25
Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia Karen Khachatryan, Anna Dreber, Emma von Essen, Eva Ranehill Forthcoming in Journal of Economic Behavior and Organiza6on (2015)

Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Citation preview

Page 1: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Gender  and  Preferences  at  a  Young  Age:  Evidence  from  Armenia  

Karen  Khachatryan,  Anna  Dreber,  Emma  von  Essen,  Eva  Ranehill    

Forthcoming  in    Journal  of  Economic  Behavior  and  Organiza6on  (2015)  

Page 2: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Why  are  these  preferences  relevant?  

o  Individuals  that  display  more  compeJJveness  and  less  altruism  earn  more  

o  Some  of  the  gender  gap  in  earnings  can  be  explained  with  a  set  of  psychological  factors,  including  aNtudes  toward  risk  and  compeJJon  

o  Individuals  who  self-­‐report  that  they  are  less  willing  to  take  risks  work  in  occupaJons  with  more  stable  earnings,  that  tend  to  pay  less  on  average  due  to  compensaJng  wage  differenJals  

Bonin  et  al.  2007,  Manning  and  Swaffield  2008,  Flory  et  al.  2010,  Manning  and  Saidi  2010,  Dohmen  et  al.  2011  

Page 3: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

CompeJJveness  

Measures    •  Performance  in  tournaments  vs  piece-­‐rate  schemes  •  Self-­‐selecJon  into  tournaments  vs  piece-­‐rate  schemes  

 What  do  we  know  about  adults?  

 Women  are  (if  anything)…  

•  Less  compeJJve  – Math  related  tasks  

•  Less  risk  taking  •  More  altruisJc  

 Croson  and  Gneezy  2009,  Engel  2011  

 

Page 4: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

CompeJJveness  in  children/adolescents  

o  If  gender  gap,  boys  more  compeJJve  –  Israel,  Sweden,  Austria,  Colombia:  Running,  skipping  rope,  dancing,  children  and  adolescents  

–  Austria  and  UK:  Math  and  mazes,  children  and  adolescents  o  ExcepJon  

–  Sweden:  Skipping  rope,  math    •  Performance  change  in  both  

Gneezy  &  RusJchini  2004,  Booth  &  Nolen  2009,  Dreber  et  al.  2011,  Suber  &  Rutzler  2010,  Cárdenas  et  al.  2011  

Page 5: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Impacts  of  culture  and  context  

•  Massai  vs  Khasi  –  Patriarchal  vs  matrilineal  society  

•  Running  –  Difference  between  GR04  and  DvER11  

•  Single-­‐sex  vs  coeducaJonal  schools  •  SelecJon  issues?  

•  Colombia  vs  Sweden    –  Unexpected  findings:  No  gender  gaps  in  Colombia  in  all  tasks,  but  some  gender  gaps  in  Sweden  

 

 Gneezy  &  RusJchini  2004,  Booth  &  Nolen  2011,  Dreber  et  al.  2011,  Cárdenas  et  al.  2011,  Dreber  et  al.  2011,  Grosse  &  Riener  2010,  Gunther  et  al.  2009,  Wozniak  et  al.  2010    

Page 6: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Risk  taking  in  children  and  adolescents  

o  If  gender  gap,  boys  more  risk  taking  –  US:  9-­‐13  and  14-­‐20  yr  olds  

•  Only  study  with  no  gap,  smallest  sample  size  –  Netherlands:  15-­‐16  yr  olds  –  UK:  15  yr  olds  –  Sweden  and  Colombia:  9-­‐12  yr  olds  

Harbaugh  et  al.  2002;  Booth  &  Nolen  2011,  Borghans  et  al.  2009,  Cárdenas  et  al.  2011,    

Page 7: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Impact  of  tasks  

•  Some  influence  –  ShooJng  baskets  vs  solving  anagrams  –  Running  vs  skipping  rope  and  dancing  –  Solving  mazes/math  vs  word  search  

Gneezy  &  RusJchini  2004b,  Dreber  et  al.  2011,  Grosse  &  Riener  2010,  Gunther  et  al.  2009,  Wozniak  et  al.  2010,  Cárdenas  et  al.  2011  

Page 8: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

QuesJons  and  ContribuJon  

o  What  causes  the  gender  gap  in  these  preferences?  

– When  does  it  emerge?  • What  is  the  influence  of  the  country  in  which  the  study  is  performed  (culture  and  context)?  • What  is  the  influence  of  tasks?    •  Extrinsic  vs  intrinsic  moJvaJon?  

o  How  do  these  preferences  develop?  –  InteresJng  to  compare  different  age  groups  

Page 9: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

What  we  do:  Our  Experiments  

o  824  children  aged  7-­‐16  in  Yerevan,  Armenia    

-  in  school  grades  2  to  10    

-  48%  girls    

o  CompeJJon  tasks  in  the  gym  class  

o  CompeJJon  tasks  in  the  classroom  

o  Risk  task  in  the  classroom  

o  Dictator  game  in  the  classroom  

Page 10: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

CompeJJon  in  the  Gym  Class  Years  2  to  9  

o  Running  vs  Skipping  rope  (“gendered  tasks”?)  –  Stage  1:  run  individually  4  x  13  meters  /  Skip  rope  individually  

–  Stage  2:  repeat  stage  1  but  in  matched  pairs  

o  Measure  of  compeJJveness  

–  speed  in  stage  2  —  speed    in  stage  1  

–  number  of  jumps  in  stage  2  —  number  of  jumps  in  stage  1  

–  could  also  use  a  relaJve  measure  

Page 11: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

 CompeJJon  in  the  gym  class  Boys  run  faster  than  girls  in  any  age  group,  but  Girls  improve  more,  significantly  more  compeJJve  in  running  task    Girls  are  beber  in  skipping  rope,  but  no  significant  difference  in  performance  change  reacJon  to  compeJJon  

   

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

Table 1 – Individual and competitive performance in running (seconds)and skipping rope (number of jumps)

Running task Skipping rope task

Age Group Gender Round 1 Round 2 SR testa Round 1 Round 2 SR test Nb

(Ind.) (Comp.) (p≠value) (Ind.) (Comp.) (p≠value)

Grades 2 to 6 Boys 17.39 16.98 < 0.001 5.92 6.39 0.340 154/153Girls 18.95 18.32 < 0.001 8.38 7.12 0.692 147/145

Grades 7 to 9 Boys 15.13 14.92 < 0.001 67.10 72.27 < 0.001 100/96Girls 16.94 16.27 < 0.001 70.92 79.45 < 0.001 91/88

All Grades Boys 16.50 16.17 < 0.001 29.66 31.79 0.002 254/249Girls 18.19 17.54 < 0.001 32.50 34.44 0.016 238/233

aWilcoxon matched pairs signed-rank test, testing whether (Ind.)=(Comp.), henceforth SR test in the tablesb5 boys and 5 girls took part in the running task, but did not complete the skipping rope task.

-0.41

-0.63

-0.21

-0.66

p=0.023 p=0.001

0

-.2

-.4

-.6

-.8

Sec

onds

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 9

Running

0.47 -1.26

5.51

8.93

p=0.800

p=0.527

02468

10

Jum

ps

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 9

Skipping Rope

Boys Girls

By Task, Age Group and GenderAverage Performance Change: PE Tasks

Figure 1. Gender and competitiveness in running and skipping rope

9

Page 12: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

Table 1 – Individual and competitive performance in running (seconds)and skipping rope (number of jumps)

Running task Skipping rope task

Age Group Gender Round 1 Round 2 SR testa Round 1 Round 2 SR test Nb

(Ind.) (Comp.) (p≠value) (Ind.) (Comp.) (p≠value)

Grades 2 to 6 Boys 17.39 16.98 < 0.001 5.92 6.39 0.340 154/153Girls 18.95 18.32 < 0.001 8.38 7.12 0.692 147/145

Grades 7 to 9 Boys 15.13 14.92 < 0.001 67.10 72.27 < 0.001 100/96Girls 16.94 16.27 < 0.001 70.92 79.45 < 0.001 91/88

All Grades Boys 16.50 16.17 < 0.001 29.66 31.79 0.002 254/249Girls 18.19 17.54 < 0.001 32.50 34.44 0.016 238/233

aWilcoxon matched pairs signed-rank test, testing whether (Ind.)=(Comp.), henceforth SR test in the tablesb5 boys and 5 girls took part in the running task, but did not complete the skipping rope task.

-0.41

-0.63

-0.21

-0.66

p=0.023 p=0.001

0

-.2

-.4

-.6

-.8

Seco

nds

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 9

Running

0.47 -1.26

5.51

8.93

p=0.800

p=0.527

02468

10

Jum

ps

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 9

Skipping Rope

Boys Girls

By Task, Age Group and GenderAverage Performance Change: PE Tasks

Figure 1. Gender and competitiveness in running and skipping rope

9

Page 13: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Running:  Gender  of  the  opponent  effect?    

Change  in  performance  in  the  compeJJve  environment  according  to  the  gender  composiJon  of  the  pairs  

Both  boys  and  girls  compete  more  with  girls!    

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

Table 2 – Impact of opponent’s gender on competitiveness

Running againsta Skipping rope againstb

Sample of Boys Girls p≠value N Boys Girls p≠value N

Boys -0.25 -0.49 0.008 162/91 2.12 2.74 0.558 129/118Girls -0.46 -0.76 0.006 94/144 2.57 2.60 0.996 118/118

aAverage performance change (in seconds)bAverage performance change (number of jumps)

However, in the skipping rope task we do not find any gender di�erences in competitiveness (forthe whole sample: p = 0.917).13

Comparing the size of the gender gaps in performance change across age groups, we do notfind any significant di�erences in neither running (p = 0.341) nor skipping rope (p = 0.107).

In both running and skipping rope tasks, the students could observe each other while per-forming in the second round. In Table 2 we therefore explore whether the response to com-petition varies with the gender of the opponent in our sample. In running, both boys andgirls improve their performance significantly more when competing against a girl versus a boy(boys: p = 0.002; girls: p = 0.006), and this di�erence is mainly driven by the older students.14

However, we find no significant opponent gender e�ects in the skipping rope task.Summarizing, we have the following result:

Result 1. Girls are significantly more competitive than boys in the running task, while thereis no significant gender di�erence in the skipping rope task.

3.1.2. Competitiveness as a reaction to competition in math and word search

Table 3 shows average performance in the two classroom tasks under individual (piece rate)and competitive (tournament) incentives across ages, for boys and girls separately. We observe,again, that the gender gap in the first stage piece rate performance largely corresponds to thegender stereotype of the task. Girls perform significantly better in the verbal task (p < 0.05

13We have also conducted the same analysis using two measures of relative performance. We calculate theperformance change for each individual relative to the class mean, since the classmates can see each other whenthey perform in this part of the experiment. Further, older and younger children conducted di�erent types of theskipping rope tasks. This measure of relative performance change takes this into account. The second measureis the relative change in individual performance over the two rounds, i.e. (performance in round 2 - performancein round 1)/(performance in round 1). For both relative measures used, the main results are qualitatively similar(for younger children in the running task while the first relative measure of performance change is significantat conventional levels, the second relative measure is significant at 10% in a two sided test, and at 5% in a onesided test). Furthermore we have also checked if there are any di�erences in the variance of performance changebetween boys and girls. While for the running task there are no significant di�erences for any age group (p < 0.05for all pairwise comparisons, Levene’s test), in the skipping rope task girls have a significantly higher variance:at the median p = 0.033 for younger, p = 0.086 for older and p = 0.016 overall, Levene’s test. However, thisgender di�erence in variance also vanishes when we look at the relative performance change (all p > 0.5).

14In the sample of younger children, while this di�erence is not significant for boys (p = 0.378), it is marginallysignificant for girls (p = 0.073).

10

Page 14: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Math  task  

Appendix B

In this appendix I present examples of the experimental tasks used in the classroom for

the competitiveness part of the study.

Table 14 shows a few examples of the math exercises that the children solved for

various years. Children in Years 2 to 5 had only to add two 2-digit numbers, while

children in Years 6 to 7 had to both add and subtract two 2-digit numbers. Children in

Years 8 to 10 had to add and subtract in a random sequence of three 2 digit numbers.

All the numbers and mathematical operations were randomly generated to insure that

the level of di✏culty of the math task was the same throughout all the stages of the

experiment for each of the year categories.

Table 14: Examples of Math Tasks for Various Years

Years 2 to 3 Years 4 to 5 Years 6 to 7 Years 8 to 101 + 12 = . . . 82 + 18 = . . . 93 + 67 = . . . 96 + 93 + 3 = . . .

3 + 5 = . . . 48 + 10 = . . . 63 � 38 = . . . 33 � 9 � 85 = . . .

11 + 4 = . . . 47 + 14 = . . . 2 � 38 = . . . 83 + 97 + 14 = . . .

17 + 18 = . . . 39 + 6 = . . . 71 + 52 = . . . 31 � 39 + 28 = . . .

10 + 23 = . . . 68 + 16 = . . . 89 � 47 = . . . 9 � 41 � 75 = . . .

Figure 20 shows an example of the word search task that was used in the experiment.

The children had to find and circle words in any direction on a straight line. Since these

word search puzzles were not generated by a computer there might have been slight

di�erences in the di✏culty of the word search task in di�erent stages of the experiment.

We used the same puzzle within each stage of the experiment for all the children.

37

Page 15: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Word  search  task  

!"# 1

$ % & ' ( ) ( * ( + , & -

. , / & 0 - 1 2 3 2 4 25 6

7 8 25 6 9 : $ ; < = 25 > -

9 2 ? 9 ( ? 3 ! @ A B ( :

. * . 7 . ) ( * & ? ( B 0

? ( = . ? 6 & C D ; ? 2 7

. 6 . B . ? . 7 & 0 ( @ .

7 % A ) . - 2 ( 8 25 7 & C

. 7 $ = C @ : 6 % < 25 ! 25

C 9 & 7 ( ? 2 + 7 0 4 ( E

. > 25 C = $ . 25 % . . , .

< . ? F 8 - ; * / G ) & 1

25 , & D 7 . 1 . B E . * A

Page 16: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

CompeJJveness  in  Math  and  Verbal  tasks   1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

Table 3 – Individual and competitive performance in the mathematicaland verbal tasks

Math taska Verbal taskb

Age Group Gender Stage 1 Stage 2 SR test Stage 1 Stage 2 SR test N

(Ind.) (Comp.) (p≠value) (Ind.) (Comp.) (p≠value)

Gr. 2 to 6 Boys 17.44 20.83 < 0.001 5.31 5.56 0.071 224c

Girls 16.36 20.12 < 0.001 5.80 6.09 0.137 204

Gr. 7 to 10 Boys 8.12 10.32 < 0.001 9.20 9.74 0.112 164Girls 7.96 8.29 < 0.001 10.61 11.61 0.003 173

All Grades Boys 13.50 16.18 < 0.001 6.95 7.33 0.018 388Girls 12.51 15.62 < 0.001 8.01 8.62 0.001 377

aAverage number of correctly solved math exercisesbAverage number of correct words found in the word search puzzlecOne boy did not participate in the second stage of the verbal task

for all pair-wise comparisons). However, while boys solve slightly more exercises in the mathtask, this di�erence is not significant (p > 0.10 for all pairwise tests). In the second stage, whenstudents compete against each other, both boys and girls of all ages are competitive and reactto competition by increasing their performance significantly in both tasks.15

In Figure 2 we compare the change in performance between the first and the second stagesin the two classroom tasks. We find that boys and girls of all ages react similarly to competitionand improve equally in both tasks (p≠value for the whole sample, comparing boys vs girls is0.195 for math and 0.328 for the verbal task).16 Moreover, there is no di�erence in the size ofthe gender gap across age in both tasks (math: p = 0.771; word: p = 0.428).

Summarizing, we have the following result:

Result 2: Boys and girls do not di�er significantly in terms of performance change whenreacting to competition in both a math and a verbal task.

3.1.3. Competitiveness as willingness to compete in math and word search

Contrary to the two physical tasks, both the math task and the verbal task included a thirdstage, where participants could choose to perform the task under either piece rate incentivesor competitive incentives. In the math task, 54% of the boys and 52% of the girls choose tocompete, and in the verbal task the share is 57% for boys and 56% for girls. Neither gender

15In the second stage, again, boys perform better in the math task and girls perform better in the verbal task,but only in the verbal task the gender di�erence is significant (math: p = 0.384, verbal: p = 0.001).

16This finding is also robust to using relative measures of performance change. Furthermore, we have alsochecked whether there is a gender di�erences in the variance of performance change for both tasks. While girlshave a higher variance in performance change in the verbal task (all p < 0.01, Levene’s test), this di�erencevanishes when we look at relative performance change (all p > 0.19, Levene’s test).

11

Page 17: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

3.39 3.76

1.712.36

p=0.322

p=0.327

0

1

2

3

4

5

Num

ber o

f Exe

rcis

es

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

Math Task

0.25 0.290.54

0.99p=0.920

p=0.195

0

.5

1

Num

ber o

f Wor

ds

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

Verbal Task

Boys Girls

By Task, Age Group and GenderAverage Performance Change: Classroom Tasks

Figure 2. Gender and competitiveness in math and word search

di�erence is significant (math task: p = 0.708, verbal task: p = 0.697; ‰2 test for equality ofproportions).

In Figure 3 we explore competitive choices of boys and girls in each task for the two agegroups separately. While we do not find any gender di�erence within any age group, 61% ofyounger and 43% of older students choose to compete in math, and this di�erence is significant.This is also true for each gender over age (p = 0.001 for all pairwise comparisons, ‰2 test).In the verbal task, 55% of younger and 59% of older students choose to compete. Howeverwe do not find any significant gender or age di�erences in this task (p > 0.19 for all pairwisecomparisons, ‰2 test).

Summarizing, we have the following result:

Result 3: Boys and girls do not di�er significantly in their willingness to compete.

Comparing this result to those of Cardenas et al. (2012) who use similar tasks, it appearsthat Armenian children are more competitive than both Colombian and Swedish children, inparticular if we compare Armenian girls with Swedish girls. Among Colombian children, 35% ofthe boys and 32% of the girls choose to compete in the math task and 26% of the boys and 29%choose to compete in the verbal task. Among Swedish children, the corresponding numbers are44% of the boys and 19% of the girls in the math task and 39% of the boys and 27% of the girlsin the verbal task.

12

Page 18: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

No  significant  gender  gap  in  selecJon  into  compeJJon   1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

0.61 0.60

0.43 0.43

p=0.855

p=0.924

0

.2

.4

.6

.8Co

mpe

te

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

Math Task

0.56 0.530.59 0.60

p=0.519p=0.942

0

.2

.4

.6

.8

Com

pete

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

Verbal Task

Boys Girls

By Task, Age Group and GenderShare of Individuals Willing to Compete

Figure 3. Gender and the choice to compete

3.1.4. Relative performance beliefs in the math and word search

As a measure of confidence related to performance in the classroom tasks we asked the childrenand adolescents about relative performance beliefs, both in the first stage piece rate setting andin the second stage tournament setting. In contrast to some previous research (e.g., Niederleand Vesterlund, 2007) — and interesting in relation to our null results on gender di�erences incompetitiveness in the classroom tasks — we find no gender di�erences in relative performancebeliefs for either setting (piece rate math: p = 0.286; piece rate verbal: p = 0.163; tournamentmath: p = 0.594; tournament verbal: p = 0.633). Moreover, our main results pertainingto gender and competitiveness (both performance change and willingness to compete) do notchange if we control for relative performance beliefs in a parametric regression framework.17

This result is in line with that of Cardenas et al. (2012), who also did not find that performancebeliefs mattered for willingness to compete in Colombia.

17For both tasks, when regressing willingness to compete on female, age, actual performance in the tournamentand confidence as measured via performance beliefs from the tournament, performance is significant and positivewhile confidence is significant and negative. There are also no significant gender and confidence interactions, seeSupplementary Online Appendix.

13

Page 19: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

SelecJon  into  compeJJon  

•  No  gender  effect  in  the  regression  analysis  (OLS,  logit,  probit)  

•  The  only  significant  variables  are  either  performance  under  

compeJJon  or  under  piece  rate  and  risk  aversion  

Page 20: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Risk  Preferences  o  Choose  between  a  sure  amount  and  a  gamble,  10  points  if  

heads,  0  points  if  tails  o  Sure  amount  increasing  from  2  points  to  6  in  increments  of  1,  

and  then  7.5  points  o  Our    main  measure  of  risk  aversion  relies  on  the    unique  

switching  point  (certainty  equivalent  taken  as  the  midpoint  of  the  switching  interval)  

o  18%  of  subjects  are  inconsistent,  leaving  us  with  a  sample  of  624  subjects  

o  AlternaJve  measure  of  risk  taking:  number  of  risky  choices    –  Results  are  qualitaJvely  the  same  

Page 21: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Risk  Preferences    Boys  are  more  risk  taking  than  girls  .4

0.1

.2.3

Frac

tion

0 1 2 3 4 5 6Number of risky choices

Grades 2 to 6

0.1

.2.3

.4Fr

actio

n

0 1 2 3 4 5 6Number of risky choices

Grades 7 to 10

0.1

.2.3

Frac

tion

0 1 2 3 4 5 6Number of risky choices

All Grades

Number of risky options choses

0.1

.2.3

.4Fr

actio

n

2 4 6 107.5Sure amount in the switching row

Boys Girls

Grades 2 to 6

0.1

.2.3

.4Fr

actio

n

2 4 6 107.5Sure amount in the switching row

Grades 7 to 10

0.1

.2.3

Frac

tion

2 4 6 107.5Sure amount in the switching row

All Grades

Sure option in the switching row

By Risk Measure, Age Group, and GenderDistribution of Risk Preferences

Figure 1. Distribution of risk preferences

3

Page 22: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Risk  Preferences    Boys  are  more  risk  taking  than  girls  

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

2.862.52

3.24

2.48

p<0.001p=0.116

0

1

2

3

4

Aver

age

num

ber o

f ris

ky c

hoic

es

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

By Age Group and GenderRisk Preferences

Boys Girls

Figure 4. Gender di�erences in risk preferences

3.2. Risk preferences

In this section we test whether there are gender di�erences in risk preferences in our sample.We measure risk preferences from six incentivized choices in the classroom. In our sample of762 students that took part in the risk task, about 19% are inconsistent in their choices, i.e.have multiple switching points. (There is no gender di�erence in being inconsistent in the wholesample, or any of the age groups considered: all p > 0.25, ‰2 equality of proportions test, seeSupplementary Online Appendix.) Inconsistent participants are excluded if we look only atthe unique switching point. We therefore use the number of risky options chosen as our mainmeasure of risk preferences. We find that, on average, boys choose the risky option significantlymore often than girls (boys: 3.02, girls: 2.50, p < 0.001). On average boys make 21% morerisky choices compared to girls.

In Figure 4 we display gender di�erences in risk preferences in the two age groups separately.While boys make significantly more risky choices than girls do, this gender di�erence doesnot reach statistical significance for younger children. It appears that in our sample genderdi�erences in risk taking arise around the age of puberty, and this is mainly due to increasedrisk taking of boys with age, see also Table 7 in Appendix C. Using the other risk measurebased on the unique switching point, and thus excluding inconsistent participants, our resultsare qualitatively the same. Testing whether the gender gap in risk taking is larger among olderchildren than younger ones, we find that while this di�erence is marginally significant with ourmain measure of risk (p = 0.06), it is significant for the other measure that excludes inconsistentparticipants (p = 0.04). However, we should also note that the distribution of risk preferencesis remarkably di�erent across our age groups, see Supplementary Online Appendix.

Summarizing, we have the following result:Result 4: Boys are more risk taking than girls, and this gender gap gets bigger in adoles-

cence.

14

Page 23: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Altruism  from  DG  Girls  donate  9%  more  on  average  then  boys  do,  but  doesn’t  vary  with  age  

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

50.58

58.88

52.70

61.66

p=0.007 p=0.014

30

40

50

60

70Av

erag

e po

ints

giv

en to

cha

rity

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

By Age Group and GenderAltruistic Preferences

Boys Girls

Figure 5. Gender di�erences in altruism

These results on risk preferences are in line with those of Cardenas et al. (2012), who findthat Colombian boys on average take 40% more risk than girls, with the corresponding numberin Sweden being 15%.

3.3. Altruism

In this section we look at gender di�erences in altruism as measured via donations in a dictatorgame. We find that girls give significantly more than boys (p < 0.001), and this is the case forboth age groups, see Figure 5. Girls donate on average 60.16 points whereas boys donate 51.48points out of 100 to the charity organization (an orphanage). The modal allocation is the 50–50split for the sample as a whole, as well as for both genders and age groups.

Testing whether the gender gap in altruism is bigger among the older students, we do notfind a significant di�erence (p = 0.89). Hence, we have the following result:

Result 5: Girls are significantly more altruistic than boys.

3.4. The relationship between competitiveness, risk preferences, and altruism

We also explore the relationship between competitiveness, risk taking, and altruism, since thethree areas often are related, yet are three separate concepts, and gender di�erences are oftenfound in all three preferences. A correlation analysis between all the behaviors we examineshows that while competitiveness and risk taking are related, our measure of altruism is neitherrelated to competitiveness nor to risk taking, see Supplementary Online Appendix.18

Like many others, e.g. Niederle and Vesterlund (2007); Niederle and Yestrumskas (2008),we find a positive relationship between risk taking and willingness to compete in math and

18We have also tested whether the distributions of donations di�er between those who choose to compete andthose who do not, across tasks, age and gender. For both tasks none of these (pairwise) comparisons is significant,all p > 0.20.

15

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65

50.58

58.88

52.70

61.66

p=0.007 p=0.014

30

40

50

60

70

Aver

age

poin

ts g

iven

to c

harit

y

Grades 2 to 6 Grades 7 to 10

By Age Group and GenderAltruistic Preferences

Boys Girls

Figure 5. Gender di�erences in altruism

These results on risk preferences are in line with those of Cardenas et al. (2012), who findthat Colombian boys on average take 40% more risk than girls, with the corresponding numberin Sweden being 15%.

3.3. Altruism

In this section we look at gender di�erences in altruism as measured via donations in a dictatorgame. We find that girls give significantly more than boys (p < 0.001), and this is the case forboth age groups, see Figure 5. Girls donate on average 60.16 points whereas boys donate 51.48points out of 100 to the charity organization (an orphanage). The modal allocation is the 50–50split for the sample as a whole, as well as for both genders and age groups.

Testing whether the gender gap in altruism is bigger among the older students, we do notfind a significant di�erence (p = 0.89). Hence, we have the following result:

Result 5: Girls are significantly more altruistic than boys.

3.4. The relationship between competitiveness, risk preferences, and altruism

We also explore the relationship between competitiveness, risk taking, and altruism, since thethree areas often are related, yet are three separate concepts, and gender di�erences are oftenfound in all three preferences. A correlation analysis between all the behaviors we examineshows that while competitiveness and risk taking are related, our measure of altruism is neitherrelated to competitiveness nor to risk taking, see Supplementary Online Appendix.18

Like many others, e.g. Niederle and Vesterlund (2007); Niederle and Yestrumskas (2008),we find a positive relationship between risk taking and willingness to compete in math and

18We have also tested whether the distributions of donations di�er between those who choose to compete andthose who do not, across tasks, age and gender. For both tasks none of these (pairwise) comparisons is significant,all p > 0.20.

15

Page 24: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Summary  of  Results  

o  Girls  are  significantly  more  compeJJve  than  boys  in  a  running  task  

o  In  all  other  tasks  and  measures,  girls  and  boys  are  equally  compeJJve  o  Boys  and  girls  choose  to  compete  at  equal  rates  in  either  task  

o  Boys  are  more  risk  taking  than  girls  and  the  gender  gap  appears  around  the  age  of  puberty  

o  Boys  are  less  altruisJc  than  girls  o  Culture  and  context  play  even  a  bigger  role  than  iniJally  

expected,  and  not  always  in  predictable  ways  

 

Page 25: Gender and Preferences at a Young Age: Evidence from Armenia

Thank  You!