33
Double Representation of the Body Surface within Cytoarchitectonic Areas 3b and 1 in “SI” in the Owl Monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) MICHAEL M. MERZENICH,I JON H. KAAS,’ MRIGANKA SUR CHIA-SHENG LIN AND Coleman Laboratory, Departments of Otolaryngology and Physiology, University of California at San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143; and the Departments of Psychology, Electrical Engineering and ‘Anatomy, Vanderbilt University, Nashuille, Tennessee 37240 ABSTRACT Microelectrode multiunit mapping studies of parietal cortex in owl monkeys indicate that the classical “primary” somatosensory region (or “SI”) including the separate architectonic fields 3a, 3b, 1, and 2 contains as many as four separate representations of the body rather than one, An analysis of receptive field locations for extensive arrays of closely placed recording sites in parietal cortex which were later related to cortical architecture led to the following conclusions: (1) There are two large systematic representations of the body surface within “SI.” Each is activated by low threshold cutaneous stimuli; one representation is coextensive with Area 3b and the other with Area 1. (2) While each of these representations contains regions of cortex with topological or “somatotopic” transformations of skin surface, the representations have many discontinuities where adjoining skin surfaces are not adjoining in the representations. Thus, the representations can be considered as composites of somatotopically organized regions, but cannot be accurately depicted by simple continuous homunculi. Lines of discontinuity often cut across dermatomes and seldom follow dermatomal boundaries, i.e., neither cutaneous representation constitutes a systematic representation of dermatomal skin fields. (3) While the two cutaneous fields are basically similar in organization and are approxi- mate mirror images of each other, they differ in important details, i.e., lines of discontinuity in the representations and the sites of representations of dif- ferent specific skin surfaces differ significantly in the two representations. (4) The two cutaneous representations also differ in size and in the relative propor- tions of cortex devoted to representation of various body parts. Because the pro- portions in each representation differ, they cannot both be simple reflections of overall peripheral innervation density. (5) All or part of Area 2 contains a sys- tematic representation of deep body structures. These conclusions are consistent with a view of the anterior parietal region as containing functionally distinct fields at least partially related to different subsets of receptor populations and coding or representing different aspects of somatic sensation. We suggest that the “SI” region of primates be redefined as a parietal somatosensory strip, the Area 1 representation as the posterior cuta- neous field, and, for reasons of probable homology with “SI” of other mammals, the Area 3b representation as SI proper. 5 Present address, Department of Physiology, University of Virginia Medical School, Charlottsville, Virginia 22901 J. COMP. NEUR. (1978) 181: 41-74. 41

Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    4

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

Double Representation of the Body Surface within Cytoarchitectonic Areas 3b and 1 in “SI” in the Owl Monkey (Aotus trivirgatus)

MICHAEL M. MERZENICH,I JON H. KAAS,’ ’ MRIGANKA SUR CHIA-SHENG LIN

AND

’ Coleman Laboratory, Departments of Otolaryngology and Physiology, University of California at San Francisco, San Francisco, California 94143; and the Departments of Psychology, Electrical Engineering and ‘Anatomy, Vanderbilt University, Nashuille, Tennessee 37240

ABSTRACT Microelectrode multiunit mapping studies of parietal cortex in owl monkeys indicate that the classical “primary” somatosensory region (or “SI”) including the separate architectonic fields 3a, 3b, 1, and 2 contains as many as four separate representations of the body rather than one, An analysis of receptive field locations for extensive arrays of closely placed recording sites in parietal cortex which were later related to cortical architecture led t o the following conclusions: (1) There are two large systematic representations of the body surface within “SI.” Each is activated by low threshold cutaneous stimuli; one representation is coextensive with Area 3b and the other with Area 1. (2) While each of these representations contains regions of cortex with topological or “somatotopic” transformations of skin surface, the representations have many discontinuities where adjoining skin surfaces are not adjoining in the representations. Thus, the representations can be considered as composites of somatotopically organized regions, but cannot be accurately depicted by simple continuous homunculi. Lines of discontinuity often cut across dermatomes and seldom follow dermatomal boundaries, i.e., neither cutaneous representation constitutes a systematic representation of dermatomal skin fields. (3) While the two cutaneous fields are basically similar in organization and are approxi- mate mirror images of each other, they differ in important details, i.e., lines of discontinuity in the representations and the sites of representations of dif- ferent specific skin surfaces differ significantly in the two representations. ( 4 ) The two cutaneous representations also differ in size and in the relative propor- tions of cortex devoted to representation of various body parts. Because the pro- portions in each representation differ, they cannot both be simple reflections of overall peripheral innervation density. ( 5 ) All or part of Area 2 contains a sys- tematic representation of deep body structures.

These conclusions are consistent with a view of the anterior parietal region as containing functionally distinct fields a t least partially related to different subsets of receptor populations and coding or representing different aspects of somatic sensation. We suggest that the “SI” region of primates be redefined as a parietal somatosensory strip, the Area 1 representation as the posterior cuta- neous field, and, for reasons of probable homology with “SI” of other mammals, the Area 3b representation as SI proper.

5 Present address, Department of Physiology, University of Virginia Medical School, Charlottsville, Virginia 22901

J. COMP. NEUR. (1978) 181: 41-74. 41

Page 2: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

42 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS. M. SUR AND C:S. LIN

Over the last four decades, electrophysiolog- ical mapping experiments conducted in a number of mammalian species have estab- lished the view that the parietal cortex of all mammals contains a t least two systematic representations of the body surface, i.e., the first (first discovered) and second somatosen- sory areas, SI and SII. Many of the early pioneering experiments were on macaque monkeys (Marshall et al., ’37; Woolsey et al., ’42). These studies demonstrated an orderly progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central parietal cortex that later became known as “SI” (Woolsey and Fairman, ‘46). From the beginning, i t was rec- ognized that this “SI” of macaque monkeys encompassed three of the architect.onic fields of Brodmann (’09), Areas 3 , l and 2. The more caudal parietal Areas 5 and 7, and the precen- tral motor field, Area 4, were excluded from the representation on the basis of a sharp de- cline in the amplitude of potentials evoked by somatosensory stimuli whenever the re- cording electrode was moved onto the surface of these Areas.

Initial reports on the postcentral parietal cortex in macaques emphasized the mediolat- eral Organization. The mediolateral sequence of representation was found to parallel that defined concurrently by electrical stimulation in the anterior parietal cortex in man (Pen- field and Boldrey, ’37). That is, stimulating the tail evoked surface potentials from re- cording sites in the extreme medial aspect of the representational region; more laterally, the postaxial leg, then the foot, the preaxial leg, the body, the arm, the hand, and final- ly the head were successively represented (Woolsey e t al., ’42). In these studies, the ros- trocaudal changes in the body surfaces repre- sented in “SI” were less dramatic; receptive fields tended t o remain in the same general body region. Determination of the rostrocau- dal order of the representation was aggra- vated by the fact that the most rostral half of the responsive region including nearly all of Area 3 was buried in the central sulcus, and was accessible for exploration by surface elec- trodes only after extensive cortical ablations. Possibly for these reasons, the rostrocaudal or- ganization of the responsive zones was not stressed. In later reviews of the organization of somatosensory cortex in mammals, Woolsey (’54, ’58, ’64) summarized both the mediolat- eral and rostrocaudal organization of post- central parietal cortex of macaque monkeys

with the now familiar distorted “homun- culus” (or “simiunculus”) of the contralateral half of the body surface, which was superim- posed on a lateral view of the brain to show the major features of “SI” organization. The homunculus portrayed the body on the brain with the distal digits of the hands and feet pointing rostrally along the rostral border of Area 3 (actually, of 3a), the head laterally, and the back of the animal caudally (along the caudal border of Area 2).

A difficulty with the concept of the organi- zation of postcentral parietal cortex as a homunculus overlying three or four major architectonic fields became evident with the publication of the microelectrcde studies of the properties of single neurons in parietal cortex, by Powell and Mountcastle (’59a,b) and Mountcastle and Powell (’59). These in- vestigators demonstrated that neurons in each of three architectonic fields (actually, 3b, 1,2) of “SI” tended to have different modes of response to sensory stimuli. Thus, cells of Area 2 responded almost exclusively to the stimulation of deep rather than cutaneous re- ceptors; neurons in Area 1 responded to both types of stimulation; and cells in Area 3b were predominantly activated by light tactille stim- ulation of cutaneous receptors. This discovery meant that if the proposed concept of the ros- trocaudal organization of postcentral parietal cortex were valid, then different body parts would be subserved by populations of neurons with decidedly different functional properties. However, i t was still possible to conclude that distal and proximal body parts have different roles in sensory experience, and to argue that the homunculus made functional sense in that the input from cutaneous receptors would be predominant rostrally in Area 3b, where the highly sensitive finger tips were located in the representation, while neurons activated by the stimulation of joints and deep body tissues would be predominant in Area 2, where the less sensitive proximal parts of the limbs and the back were represented.

The concept of an unequal distribution of re- ceptor inputs to a single cortical representa- tion, “SI,” became less attractive as further microelectrcde studies of neurons in parietal cortex extended the basic discoveries of Powell and Mountcastle. It gradually bt, ’came apparent that neurons in Area 2 are principal- ly responsive to the rotation of joints arid the stimulation of periosteum for fascia (or other “deep” structures); Area 1 neurons are acti-

Page 3: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 43

vated by cutaneous input from quickly adapt- ing receptors; Area 3b derives its input from cutaneous quickly and slowly adapting recep- tor classes; while Area 3a processes informa- tion from muscle afferents and other deep re- ceptors (Mountcastle and Powell, ’59; Powell and Mountcastle, ’59b; Werner and Whitsel, ’68; Phillips et al., ’71; Paul et al., ’72a; Yumiya et al., ‘74; Lucier et al., ’75; Tanji, ’75; Heath et al., ’76; Krishnamurti et al., ’76). Furthermore, the boundaries between neu- rons with different response properties were often recognized as being sharp rather than gradual, and were felt to coincide with cy- toarchitectonic boundaries. Such stepwise changes in functional zones are obviously less compatible with the concept of a single body representation overlying these fields than was the earlier postulation of gradients of func- tional change.

A second concept of the organization of the postcentral parietal cortex in primates avoided the apparent inconsistency of the single neuron data with the idea of a single homunculus extending over Areas 3, 1 and 2. Powell and Mountcastle (’59b) first suggested that the afferent fibers relayed from a single dorsal root relate to a narrow rostrocaudal band across all the “SI” architectonic fields. While such a statement is not clearly in con- flict with the single homunculus viewpoint, it is not far from the more specific concept that each body part is represented in a band across the three or four architectonic fields, so that each body part is in all fields and can be sub- served by all neuronal types. This “rostrocau- dal band” concept received support from ob- servations that neurons at the same mediolat- era1 level in the separate architectonic fields were found to have similar receptive field locations (Powell and Mountcastle, ’59b; Werner and Whitsel, ’71; Pubols and Pubols, ’71). However, even strong supporters (Pubols and Pubols, ’72) of the “rostrocaudal band” concept recognized evidence for somatotop- ic organization within “rostrocaudal bands” that was clearly inconsistent with a simple rostrocaudal “isorepresentation” of all body surfaces in the postcentral region (which is, in fact, a topological impossibility).

A third viewpoint on the organization of the “SI” region of primates emerged from an ex- tensive series of microelectrode mapping stud- ies on squirrel and macaque monkeys, first initiated by Werner and Whitsel (’68, ’71; Dreyer et al., ’74, ’75; Whitsel et al., ’71).

While these authors concentrated on defini- tion of the mediolateral organization of the postcentral parietal cortex (which they de- scribed as a dermatomal sequence), they also indicated the general features of their concept of the rostrocaudal organization. As in the original reports of Woolsey et al. (’421, these investigators regarded “SI” as encompassing several architectonic fields, i.e., Areas 3a, 3b, 1 and 2. Larger body units such as the face, arm, hand, trunk, and leg were portrayed as rostrocaudal bands extending across all of these architectonic divisions (fig. 12 of Whitsel et al., ’71). In this regard, their view- point was similar to the “rostrocaudal band” concept stemming from the experiments of Powell and Mountcastle (’59b). However, in their view, a general somatotopy is preserved within these rostrocaudal bands. The repre- sentation of some body surfaces are depicted as extending over several architectonic divi- sions; others are confined to one or two divi- sions; still others are duplicated in the repre- sentation, so as to occur both rostrally in Areas 3a and 3b and caudally in Areas 1 and 2 (see, for example, fig. 9 in Whitsel e t al., ’71). For example, the volar surface of the hand is shown as extending over Areas 3a, 3b, 1 and 2 with the distal tips of the digits at the rostral margin of 3a and the proximal part of the hand near the caudal margin of Area 2. By contrast, the representation of the dorsal hand is shown exclusively or mainly in Area 3b. Finally, the abdomen is shown both ros- trally in Areas 3a and 3b and caudally in Areas 1 and 2. These two representations of the abdomen are separated by the back, located near the border of Areas 3b and 1. It is clear that even this complicated scheme only partially solves the problem of the incon- gruency of receptor segregation within dis- tinct architectonic fields and the orderly map of the body surface across these fields.

A quite different possibility for the organi- zation of the postcentral parietal cortex was suggested by the experiments of Paul et al. (’72a,b, ’75). In microelectrode mapping ex- periments of the hand region of Areas 3b and 1 of macaque monkeys, they discovered two sep- arate and topographical representations of the volar hand “in its entirety.” Each of the two representations was found to correspond to a separate architectonic subdivision with distinct neural properties. Thus, the rostral representation of the hand was confined to Area 3b and was dominated by input from

Page 4: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

44 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS. M. SUR AND C . - S . LIN

quickly and slowly adapting cutaneous mech- anoreceptors, while the caudal representation was limited to Area 1 and was activated by input from quickly adapting cutaneous mech- anoreceptors. Area 2 was not systematically explored, but limited recordings there sug- gested a third representation of the hand re- sponsive to higher threshold stimulation of "deep" receptors. The results of Paul et al. ('72a) raise the possibility that architectonic subdivisions of the postcentral parietal cortex actually cons tit u t e separate represen tations of the body, each processing input from a dis- tinct subset of receptors. The present mapping experiments were designated to gather evi- dence for or against this last possibility.

The owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus) was used in these experiments because the central sulcus consists of only a very shallow, short dimple on the cortical surface (e.g., see fig. 151, and because there are no sulci in parietal cortex caudal to the parietal somatosensory cortical region. Thus, the entire surfaces of cy- toarchitectonic Areas 4, 3a, 3b, 1, 2, 5 and 7, except those buried in the medial wall or Syl- vian fissure, are exposed on the lateral corti- cal surface. The results demonstrate that there are separate and apparently complete representations of the contralateral body within the separate architectonic fields of the "SI" region.

A preliminary report of some of these re- sults has been published elsewhere (Kaas et al., '76).

METHODS

Experiments were conducted with nine adult owl monkeys (Aotus trivirgatus). Ani- mals were initially anesthetized with keta- mine hydrochloride (50 mg/kg, IM). They were maintained with supplementary injections of ketamine a t a surgical level of anesthesia. Body temperature was monitored, and kept a t 37" throughout these long (20-50 hours) re- cording experiments. Parietal cortex was ex- posed via a wide craniotomy. An acrylic dam was constructed around this skull opening to maintain a pool of silicone over the exposed brain surface. A high resolution photograph of the brain surface was then taken. The surface vasculature (observed with a dissecting mi- croscope) was used to site electrode penetra- tions on this photograph.

Glass-coated platinum-iridium microelec- trodes with impedances of 2-3 Ma were in- troduced in penetrations perpendicular to the

surface of the anterior parietal cort.ex. The amplified electrode output was fed to an oscilloscope, and to a loudspeaker. Mapping was usually initiated just posterior to a shallow dimple (a poorly developed central sulcus) commonly falling a t or near the boundaries of somatosensory cortex and motor cortex. The vertical axis of Penetrations was set BO that penetrations about midway be- tween the midline and the crest of the Sylvian sulcus were precisely normal to the cortical surface. All penetrations in any given experi- ment over the relatively flat parietal cortex were parallel with this axis.

In most experiments, an effort was made to map a given sector of the somatosensory cor- tex in great detail. Up to nearly 500 (usually several hundred) penetrations were made in each hemisphere studied. Electrode penetra- tions were usually spaced a t 200- to 300-gm intervals. The most extensive mapping experi- ments were within cytoarchitectonic Areas 3b and 1, as later verified in serial sagittal brain sections. More limited recordings were ob- tained rostrally in motor cortex, and caudally in Areas 2 and 5.

Within Areas 3b and 1, neurons in the mid- dle cortical layers were strongly driven by light tactile stimulation. Receptive fields were defined using fine black glass probes while the relevant body part was carefully stabilized to prevent movement. All receptive fields derived in these maps were minimum fields, defined as the skin region from which a distinct response could be evoked by very light tactile stimulation. There was a consistent at- tempt to determine whether input arose from cutaneous receptors, or from joint or other deep receptors. Outlines of receptive fields were drawn on enlarged photographs of body surfaces of the owl monkey.

In initial experiments, the receptive field boundaries were determined for different neu- rons or clusters of neurons at a number of depths across the active depths of cortex with- in single penetrations. They were found to be remarkably constant throughout vertical pen- etrations into the cortex, as described by others in other species (e.g., Mountcastle, '57; Powell and Mountcastle, '59b; Werner and Whitsel, '68; Whitsel et al., '71; Paul et al., '72a; Dreyer e t al., '75). In later experiiments (in which the most extensive maps were de- rived), one or two very careful receptive field determinations were made in each penetra- tion. Although receptive fields of isolated neu-

Page 5: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 45

Fig. 1 The organization of the two repreaentations of the contralateral body surface within two cytoar- chitectonic fields (3b, 1) in the anterior parietal cortex of the owl monkey, Aotus triuirgatus. Sectors within each map representing the surfacea of the leg, sole, individual toes, preaxial thigh, trunk, arm, wrist, palm, individual fingers, chin, chin vibriesae, mandibular vibrisaae, lower lip and upper lip are demarcated. Cortex representing the dorsal hairy surfaces of the fingers and toes are shaded. This reconstruction is based on re- ceptive fields from 486 electrode penetrations in the “SI” region in a single owl monkey. Similar patterns of organization were found in other monkeys.

rons were sometimes mapped, any given recep- tive field in the illustrated maps must be assumed to have been derived for small clus- ters of neurons (Paul e t al., ’72a; Allman and Kaas, ‘74; Merzenich et al., ’75). Without ex- ception, all neurons studied at any given corti- cal locus had identical or nearly identical re- ceptive fields.

Following recording experiments, small in- jections of horseradish peroxidase and/or triti- ated proline were introduced at physiological defined locations. At this time, small elec- trolytic lesions were introduced a t physiologi- cally significant landmarks (usually a t the borders of representations), and a series of

surface locations were marked with carbon black to facilitate transfer of mapping data to photographs of the brain surface in that mon- key. The animal was maintained with keta- mine anesthesia for 20 to 30 hours after the termination of the recording experiment in order to allow the transport of the anatomical tracers. (Cerebral edema was, surprisingly, not a serious problem in these very long pro- cedures in the owl monkey.) The animal was then perfused intracardially with saline fol- lowed by 8% formol-saline. The brain was kept overnight in fixative (at 4 W , passed through several increasing concentrations of sucrose in formalin ending in 30% sucrose, and the cor-

Page 6: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

46 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C:S. LIN

Fig. 2 Receptive fields defined within a row of electrode penetrations crossing the regions of Areas 3b and 1 representing the third digit and distal palm. The locations of electrode penetrations are shown on the right on a reconstruction of the mapped portion of Areas 3b and 1. The border between Areas 3b and 1 was defined architectonically. Receptive fields defined within penetrations in Area 3b are outlined with a solid line; those defined within Area 1 are crosshatched. Receptive fields drawn off the digits are on the dorsal hairy surfaces of the fingers a t that location. The shaded areas in the reconstruction of the experiment are the regions of representation of the hairy surfaces of the digits

tex cut frozen in the sagittal plane a t 25 or 50 pm section thickness. Usually, every fifth brain section was processed for autoradiogra- phy and counterstained with cresyl violet. Another set of every fifth section was stained for myelin with hematoxylin. All remaining sections were stained with cresyl violet for cells. Experiments were later reconstructed, so that results of brain cytoarchitecture could be systemically related to mapping and tracer studies. Results of HRP and tritiated amino acid tracer studies are described elsewhere (Lin et al., ’78).

RESULTS

There are two highly ordered representa- tions of the contralateral body surface within the “primary somatosensory cortex” (“SI”) in

the owl monkey. The basic pattern of repre- sentation of the body surface within these two large cortical subdivisions is shown schemat- ically in figure 1. The relationship of the two areas is complex when considered in detail, and they are not identical. The two represen- tations have been found to occupy cytoarchi- tectonic Areas 3b and 1 (as will be described later). In order to appreciate how body surface regions are represented in somatosensory coy- tex, i t is useful to consider in detail the pat- tern of representation of specific skin surfaces within these two fields.

Representation ofthe hand in cytoarchitectonic Areas 3b and 1

The hand is represented completely within

Page 7: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

A ORGANIZATION

A OF PRIMATE “SI” 47

Fig. 3 Receptive fields defined for neurons within two rows of penetrations crossing Area 3b and Area 1 in the region representing the thumb and thenar eminence (row D, in the reconstruction to the right), and in the region representing the fifth digit and fourth interdigital and insular palmar pads (row Ds).

Area 3b and completely within Area 1. The two representations are roughly “mirror im- ages.” The tips of the fingers in Area 3b are represented along its anterior border and in Area 1 along its posterior border (fig. 4; figs. 2, 3). The rest of the glabrous skin of the fingers is represented twice in between with a rever- sal along the palm a t the 3-1 border (figs. 2-5).

There is a highly ordered representation of the palmar pads in both representations (figs. 4, 5). The basic pattern of representation, de- rived from a relatively complete map of the hand obtained in a single experiment, is shown diagrammatically in figure 5. Within each representation, the thenar and hypothe- nar eminences are displaced from each other and represented in regions lateral and medial (respectively) to the representation of the in- terdigital pads and fingers. This split repre- sentation of the basal palm is more exagger- ated in Area 1 than in Area 3. In rows of pene- trations extending across the middle of the

hand Le., the row illustrated in fig. 2), the line of reversal (in fig. 2, between electrode penetrations 4 and 5) runs across the base of the interdigital and insular pads. Thus, the representation of the glabrous surface of the hand is discontinuous. Topological discon- tinuities were found between the adjoining surfaces of the thenar and hypothenar emi- nences, the radial and ulnar insular pads, part of the border of both thenar and hypothenar eminences with adjacent interdigital pads, and in Area 1, between the most proximal and the more distal aspects of the thenar emi- nence.

The representation of the glabrous surfaces of the hand in Area 3b is a precisely ordered array. This order is evident in very fine grained mapping, such as that conducted in experiment 76- 10, from which receptive fields derived in a continuous row of penetrations across the Area 3b hand representation are shown in figure 4. Penetrations over most of

Page 8: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

48 M. M. MERZENICH. J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C.3 . LIN

= / 3b

76-10 Fig. 4 Receptive fields defined for neurons within penetrations along two long rows within the hand representations

in Areas 3b and 1. Receptive fields for penetrationa in Area 3b are on the left; receptive fields for penetrations in Area 1 are on the right. The locations of penetrations in the two rows (dots and open circles) are shown in the reconstructlon of the map derived in this study (bottom).

this row were about 200-250 p m apart. In this and similar rows, the centers of successively defined fields shifted in a highly predictable way, usually with significant overlap of mini- mum receptive fields (like the partially shift- ed overlap for receptive fields defined in adja- cent cortical columns, by Mountcastle, '57, and Powell and Mountcastle, '59b). The repre- sentation of the ventral, glabrous surface of

the hand in Area 3b was strictly segregated from the representation of the dorsal surfaces of the fingers and hand.

The representation of the glabrous surfaces of the hand in Area 1 was, at first glance, a mirror image of t ha t in Area 3b, but when con- sidered in detail several obvious differences were apparent. The orderliness of the A,rea 1 representation was not as strict as that of

Page 9: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 49

n

U / ” -

Fig. 5 The representations of the glabrous hand in Areas 3b and 1. The hand surface (below) is split be- tween the pads of the palm and distorted (upper left) to fit the confines of area 3b in the cortical representa- tion (upper right). The Area 1 representation is roughly a mirror image of the Area 3b representation. The detailed reconstruction of the hand representations shown on the upper right is based on a single extensive experiment. Receptive fields in Area 1 frequently extended over more than one pad, so that the indicated boundaries in the representation are not absolute. Asterisks in Area 1 indicate penetrations with neurons with very large receptive fields suggestive of Pacinian corpuscle input. The representations of the dorsum of the hand are shaded. Receptive fields in Hd (hand dorsum) were restricted to the hand dorsum; receptive fields for Hw (hand and wrist dorsum) included both parts of the wrist and hand dorsum. The digits (D,-D,) and interdigital pads (1-4) are numbered in the standard manner. Ir and Iu, radial and ulnar insular pads, T and H, thenar and hypothenar pads.

Area 3b. Receptive fields were commonly sig- nificantly larger within Area 1 than within Area 3b (see fields illustrated in figs. 2-41. Fields over the palmar pads, for example, fre- quently extended over an entire pad, or over broad sectors of adjacent pads. Nonetheless, the sequence of representation of the pads was very similar t o that of Area 3b, with two ex- ceptions. First, there was a small representa- tion of the basal aspect of the thenar eminence in the extreme medial aspect of the Area 1 hand representation, adjacent to the repre- sentation of the wrist, and far from the repre- sentation of the rest of the thenar eminence on the extreme lateral side of the hand repre-

sentation (fig. 5). Second, receptive fields along the base of the palm for neurons in Area 1 commonly included both the glabrous skin and some of the hairy skin of the wrist. Recep- tive fields for neurons in the Area 3b glabrous representational region never crossed the glabrous-hairy skin boundary.

The most striking difference in the hand representation of the two Areas, however, was in the pattern of representation of the dorsal hairy surface (fig. 5). Within Area 3b, the rep- resentation of the dorsal surfaces of the fingers was split. The representation of the hairy surfaces of the first and sometimes the second digit were on the far lateral aspect of

Page 10: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

50 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C.-S. LIN

Fig. 6 Receptive fields defined within a row of penetrations crossing Area 3b and 1 in the region repre- senting the volar wrist. Details as in figure 2.

the hand representation adjacent to the glabrous thumb. The hairy surfaces of the fifth, fourth, third and sometimes the second digits were on the extreme medial aspect of the hand representation adjacent to the glabrous surfaces of the fifth digit. The hairy surfaces were represented as a continuation of glabrous surfaces. Thus, the representation of the outer glabrous margin of the thumb in the lateral region moved onto the dorsal surface and marched across the hairy surfaces of dig- its 1 and sometimes 2. In the medial region, the representation progressed from the outer glabrous margin of digit 5 and then, in order across the hairy surfaces of digits 5, 4, 3 and sometimes 2. The representation of the back of the hand in Area 3b was continuous with the medial representation of the hairy finger sur- faces (digits 5,4,3,2-var.I. The representation

of the hairy surfaces of the hand within Area 1 was very different. There, the dorsal surfaces of the fingers were represented within a band of cortex between the representation of the distal and middle phalangeal pads (fig. 51, or (in one case see fig. 4) between the representa- tion of the proximal phalangeal pads and the palm. Thus, the dorsal hairy surfaces of the fingers in Area 1 were represented in an island of cortex completely encircled by the repre- sentation of the glabrous surfaces of the hand, and thereby creating a discontinuity in their representation by separating fingertips (or fingers) from the rest of the fingers (or palm). The hairy skin of the hand dorsum in Area 1 was represented in a region medial to the rep- resentation of the hand and volar wriist, far separated from the representation of tlhe dor- sal surfaces of the fingers. The piecemeal rep-

~

Page 11: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 51

Fig. 7 Receptive fields defined within a row of penetrations crossing Areas 3b and 1 in the region of rep resentation of the dorsal surface of the wrist. Details as in figure 2.

resentation of the hairy surfaces of the hand in Areas 3b and 1 is again illustrated in detail in the schematic drawing in figure 5.

Representation of the wrist and arm in Areas 3b and 1

There are also two separate representations of the wrist and arm in Areas 3b and 1. The pattern of organization is complicated by the fact that the volar wrist surface was repre- sented separately, and by the fact that the order of representation of the arm in Area 1 was often marked by discontinuities which differed in detail from monkey to monkey.

The volar wrist was represented within a narrow strip of cortex immediately adjacent t o the representation of the hypothenar emi- nence, in both Areas 3b and 1. Volar wrist fields were in continuous sequence with fields

on the eminence. As noted earlier, in Area 1 fields commonly overlapped the hairy wrist and glabrous hypothenar (or thenar) emi- nence surfaces, while in Area 3b, fields did not cross the hairy-glabrous skin boundary. The ulnar side of the volar wrist was represented toward the anterior margin of Area 3b, and near the posterior margin of Area 1 (fig. 6). Proceeding toward the 3- 1 border in either field, successively defined receptive fields shifted over the wrist toward its radial margin, which was represented along the 3-1 border.

The dorsal wrist was represented within a narrow strip of cortex just medial to the repre- sentation of the volar wrist in an ulnar to radial to ulnar sequence identical to that of the volar wrist. Thus, as shown by example in figure 7, the ulnar side of the wrist dorsum

Page 12: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

52 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C.-S. LIN

Brown Fur Dorsal Surfaces

White Fur

Ventral Surfaces

Hand Dorsum

Dorsal Wrist

Ventral Wrist

Fig. 8 The patterns of representation o f the arm and trunk in Areas 3b and 1 in a single monkey (76.9). Representation of the brown fur of the arm dorsum, aide and back of the trunk and lateral thigh (shaded re- eon) and of the white fur of the ventral arm, axilla, chest, abdomen and medial thigh (not shaded) are shown. The patterns of representation of the arm and trunk differ in detail among individual monkeys (see text)

was represented near the anterior border of Area 3b and near the posterior border of Area 1. Proceeding toward the 3-1 border in either field, successively defined receptive fields shifted systematically toward the radial mar- gin of the wrist dorsum, which was, again, rep- resented along the 3-1 border. Note that given identical sequences in representation of the dorsal and ventral surfaces of the wrist, much of the representations of the two surfaces are discontinuous. Thus, recording sites 6 in figure 6 and 6 in figure 7 are close to each other on the brain but correspond to distant receptive fields located on the middle of the ventral and dorsal surfaces of the wrist, re- spectively.

The forearm and upper arm were also repre- sented twice within Areas 3b and 1. Receptive fields defined near the anterior border of Area

3b or near the posterior edge of Area 1 were usually found to lie along or near the radial margin of the arm. Proceeding toward the 3-1 border within Area 3b, successively defined receptive fields shifted across the ventral sur- face of the arm toward the ulnar margin, and moved around the arm over the dorsal surface to return to a location near the radial margin, which was again represented along the 3-1 border. Thus, one can imagine the skin of the arm as being slit down the radial margin and laid out on the cortex of Area 3b with the ven- tral surface represented more anteriorly and the dorsal surface toward the 3-1 boundary.

The representation of the arm within Area 1 was difficult to reconstruct in detail. For some anterior-posterior recording sequences, suc- cessively defined receptive fields for Area 1 marched around the arm a second time in the

Page 13: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 53

Fig. 9 Receptive fields defined within a long row of penetrations crossing Areas 3b and 1 in the region representing the chest and anterior back. Note the reversal in representational sequence across the 3b-1 border. Details as in figure 2.

same direction as was seen in Area 3b (as in the experiment illustrated in fig. 8). In other sequences, reversals or discontinuities in rep- resented skin location were recorded within the Area 1 arm representation. Thus, al- though it is clear that there is a single and complete representation of arm surfaces in Area 1, as in the more clearly ordered Area 3b, a simple pattern of representation could not be defined that fit all derived mapping data in the several monkeys in which the arm repre- sentation was relatively completely mapped. These data indicated that, in fact, there must

be significant variation in the way arm sur- faces are represented within Area 1 in dif- ferent individual monkeys.

Representation of the trunk The double representation of the trunk was

difficult to illustrate completely in most mon- keys because we found the representation of the back in Area 1 to be small (as in the case illustrated in fig. 8). Only a small number of receptive fields on the back have been defined in all experiments. On the other hand, the rep- resentation of the skin of the chest and abdo-

Page 14: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

54 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H [. KAAS, M. SUR AND C:S. LIN

3b

1

1 mm

Fig. 10 Reconstruction of a detailed map of the representations of the foot, from an experiment in one of several monkeys in which the foot region was completely mapped. The heel and the interdigital, insular, hypothenar and thenar pads are identified on the diagram at the left. Boundaries between the pads of the sale (dashed lines) are not absolute in Area 1 since receptive fields frequently extended over more than one pad.

men actually appeared to be larger within Area 1 than within Area 3b. However, all map- ping data were consistent with a crude mirror- image representation of the trunk, with the top of the back represented along the anterior margin of Area 3b and near the posterior margin of Area 1, and with the skin along the contralateral side of the midline of the chest and abdomen (the line of reversal) repre- sented along the 3-1 boundary (fig. 9). The shoulder and back of head were located in the lateral aspect of the trunk representation, continuous with the arm, and far removed from the topologically adjacent face. In rows of penetrations passing in the lateral to medi- al direction within the trunk representation of either field, there was a systematic shift of receptive fields down the trunk, from the neck and shoulders and upper chest to the lower back and abdomen.

Representation of the foot in Areas 3 b and 1

Figure 10 is a schematic drawing of the rep- resentation of the contralateral foot within

Areas 3b and 1. The figure was based upon one of several reconstructions of complete maps of the foot derived in individual monkeys. Again there was a double representation of foot sur- faces in the two fields. The representation of the glabrous surfaces of the foot within Area 3b was highly ordered, and was similar in many respects to the Area 3b representation of the hand. The great toe (D1) was in the lat- eral aspect of the representation. The other digits were represented in order, progressing toward the midline, in a lateral-to-medial sequence. The distal phalanges of t'he toes were represented along the anterior border of Area 3b (figs. 11, 12). Proceeding toward the 3-1 border, successively defined receptive fields shifted down the digits onto the pads of the sole toward the base of the foot, which was represented along the 3-1 border. The repre- sentations of the foot pads in Area 3b were not separated from each other as were those of the palmar pads (fig. 10). The one notable excep- tion was that some of the receptive fields on the thenar eminence were recorded in the me- dial aspects of foot representations in Areas

Page 15: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 55

l m m - Fig. 11 Receptive fields defined within a row of penetrations crossing Areas 3b and 1 in the region repre.

aenting the great toe. Details as in figure 2.

3b and 1. Therefore, these recording sites re- lated to the thenar eminence were separated from those related to the representation of the rest of the thenar eminence.

Within Area 1, digits were represented in a largely anterior to posterior sequence, with the great toe near the middle of Area 1 and only the third (variable), fourth and fifth dig- its in contact with the posterior border of Area 1 (figs. 10-12). Thus, the representation of the foot within Area 1 was not a mirror image of that in Area 3b, although they were topo- graphically very similar. The representation of the sole in Area 1 was like that in Area 3b. As in the hand representations, receptive fields for the pads of the sole were much larger in Area 1 than in Area 3b, and commonly ex- tended over entire pads, or over adjacent pads.

The dorsal surfaces of the toes (as with the fingers) were ordered in a very different way within the two representations. In the Area 3b foot, the representation of the dorsal, hairy

surfaces of the toes was split (in a manner similar t o the split of the representation of the hairy fingers in Area 3b), with the dorsum of the great toe represented in a region lateral to that of the glabrous surface of the great toe, and with the hairy surface of digits 5 ,4 ,3 and 2 represented in order in the region medial to the representation of the glabrous surfaces of the fifth digit. As with the hand, the repre- sentational sequence moved around the outer surfaces of the first and fifth digits onto the adjacent hairy skin, and then marched over the hairy surfaces of the toe or toes. The repre- sentation of the foot dorsum was continuous with that of the fifth, fourth, third and second digits on the medial side of the Area 3 foot rep- resentation.

In contrast to the Area 3b representation, the dorsal surfaces of the foot within Area 1 were represented entirely on the lateral side of the foot representation, adjacent to the repre- sentation of the distal phalanges of digits 1, 2

Page 16: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

56 M. M. MERZENICH. J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C.-S. LIN

1 mm - F A C E

Fig. 12 Receptive fields defined within a row of penetrations crossing areas 3b and 1 in the region repre- senting the second pedal digit, second interdigital pad, insular pad and heel. The representational sequence reverses between penetrations 6-7, near the proximal end of the glabrous foot. Details as in figure 2.

and 3 (figs. 10-12). Successively defined recep- tive fields in a lateral-to-medial sequence in this region marched from the proximal foot to the distal foot. Hairy surfaces on the distal phalanges of the toes were represented in the cortex adjacent to the representation of gla- brous phalanges.

Representation of the leg The region representing the leg on the lat-

eral surface of the cortex was explored, but since much or most of the leg representation was on the medial wall in the owl monkey, de- rived representational data were fragmenta- ry. Available data were consistent with a re- representation of leg surfaces in Areas 3b and 1. The preaxial surface of the thigh was mapped in entirety within both representa- tions. A few postaxial fields were also defined in Area 1 and 3b in this region.

As in some other species of primates (DIS- CUSSION), part of the leg was found both medi-

al and lateral to the foot representation in somatosensory cortex of owl monkeys. While most of the leg was represented medial to the foot, significant portions were represented in cortex immediately lateral to the foot in both Areas 3b and 1. Both lateral leg representa- tions were small (table 11, but the leg repre- sentation lateral to the foot was consistently larger in Area 1 than in Area 3b (in some ex- periments, no leg representation could be foundlateral to the foot in Area 3b). The later- al representation of the leg in Area 1 was almost totally restricted t o the preaxial sur- face of the thigh, which appeared to be repre- sented there in its entirety. Receptive fields for sequences of recording sites in this region formed continuous sequences with those on the abdomen and lower back, indicating that the representations of the thigh and adjoining abdomen and back, are continuous. In con- trast, rows of recording sites never revealed continuous sequences of receptive fields from

Page 17: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 57

the thigh to the lower leg. For example, in lat- eromedial rows of recording sites in the lateral leg representation in Area 1, a discontinuity was consistently found such that skin from the knee was adjacent to the cortical repre- sentation of the dorsal foot.

Surprisingly, the complete preaxial thigh in Area 3b was found on the medial side of the foot representation, where it has been mapped in detail. Thus, there are double representa- tions of parts of the preaxial thigh in each of Areas 3b and 1, with the two representations lying on different sides of the representation of the foot.

Representation of the face in Areas 3b and 1

The region of the face representation of the lateral surface of the cortex has been mapped in detail, but the representational area buried within the Sylvian fissure has not been sys- tematically explored. The buried portion in- cludes significant parts of the Area 3b and especially the Area 1 face representations. Nonetheless, the general pattern of repre- sentation of the face was evident. There is clearly a double representation of the face within the two cytoarchitectonic fields. The midline of the lower face (lower lip, chin vibrissae, chin) was represented at or near the anterior border of Area 3b, and at or near the posterior border of Area 1 (fig. 13). The region of the representation of the contralateral upper lip and mandibular vibrissae a t or near the midline was along the 3-1 border. The two representations in Areas 3b and 1 were topo- graphically similar, with areas on the face represented in roughly mirror-image sequence proceeding away from the 3-1 border. An example of this re-representation of the face is illustrated by the receptive fields for a long row of penetrations illustrated in figure 13. Beginning with a receptive field near the mid- line on the chin in the Area 3b representation (penetration 1, fig. 131, successive receptive fields were located first near the midline on the chin and lower lip, then progressing toward the corner of the mouth on the lower lip, and then toward the midline on the upper lip. A reversal in the sequence of the repre- sentation was recorded a t the 3-1 border (be- tween penetrations 8 and 9 in fig. 13). Recep- tive fields for the remaining recording sites crossing Area 1 retraced a sequence similar to that of Area 3b.

It should be noted that the representation of

the chin was immediately adjacent to that of the glabrous skin of the first digit in Area 3b, and to the thenar eminence in Area 1 (figs. 5, 13). The representation of the chin vibrissae was adjacent to that of the glabrous thumb in Area 3b, and of the thenar eminence in Area 1. Finally, the representation of the mandibular vibrissae was adjacent to the representation of the dorsum of the finger in Area 3b, and to the thenar eminence in Area 1. No transition from the radial hand to the face via interven- ing skin could be recorded. This step was dis- continuous, within the finest possible grain of recording. As noted earlier, contiguous inter- connecting skin surfaces (arm, shoulder, side of head) were found to be represented within the cortex in a region medial to the repre- sentation of the hand and wrist, and far from that of the face.

A very large representation of the contra- lateral teeth was mapped within Area 3b. Our maps never extended into the probable loca- tion of the Area l representation of the teeth, or the tongue. The teeth were represented lat- eral and rostra1 to the lower lip in Area 3b. In penetration rows directed rostralward in Area 3b, successive receptive fields shifted from near the midline on the lower lip onto the lower incisors and then t o the more lateral lower teeth. Still further rostralward, recep- tive fields crossed to the lateral upper teeth, and then moved medialward toward the upper incisors.

The proportion areas of cortex devoted to different skin surfaces within

Area 3b and Area 1 It is of interest to note that the proportional

representation of different body surfaces within the Area 3b representation is similar, but definitely not identical to that within the Area 1 representation. These two large body surface representations are not scale versions of each other.

To illustrate how the areas of representa- tion of different body surfaces vary in the two fields, areas from maps have been measured planimetrically. Measurements derived from one extensively studied owl monkey (consist- ent with similar, more fragmentary data de- rived in other monkeys) are recorded in table 1. Excluding the incompletely mapped face, leg, tail and genital representations, the Area 3b representation was about 35-40% larger than that in Area 1. The representation of the hand was about one and a half times larger in

Page 18: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

58 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C:S. LIN

TABLE 1

Area of representation ofdifferent body surfaces within areas 36 and 1 in Owl Monkey 76-9

Represented body surface

Area 3b Area 1 repreaentatlon representation

(mm') (mm') Area 3b/area 1

Hand Glabrous digits Palm Dorsum

Wrist Arm Trunk Leg (lateral to foot) Foot

Glabrous digits Sole Dorsum

Total '

11.5 5.8 4.6 1.1

0.8 1.9 2.7 0.1 4.9

1.9 2.2 0.8

21.8

7.6 2.3 4.2 1.1

1.5 1.8 2.3 0.7 2.7

0.6 1.6 0.5

15.9

1.51 2.52 1.10 1.00 0.53 1.06 1.17 0.14 1.81 3.17 1.38 1.60 1.37

' Excluding leg lateral to foot, and ~ncompletely mapped face, Leg medial to foot, tail and genital representations

Area 3b than in Area 1. The representation of the foot was nearly twice as large. In contrast, the representation of the trunk and arm were nearly equal in the two Areas, and the repre- sentation of the wrist was actually larger in Area 1 than in Area 3b.

It is evident from table 1 that the biggest disparity between Areas 1 and 3b was in the proportional areas of the representations of subdivisions of the hand and foot. The area representing the glabrous surfaces of the hand digits in Area 3b, for example, was over twice as large as that in Area 1; while the areas rep- resenting the palm and the dorsal hand were nearly equal in Area 3b and Area 1. As an even more striking example, the area of representa- tion of the glabrous surfaces of the foot digits in the Area 3b representation was over three times as large as in Area 1. In contrast, the representations of the sole and dorsal foot in Areas 3b and 1 were more nearly equal. Thus, it would appear that much of the difference in the overall sizes of the two representations can be attributed to a proportionately larger representation of the glabrous surfaces of the fingers and toes in Area 3b.

Fields bordering Areas 3 b and 1 Penetrations were introduced into Areas

3a, 4 and 2 in all mapping experiments. The cortical region posterior to Area 1 (including Area 2) was extensively mapped in two ex- periments.

In penetrations anterior to Area 3b, neurons were not driven by light tactile stimulation. Numerous lesions were introduced along this anterior border of the anterior cutaneous

somatosensory representation. Medially, this boundary fell along a distinct Area 3b-3a border; laterally, Area 3a was very narrow or absent, and Area 3b appeared to directly border Area 4. In some penetrations into Area 3a, neurons were driven by deep (never cuta- neous) stimuli, while in other penetrations neurons were driven poorly if a t all even by stimulation of deep body tissues. If neurons in Area 3a were responsive to deep stimuli, the Area 4 border could be detected by a marked reduction in responsiveness.

In some instance, intracortical stimulation was employed to confirm the position of the caudal border of Area 4. Those experiments were conducted in Area 4 adjacent to the Area 3b representation of the face and hand. With- in this region, Area 3a is very narrow or ab- sent. The rostra1 boundary of Area 3b defined through use of light tactile stimulation corre- sponded closely (within a few 100 km) to the caudal border of Area 4 defined by low threshold movements produced by intretcorti- cal "microstimulation."

Two classes of responses were encountered in a broad region caudal to Area 1. In a very

Fig. 13 Some features of representations of the face within Areas 3b and 1. As in other primates, there i s a disproportionately large representation of the anterior face, especially of the lips and the vibrasse of the chin and upper jaw. These skin surfaces (indicated in the d:rawing at the upper right) are represented in Areas 3h and 1 as shown in the drawing at the lower left. Receptive! fields defined within a long row of penetrations crossiing the Area 3h and Area 1 face representations are drawn at the lower right (Area 1 fields, right Area 3b fields, left), Pene- trations in this irregular row were chosen to show approx- imately corresponding lines of receptive fields for both cy- toarchitectonic areas.

Page 19: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE "SI" 59

m 3

Page 20: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

60 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C . 3 . LIN

. \3b\ 1 I ! \ \

FACE/ 5 mm

Owl Monkey 76-14 Fig. 14 The basic pattern of the representation of deep body structures within the cortical region caudal

to Area 1. This schematic drawing summarized results obtained in an extensive mapping experiment con- ducted in Areas 3b and 1 and the cortex caudal to Area 1. In the caudal representation (including and poesi- bly coextensive with Area 21, neurons responded specifically to stimulation of joint or other “deep” receptors in the indicated body regions (see text). Circles mark penetrations within which neurons were not obviously driven by tactile or deep somatic stimulation.

narrow strip immediately adjacent to Area 1, neurons were commonly driven by light tac- tile (cutaneous) stimulation over very large receptive fields. These receptive fields com- monly extended over the entire body surface region represented in adjacent Area 1. Thus, adjacent to the representation of the abdomen in Area 1, for example, neurons in this narrow zone might have receptive fields that ex- tended over part of the chest, the entire abdo- men, and the medial surface of the leg. In a few exceptional cases, such receptive fields appeared to extend across the midline. Input to these neurons appeared to be cutaneous, and not from the Pacinian receptors. These re- sponses were never recorded more than a few 100 pm from the border of Area 1, and fre- quently neurons in penetrations just posterior to Area 1 could not be driven by cutaneous stimulation. Nonetheless, these data suggest that there might be a very narrow, third cuta- neous or mixed modality representation of the body surface in “SI” of the owl monkey.

In a much broader and clearly delimited field behind Area 1 (probably coextensive with Area 21, neurons could be driven by manipula- tion of joints or other “deep” stimulation, and were never excited by cutaneous stimulation.

Rows of recording sites revealed that there is an orderly representation of deep body struc- tures within this region, with the sequence of representation paralleling that of skin sur- faces represented in the nearby cutaneous sensory strip (fig. 14). Input a t many loci within this field appeared to be derived from joint receptors. However, i t was not possible to unequivocally define the receptor source of this “deep” input (especially when the effec- tive stimulation required movement of the shoulder girdle, neck, back or hips).

The relation of electrophysiologtcal results to cortical architecture

The cytoarchitecture of the somatosensory and motor areas of the cerebral cortex olf New World monkeys had been described for mar- mosets by Brodmann (’09) and Peden arid von Bonin (’471, for squirrel monkeys by Rlosabal (’67), Sanides (’67) and Jones (’751, and for cebus monkeys by von Bonin (’38). Since char- acteristics of the somatic fields appear to be very similar in these monkeys and in owl mon- keys, the major architectonic features are only briefly reviewed below. These features are apparent in figure 15, which shows photo- micrographs of Areas 4, 3a, 3b, 1 and 2 in owl

Page 21: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 61

monkeys. Since they have general acceptance, Brodmann’s (’09) numbers are used, although Sanides (‘681, Peden and von Bonin (’47) and von Bonin (’38) used other nomenclature. The two architectonic fields of most interest for the present study are Areas 3b and 1.

The major features of Area 3b are easy to appreciate in the owl monkey because this subdivision is located in relatively flat cortex (see, for example, fig. 15A). Unlike most pri- mates there is no central fissure to distort the architectonic features. However, owl monkeys generally do have a short, shallow dimple in the expected location of the middle part of the central fissure, and this dimple is shown in figures 15B and C. The dimple is rarely deeper than that shown in figure 15, and i t does not significantly alter the laminar pattern of the underlying cortex. When the dimple occurs, i t is over Area 3b with usually more of 3b ros- tral than caudal to the dimple. Therefore the dimple is a good landmark in recording experiments.

Area 3b is a koniocortical or hypergranular field identified by the densely packed gran- ular cells in the outer cell layers. The cell packing in layer IV is most dense, but cells are also tightly packed in layers I1 and 111 so that layers 11-IV sometimes appear to fuse (fig. 15A). Layer VI is slightly more dense than in adjoining cortex so that the relatively light layer V stands out in contrast to the layers above and below. Area 1 is marked by a less dense but obvious inner granular cell layer, and a less dense layer V. The transition from Area 1 to more caudal parietal cortex, Area 2, is denoted by an increase in layer I11 and layer V pyramidal cells and more densely packed cells in layer IV. Area 3a has both the larger pyramidal cells in layer V characteristic of motor cortex and a clear inner granular layer characteristic of sensory fields, and therefore has been called a transitional or intermediate field. However, Area 3a has distinct bound- aries with Areas 4 and 3b, and does not appear as a gradual transition from motor to sensory fields. Rather, we agree with Sanides (’68) that Area 3a is a separate field with a “char- acter” that does not “allow one to consider it a subdivision of Area 3.” The motor field or Area 4 is rostral to the central dimple, and is char- acterized by large pyramidal cells in layer V, overall low density of neurons, and the lack of a clear granular cell layer. Area 4 is several millimeters wide, and gradually merges later- ally with cortex having smaller pyramidal

cells. Stimulation experiments suggest that this cortex is also part of the motor repre- sentation.

In alternate brain sections stained for my- elinated fibers, Area 3b is more densely myel- inated than is Area 1, and we found this feature useful in distinguishing between the two fields. The myeloarchitecture of postcen- tral cortex has been described in detail in the squirrel monkey by Sanides (’68) and Jones (’75) and the overall features are simi- lar in owl monkeys.

In all recording experiments, electrolytic lesions were made along the boundaries of the two body surface representations in order to relate the recording results to cortical archi- tecture. Two examples of brain sections with such marking lesions are shown in figures 15B and C. Cytoarchitectonic field boundaries were completely reconstructed from this his- tological material. The field boundaries drawn in all illustrations represent such objectively defined lines, drawn solely from histological reconstructions. Coincidence of the distinct cytoarchitectonic and functional boundaries of these fields is remarkable. Zn all instances: (1) Lesions placed at the electrophysiological- ly defined border between the rostral and the caudal cutaneous representation of body sur- faces were later found to be along the border between cytoarchitectonically defined Areas 3b and 1 (fig. 15B); (2) lesions placed along the caudal border of the caudal cutaneous field were between Areas 1 and 2 (fig. 15B); (3) lesions located along the rostral border of the rostral cutaneous field were between Areas 3a and 3b (fig. 15C).

Zndividual variations in the organization ofAreas 36 and 1

As has been noted earlier, these mapping studies have revealed that there is unequivo- cal variation in the detailed pattern of repre- sentation in different individual monkeys. Examples include: (1) The arm representation in Area 1 was different in detail in every mapped monkey. In Area 3b, the region of the rostral margin of the arm representation was different in individual owl monkeys. (2) In most owl monkeys, the hairy skin of the dorsal fingers in Area 1 was represented continuous- ly between the representation of the glabrous fingertips and that of the middle and proximal phalanges. In one extensively mapped excep- tional case, they were represented in a region between a representation of the entire gla-

Page 22: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

62 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C.-S. LIN

brous surface of the digits and the distal palm. (3) In one monkey, the representation of the back in Area 1 was clearly defined throughout the mediolateral extent of the representation, and the back representation occupied a rela- tively large proportion of the total Area 1 trunk representation. In all other monkeys, the Area 1 back representation was very small, and receptive fields on the back were defined only a few penetrations in the most posterior aspect of the field. The pattern of representation appeared to differ in fine detail in each owl monkey. Certainly, individual var- iation in the organization of sensory areas is much greater than has been generally ap- preciated.

DISCUSSION

The microelectrode mapping results of the present study lead to the conclusions that each of two separate architectonic fields in owl monkeys is occupied by a map of the skin surface, that the two maps differ in organiza- tion, and that a third architectonic zone is devoted to non-cutaneous "deep" receptors. The results obviously require modifications and revisions of some of the current concepts of "SI" organization in primates, and new terminology will be needed to reflect these changed concepts. In addition, the results and conclusions raise the important questions: (1) What are the homologues of these separate representations in other mammals, and (2) how did these multiple representations evolve? These issues are considered further below.

1. Current tenets ofsomatosensory organization

The parietal somatosensory region has been mapped in a number of primate species, in- cluding the slow loris, marmoset, squirrel monkey, cebus monkey, spider monkey, ma- caque monkey, gibbon, chimpanzee, and man. From these studies, a concept of the organiza- tion of the primate parietal somatosensory cortex has arisen that includes a t least six principal tenets. First, there is a single repre- sentation of the contralateral body surface V'SI") within the parietal sensory strip. Sec- ond, this representation overlies more than one clearly delimitable cytoarchitectonic field. Third, the two-dimensional organization of "SI" is (a) a somatotopic homunculus; (b) a sequence of rostrocaudal bands each devoted to a single body part; or (c) some combination

of these two views. Fourth, the representation of the body surface follows a dermatomal sequence. Fifth, the proportional representa- tion of different body surface regions is a re- flection of peripheral innervation density. Sixth, "SI" is the primary functional division of somatosensory cortex. While investigators have derived evidence contrary to almost all of these commonly held tenets, these concepts of somatosensory cortex still dominate. There- fore, it is useful to start by reviewing evidence for and against each of these ideas, and to sug- gest modifications and revisions that are con- sistent with present and previous results.

(a) There is a single representation of the body surface in the parietal somato- sensory strip in primates

Investigators who have studied the repre- sentation of the body surface within the parietal somatosensory strip using microelec- trode and surface evoked potential mapping techniques have described a single representa- tion of the body occupying the "SI" region in the New World marmoset (Woolsey, '52, '54), squirrel monkey (Benjamin and Welker, '57; Werner and Whitsel, '681, cebus monkey (Hirsh and Coxe, '58) and spider monkey (Chang et al., '47; Pubols and Pubols, '71, '72) ; in the Old World rhesus monkey (Marshall e t al., '37; Woolsey et al., '42; Mountcastle and Powell, '59; Werner and Whitsel, '68; Whitsel et al., '71; Dreyer e t al., '74, '75); in the gibbon (Woolsey et al., '60; Welt, '63); the chim- panzee (Woolsey et al., '43; Woolsey et al., '60; Welt, '63); and in humans (Penfield and Boldrey, '37; Penfield and Rasmussen, '50). However, there have been notable exceptions to the idea of a single "SI" representation. Most importantly, Paul et al. ('72a) presented evidence for two complete and separate cuta-

I

Fig. 15 Some features of the cytoarchitecture of the parietal somatosensory strip illustrated in Nissl-stained parasagittal sections. Boundaries of cytoarchitectonic Areas 4,3a, 3b, 1 and 2 are indicated by arrows. Lesions in B were introduced at locations determined physiologically to mark the approximate boundaries of the posterior body surface representation. In all such instances, these marks fell at or very near the Area 1 boundaries. The lesion in C was one of the many introduced at the site of the first penetration rostra1 to the anterior body surface repre- sentation. All such lesions fell along the 3b-3a (or far lat- erally, along the 3b-4) boundary. Boundaries of Areas 3b and 1 were defined solely on cytoarchitectonic bases from material like that shown in A X in all illustrated cases. Note the very shallow, incomplete (absent at the level shown in A) central sulcus. It Is rarely deeper than as seen in B and C. Marker bar, 1 mm.

+

Page 23: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

Figure 15

Page 24: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

64 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS. M. SUR AND C.-S. LIN

neous representations of the glabrous hand surface in “SI” of macaque monkeys (other parts of “SI” were not explored). A second ex- ception is the recent report of a cutaneous body representation completely within only one of the architectonic fields of the “SI” re- gion of the prosimian, slow loris (Krishnamur- t i et al., ’76). While further representations were not identified in these experiments, the finding of a complete representation re- stricted to only part of “SI” clearly challenges the concept of “SI” as a single representation. Furthermore, cortex adjoining the cutaneous representation was responsive to somatosen- sory stimulation. A third report which was strongly suggestive of multiple representa- tions was that of Zimmerman (’68). This in- vestigator noted separate rostral and caudal foci of evoked responses to light tactile stimu- lation of the digit tips in the “SI” region of somatosensory cortex of the squirrel monkey. Zimmerman argued that these foci indicated two “entirely separate and differently orga- nized sensory areas” for three reasons. First, the rostral and caudal foci were discon- tinuous; second, the two zones differed in re- sponse characteristics such as the latency, fol- lowing frequency and threshold; third, experi- ments involving spreading depression showed that responses in the two foci were indepen- dent. Unfortunately, the experiments of Zim- merman had limited impact, perhaps because he regarded the rostral somatosensory zone as within “somatomotor” cortex rather than as a functional division of “SI” cortex. To us, it ap- pears that both the rostral and caudal foci were within the region commonly considered the “SI” field in squirrel monkeys.

While there are few examples of direct challenges to the concept of “SI” as a single representation, there are many examples of data that are inconsistent with that concept. Starting with the initial pioneering studies on macaque monkeys, Woolsey e t al. (’42) de- scribed a second representation of the thumb on the caudal bank of the central fissure that was “at some distance” from the representa- tion of the thumb on the dorsal surface of post- central parietal cortex. Likewise, microelec- trode single unit and mapping studies (Powell and Mountcastle, ’59b; Whitsel e t al., ’71; Dreyer et al., ’75) have consistently noted the activation of nonadjacent cortical locations within “SI” from the stimulation of a single body location. Dreyer e t al. got so far as to con- clude that, for each body part of “SI,” “a single

region in the periphery is represented several times in widely separate locations.” This view- point is not far from a formal rejection of the single “ST” concept.

While there have been a few conclusions and many observations in conflict with the concept of “SI” as a single representation in primates, this concept has persisted. We believe that the present extensive mapping of the “SI” region of the owl monkey presents unequivocal evidence that there are two cuta- neous representations in owl monkeys. Fur- thermore, more limited mapping results indi- cate a third representation that is devoted to non-cutaneous receptors. Thus, we feel tha t the concept of a single representation in “SI” must be abandoned for owl monkeys, and in view of these and previous results, must be placed in serious doubt or abandoned for other primates.

(b) The body representation in the parietal somatosensory strip occupies more than one architectonic field

It has been recognized from the first surface evoked potential somatosensory mapping studies (Marshall e t al., ‘37; Woolsey et al., ’42) that “SI,” as described in macaque mon- keys, would include more than one cytoarchi- tectonic field. These authors concluded that the “SI” representation occupied Brodmann’s Areas 3 , l and 2, but not the adjoining Areas 4 and 5. The viewpoint that “SI” includes Areas 3, 1 and 2 has prevailed for macaques and other primates (Whitsel et al., ’71; Whitsel et al., ’72; Dreyer et al., ’74, ’75).

However, i t has been puzzling why a single representation should have distinct architec- tonic subdivisions. Such distinct and different architectonic “subunits” are not found within other cortical sensory representations (al- though subregions of some structural special- ization occur; the monocular and binocular portion of striate cortex can be distinguished, for example). The usual way to account for the architectonic differences within “SI” has been to relate them to varying extents of sub- modality segregation within the field (Powell and Mountcastle, ’59b; Werner and Whitsel, ’68; Whitsel et al., ’71; Dreyer et al., ’75). Such segregation, of course, implies either separate maps or rostrocaudal bands of “isorepresenta- tion” (see below).

Not all investigators have included more than one architectonic field within “SI.” In macaque monkeys Paul e t al. (’72a,b) argued

*

Page 25: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 65

for a representation of the hand within Area 3b, a second representation of the hand within Area 1, a possible third “deep” representation in Area 2, and other non-cutaneous activation within Area 3a. Krishnamurti et al. 1‘76) re- ported a complete body surface representation that was coextensive with somatic koniocor- tex or Area 3b of the slow loris (Sanides and Krishnamurti, ’67). These conclusions agree with the present results, which indicate that there is a large body surface representation that is coextensive with Area 3b and a second somewhat smaller body surface representa- tion that is congruent with Area 1. Area 3a is outside this cutaneous sensory strip. And there is an orderly representation of deep body structures (probably coextensive with Area 2) within the cortical region caudal to Area 1.

(c) The representation of the body surface is a homunculus, or forms rostrocaudal bands of isorepresentation

There have been two different basic view- points as to how a single body representation, “SI,” lies across the several architectonic fields in primates. Many investigators have summarized the pattern of the representation of the body surface on the parietal cortex by describing or drawing a “homunculus” (or “simiunculus”) , a body figure distorted to il- lustrate the proportional representation of different skin surfaces. The homunculus has been shown or described as having the same overall orientation on the brain of prosimians (Krishnamurti et al., ’76, where it was con- fined to Area 3b), marmosets (Woolsey, ’521, squirrel monkeys (Benjamin and Welker, ’57), macaques (Woolsey, ’58, ’641, chimpanzees (Woolsey, ’641, and humans (Penfield and Boldrey, ’37; Penfield and Rasmussen, ’50). The distal limbs and digits are rostral, and back and dorsal midline are caudal, the head is lateral, and the tail medial in these figurative illustrations of representational patterns.

Other investigators have concluded that each different body region is represented (at least for the most part) across “SI” within a rostrocaudal cortical strip extending from margin to margin. The concept of rostrocaudal bands was first introduced in the landmark studies of Powell and Mountcastle (‘59b) in describing the functional organization of “SI” in macaque monkeys. The concept has also been advanced in a study of spider monkey somatosensory cortex by Pubols and Pubols (‘71, ’72), and to some extent for squirrel and

macaque monkeys by Werner and Whitsel (’68, ’71) and macaque monkeys by Whitsel et al. (’71) and Dreyer et al. (’74, ’75).

Both points of view reasonably account for certain sets of observations. For example, pro- gressions of recording sites in “SI” commonly result in simple somatotopic sequences of re- ceptive fields. And recording sites a t different positions a t the same rostrocaudal level are often activated by the same body part. But most investigators recognized that a strict adherence to either point of view would not account for all the observations. In particular, it has been necessary to admit to some degree of somatotopic organization within “rostro- caudal bands,” and major discontinuities and re-representations in “the single somatotopic map” have been noted. We have presented evi- dence for a third point of view (also see Paul et al., ’72a). There are clearly not rostrocau- dal strips across the architectonic fields in owl monkeys. Rather, separate architectonic fields have separate representations. Further- more, the concept of a homunculus does not accurately reflect the organization of either of the two cutaneous representations that we have demonstrated. Rather, the representa- tions are better described as composites of subunits or sectors, each of which is internally somatotopic. Within each sector, progressions of recording sites produce simple progressions of receptive fields. However, adjoining neu- rons on two sides of a “sector” border might re- late to quite distant or discontinuous body parts. Thus, the representations are not sim- ple or topological transformations of the com- plete contralateral body surface, and there- fore are not homunculi. We prefer to consider the representations as composites of somato- topic regions.

(d) The representation follows a dermatomal sequence

All three concepts of somatosensory cortex organization could, in principle, be reasonably (but not completely) consistent with a further postulate or organization: that the medial-to- lateral sequence of representation follows the dermatomal order. There has been some dis- agreement as to the actual order of the medi- al-to-lateral sequence of body surface repre- sentation in primates, but a number of inves- tigators suggested (as did earlier neurologists such as Head, ’20, and Benisty, ’28) that this sequence follows the dermatomal order (Bard, ’38; Woolsey et al., ‘42; Woolsey et al., ’43;

Page 26: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

66 M. M. MERZENICH. J. H. KAAS, M. SUR A N D C.-S. LIN

Powell and Mountcastle, ’59b; Werner and Whitsel, ’68, ’71). Holding a more extreme form of the concept of dermatomal organiza- tion of “SI,” Powell and Mountcastle (’59b) and Werner and Whitsel (’68, ’71) argued that the representation of the body in “SI” is actually a simple representation of the der- matomes, which are laid out over the rostro- caudal dimension of “SI,” and represented in order in its lateral-to-medial dimension.

On the other hand, many investigators have described exceptions to the dermatomal sequence of representation. Woolsey and col- leagues (’42) first noted the discontinuity be- tween the representation of the hand and two parts of the head, i.e., the dorsal head is repre- sented in the cortical region medial to the hand, while adjoining head surfaces are repre- sented lateral to the hand. This “splitting of the occiput” has been recorded in nearly all the primate species that have been studied. Other exceptions to dermatomal sequences were reported later in the chimpanzee and gib- bon (genitalia adjacent to trunk representa- tion, foot lateral to a complete leg representa- tion) (Woolsey et al., ’60); in humans (entire leg lateral to foot) (Penfield and Rasmussen, ’50) ; in cebus monkeys (genitalia represented on both sides of the foot) (Hirsch and Coxe, ’58); and in spider monkeys (head lateral to hand) (Pubols and Pubols, ’71, ’72).

The detailed maps of the two cutaneous fields in the owl monkeys allow us to evaluate the usefulness of considering these repre- sentations as “metameric maps”, i.e., maps of the dorsal root cutaneous fields. For both rep- resentations there are numerous examples, both of regions where the dermatomal se- quence is not followed, and where discontinui- ties between the representations of adjoining skin surfaces are not along “dermatomal boundaries.” Among obvious examples are the following: (1) As many others have noted, the representation of the face is immediately adjacent to that of the thumb and thenar emi- nence in both fields. In the dermatomal sequence, of course, fields on the arm, shoulder and head dorsum should be repre- sented between the hand and face. The skin field represented in the dermatomes innervat- ing the arm and shoulder are actually repre- sented in both cortical fields 3b and 1 in cortex distant from the representation of the hand. This separation of the hand from adjoining skin surfaces has also been described in other species (Woolsey et al., ’42; Benjamin and

Welker, ’57; Woolsey et al., ’60; Penfield and Boldrey, ’37; Paul et al., ’72a; Pubols and Pubols, ’72). (2) The representation of the dor- sal surfaces of the fingers is separated from that of the rest of their dermatomal fields on the hand and arm. Moreover, the dorsal fingers are represented differently in Areas 3b and 1. Especially in Area 1, lines of discon- tinuity in the representations of fingers fail t o follow even a short part of any dermatomal boundary. A similar segregation of the Area 3b representation of the volar and dorsal hand was also recorded in the slow loris, by Krish- namurti et al. (’76). (3) The representation of the hand and volar wrist is discontinuous with that of the rest of the arm. Given this discon- tinuity, the dermatomes would be cut off a t the wrist, and in the hand-wrist sector there would be fragments of several dermatomes. (4) If dermatomal fields occur in the region of representations of the arm, they are aligned successively in the mediolateral and not the rostrocaudal cortical dimension in both fields. (5) The representation of part of the preaxial leg is found lateral to the foot within Area 1 (the lateral representation in Area 3b is very small or absent). The split of the preaxial and postaxial leg is along dermatomal lines, and this finding in other primates has been one of the major arguments for a systematic repre- sentation of dermatomes in cortex (Woolsey et al., ’421, i.e., according to the dermatomal sequence, and the preaxial leg should be repre- sented lateral to the foot? However, a t least most of the preaxial leg in Area 3 is clearly represented in the cortex medial to the foot representation, and out of the dermatomal sequence. (6) The hairy skin of the toes is not represented with the remainder of the der- matomes covering these surfaces, in either Areas 3b or 1. Again, the pattern of repre- sentation of the dorsal digits is different for the two fields, and again, in both instances, lines of discontinuity that cut across these skin fields cut through dermatomes. (7) In somewhat different ways in Areas 1 and 3b the representation of the foot is largely dis- continuous with that of the leg. However, der- matomes in this region cross the dorsal foot to extend up onto the leg.

’+

.~

~

~

The preaxial leg has been described as being represented on the lateral side of the foot representation in squirrel monkeys (Ben- jamin and Welker, ’57; Werner and Whitsel. ’68), spider monkeys (Pubols and Pubols, ’72) and rhesua monkeys (Woolsey et al., ‘42; Werner and Whitgel, ’68). Investigations of somatosensory cortex or- ganization in apes and man (Penfield and Boldrey. ’37; Penfield and Rasmussen, ’50; Woolsey et al.. ’60) report that the entlre leg is rep resented lateral to the foot.

Page 27: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 67

Can any discontinuities really be described as falling along dermatomal boundaries? It is possible that the lines of discontinuity along the arm and leg representations follow der- matomal boundaries. Our mapping data is not precise enough to define these lines with suffi- cient accuracy to resolve this question. The lines splitting the hairy skin of the toes (and possibly the fingers) in Area 3b (but not Area 11, and lines of discontinuity which split the palm and which separate off a sector of the thenar eminence could conceivably follow a dermatomal boundary over a short distance. The dermatomal boundaries defining the pre- axial thigh may be followed over a part of their length in the Area 1 representation of the thigh. In all of these instances, on the other hand, the sector of the skin field repre- sented discontinuously would probably never include all of any one dermatome. We con- clude that while both the dermatomal se- quence and the body maps reflect in a general way the spatial outlay of the skin surface, the organizations of the body maps are not a con- sequence of the dermatomal sequence.

(e) The proportion of representation of different body surface regions is a simple reflection of cutaneous peripheral innervation density

As the observation that the parts of body surfaces with the greatest peripheral innerva- tion density occupy proportionately more of “SI” became appreciated, the idea has arisen that there might be a simple relation between numbers of peripheral receptors per unit area and the cortical area representing the periph- eral unit. Beginning with the insightful dis- cussion of the somatosensory cortex in the re- port of Woolsey et al. (’42), most mapping studies have alluded to this basic concept. However, quantitive evidence for the hypothe- sis that the somatosensory cortical repre- sentations are simply a reflection of periph- eral innervation density is limited to that presented in the report of Lee and Woolsey (’75). These investigators found a proportional relationship between the numbers of fibers in- nervating individual vibrissae on the face and the number of neurons in distinct groupings (termed barrels) of cells in layer IV of SI cor- tex devoted to individual vibrissae. Although the numbers of peripheral fibers and the num- bers of layer IV neurons for each whisker varied, there was a constant proportion of 17

central neurons for each peripheral fiber. From these findings, Lee and Woolsey (’75) proposed that the distortion of the body repre- sentation is directly related to the quantity of peripheral innervation and is the result of a “peripheral scaling factor.”

Attractive as this concept is, it cannot be applied without modification to the parietal somatosensory strip. Since the proportional representations of different skin areas are dif- ferent in these two fields, the same peripheral scale factor obviously cannot be applied to both. It is also apparent that several features of a t least the Area 1 somatosensory repre- sentation do not follow overall peripheral in- nervation patterns. The relatively small rep- resentations of the glabrous surfaces of the fingers and toes as compared with the rela- tively large representations of the palm and sole in Area 1 are examples (table 1). Thus, there is not a simple relationship between pe- ripheral innervation density and cortical ex- pansion. An alternative view is that func- tional specialization within cortex is a factor in the distortion of the representations, and consequently, the number of cortical neurons related to the peripheral input varies within representations, and from representation to representation. It is also possible that a modified form of the peripheral-central rela- tionship exists, but that it applies for distribu- tions of different specific afferents or sub- modalities for each cortical representation. Thus, each representation would reflect the differential distribution of a separate set of peripheral receptor or primary fiber types. Nonetheless, any application of the “periph- eral scaling” principal to parietal somatosen- sory cortical fields must await further direct evidence of the kind derived by Lee and Woolsey (’75).

(f) “SI” is the basic functional subdivision of somatosensory cortex

Most anatomical, ablation-behavioral, and electrophysiological studies of “SI” have been predicated on the concept that “SI” is a single large functional unit. As a result, and despite the clear architectonic subdivisions, limited attention has been paid to where recording sites, lesions, or injections have been located within this strip. Perhaps the most compelling argument against considering “SI” of pri- mates as a single functional unit is the evi- dence that this region of cortex contains sepa- rate, complete and detailed representations of

Page 28: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

68 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND (2.3. LIN

receptor surfaces that are coextensive with the separate and distinct architectonic fields. However, three other types of evidence sup- port the hypothesis of functionally separate areas within “SI.”

The first type of additional evidence is that the overall response characteristics of neu- rons differ in each of the “SI” architectonic subdivisions. This difference was noted in the early study of Powell and Mountcastle (’59b), although they regarded “SI” as a functional unit and argued that there was a graded dis- tribution of neurons with different response characteristics across the architectonic fields. Subsequently, a number of investigators have presented evidence that different receptor populations ultimately project to Areas 3a, 3b and 2 (Paul et al., ’72a,b; Krishnamurti et al., ’76; Tanji, ’75; Heath et al., ’76). From their studies, it is reasonable to conclude that mus- cle afferents feed into Area 3a, input from per- haps different types of cutaneous receptors into Areas 3b and 1, and joint and other “deep” receptors into Area 2.

There may be some differences in the over- all neuronal response properties of the ar- chitectonic fields in different primates. While neurons in Areas 3b and 1 of macaque mon- keys apparently differ in adaptation rate to a steady cutaneous stimuli (Paul et al., ’72a) we have been unable t o demonstrate this segrega- tion of slowly and quickly adapting inputs in the owl monkey. While neuronal responses were not studied in detail for adaptation prop- erties, both fields appeared to have neurons that were basically quickly adapting. (This does not mean, of course, that differences in adaptation rate do not exist or that both popu- lations derive their input solely from quickly adapting receptor classes.) On the other hand, the response properties of neurons in Areas 3b and 1 in owl monkeys were different in recep- tive field size and numbers of discharges in re- sponse to transient stimuli. Furthermore, as in macaque monkeys (Paul et al., ’72a), Pa- cinian-like responses (Mountcastle et al., ’69) were limited to Area 1 recording sites and were not found in Area 3b. Also in concurrence with the observations of Paul et al. (‘72a), no joint receptor input was recorded in Areas 3b and 1 in anesthetized owl monkeys; joint receptor input was found in Area 2.

A second additional reason for considering “SI” of primates as several functionally dis- tinct fields is that each of the separate repre- sentations has a different pattern of anatomi-

cal connections. Most significantly, Vogt (’76) found that Areas 3b and 1 are reciprocally in- terconnected, a finding that might be ex- pected between separate cutaneous repre- sentations but would be surprising between parts of a single representation. Similarly, electrophysiological evidence indicates that Area 3a is topographically interconnected with motor cortex (Zarzechi et al., ’76). It is also of interest that the commissural connec- tions revealed by lesions restricted to Areas 1 and 2 result in three contralateral bands of de- generation corresponding to Area 3a, the boundary of Areas 3b and 1, and the boundary of Areas 2 and 5 (Shanks et al., ’75). Such multiple connections between somatosensory cortex of one hemisphere and somatosensory cortex of the other hemisphere would be puz- zling if the region contains only a single repre- sentation. The idea of a single representation across the somatosensory architectonic fields also is difficult to relate to other anatomical observations. Thus, the different “SI” archi- tectonic fields project t o different loci in the dorsal column nuclei (Weisberg and Rustioni, ’76) and spinal cord (Jones, ’76; Coulter and Jones, ’76). In addition, the results of several types of experiments (Clark and Powell, ’53; Jones and Powell, ’70; Jones, ‘75; Lin et al., ‘78) have suggested that the ventroposterior nucleus projects heavily to Area 3b, and less heavily to Areas 1 and 2. All of these patterns of connections are more readily explained by the present concept of separate representa- tions occupying separate architectonic fields than by the traditional concept of a single body representation and functional field.

A third type of evidence suggesting the sub- division of “SI” of primates into more than one unit of functional significance comes from a recent report of Randolph and Semmes (’74) on the behavioral consequences of ablating parts of Areas 3b, 1, or 2 in macaque monkeys. These investigators trained monkeys on tac- tile discrimination tasks and found that lesions of the hand region of Area 2 were fol- lowed by impairments on tasks involving the discrimination of angles; similar lesions of Area 1 affected discriminations of texture; while lesions of Area 3b led to severe impair- ments on both types of tasks.

In summary, the electrophysiological map- ping data of the present report, studies of the distributions of neurons with different re- sponse characteristics, the cortical architec- ture, the connections of the parietal lobe, and

Page 29: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 69

Somatosensory Strip

Cutaneous Field

Fig. 16 Visual, auditory and somatosensory areas in the owl monkey. The schematic maps of the auditory representations are redrawn from the studies of Imig et al. (‘77); the summary of visual mapping studies is redrawn from Allman and Kaas (‘76). The posterior broken line marks the approximate boundaries of visually responsive cortex. The anterior broken line marks the posterior border of cortex activated by somatic stimuli in the present experiments. Refer to figure 1 for a detailed reconstruction of Area 3b and Area 1 body surface representations.

the behavioral effects of ablations of architec- tonic subdivisions of postcentral cortex all argue against the concept of “SI” as the ba- sic functional subdivision of somatosensory cortex.

2. A redefinition of theprimate “SI”and suggested terminology

Our conclusion that there are two separate representations of the body surface within Areas 3b and 1 of the classically defined “pri- mary somatosensory cortex” or “SI” intro- duces the problem of redefining the cortical subdivisions of somatosensory cortex in pri- mates. We believe that there are enough simi- larities between the representation in Area 3b of monkeys and the “SI” representation of most other mammals to argue that Area 3b is the homologue of the non-primate “SI.” There- fore, we suggest the term “SI proper” for the 3b subdivision of primate somatosensory cor- tex, to distinguish it from the traditional pri- mate “SI,” which these studies suggest is com-

prised of as many as four body representations in monkeys, apes and man.

Three types of evidence suggest the homolo- gy of “SI proper” of primates with “SI” of other mammals. First, the overall organiza- tion of “SI,” as generally described for non-pri- mates (see Sur et al., ’78, for review), is basically similar to that of “SI proper.” For example the representations of the digits of the forepaw and foot are oriented rostrally in both “SI” and “SI proper.” The most signifi- cant reported difference is that the dorsal trunk is usually portrayed as caudal to the ab- domen in “SI” (e.g., see Woolsey, ’64), while the reverse order occurs in “SI proper.” How- ever, the proportionately small representation of the trunk has been poorly documented in most species, and in “SI” of a t least one mam- mal, the grey squirrel (Nelson and Sur, ’77; Sur et al., ‘78), the representation of the trunk is like that of the owl monkey, with the dor- sum rostra1 and the abdomen caudal in the field.

Page 30: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

70 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C:S. LIN

The cytoarchitecture of “SI proper” and non-primate “SI” is also very similar. Both fields are characterized by a well-developed layer IV with small, tightly packed granule cells. Finally, both “SI” of non-primates (Jones and Powell, ’69; Hand and Morrison, ’72) and “SI proper” (Jones, ’75; Jones and Powell, ’70; Clark and Powell, ’53) receive a major input from the ventroposterior nucleus, and this input terminates largely in layer IV. For these reasons, we feel that it is probable that Area 3b of primates is homologous with “SI” as described for most non-primates, and that the term “SI proper” both suggests the likely homology, and distinguishes the Area 3b representation from the rest of the post- central somatosensory strip of monkeys.

With the exception of “SIII” in the cat (Darian-Smith et al., ’661, a systematic somat- ic representation has not been described for cortex caudal to “SI” in nonprimates. Thus, there is little data to suggest that the repre- sentation in Area 1 of monkeys is or is not ho- mologous with any field in other mammals. For the present, we prefer the term, “posterior cutaneous field” (PCF) for the Area 1 repre- sentation, since it is both non-committal in regard to homologies, and descriptive with regard to location and function.

Finally, we suggest that the “deep” and cu- taneous somatosensory fields, 3a, 3b, 1, and 2, of parietal cortex which currently are collec- tively termed “SI,” be renamed the “parietal somatosensory strip” (PSS).

3. Evolution of the parietal somatosensory strip in primates

The generality of finding a body representa- tion with the same relative location, the same overall orientation and arrangement of the body parts, a characteristic cytoarchitecture, and similar connections from the ven- troposterior nucleus in “SI” of a wide range of mammalian species? and in “SI proper” of monkeys indicates that this subdivision of neocortex must have arisen early in mamma- lian or premammalian evolution, and is a basic feature of mammalian neocortex proba- bly common to all mammals. On the other hand, the same argument cannot be made for the posterior cutaneous field. There is a gen- eral lack of evidence for a second cutaneous representation (except, perhaps, for cats; see above) that is posterior to “SI” in non-pri- mates. Furthermore, the posterior cutaneous

field appears to vary significantly in response characteristics and in organization within the primate order. In the prosimian, slow loris, the somatosensory field on the caudal border of “SI proper” requires rather strong stimuli for activation (Krishnamurti et al., ’761, and probably for this reason the organization of the representation remains uncertain. In both New World monkeys (present study) and Old World monkeys (Paul et al., ’72a), the PCF re- sponds to low threshold cutaneous stimuli, but the neural responses appear to be more rapidly adapting in Old World monkeys. Finally, the organization of a t least part of the PCF ap- pears to be quite different in Old and New World monkeys. The evidence indicates that the hand representation in PCF is oriented with the fingertips caudally in New World monkeys (present study) and rostrally in Old World monkeys (Paul et al., ’72a). These vari- ations in PCF characteristics suggest that the field was not well developed in early primates, and perhaps existed as a high-threshold, crudely organized fringe such as it appears to be in present day prosimians.

We know too little about the interspecies comparative features of the Area 3a and Area 2 fields to make reasonable suggestions about their evolution. There is evidence for muscle afferent input to a narrow band of cortex be- tween “SI” and motor cortex in a range of mammals, and the 3a field may be a wide- spread feature of mammalian brains. The Area 2 “deep” body field is, of course, only known for Old and New World monkeys, and even in these primates, the organization has not been determined in very much detail.

The significance of differences in the orga- nization of the cutaneous fields in the various primates is not known. In reviewing earlier studies, there are difficulties in determining whether the described representational orga- nization applies to Area 3b, to Area 1, or to both fields. Nonetheless, there is no question that quite marked differences in the repre- sentational sequence in either or both fields occur. For example, as reviewed earlier, the entire leg is represented lateral to the foot in

’Among the more complete maps of non-primate mammals are those derived in the opossum (Lende, ‘63; Pubols et al., ’76). the marsupial wallaby (Lende, ’631, the pig and sheep (Wmlsey and Fairman, ‘46; Johnson et al.. ‘74). rabbit (Wwlsey. ’64). rat (Woolsey, ’64; C. Welker. ’711, grey squirrel (Sur et al., ’781, dug (Pinto Hamuy et al., ‘56) guinea pig and capybara (Camps and Welker, ,761, llama (Welker et a]., ’76). hyrax (Welker and Carlson. ’761, beaver (Carlson and Welker, ’76), and raccoon (Welker et al., ‘64).

Page 31: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 71

apes and humans; the leg is represented almost completely on the medial side of the foot in owl monkeys; and the leg is split with the postaxial leg medial and the preaxial leg lateral to the foot in Old World monkeys. Perhaps, other differences among parietal somatosensory strip representations in dif- ferent primates will be described as the orga- nization of the PSS fields in more primates are mapped in detail. However, it is hardly surprising that species differences exist when the representations differ in organization to some extent from owl monkey to owl monkey. Thus, field organization in primates appears to be less constant and more subject to in- traspecies and interspecies variation than for- merly thought.

,

5 . A note on multiple representation of sensory epithelia in the neocortex

With the completion of the present studies, most of the neocortex caudal to the central sulcus has been mapped in the owl monkey. Nearly all of the parietal, occipital, and tem- poral cortex has been found to be occupied by retinotopically, cochleotopically, or somato- topically organized fields (fig. 16) (Allman and Kaas, ’76; Imig et al., ’77). For each of the six visual, the three somatic, and the four auditory areas that have been mapped in the owl monkey, there is evidence that the repre- sentations occupy delimitable cytoarchitec- tonic fields. Given the present redefinition of the organization of somatosensory cortex, no representations of the sensory epithelia in the neocortex overlie more than one generally rec- ognized cytoarchitectonic field. There can be little question that the functional subdivi- sions of neocortex can be distinguished by structural characteristics. And it is of great theoretical significance to concepts of the genesis of perception in the forebrain that all or nearly all of these fields in the parietal, oc- cipital and much of the temporal cortex embody systematic spatial representations of individual sensory epithelia.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors gratefully acknowledge the technical assistance of Laura Symonds, Randy Nelson and Leona Wayrynen. In some of these studied animals, Evelynn McGinnis carefully defined the caudal Area 4 border by use of mi- crostimulation. This work was supported by

NIH Grant NS-10414 and NSF Grant BNS 76-81824.

LITERATURE CITED Allman, J. M., and J. H. Kaas 1976 Representation of the

visual field on the medial wall of occipital-parietal cortex in the owl monkey. Science, 191: 572-575.

Bard, P. 1938 Studies on the cortical representation of somatic sensibility. Bulletin N. Y. Acad. Med., 14: 585-607.

Benisty, M. 1928 Lea lesionsde la zone Rolandique (Zone motrice e t zone sentive) por blessures de querre. Paris (cited by M. Hines, Physiol. Rev., 9: 462-574.)

Benjamin, R. M., and W. I. Welker 1957 Somatic receiving areas of cerebral cortex of squirrel monkey (Sairniri sciureus). J. Neurophysiol., 20: 286-299.

Bonin, G. von 1938 The cerebral cortex of the cebus mon- key. J. Comp. Neur.. 69: 181-227.

Brodmann, K. 1909 Vergleischende Lokalisationslehre der Groashirnride. Barth, Leipzig, 324 pp.

Camps , G. P., and W. I. Welker 1976 Comparisons between brains of a large and small Hystricomorph rodent: capybara, Hydrochoerus and guinea pig, Cauia; neocorti- cal projection regions and measurements of brain sub- divisions. Brain Behav. and Evol., 13: 243-266.

Carbon, M., and W. I. Welker 1976 Some morphological, physiological and behavior specializations in North American beavers. (Castor canadensisi. Brain Behav. and Evol., 13: 302-326.

Chang, H.-T., C. N. Woolsey, L. W. Jarcho and E. Henneman 1947 Representation of cutaneous tactile sensibility in the cerebral cortex of the spider monkey. Fed. Proc., 6: 89.

Clark, W. E. LeGros, and T. P. S. Powell 1953 On the thalamo-cortical connexions of the general sensory cortex of Macaca. Proc. Royal SOC. B, 141: 467-487.

Coulter, J. D., and E. G. Jones 1976 Subcortical projections from cytoarchitectonic fields of the somatic sensory cor- tex in monkey. Neuroscience Abst., 2: 1306.

Darian-Smith, I., J. Isbister, H. Mok and T. Yokota 1966 Somatic sensory cortical projection areas excited by tac- tile stimulation of the cat: A triple representation. J. Physiol. (London), 182: 671-687.

Dreyer, D. A,, P. R. Loe, C. B. Metz and B. L. Whitsel 1975 Representation of head and face in postcentral gyrus of the Macaque. J. Neurophysiol., 38: 714-733.

Dreyer, D. A., R. J. Schneider, C. B. Metz and B. L. Whitsel 1974 Differential contributions of spinal pathways to body representation in postcentral gyrus of Macaca mulatta. J. Neurophysiol., 37: 119-145.

Hand, P. J., and A. R. Morrison 1972 Thalamo-cortical rela- tionships in the somatic sensory system as revealed by silver impregnation techniques. Brain Behav. and Evol., 5: 273-302.

Head, H. 1920 Studies in Neurology. Vol. I. Oxford, London.

Heath, G. J., J. Hore and C. G. Phillips 1976 Inputs from low threshold muscle and cutaneous afferent8 of hand and forearm to Areas 3a and 3b of baboon’s cerebral cor- tex. J. Physiol. (London), 257: 199-227.

Hirsch, J. F., and W. S. Coxe 1958 Representation of cuta- neous tactile sensibility in cerebral cortex of Cebus. J. Neurophysiol., 21: 481-498.

Imig, T. J., M. A. Ruggero, L. M. Kitzes, E. Javel, and J. F. Brugge 1977 Organization of auditory cortex in the owl monkey (Aotus triuirgatus). J. a m p . Neur., 171: 111-128.

Johnson, J. I., E. W. Rubel and G. I. Hatton 1974 Mechano- sensory projections to cerebral cortex of sheep. J. Comp. Neur., 158: 81-108.

Page 32: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

72 M. M. MERZENICH, J. H. KAAS, M. SUR AND C:S. LIN

Jones. E. G. 1975 Lamination and differential distribu- tion of thalamic afferents within the sensory-motor cor- tex of the squirrel monkey. J. Comp. Neur., 160: 167-204.

1976 Cells of origin of afferent projections from the monkey first somatic sensory area. Neuroscience Abst., 2: 913.

Jones, E. G., and T. P. S. Powell 1969 The cortical projec- tion of the ventroposterior nucleus of the thalamus in the cat. Brain Res., 13: 298-318.

1970 Connections of the somatic sensory cortex of the rhesus monkey. 111. Thalamic connections. Brain, 93. 37-56.

Kaas, J. H.. M. M. Merzenich, C:S. Lin, and M. Sur 1976 A double representation of the body in “primary somatosen- sory cortex” (‘‘SI”) of primates. Neuroscience Abst., 2: 914.

Knight, P. K. 1977 Representation of the cochlea within the anterior auditory field (AAF) of the cat. Brain. Res., 130: 447-467.

Krishnamurti, A,, F. Sanides and W. I. Welker 1976 Micro- electrode mapping of modality-specific somatic sensory cerebral neocortex in slow loris. Brain Behav. and Evol., 13: 367-383.

Lee, K . J., and T. A. Woolsey 1975 A proportional relation- ship between peripheral innervation density and cortical neuron number in the somatosensory system of the mouse. Brain Res., 99: 349-353.

Lende, R. A. 1963 Cerebral cortex: A sensorimotor amalgam in the Marsupialia. Science, 141: 730-732.

Lin, C.-S., M. M. Merzenich, M. Sur and J. H. Kaas (1978, in preparation) Connections of Areas 3b and 1 of the parietal somatosensory strip with the ventroposterior nu- cleus in the owl monkey (Aotus triuirgatus).

Lucier, G. E., D. C. Ruegg and M. Wiesendanger 1975 Re- sponses of neurons in motor cortex and in Area 3a to con- trolled stretches of forelimb muscles in Cebus monkeys. J. Physiol. (London), 251: 833-853.

Marshall, W. H., C. N. Woolsey and P. Bard 1937 Cortical representation of tactile sensibility as indicated by corti- cal potentials. Science, 85: 388-390.

Merzenich, M. M., P. L. Knight and G. L. Roth 1975 Repre- sentation of cochlea within primary auditory cortex in the cat. J. Neurophysiol., 38: 231-249.

Mountcastle, V. B. 1957 Modality and topographic prop- erties of single neurons of cat’s somatic sensory cortex. J. Neurophysiol., 20: 408-434.

Mountcastle, V. B., and T. P. S. Powell 1959 Central nervous mechanisms subserving position sense and kinesthesis. Bulletin Johns Hopkins Hospital, 105: 173-200.

Mountcastle, V. B., W. H. Talbot, H. Sakata and J. Hy- varinen 1969 Cortical neuronal mechanisms in flutter- vibration studies in unanesthetized monkeys. Neuronal periodicity and frequency discrimination. J. Neu- rophysiol., 32: 452-484.

Nelson, R. J., and M. Sur 1977 Organization of primary somatosensory cortex @MI) in the grey squirrel. Anat. Rec., 187: 666.

Paul, R. L., M. M. Merzenich and H. Goodman 1972a Repre- sentation of slowly and rapidly adapting cutaneous mech- anoreceptors of the hand in Brodmann’s areas 3 and 1 of Macaca mulatta. Brain Res., 36: 229-249.

Paul, R. L., H. Goodman and M. M. Merzenich 1972b Al- terations in mechanoreceptor input to Brodmann’s areas 1 and 3 of the postcentral hand area of Macaca mulatta after nerve section and regeneration. Brain Res., 39: 1-19.

Paul, R. L., M. M. Merzenich and H. Goodman 1975 Mecha- noreceptor representation and topography of Brodmann’s areas 3 and 1 ofMacaca mulatta. In: The Somatosensory

System. H. H. Kornhuber and G. Thieme, eds. Stuttgart,

Peden, J. H., and G. von Benin 1947 The neocortex of Hapale. J. Comp. Neur., 86: 37-63.

Penfield, W., and E. Boldrey 1937 Somatic motor and senso- ry representation in the cerebral cortex of man as studied by electrical stimulation. Brain, 60: 389-443.

Penfield, W., and T. Rasmussen 1950 The Cerebral Cortex of Man. Macmillan, New York.

Phillips, C. G., T. P. S. Powell and M. Wiesendanger 1971 Projection from low-threshold muscle afferents of hand and forearm to Area 3a of baboon’s cortex. J. Physiol. (London), 21 7: 419-446.

Pinto Hamuy, T., R. B. Bromiley and C. N. Woolsey 1956 Somatic afferent areas I and I1 of the dog’s cerebral cor- tex. J. Neurophysiol., 19: 485-499.

Powell, T. P. S., and V. B. Mountcastle 1959a The cytoar- chitecture of the postcentral gyrus of the monkey Macaca mulatta. Bulletin Johns Hopkins Hospital, 105: 108-131.

Some aspects of the functional organiza- tion of the cortex of the postcentral gyrus of the monkey. A correlation of findings obtained in a single unit analy- sis with cytoarchitecture. Bulletin Johns Hopkins Hospital, 105: 133-162.

Pubols, B. H., and L. M. Pubols 1971 Somatotopic organiza- tion of spider monkey somatic sensory cerebral cortex. J. Comp. Neur., 141: 63-76.

Neural organization of somatic sensory representation in the spider monkey. Brain Behav. and Evol., 5: 342-366.

Pubols, B. H., L. M. Pubols, D. J. DiPette and J. C. Sheely 1976 Opossum somatic sensory cortex: A microelectrode mapping study. J. Comp. Neur., 165: 229-246.

Randolph, M., and J. Semmes 1974 Behavioral consequence of selective subtotal ablation in the postcentral gyrus of Macaca muhtta. Brain Res., 70: 55.70.

Rosabal, F. 1967 Cytoarchitecture of the frontal lobe of the squirrel monkey. J. Comp. Neur., 130: 87-108.

Sanides, F. 1968 The architecture of the cortical taste nerve areas in squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus) and their relationships to insular, sensorimotor and prefron- tal regions. Brain Res., 8: 97-124.

Sanides, F., and A. Krishnamurti 1967 Cytoarchitectonic subdivisions of sensorimotor and prefrontal regions and of bordering insular and limbic field in slow loris (Nyc- ticebus coucang coucang). J. Hirnforsch., 9: 225-252.

Shanks, M. F., A. J. Rockel and T. P. S. Powell 1975 The commissural fiber connections of the primary somatic sensory cortex. Brain Res., 98: 166-171.

Sur, M., R. J. Nelson and J. H. Kaas 1978 The representa- tion of the body surface in somatosensory Area I of the grey squirrel. J. Comp. Neur., 179: 425-450.

Tanji, J. 1975 Activity of neurons in cortical area 3a during maintenance of steady postures by the monkey. Brain Res., 88: 549-553.

Tigges, J. , W. B. Spatz and M. Tigges 1973 Reciprocal point- to-point connections between parastriate and striate cor- tex in the squirrel monkey (Saimirii. J. Comp. Neur., 148: 481-490.

Vogt, B. A. 1976 The origin, course and termination of intrinsic connections between Areas 3 and 1-2 in the mon- key. Anat. Rec., 184: 554.

Wisenberg, J. A., and A. Rustioni 1976 Cortical cells pro- jecting to the dorsal column nuclei of cats. Anatomical study with the horseradish peroxidase technique. J. Comp. Neur., 165: 425-437.

Welker, C. 1971 Microelectrode delineation of fine grain somatotopic organization of SmI cerebral neocortex in albino rat. Brain Res., 26: 259-275.

pp. 262-269.

1959b

1972

Page 33: Double representation of the body surface within ...web.mit.edu/surlab/publications/1978_MerzenichKaasSurLin.pdf · progression or representation of body parts in a wide band of post-central

ORGANIZATION OF PRIMATE “SI” 73

Welker, W. I., H. 0. Adrian, W. Lifschitz, R. Kaulen, E. Caviedes and W. Gutman 1976 Somatic sensory cortex of llama lLarna glama). Brain Behav. and Evol., 13: 284-293.

Welker, W. I., and M. Carbon 1976 Somatic sensory cortex of hyrax (Procavia). Brain Behav. and Evol., 13: 294-301.

Welker, W. I., J. I. Johnson and B. H. Pubols 1964 Some morphological and physiological characteristics of somat- ic sensory system in raccoons. Am. Zool., 4: 75-94.

Welt, C. 1963 Topographical organization of the somatic sensory and motor areas of the cerebral cortex of the gib- bon (Hylobated and chimpanzee (Pan). Ph.D. Thesis, University of Chicago.

Werner, G., and B. L. Whitsel 1968 Topology of the body representation in somatosensory area I of primates. J. Neurophysiol., 31: 856-869.

The functional organization of the soma- tosensory cortex. In: Handbook of Sensory Physiology, A. Iggo, ed. Springer, New York, pp. 621-700.

Whitsel, B. L., D. A. Dreyer and J. R. Roppolo 1971 Deter- minants of body representation in postcentral gyrus of Macaques. J. Neurophysiol., 34: 1018-1034.

Whitsel, B. L., L. M. Petrucelli, H. Ha and D. A. Dreyer 1972 The resorting of spinal afferents as antecedent to the body representation in the Wtcentral gyrus. Brain Behav. and Evol., 5: 303-341.

Woolsey, C. N. 1952 Patterns of localization in sensory and motor areaa of the cerebral cortex. In: The Biology of Mental Health and Disease. Milbank Memorial Fund, Hoeber. New York, pp. 193-206. - 1954 Localization patterns in a lissencephalic

primate (Hapale jacchus). Amer. J. Physiol., 178: 686. 1958 Organization of somatic sensory and motor

1971

areas of the cerebral cortex. In: Biological and Biochem- ical Bases of Behavior. H. F. Harlow and C. N. Woolsey, eds. University of Wisconsin, Madison, pp. 63-81. - 1964 Cortical localization as defined by evoked potential and electrical stimulation studies. In: Cerebral Localization and Organization. G. Schalter Brand and C. N. Woolsey, eds. University of Wisconsin, Madison, pp.

Woolsey, C. N., andD. Fairman 1946 Contralateral, ipsilat- eral, and bilateral representation of cutaneous receptors in somatic areas I and I1 of the cerebral cortex of pig, sheep, and other mammals. Surgery, 19: 684-702.

Woolsey, C. N., W. H. Marshall and P. Bard 1942 Repre- sentation of cutaneous tactile sensibility in the cerebral cortex of the monkey aa indicated by evoked potentials. Bulletin Johns Hopkins Hospital, 70: 399-441. - 1943 Note on organization of tactile sensory

area of cerebral cortex of chimpanzee. J. Neurophysiol., 6: 287-291.

Woolsey, C. N., R. Taaker, C. Welt, R. Ladpli, G. Camps , H. I). Potter, R. Emmera and H. Schwassmann 1960 Or- ganization of pre- and postcentral leg areas in chim- panzee and gibbon. Trans. Amer. Neurol. Assoc., 85:

Yumiya, H., K. Kubota and H. Asanuma 1974 Activities of neurons in area 3a of the cerebral cortex during voluntary movements in the monkey. Brain Res., 78: 169-177.

Zarzechi, P., Y. Shimada and H. Asanuma 1976 The projec- tion of group I afferents to the motor cortex in Area 3a. Neuroscience Abst., 2: 958.

Zimmerman, I. D. 1968 A triple representation of the body surface in the sensorimotor cortex of the squirrel monkey. Exp. Neurol., 20: 415-431.

17-26.

144-146.