28
Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of duration of preschool attendance and social composition * Names of the authors: Nicole Biedinger, Mannheimer Centre for European Social Research, Birgit Becker, Mannheimer Centre for European Social Research, Inge Rohling, Health Service of County and City of Osnabrueck. Presented by: Nicole Biedinger Please do not quote! * This paper will be published soon. I acknowledge that the article has been accepted for publication in the European Sociological Review published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved. 1

Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of duration of preschool attendance and social composition*

Names of the authors:

Nicole Biedinger, Mannheimer Centre for European Social Research, Birgit Becker, Mannheimer Centre for European Social Research, Inge Rohling, Health Service of County and City of Osnabrueck.

Presented by: Nicole Biedinger

Please do not quote!

* This paper will be published soon. I acknowledge that the article has been accepted for publication in the European Sociological Review published by Oxford University Press. All rights reserved.

1

Page 2: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Abstract

Ethnic inequality in education is a well-established topic among the scientific community. We

assume that ethnic inequality is constituted early in life – before a child has even started

school. Differences between natives and immigrants with regard to preschool attendance (if,

when, and which preschool is attended) may account for some of the ethnic educational

inequality upon entering school. We use the school entrance examination data of the City of

Osnabrueck (Germany) for the years 2000 to 2005 to analyse the school readiness of six-to-

seven-year old children as an indicator of early school success. It is apparent that the amount

of preschool experience improves school readiness, even when controlling for family

background. While this is true for all children, immigrant children nonetheless exhibit lower

scores on school readiness when all these individual explaining factors are controlled for.

Multilevel analysis shows that the ethnic effect differs among preschools. A preschool’s

influence depends on its social composition: Preschools with a beneficial social composition

are better able to promote children’s development than those with a poorer learning context.

Immigrant children benefit particularly from longer attendance at preschools with a positive

context.

2

Page 3: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

1. Introduction Ethnic inequality in education is a well-established topic among the scientific community in

Western countries. Immigrants perform worst, for example, in Germany, the United States,

and the United Kingdom (for details see Alba et al., 1994; Betts and Lofstrom, 2000; Kristen,

2005; Lange and Rupp, 1992; Neild, 2001; OECD, 2006; Portes and MacLeod, 1999; West,

2006). The search for the causes behind and the potential cures for these problems has

focussed almost exclusively on schools and on the schooling process. Especially since the

“Program for International Student Assessment (PISA)” (for details see Entorf et al., 2004)

was conducted and whose results in most countries exposed huge drawbacks on the part of

immigrant pupils, research efforts have even been intensified (cf. Marks, 2005). In particular,

the early childhood years are increasingly seen as a period crucial to the growth and

consolidation of important skills necessary for successful school transition and later academic

functioning (e.g. Barnett, 1995; Entwisle, 1995; Gormley et al., 2005).

Major individual differences emerge well before children arrive at school. We will focus on

the role of preschool for the early educational success of children. Preschool attendance has

been shown to positively influence children’s development and school success, although the

debate about the long-term effect is ongoing (e.g. Aughinbaugh, 2001). One main finding

from the literature on early child development is that it is not only a matter of whether

children attend preschools, but also a matter of which preschool they attend. The quality of a

preschool has proven to be an important moderating factor of the preschool influence (Love et

al., 2003). Thus, we will not only focus on the individual preschool attendance, but also take

into account the differences among preschools. Hence, in this paper, we use a multilevel

design to study preschool effects. An interaction of preschool duration and preschool context

also seems plausible, meaning that long preschool attendance can only yield positive effects if

the quality of the preschool attended is high or its social composition is beneficial. Some

findings indicate that disadvantaged children can especially profit from attending high-quality

preschools (e.g. Magnuson and Waldfogel, 2005). Since ethnic minorities are mostly

disadvantaged, the consideration of individual preschool attendance, as well as preschools’

context variables may help to explain the early ethnic educational gap.

Our analysis is based on school entrance examination data of the City of Osnabrueck for the

years 2000 to 2005. The school entrance examination takes place in all of the federal states in

3

Page 4: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Germany. A paediatrician examines the child’s development and then gives a

recommendation whether or not the child is ready to start school. The recommendation relies

on certain standardised tests, as well as on the paediatrician’s own assessment. We use

multilevel analysis to test the impact of preschool attendance on the children’s school

readiness and on the early ethnic educational gap. In the following section we will give first of

all a short review of current research and explanations of ethnic inequality in education

(section 2). Then we will provide a short introduction to the German preschool system, given

the origin of the data (section 3). Thereafter, we will introduce our analysis and the results

(section 4), followed by conclusions and discussions (section 5).

2. Ethnic educational inequality

Ethnic educational inequality is a global phenomenon and can be found in most Western

societies. In the United States ethnic and racial disadvantages are hard to separate. Although

racial and ethnic inequality cannot be equated, similar mechanisms are used to explain the

phenomenon. To keep matters simple, within our theoretical framework we do not distinguish

between racial and ethnic inequality. From 1972 up to 2004, status dropout rates for white,

black, and Hispanic young adults have declined in the United States, with rates remaining

lowest for whites and highest for Hispanics. Additionally, college enrolment among minority

groups is less frequent, and gaps between whites and blacks with regard to completion of

bachelor’s or advanced degrees have even widened over time (Livingston, 2006). These

disadvantages in the United States may be comparable to disadvantages of the main ethnic

groups in the United Kingdom, where especially Pakistani and Bangladeshi are deprived. The

gap between the highest- and the lowest-achieving ethnic groups continues to grow. At Key

Stage 1 (at age 7 years), there is a achievement gap of 15 percentage points between the

highest- and lowest-attaining ethnic group, but at Key Stage 4 (at age 16 years), the equivalent

gap has widened to 42 percent (Department for Education and Skills, 2005). In the case of

Germany, the situation of immigrant children also is very well established; there exist gaps

between various ethnic groups. Especially Turkish children are disadvantaged (e.g. Alba et

al., 1994; Dinkel et al., 1999). Equal descriptions may be given for most Western countries

(see Bade, 2001). We assume that educational disadvantages are set in quite early during

preschool education. In the case of the United States, both black and Hispanic children scored

about two-thirds of a standard deviation below whites in math and just under half a standard

deviation below whites in reading at age 6 (Duncan and Magnuson, 2005). In the case of

4

Page 5: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Germany, some similar descriptions can be made (see Mengering, 2005; Schöler et al., 2004).

This leads to the assumption that the foundation of ethnic educational inequality is constituted

even before children start school. Moreover, this kind of ethnic disadvantage at an early age is

disturbing if one considers that skills can be treated as human capital and tend to accumulate

(Kalter, 2003). Even though these disadvantages might still be quite small at early ages, in the

long run, the accumulation process can produce great differences.

Ethnic, as well as racial, educational inequality can be explained by the lack of resources

within families (most notably economic, cultural, social, and language resources), the

unfavourable choices some families make (self-selection), and by discrimination. Empirically,

the transition between resources and self-selection is smooth because they more or less

depend on each other or are highly correlated. We try to separate these explanations as far as

possible.

The most central issue in current research on ethnic and racial inequality is the resources

within families. These resources consist mainly of cultural and human capital (for details

about cultural capital, see DiMaggio, 1982; about human capital, see Becker, 1964). The

cultural capital of parents directly stimulates the activities of their children. Sullivan

concludes that cultural capital is transmitted within homes and has significant effects on

children’s performance in educational systems (Sullivan, 2001). Furthermore, cultural capital

among ethnic groups differs significantly from the cultural capital among natives (Portes and

Rumbaut, 2001). Ethnic inequality in the cultural capital of the receiving country can

therefore be described as the result of an accumulation process: Since ethnic groups often

possess less of the cultural capital of the receiving country (especially in the first generation),

parents thus have less capital to transmit to their children. An analogous mechanism holds

true for the case of human capital (Borjas, 1992): If parents are highly educated, the

probability rises that their children will also be highly educated. It is assumed that highly

educated parents may be better able to help their children. Conclusively, a child’s chance of a

positive school outcome depends on the (educational) resources of its parents. This is true for

natives as well as for immigrant children. In comparison to native parents, immigrants have a

shortage of the capital particular to the receiving country (e.g. language and specific

knowledge). In sum, the social and ethnic backgrounds, especially family origin, have

systematic effects on educational attainment amongst social, ethnic, and racial groups (e.g.

Blau and Duncan, 1967; Mare, 1980).

5

Page 6: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

In the case of ethnic inequality, language constitutes the most important type of human

capital. It is a fundamental resource which parents exhibit and which can be transferred

directly to the personal property of their children (e.g. Borjas, 1992). Language proficiency

per se is determined by family background. Worswick showed in the case of Canada that the

shortage of language as capital explains disadvantages among immigrant children (Worswick,

2004). If the children’s mother tongue corresponds to the official language, then there are no

educational disadvantages for immigrant children. In the case of Germany, the educational

inequality among immigrant children is also mainly explained by language proficiency, but

disadvantages still persist after controlling for language (e.g. Helmke et al., 2002).

Apparently, children have linguistic disadvantages if their parents use languages different

from the language of the receiving country (Bleakley and Chin, 2004). Boudon (1974)

describes these transmissions of parental capital as primary effects. He distinguishes between

the so-called primary and secondary effects of stratification. While the primary effect

describes the initial differentials in school achievement, for instance, ability differentials

between social and ethnic groups, the secondary effect refers to the families’ choices of their

children’s educational tracks.

The secondary effect described by Boudon points out that ethnic groups exercise a kind of

self-selection. Parental choices are mainly explained in light of the opportunities they exhibit.

The opportunities determining parents’ choices are influenced by their economic and cultural

resources, their social capital, their language proficiency, and their residential areas. The

direct influence of resources has already been discussed. These factors indirectly lower

families’ options to choose and influence the choices parents may make. Therefore, families’

endowment with resources affects ethnic educational inequality both directly and indirectly.

Kristen verified one established indirect effect in the case of Germany. She found that

especially a lack of information about the German school system put parents of Turkish

children at a disadvantage when choosing schools for their children (Kristen, 2005). The lack

of information is possibly due to a lack of proficiency in the German language and the lack of

advantageous social capital. This is one example of how resources (and language) indirectly

influence parents’ choices. Additionally, school attainment could also be explained by parents

making choices that are influenced by their residential area. Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn

found three theoretical frames for understanding how neighbourhoods affect children’s well-

being: institutional resources, relationships and norms, and collective efficacy (for details, see

Leventhal and Brooks-Gunn, 2003). These three sets of factors affect the choices of the

6

Page 7: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

parents and therewith the children’s well-being. In the case of ethnic inequality, residential

segregation has to be considered as well. Western societies differ greatly in the degrees and

the development of residential segregation (for details, see Friedrichs, 2000; Fong and

Shibuya, 2005). Nevertheless, residents of different neighbourhoods have access to different

institutional resources and have different relationships and norms, which may influence the

educational outcome of immigrant children. Additionally, segregated residential areas are

reflected within the educational system as segregated school classes. And in turn, segregation

within classes leads to different levels of educational attainment (e.g. Lanfranchi, 2002).

Therefore, ethnic inequality in education can also be explained partly by the choices of

parents, who often follow given opportunities (e.g. segregated residential areas). We term this

aspect self-selection, because while immigrant parents are supposed to have choices, there

exist nonetheless distinct opportunity structures, as well as some restrictions.

A further explanation of ethnic inequality may lie in mechanisms of discrimination

(Carmichael and Hamilton, 1967; Feagin and Booher Feagin, 1986; for Germany: Gomolla

and Radtke, 2002). Because of data limitations, our approach will not analyse the issue of

discrimination (for details about kinds of discrimination, see Kalter and Granato, 2002).

However, we think that it may also be a central cause of ethnic educational inequality.

We assume that ethnic educational inequality is already established before children start

school. Therefore, we shall extend these basic explanations to include an earlier stage: We

assume that the preschool years are a very crucial period. We will analyse early educational

inequality using the example of school readiness. School readiness itself is the acquisition of

those abilities that are necessary in order to start school. Preschool gives children one main

opportunity (aside from their family background) to acquire important skills necessary for

their educational success. Thus, preschool also strongly determines their school readiness by

stimulating the development of such skills mainly through beneficial activities. We assume

that differences in school readiness may lead to growing differences in educational careers.

Therefore, the preschool represents an opportunity to achieve better academic outcomes (cf.

Andersson, 1992; Goodman and Sianesi, 2005; Magnuson et al., 2004). Besides the rather

individual influence of the duration of preschool attendance, different preschools have

different characteristics, and these characteristics may have an independent influence on

children. We assume that preschools with a positive learning context and a high quality

improve the school readiness of children more than below-average preschools may do. Since

7

Page 8: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

our data do not include a measure of quality, we will focus on the children’s social

composition as an important determinant of the learning environment. These assumptions can

be deduced from the effects of class composition on the achievement of the pupils (cf. Dar

and Resh, 1986; U.S. Department of Education and National Center for Education Statistics

by Kristin Denton and Jerry West and Jill Walston, 2003). Dar and Resh found in an Israeli

sample that classroom intellectual composition positively affects the pupil’s objective

academic achievement, beyond the potent effect of his or her personal intellectual resources.

A prolific body of research on early childhood programmes in the United States focuses on

the effects of programmes intended for disadvantaged children (e.g. ethnic minorities, low-

educated, single-parent and low-income families). Such programmes include small-scale

models, or experiments such as the High/Scope Perry Preschool Project, as well as ongoing,

large-scale public programmes such as Head Start. Albeit not completely free from

methodological problems like small sample sizes, short follow-up periods, or non-random

attrition, the evidence from these studies has consistently pointed to short-term cognitive

improvements, as well as to long-term gains in terms of academic achievement, employment,

and earnings, and to a reduction in special education placement, and a reduction of crime (cf.

Aughinbaugh, 2001; Haskins, 1989) (For reviews of programmes and studies in the US and

UK, see: Currie and Thomas, 1995; Garces et al., 2002 ; Gramlich, 1986; Lamb, 2000;

Sammons et al., 2004; West, 2005). Thus, the choice of when and which preschool children

attend is an important determinant for their acquisition of skills. Additionally, we assume that

immigrant children may especially profit from attending a beneficial preschool for as long a

time as possible. Immigrant children in preschools have the chance to make contact with the

culture and the language of the receiving country (besides the other beneficial effects of

stimulating activities and contact with other children), which additionally stimulates their

development and increases the chance of a better academic outcome (cf. Magnuson and

Waldfogel, 2005). Rumberger and Tran already found differences between the influence of

child care arrangements and Head Start programmes on the language proficiency of English-

speaking and non-English-speaking children (Rumberger and Tran, 2006). This leads to the

assumption that the quality of the preschool does indeed matter. In sum, preschool per se is

very important to children’s development, but perhaps even more so for immigrant children:

It is assumed that preschool may compensate for some disadvantages within immigrant

families (Brandon, 2004).

8

Page 9: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Even though the disadvantages of ethnic or racial groups might still be quite small at early

ages, in the long run, the accumulation process produces great differences. The differences at

the beginning of school may, of course, be explained by resources, self-selection, and

discrimination just as for ethnic educational inequality per se. We assume, independently of

these causes, that the amount of preschool experience and the social composition of preschool

groups have an additional direct effect on school readiness. Therefore, we analyse the

importance of preschool over and above family background for further school careers in the

case of Germany. We do not analyse the influences of language, self-selection, or

discrimination, since our focus is on the subsidiary influence of preschool for school readiness

when controlling for family background.

3. Data The German preschool system The German preschool system is very homogeneous. The typical, and often only, chargeable

preschool establishment available for children from age three years until they enter school is

the so-called kindergarten (Jonen et al., 2005: 68-75). Kindergarten provides care, but for the

last fifteen years the strengthening of the educational mission is claimed as well. However,

even today, there is no systematic link to schools. Kindergarten is usually operated by the

community or by non-profit organizations. The quality is generally regulated at the state level,

with a focus on structural features such as staff-to-child ratios, group size, or building

standards. Although in Germany, attendance at preschool is not mandatory, preschool

nonetheless is intended to prepare children for school. By a law of 1996, preschool is

supposed to be available to every child, and a preschool can not refuse any child for reasons

other than an obvious shortage of places. Furthermore, the preschool is supposed to help

parents to meet their work and family life responsibilities, while at the same time providing

the first stage of education within the general educational system.

In Germany, attending preschool for at least one year before school begins has become the

norm for young children. In the year 2003, about 90 percent of the children between five and

six years of age attended preschool (Statistisches Bundesamt, 2004: 218). But differences in

attendance rates still persist; especially immigrant children on average start preschool a few

months later than German children (also see Ondrin and Spiess, 1998: 39). These differences

in the time children have spent in preschools could be one reason for some of the ethnic

9

Page 10: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

differences at the end of preschool, respectively at the start of school. Ethnic differences

between preschool contexts have not yet been analysed.

Sample The empirical results are based on data of the school entrance examination of the City of

Osnabrueck for the years 2000 to 2005. Osnabrueck is the third largest city of the Federal

State of Lower Saxony and has about 160,000 residents. The school entrance examination

takes place in all federal states in Germany. Paediatricians examine the children’s

development and recommend whether or not a child is ready to start school. The given

recommendation is mandatory, but school principals generally use it as a baseline. This

examination is compulsory, but without penalties if children do not take part. In sum, nearly

all children go through this process. During the examination, a number of medical and

developmental tests in different domains are performed. In the case of Osnabrueck, the school

entrance examination has been arranged in a highly standardized manner since the year 2000

(for details, see Rohling, 2002). The examination of a normally developed child takes about

40 to 60 minutes. Before visiting the paediatrician, parents fill out a short questionnaire. The

questions cover preschool experiences of the children, demographic information, and social

background.

The data as a whole are not representative of Germany. However, it is not intended here to

provide concrete information about the school readiness of children in Germany, but rather to

test general hypotheses among some variables. The data are applicable for this purpose; thus

ethnic inequality in school readiness as determined by preschool education und family

background can be analysed.

In the years 2000 to 2005, 8,601 school entrance examinations took place. The following

analysis contains 6,777 cases. We lost about 1,900 cases because we considered only regular

examinations and dropped all cases with missing values for the model variables, aside from

the educational and occupational variables of parents, for which missing-variables have been

generated. The variables are operationalised in the following way:

School readiness (dependent variable):

The school readiness of children was measured with the help of six subscales: gross motor

skills, fine motor skills, perception, language, cognition, and working habits. All subscales

10

Page 11: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

range from 0 to 2. Children with disorders score 0 points, children with some developmental

deficits score 1 point, and fully developed children score 2 points. A factor analysis with the

principal-component factor method revealed only one main factor (eigenvalue: 2.42), which is

used as a score of the overall development of the children.

Preschool attendance (L1 variable): 1: more than two years of preschool experience, 0: less

than two years of preschool experience.

Preschool composition (context) (L2 variable):

We construct an index of the social composition of the population of preschool children. The

social composition determines the learning context in preschool and thus may be an important

moderating variable of the preschool effect. The index consists of the following composition

variables within the relevant preschool:

• Percentage of families with at least one parent with a college degree,

• Percentage of families with at least one parent without any formal education (negative),

• Percentage of families with both parents unemployed (negative),

• Percentage of families with both parents living in the household,

• Percentage of families making special efforts to ensure the health of their children,

• Mean number of children in the families (negative),

• Percentage of children with a lack of proficiency in the German language (negative).

A factor analysis using the principal-component factor method reveals one main factor

(eigenvalue: 4.33), which constitutes the learning context. A factor score for each preschool is

computed.

The operationalisation of the family background and demographic variables is shown in table

I.

-- About here, table I --

4. Results Table II shows the descriptive statistics for all variables separately for German and immigrant

children. There exist considerable differences in school readiness between Germans and

11

Page 12: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

immigrants. This result confirms our assumption that ethnic inequalities already exist at an

early stage of education - at the beginning of children’s elementary education.

-- About here, table II --

There are also clear differences in other characteristics. Immigrant children are on average

slightly older than Germans at the time of their school entrance examination. In immigrant

(basically Turkish) families, there are also significantly more siblings than in German

families. The family situation in German and in immigrant families differs only slightly, there

being a greater number of intact immigrant families. German parents have on average a better

education than parents in immigrant families. It can not be overlooked that many immigrant

parents have no formal education at all. Additionally, more German parents than immigrant

parents are employed. In both groups, fathers, rather than mothers, are employed and then

about three times more often than the mothers. While there are just a few children who never

attend preschool, German children attend preschool longer than immigrant children do: 83 per

cent of the German children attend preschool for more than two years. That is true for just 60

per cent of the immigrant children. Furthermore, German and immigrant children attend very

different types of preschools. The composition of the preschools attended by German children

is significantly more beneficial than that of the preschools attended by immigrant children.

Our main focus rests upon the explanation of ethnic inequality in school readiness and we

assume that preschool attendance, as well as family background, plays a fundamental role.

We postulate that not only the duration of preschool attendance, but also the composition of

the preschool population is important. To examine the group effect of preschools, we

calculated some hierarchical linear models, using STATA (for details see Rabe-Hesketh and

Skrondal, 2005; Snijders and Bosker, 1999). When the explanatory variables at level 1 are

denoted as , the explanatory variables at level 2 are denoted as , and the outcome (school

readiness) is denoted as , the hierarchical linear model leads to the following formula for

individual i in preschool j (see Snijders and Bosker, 1999: 76):

ijx jz

ijY

. ijhij

p

hhjj

q

k

p

hhijkjhk

q

kkjk

p

hhijhij RxUUxzzxY ++++++= ∑∑∑∑∑

== === 10

1 110

1000 γγγγ

This formula includes the main effects of each X and Z variable, as well as all cross-level

product interactions. The groups are characterised by p+1 random coefficients to .

These random coefficients are independent between groups, but may be correlated within

jU 0 pjU

12

Page 13: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

groups. It is further assumed that the vector ( , …, ) is independent of the level-one

residual and that all these residuals have a mean of zero, given the values of all

explanatory variables. The variances and covariances are denoted as follows (see Snijders and

Bosker, 1999: 68, 76):

jU 0 pjU

ijR

2)var( σ=ijR

200 )var( rU j =

2)var( hhj rU =

hkkjhj rUU =),cov( .

This hierarchical linear model implies that there is variation between the preschools in the

general level of children’s school readiness (random intercept). But we also assume that the

slopes of certain level 1 variables are random and depend (at least to some extent) on level 2

variables. For theoretical reasons, we assume that the ethnic effect and also the duration of

preschool attendance vary among preschools (random slope). Longer preschool attendance

will result in various developmental outcomes, depending on the preschool context. While

longer attendance at a preschool with a positive context may lead to especially strong positive

effects, longer attendance at a preschool with a negative context may even do harm.

Analogous thereto, some preschools may be better able than others to fulfil immigrant

children’s needs, so that differences among preschools with regard to the ethnic effect are also

plausible. We consider only one level 2 variable, the social composition of the preschools;

hence, our final model contains two cross-level interactions: ethnic origin x preschool context,

and attendance duration x preschool context.

Table III shows the results of our analysis on school readiness of children.

-- About here, table IIIa --

The first step in table IIIa is to show the empty model without any explanatory variables. It is

obvious that there is variation among different preschools regarding the school readiness of

their children (model 1). In model 2, a significant negative influence of the immigration status

on development is found after additionally controlling for age, sex, and year of investigation.

This confirms the descriptive statistics of table II. Immigrant children score a third of a

standard deviation lower on the school readiness index than native children do. It can also be

seen that girls and older children score higher on school readiness than do boys and younger

13

Page 14: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

children. Model 3 adds the family background: Children of highly educated parents have a

higher probability of being ready for school than do children of less educated fathers and

mothers. There is also a positive effect of the parents’ occupational status and of the presence

of both parents in the household. In sum, all family background variables significantly affect

the school readiness of children in the manner expected. The influence of the immigration

status is clearly reduced after controlling for family background. Furthermore, the variance

among preschools is also slightly reduced, meaning that certain parts of the group variance are

based on differences in the distribution of certain individual variables. Model 4 additionally

controls for the duration of preschool attendance. Controlling for preschool attendance leads

to a reduction of some other effects. Preschool attendance per se improves the school

readiness of children – independent of all other influences. This shows that the quantity of

preschool experience (duration of attendance) is an important factor for explaining early

educational outcomes.

-- About here, table IIIb--

In model 5, an interaction of the ethnic origin and the duration of preschool attendance is

tested (see table IIIb). This interaction effect is significant and positive, meaning that the

negative effect of immigrant status is reduced for immigrant children with more preschool

experience. Or stated the other way around: Immigrant children can gain more from attending

a preschool for a longer period. Model 6 tests the hypothesis that the influences of the

immigration status and the amount of preschool experience differ among preschools.

Therefore, a random slope of immigration status and preschool attendance is added. The

ethnic effect varies significantly among preschools. Also, the effect of the duration of

attendance differs according to preschool. The extension of the model leads to a change in the

effect of the interaction between preschool attendance and immigration status, which is

reduced and no longer significant. A likelihood-ratio test shows that models 5 and 6 are

significantly different (LR (5) = 92.61). This demonstrates the correctness of the

assumption that immigration status and preschool attendance have a different influence in

various preschools.

In model 7, a level 2 variable is added, the social composition of the preschools: This

constitutes the learning context for the children. This context variable has a significant

positive effect on the school readiness of children. Attending a preschool with a beneficial

14

Page 15: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

social composition seems to be more important than the duration of preschool attendance. But

in this model, each variable, the individual attendance duration and the preschool context, has

an independent effect. In the final model (model 8), a three-way interaction effect among

immigrant status, duration of preschool attendance, and preschool context is tested (all two-

way interactions are also included). This tests the assumption that the duration of preschool

attendance is more important for immigrant children than for natives, but only in positive

learning contexts. The existence of such an effect can be verified, and it is demonstrated in

figure 1.

-- About here, figure 1 --

Figure 1 shows the effects of the duration of preschool attendance and preschool context on

the school readiness of children. The social composition of the preschool is more important

for a child than the amount of his or her preschool experience. German and immigrant

children profit from attending a preschool with a positive learning context. In preschools with

a beneficial social composition, all children can benefit from a longer attendance (the longer,

the better), but immigrant children can profit more. The opposite is true for preschools with a

disadvantageous social composition: In these preschools, the duration of attendance has no

effect at all on immigrant children, while German children still can profit from a longer

preschool experience.

In sum, the school readiness of children depends on their family background and preschool

attendance. There really do exist differences between German and immigrant children with

regard to this early educational outcome. It is noteworthy that in preschools with beneficial

compositions, hardly any ethnic differences are left. The last results show that for German

children, the quantity of preschool experience is not that important as long as the preschool

context is positive. But immigrant children ought to attend preschools with a positive context

and they ought to attend these preschools for more than two years. In brief: The quantity and

the quality of a preschool do indeed matter.

5. Conclusion and discussion Ethnic educational inequality is not just a phenomenon in traditional school systems; it is also

a phenomenon in the preschool system. The present paper shows alarming ethnic differences

15

Page 16: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

in school readiness in the case of Germany. Immigrant children are already disadvantaged

before they have even started school. We assume that these disadvantages will hardly be

compensated for during their educational career, but we do not exclude the possibility that

some children might catch up.

This paper analyses ethnic inequality in school readiness as one aspect of early education,

considering both family background and preschool. We assume that besides other influences,

preschools have additional and independent effects on school readiness. On the one hand, we

demonstrate that family background is very important, and on the other hand, that preschool is

also explanatorily relevant. To test our assumptions that preschool experience and social

composition have a direct effect on school readiness, we calculated hierarchical linear models.

The results showed that it is important that a child attend preschool for at least two years.

What we did not show, but already know from other analyses, is that in the case of Germany,

there exists a linear effect of preschool attendance if children attended preschool for more

than one year (see Becker and Biedinger 2006). Moreover, the results indicate that children

should attend preschools with a beneficial social composition since this offers them a positive

learning context. All children can profit from attending preschools with positive contexts, but

immigrant children especially profit from attending such preschools for more than two years.

That confirms findings by Ding and Davison, who found that immigrant children must learn

more in order to compensate for the gap between themselves and native children (Ding and

Davison, 2005). Transferred to our results, immigrant children should not only attend

preschools for as long as possible, but they should attend preschools with special social

compositions in order to close this gap.

Nevertheless, some of our results are problematic. Preschool effects may be due to selection.

Perhaps especially those immigrant parents who tend to take their child to preschool at an

early age and to special-composition preschools are unique, and differ from other immigrant

parents in terms of some other (unmeasured) criteria. Furthermore, the cognitive development

at the time of entrance into kindergarten is not measured or controlled. Perhaps immigrant

children who are farther along in their development attend kindergarten earlier and for a

longer period. We need longitudinal data to control for this in detail. Unfortunately, data of

this kind are not available at the moment in Germany. On the other hand, the exact

mechanism behind the way(s) in which preschool influences school readiness is not

explained. We can only assume that there are a great many variables involved, e.g. social

16

Page 17: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

composition and size of groups, and/or the type of education and motivation of the teacher.

Additionally, our dependent variable is based on the examination by a paediatrician, and in

some ratings, it is conceivable that subjective influences might disadvantage children with a

migration background. But since most tests are highly standardised, this potential bias is

presumably rather small.

As a main result, we can see that ethnic educational inequality in Germany begins within

preschool education. Many immigrant children have more than one disadvantage. Because

immigrant families are often in a worse economic situation – like the working class – the

primary and secondary effects of Boudon are also at work at this early stage. These can be

indicated as a shorter duration of preschool attendance. Additionally, there exist some specific

ethnic disadvantages, basically caused by language deficits, self-selection, and mechanisms of

discrimination. The lesser amount of preschool experience on the part of immigrant children

could also be explained by a specific ethnic component, e.g., the absence of cultural

knowledge (e.g., information about term of application) (cf. Santel, 2000). The role of cultural

knowledge and attitudes in ethnic educational inequality has not yet been analysed clearly.

Our results bear implications for research on ethnic inequality, especially in Europe. The

research on ethnic inequality has focussed so far on the explanation of ethnic inequality in

labour forces or in educational systems. There has been little or no focus on early periods of

education or even on preschool systems. We maintain that immigrants often start their

educational careers burdened by a sum of disadvantages. Preschool is supposed to be one

important opportunity to compensate for disadvantages caused by the lack of resources within

immigrant families. As we have shown, the quantity, in terms of the amount of preschool

experience, and the (beneficial) social composition of preschool are important. Immigrant

children should mainly attend high-quality preschools for a longer period, because they suffer

even more if they do not. If immigrant children continue to attend poor-quality preschools, or

good ones only for a short time, their educational disadvantages will increase rather than

decrease.

Consequently, residential segregation is one main factor which determines compositions

within preschools: Immigrant families often live in areas with a disadvantageous social

composition. Since most families choose preschools which are located near their homes,

immigrant children have a greater risk for attending preschools with less beneficial learning

17

Page 18: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

contexts. Therefore, long-term measures of urban planning to reduce residential segregation

should be addressed in the range of politics. Since this is a difficult endeavour, the focus of

action should also be on improving the quality of preschools in socially disadvantaged

locations. A high preschool quality (in terms of qualification level of the personnel,

stimulating materials and activities, special programmes for disadvantaged children, etc.)

might compensate for the less beneficial social composition within preschools of such an area.

This, of course, is already being attempted in many places. Results on the success of such

programmes are rather mixed. There is still a great need for research to discover which

children can profit from which preschool attendance, and which conditions in preschools

might lead to greater educational equality at school start.

18

Page 19: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Tables and figures Table I: Operationalisations of model variables

Variable name Description Values Family background Number of siblings Number of siblings of the child Range [0; 10] Family situation Both parents present in the household or another

family constellation (e.g. single parent) 0: different family situation 1: both parents in the household

Education of father/mother

Dummy variables indicating the highest educational level of the child’s father/mother

- no formal education - vocational training - college degree

Employment status of father/mother

Dummy variable for the father’s/mother’s employment status

0: not employed 1: employed

Health security Effort to have medical prevention (approximate value of the parents’ effort on behalf of their children)

0: no 1: yes

Demographic characteristics of the child Immigrant children Ethnic origin of the child. A child is categorised as

an immigrant if neither parent was born in Germany. If parents were born in different countries, the native country of the mother is used as the basis for classification.

0: native 1: immigrant

Age Age of the child in months up to the time of the examination

Range [64.4; 90.7]

Sex Sex of the child 0: boy 1: girl

19

Page 20: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Table II: Descriptive statistics

Germans mean (std)

Immigrantsmean (std)

Score on school readiness 0.11 (0.93) -0.20 (1.05) *

Age in months 74.52 (3.51) 74.85 (3.67) *

Sex (female) 0.48 (0.50) 0.50 (0.50)

Number of siblings 1.19 (0.95) 1.46 (1.14) *

Family situation 0.86 (0.35) 0.89 (0.31) * Education of mothera,b

- No education - Voc. training - College degree (number of missing values)

0.07 (0.26)0.68 (0.47)0.25 (0.43)

(813)

0.44 (0.50)0.43 (0.49)0.14 (0.34)

(617)

***

Education of fathera,b

- No education - Voc. training - College degree (number of missing values)

0.04 (0.21)0.59 (0.49)0.36 (0.48)

(1208)

0.32 (0.47)0.49 (0.50)0.19 (0.39)

(702)

***

Employment status of mothera

(number of missing values)

Employment status of fathera

(number of missing values)

Health security

0.35 (0.48)(647)

0.94 (0.24)(1148)

0.95 (0.22)

0.22 (0.42)(326)

0.78 (0.42)(506)

0.78 (0.42)

*

* *

Preschool attendance

- More than 2 years

0.83 (0.38)

0.60 (0.49)

*

Context (social composition) 0.33 (0.78) -0.43 (0.97) *

N 5017 1760

Source: School entrance examination of Osnabrueck in the years 2000 to 2005, own calculations Notes: Means, standard deviation in brackets a) As far as education and employment status of parents are concerned, there are generally many missing values. The number of missing values is given in brackets. We did not want to reduce the number of cases in the analysis, so we constructed four missing-dummies. b) Differences to one are caused by truncation errors. * Differences between German and immigrant children are significant at p≤ 0.01.

20

Page 21: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Table IIIa: Preschool influence on school readiness (hierarchical linear model) Model 1 Model 2 Model 3 Model 4 Immigrant childrena -0.29 (0.00)* -0.15 (0.03)* -0.12 (0.03)*

Age in Months 0.04 (0.03)* 0.04 (0.00)* 0.04 (0.00)* Sexb 0.29 (0.02)* 0.29 (0.02)* 0.29 (0.02)* Year of investigationc

- 2001 - 2002 - 2003 - 2004 - 2005

0.050.180.220.250.29

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

0.010.120.170.210.26

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

0.000.110.160.190.23

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

Number of siblings -0.06 (0.01)* -0.06 (0.01)* Family situationd 0.11 (0.04)* 0.10 (0.04)* Education of mothere

- Voc. training - College degree - Missing values

0.160.210.02

(0.04)(0.05)(0.05)

* *

0.150.200.02

(0.04)(0.05)(0.05)

* *

Education of fathere

- Voc. training - College degree - Missing values

Employment status of motherf

- Employed - Missing values

Employment status of fatherf

- Employed - Missing values

0.100.240.06

0.090.09

0.080.06

(0.05)(0.05)(0.06)

(0.03)(0.04)

(0.04)(0.05)

* * * * *

0.090.230.05

0.070.09

0.080.05

(0.05)(0.05)(0.06)

(0.03)(0.04)

(0.04)(0.05)

* * *

Health security 0.16 (0.04)* 0.13 (0.04)* Preschool attendanceg 0.20 (0.03)* Constant -0.13 (0.09) -3.48 (0.25)* -4.16 (0.26)* -3.95 (0.26)* Level 2:

20r (var( )) jU 0

0.58 (0.10)* 0.56 (0.10)* 0.50 (0.09)* 0.47 (0.09)*

Level 1: σ2 (var( )) ijR 0.76 (0.04)* 0.71 (0.01)* 0.68 (0.01)* 0.67 (0.01)*

Number of children 6777

Number of preschools 78

Source: School entrance examination of Osnabrueck in the years 2000 to 2005, own calculations Notes: Regression coefficients, standard errors in parentheses Reference groups: a) Germans, b) boys, c) 2000, d) family situation without two parents, e) no education, f) unemployed, g) less than 2 years of preschool attendance. * p ≤ 0.05.

21

Page 22: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Table IIIb: Preschool influence on school readiness (hierarchical linear model) [continued] Model 5 Model 6 Model 7 Model 8 Immigrant childrena -0.20 (0.05)* -0.20 (0.06)* -0.21 (0.06)* -0.24 (0.06)*

Age in Months 0.04 (0.00)* 0.04 (0.00)* 0.04 (0.00)* 0.04 (0.00)* Sexb 0.29 (0.02)* 0.29 (0.02)* 0.29 (0.02)* 0.29 (0.02)* Year of investigationc

- 2001 - 2002 - 2003 - 2004 - 2005

0.000.110.160.190.23

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

-0.000.110.170.190.24

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

-0.000.110.170.190.24

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

-0.000.110.170.190.24

(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)(0.04)

* * * *

Number of siblings -0.06 (0.01)* -0.05 (0.01)* -0.05 (0.01)* -0.05 (0.01)* Family situationd 0.10 (0.04)* 0.10 (0.04)* 0.10 (0.04)* 0.09 (0.04)* Education of mothere

- Voc. training - College degree - Missing values

0.150.200.02

(0.04)(0.05)(0.05)

* *

0.160.210.02

(0.04)(0.05)(0.05)

* *

0.160.210.02

(0.04)(0.05)(0.05)

* *

0.150.200.02

(0.04)(0.05)(0.05)

* *

Education of fathere

- Voc. training - College degree - Missing values

Employment of motherf

- Employed - Missing values

Employment of fatherf

- Employed - Missing values

0.090.220.05

0.070.09

0.070.05

(0.05)(0.05)(0.06)

(0.02)(0.04)

(0.04)(0.05)

* * *

0.090.230.06

0.060.08

0.070.05

(0.05)(0.05)(0.06)

(0.02)(0.04)

(0.04)(0.05)

* * *

0.090.220.06

0.060.08

0.070.05

(0.05)(0.05)(0.06)

(0.02)(0.04)

(0.04)(0.05)

* * *

0.090.220.06

0.060.08

0.070.05

(0.05)(0.05)(0.06)

(0.02)(0.04)

(0.04)(0.05)

* * *

Health security 0.12 (0.04)* 0.13 (0.04)* 0.13 (0.04)* 0.13 (0.04)* Preschool attendanceg 0.15 (0.03)* 0.17 (0.06)* 0.17 (0.05)* 0.16 (0.06)* Immigrants*preschool 0.11 (0.05)* 0.05 (0.06) 0.03 (0.06) 0.07 (0.06) Preschool context 0.27 (0.06)* 0.28 (0.07)* Preschool*context 0.01 (0.06) Immigrants*context -0.11 (0.06) Immigrant*preschool*context 0.12 (0.06)* Constant -3.93 (0.26)* -3.90 (0.25)* -3.93 (0.25)* -3.95 (0.25)* Level 2:

20r (var( )) jU 0

0.48 (0.09)* 0.39 (0.08)* 0.32 (0.07)* 0.30 (0.06)*

21r (var( ))jU1

h 0.10 (0.03)* 0.10 (0.03)* 0.10 (0.03)* 2

2r (var( ))jU 2i 0.12 (0.04)* 0.11 (0.04)* 0.11 (0.04)*

Level 1: σ2 (var( )) ijR 0.67 (0.01)* 0.65 (0.01)* 0.65 (0.01)* 0.65 (0.01)*

Number of children 6777

Number of preschools 78

Source: School entrance examination of Osnabrueck in the years 2000 to 2005, own calculations Notes: Regression coefficients, standard errors in parentheses Reference groups: a) Germans, b) boys, c) 2000, d) family situation without two parents, e) no education, f) unemployed, g) less than 2 years of preschool attendance, h) random slope immigration status, i) random slope of preschool attendance duration. * p ≤ 0.05.

22

Page 23: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

-2

-1,5

-1

-0,5

0

0,5

1

< 2 years > 2 years

preschool attendance duration

scho

ol re

adin

ess

German children,negative context

Immigrant children,negative context

German children,positive context

Immigrant children,positive context

Figure 1: Influence of preschool on school readiness. Source: School entrance examination of Osnabrueck in the years 2000 to 2005, own calculations. Notes: Predicted values of model 8 are displayed for mean immigrant and German children (= all independent variables are set at mean). A positive preschool context means that the composition variable is two standard deviations above the mean of all preschools and a negative context means that the composition variable is two standard deviations below the mean.

23

Page 24: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Reference List

Alba, R. D., Handl, J. and Müller, W. (1994). Ethnic Inequalities in the German School System, Mannheimer Zentrum für Europäische Sozialforschung (MZES) Arbeitspapier, 8.

Andersson, B.-E. (1992). Effects of Day-Care on Cognitive and Socioemotional Competence of Thirteen-Year-Old Swedish Schoolchildren, Child Development, 63, 20-36.

Aughinbaugh, A. (2001). Does Head Start Yield Long-Term Benefits?, The Journal of Human Resources, 36, 641-665.

Bade, K. J. (2001). Einwanderungskontinent Europa: Migration und Integration am Ende des 20. Jahrhunderts. In Bade, K. J. (Eds) Einwanderungskontinent Europa: Migration und Integration am Beginn des 21. Jahrhunderts. Osnabrück: Universitätsverlag Rasch, 19-48.

Barnett, S. W. (1995). Long-Term Effects of Early Childhood Programs on Cognitive and School Outcomes, The Future of Children, 5, 25-50.

Becker, B. and Biedinger, N. (2006). Ethnische Bildungsungleichheit zu Beginn der Schulzeit, Kölner Zeitschrift für Soziologie und Sozialpsychologie, 58, 660-684. Becker, G. S. (1964). Human Capital. A Theoretical and Empirical Analysis with Special Reference to Education. New York: National Bureau of Economic Research.

Betts, J. R. and Lofstrom, M. (2000). The Educational Attainment of Immigrants. Trends and Implications. In Borjas, G. J. (Eds) Issues in the Economics of Immigration. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 51-115.

Blau, P. M. and Duncan, O. P. (1967). The American Occupational Structure. New York: John Wiley & Sons.

Bleakley, H. and Chin, A., (2004). What Holds Back the Second Generation? The Intergenerational Transmission of Language Human Capital Among Immigrants. http://www.uh.edu/~achin/research/bleakley_chin_intergenerational_language_oct2004.pdf.

Borjas, G. J. (1992). Ethnic Capital and Intergenerational Mobility, The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 107, 123-150.

Boudon, R. (1974). Education, Opportunity, and Social Inequality. Changing Prospects in Western Society. New York: Wiley & Sons.

Brandon, P. D. (2004). The Child Care Arrangements of Preschool-Age Children in Immigrant Families in the United States, International Migration, 42, 65-87.

Carmichael, S. and Hamilton, C. O. (1967). Black Power. The Politics of Liberation in America. London: Penguin.

Currie, J. and Thomas, D. (1995). Does Head Start Make a Difference?, The American Economic Review, 85, 341-364.

Dar, Y. and Resh, N. (1986). Classroom Intellectual Composition and Academic Achievement, American Educational Research Journal, 23, 357-374.

24

Page 25: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Department for Education and Skills (2005). Ethnicity and Education: The Evidence on Minority and Ethnic Pupils, DfES Research Topic Paper, RTP-01-05.

DiMaggio, P. (1982). Cultural Capital and School Success: The Impact of Status Culture Participation on the Grades of U.S. High School Students, American Sociological Review, 47, 189-201.

Ding, C. S. and Davison, M. L. (2005). A Longitudinal Study of Math Achievement Gains for Initially Low Achieving Students, Contemporary Educational Psychology, 30, 81-95.

Dinkel, R. H., Luy, M. and Lebok, U. (1999). Die Bildungsbeteiligung deutscher und ausländischer Jugendlicher in der Bundesrepublik Deutschland. In Lüttinger, P. (Eds) Sozialstrukturanalysen mit dem Mikrozensus. Mannheim: ZUMA Nachrichten Spezial, 354-375.

Duncan, G. J. and Magnuson, K. A. (2005). Can Family Socioeconomic Resources Account for Racial and Ethnic Test Score Gaps?, The Future of Children. School Readiness: Closing Racial and Ethnic Gaps, 15, 35-53.

Entorf, H. and Minoiu, N. (2004). PISA Results: What a Difference Immigration Law Makes, IZA Discussion Paper: Forschungsinstitut zur Zukunft der Arbeit.

Entwisle, D. K. (1995). The Role of Schools in Sustaining Early Childhood Program Benefits, The Future of Children, 5, 133-144.

Feagin, J. R. and Booher Feagin, C. (1986). Discrimination American Style - Institutional Racism and Sexism. Melbourne: Krieger Publication.

Fong, E. and Shibuya, K. (2005). Multiethnic Cities in North America, Annual Review Sociology, 31, 285-304.

Friedrichs, J. (2000). Ethnische Segregation im Kontext allgemeiner Segregationsprozesse in der Stadt. In Harth, A., Scheller, G. and Tessin, W. (Eds) Stadt und soziale Ungleichheit. Opladen: Leske + Budrich, 174-193.

Garces, E., Thomas, D. and Currie, J. (2002). Longer Term Effects of Head Start, The American Economic Review, 92, 999-1012.

Gomolla, M. and Radtke, F.-O. (2002). Institutionelle Diskriminierung. Die Herstellung ethnischer Differenz in der Schule. Opladen: Leske + Budrich.

Goodman, A. and Sianesi, B. (2005). Early Education and Children´s Outcomes: How Long Do the Impacts Last?, Fiscal Studies, 26, 513-548.

Gormley, W. T., Gayer, T., Phillips, D. and Dawson, B. (2005). The Effects of Universal Pre-K on Cognitive Development, Developmental Psychology, 41, 872-884.

Gramlich, E. M. (1986). Evaluation of Education Projects: the Case of the Perry Preschool Program, Economics of Education Review, 5, 17-24.

Haskins, R. (1989). Beyond Metaphor. The Efficacy of Early Childhood Education, American Psychologist, 44, 274-282.

25

Page 26: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Helmke, A., Hosenfeld, I., Schrader, F.-W. and Wagner, W. (2002). Sozialer und sprachlicher Hintergrund. In Helmke, A. and Jäger, R. S. (Eds) Das Projekt MARKUS. Mathematik-Gesamterhebung Rheinland-Pfalz: Kompetenzen, Unterrichtsmerkmale, Schulkontext. Landau: Verlag Empirische Pädagogik, 71-153.

Jonen, G., Eckhardt, T. and Jeuthe, E., (2005). The Education System in the Federal Republic of Germany 2003. A Description of Responsibilities, Structures and Developments in Education Policy for the Exchange of Information in Europe. http://www.kmk.org/dossier/preschool.pdf.

Kalter, F. (2003). Chancen, Fouls und Abseitsfallen. Migranten im Deutschen Ligenfußball. Wiesbaden: Westdeutscher Verlag.

Kalter, F. and Granato, N. (2002). Demographic Change, Educational Expansion, and Structural Assimilation of Immigrants. The Case of Germany, European Sociological Review, 18, 199-216.

Kristen, C. (2005). School Choice and Ethnic School Segregation. Primary School Selection in Germany. Münster: Waxmann.

Lamb, M. E. (2000). The Effects of Quality of Care on Child Development, Applied Developmental Science, 4, 112-115.

Lanfranchi, A. (2002). Schulerfolg von Migrationskindern. Die Bedeutung familienergänzender Betreuung im Vorschulalter. Opladen: Leske + Budrich.

Lange, R. d. and Rupp, J. C. C. (1992). Ethnic Background, Social Class or Status? Developments in School Attainment of the Children of Immigrant in the Netherlands, Ethnic and Racial Studies, 15, 284-303.

Leventhal, T. and Brooks-Gunn, J. (2003). Moving on Up: Neighborhood Effects on Children and Families. In Bornstein, M. H. and Bradley, R. H. (Eds) Socioeconomic Status, Parenting, and Child Development. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 209-230.

Livingston, A. (2006). The Condition of Education 2006 in Brief (NCES 2006-072). Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

Love, J. M., Harrison, L., Sagi-Schwartz, A., van Ijzendoorn, M. H., Ross, C., Ungerer, J. A., Raikes, H., Brady-Smith, C., Boller, K., Brooks-Gunn, J., Constantine, J., Kisker, E. E., Paulsell, D. and Chazan-Cohen, R. (2003). Child Care Quality Matters: How Conclusions may vary with Context, Child Development, 74, 1021-1033.

Magnuson, K. A., Meyers, M. K., Ruhm, C. J. and Waldfogel, J. (2004). Inequality in Preschool Education and School Readiness, American Educational Research Journal, 41, 115-157.

Magnuson, K. A. and Waldfogel, J. (2005). Early Childhood Care and Education: Effects on Ethnic and Racial Gaps in School Readiness, The Future of Children. School Readiness: Closing Racial and Ethnic Gaps, 15, 169-196.

Mare, R. D. (1980). Social Background and Educational Continuation Decisions, Journal of the American Statistical Association, 75, 295-305.

26

Page 27: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Marks, G. N. (2005). Accounting for Immigrant Non-Immigrant Differences in Reading and Mathematics in Twenty Countries, Ethnic and Racial Studies, 28, 925-946.

Mengering, F. (2005). Bärenstark - Empirische Ergebnisse der Berliner Sprachstandserhebung an Kindern im Vorschulalter, Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaften, 8, 241-262.

Neild, R. C. (2001). Same Difference: School Choice and Educational Access in an Urban District. Ann Arbor: UMI.

OECD (2006). Where Immigrant Students Succeed - A Comparative Review of Performance and Engagement in PISA 2003. http://www.oecd.org/dataoecd/2/38/36664934.pdf.

Ondrin, J. and Spiess, K. C. (1998). Care of Children in a low Fertility Setting: Transitions between Home and Market Care for Pre-School Children in Germany, Population Studies, 52, 35-48.

Portes, A. and MacLeod, D. (1999). Educating the Second Generation: Determinants of Academic Achievement Among Children of Immigrants in the United States, Journal of Ethnic and Migration Studies, 25, 373-396.

Portes, A. and Rumbaut, R. G. (2001). Legacies, The Story of the Immigrant Second Generation. New York: Russel Sage Foundation.

Rabe-Hesketh, S. and Skrondal, A. (2005). Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling Using Stata. Texas: Stata Press.

Rohling, I. (2002). Gesundheit und Entwicklungsstand der Osnabrücker Schulanfänger. Multifaktorielle Analyse der Ergebnisse der Schuleingangsuntersuchungen unter besonderer Berücksichtigung des Jahrgangs 2001. http://www.loegd.nrw.de/1pdf_dokumente/2_gesundheitspolitik_gesundheitsmanagement/sammlung-kgberichte/zentraler-berichtsserver/niedersachsen/osnabrueck_stadt_schulanfaenger.pdf.

Rumberger, R. W. and Tran, L., (2006). Preschool Participation and the Cognitive and Social Development of Language Minority Students. http://lmri.ucsb.edu/publications/06_rumberger-tran.pdf.

Sammons, P., Elliot, K., Sylva, K., Melhuish, E., Siraj-Blatchford, I. and Taggart, B. (2004). The Impact of Pre-School on Young Children's Cognitive Attainments at Entry to Reception, British Educational Research Journal, 30, 691-712.

Santel, B. (2000). Die Lebenslage junger Migranten: Zur Problematik der "Dritten Generation", Eine Tagung der Friedrich-Ebert-Stiftung am 3. Mai 1995 in Bochum: Die dritte Generation: integriert, angepaßt oder ausgegrenzt?

Schöler, H., Dutzi, I., Roos, J., Schäfer, P., Grün-Nolz, P. and Engler-Thümmel, H., (2004). Einschulungsuntersuchung 2003 in Mannheim. http://www.ph-heidelberg.de/wp/schoeler/Arbeitsbericht16.pdf.

Snijders, T. A. B. and Bosker, R. J. (1999). Multilevel Analysis. An Introduction to Basic and Advanced Multilevel Modeling. London: SAGE Publications.

Statistisches Bundesamt (2004). Statistisches Jahrbuch 2004 für die Bundesrepublik

27

Page 28: Early ethnic educational inequality: the influence of

Deutschland. Wiesbaden.

Sullivan, A. (2001). Cultural Capital and Educational Attainment, Sociology, 35, 893-912.

U.S. Department of Education and National Center for Education Statistics by Kristin Denton, Jerry West and Jill Walston (2003). Reading - Young Children's Achievement and Classroom Experiences, NCES 2003-070. Washington, DC: National Center for Education Statistics.

West, A. (2006). School Choice, Equity and Social Justice: The Case for more Control, British Journal of Educational Studies, 54, 15-33.

West, J. (2005). Early Childhood Longitudinal Study. Kindergarten Class of 1998-99. http://nces.ed.gov/ecls/pdf/ksum.pdf.

Worswick, C. (2004). Adaptation and Inequality: Children of Immigrants in Canadian Schools, Canadian Economics Association, 37, 53-77.

28