Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositional "need to evaluate" on N400 (ERP) measures of automatic evaluative

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    1/21

    Experts with a cause 1

    Running head: AUTOMATIC EVALUATIVE PROCESSING IN POLITICAL EXPERTS

    Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise

    and the dispositional need to evaluate on N400 measures of automatic evaluative processing

    Brian C. Woolfrey

    University of Minnesota

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    2/21

    Experts with a cause 2

    Abstract

    In 2003 Morris and colleagues conducted an ERP study of automatic evaluative processing in

    which they found that N400 responses during a lexical decision task were significantly

    attenuated when a target adjective was affectively congruent with a political prime word. Using

    this modified N400 attenuation paradigm as a psychophysiological measure of automatic

    evaluative processing, I propose to further study the relationship between political expertise, the

    dispositional need to evaluate, and the tendency to automatically evaluate political stimuli.

    Specifically, I predict to find an interactive effect betweenpolitical expertise (PE) and the need

    to evaluate (NE) on ERP measures of automatic evaluative processing, just as Federico and

    colleagues (2007) found an interactive effect between PE and NE on measures of ideological

    constraint. If so, this would support the idea that automatic processing tendencies may be one of

    the mechanisms behind the ideological organization of political information in high-NE political

    experts.

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    3/21

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    4/21

    Experts with a cause 4

    Awareness of this highly asymmetric distribution of ideological thinking inevitably led

    researchers to wonder what it was about political involvement that seemed to facilitate the use of

    ideology. One of the most popular answers to this question was that it may have something to do

    with the amount of political information one has access to. People who are more involved in

    politics are naturally exposed to more political information than people who are less involved in

    politics. Many of them are even described by Krosnick (1990) as political "information junkies"

    who spend a great deal of time seeking out and reflecting on political information. As

    consequence, they usually have more complex and well-developed political schemas, or

    "organized clusters of information about political institutions, actors and abstract political ideas",

    from which they can draw to use as context when making political decisions (Federico, 2007,

    222). In psychological literature, this is referred to as political "expertise", and can be measured

    fairly accurately by simple tests of relevant political-domain knowledge (Krosnick 1990).

    Because involvement in politics is closely associated with levels of political expertise,

    many researchers were not surprised to find that, like political involvement, political knowledge

    is also positively correlated with ideological constraint (Zaller, 1992). What's more, this

    relationship was found to exist regardless of a person's actual level of political involvement.

    Because of this, it was speculated that the acquisition of political information may be a key

    variable in the development of ideological attitudes. According to this conceptualization,

    acquiring new political information may inevitably facilitate the use of ideological thinking

    because complex social information is cognitively organized in terms of schemas, and ideology

    is the natural of schema by which political information is organized. Evidence to support this

    idea comes from Zaller (1992) who showed that the mere contemplation of a political issue

    before making a political decision was more likely to increase ideological constraint in political

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    5/21

    Experts with a cause 5

    experts than political novices. Among other things, this suggests that the development of

    ideological constraint may be "contingent on the complexity of political knowledge structures"

    (Federico, 2007).

    The problem with this idea, however, however, is that not allpolitical experts are prone

    to think of political issues in ideological terms. Although the correlation between political

    expertise and ideological consistency is moderate to strong, it is by no means perfect. Some

    professors of philosophy, for example, boast an intimate understanding of abstract political ideas

    but at the same time refuse to take an ideological stand on many political issues. Although they

    are certainly a minority among their learned peers, low-constraint political experts do exist. As

    such, it does not seem very likely that the acquisition of political information alone is responsible

    for the facilitation of ideologically-organized thinking. Rather, it may be more likely that

    political expertise is a necessary but not fully sufficient condition for ideological thought.

    What is it then, if not political expertise alone, that facilitates the use of ideology? Recent

    findings by Federico and colleagues (2007) suggest that the answer to this question may have

    something to do with motivation. Specifically, he examined a motivational personality factor that

    had been shown to be moderately correlated to political activism called "need to evaluate" (NE).

    Originally developed by Jarvis and Petty (1996), the need to evaluate is a construct that reflects

    the tendency of a person to spontaneously evaluate objects or experiences in terms of good or

    bad. People who score high on the NE scale typically experience many "evaluative thoughts" and

    hold relatively strong attitudes toward a variety of objects, people and ideas (Bizer, 2004). This

    includes everything from public policies that directly affect a person's life to relatively remote

    and inconsequential things such as abstract art. In an extensive analysis of National Elections

    Study (NES) data, Federico found that this trait has a robust mediating effect on the relationship

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    6/21

    Experts with a cause 6

    between political expertise and attitude constraint. Political experts tended to score higher on

    measures of ideological constraint overall, but this effect was limited to only those experts who

    scored high in measures of NE. Such an interaction between NE and political expertise is highly

    consistent with the idea that the acquisition of political information itself is not sufficient to

    facilitate ideological thinking, but instead may function as a fuel of sorts that interacts with

    personality or external motivational factors to facilitate ideological thinking.

    If so, this may suggest that political experts who have a high level of evaluative

    motivation, such as party elites, may actually process political information in a fundamentally

    different way than their low NE counterparts. One distinction that has been made between high

    and low NE individuals in previous literature involves attitude formation styles. Specifically,

    recent findings suggest that high-NE individuals tend to form their attitudes in an "on-line"

    fashion, whereas low-NE individuals tend to form their attitudes in a more "memory-based" way.

    On-line attitudes, as first described by Hastie and Park (1986), supposedly result from a process

    in which an individual spontaneously evaluates each new piece of information and integrates

    each of those evaluations into an overall working tally of evaluations. When prompted for an

    opinion, this working tally is simply reported as the final attitude. Memory-based attitude

    formation, in contrast, involves the relative deferral of judgment until a judgment is called for

    and requires on-the-spot integration of immediate context and relevant information that a person

    is able to recall. Based on this theory, we would expect that people who form their opinions in an

    on-line fashion would be able to report their attitudes or evaluations more quickly upon request

    than people who use memory-based attitude formation strategies. Indeed, this was exactly the

    type of distinction that was originally used by Hastie and Park (1986) as evidence that individual

    differences in processing styles exist. Thus, when Jarvis and Petty (1996) found that high-NE

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    7/21

    Experts with a cause 7

    participants reported evaluations of everyday objects significantly faster than low-NE

    participants, they reasonably concluded that high trait NE is associated with a more on-line

    process of attitude formation and low trait NE is associated with a more memory-based process

    (as cited in Bizer, 2004).

    Further research by Hermans, Houwer, and Eelen (2001) suggest that these apparent

    differences in attitudinal formation styles may be related to differences in "automatic" processing

    tendencies. In this study, high-NE participants identified words to be affectively congruent with a

    positive or negative prime word faster than they identified words to be incongruent with the

    prime word. Low-NE participants, however, showed no statistical difference in the time it took to

    identify the congruency of the words pairs. This evidence ofaffective priming in response to

    semantic stimuli lends strong support to the idea that high-NE individuals may process affective

    information in a more "automatic" way than low-NE individuals. According to this line of

    thinking, otherwise known as the "hot cognition" hypothesis, all socio-political concepts are

    coded into long-term memory with a corresponding positive or negative affective "charge" that is

    quickly and automatically activated upon any future presentation of that same stimuli (Morris,

    2003). Thus, people who tend to chronically evaluate incoming stimuli should possess

    particularly strong evaluative associations. Moreover, in a decision-making scenario in which

    multiple pieces of information are presented to a high-NE individual, each new piece of

    information is thought to automatically activate an entire network of evaluative associations. If

    two or more of these networks happen to contain the same attitudinal valence nodes (e.g. "good"

    or "bad"), these overlapping valences are strengthened and primed through converging

    activation. It is through this cognitive mechanism by which an evaluative "tally" may be kept

    during on-line evaluative processing. Additionally, this redundant strengthening and priming of

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    8/21

    Experts with a cause 8

    evaluative judgments may offer a plausible explanation as to why Lodge and Taber (2000) found

    that affective true/false judgments were made about twice as fast as more "cognitive" true/false

    judgments regarding a hypothetical congressman (as cited in Morris, 2003), and why Bizer

    (2006) found that attitudes created through on-line processing were stronger and more resilient

    than attitudes created through memory-based processing.

    More importantly, this model may have considerable explanatory power regarding

    Federico's (2007) findings of a possible interactive effect of NE and political sophistication on

    ideologically-organized thinking. The more political information high-NE individuals are

    exposed to, the stronger and more redundant the evaluative networks of these people would

    theoretically become. Eventually, it may be the case that new political information starts to

    become cognitively organized in terms ofcommon evaluations rather than simply being

    organized in terms of "cold" nomological networks (see Figure 1 for a crude hypothetical

    example). Such a model may provide a reasonable explanation as to why high-NE political

    experts are more ideologically constrained in their thinking compared to low-NE experts and

    political novices.

    Simple behavioral evidence, however, has never been considered sufficient grounds on

    which to base an entire complex model of information-processing (Stanovich, 2007). In the

    domain of cognitive psychology, such models have only been considered valid if they are

    supported by converging evidence from multiple domains of measurements such as self-report

    data, behavioral-performance measurements, neuropsychological dissociations and, most

    notably, psychophysiological measurements of brain function. The latter has been proven to be

    particularly useful in helping to identify distinct neural processes and subsystems involved in

    information processing. Recently, the tools of cognitive neuroscience have begun to be used as

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    9/21

    Experts with a cause 9

    converging evidence by political psychologists as well. Although controversial in some circles

    due to the misuse or oversimplification of psychophysiological evidence, the

    psychophysiological approach has been particularly successful in the past at identifying

    automatic cognitive-political processes that have previously only been inferred by behavioral

    observations (Weston, 2007).

    With this in mind, Morris and colleagues (2003) took the first step in using

    psychophysiological methods to provide converging evidence for the "hot cognition" hypothesis

    of political information processing. In this study, event-related-potential (ERP) recordings of

    brain activity were interpreted as evidence that participants evaluated political stimuli in a

    relatively automatic way and that these automatic evaluations affected later processing of

    political information. To do this, they measured changes in a negatively-deflected ERP wave that

    occurs about 400 milliseconds after the onset of stimulus presentation. In the domain of

    cognitive neuroscience, the amplitude of this "N400" wave has been established as a fairly

    reliable measure of semantic expectancy. When a word is presented to a participant, semantically

    unexpected words typically produce a greater N400 wave amplitude (greater peak negativity)

    than semantically primed words (Kutas & Hillyard, 1980, as cited in Morris, 2003). This

    difference has been interpreted by many to reflect underlying levels of neural processing activity.

    When a word is semantically primed through inference or the activation of a nomological

    network, the node that the word occupies in a nomological network is thought to become

    activated in such a way that neural pathways from this node from long-term memory to working

    memory are put into a state of "readiness" and thus take relatively less bottom-up processing to

    access (Morris 2003). It is this type of facilitation that is thought to be reflected in the relative

    attenuation of the N400 wave during semantic priming or inference tasks.

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    10/21

    Experts with a cause 10

    Morris and colleagues (2003) adapted this well-established cognitive neuroscience

    paradigm to test whetheraffective incongruity in the domain of political information would also

    produce changes in the N400 wave. In this experiment participants were presented with a

    political object prime word and then a moment later were presented with an affective target

    adjective that was either affectively congruent or affectively incongruent with the prime word

    (depending on evaluative pre-testing and careful selection of these pairs for each participant).

    When the second word was presented, subjects were told to make a judgment as to whether the

    target adjective was positive or negative in valence. As predicted, it was found that affectively

    incongruent political object prime / affective target pairs elicited an enhanced N400 negativity

    relative to affectively congruent prime/target pairs. This is important because it provides

    psychophysiological evidence of a "priming" effect of political stimuli on evaluative adjectives

    and thus supports the hot cognition theory that affective evaluations are stored along with

    semantic political information.

    As already discussed, automatic processing styles have already been linked to the need to

    evaluate (NE) trait through behavioral evidence. Because of this I hypothesize that the difference

    in N400 peak negativity between affectively congruent and affectively incongruent political

    object - affective target pairs, oraffective facilitation scores, will be greater in high-NE

    individuals than in low-NE individuals. Also, because the evaluative networks of political

    experts with high-NE should be even stronger based on the theoretical model discussed earlier, I

    predict to find an interaction between NE and PE such that political experts with high-NE show a

    far greater difference in affective facilitation than any other group. If so, this may lend

    preliminary support to the idea that automatic evaluative processing styles may be at least one of

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    11/21

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    12/21

    Experts with a cause 12

    prime/target pairs will then be calculated for each group and used in the final analysis. Also,

    because previous behavioral studies have found that there is no statistical difference in affective

    facilitation between positive and negative stimuli, these differences will be ignored and both

    positive and negative stimuli will be collapsed into the congruent and incongruent groups.

    Lastly, a vertical electro-oculogram (EOG) electrode will be used to detect and discard trials with

    ocular artifacts.

    Stimuli and Procedure

    Except for added measures of NE and political expertise, the paradigm used in this

    experiment will be virtually identical to that used by Morris (2003). Accordingly, participants

    will come in two days before their scheduled experimental session for pre-testing. During this

    phase of the experiment participants will complete a computerized "prime-selection" task in

    which they indicate their attitudes (positive or negative) toward 36 different political "attitude

    objects" as quickly as possible. Based on decision latency measures, the 5 strongest positive and

    5 strongest negative attitude objects will then be selected as the prime words for that individual

    in the following experimental session. Also during this pre-test session, the NE self-report

    questionnaire and the test of political knowledge will be administered to participants.

    During the actual experimental session, each participant will sit in a comfortable chair

    and all political prime and target words will be presented in 36-point red font against a solid gray

    background on a high-definition, high-contrast computer screen situated approximately 90

    centimeters from the participants' eyes. All florescent lights will be turned off to reduce 60hz

    EEG noise. To ensure that participants attend to the prime, they will told that the prime words

    that are presented on the screen are "memory" words that may be used in a memory test later in

    the study. As in Morris' (2003) study, these prime words will be presented for 150ms each,

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    13/21

    Experts with a cause 13

    followed by a delay of 100ms before the target word is presented. The target word will remain on

    the screen for 1 second and then be followed immediately by an "R" to indicate that the

    participant must make a judgment as to whether the target word was positive or negative in

    valence. These target words will be from the same list of 15 "clearly positive" and "clearly

    negative" adjectives used by Morris (2003). Each target word will appear a total of two times in a

    randomized order for each participant and be preceded by either a positive or negative prime.

    Thus, each prime word will appear a total of six times and be paired three times with a negative

    target and three times with a positive target.

    Results

    Once all measurements are taken and the N400 components are averaged within each

    group for both congruent and incongruent prime-target pairs, the differences in average

    amplitudes between the congruent and incongruent pairs will then be calculated for each group.

    Based on Morris' (2003) study, these difference scores should reflect the degree of affective

    priming or facilitation that took place in each group and thus provide a dependant measure of

    automatic evaluative processing. For statistical analysis, data will be entered into a 2 (NE) X 2

    (PE) between-groups analysis of variance with the "affective facilitation" score serving as the

    dependant measure.

    If my first hypothesis is true, we should find a main effect for both the need to evaluate

    and political expertise such that the high-NE and high-expertise groups show greater affective

    facilitation than the low-NE and low-expertise groups. If my second hypothesis is true, post-hoc

    analysis will reveal that there is an interactive effect between NE and expertise such that only the

    high-NE / high-expertise group will show statistically significant affective facilitation scores. No

    other difference should be significant (See Figure 2 for hypothetical representation).

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    14/21

    Experts with a cause 14

    Discussion

    If a main effect of NE was found using Morris' N400 attenuation paradigm as a

    dependent measure of automatic evaluative priming, this would be consistent with previous

    behavioral findings that NE is related to automatic processing, or "hot cognition". A more

    important finding would be the possible interactive effect of NE and political expertise on N400

    measures of automatic evaluative processing. This evidence would lend support to the model of

    overlapping and redundant affective networks in high-NE political experts and, when compared

    to Federico's (2007) findings of an interactive effect between NE and political expertise on

    measures of ideological constraint, would suggest the possibility that automatic processing

    tendencies may be one of the mechanisms behind the apparent ideological organization of

    information in high-NE political experts. If so, this may have far-reaching implications on how

    we understand political expertise and motivated decision-making.

    For this study to be interpreted properly, however, several limitations must first be

    addressed. First, the variables in this proposed study are all pre-existing factors. Pseudo-

    experiments like this can only give us information about associations between variables. Even

    when measures are taken from multiple domains (such as behavioral and psychophysiological),

    we can still only speculate about underling causal relationships between these variables. There

    are also other limitations. Morris' (2003) study only provided preliminary support for the

    hypothesis that N400 attenuation was a valid measure of affective priming. As he cautioned, it

    could be that the N400 wave reflects or includes some other processes that we do not yet

    understand. Also, the procedure of dichotomizing continuous variables such as political expertise

    and NE may not be the best way to analyze an interaction because of the problems it creates with

    regard to statistical power. A similar problem may also arise out of the fact that the proposed

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    15/21

    Experts with a cause 15

    sample is limited to college students who are unlikely to represent the entire domain of political

    expertise. A potential solution to both of these problems might be to recruit additional

    participants from a group of political party elites and then analyze the data in a more

    parametrically-oriented way.

    Despite these few weaknesses, the proposed study would take an important step toward

    clarifying the role of evaluative motivation and political expertise with regard to political

    decision-making, and would likely encourage new studies to explore the "black box" of attitude

    cognition in more detail. Future studies would most likely include functional MRI investigations

    into neural differences in political information processing styles. Additionally, evidence of

    distinctive processing styles may inspire investigation into the merits and weaknesses of each in

    relation to real-world political situations. Studies have shown that that although most political

    experts are better at remembering political information than political novices, they are only

    slightly better than chance when it comes topredictingactual political and social events (Tetlock,

    2005). Could this be because the majority of political experts are influenced more heavily by

    automatic processing and therefore ignore important contextual information in favor of

    programmed ideological schemas? Or is this effect pushed by memory-based thinkers who may

    evaluate political stimuli less efficiently than on-line thinkers? Any future study on this topic

    would be fascinating and would undoubtedly have a profound effect on the way we think about

    political expertise and motivation.

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    16/21

    Experts with a cause 16

    References

    Bizer, G. Y., Krosnick, J.A., Holbrook, A.L., Wheeler, S.C., Rucker, D.D., Petty, R.E. (2004).

    The impact of personality on cognitive, behavioral, and affective political processes: The

    effects of need to evaluate.Journal of Personality, 75, 995-1028.

    Bizer, G. Y., Tormala, Z.L., Rucker, D.D., Petty, R.E. (2006). Memory-based versus on-line

    processing: Implications for attitude strength.Journal of Experimental Social

    Psychology, 42, 646653.

    Converse, P. E. (1964). The Nature of Belief Systems in Mass Publics. In D. Apter (Ed.),

    Ideology and Discontent, New York: Free Press.

    Converse, P. E. (2000). Assessing the Capacity of Mass Electorates.Annual Review of Political

    Science, 3, 331-353

    Federico, C. M., Schneider, M.C. (2007) Political expertise and the use of ideology: Moderating

    effects of evaluative motivation.Public Opinion Quarterly, 71, 221-252

    Hastie, R., Park, B. (1986). The relationship between memory and judgment depends on whether

    the judgment task is memory-based or on-line.Psychological Review, 93, 258268.

    Hermans, D., Houwer, J., Eelen, P. (2001). A time analysis of the affective priming effect.

    Cognition and Emotion, 15, 143165.

    Jarvis, W. B. G., Petty, R. E. (1996). The need to evaluate.Journal of Personality and Social

    Psychology, 70, 72194.

    Jennings, M. K. (1992). Ideological thinking among mass publics and political elites. Public

    Opinion Quarterly, 56, 420-440

    Krosnick, J. A., (1990). Expertise and political psychology. Social Cognition, 8, 1-8

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    17/21

    Experts with a cause 17

    Morris, J. P., Squires, N.K., Taber, C. S., Lodge, M. (2003) Activation of Political Attitudes: A

    Psychophysiological Examination of the Hot Cognition Hypothesis.Political

    Psychology, 24, 727-745.

    Stanovich, K. E. (2007).How to think straight about psychology. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Tetlock, P. E. (2005).Expert Political Judgment: How Good Is It? How Can We Know? New

    Jersey: Princeton University Press

    Weston, D. (2007). The political brain: The role of emotion in deciding the fate of the nation .

    Cambridge: Public Affairs

    Zaller, J. (1992). The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. New York: Cambridge University

    Press.

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    18/21

    Experts with a cause 18

    Figure Caption

    Figure 1. Hypothetical conceptualizations of "cold" (top) vs. "ideological" (bottom) cognitive

    organization of political information.

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    19/21

    Political Party

    Republican Democrat

    High Taxes

    War

    Death Penalty

    Gun Control

    + -

    +

    -

    +

    -

    + -+

    -

    -+

    -

    -

    Obama

    Figure 1

    Political Party

    War

    - +

    Democrat

    Obama

    High Taxes

    Republican

    Gun Control

    Death Penalty

    Bush

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    20/21

    Figure Caption

    Figure 2. Hypothetical results showing the possible interactive relationship between political

    expertise and the need to evaluate (NE) on N400 affective facilitation (N4AF) scores.

  • 8/14/2019 Experts with a cause: proposal to investigate the possible interactive effect of political expertise and the dispositio

    21/21

    N400

    Affective

    FacilitationScore

    (N4AF)

    Political Novices Political Experts

    High-NE

    Low-NE

    Figure 2