42
ED 094 862 AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE NOTE AVAILABLE FROM EDRS PRICE DESCRIPTORS DOCUMENT RESUME PS 007 324 Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H. School Readiness Factor Analyzed. Merrill-Palmer Inst., Detroit, Mich. [71] 44p. The Merrill-Palmer Institute, 71 E. Ferry Avenue, Detroit, Michigan 48202 (30.25) HF-$0.75 HC-$1.85 PLUS POSTAGE *Factor Analysis; Learning Readiness; Maturity Tests; Middle Class; Physical Development; *Preschool Programs; *Readiness; Readiness (Mental); Reading Readiness; *Research Methodology; *School Readiness Tests; Sex Differences; Standardized Tests; Statistical Analysis; Teacher Attitudes ABSTRACT This paper is an empirical statistical analysis and interpretation of data relating to school readiness previously examined and reported on a theoretical basis. A total of 118 white, middle class children from six consecutive kindergarten groups in Dearborn, Michigan were tested with seven instruments, evaluated in terms of achievement, ability, and overall maturity by their teachers, and physically examined and x-rayed. In comparing these diversified bodies of information, which also included sex and chronological age, 69 independent variables related to school readiness were counted which represented the range of phenomena to be factor analyzed. Performance of the analysis permitted the synthesir of new entities, or factors, which were far fewer in number than the initial raw variables. Analysis findings revealed seven facets of readiness which together are basic determinants of overall school readiness. They include cognitive readiness, chronological age, reading readiness, body of knowledge, perceptual differentiation, physical development status, and biochemical maturity factor. In the interpretation of each factor, the most highly loaded variables are listed and examined. Discussion focuses on the changed readiness concept, sex differences, chronological age, the importance of knowledge, and the accomplishments, limitations, and implications of this factor analysis. (SDH)

DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

ED 094 862

AUTHORTITLEINSTITUTIONPUB DATENOTEAVAILABLE FROM

EDRS PRICEDESCRIPTORS

DOCUMENT RESUME

PS 007 324

Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H.School Readiness Factor Analyzed.Merrill-Palmer Inst., Detroit, Mich.[71]44p.The Merrill-Palmer Institute, 71 E. Ferry Avenue,Detroit, Michigan 48202 (30.25)

HF-$0.75 HC-$1.85 PLUS POSTAGE*Factor Analysis; Learning Readiness; Maturity Tests;Middle Class; Physical Development; *PreschoolPrograms; *Readiness; Readiness (Mental); ReadingReadiness; *Research Methodology; *School ReadinessTests; Sex Differences; Standardized Tests;Statistical Analysis; Teacher Attitudes

ABSTRACTThis paper is an empirical statistical analysis and

interpretation of data relating to school readiness previouslyexamined and reported on a theoretical basis. A total of 118 white,middle class children from six consecutive kindergarten groups inDearborn, Michigan were tested with seven instruments, evaluated interms of achievement, ability, and overall maturity by theirteachers, and physically examined and x-rayed. In comparing thesediversified bodies of information, which also included sex andchronological age, 69 independent variables related to schoolreadiness were counted which represented the range of phenomena to befactor analyzed. Performance of the analysis permitted the synthesirof new entities, or factors, which were far fewer in number than theinitial raw variables. Analysis findings revealed seven facets ofreadiness which together are basic determinants of overall schoolreadiness. They include cognitive readiness, chronological age,reading readiness, body of knowledge, perceptual differentiation,physical development status, and biochemical maturity factor. In theinterpretation of each factor, the most highly loaded variables arelisted and examined. Discussion focuses on the changed readinessconcept, sex differences, chronological age, the importance ofknowledge, and the accomplishments, limitations, and implications ofthis factor analysis. (SDH)

Page 2: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

11y Of f. Vf s{\I DU( , *1.1*41r

11 th1,111/11t1.)04 5,04

1

.1`.

. -.51141

. 1

Apro'aworms40,0444

SCHOOL READINESS FACTOR ANALYZED

byanton brenner AND leland h. stott

the merrill-palmer institute

Page 3: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

The fat0;00 he, beta1,pootted

fhr011Oilu4 the years byijm,ndfre On of Henry

ford MuseumAnd Or4.0441(1

11(tvder,9 Dr Dona'd A SIPnr"PVCI0,4, 'rent

Caddy. VPre, dent,

Acfmetetret,on, James A fowler, 0,rector of Ed,

1,0,, andMeth Sttoef-et

fo,mat Pr n, 041Gr.enCeld VdlaN

Schools now Manager.School Servces

Gy,s,on They alto ma'evadable the serv.ces of a sel,es of tchool

psycholng.sts at t:.Greenteld V liege Schools

Here our ts,ents go fotemOtt to LHalmo Hofmann tot h.1 most attli,atle

cot.or,bot,on, thenOre Jerry tkowti.

Geese andTom gc,t4,ey, who it'

shared ,n the10.nt efforts to mere lh

a worthyvhdeprotest

bo not leo!, tha t,jv flys ffheparents, and rhr toup,t1

of theGreenfla'cl

Vdlage Schooltdeattve our thantra for their

on gongrSel'etf aid

coop,rat.on The author's also want soexpress the., hankf to Drs Joseph A

Johnston, Gordon Man .

ihrn, and VeymontiMe11,nger from Henry ford

Hosp,fal, Detroit.

for the. tontnved tooperet.ontoneutteton and

Cnanc,e1 cup.

port DeeJohneton and Manton were in carge of the

physicalexam,nat,ont; Dr.

Med,nger of the hormonestudy,. Thanks go

equally to Dr S. Odell Pyle,Merr,11Pel.ner Inst,toe end Case

Western Reservetln,versdy.

Cleveland, who read the x-rays todetarmove the children's skeletal ago,

Page 4: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

CONTENTS

DESCRIPTION AND DEVELOPMENT OF PROJECT 5

Our concept of school readiness 5

The need for a factor/analysis study 6

Hypotheses to be tested 7

Research population and raw data 8

Readiness factor analyzed 9

Table I: Illustration of raw data as prep., -d for IBM corn' utation 10

FINDINGS 11

The result: Seven readiness factors to be interpreted 11

INTERPRETATION 11

Factor A: Cognitive Readiness 11

Table Principal exis solution, Variables 1.35 12

Variables 36-69

Factor B: Chronological Age 17Table III: Ro*ated facto-s. VerimaK solution. Variables 1-35 18

Variables 36.69 19

Factor C: Reading Readiness 21

Factor D: Body of Knowledge 23

Fetter G: Perceptual Differentiation 24

Factor F. Physical Developmental Status 25

Actor E: Biochemical Maturity Factor 27

DISCUSSION 30

The changed readiness concept 30

Sex differences 33

The role of chronological age 35

The importance of knowledge 37

Accomplishments and limitations of our factor analysis 39

SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS 43

REFERENCES 46

.,1/41

Page 5: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

School Readiness Factor AnalyzedAnton Brenner and Leland H. Stott*

The study reported here is part of a Iong.range research project onchildren's readiness for school which originated in 1953 and wascarried on through 1968. It was conducted by the MerrillPalmer Ins-titute, Detroit, with the cooperation of Henry Ford Hospital, Detroit;the G-eenfield Village Schools, Dearborn; the Detroit Public Schools;and many schools in and nut of Michigan. The project was designedto develop a ceniprehensive concept of school readiness, to further ourunderstanding of the characteristics of children indicative of theirdegrees of readiness, and to learn about the kinds of demands whichschools set up for children. The development of methods of measuringreadiness was another major objective.

The earlier phase of the project was exploratory in nah.re, to befollowed by a program testing the findings, first on a smaller and thenon a larger scale. All studies, including the factor analysis presentedhere, were carried out in such a way that they were consistent withour c ,n:ept of readiness. They must be understood within this fameof reference. It will therefore be helpful first to give a selected e (poseof our ideas on readiness published in detail elsewhere.

DESCRIPTION AND DEVELOPMENT OF PROJECT

Our Coicept of Readiness

Readiness, like life, is being and becoming. It is process and theresult of process. "At the beginning of the life cycle the organism isweakly developed in structure and function; is inexperienced withnurture and environment and has little learning [Hughes, 19581."Structure, function, effectivity, cognition, personality, and readinessare parts and results of on-going developmental processes and changes.Growth, development, and learning take place through constant inter-action between an individual and his environment. This leads togradually increasing accumulation and differentiation in the individualorganism with an increase in his ability to perceive, to analyze, and tosynthesize experience both from within and beyond himself. The moredevelopmentally advanced the person is, the more he is able to acteffectively in pursuing and controlling his developmental tasks. Trans-lated into readiness for school this means that the more a child isable to perceive, to incorporate experience into developing behavior,and finally to analyze and synthesize it, the more he is ready for school.Readiness then can be ccnsidered a continuing function of perceptual-conceptual and personal development.

Dr. Anton Brenner of the MerrillPalmer Institute was director of the proiert. He is nowProfessor of Education at Eastern Michigan University, Ypsilanti, Michigan. Dr. Leland H.Stott is Faculty Emeritus at Merrill.Palmer.

Page 6: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

6

Readiness for school is the result of past development and learning.It is the basis, the prerequisite, for further learning. It is always a stateof development produced by heredita factors which are transactedand transformed through nurture and an individual's life experiencesto a unique organic system that allows functioning and performance ofspecific tasks.

Readiness is seen as the pupil's multidimensional functinna, potential.ity in relation to the multidimensional task requirements in school. Tounderstand the full complexity and meaning of readiness, we must seethe individual and task aspects together in each and all of the subject.object dimensions and in their multifaceted related,. !SS and inter.wovenness. Where individual volume (the child's abilities, knowledge.and experiences) is commensurate with or surpasses task volume(school demands), there is readiness. Where task volume surpassesindividual volume, there is unreadiness.

There is no single road to readiness.

Each individual is different from every other one in his geneticmakeup and with regard to the nurture affecting him. The transactionsthat occur between the developing organism and the field forcesimpinging upon him create for each child a different pattern of growth,maturation, learning, and readiness. Readiness is the result of the conslant interaction between the child's physical, mental, emotional, andsocial personality and these field forces.

Readiness for certain school demands occurs in each child in a

different way and at a different time. Each individual is different inhis functional potentiality and biopsychic readiness for school expecta-tions . nd demands which vary in themselves according to culture,social s'ratum, locale, administration, teacher and parent personalities,and pl losophies. There is great variability among pupils and schooldemani s and therefore also in the various dimensions and patterns ofreadin, ss.

The Need for a Factor Analysis Study

Prior to our factor analysis, a series of studies had been carried outover the years with the number of subjects varying from 18 to over1,000. These studies were designed to approach the problem of readiness from various angles and to shed some light on different aspects ofreadiness. From the very outset readiness was conceived of as a com-plex phenomenon of multidimensional individualtask relationships. Thetime came when it was felt that tentative formulations from earlierstudies had to be put on a more solid basis: (a) by increasing the re-search population of the study; (b) by adding varied and multidimen-sional content of information; and (c) by studying the interrelatednessand underlying commonalities of the various aspects of this complexitywhich we call readiness. Only then would we come closer to a totalunderstanding of readiness and get results which have both moredefinition and extensive meaning than single separate studies. Therewas a need for unification in analysis to give related or seeminglyindependent elements coherence and meaning in the whole. Such

Page 7: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

7

thinking obviously pointed to the need for a factor analysis study ona broad scale. Of this factor analysis study, computed in 1962 fromdata collected during the preceding nine years, we are reporting here.

Hypotheses To Be Tested

The first hypotheses or problems investigated have led to first find.ings and tentative formulations about readiness. These earlier findingsand tentative generalizations derived from them were used as newhypotheses to be wood again through our factor analysis study. Thenew hypotheses purposely include also some of our conceptualizeionsabout readiness which we now wanted to have checked through thisstudy. The thus compiled hypotheses read as follows:

Readiness is a multidimensional, individual task relationship withmultiple demands on many facets of the child's personality.

The more differentiated a child it in his personality structure andfunction, the better he is prepared for school.

Readiness is present when there is a good fit between individualvolume and task volume. Individual volume is understood as quantityand quality of preparedness. Task volume refers to the number andmagnitude of school demands.

There is no single road to readiness. Different children will arriveat readiness through different combinations of abilities, knowledge,and responses to school tasks and expectations.

A ready child has considerable knowledge and comprehension ofthe world in which he lives,

A 4LIE

Page 8: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

$

He has enough skids to communicate many of his experiences, feel-ings, and thoughts, verhally, projectively, or represtntationally (suchas in drawings).

He recognizes similarities and differences among persons, objects,colors, forms, sounds, numbers, and symbols.

Perceptual-conceptual discrimination ability is a major agent in per-sonality development, learning, and readiness for school.

Cognitive skills such as perceptual-conceptual ability to see relation-ships, to analyze and to synthesize, to see comm.:ow:Lies, to classify,to abstract and generalize, and to work with symbols are among thebest indicators of readiness.

A ready child has at least a fair amount of intelligence.

He has a good vocabulary and number comprehension.He draws general conclusiort from what he sees going on around

him and he forms ideas on the basis of his conclusions.He has imagination to use his intelligence and past experiences in

attacking new problems.It is possible to assess readiness quite accurately through focusing

in a test on the most determining factors in readiness.It is possible to assess readiness and predict success in kindergarten

or first grade with high degree of certainty using the Anton BrennerDevelopmental Gestalt Test of School Re "diness.

Judgment of children by an experienced and sensitive teacherrelates well to children's performances on standardized tests.

There are no big sex differences in school readiness.

Research Population and Raw DataFor the purposes of our factor analysis we selected six consecutive

kindergarten groups from the Greenfield Village schools in Dearborn,Michigan, a total of 118 children. Takint the position that readiness is

multidimensional complex phenomenon, result, and expression of awhole person of his language, perception, and motor skills; of hisphysical, mental, emotional, and social development in their relation-ship to multidimentional tasks; we collected and used diversified bodiesof data.

Each child was given a complete annual physical examination atHenry Ford Hospital which included height, weight, vision, teetheruption, xrays of ham.; and wrist bones to determine skeletal age,and hormonal studies through the analysis of two twenty-four hoursamples of urine of the children in 1960 and 1961 when they hadbecome 7 to 13 years old. The urinary hormone studies included:17-ketosteroids, 17hydroxycorticosteroids, and gonadotropins. Urinarycreatinine was determined to gauge the completeness of the 24-hoururine collection. The assay provided a useful index of lean body mass.

Teachers evaluated the children in terms of achievement, ability, andoverall maturity. Tests were given: the Sangren Information Test, the

Page 9: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

9

PintnerCunningham Prim try Test, the Metropolitan Readiness Test, theMonroe Reading Aptitude Test, the Monroe New Basic Reading Test,the Stanford-Binet Test, and the Anton Brenner Developmental GestaltTest of School Readiness. Two administrations of the Brenner test wereincluded, an earlier and a later one, and so were two readings ofskeletal age, one made at the time of the physical examination inkindergarten and another at the time when the hormone studies weremade.

Sex was a variable and so was chronological age at entrance of achild to kindergarten and at the time of administration of each test,teacher wdgment, or physical examination. By considering each subtestof the tests given as a variable (surface variable) and adding all vari-ables up, we arrived at a total of 69 variables. For each child, then, wehave 69 variables to measure readiness. The 69 variables represent therange of phenomena analyzed in the factor analysis, or to use Thurstone s term, they are our "domain" of readiness studied from 118children.

Although the children were selected for admission to kindergarten,10 boys and 10 girls in each group, they are an unselected researchpopulation in the sense that we did not divide them into experimentaland control groups. The six kindergarten groups were used in theirnatural composition. No child was left out. The children are represent.rive of American middle-class white population.

Table 1 illustrates how raw data were coded and prepared for IBMcomputation.

Readiness Factor Analyzed

The nature, function, and methodology of facto, analysis has beenclearly described by Cattell (1952, 1957), Harman (1960), Guilford

1968), and others. In one of our earlier publications (Brenner, 1962)we examined on a more thooretica' basis the contribution of factoranalysis to the study of readiness for school. The present paper is anempirical statistical analysis and interpretation of our data. In essencewe are looking for the truly determining readiness factors which under-lie the bewildering multiplicity of 69 surface variables presumablyrelated in one way or another to the child's readiness to learn. Factoranalysis helps us "in that it constructs from a hrst of variables theimportant wholes which need to be taken into account when seekinglaws of interpretation (Cattell, 1957j." It "aims to discover and dealwith the more massive, functional, and organic wholes instead of losingresearch perspective in a mass of atom;sticslly conceived variables(Cattell, 1957)." Factor analysis ''analyzes o the distinct factors atwork among the variables; it also groups the vari.-tiles together in wayswhich permit one to synthesize new entities far ,ewer than the initialraw variables. These new entities are our 'factors' (Thurston°, 1961 ]."

Seven factors were extracted. The unrotated axes are given in Table2. They were rotated by computer to the Verimax criterion of simplestructure. Table 3 presents the rotated factors. They are the "result" ofour factor analysis which we have now to analyze and to interpret.

Page 10: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

iri0..ftm t

7g3ZIS

Ir0

Vi7.

Y5

ES

0

6

iPa.

.Z

6cg1,.;

cA

SINVd

12111131d

ri

';:t

r I

., - , tr, .r, it-, *an in 0 tr., C;) 0

6 ,....., ,- ,.-' - , 4 .-; Iri

- 0 0- 0 0 0 - - 0

kr-J .r) 0 0

1-: 4 - xi

0 .... - 0

0

0AO

AIII3Sla4,-," re, of r I rt r a .- 0..., i .. -1,

_ .

r 4 .r *1 .I` r I

S.1.331110.3ossv

____

.t, 1 .t '0 't 't .0 M .? C 4" 4 .1. .0 z .0 ..)

'113.1.11113113H1S3V

.._

,.i'l 4 z v.+ , ,4 z z v., z , .. .04 e) G ...;

1VA113S110NO11111103

. ..-r1000000v1Okriv-,v-$04nOvI"_... v, .. ....r, .., 4. ., x .. , .., , , ., ,

1SINIWIV111 V 3

31111.1.VM3.1.11V 39V119111V1

0 .., 0 kr) r- in - 0 N e*, goe'o- r- h r- r- r,.- h h h 4

I I i i 1 ; ; ; ;

0: r i - ...". .0 - 40 .1' t- 41. r--h

e I I' I 0 I' I ell I' I I' I .- -

7C, r I kr) .4)

.0 r- 4) N; I 1 ; I

se, ot - 0,

.... I' I el ..-

41

r-

e s

r I

NOI1V1i1110ANI#310H3Sf1OH

4 7 .." en 0 .0 N "t - (0 20v,-. - ei ei , .... el .....

.0 0 0 30

el el ....

0,

,NOLLVIIIIIOANI

1V130SI vl

N.r.' `0 .0 S 0 "t N.

,..,- ei el .... ea ea - - In 1..1 ei

- 't *0 el

f.1 ell el el

40

el

AVVV8V30Ae'l e4 en /' vl gt 0, .0 vl I-- s0

-- en en fki rel rel ... el rei .. IN

f-- el en vl

eNI 01 en en

0

in

SII3liminN0 - ,... x .1. Tr kr1 0 0, 0 0-0. e$ - I 0 el -O .... e$ rs$

.I. kr) ei,

C) ...- - .-- el-----Aanis311111VN

.0 0 0 .3. .c. ,., .3 r.4 0...,, 30s el el .... el el . rsi es e$ 1-4

fel ret let - letes. "4 rsi e4 eV

1SINIWGVIV If 3

9N oi ANIN3.I.V V 3

4:3 4)f.' "t " 30 a` "r t7' : ,r `e 02 eel e4 0tr) r- .0 r- r- r- ,t0 '0 r- .0 .0 .0 40 ,t0 N N

$ I 3 3 3 3 3 I 3 $ ; $ $ $I 4::. - s cr., s M .1 t: P ,.." ON et 0 1 CO s. :VI

r+ 0 .o; z 42. ,.o z .0 1 43 4.) kr) 401 k0 lel 40 .03iviii3A

31VIIe$

-MaC.0

.-.-,-....- 4N- e$ ei - .- - ei e$ ev !e$,

1

1

el -1

I

ea ' -S318VISVA

.- r, ..t .H wif .44 . 00r 04 li c'- - e I14 1 -

S1138WIN 3SV3

0I

Page 11: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

11

FINDINGS

The Result: Seven Readiness Factors to be Interpreted

The seven factors extracted portray seven different facets of readi-ness which in their togetherness are also the basic determinants ofoverall readiness. In each factor the variables with the highest loadingsgave the factor its specific ch&tatteritation. Therefore, the interpretationof the factors was trade by checking the variables with the highestfactor loadinys. Then each significant variable was examined as to thenature of the task presented to the child, the meaning the item mayhave for him, or the kind of demand it make:. on the child. In mostcases the factors were characterized by groupings of significant vari-ables. Analysis ni the nature and meaning of these groupings suggestedthe name given to each factor. The names given are, of course, open todiscussion and can be revised because, as thurstone (1961) points out,the name for a factor depends on the context of one's philosophical

preferences and manner of speech, and of how much one alreadyknows about the dumain to be investigated."

INTERPRETATION

FACTOR A: Cognitive Readiness

Factor A is identified as cognitive readiness, the general ability tomeet the intellectual demands and perform the tasks of orimary school.Nineteen of the 69 variables included in the analyti0 came out withloadings greater than .50 in this factor. These items listed in the orderof magnitude of factor loadings are as follows:

No. VariableFactor

Loading63 Teacher Judgment of Ability .8264 Teacher Judgment of Maturity .7735 Brenner Draw a Man (2nd administration) .7736 Brenner Sentenc, Copying (2nd) .7562 Teacher J.....-lyment of Achievement .7233 Brenner Number Recognition (2nd) .6623 Metropolitan Numbers .6530 Brenner Sentence Copying (1st) .6432 Brenner Number Producing (2nd) .6326 Brenner Number Producing (1st) .6224 Metropolitan Copying .6127 Brenner Number Recognition (1st) .6034 Brenner 10 Dot Gestalt (2nd) .6053 IQ StanfordBinet .5722 Metropolitan Matching .5517 PintnerCunningham Dot Drawing .5429 Brenner Jraw a Man (1st) .5 t

5 Sangren Numbers .5328 Brenner 10 Dot Gestalt (1st) ,1!

Page 12: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

12

.1 I'Ye

If 4-e V- re. ee. 6 .0 5 48 6 .s 6n:!?....-S:-Iv-: 4.-

art- vNI.v.....141,..1,1,er t .4..1 ViDgCt--1., 4:1 4101",3 4' ' ° 3 4 7 401 ...011 Os.

,e, .... .4 et ..,t11,.`,..771. ir , -r. 111,, 7 er. et. r- VI t- FO, I

1 les 0 .1) st en re. e le. ee 4.. 4 I`'7 .", I Ile te, 0.,0,0 ."7". dome`,

f 0-

";Z* § " "fs gro CC

- 0 0 000 le. -4 0

0,.1e is43

var, .. et ,D 44.1 4 di. .. r.

er IP .1

Ir. CP. 0 IP 411.- 10 CP. 0

0 0 - et te

I.- .0 ,11 IR)sr F q "110 r- se in.0 4 or, 4 JD

...ro 411 e- On IP 0 t. g/ tetet #.4 e 14 0.4 oe% re1 P1 P1 1

r, -0r ?el .-1'4.s., rNrD1-, ae 41. R74 wlAP' g" -NA 4"e.4 et ret ret e 0. eet

Page 13: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

TA

BL

E 2

(C

ont.)

Prin

cipa

l Axi

s So

lutio

n -

Tot

al D

eck

Fac

tor

Inpu

tV

ar. #

Var

.#36

3637

3738

3839

3940

4041

4142

4243

4344

4445

4546

4647

4748

4849

4950

5051

5152

5253

5354

5455

5556

5657

5758

5859

5960

6061

6162

6263

6364

6465

6566

6667

6768

6869

69

Sen

tenc

e C

opyi

ng 1

2lid

)C

A M

onro

e R

eadi

ng A

ptitu

de T

est

MO

Vis

ual P

erce

ptio

nM

O A

udito

ry P

erce

ptio

nM

O M

otor

Per

cept

ion

MO

Art

icul

atio

nM

O L

angu

age

CA

Mon

roe

Nes

s B

asic

Rea

ding

Tes

tN

BR

Sen

tenc

e M

eani

ngN

BR

Sen

sory

Imag

ery

NB

R R

elat

ions

hips

NB

R E

mov

onal

Rea

ctio

n &

Mot

ives

NB

R V

isua

l Scr

utin

yN

BR

Pho

netic

Ana

lysi

sN

BR

Str

uctu

ral A

naly

sis

..C

A S

tanf

ord

- B

inet

MA

Sta

nfor

d-B

inet

IQ S

tanf

ord

- B

inet

CA

Phy

sica

l Exa

min

atio

nS

kele

tal A

geH

eigh

tW

eigh

tV

isio

nN

umbe

r of

Per

man

ent T

eeth

Eru

pted

Dev

elop

men

tal L

evel

--

Wi a

el...

CA

Tea

cher

Jud

gmen

tT

J of

Ach

ieve

men

tT

J of

Abi

lity

T1

of M

atur

ityS

kele

tal A

ge a

t Hor

mon

e S

tudy

Gon

adot

ropi

ns17

-Ket

oste

roid

s17

-H y

droi

tyco

rtic

oste

roid

sC

reat

inin

e

111

111

IVV

VI

VII

.487

.284

-.09

0-.

345

-.41

'.1

43-.

120

.315

.860

.330

.099

-.11

9.0

38-.

036

.603

-.03

3-

-.07

5-.

050

.149

.206

A05

.402

.026

- .2

93.1

59.0

14.3

23.1

28.4

35.1

73.0

34-.

016

-.01

3.0

19-.

087

.588

.007

.032

.185

.169

-.13

0-.

074

.397

-.04

3.1

97.2

57.0

09-.

059

.175

.233

-.87

1.2

60.0

07.1

45.1

36-.

046

.323

.095

,.0

61.0

811

-.21

2- .

329

.038

.386

.319

.6: '

.012

.361

-142

- .07

7.3

49.2

65.6

17-

.050

.369

.077

-.13

6.2

90.2

70.5

81-.

129

.3; 7

.077

.027

.349

.397

673

.042

.302

-.62

3.0

32.4

68.3

19.2

21-.

270

.069

.290

.091

.410

.345

.419

-.22

0.1

39-

.090

.089

.176

-.62

4-.

308

.209

.010

.133

- .1

03.5

82-

.213

--.4

12.2

15-

.064

.050

039

.620

.292

-.27

4.0

84-

.047

.171

.14)

.252

- .7

38.3

41-.

051

- .1

81-.

020

-.04

5.1

75-.

464

-.15

6-

.386

.117

-.34

7-.

056

.180

-.33

6-

.035

-.58

3.1

89-.

298

.043

.200

-.35

7-.

082

-.68

3.2

85-.

278

.072

.230

.105

.159

.080

.366

.028

.131

.249

--.1

14.1

02-.

142

-.10

3-.

357

.203

.198

--.3

58-.

051

-.78

8.1

28-.

189

.117

.265

-.84

8.1

17.1

53-.

220

-.07

5-.

115

.697

.225

-.13

8-.

010

-.06

1-.

147

-.24

8.7

76.2

61-

.158

-.13

9-

.132

-.03

6-.

136

.765

.200

- A

35

- .0

31-

.108

- .0

96-.

174

.092

-.48

6-.

724

--.1

44.3

35.1

42-.

233

.107

-.28

8.4

91-.

167

.201

.376

- .0

36.2

23-.

514

- .6

65-

.154

.201

.000

.045

.166

-.53

2-.

290

-.22

3.1

25-.

015

.201

.268

-.42

7-.

546

-.09

3.2

83-.

020

.036

14.7

9210

.037

5.42

63.

171

2.62

02.

409

2.23

5_

-

Page 14: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

14 //)..

The identification of this factor with the co mit; in aspect f schoolability is based largely upon its high loading: with the thre, "teacherjudgment" variables of ability, maturity, and achievement Variables63, 64, and 62 (loadings of .82, .77, .72 r.. pectivel;.). This may comeas a surprise to a good many research people who tend to belittleteacher judgments; yet it must be realized that teachers of youngchildren soon become aware of differences among children with whomthey work in daily contact differences in alertness, in attentiveness, infollowing directions, in comprehension, and in other indicators of thechildren's cognitive preparedness for successful school performance.

Another surprise may be the loading of .57 of the Stanford-Binetwhich is high but moderate when compared with tile loadings ofteacher judgment and the Brenner Gestalt variables in our factor. Itwould seem that while the Stanford-Binet intelligence scale is mostuseful during the school years as a test of "general ability," the com-position of Factor A indicates (what other researchers felt for a longtime) that the Stanford-Binet test may not be particularly discriminativeof the abilities and assets in a child which are most important in meet-ing the Immediate demands of beginning school.

The interpretation of Factor A as a cognitive factor is supported bythe fact that 10 of its most highly loaded items come from the twoadministrations of the Brenner Gestalt Test which was designed tomeasure readiness for learning.

The basic assertion is that growth, development, and learningtake place trough constant interaction between an individual andhis environment. This leads to accumulation and differentiationwithin the person which increase his ability to perceive, to analyze,and to synthesize experiences both from within and from beyondhimself. These are the processes which lead to readiness. Ourhypothesis is: the more a child is able to perceive, to incorporateexperience into developing behavior, and to analyze and synthe-size into increased degrees of differentiation and specification, themore is he ready for school.

The five Brenner Gestalt subtests are built on the principle of per-sonality and Gestalt differentiation discussed in detail in "Reality Per-ception, Perceptual Differentiation and Readiness for School" (Brenner,1958); in "A New Gestalt Test for Measuring Readiness for School"(Brenner, 1959); and in the test manual (Brenner, 1964). They focuson the conceptual relevonce of perceptual development.

Perception is understood as sensory awareness of stimuli,external or internal, which the mind immediately organizes,interprets, and associates with existing concepts or transforms intonew ones. When one level of perceptior and conception is reachedin the transaction between individual and environment, it servesas a ntr basis for more objective and more discriminating percep-tion and conception, thereby sharpening perception and concep-tion in the on-going process of personality development, learning,and maturation. In development, perception and conception areintimately interrelated [Brenner, 1959, p. 27].

Page 15: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

16

This thinking warranted test information which called for perceptualand conceptual responses in the child. The tasks of the Gestalt testrequire space and form perception, comparisons of elements and totali-ties, recognition of similarities and differences, abilities to a'ialyze andsynthesize, perceptual motor skills, intellectual visualization, the appli-cation of number concepts, and abstraction and generalization abilities.

These are all perceptual and cognitive skills needed for effectivefunctioning in the primary school situation. The Brenner Gestalt Testmeasures

the new (intellectual) instruments which enable the child to bringorder and meaning into the world which surrounds him throughconstructive organization of his expanding life experiences. Theextent to which a child possesses qualities derived from theseclasses of develoling experience and uses them at any given levelin his transaction with reality becomes indicative of his readinessfor school [Brenner, 1959, p. 29].

The high factor loadings substantiate the correctness of the basicpremises upon which the test has been constructed.

Of the remaining highly loaded items in Factor A, three are subtestsof the Metropolitan Readiness Test (Variables 23, 24, 22); one comesfrom the Pintner-Cunningham Test (Variable 17); and one from theSangren Test (Variable 5). It is of interest that they all have a strongcomponent of "gestalting" in the double sense of both recognizing andforming Gestalten, an ability which also gave the Brenner Draw a Mansubtest its high loading of .77. Because of this all-pervading strongcomponent of gestalting in Factor A, we could very well have calledthis factor "cognitive gestalting."

Number perception and comprehension and with them abstractionability run also significantly through more than one half of the vari-ables, thus emphasizing the role of number work in school which alltoo often falls back behind the emphasis on reading readiness.

We learn from Directions for Administering this test (1949) what theMetropolitan Readiness Test is intended to measure: "Among the chieffactors that contribute to readiness for beginning school work arelinguistic attainments and aptitudes, visual and auditory perception,muscular coordination and motor skills, number knowledge, and theability to follow directions and to pay attention."

In terms of the intellectual abilities hypothesized by Guilford (1967),the "operations" required of the child in responding to the test itemsincluded in the descripition of Factor A are cognitive, memorative, andconvergently proch.ctive in nature. Recognizing numbers and words,remembering, understanding verbal instructions and carrying them out,copying words and designs, following directions, and the like are thetraditionally predominant activities in the elementary classroom. Theseare by and large the abilities that teachers readily observe in the chil-dren with whom they work. They are abilities and qualities which have

een acquired by the child through experience and learning within theLimits set by a child's biologically inherited potentiality in interaction

Page 16: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

17

with his environment. The result of this intera,tion is each child'sunique functional potentiality, his individual readiness.

Summary

Factor A represents the cognitive aspect of readiness, here predominantly seen a,. an ability factor resulting from the degree of developedpersonality differentiation and the child's experiences in perceptual.conceptual "gestalting." Having the highest total loadings of all sevenreadiness factors, Factor A ranks prominently as a readiness factor.Teacher judgment, the Brenner Gestalt Test, and subtests of the Metro.politan Readiness Test make appraising this type of readiness verypossible.

FACTOR B: Chronological Age

The second factor is obviously one of age. Among the 69 variablesincluded in the analysis were the chronological ages in months of thechildren when each test was administered, when physical measureswere taken, and when the teachers made their ratings. There were 11such age values for each child. The conditions of the testing programthe fact that the time intervals between tests tended to be the same orsimilar for all the children imposed high degrees of correlationamong these age values. Because of the very narrow age range in thechildren at the time of each test, the correlation between age and testscore was low. Thus, the highly intercorrelated age values constituteda "cluster" in the correlation matrix with relatively little relationshipwith the other variables. Consequently, the age variables were segre-gated in the factor analysis with one of the rotated axes passing directlythrough that far-out cluster. All of the factor loadings of the age vari-ables on this factor were very high, most of them above .90 with verylow loadings in other variables. (See Table 3.) Hence, the CA factor.

The clustering out of the CA factor is significant. It forces us to re-examine our position with regard to the role of CA, particularly in thelight of the academic devaluation of CA as it has developed over thelast years because of the emphasis on individual differences. Notwith-standing this emphasis on individual differences, it makes sense to giveweight to the role of CA in the readiness of a child. Common sense,observation, and research force upon us the notion that growth, matura-tion, development, and learning take place through time. The longera child has lived, the more he has had contact with reality and hasaccu-nulated knowledge and experiences. The longer he has lived, thegreater are the chances that he has developed or perfected his physicaland cognitive skills. The older the child, the more he will have devel-oped emotional security, independence, social responsibility, task orien-tation, and motivation to learn at least under normal conditionsand when we speak of the population in general. That there are indi-vidual differences and that in particular cases a younger child may havedeveloped more of these personality characteristics than an individualolder child is well known. But this fact is not incommensurable with thetruth of the general statement. More experiences; more knowledge;more skills; more emotional security, independence, and social respon-

Page 17: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

Fac

tor

Var

.In

put

Var

. #

TA

BL

E 3

Rot

ated

Fac

tors

- V

erim

ax S

olut

ion

3 4 5 6 7 8 910 11 12 13 14

15 16 17 18 19 20 2)

22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35

1 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35

Sex CA

at E

ntry

to K

inde

rgar

ten

CA

at S

ang'

en In

form

atio

n T

est

SA

Nat

ure

Stu

dyS

A N

umbe

rsS

A V

ocab

ular

yS

A S

ocia

l Inf

orm

atio

nS

A H

ouse

hold

Info

rmat

ion

SA

Lan

guag

e &

Lite

ratu

reC

A a

t Pin

tner

-Cun

ning

ham

Tes

tP

C C

omm

on O

bser

vatio

nP

C A

esth

etic

Diff

eren

ces

PC

Ass

ocia

ted

Obj

ects

PC

Siz

e D

iscr

imin

atio

nP

C P

ictu

re P

arts

PC

Pic

ture

Com

plet

ion

PC

Dot

Dra

win

gC

A a

t Met

ropo

litan

Rea

dine

ss T

est

ME

T W

ord

Mea

ning

ME

T S

ente

nces

ME

T In

form

atio

nM

ET

Mat

chin

gM

ET

Num

bers

ME

T C

opyi

ngC

A B

renn

er G

esta

lt T

est 1

1st A

dmin

is.)

B. N

umbe

r P

rodu

cing

B. N

umbe

r R

ecog

nitio

nB

. 10

Dot

Ges

talt

B. D

raw

a M

anB

. Sen

tenc

e C

opyi

ngC

A 2

nd B

renn

er G

esta

lt T

est

B. N

umbe

r P

rodu

cing

(2n

d)B

. Num

ber

Rec

ogni

tion

(2nd

)B

. 10

Dot

Ges

talt

(2nd

)B

. Dra

w a

Man

(2n

d)

AB

CD

k.I-

6.2

87.0

58.0

25.2

05.1

24.0

20.2

36.0

15-

.988

.017

.018

.013

.077

.026

.229

.553

.247

.189

347

.079

294

.305

.053

.212

.692

.147

.024

.089

.533

.128

.258

.408

.091

.046

.038

.313

.074

.166

.363

.193

.052

.14-

221

.142

.062

.692

.116

.028

006

.257

.209

.001

.669

.069

.058

.383

.058

.050

.554

.427

.110

.1;6

.135

.801

.115

- .0

17.1

86.!7

120

0.1

45-.

101

.366

.327

.127

.095

-.21

6.0

86.0

93.1

05.0

83.0

85.0

52-.

442

.367

.065

.157

.292

.156

-.01

8.2

98.3

73.1

10.0

40.1

38.1

01-.

052

.440

.453

.176

.204

.241

.092

.005

.424

.329

.129

.298

.244

147

.090

-.13

2.5

38.1

48.1

16.2

79.0

06.0

67-

.048

.015

.978

.044

.050

.050

.0'4

.022

.389

.023

.066

.375

.213

.142

.188

.425

.009

.114

.326

.177

.053

.045

.155

.114

- .2

23.3

73.1

70-.

032

.401

.553

.032

.162

.162

.015

.082

.089

.651

-.08

9.1

38.3

1924

6.1

11.0

62.6

12.0

05.3

50.1

54.1

75.0

28-.

176

-.02

7-.

979

.154

.087

.198

-.01

0.0

45.6

22-

.225

.163

.133

.086

-.02

8.2

36.6

03-.

103

.095

.299

-.01

6.1

07.1

79.5

27-.

119

.125

.039

.338

.032

- .3

61.5

37-.

057

.277

.111

.343

.102

.295

.644

-.15

7.1

78.0

23.1

28.1

04-.

069

.072

.916

.103

- .0

36-.

051

-.09

0.1

51.6

34-.

057

.035

.201

-.26

9-.

127

.433

.660

.095

.145

.380

-.06

1-.

142

.240

.597

.211

.076

-.26

9.1

55.1

25-.

102

.768

-.04

6.1

21-.

020

.094

-.01

6-.

002

- CO

Page 18: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

Fact

orV

ar. #

36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69

Inpu

tV

ar. #

36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69

TA

BL

E 3

(C

ont.)

Rot

ated

Fac

tors

- V

erim

ax S

olut

ion

B. S

ente

nce

Cop

ying

(2n

d)C

A M

onro

e R

eadi

ng A

ptitu

de T

est

MO

Vis

ual P

erce

ptio

nM

O A

udito

ry P

erce

ptio

nM

O M

otor

Per

cept

ion

MO

Art

icul

atio

nM

O L

angu

age

CA

Mon

roe

New

Bas

ic R

eadi

ng T

est

NB

R S

ente

nce

Mea

ning

NB

R S

enso

ry I

mag

ery

NB

R R

elat

ions

hips

NB

R E

mot

iona

l Rea

ctio

n &

Mot

ives

NB

R V

isua

l Scr

utin

yN

BR

Pho

netic

Ana

lysi

sN

BR

Str

uctu

ral A

naly

sis

Cis

Sta

nfor

d-B

inet

MA

Sta

nfor

d-B

inet

IQ S

tanf

ord-

Bin

etC

A P

hysi

cal E

xam

inat

ion

Skel

etal

Age

Hei

ght

Wei

ght

Vis

ion

Num

ber

of P

erm

anen

t Tee

th E

rupt

edD

evel

opm

enta

l Lev

el -

Wet

zel

CA

Tea

cher

Jud

gmen

tT

J of

Ach

ieve

men

tT

i of

Abi

lity

TJ

of M

atur

itySk

elet

al A

ge a

t Hor

mon

e St

udy

Gon

adot

ropi

ns17

-Ket

oste

roid

sI7

-Hyd

ro x

ycor

ticos

tero

ids

Cre

atin

ine

AB

CD

EF

.745

.098

.066

.060

.147

A14

.225

.021

.980

.003

.079

.043

055

.044

.418

.131

.205

.363

.25u

- .0

81.1

19.3

11.0

42.0

81.1

79.3

03.1

60.1

66.4

22.0

16.1

91.1

04.0

14.0

13-.

051

.378

.141

.271

.289

.140

.026

304

.231

.070

.075

.440

.094

.082

.16U

-.00

7-.

943

-.08

1.0

05.0

91.0

8208

0.3

14.0

10-.

091

.190

-.20

7.0

51-.

303

.180

-.02

0.8

62.0

35-.

173

.016

-.0%

.175

.053

.833

-.03

6.0

77.0

34.0

04.0

94.0

03.7

90.0

49.1

18.0

63.0

86.1

64.0

48.8

46.0

79-.

275

.010

-.01

3.4

41.0

90.4

39.1

39-

.048

.088

.338

.304

.089

.574

.115

-.26

0.1

47.0

18.0

25-.

497

-.32

3.1

35.4

54.0

63-.

080

.435

- -.

195

-.24

0.4

34.2

94.0

13-.

219

.567

.200

-.02

6.4

56.0

17-.

043

-.19

9.0

49-.

855

-.01

2-.

019

-.06

4-

.155

-.01

2.0

45-.

296

-.12

_6-.

027

.187

-.60

8.2

16.0

45-.

198

.043

-.02

9.1

07.7

44.0

61.0

35-.

176

.073

-.00

8.1

93-.

854

.016

-.00

5.0

31.4

10.2

58.1

03.0

05.0

35.1

20-.

157

.003

.202

.156

-.32

8.1

79.1

01-.

209

.007

-.04

4.0

93.8

79.1

42.0

59-.

893

-.22

5.0

40.0

79.0

36.1

81.7

20.0

54.1

28.1

24.0

93-

.042

-.28

0.8

20.0

73.1

11.1

91.0

58.1

13-.

098

.770

.010

.113

-1%

.070

-.05

7.2

08.0

3!-.

115

-.36

6.0

03.8

70-.

264

.041

.077

-.06

5-

223

.079

.624

-.13

5.2

70.0

72-.

183

- -.

413

.251

.636

-.37

6.0

53-.

040

-.32

3-.

234

.244

.320

-.41

7.0

63.0

72.1

58-.

250

.273

.601

-.32

3.1

0310

.811

9.25

55.

409

4,90

74.

131

3.43

62.

742

Page 19: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

21

sibility; more task orientation and greater motivation to learn theseall contribute to the ci Ad's ability to cope with the reality of schooldemands. These all mean more readiness for school.

We will further elaborate and critically examine this problem in the"Discussion" section of this analysis. Our attempt here was to empha-size the fact that our factor analysis has revealed an Age Factor signifi-cant in readiness; and to discuss briefly the significance of this fact.

FACTOR C: Reading Readiness

Factor C has its highest loadings in a cluster of variables which aresubtests of the Monroe New Basic Reading Test. This cluster gives thefactor its basic character. The total series of Monroe Basic Reading Testshas been designed to measure the extent to which pupils in the earlystages of learning to read have progressed in various aspects of readingfrom prereading skills through a readiness and basic reading program.In keeping with the intent behind the new basic reading tests, we shallcall Factor C our reading readiness factor. Already a look at the relevantvariables and their loadings should justify this designation.

FactorNo. Variable Loading45 sensory imagery .8648 visual scrutiny .8546 perceiving relationships .8347 recognizing emotional reactions and motives .7950 structural analysis .5749 phonetic analysis .44

Moving on to an interpretive analysis we will draw heavily, but notexclusively, on Monroe herself and her own interpretation of the ideasmeasured in these tests.

As Monroe points out in her manual (no date), each item stressesand therefore tests the child's ability in an important aspect of reading.Above all, a child must be able to "live the story." In order to be ableto live the story, he needs to create the vivid sensory imagery thattransports him mentally into the setting (Variable 45). The more he canproject himself into the text and can form mental images of sight,sound, touch, smell, and taste of the persons, objects, and actionsdescribed in reading matters; the more meaningful the text becomesand the more it will help him in linking events and remembering them.It is this ability to form mental images while reading that makes read-ing a living experience. The high factor loadings of this aspect of readi-ness should therefore not come as too great a surprise.

Equally important for the understanding of a text is the ability toperceive relationships (Variable 46). As things happen in time andspace, meaningful reading requires that the child see ideas and eventsin their relatedness to other ideas and events; that he link time inproper sequence; that he be perceptive of chronology of events,actions, and characters as they relate to the present, the past, and thefuture. The child must be able to associate time with the right persons

Page 20: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

22

and places, see the logic or its absence in what happens. In short, thechild must be able to see things in their functional, logical time space,and cause and effect relationship. The more he can perceive concreteand abstract relationships and can organize his thinking in terms of theabove categories, the better will he understand and the more will hisnew reading become associated with and incorporated into his past andpresent thinking.

No proper interpretation and no understanding is possible if we arenot able to feel what other people feel and if we do not understandthe drives and motives behind their actions. Thus, recognizing emo-tional reactions, inner drives, and motives (Variable 47) becomes thethird basic interpretative ability and skill in reading readiness that isneeded for making reading a living experience. To quote Monroe her-self: "When pupils identify themselves with a character in the storyfeel the character's gladness or sadness, understand what kind of personhe is and why he reacts as he does in a given situation they are reallyliving the story."

So far, we have emphasized the need for and the value in readingreadiness of three interpretative skills: the child's ability to form sensoryimages, to perceive relationships, and to recognize emotional reactionsand motives behind people's behavior. These interpretative skills, how-ever, cannot function unless the child has developed word perceptionskills. In order to comprehend and interpret the printed message thechild must have first learned to perceive the printed symbol accurately.Visual scrutiny, Variable 48, is the prerequisite for any interpretationand understanding. The child must be able to differentiate betweenwords that look alike and yet are different; for instance, hand-hard;word-would. He must be able to see similarities and differences inform and Gestalt of letters and words; only then will he be able todifferentiate their meaning.

Unless the child can accurately scrutinize the printed word he will alsohave difficulties associating each printed word with its particular soundand meaning. In reading, correct phonetic analysis, Variable 49, de-pends first of all on correct visual perception and analysis; but then thesound of printed words, pronunciation, and phonetic analysis may helpthe child in cases where his visual memory defaulted him or when heencounters an unknown word which represents a spoken word that isfamiliar to him. We also know that people differ in their learning styles.Some learn predominantly through visual, other through auditory path-s.-'ays of perception. Thus, apart from deriving pronunciation from it,phonetic analysis becomes especially important for an auditory learningtype, an idea which has never been mentioned by Monroe.

There is another kind of perception and analysis which is essential tomeaning and understanding, and that is structural analysis, Variable 50.Monrce says: "Structural analysis is essentially a visual process thatinvolves the scrutiny of words for root, ending, prefix or suffix." Struc-tural analysis is a highly complex process as is suggested in part byanother statement by Monroe: "Basic to this ability to scrutinize a givenword form is a sensitivity for the sound, meaning and usage of inflect-ed, derived and compounded forms in total language [Manual]."

Page 21: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

23

Phonetic and structural analysis are of great help when the childencounters unknown words. Much of the success, enjoyment, andchallenge in reading can come from an enjoyment and skill in phoneticand especially structural analysis.

We cannot close this section without mentioning the extremely lowfactor loading of Variable 44, "sentence meaning." It is the first subtestin Monroe's New Basic Reading Test and represents according to Mon-roe another interpretative skill. We do not know why this variable, soessential in reading, came out with an insignificant loading. Our datado not allow a satisfactory answer.

Summary

Factor C is our reading readiness factor. It deals with three (four)abilities of the child that are most needed for interpretation and under-standing of reading material and it also includes the three word per.ception skills that are prerequisite for meaning and understanding inthe reading and listening process.

FACTOR D: Body of Knowledge

Factor D represents another aspect of readiness which is the productof past experience and learning. It is the child's accumulation and or-ganization of knowledge. It represents the "cognitive structure" withwhich the child begins his school experience and which grows withfurther experience through the constant process of "subsumption"(Ausubel, 1965).

The 14 test items whose intercorrelations brought them together as afactor are given below. As one can readily see, this is a "SangrenFactor;" that is, the Sangren Information Test supported by relateddimensions gives this factor its "Gepraege." In other words, it gives itdefinition and its specific character.

FactorNo. Variable Loading

4 Sangren nature study .697 Sangren social information .698 Sangren household knowledge .679 Sangren language and literature .55

53 Stanford-Binet IQ .4642 Monroe language .4452 Stanford-Binet mental age .43

5 Sangren number comprehension .4133 Brenner number recognition .3819 Metropolitan word meaning .3839 Monroe auditory perception .3821 Metropolitan information .3738 Monroe visual perception .36

6 Sangren vocabulary .36

Traditionally, children go to school to learn, to acquire knowlekaand skills and thus develop their intellect. From the point of view of

Page 22: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

24

the cognitive learning theorist, this development (learning) comesabout through the assimilation of new information, knowledge, andskills into the existing "cognitive structure," the organized backlog ofalready acquired knowledge, concepts, and skills, In the words ofBruner (1968):

Growth depends upon internalizing events into a "storage sys-tem" that corresponds to the environment. It is this system thatmakes possible the child's increasing ability to go beyond the in-formation encountered on a single occasion. He does his bymaking predictions and extrapolations from his stored model ofthe world [p.

Inspection of the 14 highest loaded component items of Factorshows that they are either subtests of information indicating the amountand diversity of knowledge and concepts acquired by the child hisinformation about nature, social information, household knowledge,language and literature, general vocabulary. his understanding of themeaning of words; or, they give evidence of the child's mastery of themost important instruments needed for the acquisition of this knowl-edge and thus the mastery of the world around him auditory per-ception skills, visual perception, number comprehension, intelligence,and the ability to abstract and to generalize.

The accumulation of such a large and diversified body of knowledgeand the availability of such powerful instruments with which informa-tion is pursued, processed, and organized do, indeed, in their combi-nation represent readiness as a state of being and as potentiality forfuture successful learning in school.

A close relative to Factor D, if not a family member, is Factor Gwhich we will therefore discuss next, to be followed by Factors F andE which represent biophysical and biochemical aspects of readiness.

FACTOR G: Perceptual Differentiation

Perceptual differentiation is suggested as a label for this aspect ofschool readiness. The following tabulation shows the nine variableswith significant loadings.

No. VariableFactor

Loading12 P-C Aesthetic Differences .4414 P-C Size Discrimination .4432 Brenner Number Producing .4315 P-C Picture Parts .4221 Metropolitan Information .4028 Brenner 10 Dot Gestalt .3649 Monroe Phonetic Analysis .3441 Monroe Articulation .3044 Monroe Sentence Meaning .30

In essence, content and meaning of this factor are intimately relatedto what has been said in the discussion of Factors A, C, and D. We dealhere again with perceptual-conceptual skills and the child's knowledge,

Page 23: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

25/24

with his abilities to analyze and synthesize, his ability to perceive sizeand form in space, to differentiate among visual and auditory elements,to associate objects, and to find meaning in what he perceives. All theseare faculties upon which much of the child's success in kindergarten andfirst grade depends, particularly in reading, spelling, and in workingwith numbers.

None of the component variables in Factor G has a loading suffi-ciently high to be clearly definitive of factor meaning by itself; how-ever, all of the items with loadings of .30 and above are congruentwith and contribute to the idea of perceptual-conceptual differentiation.The fact that these variables have been sifted out again, apart fromtheir role in Factors A, C, and D, gives added emphasis to the impor-tance of perceptual-cognitive skills and the role of Gestalt in preschoollearning and readiness.

FACTOR F: Physical Development Status'

This factor clearly represents physical developmental status in chil-dren. We have five variables to support this contention:

FactorNo. Variable Loading60 Developmental Level (Wetzel) .8857 Weight .8556 Height .7455 Skeletal Age .6159 Number of Permanent Teeth .33

It is a belief of old standing that the child's physical status is impor-tant in readiness to enter school and to meet the demands of school. Thecommon man, especially in farming areas, paid this factor always hisparticular attention. To be big, strong, and physically well developedwas often times taken as the sole criterion of readiness, thus givingphysique an undue weight over the importance of other factors inreadiness. The medical profession, of course, including the pediatrician,the school physician, the endocrinologist, the ophthalmologist, theorthodontist, is primarily concerned with the child's physical develop-mental status, and psychologists would be well advised to heed psycho-somatic relatedness of structure and function. Factor F may serve as areminder of this need.

The above five variables are indexes of physical growth status. "De-velopmental level" is a measure of physique which is determined byplotting height against weight on the Wetzel Grid (Wetzel, 1941).Skeletal age as assessed from x-ray plates of the hand and wrist bonesis by many people considered to be the most reliable single index ofgeneral physical maturation, but it should be used only in conjunctionwith chronological age as should also "developmental level." The

2Special thanks are due to Dr. R Mellinger for his ass stance in the preparation of sections FactorF and Factor E. For a more cl.ricled and specific analysis of our endocrinological material werefer to his article. between tediacr.ne and Physical Measures of Maturity duringGrowth" in Pyle, Waterhouse, and Greulich (1970). This same Standard of Reference book bringsalso Dr. J. A. Johnston's and Dr Gordon Manson's views on development and readiness researchfrom a pediatrician's point of view.

Page 24: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

27

importance of this developmental status factor as an aspect of schoolreadiness inheres in its relation to health. A child who is not up to parin developmental status is not likely to be a healthy child, and withoutgood physical health he is likely to fall short in meeting the demands ofschool. Concerning this relationship between growth status and health,Wetzel (1941) says:

"Noll the rl.mcal point of vew os auron the that a c 11.1d who fa,h, to growproperly ,s not healthly and that such a ch Id accordingly should become subjectto med.ral exan.,nahon . . The main obiect,ve of all organized health work if tofaoldate growth and development whether ch.% 1 snee,tically staled or not. Thereis CA, h of these endeavors in uorn.sielk able 1171phcahon that deviations In growthand development are wpm:Lied with changes in health, and that they constituteinitial signs of an otherwise unsuspected disease I p. 11801."

Wetzel points out that "development level" along with chronologi alage in the child can approximately be regarded as an indicator ofgrowth status and of "physical fitness." Physical fitness mos, su elyplays an important role in readiness for school entrance and successfullearning in school. Large differences in developmental le.els devia-tions from proper channels on the Wetzel Grid may leyitimately beregarded as representing differences if not problems in physical fitness,for instance the obese or the overly thin, undernourished ch 'd.

Apart from its significance from the standpoint o' physical fitness,this factor of physical status may also be a factor of advantage from apsychosocial point of view. A child large of structure and with theattributes of good health and physique is likely to b . reacted to byadults and his peers alke in ways which will give hint personal statusin the classroom. This would constitute an immediate advantage inrelating and adjusting o the new school situation and in functioningin it adequately.

It is interesting, though somewhat to be expected, that the numberof permanent teeth erupted became a variable in this factor of physicalstatus. Some students of anatomy consider teeth as part of the cutan-eous system; others are of the opinion that they can be properly con-sidered part of the skeletal system. Thus teething would be also an ex-pression of skeletal maturation. As the senior author of this study ob-served in his professional contacts with the Waldorf Schools years ago,the eruption of permanent teeth mostly around the age of six wasused as a basic criterion for total school readiness. This notion is part oftheir particular theosophical beliefs. It is thinking in terms of psycho-somatics, of the interrelatedness of body and mind.

Three other variables in Factor F deserve our attention. They arerelated to the endoctrine system and thus to the biochemistry of thebody. The three items listed below have loadings v,hich, though rela-tively low compared with other factor loadings, are of sufficient magni-tude to be included in this discussion along with the five aforemen-tioned physical variables.

FactorNo. Variable Loading68 17-Hydroxycorticosteroids .4267 17-Ketosteroids .3869 Creatinine .32

Page 25: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

28

The 17hydroxycorticosteroids and the 17ketosteroids are metabolites of secreted adrenal hormones. These hormones are major factorscontrolling anabolism and catabolism; accordingly, they are significantin growth and development of body moss. Creatinine is an index oflean body mass, that is. fatfree tissues. It may be expected to correlatewith the adrenal steroids which significantly affect physical growth anddevelopment and probably also mental development in the process ofmaturation.

Other endocrine factors could well have been included in our study.Thyroid and pituitary secretions merit attention. However, adrenocortical functions undergo qualitative modifications during preschool andschool years, whereas the other endocrine functions are stable andrelatively unchanging in this age group. !n the older child the gottado-tropic secretion and resulting gonadal function assume importance. Forour resesarch population the changing adrenal secretion is consideredto be the most likely significant variable. Herein lies a justification forthe inclusion of these steroid analyses in our study.

Summary

Surveying all of the eight variables which characterize our growthstatus factor, we find it constituted by five bioohysical determinants andthree biochemical determinants. Although the loadings of the latter arelower than those of the first group, they are significant. Their signifi-cance is further demonstrated in their loadings in Factor E which wedesignate as our biochemical factor. We are giving them a second lookin the following discussion of Factor E.

FACTOR E: Biochemical Maturity Factor

The factors of function:s1 readiness so far discussed refer mostly 'othe beginnings and early periods of school adjustment and learning.Factor E relates to processes of development and function which areassociated with and are aspects of the pervasive complex of physical,physiological, and psych,- ogical changes which characterize child de-velopment and, in the case of the gonadotropins, the puberal period.The items which constitute the core of this factor are bicchemical innature. They represent some of the changes in physiological functioningwhich prcmote body growth and maturation and thus prepare the individual for higher level personal functioning physically and cognitively.The highly loaded factor variables are:

No. VariableFactor

Loading65 Skeletal age at time of hormone study .8767 17-Ketosteroids .6466 Gonadotropins .6269 Creatinine .60

9 Sangren Language Subtest .43

At the time urine specimens were taken, the children's skeletal maturation status was asses:.ed. As stated earlier, skeletal maturity level, as a

Page 26: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

29

single measure, is perhaps the best indicator of the developmentalstatus of the organism as a whole. There is an especially close relation-ship between skeletal maturation and the development of the repro-ductive system. Note the very high loading of skeletal age, .87 in thisfactor. "So intimate is the cor espondence between the maturationalchanges in the reproductive and in the skeletal systems that it is pos-sible to pi edict when the menarche will occur from the assessment ofa radiogram of the hand and wrist during the prepuberal period[Greulich & Pyle, 1959]." Thus, the degree of development of theskeletal system in its tie-up with sexual development becomes a reliableindicator of the level of general body development which would in-clude the brain and the nervous system.

Since metabolism generally is controlled by the endocrine glandularsystem, hormone levels correlate with growth, development, and thefunctional state of the body. Puberal sexual changes and the associatedspurt of growth are set in motion by release of pituitary gonai.totropichormones which stimulate secretion of estrogens and androgens fromthe gonads. In the prepuberal years, the anabolic steroids are contrib-uted by the adrenal cortex which is under the control of pituitaryadrenocorticotropin. Throughout childhood, adrenal androgens are se-creted in gradually increas;ng quantity. At puberty, testicular androgengreatly augments this secretion and produces the male secondary sexcharacteristics. It is likely that in females the adrenal androgens ratherthan gonadal secretion produce the adolescent growth spurt, whileestrogens account for sox characteristics.

All androgens are excreted as 17ketosteroids. Thus, growth andmaturational effects of the anabolic hormones can be estimated fromthe 17ketosteroid assay This determinant has a loading of .64 inFactor E. Thu gonadotroic hormone factor which augments anabolichormone secretion and sparks the physical changes of puberty is loaded.62. Creatinine, a by-product of creatine metabolism in muscle tissues,is an indicator of total lean body mass which obviously depends onphysical growth, proceeding under the controlling influence of thehormones. Creatinine is loaded .60.

During the period of puberal changes organismic development gen-erally is rapid and pervasive. Psychological correlates of the structuralchanges have also been noted. According to Piaget (Inhelder, 1958),for example, it is approximately the age of 11 years, the time of theonset of puberty, which marks the beginning of the "formal operations"in the child's cognitive development. It is during 's period that he canbegin to think in purely abstract terms, conceptualize, and reason logi-cally. The intellectual skills involved in inductive and deductive reason-ing and in handling the calculus of proportions become available tohim. In general, the child at this time becomes able to function men-tally in terms of symbolic representations and abstractions at levelspreviously impossible to him.

Thus the measured levels and changes in the biochemical products ofmetabolism (Variables 65, 66, 67, and 69) signal changes in patterns ofphysiological functioning and in rate of organismic maturation. In view

Page 27: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

30

of the observed changes in levels of cognitive functioning which arepresumed to accompany the stepped-up biological maturation of thepuberal period, this factor may tentatively be regarded as representingthe structure and the processes which make the child ready for higherlevels of academic learning than he is capable of as a prepuberal child.

The loadings of .43 for the Sangren language subtest and lesserloadings in other tests of mental functioning that were fed into FactorE reflect to some degree the assumed relationship between structureand function.

DISCUSSION

The Changed Readiness Concept

The burniiig questions have always been: How does a child arrive atreadiness? Can readiness be produced? Or is readiness the result ofmaturational factors which by definition must follow their own intrinsiclaws of growth and cannot be speeded up?

Scientifically the concept of readiness is intimately related to the oldnature-nurture issue. The thinking on readiness will depend on thestand which one takes in this controversy. The final practical questionthat arises here for parents and teachers is whether or not interventionis possible, and if it is possible, whether or not it is desirable.

Around 1950 and in the early '50's when our research project wasstarted, the position generally taken was that school readiness is a

matter of maturation. Readiness cannot be produced. The developmentof readiness follows an organismic law of growth which you cannotrepeal, get around, overlook, or cheat on. Lack of recognition of thislaw means that the youngster is doomed to fail. Children inherit theirrate of growth. The speed at which they may mature is bred into them."The way you treat a child may lower his capabilities; you can makehim function on only three of his four cylinders or on seven of hiseight, but you cannot build an extra cylinder into him," said James 1.Hymes, Jr. (1955) whose words became the education bible for nurseryschool-kindergarten teachers as much a:. Dr. Spock's advice about infantcare and child-rearing became the bible for young parents. No effortmust be made to intervene. "The time will come when a child is ready.""When you high-pressure a child ... you hurt the child." Growth is theboss. Time is its worker. \You have to be patient. You ha ,e to wait untilthe child is ready. We caMbe sure that his inner wheels are turning evenif we don't see them: his intellectual wheels, social, emotional, memory,reasoning, and attention span wheels. We must hold our horses. Wemust have faith in this law of maturation. "Remember," says Hymes,"growth builds readiness . . . you don't [p. 88]."

Times have changed. The Zeitgeist has changed and with it thereadiness concept has changed (Brenner, 1966, 1967). In the sixties itis believed that readiness can be produced. Present-day thinking takesup again ideas which began to take shape in the 1930's when the IowaChild Welfare Research Station published its findings about theinfluence of various environmental settings on the child (Wellman,

Page 28: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

31

1934, 1937; Wellman & Coffey, 1936; Skeels et al., 1937; Skodak,1939). The contentions of these various researchers were far fromuniversally accepted. Out of the nature-nurture controversy which fol-lowed (Goodenough, 1940) emerged two lines of thought: one con-tinued to emphasize the effect of environment on child development,and another emphasized maturation. The latter movement became byfar the more dominant and in educational-psychological practice theprevailing movement stressing the idea that school readiness is a matterof maturation with which one should not "tamper" (Hymes, 1955,19.58; Jersild, 1946).

For some years now there has developed an upsurge of new interestin the facts of stimulation appropriate to the child's age, particularlyresearch in sensory and cultural deprivation and in cognition andperception of children in very early years. New research into the struc-ture of the intellect, into stages of human development and learning,has further contributed to the new enthusiasm about the possibilitiesof environmental stimulation, be it through general enrichment or spe-cific stimulation and tutoring of the child.

This research was bound to lead to a "new"concept of readiness, toa rather extreme environmentalist point of view. The tendency now isto neglect the maturational factor in readiness. It is now argued thatchildren can learn earlier, faster, and more than we used to think.Readiness is not a matter of maturation, time, and patient waiting(Tyler, no date). It can and must be produced. This is the duty of theparent, the teacher, the school administration, the community, the na-tion. The pressure is on. If a child is not ready for school, it is rarely, ifever, the "fault" of the child, but rather of the parent, the teacher, andall those who are responsible for the child's early development of hispotential and skills (Deutsch, 1963, 1964; Gordon, 1946).

The senior author's conceptual model of school readiness providesfor both points of view and allows for a proper balance between anexclusive biological-maturational and an exclusive environmentalistposition. The research reported in this study indicates that certainfacets of readiness for school are largely "biological" in nature, andthese presumably follow a biological time table. On the other hand,certain other aspects of general readiness are more clearly products ofearlier learning from experience. There is no aspect of readiness,though, which would not be the result of the interaction of an indi-vidual's hereditary potential with environmental forces.

We want to address ourselves, therefore, to the need for a balancedview of the role of the genetic makeup of a child and the ensuingmaturational factors on the one hand, and the need for environmentalstimulation and earlier life experiences on the other hand. Neglect ofor overemphasis on either the "nature" or "nurture" part of readinessis unrealistic and must lead to false hopes on the part of adults and toless than optimal readiness in the individual child. A "wait and bepatient" maturational attitude tends to underestimate a child's potentialand capacities for early learning. It does not utilize enough new dis-coveries in research and the possibilities which lie in environmental

Page 29: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

32

41,

=,1:400 A.14 If414411110

.1.104

-001Mr.

stimulation for inducing readiness. The belief that readiness can andmust be produced, almost under any circumstance, through environ-mental stimulation overlooks the fact that low genetic potential setslimits to the effectiveness of environmental manipulation. It also tendsto ignore differential maturational forces at work in various individuals,thus perhaps pushing a child too hard and demanding too much at aninappropriate time, which in the long run may create more unreadinessthan readiness. More points of discussion and critical evaluation canbe found in "Readiness for School and Today's Pressures" (Brenner,1967).

Page 30: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

33

Sex Differences

In 1965, Ilg and Ames published their book on "School Readiness."They report that . .. "regardless of test used and regardless of age ofsubjects, for nearly every test at every age, it is the scores of girls whichare superior and which show greater maturity of response [35, p.364]."

Much of the research literature and most textbooks in child develop-ment and learning favor girls over boys. It has become a stereotyperepeated over and over again that girls mature earlier and are clearlyahead of boys in readiness and achievements of all sorts before school,at entrance to school, and in subsequent years. Teachers in generalbelieve it and many researchers tell them that this is so. Such thinkinghas led to the suggestion that girls should enter school a year or atleast half a year earlier than boys because of their advanced maturityand accomplishments, or boys should enter later. Our factor analysisdoes not bear out these claims and conclusions. Our Variable 1 is thesex \ ariable. Table 3 shows that in none of the seven Factors has thesex variable a high or even moderate loading. The highest is one of .29in Factor A which is lower than any other coefficient used in our study.In preparing the data for computer card punching, sex was coded 1 forboys and 2 for girls. The positive.factor loading .29 thug reflcrts a slightoverall superiority of .irls in "cognitive readiness."

If we move away from this global result to a breakdown of sexdifferences in mean scores on the various subtests among the 69 vari-ables, we find:

At Kindergarten Level

No significant difference in all six subtests of the Sangren Information Test.

No significant difference in all six subtests of the Metropolitan Readiness Test.

No significant difference in five subtests: 1, 2, 3, 6, 7, of the PintnerCunninghamTest.

No significant difference in number producing and number recognition on the BrennerGestalt Test, first administration.

No significant difference in number producing, number recognition, and sentencecopying on the Brenner Gestalt Test, second administration.

At First Grade Level

No significant difference in four subtests: 1, 2, 4, 5, of the Monroe Reading Aptitude Test.

No signicant difference in six subtests: 1, 3, 4, 5, 6, 7, of the Monroe New BaskReading Test.

To sum up: 32 variables show no significant sex differences; 22 at kindergarten leveland 10 at first grade level.

Analysis of other variables reveals a consistent trend of superiorityof the girls over boys. The variables and their levels of significance ofsex differences favoring the girls are:

Page 31: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

34

Variable No.Level of

Variable Name Significance(p=value)

At Kindergarten level14 Pintner-Cunningham Discrimination of Size .0115 Pintner-Cunningham Picture Parts .0228 Brenner 10 Dot Gestalt, 1st administration .0534 Brenner 10 Dot Gestalt, 2nd administration .0529 Brenner Draw a Man, 1st administration .0135 Brenner Draw a Man, 2nd administration .00130 Brenner Sentence Copying .0562 Teacher Judgment Achievement .00163 Teacher Judgment Ability .0164 Teacher Judgment Maturity .01

At First Grade Level40 Monroe Reading Apt. Motor perception .0145 Monroe New Basic Reading Sensory imagery .05

All in all: 12 variables, namely nine subtests and three teacherjudgments of achievement, ability, and maturity show statistically sig-nificant differences in favor of the girls. Seven of the nine subtestsfavoring girls over boys were kindergarten level achievements, whileonly two refer to first grade achievement.

Evaluating now the total result of our analysis of sex differences: 32variables showing no significant difference versus 12 variables showingsignificant differences, we come to the conclusion that in our researchpopulation there is indeed a consistent trend of superiority of girlsover boys in about 'a of the specified measured abilities and per-formances; there was also a statistically insignificant difference in meanStanford-Binet IQ of 3 points (117.42 for girls, 114.56 for boys) in favorof the girls. But in % of the specified measured abilities and perform-ances there was no significant sex difference.

Our data then do not confirm the generalized findings about thescholastic superiority of girls as pointed out so often, in particular byF. R. Pauley (1951) who studied sex differences in the Tulsa schools wayback in the thirties and forties, and by llg and Ames in the 1965 GesellInstitute studies of school readiness. It is of interest to note that theabuve quoted findings from Ilg and Ames are hard to reconcile withtheir many differing findings in their own tables and specifically withtheir own observations in a most important area of readiness andlearning, namely number readiness. They found that "the manualexecution of numbers is better in girls, but the concept of numbers. . . is higher in boys [p. 59]." We believe that the latter accomplish-ment is by far the more relevant one. If true, it would speak clearlyfor greater cognitive maturity of boys rather than girls in the field ofnumber work and thus contradict the authors' claim for greater over-all maturity response in girls.

It is generally agreed that girls are usually advanced in the matura-tion of the skeletal-muscular system and therefore in various kinds ofmanipulatory and visual skills. It is also generally agreed that more

Page 32: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

35

girls learn earlier to read than boys. That boys may do better in numberwork has not found enough attention. It is cf crucial importance in thestudy of sex differences to move away from large statistical averagesand similarities and to specify sex differences with regard to specifictask performances as we have done it in this section. This enables usto be particular about tasks in which boys are superior and about othertasks in which girls are superior. Both Pau ley and the Gesell Institutereport such specific research findings, yet it seems to us that they didnot avoid the danger of falsifying the correct specific findings throughthe very process of broad and thus faulty generalizations. But we haveto go still further in spe:ifications.

Differences in responsiveness and action patterns between boys andgirls may vary with regard to age, interest, initiation, involvement, mo-tivation, social class, cultural dispositions, sex appropriateness of tasks,and styles of c&egorization (Bruner, 1966). There seem to be sex differ-ences with regard to anxiety. Teaching methods can make a difference:Is the method one of problem solving and risk taking? Then boys maydo better than girls who seem to achieve better where traditionallearing-obeying-conforming attitudes are required. There seem to besex differences in teacher-planned activities vs. teacher-pupil-plannedactivities and even within these two approaches there may be differ-ences if it comes to various subjects, meanings, skills, and drills (Shab,1967). Permissiveness or authoritarian teaching styles can make a differ-ence. Then there is the problem of identification, of "masculinity" or"femininity" in the classroom. Boys find it often difficult to identifywith the female teachers who so greatly outnumber the male teachers;there may be outright resentment or rejection with subsequent lack ofcontact and performance, if not discipline problems dynamic inter-action patterns which tend to produce lower ratings by the teacher andthus mask actual abilities in boys.

On the basis of the findings of our factor analysis and considerationslike these we would not be able to support the idea that girls shouldbegin school a year or half a year earlier than boys. There is too muchvariance in sex differences, explored, and not yet sufficiently explored,to warrant the global assumptions underlying such a recommendation.Refering to Oetzel's bibliography on sex differences, Sigel (1963) re-ports that "findings of approximately one hundred ninety-six studiesshow that boys and girls differ consistently from each other in relativelyfew areas."

Much more, and more refined, psychological as well as statisticalresearch is needed to settle the problem of sex difference in readinessand learning.

The Role of Chronological Age

We know from observation and research that five- or six-year-oldchildren may show a range of 3 to 5 years in mental age; that is, somefive- or six-year-old children may have a mental age of 4 or only 3;others may have a mental age of 7 or 8. We have found the same rangeof differences in skeletal age among our five- or six-year-olds. Seen

Page 33: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

36

from this point of view, chronological age is not a good indicator ofschool readiness. Yet our factor analysis has turned out chronologicalage as one of the 7 Factors in readiness. Is there then a rationale fordefending chronological age as a factor in readiness? Yes, there is.

The importance of chronological age as a basic factor contributing toreadiness must be emphasized because psychological literature with itsconstant emphasis on individual differences and different rates ofgrowth and development traditionally tends to devalue chronologicalage. In fact, psychological academia has been fighting chronologicalage as the universal indicator of developmental status for years, feelingquite superior to the "laymanish" administrative practice of admittingchildren to school on the basis of chronologicial age. There can be nodoubt, though, that this practice has developed not only as an admin-istrative convenience but because it is based on common observationand exper:once on a very large scale which tells us that by and largethe five-year-old is ready for kindergarten and the six-year-old is readyfor first grade in the United States as well as in other parts of the world.These biological-psychological generalities do not only exist as realitieswhich we have to face, but they have been given scientific value in theconstruction of intelligence tests where individual differences are mea-sured against the general intellectual levels of expected performancein the testing of two-year-olds, three-year-olds, four-year-olds, and allthe way up to adult levels. Well-known achievement tests also assignscientific and practical value to chronological age; for instance, theStanford Achievement Tests developed by Kelley, Ruch, and Terman.These tests use "Age and Grade Norms" and "Age Equivalents;" theyassociate low chronological age with a given grade placement, etc.These facts have been tacitly overlooked in discussions of the impor-tance of chronological age in readiness for school.

Our factor analysis suggests now very strongly that psychologicalacademia better re-assess their position regarding chronological age.The emphasis on individual differences and individual rates of growth,development, and learning was necessary and meritorious in the faceof the complete disregard of these differences at the time of the exclu-sive reign of chronological age as criterion for school readiness, schoolentrance, and promotion. Insofar as this emphasis on individual differ-ences in turn has led to an almost complete disregard of the importanceof chronological age, it has become a one-sided emphasis, although itwas an important new insight into individual development and learn-ing. Our factor analysis tells us now that the time has come to restorethe balance on a higher level of thought with implications for subse-quent practice. The two positions are not mutually exclusive. Chrono-logical age is definitely important and meaningful for growth, develop-ment, preschool learning, and readiness. The older the child is, general-ly, the more he has grown in structure and function, has accumulatedlife experiences, knowledge, concepts, and understanding of the world,around him. Common observation, i search, and our ractor analysisprove it. It be herewith re-emphasizea. Equally, recognition of indivi-dual differences in personality, ability, and experience at the samechronological age, and the recognition of differences in rates of growth,

Page 34: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

37

development, learning, and readiness must be maintained. Their im-portance is not devalued by our factor analysis. Consideratiion of both,a higher synthesis, is necessary from a scientific as well as a practicalviewpoint.

The Importance of Knowledge

The emphasis in Factor D on the importance of knowledge in readi-ness prompts us to take issue with a widespread tendency in educa-tional circles to de-emphasize the value of knowledge. Such an attitudemust be examined carefully before it can be accepted or rejected. Weagree with it when it means that there is no value in "deadwoodknowledge" or in "inert" knowledge, as Alfred North Whiteheadspeaks of it. Much of the learning in school is of this sort. For instance,learning historical dates or any other facts just to be able to pass a"test" of knowledge, or cramming knowledge into the mind to pass anexam after which the students want to forget what they have learnedas fast as possible. This is a waste of time and energy in the light ofknowledge which is educative. We must disagree, however, with anattitude which says: It is not important for a child to know facts; it ismuch more important to know where to get information, for instance,from an encyclopedia or from a library." This attitude overlooks theimportance of knowledge that is alive, that is ever present and avail-able when needed, and upon which future experiences will have to

Page 35: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

38

build. There will be many occasions in life where we cannot say, "Waita minute, I have to get an encyclopedia; I have to go first to the libraryto find out." Or, the anti-knowledge man will say: "The child does nothave to know facts. The important thing is that he can think. We mustdevelop his thinking." True enough. The development of a child'sthinking ability is one of the major tasks of education and of growingup, but thinking does not occur in a vacuum. Thinking requires subs-stances to think about and with; it requires knowledge of facts, com-parisons with other facts, ideas, other concepts. It requires skill andschooled memory for stored knowledge. Or, there is the modern argu-ment that times change so fast that what we know today is worthlesstomorrow. Here again, the knowledge of tomorrow grows out of theknowledge of today. If what was taken as a fact today will be disproventomorrow, replaced by a new "fact," it is still true that the new knowl-edge and thinking is based on the present one. New ideas and creativ-itiy have their best chance to be born out of a rich body of knowledgeand flexible, imaginative thought that has been constantly disciplinedin examining facts, events, contradictions, deviations from engravedtraces of knowledge, and so forth.

Thus, the anti-knowledge attitude is wrong in underestimating thepower of knowledge. "Knowledge is one chief aim of intellectual edu-cation," says Whitehead (1958, p. 41); "the ordered acquirement ofknowledge is the natural food for a developing intelligence." We canreformulate this idea and say: Intelligence, in order to develop, requiresordered knowledge as its natural food.

If we are willing to accept this as a general truth, it becomes specifi-cally important for children entering school. The more knowledge thechild brings to school, the more he will have "apperceptive masses" tohelp him understand and integrate new knowledge in school. Themore knowledge he brings to school the more concepts he will haveavailable for understanding oral and written language. The more knowl-edge and concepts he has the more he will sharpen his perceptions intransactions with his environment. If we know more, we see more andhear more. We have better all-around antennas for new intake, for newassimilations of the old and the new. These are the ways a child growsintellectually, expands his horizon, and deepens his understanding. Thegreater his body of knowledge of concepts and perceptual skills, allintimately related, the more successful a child will be in his beginningand subsequent school years. The less knowledge, fewer concepts andperceptual skills he has, the less is he ready for school and subsequentlearning.

These are no longer hypotheses or constructs. Their validity and theircontribution to readiness in children has been clearly evidenced andre-affirmed in our earlier reports (Brenner & Morse, 1965; Hofmann &Brenner, 1960, 1961). In all these studies, which included a pilot groupand 5 research groups tested both in kindergarten and first grade, toassess the role of knowledge and information in readiness and achieve-ment prior to kindergarten, in kindergarten, and in first grade, theSangren Information Test yielded consistently the best diagnostic and

Page 36: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

39/0

predictive result, statistically significant above the 1% level. Teacherjudgment and other test results were the validating criteria.

It is Factor D with its high loadings in the Sangren subtests whichforcefully points to the role in readiness of knowledge, perceptsand concepts, and the accompanying skills of analysis and synthesis, ofabstraction and generalization ability.

And what about so-called "enrichment programs," Head Start, pre-school programs? Are they not intended to give the child more back-ground experiences in educationally needed knowledge, to developperceptual and conceptual skills, to offer more opportunities for grow-ing a vocabulary and developing cognitive thinking? And are thesenot achievements which an environment poor in nurture did not pro-duce or allow to develop?

Once the child is in school it is the task of the school to keep knowl-edge alive, to prevent it from becoming inert knowledge, to use it, toclarify it, to channel it into new directions, to build on it. The body ofknowledge, the life experiences which the child brings to school uponentering, and his perceptual-conceptual skills are the stock-in-trade forthe teacher's work with the child. The more the child shows readinessin these respects, the more the teacher can help the child to growfurther in readiness and personality development.

An anti-knowledge attitude is undefendable when applied to readi-ness for learning and growth of personality.

Accomplishments and Limitations of our Factor Analysis

Generally speaking, factor analysis is a mathematical-statistical pro-cedure for organizing and categorizing in terms of their interrelation-ship a set of measured or otherwise observed behaviors, events, con-ditions, or attributes. It can therefore not "reveal" anything that is notinherent in the relationship which exists among the variables as mea-sured or quantified. Thus, one limitation of our factor analysis lies in theselection of the measurable, indeed of the 69 measured, variables. Aselection of other variables or a still larger number might have changedthe outcome, although we do not think it would have changed it sig-nificantly. 69 variables derived from and related to 118 children fur-nish a respectable matrix of intercorrelations and thus provide a solidfoundation for a factor analysis. Herein lies its strength and what isaccomplished for us. A second limitation stems from the fact that inreadiness there are many intangibles which escape direct measurement,but enter into behavior and performance. They can only be impliedfrom the quality of performance or must be otherwise observed.

We shall first summarize the accomplishments of our factor analysisand then speak about its limitations in assessing total readiness.

1. Our factor analysis has confirmed earlier findings and newhypotheses.

2. It has upheld our multidimensional concept of readiness and its

Page 37: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

41

value in assessing readiness because its conceptual framework isbroad and flexible enough to allow for consistency and change inemphasis when new discoveries or changed beliefs come up.

3. It has sifted out seven basic Factors in readiness, separate andyet also related among each other.

4. Within each Factor it has shown the variables which contributemost to readiness, both on the task and the individual sides.

5. It puts before us a panorama of the demanding structure on thetask side and the required bio-psychic structure in the pupil whichenables him to fulfill the task.

6. As a consequence we are a better position to diagnose readi-ness for school through relating individual structure to taskstructure.

7. As a further consequence we have now clear directions as towhat to focus on in improving measurement instruments.

8. Teacher judgment has been found to be very reliable in assess-ing readiness by judging a child's ability, maturity, and achieve-ment. Experienced and sensitive teachers can do it.

9. The new insights gained from the recognition of the need foradequate subject-object or pupil-task relationships should enablemore teachers to improve their methods in fitting the task require-ments to the abilities and functional potential of individual chil-dren especially important for the disadvantaged and gifted child

as brought to mind by Brenner (1957, 1959), Bruner (1960,1966), Skinner (1965), M. Deutsch (1963, 1964), Passow (1963),Ira Gordon (1966), and others. The time should definitely be overwhen teachers, completely misunderstanding John Dewey, wouldsay: "We teach children, not subject matter [Hofmann & Brenner,19631." A kindred mind and friend of John Dewey, the famousschool superintendent of school in Munich, Georg Kerschen-steiner, has as early as 1917 clearly spelled out this need forsubject-object adequacy and the consideration of personality struc-ture and developmental level of pupils in devising appropriatecurricula and teaching methods.

10. On a broader scale, our factor analysis is a long overdue steptoward a psychology of the structure of subject matter as dis-cussed by J. Bruner (1960). It should also help as a stimulustoward bridging the gap between psychology and education,again a matter of concern to Bruner and recently to the APA asexpressed by John Feldhusen (1966) in "Focus on EducationalPsychology." The concern is that psychologists spent half a cen-tury measuring results of teaching while neglecting teachingitself. Psychologists must "re-enter the field of education." Brunerbelieves we are at a major point where psychology will onceagain concern itself with the design of methods of assessingcognitive growth. It is our belief that the implementation of ourfactor analysis can in some measure contribute to such a design.

Page 38: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

42

The limitations of our study must be pointed out with equal force.By its very nature, our factor analysis, of advantage in finding common-alities of task and individual requirements, conceals in its final resultsdifferences among the six groups of children used and further indi-vidual differences within each group, each time in regard to levels ofachievement and differences of personality structure and ability. Thus,in the actual practice of diagnosing and predicting readiness, the psy-chologist and the teacher must find out where each individual pupilstands on the continuum of each Factor or each variable commonality,both on the task and the individual side of readiness. Expressed in a

different way: factor analysis reveals the demands, the abilities, andthe skills needed in general, abstracted from and disregarding indi-vidual differences. How much mastery of abilities and skills in meet-ing the demands of school a child has mustered depends on the indi-vidual, his high or low potential, his past learing experiences, thetutoring from parents, the quality of teaching, and a host of otherfactors influencing individual child performance. To these qualities in a

child the teacher must be sensitive in assessing readiness, in placingthe individual in the continua of our Factors and variables, and inmatching his teaching methods with the child's developmental leveland personality structure.

There are ways, however, of utilizing the result of factor analysis forthe assessment of individual differences in readiness. For this purposea further step, not carried out in this study, is required. A score must bederived for each child on each aspect of readiness as represented bythe seven Factors. Each of the seven Factors thus becomes a dimensionin terms of which the child is measured and placed in the distributionfrom highest to lowest score. To obtain such individual scores in themost rigorous way requires further computer operations. However, itcan be done in a less rigorous manner by simply marking the child'srating or score on each of the original variables which have high load-ing in a particular Factor. The sum, or some kind of average of thesevariable scores, could be used to place the child on the factor continu-um. Such raw factor scores could then be converted to standard scoresand a plotted "profile" of these scores for each child would present a

qualitative picture of the pattern of measurable aspects of his totalreadiness.

Among the host of other factors influencing total readiness are manycriteria and patterns of personality and behavior which we have estab-lished and discussed in our earlier research (Brenner, 1957, 1964, 1965,1967a, 1967b; 'Brenner & Samelson, 1959). They are not in the immedi-ate grasp of our factor analysis.

To illustrate: Factor analysis cannot directly measure motivations ofthe child, ability to pay attention, to concentrate, to follow directions, tobe self-directing; it measures these only indirectly in so far as theabilities enter performance. It cannot measure in any direct way theimpact of love, trust, belonging, and security on the child or the effectof being accepted or rejected at home or in school. It cannot measurenervous tensions, fears, anxieties, or the kind of self image a child has.It cannot measure his restlessness or his tendency toward temporary or

Page 39: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

43

permanent tiredness. Health and absenteeism are excluded from ourfactor analysis.

Similarly, it does not measure the child's striving to emulate significant adults, to pattern his life according to those who become a modelfor him or whose expectations he enjoys or hates to moat. Inter-personal skills are excluded from this type of factor analysis as aremeasures of the child's skill in finding a balance between freedom ofexpression and necessary restrictions in school. All these are importantcriteria of readines which are beyond the reach of our factor analysis.Their significance has been shown in our previous research.

There are still other limitations. Our factor analysis does not measurethe effectiveness of the teacher: the effect of his personality on thechild, his personal tempo (Yando & Kagan, 1968), his pedagogical artand craft. It does not measure peer group and community influence onthe child.

Total readiness is contingent on the seven Factors plus all the justmentioned factors which are often intangible and elusive. For a fullunderstanding and a proper diagnosis of school readiness we mustkeep this in mind. We are speaking here from the standpoint of anideal comprehensive assessment of readiness. In actual diagnostic prac-tice more or less limited approaches will have to suffice.

SUMMARY AND IMPLICATIONS

As part of a long-term comprehensive research project on schoolreadiness, this study deals with information on 118 middle-class chil-dren in kindergarten and first grade. Sixty-nine variables consisting ofthe age of the children at the time of school entrance and of testing; ofsubtests of well known standardized tests; teacher judgment of ability,maturity, and achievement; and a number of physical examination datahave been se'lcted for each of the 118 children. This information hasbeen factor-analyzed. As a trame of reference and to provide the nec-essary contextual background, our concept of readiness has been out-lined first, then the development of the project and the hypotheses tobe tested. The research population and raw data have been described.Table 1 illustrates the way that raw data were prepared for IBM computation. Table 2 gives the principal axis solution; Table 3 the rotatedfactors, verimax solution. The computer analysis resulted in sevenreadiness Factors which were iderlitfied as Factor A: Cognitive readi-ness; B: Chronological age; C: Reading readiness; D: Body of knowl-edge; E: Biochemical (maturity) factor; F: Physical developmental status;G: Perceptual differentiation. Thereafter we discussed the changed con-cept of readiness, sex differences, the role of chronological age, theimportance of knowledge in readiness, and, finally, accomplishmentsand limitations of our factor analysis.

The implications of our study may be formulated as follows:

1. The huge body of intercorrelations which led to the final sevenfactors allows for many specific studies in the future which canshed increasing light on the complex problem of school readiness.

Page 40: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

44/V r;

2. With these research results we have the knowledge necessaryto construct a test of readiness which focuses on the most salientingredients of readiness and achievement (apart from not directlymeasured emotional and social personality factors).

3. In teaching, the classroom teacher should now know better thanbefore which aspects of readiness should be emphasized on theindividual and on the task side. This has further implications forcurriculum construction and development of teaching methods, tofit content and method to the developmental level and understand-ing of individual students so as to assure maximum success inteaching and learning.

4. Our findings should caution research people not to devaluatetoo quickly the teacher's ability to judge children.

5. From our study teachers can draw confidence in their judgmentof their students' abilities, maturity, and achieveme it, providedthat the teacher is observant and sensitive to the multidimensionalaspects of individual task relationships.

6. With the recognition that a child's life experiences, his body ofknowledge, and his perceptual-cognitive skills are so vital to readi-ness, parents, teachers, and :ommunities should find themselvesadvised to provide early, many, and variegated opportunities forthe child to acquire those experiences, if possible, in preschoolyears. This should be done without undue pressure.

7. While chronological age is not a good indicator for the diag-nosis of individual readiness because of the wide ranges of indi-vidual developmental physical and mental status at a givenchronological age, it deserves to be emphasized as a natural basisfor developing organismic structures and for increasing the child'slife experiences. The older child is generally more ready for schoolthan the younger child of equal potential.

8. Our findings should be apt to encourage parents, teachers,pediatricians, endocrinologists, ophthalmologists, and orthodon-tists to cooperate in providing physical fitness for the child in allits various aspects.

9. There is still no conclusive evidence as to the superiority of girlsover boys in readiness and learning at these early age levels. Theexamination of sex differences needs to be continued. It will makeprogress only once we move away from widely accepted generali-zations and study specific differences among boys and girls inregard to specific tasks and accomplishments.

Page 41: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

46

REFERENCES

Ausubel, D. Cognitive structure and the facilitation of meaningful learning, In R. Anderson &D. Ausubel Eris.), Reading in the Psychology of cognition, Ncw York: Holt, Rinehart andWinston, 1965.

Brenner, A.A n.w gestalt test for measuring readiness for school, Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1959,Vol. 5, Spring.Nature and /tearing of readiness for school. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1957, Vol. 3,Spring.Readiness for school and today's pressures. Collected Papers, Interinstitutional Seminarin Child Development, 1966; Edison Institute, Dearborn, Michigan, 1967. Pp. 1-24.Ready or not they're on their way to school. PTA Magazine, April, 1956, Vol. 8, 20-22;also in "PTA guide to what's happening in education," New York: Scholastic Book Serv-ices, 1965.Reality perception, perceptual differentiation, and readiness for school. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly, 1958, Vol. 4, Summer.Reexamining readiness. Childhood Education, 1967, Vol. 43, 8, 453-57.The Anton Brenner Developmental Gestalt Test of School Readiness. Los Angeles: WesternPsychological Services, 1964.The contribution of factor analysis to the study of readiness for school. Collected Papers,Inter-Institutional Seminar in Child Development, 1961; Edison Institute, Dearborn, Mi,hi-gan, 1962.

Brenner, A. & Geake, R. School readiness for kindergarten through grade 9. Collected Pa-pers, Inter-Insitiitional Seminar in Child Development, 1963; Edison Institute, Dearborn,Michigan, 1964.

Bremner, A. & Morse, N. The measurement of children's readiness for school. Papers of theMichigan Academy of Science, Arts, and Letters, 1956, Vol. 41.

Brenner, A. & Samelson, N. Kindergarten behavior and first grade achievement: A casestudy exploration. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1959, Vol. 5, Spring.

Brenner, Weddington, & Hofmann. School demands. The Elementary School Journal, 1964,Vol. 64, 5, 261-64

Bruner, J. The process of education. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1960.

Bruner, J. Toward a theory of instruction. New York: Norton & Company, 1968.Bruner, J. (Ed.). Learning about learning. A conference report. US Dept, of Health, Educa

tion, & Welfare, 1966.

Cattell, R. Factor analysis. New York: Harper & Bros., 1952.

Cattell, R. Personality and motivation structure and measurement. New York: Harcourt, 1957

Dawkins, Geake, & Pyle. The degree of ascendance in twu generations of adolescents andtheir skeletal maturity. Collected Papers, Interinstitutional Seminar in Child Development,1965; Edison Institute, Dearborn, Michigan, 1966. Pp. 3043.

Deutsch, M. Papers from the Arden House Conference on Pre-School Enrichment. Introduc-tory Comments. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1964, Vol. 10, July.

Deutsch, M. The disadvantaged child and the learning process: Some social, psychologicaland developmental conditions. In A. Passow (Ed.), Education in depressed areas, NewYork: Teachers College Press, 1963.

Feldhusen, J. Focus on educational psychology. In "Educational Psychologist," Newsletter ofDivision 15, APA, April, 1966,

Goodenough, F. New evidence on environmental influence on intelligence. 39th Yearbookof The National Society for The Study of Education, Part 1, 1940.

Gordon, I. Studying the child in school. New York: Teachers College, Columbia University,1946.

Greulich, W. & Pyle, S. Radiographic atlas of skeletal maturation of the hand and wrist.2nd ed. Stanford, California: Stanford University Press, 1959.

Guilford, J. Personality. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1959.Guilford, J. The nature of human intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1968.Harman, H. Modern factor analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960.Hofmann, H. Behavior patterns in kindergarten and first grade. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly,

1957; Vol. 3, Spring, 136.145.

Page 42: DOCUMENT RESUME ED 094 862ED 094 862. AUTHOR TITLE INSTITUTION PUB DATE. NOTE. AVAILABLE FROM. EDRS PRICE. DESCRIPTORS. DOCUMENT RESUME. PS 007 324. Brenner, Anton; Scott, Leland H

47

Hofmann, H. Children's drawings as an indication of readiness for first grade. Merrill-PalmerQuarterly, 1958, Vol. 4, 3, 165 1/9.

Hofmann, H. & Brenner, A.Children's perceptual and conceptual levels in relationship to readiness for school tasks.Collected Papers, Inter Institutional Seminar in Child Development, 1959; Edison Institute,Dearborn, Michigan, 1960.Subject matter and John Dewey. Utah Education Review, March, 1963.The role of knowledge and information for kindernarten and first grade readiness. Col.leered Papers, Interinstitutional Seminar in Child Development, 1960. Edison Institute,Dearborn, Michigan, 1961.

Hughes, B. The biological basis of development. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1958, Vol. 5

Hymes, J. A child development point of view. New Jersey: Prentice Hall, 1955, 1958.

Ilg. F. & Ames, L. School readiness. New York: Harper and Rowe, 1965.

Infielder, B. & Pogo, J. The growth of logical thinking from childhood to adolescence.New York: Basic Books, 1958.

Jersild, A. Child development and the curriculum. New York: Teachers College, 1946.

Johnston, J. & Manson, G. Research and development and readiness from a pediatrician'spoint of view. In Pyle, Waterhouse, & Greulich (Eds.), loc. cit.

Kerschensteiner, G. Das Grundaxiom des Bildungspro ssssss and Seine Folgerungen Fuer dieSchulorganisation. Berlin, 1917. 1924,

Mellinger, R., Manson, J., & Peterman, L. Correlations between endocrine and physicalmeasures of maturity during growth, In Pyle, Waterhouse, & Greulich (Eds.), loc. cit.

Monroe, M. Manual for new basic reading tests. Chicago: Scott, Foresman & Company, nodate.

Passow, A. (Ed.). Education in depressed areas. New York: Teachers College, 1963.

Pau ley, F. Sex differences and legal school entrance age. Journal of Educational Research,Vol. 45, 1, 1951,

Pyle, S., Waterhouse, A., & Greulich, W. A radiographic standard of reference for the grow-ing hand and wrist. Prepared for the United States National Health Examination Survey.Yearbook Medical Publishers, Inc., Chicago, III. 60601, 1971.

Schab, F. The effects of two different approaches to remedial reading on the permanence ofthe resulting achievement. Childhood Education, October, 1967, 140.41.

Sigel, I. Sex differences in cognitive functioning, re-examined: A functional point of view.Revised version of a paper read at the 1963 Biennial Meeting of the Society for Researchin Child Development, Berkeley, California, April, 1963.

Skeels, H., Updegrafl, Wellman, & Williams. A study of enviranmental stimulation: Anorphanage preschool project. University of Iowa Studies in Child Welfare, 1938, Vol.15, 4.

Skinner, B. Why teachers fail. Saturday Review, October 16, 1965.

Skodak, M. Children in foster homes: A study of mental Development. University of IowaStudies in Child Welfare, 1939, Vol. 16, 1.

Stroebel, M. Follow-up study of pupils who attended Greenfield Village Schools, 1946-1963.Man ;script.

Thurs. ne, L. Multiple-factor analysis. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1961.Tyler, F. Issues related to readiness to learn. Theories of learning and instruction. 61st Year-

b of the National Society for the Study of Education. Pp. 210-230.Wellman, B. Growth 1.. intelligence under different school environment. Journal of Experi-

mental Education, 1934, Vol. 3, 59433.Wellman, B. Mental growth from preschool to college. Journal of Experimental Education,

1937, Vol. 6, 120 -138.Wellman & Coffey. The role of cultural status in intelligence changes in preschool children.

Journal of Experimental Education, 1936. Vol. 5, 191-202.Wetzel, N. Physical fitness in terms of physique: Development and basal metabolism.

Journal of American Medical Association, 1941, Vol. 116, 1187.1195.Whitehead, A. The aims of education. The American Library, 1958.Yando & Kagan. Effect of teacher tempo on the child. Child Development, 1968, Vol. 39, 1.