11
 Rethinking the  Ausbau   Abstand  dichotomy into a continuous and multivariate system JOSHUA A. FISHMAN Abstract There are several advantages to reconceptualizing Heinz Kloss’s dichotomy ‘‘Ausbau  vs.  Abstand’’ as separate continuous variables, only the former of which (Ausbau) is needed for corpus planning studies. Such a reconceptu- alization would not only be preferable from the point of view of measuring the connectedness between degree of  Ausbauization  and various population attitudes and academy implementation successes, but also to furthering con- tinuous variable measurement in general sociolinguistics and to appreciat- ing the co-presence possibilities of various other measures of corpus plan- ning e¤orts that have most recently been introduced. One of the greatest  gifts of Heinz Kloss to macro-sociolinguistics was the formulation of a po- lar opposite distinction between any two contrasted languages or varieties such that they are each either (a) easily recognized (i.e., judged) to be  functionally independent from one another due to the major and natural (that is, not man-made) di¤erences have already transpired between them, referred to as being ‘‘independent by  Abstand,’’ on the one hand, or (b) those whose functional independence is recognized only as a result of the human e¤ort that has been expended in order to make them appear su‰- ciently di¤erent from one another, so that the smaller and weaker of the two can be recognize d as inde pe ndent fr om the larger and st ronger one and, therefore, referred to as being ‘‘independent by  Ausbau.’’ Thus, the major theoretical contribution of Kloss, initially made well before his Nazi days (see Hutton 1999) was essentially a tripartite one, 1 one which was si- multaneously (1) judgmental and perspectival, (2) focused upon the impor- tance of org ani zed human interv ent ion into the nat ural lan guage- cha nge  processes in order that prestige- and power-related societal functions for, as well as the independence of, the contextually weaker variety were to have a chance of being recognized at all, and (3) preoccupied with ‘‘dialect avoidance e¤orts’’ more generally as a major desideratum governing inter- varie tal compariso ns in the realm of social powe r. The above-men tion ed tri- 0165–2516/08/0191–0017  Int’l . J. Soc. Lang. 191 (2008 ), pp. 17–26 6 Walter de Gruyter DOI 10.1515/IJSL.2008.022

Fishman Ausbau Abstand Revisited 08

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

s

Citation preview

  • Rethinking the AusbauAbstand dichotomyinto a continuous and multivariate system

    JOSHUA A. FISHMAN

    Abstract

    There are several advantages to reconceptualizing Heinz Klosss dichotomy

    Ausbau vs. Abstand as separate continuous variables, only the former ofwhich (Ausbau) is needed for corpus planning studies. Such a reconceptu-

    alization would not only be preferable from the point of view of measuring

    the connectedness between degree of Ausbauization and various population

    attitudes and academy implementation successes, but also to furthering con-

    tinuous variable measurement in general sociolinguistics and to appreciat-

    ing the co-presence possibilities of various other measures of corpus plan-

    ning eorts that have most recently been introduced. One of the greatest

    gifts of Heinz Kloss to macro-sociolinguistics was the formulation of a po-

    lar opposite distinction between any two contrasted languages or varieties

    such that they are each either (a) easily recognized (i.e., judged) to be

    functionally independent from one another due to the major and natural

    (that is, not man-made) dierences have already transpired between them,

    referred to as being independent by Abstand, on the one hand, or (b)

    those whose functional independence is recognized only as a result of the

    human eort that has been expended in order to make them appear su-

    ciently dierent from one another, so that the smaller and weaker of the

    two can be recognized as independent from the larger and stronger one

    and, therefore, referred to as being independent by Ausbau. Thus, the

    major theoretical contribution of Kloss, initially made well before his Nazi

    days (see Hutton 1999) was essentially a tripartite one,1 one which was si-

    multaneously (1) judgmental and perspectival, (2) focused upon the impor-

    tance of organized human intervention into the natural language-change

    processes in order that prestige- and power-related societal functions for,

    as well as the independence of, the contextually weaker variety were to

    have a chance of being recognized at all, and (3) preoccupied with dialect

    avoidance eorts more generally as a major desideratum governing inter-

    varietal comparisons in the realm of social power. The above-mentioned tri-

    01652516/08/01910017 Intl. J. Soc. Lang. 191 (2008), pp. 17266 Walter de Gruyter DOI 10.1515/IJSL.2008.022

  • partite considerations may be highly interrelated but they are, nevertheless,

    suciently independent from one another to also be considered separately,

    rather than necessarily only jointly. Finally, Ausbau and Abstand, rather

    than being totally separate, are also independently related to various yet

    other language planning dimensions and it is these last-mentioned relation-

    ships that actually disclose the major importance of Ausbau to the entire

    language planning venture in various polities (Schiman 1996; Kalogjera

    1985).

    1. What is the opposite of Ausbau?

    One of the conceptual characteristics of dichotomies is that both of their

    alternatives must be on the same dimension. However, rightly conceived,

    Ausbau and Abstand are not really on one and the same dimension. If Aus-bau signies building away eorts in corpus planning (or as I once ten-

    tatively renamed them: the concern for fostering dissimilarity-focused

    interventions in corpus planning) and if such eorts are conceptualized

    as being located at one end of a dimension, than the truly opposite end

    of that same dimension should by rights be termed Einbau, the concern

    for fostering similarity-focused emphases, instead of Klosss proposed

    Abstand. Indeed, the latter term, Abstand, is entirely unneeded in any lan-

    guage planning typology because it lacks any reference to human agency.While the building (-bau) emphasis that we are accustomed to ex-

    pect at one end of a human-engineered distancing dimension is needed,

    indeed, it manifests itself in many dierent ways (see below), but Ab-

    stand per se is not one of them. As the two morphs of its German name

    (ab-/stand ) imply, its rightful meaning is merely state or condition of

    separation and neither the directional, nor the authoritative human

    agency, nor the amount or degree of distancing that are matters of su-

    preme concern for language planning are at all properly identied by theterm. Abstand merely connotes easily and often already recognized dis-

    tancing between two varieties and, therefore, actually not a distancing

    that needs to be fostered by language planning eorts. Perhaps our own

    relatively poor German and the masterful reputation of Kloss, both as a

    typologist (with a particular fondness for dichotomies) and as a German-

    ist, have long misled us into not recognizing the logical inconsistency into

    which he misled us by stressing both Ausbau and Abstand as if they were

    two equally prominent and legitimately opposite ends of one and thesame dimension.

    18 J. A. Fishman

  • 2. The usefulness and the quantiability of dichotomous categories

    As our experience with societal data grows and develops (i.e., with the

    data of societal behavior through and toward language/languages or,

    more generally, variety/varieties), the more researchers will recognize the

    inherent problems surrounding true dichotomies. If these problems are

    not consensually solved by the investigators themselves, there is alwaysthe possibility of conscious or unconscious errors of grouping. Dichoto-

    mies are rarely natural or true in nature and the more we examine

    those few that have withstood the stresses of time the more we recognize

    that even these few constitute vis-a`-vis more precise data of the world per

    se that. If male/female can be further analyzable into degrees of

    maleness and degrees of femaleness (as certain psychological tests cur-

    rently claim and demonstrate), then certainly this is equally true with re-

    spect to degrees of ausbauness and degrees of inbauness. In principle,we should be able to entertain the question of whether a certain Flemish

    text and a contrasted Nederlands text are closer together or further apart

    than another or than a certain Nynorsk text and a Bokmaal text that we

    may want to compare to them.

    This is certainly a dierent kind of question (and a far more dicult

    one) than we could previously have entertained using the Klossian (or

    Klossian-type) designations of whether Ausbau or Einbau were present in

    either of the above cases. Furthermore, using these designations as con-tinuous measures also alerts us to their essential combinability pertaining

    to any particular text, rather than only to their inherent opposition. There

    is no good reason why any particular passage cannot reveal both certain

    Ausbau and certain Einbau characteristics. Even if this were not possible

    for any particular subset of the same few words (a possibility that we will

    need to leave for later), it is eminently possible for successive clauses, not

    to mention sentences. Although whole variety characterizations may

    still be operative for folk-linguistic or popular political purposes, theyare certainly not the only ones that are permitted to exist (that is, they

    are not necessary restrictions that must obtain even for research purposes)

    and this represents a welcome expansion of opportunity for the textual

    microanalyst in sociolinguistics. Such expansion would be inherently sim-

    ilar in type to questions being pursued in connection with authorship and

    provenance of texts (Is it likely that the same author wrote both of these

    two texts? is a question not unlike Is it likely that both of these excerpts

    stem from the same variety or Ur-text?).

    Rethinking the AusbauAbstand dichotomy 19

  • 3. Can opposite directions in language planning be implemented on the

    very same dimension?

    As is evident in much other human behavior, so also sociolinguistic be-

    havior reveals the co-presence of opposite or opposing tendencies within

    the same text or within the same variety. This is even more so (although

    by no means inevitably so) when we combine both attitudinal and linguis-tic data. Advocates of the opposition to New High Germanisms within

    the Yiddish fold (among the Avek fun daytsh Yiddishists such as M.

    Weinreich 1980 [1973] and M. Schaechter 1986) will still permit them-

    selves and others to utilize the greeting a freylekhn shabes a happy Sab-

    bath, although its rst component may be questionable because it is rec-

    ognizably of German origin. Yiddish ausbauists who would cringe at

    geburtstog (favoring, instead, the more home-grown geboyrn-tog) usually

    see nothing at all objectionable about a freylekhn shabes. The same istrue of Swiss-German ausbauists who eschew Germanisms in their Ro-

    mansh when confronted with multi-componential expressions such as

    Merci viel mal. Foreign-markedness is not all of one piece of cloth; some

    swatches of taboo-origin clear through the self-imposed censorship of

    the standard language border regulators much more easily and quickly

    than do others. Ultimately, any policy must be implemented at the micro-

    level and not just at the macro-level, as Klossians want to do.

    However, some combination of possible opposites is even more ger-mane to any discussion of Ausbauization when it is reviewed at a more

    macro-level, although the implications and consequences of such combi-

    nations may well be recognizable at many low levels as well. At the

    more macro-levels, the speech communitys awareness of the functional

    dierentiation of language varieties is usually present at a more conscious

    level. Varieties in contact often have separate names and this builds col-

    lective consciousness about them and their functionally proper or more

    appropriate use(s). Nevertheless, notwithstanding the eorts of teachers,editors, proof-readers, and other gatekeepers and name-givers, it is in the

    nature of diglossic arrangements to display more or less leakage and

    the functional border area(s) between the varieties in contact are the ones

    most commonly aected thereby. In a period of social tension and social

    stress, should a folksy letter between relatives, both of whom moved from

    a common rural to separate urban areas decades ago, and who now al-

    most never interact face-to-face but who used to always do so in Land-

    smaal when they were youngsters, now be in Landsmaal or in Ryksmaal,and if in Landsmaal, should it now be in the variety orthographically

    most distant from Ryksmaal or in a more proximate one? There are all

    kinds of written Landsmaal (or Ryksmaal) still in use and the exact ones

    20 J. A. Fishman

  • to use between two individuals are often inuenced by social variables,

    including education, income, and socially patterned and interpreted indi-

    vidual intentions. Thus, Ausbauization becomes a sliding scale that each

    individual may try to adjust, upwards or downwards, both in individual

    and societally meaningful (and meaningfully interpreted) ways. Being

    matters of degree, they can also be regarded as indicative of degrees of

    social acceptance between two interlocutors. However, matters of degreeare manifestly unrecognized (or under-recognized) if measured only via

    dichotomies and can come into their own only when continuous measure-

    ment is employed.

    4. Internal and external boundary setting

    Where to draw the line between Ausbau and Einbau eorts may be noeasier than drawing the line between any two other varieties that are on

    a cline. If and when such lines succeed in being generally accepted for

    decades or centuries, then they may seem valid enough to most of those

    involved in upholding them. The line between Asturian and Castillian

    is a very delicate matter, but it seems clearer and realer to many

    viewers now than it did until quite recently (Moreno-Fernandez 2007)

    and, as expected, this is done partially along self-interest lines, as is often

    the case when continua must be replaced by rigid boundedness. However,now that Madrid has recognized the co-ocial nature of Asturian (2007),

    the recognizability of the boundary between them (Castillian and Astur-

    ian) may take on somewhat greater clarity (not to say urgency) to larger

    numbers of interlocutors, just as the usage line between Valencian and

    Catalan may have faded somewhat due to recent authoritative an-

    nouncements that they are both merely dialects of the same Catalan

    language. This would lead us to expect that Ausbau eorts between the

    two will diminish in number and intensity from the Asturian side, just asEinbau circumstances and eorts between them may come to predomi-

    nate, at least in certain functions and in certain speech networks on the

    Castillian side.

    The recognition and accentuation of Ausbauization is also a factor that

    operates in the coming into being of new languages, although it has not

    yet received its due in that connection. The birth and death of lan-

    guages may go a long way toward explaining why more languages

    seem to be dying than ever before, at the same time that more languagesthan ever before are constantly being added to the language lists main-

    tained by SIL and other gatekeepers to language status. The very fact

    that entire families of languages have achieved literacy at roughly the

    Rethinking the AusbauAbstand dichotomy 21

  • same time (Fishman forthcoming) gives testimony to the fact that literacy

    may also be an areal phenomenon that spreads and has already long

    spread by interactional contagion (via religious, economic, and political

    bacilli). The latter may well have been fostered by Ausbau attitudes and

    sentiments that have marched across Europe, from West to East, from

    the fourteenth to the eighteenth and even into the nineteenth and twenti-

    eth centuries. Einbauization has been a much rarer phenomenon in Eu-rope, noted only in the Flemish/Netherlandish case with respect to liter-

    acy, the Landsmaal/Ryksmaal case (via the requirement that all new

    words in either be introduced as Sammorsk), and in the Moldavian/

    Rumanian case, but may yet have greater appeal on other continents

    (particularly in Africa and Latin America) where the extension of literacy

    and ocialization to more indigenous languages has only recently begun

    to gather steam and may encourage the amalgamation of smaller entities.

    Ultimately, some of the latter may need to institute self-saving Einbauiza-tion toward some of their own more minor varieties. Thus, Einbauization

    versus Ausbauization are by no means steady states, xed forever and a

    day, but, rather, distinctions between varieties that can be in fairly con-

    stant tension as sociocultural change rises and falls in conjunction with

    turbulence within and between their speech communities. That being

    said, the spread of English and of Islam (and, subsequently, of spoken

    Mandarin within the Sinosphere) will probably serve to make for more

    structural and functional similarities across hitherto dissimilar languagesthan Englishes alone would have done. The permissiveness of the En-

    glish error (i.e., the development of non-native written Englishes) not

    only encourages minority-language continuity, but new variety birth and

    stabilization as well.

    5. Ausbauization and diglossia

    Since I myself have been responsible for a good bit of the constant discus-

    sion concerning the role of diglossia in connection with the stabilization

    (or the destabilization) of the entire system of language-use balances and

    boundaries that are intricately involved in Ausbau/Einbau distinctions, a

    few words might well be pardoned here about diglossia in connection

    with them. Ausbauization sometimes grows into and fosters a diglossic ar-

    rangement between the varieties that are involved. This arrangement is

    very likely to come into being whenever the two varieties have co-existedwithin the same speech community for centuries, whether they are now

    considered related languages or not. If we add a stress on functional spe-

    cialization to the expected stresses on structural distancing that has gone

    22 J. A. Fishman

  • on over many years (particularly vis-a`-vis literacy and other statusful pur-

    suits in the religion and power domains of the culture), then we have ac-

    counted for the major dierentia that can and do often make for diglos-

    sia. When both Ausbauization and diglossia co-exist, the latter is bound

    to be the older of the two since it requires a longer historical co-presence

    between its constituents, a characteristic that structural distancing both

    intensies and is fostered by in turn. Thus, Ausbauization neither requiresnor fosters diglossia, but diglossia requires and fosters both and does so

    over a considerable length of time. Both are found to obtain rst among

    higher social circles and more prestigeful text-proximate pursuits and ac-

    tors. As a result, the U (upper) variety may have little if any inuence on

    the more informal varieties of speech and the speech networks restricted

    to them. This may very well indicate that we can look forward to the fu-

    ture continuumization of diglossia, as ever larger proportions of societies

    the world over become literate. This will not necessarily spell the end ei-ther of diglossia or of Ausbauization, but will more denitely make either

    one harder to attain or maintain.

    6. Other polarities involved in the total language planning process

    Ausbauization and Einbauization are opposite poles (A and Ei) on the

    structural distancing dimension within the total status and corpus plan-ning enterprise. As Figure 1 reveals, there are other distancing dimensions

    as well, some of a more specic nature and others of a more general kind.

    Ausbauization (along the dimension parallel to the base line) marks, as

    we have seen, attempts to escape from perceived excessive structural

    similarity that makes the weaker party appear to be merely a dialect for

    those who do not know its true merits, which are already known to

    language activist insiders. A slightly dierent concern preoccupies those

    that pursue Uniqueness (U maximal distancing from any and allother languages that, nevertheless, retains its structural harmony with

    previous word formation traditions), a route which led Estonian to an

    ex nihilo method of creating new words out of nonsense syllables during

    the 1920s, a view that necessarily distanced Estonian from Finnish and

    the other members of the Finno-Ugric family of languages. Most practi-

    tioners of Purity (Pu) planning, however, are less traumatized by any

    and all similarities to other varieties than they are by historically ac-

    knowledged borrowings from those languages/varieties that they havelong struggled with, e.g., as French has with English or (in former centu-

    ries) with German. Finally, we must also mention Classicization (C),

    a retreat into a genuine (or, in the cases of Pannication (Pa), into a

    Rethinking the AusbauAbstand dichotomy 23

  • mythic) great tradition, such as will cause other related languages/

    varieties to envy and to dream about those who have had the good luck

    to have been tempted by the siren songs of Illyria, of Malphilindo, or of

    Dravidistan, or of other similar mirages. A classic past that the wholeworld admires and treasures is too much of a prize for a small or now rel-

    atively forgotten people to give up, even though it may pose endless prob-

    lems and extra burdens for those who elect it is as their path to modern-

    ization via language planning. The simultaneous application of all eight

    dimensions (including both distancing and approximation) is uncommon

    but not impossible in accord with the model illustrated here.

    7. Closing observations

    When viewed as but one member of a club of alternative language plan-

    ning routes to modernization via withdrawal or distancing, it becomes

    easier to recognize Ausbauization as a continuum (a solution of degree)

    rather than as part of a dichotomy, such as Klosss model originally sug-

    gested. Ausbauization may be furthered via purication eorts, or via

    uniqueness eorts or via classicization eorts, all of which provide dis-tancing with a specic direction, avor, and historical justication. After

    all, distancing must have a rationale, rather than being considered as

    providing its own automatic choices among alternatives. The latter

    Dimensions: A(usbau)E(inbau); U(niqueness)Westernization; P(urication)

    V(ernacularization); C(lassicization)P(annication). Poles in italics are

    distancing.

    Figure 1. Visual portrayal of one members views of the proper goals of the language plan-

    ning agency for language X (from Fishman 2006: 115)

    24 J. A. Fishman

  • choices are derived from the fears, grievances, deprivations, insults, and

    injuries that are the normal lot in life of all small and late-modernizing

    languages that are widely viewed as being excessively and suspiciously

    structurally similar to their larger and more powerful neighbors. In mod-

    ern times, this has been the disease for which language planning (and

    even linguistics more generally) has been called upon to devise therapeu-

    tic approaches. The need for converting dichotomies into continua, forcombining approaches that aim in disparate or in roughly similar direc-

    tions, for coping with the combinability of and reversibility of human mo-

    tives, and for a greater mastery of quantitative methods among research-

    ers should make the future of corpus planning research (and of language

    planning practice) considerably dierent and, hopefully, more eective, in

    the future than it has been in the past. The continuumization of the tradi-

    tionally dichotomous Ausbau-related dimension will also render Ausbau-

    ization more relevant to other major sociolinguistic topics from which ithas thus far been unnecessarily separated (e.g., language birth, language

    death, language change, language policy, language politics, language dis-

    tance, etc., all have already been conceptualized as continua in earlier

    years).

    Stanford University

    Notes

    1. Heinz Kloss went to considerable pains, several times, to convince me and Uriel Wein-

    reich that he had never been a member of or sympathizer with the Nazi party. Even his

    admitted membership in the German armed forces during World War II was purport-

    edly as an ambulance driver and even that was involuntary. Huttons more recent reve-

    lations (1999) conrm those of a number of other post-War scholars concerning the po-

    liticized views and self-serving eorts of various German language specialists during the

    years of Nazi rule. Kloss was, in many ways, a very ne scholar and his case is instruc-

    tive for sociolinguists even today. More than most other language scholars, we run the

    risk of being co-opted by dominant ideologies that may be able to facilitate our careers

    and research eorts. I am not at all sure how many of us could resist such temptation;

    obviously Kloss could not.

    Bibliography and references

    Alexander, Neville (2004). The politics of language planning in post-Apartheid Africa. Lan-

    guage Problems and Language Planning 28, 113130.

    Beswick, Jaime (2002). Galician language planning and implications for regional identity

    restoration or elimination. National Identities 4, 282271.

    Rethinking the AusbauAbstand dichotomy 25

  • Bugarski, Ranko (2004). Language policies in the successor states of the former Yugoslavia.

    Journal of Politics 3, 189207.

    Deumert, A.; and Vandenbusche, Wim (2003). Research Directions in the Study of Lan-

    guage Standardization: Germanic Standardizations. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John

    Benjamins.

    Fernandes, Julio Viejo (2004). Asturian resurgence and impending demise of a minority lan-

    guage in the Iberian Peninsula. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 170,

    141189.

    Fishman, Joshua A. (2004). Yiddish and German: an on-again, o-again relationship. In

    Globalization and the Future of German, A. Gardt and B. Huppauf (eds.), 213227. Berlin

    and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Fishman, Joshua A. (2006). Do Not Leave Your Language Alone: The Hidden Status Agen-

    das Within Corpus Planning in Language Policy. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Fishman, Joshua A. (forthcoming). The rise of vernacular literacies in Europe. In The Birth

    and Death of European Vernacular Literacies.

    Ford, Curtis (2002). Language planning in Bosnia and Herzogovina: The 1998 Bihac Sym-

    posium. Slavic and Eastern European Journal 46, 361394.

    Greenberg, Robert (2006). Language and Identity in the Balkans: Serbo-Croatian and its Dis-

    integration. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Hutton, Christopher M. (1999). Linguistics and the Third Reich: Mother Tongue, Fascism,

    Race and the Science of Language. London: Routledge.

    Kalogjera, Damir (1985). Attitudes toward Serbo-Croatian language varieties. International

    Journal of the Sociology of Language 52, 93110.

    Khubchandani, Lachman (2000). English snowakes in Indian tropics. South Asian Lan-

    guage Review 6, 8496.

    Kloss, Heinz (1978). Die Entwicklung neuer germanischer Kultursprachen. Dusseldorf:

    Schwann (the authors own translation and revision of the much earlier German original).

    Moreno-Fernandez, Francisco (ed.) (2007). Regional varieties of Spanish on the Iberian

    Peninsula. International Journal of the Sociology of Language 184.

    Romaine, Suzanne (1996). Pidgins and Creoles as literary languages: Ausbau and Abstand.

    In Contrastive Sociolinguistics, Marlis Killinger and Ulrich Ammon (eds.), 271289. Ber-

    lin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.

    Schaechter, Mordkhe (1986). Laytish mame-loshn (Cultivated Mother Tongue). New York:

    Yidish-lige.

    Schiman, Harold (1996). Linguistic Culture and Language Policy. New York: Routledge.

    Trudgill, Peter (1992). Ausbau sociolinguistics and the perception of language status in con-

    temporary Europe. International Journal of Applied Linguistics 2, 167171.

    Weinreich, Max (1980 [1973]). The History of Yiddish. Chicago: University of Chicago Press

    (Four volumes in the Yiddish original and one in the English translation, corresponding

    to the rst two Yiddish ones).

    Weinstein, Brian (1979). Language strategists: redening political frontiers on the basis of

    linguistic choice. World Politics 3, 345364.

    26 J. A. Fishman