is

observed in the proportion of individuals who invest nothing in company stock. Here, 23

percent of the under $25,000 category invest nothing in company stock compared to 36

percent of the over $100,000 group. This difference in proportions is significant at the

one percent level. This supports results from Goodfellow and Schieber’s (1997) study of

24 different plans where low-wage earners were more likely to hold company stock than

16

Page 17: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

high-wage earners. Table III also shows fewer of the highly compensated individuals

allocating all of their contributions to company stock and more of these individuals

allocating none of their contributions to company stock.

Similar to gender, age may proxy for risk tolerance. Many life cycle theories

predict that individuals will hold less risk in their financial portfolio as they age.

Jagannathan and Kocherlachota (1996) suggest that young investors have a long stream

of future income. As individuals age, this stream of future income shortens diminishing

the value of their human capital. Therefore, they suggest that individuals should offset

this decline in the value of their human capital by reducing the risk of their financial

portfolio. Bodie, Merton, and Samuelson’s (1992) model leads to a similar prediction. In

their model, individuals can respond to low realized asset returns by increasing their

supply of labor. However, labor flexibility generally declines with age. Therefore, like

the previous model, older individuals are expected to hold more conservative investments

in their financial portfolios.

Table III is consistent with the stated life cycle hypotheses. Note that in Table III,

age is measured at the time the allocation decision is made. The 65 plus age category will

not be discussed because it includes only 6 participants. Notice the downward trend as

individuals age in the proportion of participants investing their entire contribution to

company stock. On the extreme ends, 19 percent of those between 55-64 years old invest

their entire contribution to company stock compared to 31 percent of the participants

under 35 years old. The difference in proportions is significant at the one percent level.

This trend is reversed and significant in the proportions investing nothing in company

stock.

17

Page 18: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Time employed may also proxy for risk tolerance. One hypothesis is that a

participant’s human capital is more stable the longer the individual is employed.

Therefore, employees with longer job tenures may choose to invest more in company

stock. An alternative hypothesis is that individuals with long job tenures may feel that

they have more influence on the company’s performance than new employees do so they

invest more. Finally, the length of an employee’s tenure may be a sign of company

loyalty or familiarity with the company. This also leads to the prediction of a positive

relationship.

The final hypothesis is based on Huberman’s (1998) work. Huberman coined the

phrase “familiarity bias” to describe the tendency of investors to invest heavily in what

they know. He suggests that this is a reason for high company stock holdings in 401(k)

plans. However, Benartzi (2001) presents conflicting empirical evidence that shows that

the impact of familiarity on company stock holdings is insignificant when past company

stock performance is included in the regression analysis.

Table III results report a negative relationship between time employed and company

stock holdings which is contrary to the predictions of all three theories. Here the

proportion of participants investing their entire contribution to company stock has a

marked decline with time employed. Thirty four percent of those with less than two

years experience invest their entire contribution to company stock compared to 22

percent of those with greater than 26 years experience. It is not surprising that these

results are similar to the age findings because these variables are most likely highly

correlated.

18

Page 19: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

The results show that the employee’s company division also explains some variation

in company stock holdings. One possible explanation for this is that there might be a

significant difference in the predominate occupation of the employees in each division.

Among other things, occupation type might proxy for the probability of earning stock

based compensation. For example, a corporate division may be more heavily

concentrated with executives who earn greater stock based compensation than employees

of a division predominately comprised of factory workers. Thus, the expected average

allocation to company stock would be relatively lower in the corporate division compared

to the other division. The occupation type may also provide additional information about

the employee’s education level beyond that obtained from salary information. It seems

reasonable to assume that a corporate division may be more heavily comprised of

executives with college degrees, while a factory division may have a high percentage of

blue-collar workers who are predominately high school graduates. On the other hand, the

division variables may also proxy for many other unobservables so care must be taken

not to over interpret these results.

In this study, the predominate occupation does differ between divisions. A discussion

with CitiStreet indicates that the Corporate Division consists mainly of executives, while

the employees of Division 1 and Division 2 tend to be factory workers. As predicted,

Table III shows the Corporate Division has the lowest proportion of individuals investing

their entire contribution to company stock and the highest proportion of individuals who

invest nothing in company stock. These results support the theory that either the

executives in the Corporate Division are limiting their company stock holdings to

19

Page 20: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

compensate for stock based compensation or they are doing so because they have a

relatively better understanding of the inherent risks of company stock investment.

Finally, an indicator variable highlights the participants that enrolled in the plan prior

to CitiStreet’s administration and did not make allocations during CitiStreet’s tenure.

This variable controls for the possible influence of plan administration style. Observe that

the difference in the proportions is striking and statistically significant at every

investment range between the two groups. For example, 50 percent of those who made

no allocation changes during CitiStreet’s tenure invested their entire contribution to

company stock compared to 23 percent of those who did make a change or entered the

plan during CitiStreet’s tenure. This finding strongly supports the use of this variable as

a control variable in the following regression analysis.

C. Econometric Analysis of Company Stock Holdings

The nonparametric evidence suggests that there are relationships between the

demographic variables and company stock holdings. This section will econometrically

test for the joint effects of these factors on company stock allocations. In addition, it will

control for the effects of past company stock performance.

Generally, a two-limit censored regression model is used in studies of asset

allocations. For example, Agnew, Balduzzi and Sunden (2001) use this model to study

the relationship between demographic characteristics and equity allocations in one 401(k)

plan. However, given the prevalence of company stock allocations clustered at 0, 25, 50,

75 and 100 percent, an ordered probit regression seems more appropriate for these data.

Therefore for the econometric analysis, the company stock allocations are grouped into

20

Page 21: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

six categories (0%, 1-25%, 26-50%, 51%-75%, 76%-99%, 100%) and an ordered probit

regression is used to study the effects of the individual characteristics on company stock

allocations. 6

Table IV reports the marginal effects from the ordered probit regression for each asset

allocation range. For the average participant, the marginal effects show the change in

probability of staying in that investment range given a small change in the independent

variable. In the case of indicator variables, the marginal effect is for a discrete change of

the indicator variable from zero to one.

It is clear that most of the economically significant variation is at the extreme

investment ranges (0% and 100%). The intermediate investment ranges marginal effects

while statistically significant are not economically meaningful. Therefore, the discussion

will concentrate on the 100 percent range. The results suggest that men are 3.3 percent

more likely to invest their entire contributions to company stock than women, supporting

the theory that men tend to make more risky asset allocation choices. Salary is also

significantly related to company stock holdings. The results suggest that an average

employee earning $100,000 is 3.7 percent less likely to invest his/her entire contribution

to company stock than an average employee earning $40,000. In this case, salary may be

a proxy for financial education or the amount of stock based compensation. Given the

nonparametric results, it is not surprising that individuals who made their allocation

decision during the previous administrator’s tenure are 20 percent more likely to invest

everything in company stock.

The division of employment also has a significant role in company stock holdings.

Relative to the Corporate Division, participants in Division 1 are 4 percent more likely to

21

Page 22: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

invest in company stock. Similarly, participants in Division 2 are 10 percent more likely.

The results support the hypothesis that either the executives in the Corporate Division are

limiting their company stock holdings to compensate for stock based compensation or

they are limiting their company stock holdings because they have a relatively better

understanding of the inherent risks in company stock investment. Interestingly, age and

time employed are not significantly related to company stock holdings.

Finally, the results support Benartzi’s (2001) findings that past raw buy and hold

returns are positively related to company stock holdings.6 The reported regression

includes one year buy and hold company stock returns measured prior to each

individual’s allocation decision date. The sample returns range from a minimum of

negative 26 percent to a maximum of 86 percent. The average return is 26 percent, with a

standard deviation of 21 percent. The regression predicts that a 10 percent increase in the

one year return of the company stock relevant to its sample average increases the

probability of investing the entire contribution to company stock by 1.6 percent.

VI. Naïve Diversification

A. General Findings

The analysis now shifts from a study of the factors related to a participant over-

investing in company stock, to the factors related to the practice of naïve diversification

strategies. One of the main findings is that the frequency of participants following the 1/n

heuristic is less than the percentage found in survey tests conducted by Benartzi and

Thaler (2001). In their study, several different surveys were mailed to University of

California employees. The surveys asked the employees how they would allocate their

investments to a selection of funds. The results vary with the investment choices offered

22

Page 23: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

but, in general, over 20 percent of each surveyed group chose the 1/n option. In this

study less than four percent follow the 1/n heuristic and five percent follow the modified

1/n heuristic. In fact, most participants (41 percent) allocate their entire contribution to

only one fund. Looking closer, the majority (70 percent) of those participants that invest

in only one fund invest their entire contribution to company stock. The percent of the

sample decreases with the number of funds held. A little under twelve percent hold all

four funds.

One possible explanation for why these results differ from previous empirical work is

that the participant’s choice may be influenced by the manner in which the allocations are

selected. In Benartzi and Thaler’s (2001) survey, participants selected their allocations by

filling out a form with the possible investment choices listed. In this study’s plan,

individuals chose allocations over the phone using an automated system. Interestingly,

the company stock option was the last option described on the phone. The ordering of

the company stock option might have contributed to its popularity. There is some

psychology literature that suggests that the ordering of choices influences decision

making, with either the options listed first (primacy effects) or listed last (recency effects)

having the largest influence. Understanding how these two approaches to allocation

selection ultimately influence the participant’s final choice is an interesting area for

future research.

23

Page 24: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

B. The Modified 1/n Heuristic

Using aggregate data, Benartzi and Thaler (2001) find evidence that individuals treat

company stock as a separate asset class from other 401(k) investments. As a result, the

participants tend to split their non-company stock investment evenly among the non-

company stock options. This paper refers to this practice as the modified 1/n heuristic.

This section investigates whether similar behavior applies to this plan.

This analysis complements previous work because it provides a stronger test of this

practice. It is a stronger test for two reasons. First, the tests in this paper are based on

contribution allocations rather than asset balance allocations. Therefore, the influence of

fund performance on allocations is not a concern. Second, the individual level data allows

for the calculation of the allocation of non-company stock holdings by individual rather

than by plan. This permits the examination of the distribution of company stock holdings

across individuals.

The analysis begins with an examination of the mean and median allocations to each

fund in Table V. The first two columns of Table V list the mean and median allocations

to each fund and the last two columns list the adjusted mean and median allocations to

each fund. The adjusted allocations are simply the percent allocated to the particular non-

company stock investment vehicle divided by the total invested in non-company stock

investment vehicles. The first subsample includes all the participants that invest in all

four funds and comprises roughly eleven percent of the sample. Notice that the adjusted

allocations are very close to 33.3%, which equates to evenly splitting the non-company

stock contributions among the non-company stock assets. The same exercise is repeated

24

Page 25: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

for subsamples of investors that hold three funds (including company stock). The results

again support the modified 1/n with the adjusted allocations close to 50 percent.

Kernel densities further support the findings. Panel A of Figure 1 displays a kernel

density representing company stock holdings and adjusted and unadjusted kernel

densities for the holdings of the three non-company stock investment vehicles. These

densities are drawn from the sample of participants who invest in all four funds. As

expected, the company stock density shows a multimodal distribution. The spikes at zero

and one are no longer observed because the sample is restricted to those who invest in all

four funds. Therefore, a zero or 100 percent allocation to company stock, or any

investment vehicle for that matter, is not possible.

Looking at the three other investment vehicle kernel densities, the difference is

striking between the unadjusted (denoted by a line with triangles) and the adjusted

(denoted by a line with squares) kernel densities. The unadjusted kernel densities for the

non-company stock funds have several probable allocations. However, the adjusted

distributions are strongly centered at 33%. This percentage equals the 1/n allocation an

investor would choose if allocating between three funds. This implies that after adjusting

for company stock holdings, these individuals tend to allocate their remaining assets

evenly among the other funds. This finding strongly supports Benartzi and Thaler’s

(2001) assertion that some individuals treat company stock as a separate asset class and

as a result slightly modify how they follow the 1/n rule.

Panels B-D repeat the exercise for subsamples of participants holding company stock

and two other funds. The results for Panel B and D are very similar to the earlier findings.

As expected, the adjusted kernel densities are centered at 50 percent.

25

Page 26: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

C. Econometric Analysis

The final empirical question is what type of person is most likely to follow the 1/n

heuristic? To test this a dummy variable that equals one if the individual follows the 1/n

heuristic and zero if not is constructed. An additional dummy variable is created to take

into account the impact of the company stock option on the 1/n rule. This additional

dummy variable is constructed based on the modified 1/n heuristic.

Tables VI displays the results of probit analysis using the two different 1/n dummy

variables. The marginal effects of salary and employment tenure are significant and

negative for both regressions. This suggests that high salary individuals and participants

with longer job tenure are less likely to follow the 1/n rule. In both regressions, the

average employee earning $100,000 is nearly three percent less likely to follow the 1/n

rule than an average employee earning $40,000. One theory is that the higher salaried

individuals are more educated and thus less likely to rely on simple rules for investing. It

is not clear why the employees with longer job tenures are less likely to follow the 1/n

rule.

VII. Conclusion

This paper focuses on two potentially inefficient and commonly made allocation

choices in 401(k) plans: over-investing in company stock and following naïve

diversification strategies. Given the pivotal role of 401(k) savings on an individual’s

retirement, a better understanding of who may make these uninformed allocation

decisions is important. This research contributes to the literature by highlighting what

types of individuals are most likely to make these inefficient choices. The results can help

26

Page 27: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

plan sponsors target high risk individuals and improve plan design, as well as inform the

current Social Security debate.

Three important results emerge from the company stock analysis. First,

employees tend to cluster their allocations at zero, 25, 50, 75 and 100 percent. Empirical

researchers should bear this in mind before applying standard econometric techniques to

allocation data. Second, in this plan the tendency to invest in more company stock than

the recommended limit is high. Third, demographic and employment characteristics

affect company stock allocations. Results of regressions that control for past company

stock performance suggest that the probability of over-investing in company stock for the

average participant is greater for males, decreases with salary and is lower for corporate

division workers.

The investigation of whether employees follow the 1/n heuristic confirms that

investors tend to treat company stock as a separate account. While the percent of the

individuals who follow the 1/n heuristic is lower than that found in previous studies, it

still represents 5% of the participants in this study. Regression results suggest that highly

compensated individuals are less likely to invest follow the 1/n heuristic, as well as

employees with relatively long job tenures.

Interestingly, the results show that individuals earning relatively higher salaries tend

to make more efficient decisions related to both their company stock allocations and

diversification strategies than others. Policy makers and plan sponsors should bear this in

mind. After all, even though the Social Security system provides higher replacement

ratios for individuals with relatively lower incomes, lower income participants will most

27

Page 28: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

likely depend the most on their 401(k) savings during retirement. This research shows

that this group tends to make the most inefficient choices.

28

Page 29: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

References

Agnew, Julie, Pierluigi Balduzzi and Annika Sunden, 2001, Portfolio Choice, Trading, and Returns in a Large 401(k) Plan, Center for Retirement Research, Working paper 2000-06, Boston College.

Andrews, Emily S., 1992, The Growth and Distribution of 401(k) Plans. in John Turner

and Daniel Beller, eds.: Trends in Pensions 1992, Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Labor.

Barber, Brad M., and Terrance Odean, 2001, Boys will be Boys: Gender,

Overconfidence, and Common Stock Investment, Quarterly Journal of Economics 116 (1), 261-292.

Bassett, William F., Michael J. Fleming, and Anthony P. Rodrigues, 1998, How

Workers Use 401(k) Plans: The Participation, Contribution, and Withdrawal Decisions. Federal Reserve Bank of New York Staff Report, No. 38 (March).

Benartzi, Shlomo, 2001, Excessive Extrapolation and the Allocation of 401(k)

Accounts to Company Stock? Journal of Finance 56, 1747-1764. Benartzi, Shlomo, and Richard Thaler, 2001, Naive Diversification Strategies in

Retirement Saving Plans, American Economic Review 91(1), 79-98. Bernheim, B. Douglas and Garrett, Daniel M., 1996, The Determinants and

Consequences of Financial Education in the Workplace: Evidence from a Survey of Households, National Bureau of Economic Research, Working paper 5667.

Bettis, J. Carr, John Bizjak and Michael Lemmon, 2001, Managerial Ownership, Incentive Contracting, and the Use of Zero-Cost Collars and Equity Swaps by Corporate Insiders, Journal of Financial and Quantitative Analysis 36 (3), 345-370.

Bodie, Zvi, Robert C. Merton, and William F. Samuelson, 1992, Labor Supply Flexibility and Portfolio Choice in a Life Cycle Model, Journal of Economic Dynamics and Control 16, 427-449.

Clark, Robert L. and Sylvester Schieber, 1998, Factors Affecting Participation

Levels in 401(k) Plans, in Olivia S. Mitchell and Sylvester J. Schieber, eds.: Living with Defined Contribution Plans, (University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, PA)

29

Page 30: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Clark, Robert L., Gordon P. Goodfellow, Sylvester J. Schieber, and Drew Warwick, 1999, Making the Most of 401(k) Plans: Who’s Choosing What? in Olivia Mitchell, P. Brett Hammond and Anna M. Rappaport, eds.: Forecasting Retirement Needs and Retirement Wealth, (University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, PA)

Dugas, Christine, 4 August 2000, Don’t Bank 401(k) on Employer’s Stock. If Company hits Bad Spot, Retirement Plan Can Tank, USA Today, Money Section: 3B.

Dwyer, Peggy D., James H. Gilkeson and John A. List, 2000, Gender Differences in

Revealed Risk Taking: Evidence from Mutual Fund Investors, Working paper, University of Central Florida.

Employee Tenure in the Mid-1990s, CPS Publications, 30 January 1997. Goodfellow, Gordon P. and Sylvester J. Schieber, 1997, Investment of Assets in

Self-Directed Retirement Plans, in Michael S. Gordon, Olivia S. Mitchell, and Marc M. Twinney, eds.: Positioning Pensions for the Twenty-First Century. (University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, PA).

Hinz, Richard P., David D. McCarthy, and John A. Turner, 1997, Are Women

Conservative Investors? Gender Differences in Participant Directed Pension Investments. in Michael S. Gordon, Olivia S. Mitchell, and Marc M. Twinney, eds.: Positioning Pensions for the Twenty-First Century. (University of Pennsylvania Press, Philadelphia, PA)

Huberman, Gur, 2001, Familiarity Breeds Investment, Review of Financial Studies 14

(3): 659-680.

Jagannathan, Ravi, and Narayana R, Kocherlakota, 1996, Why Should Older People Invest less in Stocks Than Younger People? Federal Reserve of Minneapolis, Quarterly Review (Summer), 11-23.

John Hancock Financial Services, 1999, The Sixth Defined Contribution Plan Survey.

Kusko, Andrea L., James M. Poterba, and David W. Wilcox, 1994, Employee

Decisions with Respect to 401(k) Plans. National Bureau of Economic Research, Working paper 4635.

Meulbroek, Lisa, 2002, Company Stock in Pension Plans: How Costly Is It? Harvard

Business School, Working paper 02-058, Harvard University. Munnell, Alicia H., Annika Sunden, and Catherine Taylor. 2000, What Determines

401(k) Participation and Contributions?, Center for Retirement Research, Working paper 2000-12, Boston College.

30

Page 31: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Ofek, Eli and David Yermack, 2000, Taking Stock: Equity-Based Compensation and the Evolution of managerial Ownership, Journal of Finance 55 (3): 1367-1384.

Papke, Leslie E. and James M. Poterba, 1995, Survey of Evidence on Employer

Match Rates and Employee Saving Behavior in 401(k) Plans, Economic Letters 49: 313-17.

Papke, Leslie E.,1995, Participation in and Contributions to 401(k) Plans: Evidence from

Plan Data. Journal of Human Resources. XXX(2): 311-25. Read, Daniel and George Loewenstein, 1995, Diversification Bias: Explaining

the Discrepancy in Variety Seeking between Combined and Separated Choices, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied :.34-49.

Sahadi, Jeanne,4 January 2001, 10:20 a.m. ET.The 401(k) Turns 20. www.cnnfn.com. Shefrin, Hersh and Meir Statman, 2000, “Behavioral Portfolio Theory,” Journal

of Financial and Quantitative Analysis 35 (2): 127-151. Sunden, Annika E., and Brian J. Surette, 1998, Gender Differences in the

Allocation of Assets in Retirement Savings Plans, American Economic Review: 207-211.

Thaler, Richard H, 1999, Mental Accounting Matters, Journal of Behavioral

Decision Making 12: 183-206. Uccello, Cori E. 2000, 401(k) Investment Decisions and Social Security

Reform, Center for Retirement Research, Working Paper 2000-4, Boston College. Vanderhei, Jack, Sarah Holden and Carol Quick, 2000, 401(k) Plan Asset

Allocation, Account Balances, and Loan Activity in 1998, EBRI Issue Brief Number 218.

31

Page 32: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Endnotes

1 For example, the law restricts professional managers from investing more than 10% of a company’s defined benefit assets in the company’s own stock and the SEC requires that “diversified” mutual funds invest no more than 5 percent of assets in one company’s stock. This is with respect to 75 percent of the portfolio’s assets. In addition, Meulbroek (2002) demonstrates that holding company stock is inefficient for all employees regardless of an individual’s level of risk tolerance. 2 To illustrate this, suppose that a 401(k) offers ten investment choices that include nine equity funds and one money market fund. An individual following the 1/n heuristic, would allocate 10% of their contributions to each fund resulting in a 90% allocation to equities. It is clear that this allocation would not be optimal for everyone especially a participant nearing retirement. 3 Thaler (1999) provides an excellent review of the mental accounting literature. He refers to mental accounting as “the set of cognitive operations used by individuals and households to organize, evaluate, and keep track of financial activities”. Shefrin and Statman (2000) apply this concept to their portfolio theory. 4 An eligible employee is an employee that may participate in the 401(k) plan if he/she chooses. 5 For example, Clark and Schieber’s (1998) found that on average 73.5% of eligible employees participated in their 401(k) plans in their analysis of plan data from 19 firms with 700 to 10,000 employees. Similarly, Munnell, Sunden, and Taylor (2000) report a 72% mean participation rate among eligible employees using the 1998 Survey of Consumer Finances data. 6 Results from the less sensible tobit regression are available upon request. They support the ordered probit findings. 7 Additional ordered probit regressions that include raw buy and hold returns calculated over different periods ranging from two to ten years are available. The regression that includes the one year returns is reported because it produced the highest pseudo r-squared.

32

Page 33: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table I. Descriptive Plan Statistics Panel A. General Statistics This table describes general statistics concerning the plan participants: contribution status (as of August 1998), gender, age in years (as of August 1998), time employed in years (as of August 1998), compensation status, division of employment and 1997 annual salary.

Obs % Mean Std Min Max

Contribution StatusParticipants Not Contributing 44,912 61%Participants Contributing 28,809 39% Total Eligible Participants 73,721 100%Gender Male 22,550 78% Female 6,259 22%Age 28,809 39.41 8.88 18.92 70.88Years Employed 28,809 10.22 7.68 0.10 47.96Highly Compensated Individual Yes 2,452 9% No 26,357 91%Division Corporate 325 1% Division 1 11,964 42% Division 2 16,449 57% Other 71 0%Salary 28,809 46,410$ 31,167$ 15,600$ 1,675,025$

Panel B. Comparison of Age-Salary Structure for U.S. Population and 401(k) Sample The table presents a comparison between the median salary by age group for the U.S. population at large and the 401(k) plan participants. The source for the U.S. population is CPS 1997.

Age Range Median 1997 Salary: Median 1997 Salary:U.S. Population* 401(k) Plan

Under 35 years old $22,846 $37,03835-44 years old $30,880 $40,75545-54 years old $33,106 $40,55555-64 years old $29,434 $39,06865+ years old $21,032 $37,337

*Source: CPS 1997

33

Page 34: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table II. Descriptive Plan Statistics by Division This table breaks down each division by demographic information: gender, 1997 annual salary, age (as of August 1998), time employed (as of August 1998), time enrolled in the plan (as of August 1998) and compensation status. HCE stands for highly compensated individual.

Division Number of Employees

% of Division Male

Mean Age (Years)

Mean Time Employed

(Years)

% Division 100% Invested

in Company Stock

Corporate 325 48% 43.05 12.29 16% Division 1 11,964 81% 38.34 10.52 23% Division 2 16,449 77% 40.11 9.95 33% Other 71 89% 41.72 11.49 25%

Division Median Salary

Mean Salary Salary-10th Percentile

Salary- 90th Percentile

% of Division HCE

Corporate 70,000$ 107,397$ 36,000$ 233,000$ 45% Division 1 39,750$ 47,866$ 25,953$ 73,790$ 10% Division 2 38,905$ 43,743$ 26,104$ 63,659$ 6% Other 160,000$ 139,563$ 99,800$ 160,000$ 99%

34

Page 35: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table III. Summary Statistics of Company Stock Holdings This table reports summary statistics for company stock holdings based on demographic characteristics. In addition to the mean and median allocations, the table presents the proportion of each demographic category invested in each of the six investment ranges. The first row of each demographic category (bolded) is considered the base category. Within each investment range and demographic category, a test of proportions is run. ** (*) stars beside the proportions denote a statistically significant difference from the base category at the one (five) percent level.

Demographic Category

Obs.

0% 1-25% 26-50% 51-75% 76-99% 100% Median MeanAll 28,809 23% 13% 26% 7% 2% 29% 50% 49%Sort by Gender: Male 22,550 23% 13% 26% 7% 2% 29% 50% 50% Female 6,259 24% * 15%** 27% * 7% 2% * 26%** 50% 47%Annual Salary: Under $25,000 2,218 23% 16% 26% 6% 1% 28% 50% 48% $25,000-$49,000 19,526 22% 13%** 26% 7% 2% * 30% * 50% 51% $50,000-$74,999 4,671 22% 15% 28% 7% * 2% * 26% * 50% 47% $75,000-$99,999 1,148 30%** 14% 26% 7% 1% 22%** 35% 42% $100,000+ 1,246 36%** 11%** 22%** 6% 1% 24%** 30% 41%Highly Compensated Individual: Yes 2,452 30% 12% 24% 7% 2% 25% 40% 44% No 26,357 23%** 14% 26% * 7% 2% 29%** 50% 50%Age: Under 35 years old 15,180 21% 13% 26% 7% 2% 31% 50% 52% 35-44 years old 9,562 25%** 14% 26% 7% 2%** 27%** 50% 47% 45-54 years old 3,377 28%** 13% 27% 5%** 2% 25%** 40% 45% 55-64 years old 684 35%** 13% 26% 5% * 1% 19%** 30% 38% 65+ years old 6 67%** 0% 33% 0% 0% 0% 0% 14%Time Employed: 0-2 years 11,133 19% 14% 25% 6% 2% 34% 50% 54% 3-5 years 5,362 22%** 15% 27% * 7% * 2% * 27%** 50% 48% 6-10 years 5,306 24%** 13% 27% * 8%** 3%** 26%** 50% 48% 11-15 years 3,227 28%** 13% 27% 7%** 2% 23%** 40% 44% 16-20 years 2,209 30%** 12% * 26% 7% * 2% 22%** 40% 43% 20-25 years 988 32%** 13% 25% 5% 1% 24%** 34% 42% 26-50 years 584 35%** 10% * 27% 4% * 2% 22%** 32% 40%Division: Corporate 325 34% 20% 26% 3% 1% 16% 25% 34% Other 71 34% 10% * 25% 4% 1% 25% 34% 42% Division 1 11,964 26%** 15% * 28% 7% * 2% 23%** 40% 44% Division 2 16,449 21%** 12%** 25% 7%** 2% 33%** 50% 53%Prior Indicator: No 22,995 25% 15% 28% 7% 2% 23% 40% 45% Yes 5,814 17%** 6%** 20%** 6%** 1%** 50%** 90% 66%

Company Stock

Allocation

Percent of Sample Within Each Investment Range

35

Page 36: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table IV. Ordered Probit Regression: Company Stock Allocations The table presents the marginal effects of an ordered probit regression of company stock allocations against participant characteristics. “Male” is a dummy variable equal to one if the participant is male, zero otherwise. “Salary” is the annual 1997 salary (unit: ten thousand dollars). “Age” is the age of the participant at the time the allocation decision is made (unit: years). “Time Employed” equals the time the participant has been employed at the time the allocation decision is made (unit: years). “Compensation Status” is a dummy variable that equals one if the individual by law is considered highly compensated, otherwise it equals zero. “Prior Indicator” equals one for individuals who were employed prior to the current plan administration and made no changes in their contribution allocations during the current plan administrator’s tenure. “Division #” is a dummy variable that equals one if the participant is in the division. The Corporate Division is the omitted dummy. “One Year Co Stock Return” is the one year raw buy and hold return earned prior to the allocation decision. Robust standard errors, reported in parentheses, are adjusted for heteroskedacity. The psuedo R-squared is the log-likelihood value on a scale from zero to one, where zero corresponds to the constant-only model and one corresponds to perfect prediction (a log-likelihood of zero). ** (*) indicates significance at the 1% (5%) level.

Number of Observations= 28,809Marginal Effects (dF/dx) Pseudo R 2 = .0204

IndependentVariables Y=0% Y=1-25% Y=26-50%

Male (1) -0.0307 ** -0.0073 ** 0.0004(0.0048) (0.0011) (0.0003)

Age -0.0020 -0.0005 0.0000(0.0017) (0.0004) (0.0000)

Age Squared 0.0000 * 0.0000 * 0.0000(0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000)

Time Employed 0.0004 0.0001 0.0000(0.0004) (0.0001) (0.0000)

Salary 0.0055 ** 0.0014 ** 0.0001(0.0012) (0.0003) (0.0000)

Prior Indicator (1) -0.1442 ** -0.0474 ** -0.0247 **(0.0041) (0.0020) (0.0019)

Compensation Status (1) 0.0015 0.0004 0.0000 (0.0103) (0.0026) (0.0001)Division 1 (1) -0.0374 * -0.0096 * -0.0008

(0.0171) (0.0045) (0.0006)Division 2 (1) -0.0925 ** -0.0221 ** 0.0007

(0.0179) (0.0040) (0.0007)Other Division (1) -0.0564 -0.0171 -0.0061

(0.0361) (0.0130) (0.0080)One Year Co Stock Return -0.1416 ** -0.0357 ** -0.0016

(0.0093) (0.0025) (0.0009)(1) dF/dx is for a discrete change of the dummy variable from 0 to 1

Dependent Variables: Y=% Invested in Company Stock

36

Page 37: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table IV. (Continued) Ordered Probit Regression: Company Stock Allocations The table presents the marginal effects of an ordered probit regression of company stock allocations against participant characteristics. “Male” is a dummy variable equal to one if the participant is male, zero otherwise. “Salary” is the annual 1997 salary (unit: ten thousand dollars). “Age” is the age of the participant at the time the allocation decision is made (unit: years). “Time Employed” equals the time the participant has been employed at the time the allocation decision is made (unit: years). “Compensation Status” is a dummy variable that equals one if the individual by law is considered highly compensated, otherwise it equals zero. “Prior Indicator” equals one for individuals who were employed prior to the current plan administration and made no changes in their contribution allocations during the current plan administrator’s tenure. “Division #” is a dummy variable that equals one if the participant is in the division. The Corporate Division is the omitted dummy. “One Year Co Stock Return” is the one year raw buy and hold return earned prior to the allocation decision. Robust standard errors, reported in parentheses, are adjusted for heteroskedacity. The psuedo R-squared is the log-likelihood value on a scale from zero to one, where zero corresponds to the constant-only model and one corresponds to perfect prediction (a log-likelihood of zero). ** (*) indicates significance at the 1% (5%) level.

Number of Observations= 28,809Marginal Effects (dF/dx) Pseudo R 2 = .0204

IndependentVariables Y=51-75% Y=76-99% Y=100%

Male (1) 0.0031 ** 0.0012 ** 0.0333 **(0.0005) (0.0002) (0.0050)

Age 0.0002 0.0001 0.0023(0.0002) (0.0001) (0.0019)

Age Squared 0.0000 * 0.0000 * -0.0001 *(0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0000)

Time Employed 0.0000 0.0000 -0.0005(0.0000) (0.0000) (0.0004)

Salary -0.0005 ** -0.0002 ** -0.0062 **(0.0001) (0.0000) (0.0013)

Prior Indicator (1) 0.0103 ** 0.0046 ** 0.2014 **(0.0004) (0.0002) (0.0073)

Compensation Status (1) -0.0002 -0.0001 -0.0017 (0.0010) (0.0004) (0.0116)Division 1 (1) 0.0037 * 0.0014 * 0.0428 *

(0.0017) (0.0006) (0.0198)Division 2 (1) 0.0093 ** 0.0035 ** 0.1012 **

(0.0018) (0.0007) (0.0189)Other Division (1) 0.0048 * 0.0020 0.0729

(0.0024) (0.0011) (0.0536)One Year Co Stock Return 0.0140 ** 0.0054 ** 0.1595 **

(0.0010) (0.0004) (0.0105)(1) dF/dx is for a discrete change of the dummy variable from 0 to 1

Dependent Variables: Y=% Invested in Company Stock

37

Page 38: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table V. Asset Allocations and Adjusted Asset Allocations This table presents the allocations and adjusted asset allocations for investors who hold company stock and invest in either two or three additional assets. The adjusted allocations reflect the percentage of the non-company stock holdings the asset class represents.

Invest in All Assets 3,317 obs

Investment Vehicle Mean AllocationMedian Allocation

Mean Adjusted Allocation

Median Adjusted Allocation

Company Stock 33% 25%Equity Income Fund 23% 25% 34% 33%

S&P 500 Index Fund 24% 25% 36% 33%GIC 20% 20% 31% 33%

Invest in Company Stock, Equity Income and S&P 500 Index Fund 4,203 obs

Investment Vehicle Mean AllocationMedian Allocation

Mean Adjusted Allocation

Median Adjusted Allocation

Company Stock 38% 35%Equity Income Fund 30% 30% 48% 50%S&P 500 Index Fund 32% 30% 52% 50%GICInvest in Company Stock, Equity Income and GIC Fund 379 obs

Investment Vehicle Mean AllocationMedian Allocation

Mean Adjusted Allocation

Median Adjusted Allocation

Company Stock 42% 50%Equity Income Fund 28% 25% 49% 50%S&P 500 Index Fund GIC 30% 25% 51% 50%Invest in Company Stock, S&P 500 Index and GIC Fund 626 obs

Investment Vehicle Mean AllocationMedian Allocation

Mean Adjusted Allocation

Median Adjusted Allocation

Company Stock 44% 50%Equity Income FundS&P 500 Index Fund 29% 25% 51% 50%GIC 28% 25% 49% 50%

38

Page 39: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Table VI. Probit Regression–1/n Heuristic The table presents the marginal effects calculated from the results of a probit regression. The dependent variables equals 1 if the participant follows the 1/n rule or the adjusted 1/n rule. “Male” is a dummy variable equal to one if the participant is male, zero otherwise. “Salary” is the annual 1997 salary (unit: ten thousand dollars). “Age” is the age of the participant at the time the allocation decision is made (unit: years). “Time Employed” equals the time the participant has been employed at the time the allocation decision is made (unit: years). “Compensation Status” is a dummy variable that equals one if the individual by law is considered highly compensated, otherwise it equals zero. “Prior Indicator” equals one for individuals who were employed prior to the current plan administration and made no changes in their contribution allocations during the current plan administrator’s tenure. “Division #” is a dummy variable that equals one if the participant is in the division. The Corporate Division is the omitted dummy. Robust standard errors, reported in parentheses, are adjusted for heteroskedacity. The psuedo R-squared is the log-likelihood value on a scale from zero to one, where zero corresponds to the constant-only model and one corresponds to perfect prediction (a log-likelihood of zero). ** (*) indicates significance at the 1% (5%) level.

Marginal Effects (dF/dx)Independent

Variables (2) (2)

Male (1) -0.0016 0.0003(0.0026) (0.0030)

Age 0.0012 0.0015(0.0009) (0.0011)

Age Squared 0.0000 0.0000(0.0000) (0.0000)

Time Employed -0.0013 ** -0.0017 **(0.0002) (0.0002)

Salary -0.0049 ** -0.0047 **(0.0008) (0.0009)

Prior Indicator (1) -0.0223 ** -0.0282 **(0.0022) (0.0026)

Compensation Status (1) 0.0140 0.0144 (0.0080) (0.0086)

Division 1 (1) 0.0142 0.0147(0.0141) (0.0154)

Division 2 (1) 0.0054 0.0059(0.0130) (0.0145)

Other (1) Dropped -0.0066(0.0381)

Number of Observations 28,738 28,809Psuedo R-squared 0.0237 0.0196(1) dF/dx is for a discrete change of the dummy variable from 0 to 1

Unadjusted 1/n Adjusted 1/n

39

Page 40: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Figure 1. Conditional Adjusted and Unadjusted Kernel Densities These graphs represent the adjusted and unadjusted kernel densities of participants that invest in three to four funds. Panel A. Sample that Invests in All Four Funds

Invest in All Funds-3,317 observations Company Stock

0 .5 1

0

2

4

6

Equity Inc Adj. Equity Inc

0 .5 10

5

10

15

Allocation Percentage

S&P 500 Adj. S&P 500

0 .5 10

5

10

15

20

Allocation Percentage

GIC Adj. GIC

0 .5 10

2

4

6

8

Panel B. Sample that Invests in Company Stock, Equity Income Fund and GIC fund

Invest in Three Funds- 379 ObservationsCompany Stock

0 .5 1

0

1

2

3

Equity Inc Adj. Equity Inc

0 .5 10

5

10

Allocation Percentage

GIC Adj. GIC

0 .5 10

5

10

40

Page 41: Inefficient Choices in 401(k) Plans: Evidence from ...media.terry.uga.edu/documents/finance/1111.pdf · mental accounting literature, participants demonstrate a tendency to treat

Figure 1. (Continued) Conditional Adjusted and Unadjusted Kernel Densities These graphs represent the adjusted and unadjusted kernel densities of participants that invest in three to four funds.

Invest in Three Funds-4,203 observationsCompany Stock

0 .5 1

0

1

2

3

Allocation Percentage

Equity Inc Adj. Equity Inc

0 .5 10

2

4

6

8

Allocation Percentage

S&P 500 Adj. S&P 500

0 .5 10

2

4

6

Panel C. Sample that Invests in Company Stock, Equity Income Fund and S&P 500 Index Fund Panel D. Sample that Invests in Company Stock, S&P 500 Index Fund and GIC Fund

Invest in Three Funds-626 Observations Company Stock

Allocation Percentage0 .5 1

0

1

2

3

4

S&P 500 Adj. S&P 500

0 .5 10

5

10

15

Allocation Percentage

GIC Adj. GIC

0 .5 10

5

10

15

41