9
 Verbal Morphology 1.0 Inflectional Prefixes  The inflectional morphology of Erromangan verbs is unusually complex for an Oceanic language, with many inflectional categories being expressed by forms belonging discontinuously in several morphotactic slots. 1.1 First Order Prefixes Prefixes from the first order express the pronominal category of the subject, as well as a variety of tense- aspect-mood distinctions. There are separate prefixes for new subjects, which express a full set of inflectional contrasts, and echo subjects which express a reduced set of contrasts. 1.1.1 New Subjects (i) Imperative  There is a distinction between singular and plural imperative, with the singular being marked by zero prefixation (th is being the only inflectional category in Erromangan marked in this way), and the plural by u-.  These prefixes are added to the basic form of the verb root. Ø-tovop utotovop (<u-etu-tovop) 2SG:IMP-BR:laugh 2PL:IMP-NEG-BR:laugh ¶laugh!· ¶don·t you all laugh!· (ii) Recent Past SG DL PL 1   yac o- INCL koku- kokli - EXCL ka ku- ka kli - 2 ko- ku- ku- 3 c o- c u- c u- Table 1 Recent Past Prefixes  The ko- series of prefixes in the table above are added to the basic form of the root in order to express the recent past. SG DL PL 1   y oc oc -tovop INCL koku-tovop ko( k  )li -tovop EXCL ka ku-tovop ka ( k  )li -tovop 2 koc -tovop ku-tovop ku-tovop 3 c oc -tovop c u-tovop c u-tovop Table 2 Recent Past of tovop 'laugh'  Verbs carrying these markers are used to encode four semantic categories of events: (a) Simple (that is non-durative and non-habitual) actions in which the event took place relatively recently in the past and which still pertain in some way at the time of utterance; (b) events in which a process began in the recent past, resulting in a change of state that is manifest in the present;

Erromangan Verbs

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 1/9

 Verbal Morphology 

1.0 Inflectional Prefixes

 The inflectional morphology of Erromangan verbs is unusually complex for an Oceanic language, with many 

inflectional categories being expressed by forms belonging discontinuously in several morphotactic slots.

1.1 First Order Prefixes

Prefixes from the first order express the pronominal category of the subject, as well as a variety of tense-

aspect-mood distinctions. There are separate prefixes for new subjects, which express a full set of inflectional

contrasts, and echo subjects which express a reduced set of contrasts.

1.1.1 New Subjects

(i) Imperative

  There is a distinction between singular and plural imperative, with the singular being marked by zero

prefixation (this being the only inflectional category in Erromangan marked in this way), and the plural by u-. These prefixes are added to the basic form of the verb root.

Ø-tovop utotovop (<u-etu-tovop)2SG:IMP-BR:laugh 2PL:IMP-NEG-BR:laugh¶laugh!· ¶don·t you all laugh!·

(ii) Recent Past

SG DL PL

1   yac o-  INCL koku-  kokli - EXCL ka ku-  ka kli - 

2  ko-  ku-  ku- 3  c o-  c u-  c u- 

Table 1 Recent Past Prefixes

 The ko- series of prefixes in the table above are added to the basic form of the root in order to express the

recent past.

SG DL PL

1   y oc oc -tovop INCL koku-tovop ko( k )li -tovopEXCL ka ku-tovop ka ( k )li -tovop

2  koc -tovop ku-tovop ku-tovop3  c oc -tovop c u-tovop c u-tovop

Table 2 Recent Past of tovop 'laugh' 

 Verbs carrying these markers are used to encode four semantic categories of events:

(a) Simple (that is non-durative and non-habitual) actions in which the event took place relatively recently in

the past and which still pertain in some way at the time of utterance;

(b) events in which a process began in the recent past, resulting in a change of state that is manifest in the

present;

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 2/9

(c) events involving the expression of perceptions in the present;

(d) events which have not yet commenced, but which are immediately imminent, can also be marked with the

recent past.

(iii) Distant Past

SG DL PL

1   yam -  INCL kom u-  kom li - EXCL kam u-  kam li - 

2  ki m -  ki m u-  ki m u- 

3    y i -  nd u-  nd u- Table 3 Distant Past Prefixes

(iv) Optative

SG DL PL

1   ya  pi -  INCL kopu-  kopli - 

EXCLka  pu- 

ka  p

li - 

2  ki  pi -  ki  pu-  ki  pu- 3   pi - pu- pu- 

Table 4 Optative Prefixes

  This category is much less frequently encountered in ordinary discourse than many of the other

inflectional categories on verbs. It expresses the following meanings:

(a) an event which the speaker expected to take place in the past but which did not actually take place;

(b) an event which did not take place, but which might have taken place if another event takes place;

(c) an event which the speaker wishes might be realized, but which has not been, and probably will not, be

realized; and,

(d) an event over which the participants have no control.

(v) Subjunctive

 The paradigms for the subjunctive and the optative are in fact identical for all subject categories except the

first person singular, where there is a contrast between the optative form   ya  pi - and the subjunctive  ya ki - .

 The subjunctive refers to events in the future which have not taken place, and which are dependent

for their realization on some other event taking place.

SG DL PL1   ya ki -  INCL kopu-  kopli - 

EXCL ka  pu-  ka  pli - 2  ki  pi -  ki  pu-  ki  pu- 3   pi - pu- pu- 

Table 5 Subjunctive Prefixes

(vi) Counterassertive

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 3/9

This inflectional category is also very infrequently used. It indicates an ability to perform an action in

the face of an assertion or an assumption that the speaker is not capable of performing the action.

SG DL PL

1   ya ki n i -  INCL kon u-  kon li - EXCL ka n u-  ka n li - 

2  ki n i -  ki n u-  ki n u- 3  n i -  n u-  n u- 

Table 6 Counterassertive Prefixes

(vii) Future

SG DL PL

1   yac o-  INCL kokw o-  kokl e- EXCL ka kw o-  ka kl e- 

2  ko-  kw o-  kw o- 3  c o-  c w o-  c w o- 

Table 7 Future Prefixes

Future prefixes are used to express the following kinds of events:

(a) Those which take place after the time of utterance, but not as a direct result of the utterance itself. This

can involve a future time that is either immediate and directly connected with the present, or a more distant

future at a time that is not directly connected with the present;

(b) events in the future which are to be obligatorily realized as a result of some external force;

(c) with first person non-singular subjects, verbs with this marking express the hortative;

(d) a main verb referring to an irrealis event which is associated with a subordinate clause marked by n e m  pgon  

¶when· or n a  g ku ¶if· , carries this marking;

(e) an irrealis complement to a verb in the present that expresses a wish or desire.

(viii) Past Habitual

Forms with this inflectional marking are used to express events in the distant past which took place

habitually, and which no longer pertain in the present.

1.1.2 Echo Subjects

 There is separate inflectional marking when a verb follows another verb in a sentence and the two share thesame subject categories, which are referred to as echo subject markers.

Before verb i n  m od i  f i e d  root e n v i ron m e n t 

 Els e wh ere 

SG m e-  m - 1DL;

2/3 NONSGm o-  m u- 

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 4/9

1PL m l e-  m li - Table 8 Echo Subject Prefixes

1.1.3 Prior Past

Immediately after the subject prefixes is a morphotactic slot that is optionally filled with prior past markers.

 These can only co-occur with categories expressing past tense.

Before verb i n  m od i  f i e d  root e n v i ron m e n t 

 Els e wh ere 

SG epm e- epm - 1DL;

2/3 NONSGepm o- epm u- 

1PL epm l e- epm li - Table 9 Prior Past Prefixes

 The prior past does not occur frequently in textual data, though when verbs with and without this

prefix are contrasted, the associated meaning difference is one between simple as against prior past.

  y i -t a  pm i     y -epm -t a  pm i  

3SG:DISTPAST-BR:try 3SG:DISTPAST-PRIOR-BR:try 

¶(s)he tried it· ¶(s)he had tried it·

1.1.4 Iterative

Before verb i n  m od i  f i e d  root e n v i ron m e n t 

 Els e wh ere 

SG u m e- u m - 1DL;

2/3 NONSG

u m o- u m u- 

1PL u m l e- u m li - Table 10 Iterative Prefixes

  This category can be used to indicate that an action is performed again, involving only a single

repetition. However, it can also be used to express the following additional semantically related notions:

(a) actions which involve a return to the physical point of origin, that is the meaning expressed in English by 

¶back·; and,

(b) actions which take place over and above what is ordinarily expected.

1.1.5 Negative

 The negative prefix has two different shapes: et w o- before an immediately following verb in a modified root

environment, and etu- elsewhere. These prefixes differ from all of the other prefixes presented thus far in that

they do not express any distinctions of number.

1.1.6 Fifth Order Prefixes

Before verb i n  m od i  f i e d  root e n v i ron m e n t 

 Els e wh ere 

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 5/9

SG e m e- e m - 1DL;

2/3 NONSGe m o- e m u- 

1PL e m l e- e m li - Table 11 Fifth Order Prefixes 

  These prefixes present a serious analytical difficulty in that it is not possible to assign any particularmeaning to them. A morpheme is normally defined as the smallest meaningful unit of linguistic analysis. In

that sense, then, it is difficult to refer to these prefixes as morphemes. However, while they do not have any 

definable meaning, they behave like ordinary morphemes in that they have consistent phonological shapes,

and they occupy a clearly recognizable morphotactic position in the word.

1.1.7 Tripartite Inflectional Marking 

  There are five inflectional categories which are expressed by means of tripartite morphological marking,

involving the presence of fifth order prefixes, in addition to the subject prefixes, and a choice between the

basic or modified form of the verb root.

(i) Present

SG DL PL

1   yacam e- ~  y oc om o INCL koku m o-  koke m l e ~ kokom l e- EXCL ka ku m o-  ka ke m l e- 

2  ke m e- ~ kom e-  ku m o-  ku m o- 3  ( c  )e m e- ~ ( c  )am e- ~ ( c  )om e-  c w o-  c w o- 

Table 12 Present Tense Prefixes

 Verbs marked for the present encode the following kinds of events:

(a) those in which the time of an action coincides with the time of the utterance;

(b) those in which a change of state is in progress at the time of utterance, but which is not yet complete;

(c) those in which a state pertains habitually over an extended period;

(d) an event in the past that was in the process of coming about;

(e) events which do not pertain at the time of the utterance but which the speaker holds will pertain in the

immediate future, or ought to pertain in the immediate future as a result of the utterance itself;

(f) a verb in a subordinate clause introduced by the irrealis subordinators n e m  pgon  ¶when· and n a  g ku ¶if· carries

this marking;

(g) events involving the expression of perceptions or feelings at the time of utterance.

(ii) Past Continuous

 The same combination of prefixes that mark the present tense is used to express the past continuous, with

the difference being expressed solely by the use of the basic form of the verb root as against the modified

root. This category expresses a continuous event in the recent past.

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 6/9

(iii) Dependent Past

 This category is used to express events which took place in the distant past, and which possess any of the

following particular characteristics:

(a) The event took place habitually over a long period of time, with the possibility that the event so described

still pertains into the present.

(b) The event took place while some other event was taking place.

(c) The event took place immediately before a subsequent event within a narrative sequence. Thus, it is

possible to open a narration relating to the distant past using a verb marked in this way as this indicates that

other events are about to be related.

(iv) Past Habitual

 This category has, in fact, two competing morphological expressions, that the past habitual could also be

marked by a combination of distant past subject markers and the modified form of the root, with no

intervening fifth order prefix. Thus,   ya nd u c  (<  y i - a nd u c  ) '3SG:DISTPAST-MR: bathe' and   y e ma nd u c  (<  y i -e m e- 

a nd u c  ) '3SG:DlSTPAST-?A/-MR:bathe' are alternative ways of saying '(s)he used to bathe'. Of the two

patterns, the one in which the fifth order suffix is present is by far the more common, with forms lacking this

marker being encountered more frequently in the speech of older speakers.

(v) Realis Conditional

  The realis conditional is expressed by combining the subject prefixes involved in the expression of the

optative and the fifth order prefix, along with a verb in its modified root form. Thus:

  ya  pe m n a ruvo (<  ya  pi -e m e- n a ruvo) pu ma nd ca i  (<pu-e m o- a nd ca i  )

lSG:OPT-M-MR:sing 3PL:OPT-EM-MR:bathe'if I had sung' 'if they had bathed'

Such verbs express a conditional meaning relating to events that took place in the past, or which are

taking place in the present. Events expressed in this way have not taken place, with the speaker hypothesizing 

about what might have happened if they had. Thus:

 N a  g ku    ya  pe m - a nd u c     ya  p-et w -ore i     ya u.

if lSG:REALCOND-MR:bathe lSG:OPT-NEG-BR:scratch lSG

'If I had bathed, I would not be scratching myself now.'

(vi) Irrealis Conditional

 This inflectional category is expressed by the subject markers used in the counter assertive, in combination

 with a fifth order prefix and the modified form of the root. In the first person singular, however, the irrealis

conditional is based on the subjunctive prefix  ya ki - , producing   ya ke m e- , rather than the counterassertive prefix

  ya ki n i - , which would result in *   ya ki n e m e-. Thus:

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 7/9

  ya ke m n a ruvo (<  ya ki -e m e- n a ruvo) n u m on tovop (<n u-e m o- n tovop)

1SG:lRRCOND-MR:sing 3PL:lRRC0ND-MR:laugh

'if I were to sing' 'if they were to laugh'

ka n e m l e n a ruvo (<ka n li -e m l e- n a ruvo)

1 PL .EXCL:IRRCOND-MR: sing 'if we were to sing'

 Transitivity (Object Suff ixes) 

 There is no overt marking of transitivity on Sye verbs. Most verbs are either inherently intransitive,

allowing no object, or transitive, requiring an object. There is also a small subset of verbs which can be used

intransitively or transitively with no change in shape, for example om on ki  'drink', v a i  pe l ac  'remove, come off'.

 Transitive verbs fall into two major groups with respect to the manner in which objects are marked: suffixed

 verbs, with which object categories are obligatorily marked by means of suffixes to the verb, and unsuffixed

 verbs, where there is no inflectional marking for object. The division of verbs into these two categories is notpredictable on the basis of verbal semantics or phonological shape. About three quarters of transitive verbs

belong in the set of suffixed verbs, while the remaining quarter is unsuffixed. Suffixed verbs can be further

subdivided into the open class of plain suffixed verbs, and the closed set of possessive suffixed verbs.

Derivational Verb Morphology 

  There are five derivational prefixes in Sye, expressing a range of modal types of meanings: ov l u-  

'simultanitive', om  proc -  'immediate', or c u-  'interruptive', er a r  'random', ov s oc -  'manner interrogative', ov   y u-  

'desiderative' and ov   y u- 'causative'.

(i) S i m u l t a n i t i ve ovlu

 This indicates that an action takes place typically while the referent of the subject noun phrase is

going from one place to another. While this is the most usual interpretation of this prefix, it can also express

the more general meaning of an action which takes place simultaneously to any other action, whether that

action is explicitly stated, or merely implicit from the context. We therefore find examples such as orv i  'cut it'

> ov l -orv i  'cut it while going' and ma h  'die' > ov l u- ma h  'die on the way'. Some derivations involving this form

have been attested in which the meaning of the derived forms are not fully predictable from the meaning of 

their constituent parts, for example om ol om ol  'fall all over' > ov l -om ol om ol  'stagger', a  g ka u  'turn around' > ov l - 

a  g ka u 'veer away'. The following examples illustrate the use of this form in sentences:

C- a v a n   m -ov l u- ma h .

3SG:RECPAST-BR:walk SG:ES-BR:SiMULT-die

¶(S)he walked and (while walking) died.·

(ii) I mm e d i a te omproc

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 8/9

 This indicates that an action has only just taken place. This form is only compatible with past tense

marking, as in:

Y -om  proc -ve l om .

3SG:DlSTPAST-BR:lMM-come

'(S)he has only just come.'

(iii) I n terrupt i ve orcu

  This form expresses the idea that one action has been interrupted before completion in order to

perform the action expressed by the verb to which it has been attached. Thus, contrast the following:

Y ac - am  pl am i .

1 SG:FUT-MR:urinate

'I will urinate.'

(iv) R a nd om  et r

 This expresses a number of loosely related meanings, each of which is described below. For the sakeof convenience, these meanings have been subsumed under the general category label of 'random', thoughrandomness is only one aspect of the meaning of this form.

Y am etrorg i  (<  yam -et  r-orog- i  ).1 SG:DlSTPAST-BR:RANDOM-BR:hear-3sG

'I just happened to hear it (though no specific person told me).'

(v)  M a nn er I n terrog a t i ve ovsoc

 This prefix can be added to both intransitive and transitive verbs, in order to ask how an action iscarried out. Thus:

K i m -ov s oc -om  pi?  K- am s oc - a r ki   n e i?  2SG:DlSTPAST-BR:HOW-BR:do 2sG:FUT-MR:HOW-BR:break firewood

'How did you do it?' 'How will you break the firewood?'

(vi) De si d er a t i ve ovyu-

 The desiderative prefix ov   y u- expresses the meaning of 'want to'. Thus:

Y am etuv   yam  (<  yam -etu-ov   y u- am  ).lSG:DISTPAST-NEG-BR:DESID-BR:speak 

'I did not want to speak.·

(vii) C a u s a t i ve ovyu -

  The prefix ov   y u- , in addition to expressing the desiderative, can also be used to express a causativemeaning in association with an intransitive verb. This dual function should be treated as homophony,however, as there are differences in morphological behavior. The modified form of the desiderative prefix isregularly am  p  y u- , while the modified form of the causative prefix is unpredictably a v   y u- .

8/9/2019 Erromangan Verbs

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/erromangan-verbs 9/9

  This is the only derivational prefix which affects transitivity, with the causee being expressed by means of a free form pronoun or noun in the object position after the derived verb. Thus:

K- a v   y -ete h ep   ya u. Y am -ov   y -oru c   n a l a u.2sG:FUT-MR:CAUS-sit lSG lSG:DlSTPAST-BR:CAUS-bathe child

'You will sit me down.' 'I bathed the child.'

(viii) Ve s t i  g i a l  t-

In addition to the productive prefixes described above, there are a dozen or so verbs in the corpus with initialt-  which appears to represent a vestigial prefix. A verb with or without t-  sometimes expresses exactly thesame meaning, for example (t-)e l e h -  'chase away' and (t-)e l a h ep 'look down' . In other cases, while there is adifference in meaning between the form with and without t- , the semantic relationship is not predictable, forexample a  g ka u 'go around' > t a  g ka u  'go to the other side', ugon  'smoky' > tugon  'set fire to·, a lsi  'chew' > t a lsi  'pound (food) with stone'.