32
CSD Students' Working Paper Series Euroscepticism in the UK: Explaining individual level support for membership of the EU. Lucy Hatton MA Politics, Department of Politics & International Studies, University of Warwick [email protected] Working Paper n. 10/ 2011 Centre for Studies in Democratisation Department of Politics and International Studies University of Warwick Coventry CV4 7AL United Kingdom http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/soc/pais/research/csd/ Centre for Studies in Democratisation

CSD Students' Working Paper Series - University of Warwick · CSD Students' Working Paper Series ... (Gabel 1998:335). Investigations undertaken by Anderson (1998) and Inglehart (1970)

  • Upload
    lehanh

  • View
    218

  • Download
    1

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

CSD

Students' Working Paper Series

Euroscepticism in the UK:

Explaining individual level support for

membership of the EU.

Lucy Hatton

MA Politics, Department of Politics & International Studies,

University of Warwick

[email protected]

Working Paper n. 10/ 2011

Centre for Studies in Democratisation

Department of Politics and International Studies

University of Warwick

Coventry CV4 7AL

United Kingdom

http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/soc/pais/research/csd/

Centre for Studies in Democratisation

The Centre for Studies in Democratisation (CSD) was established

at the University of Warwick in 1992 in response to a growing

interest in the study of democracy at a theoretical and empirical

level. Democratisation has become a central political theme and

features now prominently on the foreign policy agenda of

western countries. Members of CSD are seeking to understand

why, how and when democracies emerge, sustain or collapse.

They also investigate the reasons why democratisation can

sometimes be problematic.

Do not hesitate to contact us for more information!

Renske Doorenspleet (Director):

[email protected]

Or visit our website:

http://www2.warwick.ac.uk/fac/soc/pais/research/csd/

Abstract

To understand what affects individual level support for the European Union

is to understand the pace and direction of European integration, as the

policy outputs from Brussels are increasingly affecting the everyday lives of

European citizens, and their reactions to them impact upon the policies’

success. This essay considers what affects individual level support for the

EU in the UK, which is one of, if not the, most Eurosceptic member state. By

empirically analysing the results of the spring 2009 Eurobarometer survey

among the respondents from the UK in relation to the four main theories of

individual level support for the EU: cognitive mobilisation, utilitarian

theory, proxy theory and the effects of partisan affiliation, I discover that the

level of support a British individual expresses towards the national

government is the strongest predictor of the level of support that individual

expresses towards the EU. Importantly, however, none of these explanations

can be considered a sufficient explanatory factor for levels of support for the

EU from individuals in the UK.

Keywords: Euroscepticism; cognitive mobilisation; utilitarian theory; proxy

theory.

Euroscepticism in the UK: Explaining individual level support for

membership of the EU

Lucy Hatton

Introduction

Euroscepticism, or opposition to the process of European integration

(Abts et al 2009:8), has been a problem in the United Kingdom for a long

time (Spiering 2004:127); Britain has famously been described as the EU‟s

„awkward partner‟ (Haesly 2001:83). Public support for the European Union

among the British people has been declining for almost two decades

(Anderson and Kaltenhaler 1996:175), and the UK is consistently rated as

the member state with the lowest popular support for the EU (Anderson and

Kaltenhaler 1996:182; Grant 2008:1-2; Haesly 2001:82; Spiering 2004:134).

Many arguments have been put forward for why the UK is the most

Eurosceptic of the EU member states, including: post-war divergence,

distinctive Britishness and difference from the continent, which fosters an

„us and them‟ attitude towards the other states of Europe, economic and

cultural differences, an overtly Eurosceptic public press, and fear for the

threat to British nationalism (Grant 2008; Haesly 2001; Hooghe and Marks

2007; Spiering 2004).

However, why the UK is the most Eurosceptic member state is not at

issue in this essay. This essay attempts to answer the research question „what

factors affect an individual‟s level of support for membership of the EU?‟ by

empirically investigating the impact of various individual characteristics on

the public‟s opinion about the UK‟s membership of the EU. I aim to identify

which of the four most frequently cited theories of individual level support

2

for the EU is the most successful at explaining the levels of support for the

most Eurosceptic member state‟s membership of the EU among British

individuals.

An assessment of public support for the EU at the individual level is

important because to know what drives support for the EU is to be able to

interpret developments in European integration (Anderson 1998:570). The

policies emerging from Brussels are increasingly affecting the everyday

lives of the people in the UK, and the level of support the public gives to

these policies and the institutions which design and implement them

undoubtedly affects their success (Anderson and Kaltenhaler 1996:176). As

the recent negative responses to referenda held on the Constitutional Treaty

demonstrate, public opinion can affect the direction and pace of the

integration process (Anderson and Kaltenhaler 1996:178), and as the process

is becoming increasingly constrained by public opinion, it is vital that the

factors affecting support for the EU are identified (Gabel 1998:333),

especially among “the most Eurosceptic people in Europe” (Grant 2008:2).

The first section of this essay, „Theories and hypotheses‟, will identify

the factors that have been argued to influence individual support for the EU,

namely cognitive mobilisation, utilitarian theory, proxy theory and the

effects of partisan affiliation, as well as important control variables of age

and gender. I will then operationalise the concepts central to these theories

and systematically analyse how each affects individual level support for

membership of the EU. The discussion section will consider the results and

possible imperfections of the analysis and make suggestions for future

research in this area, before I make some concluding remarks.

3

Theories and Hypotheses

Several authors have theorised about what influences individual

support for European integration, primarily relying on analyses across the

entirety of the EU member states. However, as Anderson points out, it

should not be assumed that the citizens of every member state are affected in

the same way by the same factors (Anderson 1998:592). As such, I think it is

important to consider whether the theories generated are applicable to the

most Eurosceptic member state, the UK. The most frequently put forward

explanations for support for the EU are cognitive mobilisation, utilitarian

theory, proxy theory and partisan affiliation effects, each of which will be

discussed in turn, and the hypotheses to be tested will be identified. Figure 1

illustrates the relationships that will be analysed in the next section on the

basis of the hypotheses presented here.

Cognitive Mobilisation

It has been argued by several scholars that the level of

individual cognitive mobilisation affects the extent to which individuals

support the EU. Those who are more cognitively mobilised, that is, have a

greater interest in and knowledge of European politics, are thought to be

more supportive of the integration project (Abts et al 2009:5). This theory

was first advanced by Ronald Inglehart in 1970 who saw that the cognitive

mobilisation process will increase an individual‟s “capacity to receive and

interpret messages relating to a remote political community”, and

consequently theorised that the more politically aware citizens “could not

only be more likely to have an opinion concerning European integration;

they would also be more likely to have a favourable orientation toward it”

4

(Inglehart 1970:47-8). The basis for this argument is that as cognitive

mobilisation increases, an individual will be more familiar with and less

fearful of European integration (Gabel 1998:335). Investigations undertaken

by Anderson (1998) and Inglehart (1970) have found empirical support for

this theory: individuals with higher levels of cognitive mobilisation with

regard to the EU were more favourable supporters of integration. Gabel also

found support for the effects of cognitive mobilisation, but the effects were

only significant in the original member states (Gabel 1998:348, 351).

However, there has not been unanimous support for the

cognitive mobilisation theory. Abts et al‟s empirical analysis of individual

public support for the EU in Belgium resulted in a rejection of the cognitive

mobilisation hypothesis, and their results actually found support for the

reverse: that Belgians who are more cognitively mobilised displayed less

support for the EU (Abts et al 2009:16, 19). McLaren, similarly, found

cognitive mobilisation to have an insignificant effect on support for the EU

when compared with other factors consistent with the utilitarian theory

discussed below (McLaren 2007:243). Despite these contradicting results,

support has still been found for the cognitive mobilisation theory and so it is

necessary to investigate it in relation to the UK.

Hypothesis 1: Individuals with high levels of cognitive mobilisation are

more likely to support UK membership of the EU.

[Null hypothesis 1: There is no relationship between level of cognitive

mobilisation and level of support for membership of the EU.]

5

Utilitarian Theory

An alternative suggestion as to what affects level of support for

the EU is the utilitarian theory. Utilitarian theory is a rational, economic

theory of support for the EU, which posits that what individuals are set to

gain or lose, in economic terms, from European integration influences their

opinions towards it (Abts et al 2009:2). Those with higher economic, social

and cultural capital are likely to gain more from the central aspect of the EU,

the free market, and so more skilled, educated and wealthy individuals are

more supportive of the EU than those who are set to lose out from the

integration process (de Vries and van Kersbergen 2007:310); those with

lesser skills are not as likely to gain from the free market as the associated

economic migration has the potential to threaten their job security (McLaren

2004:900). It is therefore suggested that individuals conduct some kind of

cost-benefit analysis regarding European integration to determine whether

they, as individuals, are likely to gain or lose from the integration process,

and their resulting evaluation impacts upon the level of support they give to

the EU (Tucker et al 2002:558).

Support for the utilitarian theory has come from studies covering

many countries undertaken by McLaren (2004:905), Hooghe et al

(2007:339) and Gabel (1998:351), who all find that the more skilled and

educated the individual, the more support they express for the EU. Gabel

suggested that there may be a significant link between the cognitive

mobilisation and utilitarian theories, with the impact of utilitarian theory

merely being a reflection of the cognitive mobilisation effect (Gabel

1998:338), yet his empirical analysis found that it was in fact utilitarian

6

factors that had a significant impact upon support for integration, and not

cognitive mobilisation (Gabel 1998:351). Therefore, more skilled and

educated individuals are argued to be more supportive of the EU than the

less skilled and educated.

Hypothesis 2: Individuals with higher level skills and education are more

likely to support UK membership of the EU.

[Null hypothesis 2: There is no relationship between level of skills and

education and level of support for UK membership of the EU.]

Proxy Theory

However, Anderson argues that individuals are not informed

or knowledgeable enough about the EU to be able to conduct such an

accurate and systematic cost-benefit analysis of the impact European

integration would have on their lives (Anderson 1998:573). Because of this

lack of information, he posits that individuals rely on the more complete

information and crystallised opinions they have about their national political

institutions to form their opinions about the EU (Anderson 1998:574). This

is proxy theory, whereby an individual‟s level of support for the EU is

formulated on the basis of their level of support for national institutions, and

particularly their national government (Abts et al 2009:5), as dissatisfaction

with the EU emerges as a by-product of dissatisfaction with the national

government (McLaren 2004:903). The proxy theory is in line with the

„second-order elections‟ thesis whereby citizens use European elections and

referenda as an opportunity to voice their opinions of the national

7

government, as illustrated by Garry et al‟s analysis of the referenda on the

Nice Treaty (Garry et al 2005:204). The proxy theory therefore argues that

those who are happy with the workings of their national governments are

more likely to support the EU, and those who are less happy nationally are

less likely to (Anderson 1998:590).

Many studies find support for the proxy theory in their empirical

analyses (Anderson 1998:591; Abts et al 2009:17; Hooghe and Marks

2007:121; Garry et al 2005:216; de Vries and van Kersbergen 2007:319).

Karp et al discover an interplay between the proxy theory and the cognitive

mobilisation theory, whereby individuals who are less politically

knowledgeable and aware have no basis on which to form an opinion on the

EU and as such are more likely to rely on their opinions of the national

government to formulate their views (Karp et al 2003:287).

However, in contrast with those studies which support the idea that

dissatisfaction at the national level leads to dissatisfaction at the

supranational level is the suggestion put forth by McLaren (2007) and

Sánchez-Cuenca (2000) that in fact the converse may be true: that an

individual who is satisfied with the actions of their national government does

not see the need for any functions to be delegated to a supranational level

and so does not support the EU (McLaren 2007:234; Sánchez-Cuenca

2000:148). Even though the direction of the relationship between support for

national government and support for the EU is a contested one, the vast

majority of previous studies find support for the proxy theory.

Hypothesis 3: Individuals who are supportive of the national government

are more likely to support UK membership of the EU.

8

[Null hypothesis 3: There is no relationship between level of support for the

national government and level of support for UK membership of the EU.]

Party Affiliation

Linked with people using their opinions of the national

government to form their opinions of the EU is the theory that people use the

views of the domestic political party they support to shape their opinions on

the EU (Gabel 1998:338). That is, supporters of a pro-EU political party are

likely to be supportive of the EU, and supporters of an anti-EU political

party are unlikely to be supportive of the EU (Evans and Butt 2007:172).

The relationship between party affiliation and support for the EU in the UK

was investigated by Evans and Butt, who suggested that support for the EU

among the supporters of the Conservative and Labour parties should

fluctuate over time according to the realignment of the parties‟ stance on

Europe in the 1980s. They found support for this argument, but only up until

the 1990s when they discovered that attitudes on the EU became

independent of the partisan views of the public (Evans and Butt 2007:187).

This is partly consistent with the earlier findings of Inglehart, who

considered support for the EU in France in 1968 and found partisan

affiliation to be a strong predictor of level of support (Inglehart 1970:66).

Therefore, the political and partisan views of individuals may affect their

level of support for the EU. As currently in the UK the Conservative Party is

considered both more right wing and more Eurosceptic in outlook than the

Labour Party, it could be suggested that individuals who identify themselves

to the left will be more likely to support membership of the EU.

9

Hypothesis 4: Individuals whose political views are towards the left are

more likely to support UK membership of the EU.

[Null hypothesis 4: There is no relationship between an individual’s

political views and level of support for UK membership of the EU.]

Age

Inglehart discovered that age is a complementary factor in

explaining individual support for the EU, and finds it particularly important

among British respondents, where younger people are more supportive of

the EU (Inglehart 1970:69). This finding is supported by Tucker et al

(2002:561). Age may have an effect on the hypotheses stated above, so it

may be important to include it in this study.

Hypothesis 5: Younger individuals are more likely to support UK

membership of the EU.

[Null hypothesis 5: There is no relationship between age and level of

support for UK membership of the EU.]

Gender

Gender has also been controlled for in a number of previous

studies. Again, Inglehart and Tucker et al find a relationship between gender

and support for the EU, yet this time with contradicting results (Inglehart

1970:60; Tucker et al 2002:561). Inglehart finds men more likely to support

the EU, and considers a link between this and the cognitive mobilisation

theory, suggesting that men are more likely to be more cognitively mobilised

10

than women (Inglehart 1970:54). Tucker et al, conversely, find men

significantly less likely to support the EU than women, yet this support is

only based on analysis of three Eastern European countries (Tucker et al

2002:567). However, Abts et al also find men to be more Eurosceptic than

women, indicating that the relationship found by Tucker et al may be more

widespread across Europe (Abts et al 2009:14). Therefore it may be that

gender plays a role in influencing individual support for the EU, even if the

effect is merely intervening.

Hypothesis 6: Women are more likely to support UK membership of the EU.

[Null hypothesis 6: There is no relationship between gender and level of

support for membership of the EU.]

Figure 1: A diagram to illustrate the hypothesised relationships to be analysed.

Level of Support for the EU

Gender

Age

Level of cognitive mobilisation

Level of skill and education

Level of trust in national

government

Partisan affiliation

11

Concepts and Measurements

I test my hypotheses using data from the Eurobarometer Survey

71.1 conducted in January and February 2009. This particular survey was

selected as it includes suitable questions and information to operationalise

all of the hypotheses. Eurobarometer surveys are carried out on behalf of the

European Commission at frequent intervals to assess the opinions of the

European public on various issues related to integration. The data is

collected through face to face interviews in all member states and often in

candidate countries also, and respondents are selected using a multi-stage

random sampling process covering all citizens aged over 15 resident in the

member states and having a sufficient command of the national language

(European Commission 2009b:74)1. The analysis includes only the

responses of the 1289 individuals interviewed in the UK.

Dependent Variable – Support for Membership of the EU

The dependent variable is measured using the responses

individuals gave to the following question in the Eurobarometer survey:

„Generally speaking, do you think that the UK‟s membership of the EU is: a

good thing, a bad thing, neither good nor bad or don‟t know?‟ (European

Commission 2009b:13, QA6a). Responses are reported on a three point

scale, with 1 denoting „bad thing‟ and being considered a low level of

support for the EU, and 3 denoting „good thing‟ and considered a high level

of support. Responses of „don‟t know‟ were excluded from the analysis, as

ambivalent responses are denoted by a response of „neither good nor bad‟.

1 The sampling procedure and methodology used in the survey is reproduced in full in appendix 1.

12

Cognitive Mobilisation

I measure cognitive mobilisation by the extent to which the

respondents discuss European politics, with those who discuss European

politics more frequently being considered to have a higher level of cognitive

mobilisation. Scores for this variable are based on the response to the

question: „When you get together with friends or relatives, would you say

you discuss frequently, occasionally or never about European political

matters?‟ (European Commission 2009b:5, QA1:2). Again responses are

reported on a three point scale, with 1 denoting never and 3 denoting

frequently. „Don‟t know‟ responses are excluded from the analysis.

Level of Skills and Education

I measure level of skills and education by the age at which the

respondents left education, as identified in their response to the question:

„How old were you when you stopped full time education?‟ (European

Commission 2009b:65, D8). The responses to the question are in years, with

the older an individual left education indicating a higher level of skills and

education. Those respondents who were still in full time education, did not

know or refused to answer the question are excluded from the analysis.

Level of Support for National Government

Level of support for national government is measured by the

level of trust the respondents to the survey purport to have in the UK

government, based on their response to the question: ‘I would like to ask you

a question about how much trust you have in certain institutions. For each of

the following institutions, please tell me if you tend to trust it or tend not to

trust it: The UK Government‟ (European Commission 2009b:16, QA9:1).

13

Responses are reported on a three point scale, with 1 denoting tend not to

trust and taken to be a low level of support for the UK government, and 3

denoting tend to trust and a high level of support. „Don‟t know‟ responses

are taken to be ambivalent and so are given a score of 2 on the scale. Only

those who refused to answer the question are excluded from the analysis.

Political Views

Political views are identified by the self-positioning of the

respondents on a scale from extremely left wing to extremely right wing.

The question is phrased as follows: „In political matters, people talk of “the

left” and “the right”. How would you place your views on this scale?‟

(European Commission 2009b:65, D1); respondents are shown a scale from

one to ten, with 1 denoting furthest left and 10 denoting furthest right.

Responses between 1 and 5 are considered on the left, and between 6 and 10

on the right. Those who responded with „don‟t know‟ or refused to answer

are excluded from the analysis.

Age and Gender

Age is measured in whole years. For the gender variable, male

is coded as 1 and female as 2.

Analysis

Univariate

Regarding the dependent variable, of the UK respondents to the

survey consider the UK‟s membership of the EU a bad thing. The mean

score, 1.96, indicates a slight negative skew in the data towards perceiving

14

EU membership as a bad thing: as expected given the UK is considered a

Eurosceptic member state overall. Table 1 shows the frequencies for the

dependent variable. Very few of the respondents, a mere , reported that

they frequently discussed European political issues with their friends or

relatives. The modal response was never: the majority of the people in the

UK never discuss European political issues with their friends or relatives.

The age at which the respondents left full time education ranged from 7 to

75, with a mean response of 17.6 years and the modal response being 16. A

staggering of respondents tended not to trust the UK government, with

less than half of that figure reporting to trust the national government

( ), as illustrated in figure 2. The median response to the political

views self-placement question was a score of 5, indicating that most people

reported being to the left. The youngest respondent to the survey was 15

years old and the eldest 96 years old, with the mean age being 51.52 years;

the ages of the respondents are illustrated in figure 3. A slightly higher

percentage of the sample is female ( ) compared to male ( ).

Table 1: The frequencies of responses to the question regarding level of support for UK membership of

the EU.

UK Membership of the

EU is considered a: Frequency Percentage

Cumulative

Percentage

Bad thing 417 35.2 35.2

Neither good nor bad 396 33.4 68.5

Good thing 373 31.5 100.0

Total 1186 100.0

15

Bivariate

The analysis finds a weak positive relationship between level of

support for the EU and level of political discussion ( ), and this was

found to be significant at a confidence level ( ) so null

hypothesis 1 can be rejected. This finding implies that individuals with

higher levels of cognitive mobilisation are not necessarily more likely to

support membership of the EU: we would make fewer errors in

predicting level of support for UK membership of the EU when we know

how frequently they discuss European politics; a relationship does exist, but

it is weak. However, the relationship is in the expected direction, that is,

those with a higher level of cognitive mobilisation are slightly more likely to

support UK membership of the EU, as suggested in hypothesis 1.

16

Figure 2: The level of trust in the UK Government as reported by the UK respondents to the

Eurobarometer 71.1 survey.

Figure 3: The distribution of ages of the UK respondents to the Eurobarometer 71.1 survey

17

A weak positive relationship is also uncovered between the

level of support for the EU and the age at which the respondents left

education, with the latter explaining ( ) of the variation in the

former. The relationship is significant ( ) and so null hypothesis 2

can be rejected. The regression formula ( ) indicates that

for each extra year an individual remains in education, he or she moves

points towards the more supportive end of the spectrum of level of support

for membership of the EU. The relationship is shown to be in the expected

direction: the more skilled and educated a British individual is, the more

likely they are to be supportive of membership of the EU, as suggested in

hypothesis 2, yet the relationship is not particularly strong.

A much stronger relationship is found between level of support

for the EU and level of support for the national government ( ). A

chi square test demonstrates that this relationship is significant at a

confidence level ( ), allowing us to reject null hypothesis 3. We

would make almost fewer errors in predicting an individual‟s level of

support for the EU when taking into account their level of support for the

UK government. This indicates that the more support a British individual

provides for his or her national government, the more support he or she is

likely to give to the UK‟s membership of the EU, as expected in hypothesis

3, which can thus be confirmed.

The analysis finds a weak negative relationship between

ranking on the scale of political views and support for UK membership of

the EU ( ), and the relationship is significant at the 99.9%

confidence level ( ) thus null hypothesis 4 can be rejected. This

18

indicates that the respondents who identified their views as more left wing

were more likely to support the UK‟s membership of the EU, but the

relationship is weak: we would only make fewer errors in predicting

level of support for UK membership of the EU through knowing the

respondents‟ placement on the scale of political views. The direction is,

however, as predicted in hypothesis 4.

The relationship between age and level of support for the EU is

also weak, with age explaining just of the variation in support

( ). Although this relationship is very weak, it is significant

( ), null hypothesis 5 can be rejected, and it is in the same direction

as suggested in hypothesis 5. The regression line formula ( )

illustrates that an increase in one year in age of the respondents is likely to

result in a move of 0.09 points less support for the UK‟s membership of the

EU. Therefore, as predicted, younger people in the UK are more likely to

support membership of the EU, but the relationship is very weak.

A moderate to weak relationship has been found to exist

between gender and level of support for the UK‟s membership of the EU

(Cramer‟s ), and the relationship is significant ( ), so null

hypothesis 6 can be rejected. It appears that men are more likely to support

the EU than women, as illustrated in figure 4, and women tend to be more

ambivalent. The results indicate that when predicting level of support for

membership of the EU within the UK, knowing an individual‟s gender

decreases the amount of errors made by ( ). Therefore,

inconsistent with the findings of Tucker et al (2002) and Abts et al (2009),

and contrary to hypothesis 6, men in the UK are slightly more likely to

support EU membership than women. The findings of the bivariate analysis

19

are summarised in table 2.

Figure 4: The differences of opinions on the UK’s membership of the EU on the basis of gender.

Table 2: The strength and direction of the bivariate relationships between the level of support for UK

membership of the EU and the hypothesised influential factors. All relationships are significant at

α=0.01.

Level of

Cognitive

Mobilisation

Level of

Skills and

Education

Level of

Support for

National

Government

Political

Views Age Gender

Level of

Support for

EU

Membership

Weak

Positive

Weak

Positive

Moderate

Positive

Weak

Negative

Weak

Negative Moderate

20

Multivariate

As my bivariate analysis has discovered the strongest

relationship between level of support for national government and level of

support for membership of the EU, I want to check that this relationship is

direct and that my control variables of age and gender do not affect it, as

suggested might be the case by Inglehart (1970) and Tucker et al (2002).

When re-analysing the relationship between level of support for the UK

government and level of support for membership of the EU whilst

controlling for age, it is evident that the relationship is direct and age does

not significantly affect it. When the effects of age have been removed from

the measure of association between level of support for the UK government

and level of support for the EU, the relationship remains moderate-strong

and positive ( ). When controlling for gender, the moderate-strong

relationship between level of support for membership of the EU and level of

support for the national government remains exactly the same ( )2.

This indicates a direct relationship between the variables: the relationship

found earlier between level of support for UK membership of the EU and

level of support for the UK government is not affected by the age or gender

of the respondents. The zero-order and partial gamma values are displayed

in table 3.

Gamma

Zero-Order

1st-Order Partial

(Controlling for age)

1st-Order Partial

(Controlling for gender)

0.481 Table 3: The zero-order and 1

st-order partial gammas for the relationship between level of support for

the UK government and level of support for membership of the EU when controlling for age and gender.

2 The partial bivariate tables constructed for these calculations are available in appendices 2 and 3.

21

When comparing the effect of all of the independent variables

on the level of support for UK membership of the EU, the standardised

multiple regression formula

confirms the expectation that level of support for the national government

( ) has the greatest impact on levels of support for EU membership.

According to the formula, the second most influential factor is political

views ( ), followed by age ( ) and level of education ( ). The factors with

the least effect on the level of support for UK membership of the EU are

found to be level of cognitive mobilisation ( ) and gender ( ). Level of

cognitive mobilisation, level of education and level of support for the

national government all have positive overall effects on level of support for

UK membership of the EU, whereas the effects of political views, age and

gender are negative.

A calculation of the multiple correlation coefficient indicates

that there is something significant missing from this analysis, as taken all

together the variables presented here are able to explain only of the

variation in levels of support for membership of the EU ( ). This

means that of the variation in levels of support for UK membership of

the EU must be explained by other factors that have not been included here.

Discussion

The findings report that the most successful theory used to

explain levels of individual support for UK membership of the EU is proxy

theory: British individuals who report a high level of support for the UK

22

government are more likely to support UK membership of the EU. This

finding is consistent with the previous studies undertaken by Anderson

(1998), de Vries and van Kersbergen (2007), and McLaren (2007). The

relationships hypothesised for the other theories, although generally

consistent with the directions predicted (excluding the case of gender which

was opposite that hypothesised), were weaker than I expected based on the

evidence in previous studies. Most strikingly, as the main theories presented

here were only able to explain less than a quarter of the variation in levels of

support for the EU, it is clear that something important is missing from the

theories or analysis presented here and that further investigations are

required to discover what the key factors are that can better explain

individual level attitudes towards the EU in the UK.

As such, it would be foolish to claim that the results reported

here are not without flaw. Using the results of just one survey carried out at

one point in time may limit the validity of my findings, and a future study

examining the dynamics of the relationships investigated here over a longer

period of time would paint a more detailed, valid, and potentially different,

picture. It may be that, whatever the missing influential factors are, they are

too personal or non-quantifiable and as such impossible to identify and

measure the effects of, but nevertheless additional investigation is required.

It could also be, as suggested by Anderson, that the UK is a special case with

regard to what affects the public‟s views on Europe (Anderson 1998:592),

and that there is an important explanatory variable that has not been

hypothesised here which is specific to the UK. Alternatively, there may be

more effective measures of the concepts analysed, for example, an

individual‟s income has been used in some previous studies to operationalise

23

the utilitarian theory of support for the EU, on occasion with very promising

results (McLaren 2004). The use of income was not used in this essay as the

data was not available, and equally significant results have been found in

past studies when the utilitarian theory is operationalised using level of

education (Hooghe, Huo and Marks 2007; Abts et al 2009).

In conclusion, it is possible to say that of the four main

competing theories of individual level support for European integration, in

the EU‟s most Eurosceptic member state, the UK, proxy theory is the most

successful explanation for levels of support for UK membership of the EU.

In the UK, those who are more supportive of the national government are

significantly more likely to support the UK‟s membership of the EU, and

those who are less supportive of the UK government are significantly less

likely to support EU membership. However, the results have indicated that

there is something substantial missing from this model of individual EU

support, and further research is required in order to identify what it might be.

24

Appendix 1: The technical specifications of the data sampling

and collection for the Eurobarometer survey 71.1, Spring 2009.

Reproduced from European Commission (2009b:74).

“EUROBAROMETER 71.1

TECHNICAL SPECIFICATIONS

Between the 16th

of January and the 22nd

of February 2009, TNS Opinion & Social, a

consortium created between Taylor Nelson Sofres and EOS Gallup Europe, carried out

wave 71.1 of the EUROBAROMETER, on request of the EUROPEAN COMMISSION,

Directorate-General for Communication, “Research and Political Analysis”.

The EUROBAROMETER 71.1 covers the population of the respective nationalities of

the European Union Member States, resident in each of the Member States and aged 15

years and over. The EUROBAROMETER 71.1 has also been conducted in the three

candidate countries (Croatia, Turkey and the Former Yugoslav Republic of Macedonia)

and in the Turkish Cypriot Community. In these countries, the survey covers the national

population of citizens and the population of citizens of all the European Union Member

States that are residents in these countries and have a sufficient command of the national

languages to answer the questionnaire. The basic sample design applied in all states is a

multi-stage, random (probability) one. In each country, a number of sampling points was

drawn with probability proportional to population size (for a total coverage of the

country) and to population density.

In order to do so, the sampling points were drawn systematically from each of the

"administrative regional units", after stratification by individual unit and type of area. They thus represent the whole territory of the countries surveyed according to the

EUROSTAT NUTS II (or equivalent) and according to the distribution of the resident

population of the respective nationalities in terms of metropolitan, urban and rural areas. In each of the selected sampling points, a starting address was drawn, at random. Further

addresses (every Nth address) were selected by standard "random route" procedures,

from the initial address. In each household, the respondent was drawn, at random

(following the "closest birthday rule"). All interviews were conducted face-to-face in

people's homes and in the appropriate national language. As far as the data capture is

concerned, CAPI (Computer Assisted Personal Interview) was used in those countries

where this technique was available.”

25

Appendix 2: Partial bivariate tables showing the relationship

between level of support for the national government and level

of support for UK membership of the EU when controlling for

the effect of gender.

Male

Level of Support for National Government Tend not to

trust

Don‟t

know

Tend to

trust

Total

Level of

Support for

Membership

of the EU

Bad thing 169

(45.7%)

1

(6.7%)

42

(20.5)

212

(35.9%)

Neither good nor

bad

106

(28.6%)

3

(20.0%)

57

(27.8%)

166

(28.1%)

Good thing 95

(25.7%)

11

(73.3%)

106

(51.7%)

212

(35.9%)

Total 370

(100.0%)

15

(100%)

205

(100%)

590

(100%)

Gamma = 0.467

Female

Level of Support for National Government Tend not to

trust

Don‟t

know

Tend to

trust

Total

Level of

Support for

Membership

of the EU

Bad thing 172

(42.7%)

6

(16.2%)

27

(17.3%)

205

(34.4%)

Neither good nor

bad

156

(38.7%)

20

(54.1%)

54

(34.6%)

230

(38.6%)

Good thing 75

(18.6%)

11

(29.7%)

75

(48.1%)

161

(27.0%)

Total 403

(100%)

37

(100%)

156

(100%)

596

(100%)

Gamma = 0.495

26

Appendix 3: Partial bivariate tables showing the relationship

between level of support for the national government and level

of support for UK membership of the EU when controlling for

the effect of age.

Age group 15-34

Level of Support for National Government Tend not to

trust

Don‟t

know

Tend to

trust

Total

Level of

Support for

Membership

of the EU

Bad thing 40

(26.5%)

2

(10.0%)

8

(8.3%)

50

(18.7%)

Neither good nor

bad

62

(41.1%)

11

(55.0%)

39

(40.6%)

112

(41.9%)

Good thing 49

(32.5%)

7

(35.0%)

49

(51.0%)

105

(39.3%)

Total 151

(100%)

20

(100%)

96

(100%)

267

(100%)

Gamma = 0.359

Age group 35-54

Level of Support for National Government Tend not to

trust

Don‟t

know

Tend to

trust

Total

Level of

Support for

Membership

of the EU

Bad thing 99

(40.4%)

2

(15.4%)

22

(20.0%)

123

(33.4%)

Neither good nor

bad

92

(37.6%)

5

(38.5%)

31

(28.2%)

128

(34.8%)

Good thing 54

(22.0%)

6

(46.2%)

57

(51.8%)

117

(31.8%)

Total 245

(100%)

13

(100%)

110

(100%)

368

(100%)

Gamma = 0.464

27

Age group 55-74

Level of Support for National Government Tend not to

trust

Don‟t

know

Tend to

trust

Total

Level of

Support for

Membership

of the EU

Bad thing 149

(51.9%)

3

(20.0%)

22

(21.2%)

174

(42.9%)

Neither good nor

bad

82

(28.6%)

6

(40.0%)

27

(26.0%)

115

(28.3%)

Good thing 56

(19.5%)

6

(40.0%)

55

(52.9%)

117

(28.8%)

Total 287

(100%)

15

(100%)

104

(100%)

406

(100%)

Gamma = 0.548

Age group 75-100

Level of Support for National Government Tend not to

trust

Don‟t

know

Tend to

trust

Total

Level of

Support for

Membership

of the EU

Bad thing 53

(58.9%)

0

(0.0%)

17

(33.3%)

70

(48.3%)

Neither good nor

bad

26

(28.9%)

1

(25.0%)

14

(27.5%)

41

(28.3%)

Good thing 11

(12.2%)

3

(75.0%)

20

(39.2%)

34

(23.4%)

Total 90

(100%)

4

(100%)

51

(100%)

145

(100%)

Gamma = 0.489

28

Bibliography

Abts, Koen, Heerwegh, Dirk and Swyngeouw, Marc (2009) „Sources of Euroscepticism:

Utilitarian Interest, Social Distrust, National Identity and Institutional Distrust‟ in World

Political Science Review 5(1) Article 3

Anderson, Christopher J (1998) „When in Doubt Use Proxies: Attitudes towards

Domestic Politics and Support for European Integration‟ in Comparative Politics Studies

31(5) p.569-601

Anderson, Christopher J and Kaltenhaler, Karl C (1996) „The Dynamics of Public

Opinion toward European Integration, 1973-93‟ in European Journal of International

Relations 2(2) p.175-199

de Vries, Catherine E and van Kersbergen, Kees (2007) „Interests, Identity and Political

Allegiance in the European Union‟ in Acta Politica 42(2) p.307-328

European Commission (2009a) The Europeans in 2009, TNS Opinion and Social,

Available at: http://ec.europa.eu/public_opinion/archives/ebs/ebs_308_en.pdf

European Commission (2009b) Eurobarometer 71.1 January-February 2009 Basic

Bilingual Questionnaire, TNS Opinion and Social, Available at:

http://www.gesis.org/fileadmin/upload/dienstleistung/daten/umfragedaten/eurobarometer/

eb_standard/standardEBs_overview.pdf?download=true

Evans, Geoffrey and Butt, Sarah (2007) „Explaining Change in British Public Opinion on

the European Union: Top Down or Bottom Up?‟ in Acta Politica 42(2) p.173-190

Gabel, Matthew (1998) „Public Support for European Integration: An Empirical Test of

Five Theories‟ in The Journal of Politics 60(2) p.333-354

Garry, John, Marsh, Michael and Sinnott, Richard (2005) „‟Second-order‟ versus „Issue-

voting‟ Effects in EU Referendum: Evidence from the Irish Nice Treaty Referendums‟ in

European Union Politics 6(2) p.201-222

Grant, Charles (2008) Why is Britain Eurosceptic? Centre For European Reform Essays,

Centre For European Reform, London [online] Available at:

http://www.cer.org.uk/pdf/essay_eurosceptic_19dec08.pdf

Haesly, Richard (2001) „Euroskeptics, Europhiles and Instrumental Europeans: European

Attachment in Scotland and Wales‟ in European Union Politics 2(1) p.81-102

Hooghe, Liesbet and Marks, Gary (2007) „Sources of Euroscepticism‟ in Acta Politica

42(2) p.119-127

Hooghe, Liesbet, Huo, Jing Jing and Marks, Gary (2007) „Does Occupation Shape

29

Attitudes on Europe? Benchmarking Validity and Parsimony‟ in Acta Politica 42(2)

p.329-351

Inglehart, Ronald (1970) „Cognitive Mobilization and European Identity‟ in Comparative

Politics 3(1) p.45-70

Karp, Jeffrey A, Banducci, Susan A, and Bowler, Shaun (2003) „To Know it is to Love

it?: Satisfaction with Democracy in the European Union‟ in Comparative Political

Studies 36(2) p.271-292

McLaren, Lauren M (2004) „Opposition to European integration and fear of loss of

national identity: Debunking a basic assumption regarding hostility to the integration

project‟ in European Journal of Political Research 43(6) p.895-911

McLaren, Lauren (2007) „Explaining Mass-Level Euroscepticism: Identity, Interests and

Institutional Distrust‟ in Acta Politica 42(2) p.233-251

Sánchez-Cuenca, Ignacio (2000) „The Political Basis of Support for European

Integration‟ in European Union Politics 1(2) p.147-171

Spiering, Menno (2004) „British Euroscepticism‟ in European Studies 20(1) p.127-149

Tucker, Joshua A, Pacek, Alexander C and Berkinsky, Adam J (2002) „Transitional

Winners and Losers: Attitudes toward EU Membership in Post-Communist Countries‟ in

American Journal of Political Science 46(3) p.557-571