Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

  • Upload
    ivana-

  • View
    218

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    1/17

    Cogni t i on, 20 (1985) 243-259

    3

    Syntactic frozenness in processing and remembering

    idioms

    RAYMOND W. GIBBS, Jr.

    GAYLE P. GONZALES

    Uni versi t y f Cali fornia, Santa Cruz

    Abstract

    Three experi ments exami ned t he eff ect of synt act i c ro .zenness on underst andi ng

    and memory for i di omat i c expressi ons. I n Experi ment 1 w e est abl i shed a ro-

    zenness cont i nuum by aski ng subj ect s to j udge w het her idi oms in di fferent

    synt acti c orms mai nt ai ned thei r di omat i c meani ngs. The resul t s of Experi ment

    2 indicated

    that

    subjects processed idi omat i c

    expressions

    more quickly than

    t hey did noni di omat i c, control st ri ngs. Moreover, subjects are aster at proces-

    si ng frozen idi oms than t hey are at understandi ng f l exible ones. The fi nal

    experi ment found t hat he degree of synt act i c rozenness has an eff ect on memo-

    ry or i dioms in t hat 7exibl e i di oms w ere recal l ed more oft en than w ere rozen

    ones. These data overal l support t he i dea that di oms are part of t he normal

    l exi con, but are accessed di fferent i al l y accordi ng t o t hei r degree of synt acti c

    f

    ozenness.

    Introduction

    Idiomatic expressions differ in the extent to which they can be syntactically

    transformed and still retain their idiomatic meanings. Thus,

    John kicked the

    bucket can be interpreted as “John died,”

    but its passive form,

    The bucket

    w as ki cked by John,

    is usually not seen as having an idiomatic meaning.

    Other idioms, such as

    John t hrew in t he t ow el

    (meaning “John gave up”) can

    be passivized, as in

    The t ow el was hrow n in by John,

    and still be thought of

    as idiomatic. Fraser (1970) has suggested that idioms do not form a homoge-

    neous class with respect to the transformations they may undergo. He propo-

    *This research was supported by a Faculty Research Grant from the University of California, Santa Cruz.

    We wish to thank Keith Brittany, Elizabeth Fekkes, and Wayne Iba for their assistance in running subjects

    and Rachel Mueller for her comments on an earlier draft of this paper. Reprint requests may be sent to:

    Raymond W. Gibbs, Jr., Program in Experimental Psychology, Clark Kerr Hall, University of California,

    Santa Cruz, CA 95064, U.S.A.

    OOlO-0277/85/ 5.60

    0 Elsevier Sequoia/Printed in The Netherlands

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    2/17

    244

    R. W. Gibbs and G. P. Gonzales

    sed that idioms could be organized into a “frozenness hierarchy” ranging

    from expressions which undergo nearly all traditional transformations without

    losing their idiomatic meanings (e.g.,

    “throw in the towel”) to those expres-

    sions which will not undergo even the most simple transformations and still

    retain their idiomatic interpretations (e.g., “face the music”).

    The purpose of the present paper is to show that people are indeed sensi-

    tive to the syntactic frozenness of idioms and that this affects processing and

    memory for these expressions. Previous research on this issue failed to find

    any effect of syntactic frozenness on the speed with which idioms are proces-

    sed. Swinney and Cutler (1979) examined the time it took subjects to verify

    idioms, which differed in their frozenness level, as being meaningful English

    phrases. Their results showed no significant differences in processing times

    among different levels of Fraser’s (1970) hierarchy. Swinney and Cutler sug-

    gested that these data support the idea “that idioms are to some extent unified

    with respect to access, retrieval, and representation in the lexicon” (p. 532).

    Although Swinney and Cutler’s data appear convincing, there is reason to

    doubt their conclusion. In particular, the organization of Fraser’s (1970) fro-

    zenness hierarchy may not necessarily be representative of most English spea-

    kers’ mental lexicons. This seems especially likely since the hierarchy was

    based solely upon Fraser’s own intuitions. We attempted in Experiment 1 to

    empirically establish a frozenness continuum by asking subjects to judge whe-

    ther idioms in various syntactic forms maintained their idiomatic interpreta-

    tions. Following this, we went on in Experiments 2 and 3 to examine whether

    there are any behavioral differences in processing and memory for idioms

    depending on their degree of syntactic frozenness.

    Experiment

    Method

    Subjects

    Thirty-four undergraduates from the University of California, Santa Cruz

    participated as subjects to fulfill a course requirement. All the subjects were

    native speakers of English.

    Stimuli and design

    Thirty-two verb particle idioms were chosen from Boatner, Gates and Makkai

    (1975). These expressions were rated by 34 subjects as being equally familiar.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    3/17

    Synt ax and i di oms 245

    Table 1.

    Example of di fferent t ransformat i ons and t hei r corresponding paraphr ases

    i n Experi ment 1

    Gerund Nominalization

    Her father’s laying down the law prevented her from going to the dance.

    Her father’s giving strict orders prevented her from going to the dance.

    Adverb Insertion

    The

    boss will quickly lay down the law if anyone shows up late.

    The boss will quickly give strict orders if anyone shows up late.

    Part i cle M ovement

    They will lay the law down if the party gets too wild.

    They will give strict orders if the party gets too wild.

    Passive

    The

    law will be laid down when Jane’s boyfriend finds out where she’s been.

    Strict orders will be given when Jane’s boyfriend finds out where she’s been.

    Act ion Nominal izat ion

    The supervisor’s laying down the law was just what the staff needed.

    The supervisor’s giving of strict orders was just what the staff needed.

    All the idioms were syntactically manipulated according to the transforma-

    tions in levels 1 through 5 (with 1 being most frozen) of Fraser’s (1970)

    frozenness hierarchy: (1) gerund nominalization, (2) adverb insertion, (3)

    particle movement, (4) passive, and (5) action nominalization.’ Verb particle

    idioms were chosen in particular so that all five transformation operations

    could be performed on each idiomatic expression. Each transformed idiom

    was then presented in an appropriate sentential context and was paired with

    a sentence that paraphrased its idiomatic meaning. The paraphrase for each

    idiom was obtained from Boatner et al. (1975) to ensure both consistency and

    conventionality of meaning. Table 1 presents an example of one idiom in

    each of its five syntactic forms and the corresponding paraphrases. In total,

    subjects saw 160 idiom-paraphrase pairs.

    Procedure

    Subjects were presented with a booklet containing a written set of instructions

    and all the experimental material, which was randomly ordered. They were

    ‘The use of these syntactic transformations does not imply anything about their current status in linguistic

    theory.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    4/17

    246

    R. W . Gibbs and G. P. Gonzal es

    instructed to read each idiom-paraphrase pair and judge the degree to which

    the first sentence was similar in meaning to the second. Subjects were asked

    to make this judgment on a seven-point scale where 1 indicated that the two

    sentences were not at all similar in meaning and 7 indicated that the sentences

    were highly similar in meaning. The instructions emphasized that subjects

    should not make the judgments according to what the first sentence might

    mean if it were presented in a different form, but that all judgments should

    be made according to the way the first sentence was actually presented.

    Results and discussion

    Subjects’ ratings of the idiom-paraphrase pairs reflect their intuitions about

    the degree to which an idiom can be syntactically transformed and still retain

    its idiomatic interpretation. High numerical ratings indicate that subjects vie-

    wed a particular syntactic form as maintaining the idiomatic meaning, while

    low ratings indicate that an idiom in some transformed state did not retain

    its idiomatic meaning.

    Two different analyses of subjects’ ratings were performed. The first ana-

    lysis looked at subject’s mean ratings for the different syntactic forms collap-

    sing across all the individual idiomatic expressions. The mean frozenness

    ratings for the five transformations are presented in Table 2.

    An analysis of variance on these data revealed a significant main effect of

    transformation type, F(4, 124) =

    11.04,

    p <

    0.0001. Further examination of

    the individual means showed that subjects’ generally viewed idioms that have

    undergone the Action Nominalization transformation (5 33) as more idioma-

    tic than any other syntactic operation (p < 0.001) for all comparisons). Fraser

    (1970) also saw idioms which could undergo Action Nominalization as being

    the most flexible. Nonetheless, subjects’ ratings of the other syntactic forms

    do not correspond in any regular way with Fraser’s frozenness hierarchy.

    Table

    2.

    Frozemess razi ngs or di fferent synt acti c orms

    Syntactic operation

    Rating

    Gerund Nominalization

    Adverb Insertion

    Particle Movement

    Passive

    Action Nominalization

    4.75 (k 0.13)

    4.22 (+ 0.20)

    4.46 (-+ 0.26)

    4.44 (k 0.15)

    5.58 (+ 0.13)

    Nole: Standard errors are shown in parentheses.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    5/17

    Synt ax and idi oms

    247

    The second, and perhaps most important, analysis concerns subjects’ ra-

    tings for each individual idiom. Subjects’ ratings for each idiom in its five

    syntactic forms were averaged and these means are presented in Table 3.

    As can be seen, the idioms vary in the extent to which they can undergo

    different syntactic operations and still maintain their idiomatic meanings,

    F(31, 1023) = 15.48,

    p <

    0.0001. Some expressions, such as

    t ake under your

    wing,

    are seen as being very frozen in their syntactic form, while others, such

    as

    make up your mi nd,

    are viewed as being more flexible in the kinds of

    syntactic operations they may undergo,

    Table 3.

    I di oms and t hei r rated rozenness

    Idiom

    Rating

    take under one’s wing

    3.60

    turn back the clock

    3.75

    cut down to size

    3.80

    put on some weight

    3.84

    sitting on pins and needles

    4.05

    wait on hand and foot

    4.08

    go against the grain

    4.10

    jump down one’s throat

    4.14

    breathe down one’s neck

    4.21

    pull up stakes

    4.22

    cry over spilled milk

    4.23

    let down one’s hair

    4.38

    throw up one’s hands

    4.48

    put on a good face

    4.49

    skating on thin ice

    4.56

    stew in one’s own juice

    4.68

    bark up the wrong tree

    4.74

    come apart at the seams 4.76

    bring down the house

    4.83

    get away with murder

    4.87

    dance up a storm

    4.92

    bite off more than one can chew

    4.95

    throw in the sponge

    5.00

    beat around the bush

    5.09

    blow off some steam

    5.17

    put down one’s foot

    5.21

    wear out one’s welcome

    5.42

    keep up one’s end

    5.66

    lay down the law

    5.70

    roll out the red carpet 5.72

    turn over a new leaf

    5.59

    make up one’s mind

    6.u)

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    6/17

    248 R. W . Gi bbs and G. P. Gonzal es

    One interesting idea, suggested by Cutler (1982), is that idioms which have

    been in the language longer may be more frozen in their syntactic form. To

    test this we computed a Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient be-

    tween the frozenness ratings and the earliest date an idiom is cited in the

    Oxford English Dictionary (1933, and supplement). We were able to find

    citations for 30 of the expressions used in Experiment 1 and found that r =

    0.27,~ < 0.10 for exact citations, but a r = 0.38,~ < 0.025 when some variation

    was allowed (e.g.,

    “dance on thin ice” for “skating on thin ice”). This corre-

    lation suggests that idioms which are least susceptible to syntactic transforma-

    tions (i.e., more frozen) are also those which have existed longer in the

    language.

    Generally the results of Experiment 1 indicate that people are indeed

    sensitive to the syntactic frozenness of idioms. The question remains as to

    whether the degree of syntactic frozenness affects how people access and

    process idiomatic expressions. This issue is addressed in Experiment 2.

    Subjects in Experiment 2 were presented with phrases that either had

    idiomatic meaning, like

    sit t i ng on pi ns and needl es,

    or nonidiomatic, literal

    phrases, like

    sew i ng w i t h pi ns and needl es.

    The subjects’ task was to read

    each visually presented word string and decide if it formed a meaningful

    phrase in English. Half of the meaningful idiomatic phrases were expressions

    that were rated in Experiment 1 as being more or less frozen in their syntactic

    form, and the other half were idioms that were more or less flexible in their

    syntactic form. We hypothesized that idioms are

    not

    represented with equal

    status in the lexicon and that frozen idioms should be more lexicalized than

    flexible idiomatic expressions. Consequently, our prediction was that subjects

    should be much faster at making the phrase classification responses for frozen

    idioms than they would be for idioms that are more flexible in their syntactic

    form. In addition, we also expected to replicate the Swinney and Cutler

    (1979) finding that idiomatic expressions can be processed faster than noni-

    diomatic control strings. This result would confirm the hypothesis that people

    need not analyze the literal meanings of idioms before determining their

    nonliteral, idiomatic interpretations (see Gibbs, 1980, 1985).

    Experiment 2

    Method

    Subj ects

    Forty-two undergraduates from the University of California, Santa Cruz par-

    ticipated in the experiment in order to fulfill a course requirement.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    7/17

    Synt ax and idi oms

    249

    Mater ials

    The

    experimental materials consisted of twenty idiomatic words strings. Ten

    of the idiomatic word strings were the most frozen expressions from Experi-

    ment 1, and the remainder were the ten most flexible idioms as determined

    by subjects’ ratings in Experiment 1. These were matched with twenty noni-

    diomatic, control strings. The control strings were constructed by replacing

    the first two words in each idiomatic string with words of relatively equal

    length (in both letters and syllables). Each control string formed a literal,

    grammatically acceptable English phrase. Overall, the frozen idioms were on

    average 21.7 characters in length, while the control strings for these frozen

    idioms were 19.7 characters. The flexible idioms were 20.4 characters long

    and the controls for the flexible idioms were 19.6 characters in length. These

    means did not differ significantly from each other as determined by an analy-

    sis of variance. Therefore, differences in response times to the experimental

    stimuli could not be attributed to the length of the word strings.

    In addition, 20 word strings that did not form an acceptable English phrase

    (i.e.,

    “no sleep it floor out”;

    “wish out the table to”) were created. These

    unacceptable word strings were roughly equivalent to the acceptable phrase

    strings in number of words and characters. Subjects were expected to respond

    “No” (“Not meaningful”) to these targets, The data for these word strings

    were not included in the data analysis.

    esign

    Subjects were randomly assigned to one of two groups, each of which was

    the complement of the other. If a given idiomatic phrase was presented to

    one group, the corresponding nonidiomatic, control string was shown to the

    other group. Each group of targets contained 6 practice phrases, 20 filler

    phrases, and 20 experimental targets. Half of the experimental targets were

    idiomatic phrases and half were nonidiomatic word strings. In each group, 5

    of the idiomatic phrases were frozen and 5 were flexible in their syntactic

    form, as determined by the ratings in Experiment 1.

    Procedure

    Subjects were tested individually. The 40 word strings were randomly presen-

    ted in lower-case letters on a CRT display screen under the control of an

    IBM personal computer, which recorded all response latencies. Subjects were

    instructed to read each word string and decide, as quickly as possible, whether

    or not each of these formed a meaningful phrase in English. All subjects were

    instructed to indicate their decision by pressing one of two buttons on a

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    8/17

    250

    R. W . Gi bbs and G. P. Gonzal es

    keyboard which was in front of them. Once the response to the target had

    been made, there was a 2-second delay and the next word string appeared

    on the screen. Six practice phrases were then given followed by the 40 expe-

    rimental stimuli. Response latencies were measured from the onset of the

    word string to when the subject pressed one of the two buttons,

    Results and discussion

    Response times longer than 5 seconds (less than 1% of all responses) were

    counted as errors and eliminated from the analysis. These outliers represen-

    ted cases in which experimental instructions could not have been followed,

    as when subjects’ attention wandered from the task. The response times to

    make the phrase classification judgments and the proportion of errors are

    presented in Table 4. Only responses in which subjects made a correct respon-

    se were included in the analysis. Error rates were also tallied for each subject

    in the experiment. The overall rates were nearly identical for the four expe-

    rimental conditions (6.75%), and consequently will not be discussed further.

    A two-way analysis of variance was performed with Frozenness (frozen vs.

    flexible) and Sentence Type (idiom vs. control) as within-subject factors.

    Each of the analyses were performed twice: once treating subjects as a ran-

    dom factor while collapsing over material (FI), and once treating materials

    as the random factor while collapsing over subjects (F2). Min F statistics are

    also reported as suggested by Clark (1973).

    These analyses revealed a significant effect of Sentence Type (idioms vs.

    control strings), [FI(l, 41) = 17.17, p < 0.001; K?(l, 19) = 9.45, p < 0.001;

    min F’(1, 62) = 6.09, p < 0.0251, and a significant interaction between Fro-

    zenness (frozen vs. flexible) and Sentence Type [FI(l, 41) = 7.59, p < 0.01;

    F2(1, 19) = 4.21,

    p <

    0.05; min F’(1, 61) = 2.72,

    p <

    0.101. Further analysis

    on the individual means indicated that subjects were much faster at respond-

    Table

    4.

    M ean lat encies (mi l l iseconds) t o di oms and control str i ngs

    Sentence type

    Frozenness level

    Idioms Control strings

    Frozen

    Flexible

    1115 (k 46) 1333 (+ 73)

    1240 (+ 49) 1291(+- 55)

    Note: standard errors are shown in parentheses.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    9/17

    Syntax and idioms

    251

    ing to the frozen idioms (1115 ms) than to the matched control strings (1333

    ms), t(82) = 2.54, p < 0.01. Moreover, subjects were also faster at responding

    to the frozen idioms (1115 ms) than to the flexible ones (1239 ms), t(82) =

    1.86,

    p < 0.05.

    All other differences were not statistically reliable.

    The finding that subjects were faster in responding to the idiomatic expres-

    sions than to the control strings replicates Swinney and Cutler’s (1979) results

    and is consistent with Gibbs’s (1980, 1985) data that idioms are com-

    prehended faster than literal uses of the same expressions. As such these

    results confirm the hypothesis that people need not analyze the literal mean-

    ings of idiomatic expressions before deriving their nonliteral, idiomatic in-

    terpretations.2 It appears unlikely, then, that idioms are represented in a

    special, separate idiom dictionary (see BobTow and Bell, 1973; Weinreich,

    1969), which is consulted after a literal analysis of the expression was found

    to be defective.

    One could argue, however, that the idiom-control string differences are

    due to subjects being in a special “idiom processing mode” since they were

    presented with many idioms in the experiment. To check for this, we ran-

    domly matched subjects from the two experimental groups and analyzed the

    response times for the first occurrence of an idiom in one group, with its

    matched control in the other group. This analysis showed that subjects were

    faster overall in responding to the first idiom (1105 ms) than they were in

    responding to the matched control strings (1273 ms), 41) = 1.96,

    p <

    0.05.

    Thus, even before subjects could be put into an “idiom mode of processing”

    there was a reaction time advantage for the idiom phrases over the control

    strings. A similar finding is reported by Swinney and Cutler (1979).

    One other possible interpretation of the results merits consideration. The

    advantage shown by frozen idioms in this experiment may be simply due to

    there being a higher likelihood of occurrence for the sequence of lexical items

    forming these expressions than for those items forming the flexible idioms.

    In some sense, this notion of transitional probability fits in well with our

    hypothesis that idioms which are restricted in the kinds of syntactic operations

    they may undergo are more predictable than are idioms which are syntacti-

    cally flexible. However, it seems necessary to further examine the idea that

    predictability alone might account for the faster processing of frozen idioms,

    as well as the general advantage of idioms over the nonidiomatic controls.

    To test this idea, we ran a control study similar to one suggested by Swin-

    ney and Cutler (1979, see experiment 1). Sixteen undergraduates from the

    University of California, Santa Cruz were presented with a typed list of all

    *See Gibbs (1985) for the results of two studies which suggest that people do not always analyze the literal

    meanings of idioms in parallel to determining their nonliteral, idiomatic interpretations.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    10/17

    252 R. W . Gi bbs and G. P. Gonzal es

    the idioms and nonidiomatic phrases, each with its final two words omitted.

    These materials were randomly intermixed with 80 “filler” nonidiomatic

    phrases, which were roughly similar in length to the experimental materials.

    The purpose of these “filler” items was to decrease the possibility of subjects

    noticing that all of the experimental materials could be idioms. Subjects were

    asked to write down the first word or words that came to mind to complete

    each phrase. The number of completion responses that were identical to

    either the different idioms or the control materials were tabulated. These

    data showed that subjects were

    not

    any better at completing frozen idioms

    (38%) than they were at completing flexible idioms (43%), F(1, 15) = 0.043,

    p < 0.10. This suggests that the transitional probability per se for the frozen

    idioms was not making subjects respond faster to these expressions than to

    the flexible idioms. An analysis of the idiom/control pairs revealed that the

    number of completion responses was higher for the idioms than for the con-

    trol phrases in 15 out of the 20 total pairs. For one pair there were equal

    number of completion responses to the idiom and its control phrase, and 4

    control phrases were completed more often than their corresponding idioms.

    For purposes of comparison, the data were divided in two unequal groups.

    One group comprised the 15 idiom/control pairs which had the idiom comple-

    tion advantage over their respective controls. The other group comprised the

    1 idiom/control pair that had equal number of completion responses to the

    idiom and its control phrase, and the 4 control phrases that were completed

    more often than their corresponding idioms. The main reaction time data for

    each of these conditions can be seen in Table 5.

    An analysis of variance was performed on the reaction-time data with

    Phrase Type and Response Completion Category as the independent vari-

    ables. The main effect of Phrase Type was not significant, F(1, 18) = 0.03,

    Table 5. Mean latencies

    (mil l i seconds) t o di om and cont rol phrases as a

    function f

    response completion category

    Response completion category

    Phrase type

    Control advantage*

    Idiom advantage**

    over idiom + no idiom/

    over control

    control difference

    Idiom

    Control

    -

    *@I = 5).

    **+I = 15).

    1208

    1182

    1271

    1309

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    11/17

    Synt ax and di oms

    253

    p >

    0.10; while the effect of Response Completion Category was highly

    reliable, F(1,18) = 21.93,

    p <

    0.001. Nonetheless, the crucial interaction

    between these two factors was not significant, F(1, 18) = 1.42,

    p >

    0.10. As

    such, these data demonstrate that transitional probability does not play a

    significant role in the effects reported in the present study.

    The most important result of Experiment 2 is that subjects process frozen

    idioms faster than they do flexible ones. This suggests that idioms are not

    unified with respect to access, retrieval, and representation in the lexicon.

    People appear to be sensitive to the linguistic frozenness of an idiomatic

    expression and this affects the speed with which they are understood. In

    Experiment 3 we examined whether linguistic frozenness had any effect on

    memory for idioms.

    Subjects listened to a tape recording of a series of stories, some of which

    ended in idiomatic expressions. If an idiom’s syntactic productivity influences

    the speed with which it is processed, then idioms that take longer to under-

    stand should be more memorable than those that were comprehended

    quickly. This additional processing time reflects the difficulty subjects have

    in accessing flexible expressions, which should make these idioms more dis-

    tinctive in memory and consequently should be recalled more often.

    Experiment 3

    Method

    Subj ects

    Thirty-six undergraduates from the University of California, Santa Cruz

    served as subjects in order to fulfill a course requirement.

    M at eri al s and desi gn

    The

    experiment employed a cued-recall paradigm. For the acquisition part

    of the experiment, subjects listened to a tape recording ,of 40 stories, 20 of

    which ended in idiomatic expressions and 20 of which ended in nonidiomatic

    utterances. Half of the idiom stories ended in frozen idioms and half in

    flexible ones. These expressions were identical to those used in the previous

    study. Table 6 presents examples of stories ending irra frozen idiom, a flexible

    idiom, and a nonidiomatic utterance.

    rating

    study was performed to ensure that the last lines of these stories

    were interpreted as they were intended to be. Twelve subjects participated

    in the study. Subjects read each story and made a judgment as to whether

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    12/17

    254 R. W . Gi bbs and G. P. Gonzal es

    Table 6.

    Exampl es of stor i es used in Experi ment 3

    Frozen idiom

    Jerry loved to ski. He was pretty good so he entered a big skiing contest one day. When it was

    his turn, he tried to go too fast and fell down and lost the race. Jerry later moaned to a friend,

    “I wish I could turn back the clock.”

    Flexible idiom

    Erica’s father did not like the idea of his 15-year old daughter dating. One month Erica actually

    went out every weekend with a different boy. Finally, Erica’s father had had enough. He said

    to her, “I’m going to put down my foot.”

    No

    id iom

    Mike had just bought himself a new car. His parents and friends warned him to drive carefully,

    but Mike just ignored them. One day, he had an accident and destroyed the car. He cried to

    a friend, “Now what am I going to do?”

    each final sentence was idiomatic or literal in its context. They also rated, on

    a six point scale, how well the final sentence of each story fit into the context.

    Results showed that subjects rated the frozen idioms as being idiomatic 98%

    of the time, and the flexible idioms as being idiomatic 96% of the time. The

    nonidiomatic targets were rated as being literal 100% of the time. An analysis

    of the ratings indicated that there was no significant difference between the

    overall effectiveness of frozen idiom, flexible idiom, and nonidiomatic stories

    in inducing the meanings of their last lines. These results suggest that there

    are no differences in the story contexts that would bias either understanding

    or memory for the target sentences.

    In the acquisition part of the study, subjects heard the 40 stories presented

    in a random order. In addition to these contexts, there was one filler story

    at the beginning of the tape and one at the end. These were included to

    prevent primacy and recency effects from occurring in the subsequent recall

    test. For the recall phase of the experiment, subjects were given a list of 20

    cues, each of which was related to the setting of a story ending with an

    idiomatic expression. For the 2 idiom stories presented in Table 6, for exam-

    ple, subjects were given the cue

    skiing

    for the frozen context, and

    dating

    for

    the flexible story. Since memory for the nonidiomatic utterances was not of

    interest, there were no recall cues for these stories.

    Procedure

    Subjects were run in groups of six. They were instructed to pay close attention

    to the materials presented because they would be asked some questions about

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    13/17

    Synt ax and idi oms

    255

    them afterwards. The tape containing the stories was then played to the

    subjects. Following this, subjects read some unrelated stories for approxi-

    mately 15 minutes. Immediately afterwards, subjects were given an answer

    sheet containing the 20 recall cues. They were asked to look at the cues and

    write down the final sentence of the story that corresponded with the cue.

    They were given 15 minutes to complete the memory task.

    Results and discussion

    A sentence was scored as being correct if it contained all the important

    content words and had the same syntactic structure as the target sentence.

    Subjects recalled more instances of flexible idioms (46%, se = 0.03) than

    they did frozen idioms (32%, se

    = 0.03) [FI(l, 35) = 23.06,

    p <

    0.0001;

    F2(1, 19) = 4.83,

    p < 0.05;

    min F’(1, 49) = 4.51,

    p <

    0.051. These results

    support the hypothesis that flexible idioms are more memorable than frozen

    expressions because of the difficulty subjects have in retrieving flexible idioms

    from the mental lexicon. This additional processing makes these expressions

    more distinctive and consequently people remember them better.

    One could argue that the memory superiority for the flexible idioms is due

    to their being more closely related in meaning to their nonidiomatic, literal

    interpretations. Thus, subjects process both the literal and idiomatic mean-

    ings of flexible idioms, but not so for frozen ones, which results in these

    expressions being more distinctive and memorable. This hypothesis seems

    unlikely given the results of Experiment 2 where subjects took less time,

    although not significantly so, to process flexible idioms than their nonidioma-

    tic control strings. This indicates that people are not processing the literal

    meanings of these expressions before going on to determine their idiomatic

    meanings. Moreover, the results of Gibbs (1985) suggest that people do not

    necessarily process the literal meanings of idioms at any point during com-

    prehension. These findings cast doubt on any explanation of the present

    results that the memory superiority for flexible idioms is due to their idiomatic

    and nonidiomatic meanings being interpreted. Rather, the data appear to be

    due to the difficulty subjects have in accessing flexible idioms from the mental

    lexicon.

    General discussion

    Much of the interest in idiomatic expressions derives from the fact that they

    are not amenable to traditional compositional analysis and because idioms

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    14/17

    256 R. W . Gi bbs and G. P. Gonzal es

    differ widely in terms of their syntactic productivity. Linguists have been

    particularly concerned with idioms since one argument for the existence of

    transformations presumes that certain idioms can appear in more than one

    syntactic form (cf. Culicover, 1982; Perlmutter and Soames, 1979), although

    more recent proposals have tried to argue for the direct generation of surface

    structures (see Bresnan, 1982). Despite this interest on the part of linguists,

    little work in psychology has been devoted to the role of syntactic frozenness

    in people’s intuitions about different idioms and how these idioms are proces-

    sed and remembered.

    The results of the present studies indicate that the degree of syntactic

    frozenness of an idiom does indeed affect peoples’ behavior in understanding

    and remembering these expressions. In Experiment 1 we found that people

    view some idioms as retaining their idiomatic interpretations regardless of

    their syntactic forms, while other idioms appear to be quite restricted in the

    forms they can take. Experiment 2 demonstrated that people process frozen

    idioms faster than they do flexible ones. Moreover, idiomatic expressions

    were processed more quickly than nonidiomatic, control strings. As Experi-

    ment 3 showed, the differential speed with which idioms are processed sub-

    sequently affects their memorability. Idioms that are more flexible and take

    longer to process are generally recalled more often than are frozen expres-

    sions. This is so because it is more difficult for subjects to process flexible

    idioms, which makes these expressions more distinctive and memorable.

    These findings support the idea that idioms are part of the normal lexicon,

    but are represented differentially depending on their degree of syntactic fro-

    zenness. Frozen idioms appear to be more lexicalized than flexible ones and

    consequently can be accessed faster from the mental lexicon. This makes

    good sense since the representation of expressions, such as

    kick the bucket

    does not need to take into account other possible syntactic forms. Other

    phrases, like

    lay down the law

    can be viewed as idiomatic in a variety of

    forms, and are not as strong in their lexical status as more frozen expressions.

    This conclusion poses a question as to how idioms are represented in the

    lexicon. Specifically, what would a model of the lexicon look like that incor-

    porated different degrees of representation for idioms which vary in their

    syntactic frozenness? One possibility is that idioms have respresentations for

    each of their permissible syntactic forms. Whereas frozen idioms, such as

    turn

    back the clock

    have a limited number of entries, flexible idioms, such as

    lay

    down the law

    have multiple entries corresponding to each of their syntactic

    constructions. Although frozen and flexible idioms examined in the present

    studies were viewed as equally familiar to subjects; it is likely that the pro-

    cessing-time advantage for frozen idioms is a. result of people’s familiarity

    with these expressions’

    particular

    syntactic forms. For example, people may

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    15/17

    Synt ax and i dioms

    257

    be just as familiar with a frozen expression, like turn back

    t he clock

    as they

    are with flexible idioms, such as lay down

    the law

    when these phrases are

    seen in all of there different permissible forms. However, because frozen

    idioms are seen in fewer syntactic forms, each individual construction will be

    viewed as more familiar. Consequently, subjects may be more familiar with

    the specific syntactic construction

    turn

    back t he clock

    than they are with the

    particular construction

    lay dow n the aw .

    This may be so despite the findings

    in Experiment 2 that subjects were equally good at completing the endings

    of frozen and flexible idioms. After all, the lack of a significant difference in

    the number of successful completions for the two types of idioms does

    not

    mean that there are no differences in the speed with which people retrieve

    these different kinds of idioms from the mental lexicon.

    One way of incorporating these different representations for idioms is to

    assume that the internal lexicon can be characterized as a

    t hreshold model .

    This model suggests that idioms can be viewed as different logogens (cf.

    Morton, 1969, 1979), each with different threshold levels depending on the

    frequency of their specific syntactic constructions. Frozen idioms would have

    lower resting thresholds than flexible idioms and less information would be

    required from the input stimulus to bring the logogen for a frozen idiom to

    its threshold level. Consequently, people will process these expressions faster

    as shown in Experiment 2.

    Another way of incorporating different degrees of representation for

    idioms is to assume a

    search model

    (cf. Becker, 1979; Forster, 1979). In this

    model, lexical access is a process by which an input stimulus is matched to

    the representation of a word or phrase in the lexicon. Partial analysis of a

    stimulus results in the generation of a set of candidates, which is then searched

    serially until a match with the stimulus is found. The set of candidates

    is typically held to be searched in order of frequency of occurrence of the

    item represented (cf. Forster, 1979). If idioms have multiple entries, it would

    be expected that the entries for the most frequent syntactic forms would be

    found first from the search. In this way, idioms with restricted syntactic forms,

    which are most frequent, would be recognized faster than idioms that could

    be seen in a variety of syntactic forms.

    This discussion is based on the reasonable assumption that retrieving

    idioms from the lexicon operates in a similar manner to normal lexical access

    processes. The application of both the

    threshold

    and

    search models

    to the

    representation of idioms in the mental lexicon is somewhat speculative. Both

    models, however, appear equally capable of accounting for the processing-

    time differences found between frozen and flexible idioms. One other possi-

    bility is that people do not have multiple representations for each of the permis-

    sible syntactic forms an idiom can take. Instead, people may have abstract

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    16/17

    258 R. W. Gibbs and G. P. Gonzales

    knowledge about the possible syntactic operations that can be applied to any

    particular idiom. This idea has some intuitive appeal given that most idioms

    are derived from novel, metaphorical expressions which presumably are not

    represented as phrasal units in the mental lexicon. When the process of

    idio-

    matizution

    (cf. Chafe, 1970) occurs and an expression becomes more lexicali-

    zed, there must be some constraints on what possible syntactic forms the

    idiom can take.

    This brings us to an important question left unanswered. What it is about

    idioms that makes some of them syntactically frozen and others flexible?

    Although many formal syntactic devices have been offered to account for the

    syntactic deficiencies of idioms (see Dong, 1971; Fraser, 1970; Katz, 1973;

    Newmeyer, 1974; Weinrich, 1969), there is good reason to believe that the

    limited productivity of idioms must be due to the relation between syntax,

    semantics, and pragmatics (Nunberg, 1978; Strassler, 1982). Some recent

    proposals have even suggested that idioms should be treated as analyzable

    strings with the meanings of individual lexical items contributing both to their

    overall idiomatic meanings and their syntactic limitations (Nunberg, 1978;

    Wasow, Sag and Nunberg, 1982). Psycholinguistic studies have shown that

    people do not ordinarily compute the complete literal meaning of an idiom

    during comprehension. Nonetheless, people may still have some knowledge

    of the internal semantics of idioms which constrains the possible syntactic

    forms these expressions may take. This could be accomplished by the momen-

    tary examination of an idiom’s individual lexical items (Estill and Kemper,

    1981; Swinney, 1981). Exactly how an idiom is represented in the mental

    lexicon will depend on further investigations of the semantic relationships

    among its parts and how people’s acceptance and understanding of idioms is

    affected by their internal structures.

    References

    Becker, C. (1979) Semantic context and word frequency in visual word recognition.

    J. exp. Psychol.: Hum.

    Percep. Perf 5, 252-259.

    Boatner, M., Gates, J., and Makkai, A. (1975) A Di cfionary ofAmeri can Idi oms. New York: Baron’s Educa-

    tional Series.

    Bobrow, S., and Bell, S. (1973) On catching on to idiomatic expressions. Mem. Cog., 1, 343-346.

    Bresnan, J. (ed.) (1982) The

    Mental Represenrati on ofGramma l Rel ati ons.

    Cambridge, MA.: M.I.T. Press.

    Chafe, W. (1970) Meaning and t he Sw ucfur e of L anguage. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Clark, H. (1973) The language-as-fixed-effect fallacy: A critique of language studies in psychological research.

    1. verb. Learn . verb. Behav., 12, 335-359.

    Culicover, P. (1982) Synfux. New York: Academic Press.

    Cutler, A. (1982) Idioms: The older the colder. Ling. Inq., 13, 317-320.

  • 8/15/2019 Gibbs, Syntactic Frozenness in Processing and Remembering Idioms

    17/17

    Synt ax ahd idi oms

    259

    Dong, P. (1971) The applicability of transformations to idioms. Papers rom t he Seventh Regional M eet i ng of

    the Chicago Linguisric Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic Society.

    Estill, R., and Kemper, S. (1981) Interpreting idioms. J.

    Psychol i ng. Res., 9, 559-568.

    Forster, K. (1979) Levels of processing and the structure of the language processor. In W. Cooper and E.

    Walker (eds.), Sentence

    Processi ng: Psycholi nguisti c studies presented to M erri ll Garrett.

    Hillsdale, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Fraser, B. (1970) Idioms within a transformational grammar. Found. Lang., 6, 22-42.

    Gibbs, R. (1980) Spilling the beans on understanding and memory for idioms in conversation. Mem. Cog., 8

    449-456.

    Gibbs, R. (1985) Skating on thin ice: Literal meaning and understanding idioms in conversation.

    Discourse

    Processes, 9, in press.

    Katz, J. (1973) Compositionality, idiomaticity, and lexical substitution. In S. Anderson and P. Kiparsky (eds.),

    A Festschrift or M orris Hal le. New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

    Morton, J. (1969) Interaction of information in word recognition. Psychol.

    Rev.,

    76, 165-178.

    Morton, J. (1979) Word recognition. In J. Morton and J. Marshall (eds.), Psychol i nguist i cs: 2. St ructur e and

    Processes.

    London: Paul Elek.

    Newmeyer, F. (1974) The regularity of idiom behavior,

    Lingua, 34, 327-342.

    Nunberg, G. (1978) The pragmatics of reference. Indiana Linguistics Club.

    Perlmutter, D., and Soames, S. (1979) Synf acti c Ar gument uri on and he Sfru crure of Engl i sh. Berkeley: Univer-

    sity of California Press.

    Strassler, J. (1982) Idioms in English. Tubingen: Gunter Narr Verlag.

    Swinney, D. (1981) Lexical processing during sentence comprehension: Effects of higher order constraints and

    implications for representation. In T. Myers, J. Laver, and J. Anderson (eds.),

    The Cogni t i ve Represen-

    fat i on of Speech.

    Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Swinney, D., and Cutler, A. (1979) The access and processing of idiomatic expressions. 1. verb. Learn. verb.

    Behav., 18, 523-534.

    Wasow, T., Sag, I., and Nunberg, G. (1982) Idioms: An interim report. Unpublished mimeo, Stanford

    University.

    Weinreich, U. (1969) Problems in the analysis of idioms. In J. Puhvel (ed.),

    Substance and struct ure of

    Language.

    Los Angeles: University of California Press, 23-81.

    On examine, au tours de trois experiences, l’effet du “gel” syntaxique sur la comprehension et la m6morisation

    des expressions idiomatiques. Dans l’exp6rience 1) on Btabli un continuum de gel en demandant aux sujets

    de juger si les expressions idiomatiques maintiennent leurs sens idiomatique dans des formes syntaxiques

    differentes. Les resultats de la seconde experience indiquent que les sujets traitent les expressions idiomatiques

    plus rapidement que des suites controles non idiomatiques. En outre les sujets sont plus rapides pour calculer

    ies expressions idiomatiques, gelCes qu’ils ne Ie sont pour comprendre Ies expressions idiomatiques flexibles.

    La demihre experience montre que le degre de gel syntaxique a un effet sur Ia memorisation des expressions

    idiomatiques. On se souvient plus des expressions idiomatiques flexibles que de celles qui sont geldes. Ces

    donn6es appuient I’idCe que les expressions idiomatiques font partie du lexique normal mais qu’on acci?de de

    fa9on diff&ente selon leur degre de gel syntaxique.