14
This article was downloaded by: [Chinese University of Hong Kong] On: 21 December 2014, At: 16:32 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cedp20 Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning Regina Pauli a , Changiz Mohiyeddini a , Diane Bray a , Fran Michie a & Becky Street a a Roehampton University , UK Published online: 29 Nov 2007. To cite this article: Regina Pauli , Changiz Mohiyeddini , Diane Bray , Fran Michie & Becky Street (2008) Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning, Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 28:1, 47-58, DOI: 10.1080/01443410701413746 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410701413746 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

  • Upload
    becky

  • View
    212

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

This article was downloaded by: [Chinese University of Hong Kong]On: 21 December 2014, At: 16:32Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

Educational Psychology: AnInternational Journal of ExperimentalEducational PsychologyPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/cedp20

Individual differences in negative groupwork experiences in collaborativestudent learningRegina Pauli a , Changiz Mohiyeddini a , Diane Bray a , Fran Michiea & Becky Street aa Roehampton University , UKPublished online: 29 Nov 2007.

To cite this article: Regina Pauli , Changiz Mohiyeddini , Diane Bray , Fran Michie & Becky Street(2008) Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning,Educational Psychology: An International Journal of Experimental Educational Psychology, 28:1,47-58, DOI: 10.1080/01443410701413746

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01443410701413746

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to orarising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &

Page 2: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 3: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Educational PsychologyVol. 28, No. 1, January 2008, 47–58

ISSN 0144-3410 print/ISSN 1469-5820 online© 2008 Taylor & FrancisDOI: 10.1080/01443410701413746http://www.informaworld.com

Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Regina Pauli*, Changiz Mohiyeddini, Diane Bray, Fran Michie and Becky Street

Roehampton University, UKTaylor and FrancisCEDP_A_241259.sgm

10.1080/01443410701413746Educational Psychology0144-3410 (print)/1469-5820 (online)Original Article2007Taylor & Francis2700000002007Dr. [email protected] This paper reports the development of the Negative Group Work Experiences questionnaire(NGWE), an assessment tool for measuring negative experiences of group work. Study 1involved two samples of undergraduate psychology students (second-year sample n = 425;first-year sample n = 443), who completed research modules incorporating substantialelements of assessed group work. Participants completed a 39-item inventory designed tomeasure their negative experiences of group work as part of their general module evaluation.Exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis of the second-year sample indicated fourinterpretable and reliable factors: lack of group commitment, group fractionation, taskdisorganisation, and storming. Confirmatory factor analysis of the first-year data set confirmedand cross-validated the factor structure of the second-year sample. Study 2 (n > 254)confirmed the criterion validity of the NGWE. The NGWE is proposed as a useful tool forevaluating group processes, especially in large group teaching which involves collaborativegroup work.

Introduction

Small group work is an effective and widely used teaching and learning method in higher educa-tion. Co-operative learning in small groups has educational and social benefits for students, andalso has advantages in terms of sharing limited resources more effectively. Boud, Cohen, andSampson (1999) and Gupta (2004), among others, point out the advantages of peer learning inachieving generic learning outcomes, including the development of team skills, communicationskills, critical reflection skills, and self-directed learning skills—all of which combine into life-long learning skills. Cartney and Rouse (2006) have argued that small group work may serve toimprove the social and academic integration of students, and thus improve retention. It has alsobeen suggested that team-based teaching and learning methods allow for increased complexity inthe learning environment as well as in the tasks that can be assigned (Mello, 1993), thus increas-ing students’ preparedness for the complex environments they are expected to function in oncompletion of their degrees. Group work can also be seen to have advantages in terms of manag-ing resources: teaching resources can be deployed more effectively when they are shared ingroups, especially large groups. In this sense, student-led work in small groups may be a solutionto the decline in small group teaching (Livingstone & Lynch, 2000).

However, despite all the potentially beneficial effects of collaborative learning in highereducation, there is a persistent body of literature on negative perceptions of small group work.Many students and lecturers remain sceptical as to the value of group work. In recent years a smallbody of literature has examined students’ perceptions of collaborative learning (Gatfield, 1999;

*Corresponding author. Email: [email protected].

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 4: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

48 R. Pauli et al.

Walker, 2001; White, Lloyd, Kennedy, & Stewart, 2005). Frequent objections to group workinclude the potential for conflict between group members, and the possibility of individual groupmembers not doing their share of the work (Mello, 1993). Students frequently fear that assessedgroup work may negatively impact on their overall grades, whereas instructors tend to object ongrounds of difficulty in managing the group process. There is evidence that instructors appear toget the blame if group work goes wrong, but little acknowledgement if it is successful (Feichtner& Davies, 1985).

Negative perceptions of group work appear to be mediated by cognitive and psychologicalfactors such as metacognitive awareness and social anxiety (Cantwell & Andrews, 2002), andalso by cultural factors (collectivist vs. individualist; Gatfield, 1999). Previous experience ofworking in groups, the amount of instruction (Colbeck, Campbell, & Bjorklund, 2000), themethod of group assessment (Lejk, Wyvill, & Farrow, 1996), and the method of group formation(Daly & Worrell, 1993; Feichtner & Davis, 1985) all appear to play a role in how successful groupwork can be in generating positive learning experiences for students.

Livingstone and Lynch (2000) argue that the negative perceptions of group work voiced bystudents and academics may be due largely to what they call “myths” about group work relatingto the tension between social learning and individualistic learning. However, Livingstone andLynch (2000) also point out that poorly designed and executed group work assignments mayserve to reinforce these myths. Whether they are real or just myths, a number of phenomena thatcould negatively impact on a group’s ability to perform to maximum benefit for all groupmembers have been identified by previous authors.

Sources of problems in group work

Poor group work outcomes may be due to a variety of issues arising from the context of plannedcollaborative work. Tutors play a major role in designing group work and associated assessment,and while they clearly aim to minimise potential problems a number of potential pitfalls mayremain even in well designed group projects. These are related to motivational, interactional, andlogistical issues (Boud et al., 1999; Salomon & Globerson, 1989; Slavin, 1996) and includeunmotivated or incompetent peers, group process deficiencies (e.g., lack of communication,bullying, or dysfunctional group interaction), and logistical problems such as managing workload(Feichtner & Davis, 1985).

Motivational difficulties. Motivational issues may arise when the group work task lacks a trulyinteractional focus. Although groups can succeed even if some group members fail to participate(fully) in the group task, conflict can arise if individual members fail to engage with the task. Thishas been referred to most commonly as “social loafing” (Karau & Williams, 1993), but also as“free-loading” (Daly & Worrell, 1993) or “free-riding” (Salomon & Globerson, 1989), and mayarise when one or more students fail to contribute to the group effort because they assume thework will be done by more talented or more motivated group members.

An associated phenomenon is the “sucker” phenomenon (Salomon & Globerson, 1989),whereby one or two group members take on the responsibility for doing all of the group’s work,because other group members are either unwilling or unable to contribute. The reverse effect maymanifest (avoidance of the sucker effect): motivated and competent students may withdraw fromthe group effort because they perceive others as not contributing to the same level as themselves.

Status differential among group members may lead to higher status group members dominat-ing group activity and low status members interacting and influencing group processes less. Thismay lead to learned helplessness and self-perceived incompetence in low status group membersas a result of their efforts being rejected by the higher status members. Under these conditions,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 5: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Educational Psychology 49

optimal learning cannot be achieved. Underperformance in groups may also result from a group-negotiated least effort approach, particularly to undesirable group tasks, or from a diffusion ofresponsibility, particularly in larger teams (Salomon & Globerson, 1989).

Interactional difficulties. Interactional issues that interfere with successful engagement with agroup task are also well documented. Mello (1993) argues that most groups can manage interper-sonal conflicts on their own, but notes that tutors are responsible if this is not the case.

Interpersonal conflict can be seen as a normal stage in the development of groups. “Storming”has been conceptualised as a stage in group development which is characterised by high individ-ual and group needs and low task focus. Interactional difficulties are particularly apparent in thisstage: personality clashes may become apparent and the group may argue about how to operate(Tuckman, 1965). If this stage of group development cannot be successfully resolved, the groupmay remain dysfunctional and unable to focus effort on the group task or, at worst, unable tocomplete the group task (Gibbs, 1995).

Interactional difficulties have also been examined in relation to differences in the approachtaken to conflict resolution. Desivilya and Eizen (2005), for example, discuss conflict manage-ment patterns which vary along two dimensions: engagement versus avoidance, and constructiveversus destructive. Negative group work experiences could arise in relation to the destructive endof the dimension, resulting either in avoidance (destructive/avoiding) or dominance (destructive/engaging).

Logistical difficulties. A further source of potential group problems is logistical in nature. Forexample, pressure of work may make a group unable to perform to its best ability (Livingstone &Lynch, 2000), and it is therefore important to integrate group work in such a way that studentshave the opportunity to get to know each other and establish ways of working with each otherbefore being expected to engage in assessed tasks (Gibbs, 1995).

Feichtner and Davis (1985) have reviewed a number of logistical issues that may impact onthe success of group work, both from the instructor’s and the student’s points of view. Group sizeand composition, for example, can impact on whether a group has the necessary resources to besuccessful. If groups are too large, or if they are expected to meet up outside class time, there maybe difficulty in co-ordinating group members’ efforts. Arranging times to meet can be a majorproblem for groups (Edgerton & McKechnie, 2002), particularly for mature students and thosewith external commitments.

Measuring problems in group work

It is clear that there is potential for many things to go wrong in group work, even when instructorstake care to design the group activity carefully. There has, however, been little work to dateinvolving the measurement of negative group work experiences in a systematic way. Most authorsdiscussing negative perceptions of group work present either anecdotal evidence or case studies(Feichtner & Davis, 1985; Salomon & Globerson, 1989), or focus specifically on the measure-ment of attitudes towards group work and satisfaction with assessment methods (Cantwell &Andrews, 2002; Gatfield, 1999; Walker, 2001; White et al., 2005)—this work is specificallyconcerned with students’ feelings and attitudes towards group work. One concern is that thisapproach may not represent the full spectrum of possible negative experiences. It is also possiblethat certain types of negative experience are more common than others, and that negative experi-ences vary in severity and impact on group performance. It could be that negative myths aboutgroup work are perpetuated without any real foundation in students’ experience of group work.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 6: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

50 R. Pauli et al.

This paper reports on the development of a questionnaire designed to measure problemsoccurring in student group work. As instructors using group work in the context of large groupteaching, where informal feedback from groups or evaluation of group work processes on thebasis of observation is not always possible, we have felt a need for a means of measuring negativegroup work experience for the purpose of evaluating our practice. While our primary motivationwas to develop an instrument to assess the extent to which problems occur in groups of students(evaluation of group work processes), this type of questionnaire also enables researchers to inves-tigate not only antecedents of dysfunctionality in groups but also the effects any problems mayhave on task performance, individual student performance, and transferable skill learning ingeneral. Furthermore, it may lend empirical support to some of the negative group work phenom-ena identified in the literature.

The questionnaire was constructed from a review of the literature, students’ qualitativeaccounts of group work experience, and our own experience as facilitators of group work in theacademic context. The assumption was that negative experiences of group work would be multi-dimensional, given the logistical, motivational, and interactional perspectives discussed in theliterature. It could be argued that different types of negative experience may cluster in such a wayas to identify different types of group dysfunctionality.

Method

Development of the questionnaire

Items for the questionnaire were developed on the basis of a qualitative analysis of written reflec-tions on group work experience produced by students as part of their module assessment, a reviewof the relevant literature, and the authors’ personal reflections on problems arising in the facilita-tion of small group work. Initially, 32 items, ranging from relatively benign logistical problems(e.g., “Other group members never being available to meet”) to more serious interactional prob-lems (e.g. “Arguments e.g. shouting in the group”), were compiled. These were supplemented byseven items describing positive group work behaviour (e.g. “Working together in a problem-focused manner”). The positive items were added as a means of addressing response bias, butwere not included in the analyses reported in this paper.

The resulting 39-item scale utilised a four-point response scale (0 = “No experience of this inmy group”, 1 = “Occasional experience of this in my group”, 2 = “Frequent experience of this inmy group”, and 3 = “Constant experience of this in my group”).

Participants

Study 1. Six cohorts of second-year undergraduate psychology students from two differentresearch methods modules, Questionnaire and Survey Design (n = 173) and QuantitativeResearch Methods (n = 252), participated in the questionnaire development phase of thisresearch. The 425 participants were predominantly female (89%), which is roughly representativeof the gender distribution observed in psychology undergraduate students at this university in thesouth-east of England. The mean age was 23.6 years (SD = 7.08). All participants worked ingroups of three to six, with five being the norm, to complete part of their coursework on themodule.

The NGWE questionnaire was also administered to an independent sample of first-yearstudents (n = 543) as part of a larger research project investigating the impact of electronicresources on teaching and learning in undergraduate psychology students. In this sample 87.4%were female and 12.3% male. The mean age was 22.8 (SD = 6.75). Group work was organised inthe same way as in the second-year sample.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 7: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Educational Psychology 51

Study 2. A number of additional questions were included in the second-year questionnaire forvalidation purposes. These concerned the current status of the group, how problems wereresolved in the group, and the extent to which problems in the group had been resolved success-fully. Data from these criterion validity items were available from 254 participants from thesecond-year sample; these were the participants who had reported group work problems.

Procedure

All groups completed the NGWE during the final class of their module, after submitting theirassignments but prior to receiving feedback on their group or individual work. Participants wereinstructed to complete the questionnaire in relation to the group in which they had worked duringthe module. If participants had changed groups during the module, they were asked to respond tothe questionnaire in relation to the group which they had first been allocated to. The questionnairewas distributed as part of the general module evaluation questionnaire. Participation was volun-tary and the response rate varied between 30% and 97% across all cohorts, with lower participa-tion evident in larger cohorts.

Results: study 1

Factorial structure of the NGWE: principal components analysis

Exploratory principal components analysis1 with direct oblimin rotation was conducted on thenegative group experience items for the second-year sample. Missing values were substitutedwith means. The scree plot indicated a four-factor solution (with three-factor or five-factor solu-tions also possible). The four-factor solution was adopted as it represented the clearest, mostinterpretable solution. The four components accounted cumulatively for 58.89% of the variance.Of the original 32 items, 11 were complex, and 10 of these were eliminated from further analysison grounds of conceptual clarity.

Cross-validation of factor structure

A two-step procedure was conducted to test the factorial structure of the NGWE (Anderson &Gerbing, 1988). In the first step, confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) was used to test the overallmodel in the second-year sample. In order to establish its metric, the factor loading for the firstitem on each latent variable was constrained to 1.0. In the second step, invariance analysis usingmulti-sample analysis was applied to test the stability of the model from the first step, only thistime the first step had been applied to the first-year sample.

Confirmatory factor analysis

The most common statistical test for the assessment of model fit in confirmatory factor analysisis the chi-square goodness of fit test (χ2). This test estimates discrepancies between the observedcovariance matrices and those implied by the model. A lower chi-square value indicates better fit.The chi-square statistic assesses the absolute fit of the model to the data. However, it is sensitiveto sample size and often inflates Type 1 error (Bollen, 1989; Cohen, 1988). Therefore, it is neces-sary to use additional indices to evaluate model fit. The comparative fit index (CFI) and the root-mean-square error of approximation (RMSEA) indicate the closeness of fit. Cut-off values closeto .95 for the CFI and close to .08 for the RMSEA demonstrate adequate fit of the model (Browne& Cudeck, 1993; Hu & Bentler, 1999).

The relative CFI, the noncentrality index (RNI), and the nonnormed fit index (NNFI) areincremental fit indices (Bentler & Bonett, 1980; McDonald & Marsh, 1990). They were selected

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 8: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

52 R. Pauli et al.

on the basis of performance in simulation studies (Hu & Bentler, 1999; Marsh, Balla, & Hau,1996). Minimally acceptable fit is indicated by threshold CFI, RNI, and NNFI values of .90(Bentler & Bonett, 1980; McDonald & Marsh, 1990). Values close to .95 indicate a good modelfit (Hu & Bentler, 1999).

Another minimum sample discrepancy function, the χ2/df ratio, has been suggested as auseful criterion. Bollen and Long (1993) suggest χ2/df not above two to five times the degrees offreedom. Akaike’s information criterion (AIC) and ∆χ2 have also been reported as appropriatefor the purposes of model comparison (Akaike, 1987). In order to test both the configural andmetric invariance across the datasets, models were assessed for fit using ∆χ2, AIC, NNFI, CFI,and RMSEA.

With respect to the data from the second-year sample, the hypothesised four-factor measure-ment model represented a good fit and can be accepted (see Table 1). Table 2 shows the latentfactor loadings. The four factors obtained in the confirmatory factor analysis were clearly inter-pretable.

The first component comprises six items relating to logistical difficulties in the group to dowith meeting up as a group and to group member failure to complete work as agreed. Theresulting subscale can therefore be labelled “lack of group commitment” and is characterised bylack of communication within the group and individuals’ lack of engagement with the grouptask.

The second component combines five items which are specifically concerned with group fail-ure to address the task appropriately. In comparison to the first component there is no indicationof individuals’ lack of commitment to the group, although issues around effective communicationin the group are implicated. This subscale is primarily characterised as “task disorganisation”.

The third component has five items which all represent an aspect of difficulty between indi-vidual group members on a personal level. This subscale measures group processes relating tostorming, to borrow from Tuckman’s (1965) conceptualisation of group formation.

The last component contains six items indicating severe dysfunctionality in the group, andincludes interactional, communicative, and organisational elements. This appears to represent asubscale measuring fractionation of the group.

Intercorrelations of factors

Table 3 shows the correlations between latent factors. It can be seen that all four factors havemoderate to high intercorrelations. The highest correlations were between interactional factors(storming and fractionated groups) and motivational/logistical factors (lack of commitment andtask disorganisation), respectively. Due to these high intercorrelations we tested two furthermodels: a three-factor model (in which the third and fourth factors were pooled) and a two-factormodel (in which the first two factors were additionally pooled). The overall fit of the four-factormodel (Table 1) is clearly better than the three-factor model (∆χ2 = 67.29, ∆df = 3, p < .05) or thetwo-factor model (∆χ2 = 477.17, ∆df = 6, p < .001).

Table 1. Confirmatory factor analysis.

Model χ2 df pRMSEA(90% CI)

p (RMSEA < 0.05) AIC CFI NNFI RFI χ2/df

Four factors 575.58 203 .00 .065 (.058–.071) .00 665.06 .97 .96 .95 2.83Three factors 642.87 206 .00 .071 (.064–.077) .00 736.87 .90 .89 .84 3.12Two factors 1052.75 209 .00 .097 (.092–.010) .00 1140.75 .81 .79 .75 5.03

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 9: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Educational Psychology 53

Table 2. Latent factor loadings of NGWE items.

Factor 1 Factor 2 Factor 3 Factor 4

Lack of group commitment

A member of your group not turning up to group meetings arranged outside timetabled sessions without explanation

.78

A group member always making excuses for not making group meetings or completing work

.78

Group member not completing their allocated work .85Difficulty in contacting a group member .70Other group members not doing their share by the agreed deadline

.85

Other group members never being available to meet .70

Task disorganisation

Difficulty finalising a piece of work .75Difficulty dividing up the work for the group task fairly .58Difficulty in keeping the whole group focused on the task .75Always doing things late .67Group problems seem to arise as deadline stress looms .69

Storming group

Arguments (including, e.g., shouting) in the group .71Other members falling out with each other .74Gossiping in the group about another group member behind their back

.70

Other group members being inflexible when their work needs to be changed

.59

You falling out with another group member .53

Fractionated group

You feeling isolated or excluded by your group .55Difficulty in deciding roles (e.g., who leads) .56Development of factions in the group .66Exclusion of another group member .65Members of your group turning on you or picking on you .62Group members not talking to each other over a sustained period

.65

Table 3. Factor correlation matrices for latent factors.

Factor 1: Lack of group commitment

Factor 2: Task disorganisation

Factor 3: Storming group

Factor 2: Task disorganisation .78Factor 3: Storming group .58 .68Factor 4: Fractionated group .47 .65 .83

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 10: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

54 R. Pauli et al.

Cross-validation of factor structure: invariance analysis

The invariance of the structure of the NGWE was tested using a multi-step procedure (Bollen,1989; Motl et al., 2000). Two nested models were tested. These involved invariance in factorloadings (i.e., equality of coefficients linking the items with latent variables; Model 1) and invari-ance in overall structure (i.e., same pattern of fixed, freed, and constrained factor loadings andfactor variances-covariances; Model 2). The invariance was evaluated by a χ2 difference test,RMSEA with 90% CI, CFI, and NNFI. Table 4 shows the results of the invariance analysis in thefirst-year sample.

Model comparison shows (χ2diff = 21.08, df = 10, n.s.) that Model 2 fits the data well and

can be accepted. The structure of the NGWE is invariant across the first-year and second-yearsamples.

Psychometric properties of the NGWE and its subscales

Table 5 shows means, standard deviations, and Cronbach’s alphas for both samples. All subscalesshowed good reliability in both samples. Mean scores are relatively low, indicating low levels ofnegative experiences, particularly on the storming and fractionated group subscales. Students intheir second year yield lower means across all subscales than students in their first year. Althoughthere is evidence that the whole range of all scales was used, the score distributions on allsubscales and the total scale are positively skewed in both samples.

Results: study 2

Validity of the NGWE

Gender and age variables were examined in relation to NGWE scores. There were no genderdifferences in this sample (t[284] < 1.1, n.s.). Age was correlated with lack of commitment(r[300] = .13, p = .026) and task disorganisation (r[295] = .14, p = .014), but not with stormingor fractionation (r[291] < .1, n.s.). The criterion validity of the NGWE was therefore examined

Table 4. Structural equation model analyses testing for invariance across the samples.

Model χ2 df RMSEA (90% CI) CFI NNFI

Model 1 1498.15 424 .072 (.068–.076) .96 .96Model 2 1519.23 434 .072 (.068–.072) .96 .96

Table 5. Means, standard deviations and Cronbach’s alpha for subscales of the NGWE questionnaire by year group.

nSecond year Mean (SD)

(n = 425) Alpha

First year Mean (SD)

(n = 543) Alpha

Lack of commitment 6 .88 (.86) .90 1.13 (.86) .90Task disorganisation 5 .89 (.74) .82 1.04 (.71) .78Storming group 5 .43 (.59) .77 .70 (.70) .78Fractionated group 6 .32 (.49) .78 .52 (.57) .78Total NGWE 22 .63 (.55) .92 .88 (.59) .92

Note: n = number of retained items in each subscale.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 11: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Educational Psychology 55

for the whole sample in relation to group outcome, how problems were dealt with, and, in the caseof group problems, the success of intervention.

Group outcome was measured by asking participants (n = 346) to rate group status duringevaluation on a three-point scale. Participants indicated whether their group was working togetherwith minor problems, working together with major problems, or had broken up. All subscales andthe total NGWE correlated highly with this measure (p < .0005). Higher levels of negative groupexperiences were associated with less favourable group outcomes.

Participants who reported experiencing group problems (n = 254) also indicated how groupproblems had been addressed. A measure assessing levels of intervention (problems notaddressed at all, problems resolved within the group, or problems resolved with tutor interven-tion) revealed small but significant negative correlations with the extent of negative group workexperiences reported, across all subscales and the total score, indicating that more negative expe-riences were reported when problems were not addressed, compared with internal group resolu-tion and tutor-mediated resolution.

Finally, participants who had experienced group problems (n = 264) were asked to rate theextent to which they felt the problems had been resolved successfully. High positive correlationswith this measure indicate that the higher the level of negative group experiences reported, theless successful attempts at problem resolution were perceived to be. Table 6 shows Spearman’scorrelations between the criterion questions and NGWE scores.

Discussion

The principal aim of this research was to develop and validate a research and evaluation instru-ment to measure negative group work experiences. Confirmatory factor analysis revealed fourinterpretable components. Negative group experiences were thus captured along four dimensions:two relating to motivational/logistical group problems (lack of group commitment and task disor-ganisation) and two to interactional group problems (storming and group fractionation).

The lack of commitment factor includes items that relate to both motivational (failure tocomplete work) and logistical problems (inability to meet up as a group), indicating that althoughfailure to contribute to the group task may have different causes these are not differentiated interms of negative group work experiences. Social loafing (Daly & Worrell, 1993; Karau & Will-iams, 1993) and genuine difficulties in organising group work (Edgerton & McKechnie, 2002)are not differentiated as antecedents of negative group work experience. The indication is thatboth are interpreted as unwillingness to engage with the group.

In comparison, the second factor, task disorganisation, appears to have a purely motivationalbasis. The focus seems to be inability to address the task appropriately. Group conflict does not

Table 6. Spearman’s rho correlations between NGWE scores and group outcome, problem resolution, and perceived success of problem resolution.

Group outcome (n = 346)

Level of intervention in resolving

problems (n = 254)

Perceived success in resolving

problems (n = 264)

Lack of group commitment .46*** −.16* .45***Task disorganisation .45*** −.22*** .51***Storming group .36*** −.21** .43***Fractionated group .36*** −.23*** .39***

*p < .05; **p < .01, ***p < .001

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 12: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

56 R. Pauli et al.

necessarily play a major causative role. The problem is entirely associated with disorganisationwith respect to the group task. It could be argued that this is the type of problem experiencedby groups who never move beyond Tuckman’s (1965) forming stage of group development.Problems may be due to lack of leadership in the group and/or ineffective communication skills.The outcome is a lack of task focus, leading to pressure on the group when the group task needsto be delivered. This is a relatively common source of negative group experiences.

The group fractionation factor comprises items indicating severe interactional and communi-cative difficulties in the group. Furthermore, there is some indication that group members may beunable to address the issues arising in the group. This factor can be interpreted in relation toDesivilya and Eizen’s (2005) avoiding/destructive type of group management. There is a generallack of communication, leading to factions and/or isolation of individual group members, andresulting in potentially destructive tendencies which manifest themselves in negative group expe-riences. Such difficulties with communication in the group may well be related to previous groupwork experience and the extent to which group members know each other prior to being asked towork in a group. However, it is difficult to see how groups reporting these types of problemscould work effectively on a group task.

In comparison, the final factor, storming, is characterised by communication between groupmembers—albeit dysfunctional communication. This factor encompasses items indicating vola-tile group interaction which may be destructive (engaging/destructive in Desivilya and Eizen’s2005 terms). Individual needs take precedence over the group task, conceptualised by Tuckman(1965) as the storming stage of group development. Whether this represents intrinsic dysfunction-ality, or whether this is a normal stage of group development as conceptualised by Tuckman(1965), remains in question. However it is clear that this kind of dysfunctionality has an impacton task performance, which cannot be optimal unless conflict can be resolved appropriately.

Confirmatory factor analysis demonstrated that the four-factor model yielded the best fit.While it is possible to conceptualise a theoretical two-factor model based on Desivilya & Eizen(2005), or a three-factor model based on the distinction between interactional, motivational, andlogistical difficulties (Slavin, 1996), our data clearly demonstrate that these are not viable alter-natives. Furthermore, the structure of the four-factor model was invariant in multi-sample analy-sis, providing further evidence for the stability of this model. In summary, there is good evidencein our data for the internal validity of the claim that negative group experiences can be conceptu-alised in terms of four factors. It must be noted, however, that the four factors show high inter-correlations—indicating that the factors may not be independent. Further research is required toexamine how the negative group experiences captured by the four factors interact.

The external validity of the four factors was examined in relation to group outcome, level ofintervention in group problems, and perceived success in resolving group problems. The patternof observed correlations was as expected. Higher levels of negative experiences were associatedwith worse group outcomes and less successful perceived conflict resolution, particularly in thetwo motivational/logistical subscales. Level of intervention was inversely related to negativegroup experiences, indicating that high levels of negative experience were associated with lowerlevels of intervention to address group problems. Thus, there is some evidence for the criterionvalidity of the scale; however, further research needs to address the convergent and divergentvalidity of the scales in relation to cultural, social, and cognitive factors (Cantwell & Andrews,2002), as well as personality and emotional variables (Mohiyeddini, 2001). Criterion validitycould usefully be examined further in relation to group performance variables.

A further issue arises with respect to gender. No gender differences were found in this study.However, the sample in this study was heavily biased towards female participants, which isentirely consistent with the population from which the samples in this study were drawn. Thismeans that the conceptualisation of negative group work experiences arising from this study may

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 13: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

Educational Psychology 57

only be representative of females working in predominantly female groups. Further studies needto address the question of whether the factor structure reported here is stable across gender-balanced groups and predominantly male groups.

Scores on the four subscales were relatively low and positively skewed in this sample, espe-cially on the two interactional subscales. This might be expected as the constructs measured areby nature relatively rare in well-constructed group work environments. Tutors implementinggroup work in higher education are usually very aware of potential difficulties and endeavour todesign group tasks and associated assessment in a manner that minimises potential problems. Inaddition, students’ previous experience with group work could be expected to reduce problemson subsequent occasions. In this study, the means of all scales were lower in the second-yearsample than in the first-year sample, indicating good sensitivity of the scales to subtle differencesbased on learning processes.

The NGWE is thus presented as an empirically-derived instrument demonstrating factorialstability, internal validity, and external validity, and capable of measuring negative group workexperiences in four dimensions. It is a useful tool for monitoring and evaluating groupprocesses in the context of large group teaching. While further validation is desirable, it isanticipated that the NGWE could be used to investigate the impact of negative experiences insmall group work on individual student grades. It could also be used to investigate the anteced-ents of negative experiences—such as cognitive, psychological, and cultural factors (Cantwell& Andrews, 2002; Gatfield, 1999)—and to evaluate interventions/training in the context ofgroup work.

Note

1. The factor loading matrix for the exploratory principal components analysis is available from the firstauthor.

References

Akaike, H. (1987). Factor analysis and AIC. Psychometrika, 52, 317–332.Anderson, J.C., & Gerbing, D.W. (1988). Structural equation modeling in practice: A review and recom-

mended two-step approach. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 411–423.Bentler, P.M., & Bonett, D.G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance

structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588–606.Bollen, K.A. (1989). Structural equations with latent variables. New York: Wiley.Bollen, K.A. & Long, J.S. (Eds.) (1993). Testing structural equation models. Newbury Park: Sage Publi-

cations.Boud, D., Cohen, R., & Sampson, J. (1999). Peer learning and assessment. Assessment and Evaluation in

Higher Education, 24, 413–426.Browne, M.W., & Cudeck, R. (1993). Alternative ways of assessing model fit. In K.A. Bollen & J.S. Long

(Eds.), Testing structural equation models (pp. 136–162). Newbury Park, CA: Sage.Cantwell, R.H., & Andrews, B. (2002). Cognitive and psychological factors underlying secondary

students’ feelings towards group work. Educational Psychology, 22, 75–91.Cartney, P. & Rouse, A. (2006). The emotional impact of learning in small groups: Highlighting the impact

on student progression and retention. Teaching in Higher Education, 11, 79–91.Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Colbeck, C.L., Campbell, S.E., & Bjorklund, S.A. (2000). Grouping in the dark: What college students

learn from group projects. The Journal of Higher Education, 71, 60–83.Daly, J.P., & Worrell, D.L. (1993). Structuring group projects as miniature organizations. Journal of

Management Education, 17, 236–242.Desivilya, H.S., & Eizen, D. (2005). Conflict management in work teams: The role of social self-efficacy

and group identification. International Journal of Conflict Management, 16, 183–208.Edgerton, E., & McKechnie, J. (2002). Students’ view of group-based work and the issue of peer assess-

ment. Psychology Teaching and Learning, 2, 76–81.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014

Page 14: Individual differences in negative group work experiences in collaborative student learning

58 R. Pauli et al.

Feichtner, S.M., & Davies, E.A. (1985). Why some groups fail: A survey of students’ experiences withlearning groups. The Organizational Behaviour Review, 9, 58–73.

Gatfield, T. (1999). Examining student satisfaction with group projects and peer assessment. Assessmentand Evaluation in Higher Education, 24, 365–377.

Gibbs, G. (1995). Learning in teams: A tutor guide. Oxford, UK: The Oxford Centre for Staff Development.Gupta, M.L. (2004). Enhancing student performance through cooperative learning in physical sciences.

Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 29, 63–73.Hu, L., & Bentler, M. (1999). Cut-off criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional

criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 6, 1–55.Karau, S.J., & Williams, K.D. (1993). Social loafing: A meta-analytic review and theoretical integration.

Journal of Personality and Individual Differences, 65, 681–706.Lejk, M., Wyvill, M., & Farrow, S. (1996). A survey of methods deriving individual grades from group

assessments. Assessment and Evaluation in Higher Education, 21, 267–280.Livingstone, D., & Lynch, K. (2000). Group project work and student-centred active learning: Two differ-

ent experiences. Studies in Higher Education, 25, 325–345.Marsh, H.W., Balla, J.R., & Hau, K. (1996). An evaluation of incremental fit indices: A clarification of

mathematical and empirical properties. In G.A. Marcoulides & R.E. Schumacker (Eds.), Advancedstructural equation modeling: Issues and techniques (pp. 315–353). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

McDonald, R.P., & Marsh, H.W. (1990). Choosing a multivariate model: Noncentrality and goodness offit. Psychological Bulletin, 107, 247–255.

Mello, J.A. (1993). Improving individual member accountability in small work group settings. Journal ofManagement Education, 17, 253–259.

Mohiyeddini, C. (2001). Individual attitudes toward teamwork. In W. Sarges & H. Wottawa (Eds.),Handbook of economic psychological testing (pp. 251–255). Lengerich, Germany: Pabst SciencePublishers.

Motl, R.W., Dishman, R.K., Trost, S.G., Saunders, R.P., Dowda, M., Felton, G., et al. (2000). Factorialvalidity and invariance of questionnaires measuring social-cognitive determinants of physical activityin adolescent girls. Preventive Medicine, 31, 584–594.

Salomon, G., & Globerson, T. (1989). When teams do not function the way they ought to. InternationalJournal of Educational Research, 13, 89–99.

Slavin, R.E. (1996). Research on cooperative learning and achievement: What we know, what we need toknow. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21, 43–69.

Tuckman, B.W. (1965). Developmental sequence in small groups. Psychological Bulletin, 63, 384–399.Walker, A. (2001). British psychology students’ perceptions of group-work and peer assessment. Psychology

Learning and Teaching, 1, 28–36.White, F., Lloyd, H., Kennedy, G., & Stewart, C. (2005). An investigation of undergraduate students’ feel-

ings and attitudes towards group work and group assessment. Retrieved July 19, 2006, from http://conference.herdsa.org.au/2005/pdf/refereed/paper_250.pdf

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Chi

nese

Uni

vers

ity o

f H

ong

Kon

g] a

t 16:

32 2

1 D

ecem

ber

2014