13
 Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263–275 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Agei ng Researc hReviews  j o u r n a l h o me p a g e : www.elsevier.com/locate/arr Review Cognitiveinterventions inhealthyolderadultsandpeoplewithmildcognitive impairment: Asystematicreview  JenniferReijnders a,,CarolinevanHeugten a,b ,MartinvanBoxtel a,b a Depar tment of Psych iatry and Neuropsych ology , Schoo l for Mental Healt h and Neuroscience, Maas trich t Universit y Medic al Centre, Maas trich t, The Netherlands b Depar tment of Neuropsych ology and Psychophar macol ogy, Facul ty of Psyc hology and Neurosciences, Maas trich t Universit y, Maas trich t, The Netherlands articleinfo  Article history: Rec eived 11 Apr il 2012 Rec eived in revised for m 13 Jul y 2012 Accept ed 17 Jul y 2012 Available online 25 July 2012 Keywords: Syst emati c review Cognitive interventions Healt hy older adults Mild cogni tive impair ment Randomized contr olled trial s abstract Giventhattheresearchareaof cognitiveinterventionstudiesintheagingpopulationisgrowingrapidly, itis impor tant toreviewandgaugemorerecentinterventionstudies,inordertodeterminetheevidence fortheeffectivenessof cognitiveinterventions. Thepurposeof thepresentreviewwastoupdatethe recentsystematicreviewsof Pappetal.(2009)andMartinet al .(2011),toevaluatetheeffectivenessof cognitiveinterventionsinhealthyolderadultsandpeoplewithMCI,by takin gintoaccountthemethod- ologicalqualityof theinterventionsstudies.A syst ema tic reviewof randomizedcontrolledtrials(RCT) andclinicalstudiespublishedbetweenAugust2007andFebruary2012inPubmedandPsychINFOwas performed. Thequalityof theincludedRCTs was assessedaccordingtotheCONSORTcriteriaforRCTs. A totalof thirty-vestudieswereincluded;twenty-sevenRTCsandeightclinicalstudies.Thecontent of theinterventionstudiesdifferedwidely,asdidthemethodological qualityof theincludedRCTs,but was consid erabl ylowwithanaverageof 44%of theConsortitemsincluded. Theresultsshowevidence thatcognitivetrainingcanbeeffectiveinimprovingvariousaspectsof objectivecognitivefunctioning; memoryperformance, executivefunctioning, processingspeed,attention, uidintelligence, andsubjec- tive cogni tive performance. However, theissuewhethertheeffectsof cognitiveinterventionsgeneralize toimprovement ineverydaylifeactivitiesisstillunresolvedandneedstobeaddressedmoreexplicitly infutureresearch. © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introducti on The pr opor ti on of ol der indi vi duals in our western soci et y is gr owing rapi dl y (CBS, 2009). As a c onseque nc e a la rge r amo un t of people encounter dif culti es in execut ive fun ctioning , memory, and spe ed of inf ormati on processing (Glisky, 2007). Itis anongoi ng deb atewhet hersuch dif culti es canbe pos tponedor even revert ed in a normal aging populati on, but overal l the general held opinion among re sear cher s in this eld ha s been opti mi st ic (Lust ig et al ., 2009; Valen zuelaand Sachd ev, 2009). Cognitive training programs, inv olv ing gui ded pra cti ce of spe cic cog nit ive tas ks, and cognitive stimul ati on progr ams, aimed at enh anc ing gen eral cognit ive fun c- tionin g, have been of fered to pr event or mi ni mi ze the ef fe ct s of  cognitive aging. The effects of such cognit ive interventi ons were investigatednot onlyin heal thyolder adul ts, but alsoin people with cogn itivedeteriorati on beyon d thenormalage-depen dentchanges, Cor res ponding aut hor at: Maastr icht Uni ver sit y, School for Mental Hea lth and Neuros cience, Depart ment of Psychiat ry and Neuropsyc hol ogy, PO Box 616, 6200 MDMaastr icht, The Net her lands. Tel .: +31 43 3881029; fax: +31 43 3884092. E-mai l addres s: jennifer.reijnders @maastrichtuniversity.n l (J. Reijnders) . for exampl e people with mi ld cognitive impairment (MCI ) (Lustig et al., 2009; Val enz uel a and Sac hde v, 200 9). In the las t dec ade severa l reviews hav e been conduc ted int o the effect s of cognitive interventions in heal thy olderadults and peopl e wit h MCI (Val enz uel a andSachdev, 2009; Jea n et al. , 201 0a; Mar tin et al ., 2011 ; Pa pp et al ., 2009; Ta rdif an d Si ma rd, 20 11; Teixei ra et al ., 2012). Papp et al. (2009 ) perf ormed a sys temati c reviewwit h met a-a nalyt ic techniques to analyze ran domized contro lled tri als (RCT) of cognitive interventi ons in heal thy ol der adul ts. Ten RCTs publ ished be tween 19 96 and 2007 were incl uded and anal yz ed in their review. A mean effect size of 0.16 was found across all outcome measures. Ef fect sizes were larger when outcome meas- ur es were di re ct ly rela te d to type of traini ng . Th eir conc lusi on wa s that tr aini ng impr ov es immedi at e perf or ma nc e on rela ted tasks but there was no evidence for generali zati on effects to over- al l cogn it ive functionin g. Th ey also stat ed th at thei r re vi ew was limite d by sma ll and het erogen eous studies wi th lowmet hod olog- ical ly qual ity and more RCTs are needed with sufcient foll ow-up ti me, matched acti ve cont rol gr oups and outcome measur es that can show chan ges in mo re th an one speci c doma in (Pa pp et al ., 2009). The r evie w of  Ma rt in et al . (201 1) included, in compar i- sonto Pa pp et al. (2 009),more studies fr om both he al th y ol de r adultsandMCIpopulations.Atotalofthirty-sixRTCswereincluded, 1568-1637/$ seefrontmatter © 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.arr.2012.07.003

Cognitive Interventions in Healthy Older Adults and People With Mild Cognitive

  • Upload
    icaro

  • View
    227

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Cognitive interventions in healthy older adults and people with mild cognitive.

Citation preview

  • Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275

    Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

    Ageing Research Reviews

    j ourna l ho mepage: www.elsev ier .c

    Review

    Cognitive interventions in healthy older adults animpair

    Jennifer Boxa Department o icht Ub Department o , Maa

    a r t i c l

    Article history:Received 11 AReceived in reAccepted 17 JuAvailable onlin

    Keywords:Systematic revCognitive interventionsHealthy older adultsMild cognitive impairmentRandomized controlled trials

    ive inore rervenal. (2er adtudien Aud RC

    A total of thirty-ve studies were included; twenty-seven RTCs and eight clinical studies. The contentof the intervention studies differed widely, as did the methodological quality of the included RCTs, butwas considerably low with an average of 44% of the Consort items included. The results show evidencethat cognitive training can be effective in improving various aspects of objective cognitive functioning;memory performance, executive functioning, processing speed, attention, uid intelligence, and subjec-

    1. Introdu

    The progrowing raof people enand speed odebate whein a normalamong rese2009; Valeninvolving gstimulationtioning, havcognitive ainvestigatedcognitive de

    CorresponNeuroscience,MD Maastrich

    E-mail add

    1568-1637/$ http://dx.doi.otive cognitive performance. However, the issue whether the effects of cognitive interventions generalizeto improvement in everyday life activities is still unresolved and needs to be addressed more explicitlyin future research.

    2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

    ction

    portion of older individuals in our western society ispidly (CBS, 2009). As a consequence a larger amountcounter difculties in executive functioning, memory,f information processing (Glisky, 2007). It is an ongoingther such difculties can be postponed or even reverted

    aging population, but overall the general held opinionarchers in this eld has been optimistic (Lustig et al.,zuela and Sachdev, 2009). Cognitive training programs,uided practice of specic cognitive tasks, and cognitive

    programs, aimed at enhancing general cognitive func-e been offered to prevent or minimize the effects of

    ging. The effects of such cognitive interventions were not only in healthy older adults, but also in people withterioration beyond the normal age-dependent changes,

    ding author at: Maastricht University, School for Mental Health and Department of Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, PO Box 616, 6200t, The Netherlands. Tel.: +31 43 3881029; fax: +31 43 3884092.ress: [email protected] (J. Reijnders).

    for example people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (Lustiget al., 2009; Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2009).

    In the last decade several reviews have been conducted into theeffects of cognitive interventions in healthy older adults and peoplewith MCI (Valenzuela and Sachdev, 2009; Jean et al., 2010a; Martinet al., 2011; Papp et al., 2009; Tardif and Simard, 2011; Teixeiraet al., 2012). Papp et al. (2009) performed a systematic review withmeta-analytic techniques to analyze randomized controlled trials(RCT) of cognitive interventions in healthy older adults. Ten RCTspublished between 1996 and 2007 were included and analyzedin their review. A mean effect size of 0.16 was found across alloutcome measures. Effect sizes were larger when outcome meas-ures were directly related to type of training. Their conclusionwas that training improves immediate performance on relatedtasks but there was no evidence for generalization effects to over-all cognitive functioning. They also stated that their review waslimited by small and heterogeneous studies with low methodolog-ically quality and more RCTs are needed with sufcient follow-uptime, matched active control groups and outcome measures thatcan show changes in more than one specic domain (Papp et al.,2009). The review of Martin et al. (2011) included, in compari-son to Papp et al. (2009), more studies from both healthy olderadults and MCI populations. A total of thirty-six RTCs were included,

    see front matter 2012 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.rg/10.1016/j.arr.2012.07.003ment: A systematic review

    Reijndersa,, Caroline van Heugtena,b, Martin van f Psychiatry and Neuropsychology, School for Mental Health and Neuroscience, Maastrf Neuropsychology and Psychopharmacology, Faculty of Psychology and Neurosciences

    e i n f o

    pril 2012vised form 13 July 2012ly 2012e 25 July 2012

    iew

    a b s t r a c t

    Given that the research area of cognitit is important to review and gauge mfor the effectiveness of cognitive interecent systematic reviews of Papp et cognitive interventions in healthy oldological quality of the interventions sand clinical studies published betweeperformed. The quality of the includeom/ locate /ar r

    d people with mild cognitive

    tela,b

    niversity Medical Centre, Maastricht, The Netherlandsstricht University, Maastricht, The Netherlands

    tervention studies in the aging population is growing rapidly,cent intervention studies, in order to determine the evidencetions. The purpose of the present review was to update the009) and Martin et al. (2011), to evaluate the effectiveness ofults and people with MCI, by taking into account the method-s. A systematic review of randomized controlled trials (RCT)gust 2007 and February 2012 in Pubmed and PsychINFO wasTs was assessed according to the CONSORT criteria for RCTs.

  • 264 J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275

    published bing could adults, immthrough tracompared tno longer sistrated the a review of cognitive inteen studieconclusion outcome mand contendifcult to rather difc

    Given thgrowing rapinterventioMCI, in ordFig. 1. Selection process of the systematic

    etween 1985 and 2007. Only data on memory train-be pooled. Results showed that, for healthy olderediate and delayed verbal recall improved signicantlyining compared to a no-treatment control group. Wheno an active control condition, the improvements weregnicant. For individuals with MCI, the analysis demon-same pattern (Martin et al., 2011). Only quite recently,Tardif and Simard (2011) was published, who reviewedterventions in healthy older adults and included four-s, published between 2001 and March 2011. Theirwas that improvements were observed on at least oneeasure in each study. They also noted that the qualityt of the studies included were very heterogeneous andcompare which made generalization to everyday lifeult (Tardif and Simard, 2011).at the research area of cognitive intervention studies isidly, it is important to review and gauge more recent

    n studies in both healthy older adults and people wither to determine the evidence for the effectiveness of

    cognitive inthat one shof the studioutcome mtive outcomThe purposeffectivenepeople withological qua

    2. Method

    A systemwas done iincluded stFebruary 14lier perform2009). Our following te review.

    terventions. In doing so, it is important to recognizeould consider the heterogeneity in quality and contentes and interventions and to include not only objectiveeasures (e.g. test performance) but also the subjec-e of such interventions (e.g. cognitive complaints).

    e of the present review was therefore to evaluate thess of cognitive interventions in healthy older adults and

    MCI, by taking into account the content and method-lity of the interventions studies.

    s

    atic literature search for cognitive intervention studiesn two databases: Pubmed and PsychINFO. Our searchudies that were performed between August 1, 2007 and, 2012, as we wanted to build on the results of two ear-ed systematic reviews (Martin et al., 2011; Papp et al.,initial search strategy included the intersection of therms: [cognitive intervention OR cognitive training OR

  • J. Reijnders

    et al.

    / A

    geing Research

    Review

    s 12 (2013) 263 275

    265Table 1Intervention characteristics of studies in healthy older adults (n = 21).

    Study Aim of intervention Intervention Duration Total hours ofintervention

    Control group

    Bailey et al. (2010) Improving ability to regulatelearning

    Training metacognitive skills at home such asself-testing and efcient time allocation

    2 weeks manual with lessonsand 4 assignments

    NC Waiting list

    Basak et al. (2008) To improve executive functions A real-time strategy game (rise of nation) thatcombines the speed of real-time gaming and thecomplexity of strategy games

    15 training sessions of 1.5 h45 weeks

    22.5 h No training and nocontact

    Berry et al. (2010) To improve perceptual abilities Visual cognitive training using the Sweep Seekerprogram (Posit science)

    35 sessions per week. Total of10 h training

    10 h No training

    Borella et al. (2010) To improve performance onWM tasks and generalizationto untrained task

    Verbal WM training using the categorization workingmemory span task

    3 sessions of 60 min 3 h Active control

    Bottiroli and Cavallini (2009) Improve the use of memorystrategies and transfer effectsto non-practiced tasks

    Teaching 2 memory strategies and practicing on 3computerized memory tasks which were adapted fromthe neuropsychological training software

    3 sessions of 2 h 6 h Waiting list

    Buiza et al. (2008) To improve cognitive functionsand quality of life

    Cognitive therapy based on Braak and Braaks model ofAlzheimers staging addressing memory, attention,language, visuoconstructive ability, executivefunctions, visuo-motor coordination, and praxis (EG1and 2). In EG1 issues related to well-being werediscussed

    2 years, total of 180 session 270 h No training

    Buschkuehl et al. (2008) To improve memoryperformance in oldold adults

    Computerized WM training 3 months2 sessions per week of 45 min

    18 h Active control(physicalintervention)

    Carlson et al. (2008) To enhance physical, social,and cognitive activity and, indoing so, exercise memory andexecutive functions

    Community-based program Experience Corps to helpelementary school children with reading achievement,library support and classroom behavior

    1 year15 h per week

    630 h Waiting list

    Cavallini et al. (2010) Instruction-based memorytraining to promote transfereffects to other memory tasks

    Training in mnemonics (EG1) and in EG2: alsoinstructions how to apply these to other tasks, imageryand sentence generation were practiced

    4 sessions of 2 h 8 h No training

    Fairchild and Scogin (2010) To improve both objective andsubjective memory

    Memory enhancement program addressing objectiveand subjective memory; information about agingprocess; nutrition; mood; controllability of memoryand teaching mnemonics

    6 weeks1 session per week of3060 min

    6 h Minimal socialsupport

    Hastings and West (2009) To improve self-efcacy andmemory performance

    Multi-factorial training program centred on increasingself-efcacy, including strategy training, classdiscussions, homework reading and practice exercisesSelf help participants used a manual containing thesame strategy training material and practice exercises

    6 sessions of 2 h 12 h Waiting list

    Klusmann et al. (2010) To increase cognitiveperformance

    Computer course (EG1): creative, coordinative, andmemory tasksPhysical exercise course (EG2): aerobic endurance,strength, and exibility training

    6 months75 session of 90 min

    112.5 h No contact

    McDougall et al. (2010) To increase memoryself-efcacy

    Memory training based on cognitive behavioral modelof everyday memory derived from Bandurasself-efcacy theory (SeniorWISE study)

    8 sessions and 4 boostersessions

    18 h Health promotiontraining

    Mozolic et al. (2011) To help people suppressirrelevant auditory and visualstimuli

    Individual training program focused on visual andauditory selective attention

    8 sessions of 1 h per week 8 h Educational lecture

    Noice and Noice (2009) To improve cognitive functionand affective measures

    Offering a specic type of novel, stimulating,multi-modal activity: acting course

    8 sessions of 1 h2 sessions per week

    8 h Waiting listAdditional controlgroup (CG2):singing course

    Richmond et al. (2011) To improve performance onWM tasks and generalizationto untrained task

    Complex WM training using both a verbal and a spatialWM tasks

    45 weeks5 days per week2030 min per day

    12.5 h Active control

  • 266 J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275

    Tabl

    e

    1

    (Con

    tinu

    ed)

    Study

    Aim

    of

    inte

    rven

    tion

    Inte

    rven

    tion

    Dura

    tion

    Tota

    l hou

    rs

    ofin

    terv

    ention

    Con

    trol

    grou

    p

    Sleg

    ers

    et

    al. (

    2008

    )

    To

    impro

    ve

    the

    qual

    ity

    of

    life

    ofol

    der

    adults

    Firs

    t

    trai

    nin

    g

    in

    com

    pute

    r

    use

    ;

    during

    inte

    rven

    tion

    assign

    men

    ts

    on

    the

    com

    pute

    r

    and

    inte

    rnet

    Trai

    nin

    g:

    3

    sess

    ions

    of

    4

    hIn

    terv

    ention

    :

    16

    assign

    men

    tsduring

    1

    year

    431

    h

    CG1:

    trai

    nin

    g,

    no

    inte

    rven

    tion

    CG2:

    no

    trai

    nin

    g,no

    inte

    rven

    tion

    CG3:

    no

    conta

    ctSm

    ith

    et

    al. (

    2009

    )

    To

    impro

    ve

    spee

    d

    and

    accu

    racy

    of

    auditor

    y

    info

    rmat

    ion

    pro

    cess

    ing

    Plas

    ticity

    -bas

    ed

    Adap

    tive

    Cog

    nitiv

    e

    Trai

    nin

    g

    (IM

    PACT)

    :6

    exer

    cise

    s

    with

    adju

    stin

    g

    difcu

    lty

    8

    wee

    ks5

    day

    s

    per

    wee

    k1

    h

    per

    day

    40

    h

    Act

    ive

    control

    conditio

    n

    (gen

    eral

    cogn

    itiv

    est

    imula

    tion

    pro

    gram

    )Ta

    lib

    et

    al. (

    2008

    )

    To

    impro

    ve

    cogn

    itiv

    eper

    form

    ance

    and

    PLA

    2ac

    tivi

    tyM

    emor

    y

    trai

    nin

    g:

    educa

    tion

    , mnem

    onic

    stra

    tegi

    es,

    atte

    ntion

    and, w

    orki

    ng

    mem

    ory

    exer

    cise

    s4

    sess

    ions

    of

    90

    min

    6 h

    Stan

    dar

    dou

    tpat

    ient

    care

    Tran

    ter

    and

    Kou

    tsta

    al

    (200

    8)

    To

    enhan

    ce

    uid

    inte

    llig

    ence

    per

    form

    ance

    Div

    erse

    and

    nov

    el

    men

    tally

    stim

    ula

    ting

    activi

    ties

    101

    2

    wee

    ks2

    h

    per

    wee

    k24

    h

    Social

    grou

    pm

    eetings

    Wes

    t

    et

    al. (

    2008

    )To

    impro

    ve

    self-e

    fca

    cy

    and

    mem

    ory

    per

    form

    ance

    Multi-fa

    ctor

    ial t

    rain

    ing

    pro

    gram

    centred

    on

    incr

    easing

    self-e

    fca

    cy, includin

    g

    stra

    tegy

    trai

    nin

    g,

    clas

    sdiscu

    ssio

    ns,

    hom

    ewor

    k

    read

    ing,

    and

    pra

    ctice

    exer

    cise

    s

    6

    sess

    ions

    of

    2

    h

    12

    h

    Wai

    ting

    list

    NC, n

    ot

    calcula

    ble;

    EG, e

    xper

    imen

    tal g

    roup;

    CG, c

    ontrol

    grou

    p;

    WM

    , wor

    king

    mem

    ory.

    cognitive stimulation OR cognitive rehabilitation OR brain train-ing OR memory training OR mnemonic training] AND [healthyelderly OR older adults OR aging OR mild cognitive impairmentOR MCI]. This search was supplemented by manual search fromthe referenof missing rPapp et al. (used by two

    The folloas eligible:study popuMCI, (3) antive and/orif the langusame studywas found original stuassessmentthe original

    After thedata indepestudies usinfor random(Schulz et aconsensus wity of the stConsort staheterogenecally pool thmeta-analy

    3. Results

    3.1. Studies

    In Fig. 1 thirty-ve sRTCs and eiseparately. studies incl2010; BasaBottiroli an2008; CarlsScogin, 201McDougall 2009; Rich2009; Talib2008) and ple with Met al., 20092008). Tabltion prograTables 3 anincluding o

    3.2. Study c

    The numvaried fromThe mean aet al., 2010)carried outtion was uset al., 2008et al., 2010ces of selected articles. In order to minimize the chanceelevant studies we extended the search terms used by2009) and also used a combination of the search terms

    earlier reviews (Martin et al., 2011; Papp et al., 2009).wing inclusion criteria were used to identify studies

    (1) randomized controlled trial or clinical study, (2)lation consisting of healthy older adults or people withy type of cognitive intervention, and (4) use of objec-

    subjective outcome measures. Studies were excludedage was not English. Furthermore, papers based on the

    population were excluded. When more than one paperabout a specic data set, only the main paper on thedy data was included for further analysis and quality. If the original study was published before 2007 then

    study was also not included. nal selection, two reviewers (JR and MvB) extractedndently and assessed the methodological quality of theg a standardized form of the Consort statement 2010ized controlled trials (www.consort-statement.org)

    l., 2010). In case of discrepancies between the two raters,as achieved after discussion. The methodological qual-udies was indicated by the percentage of items of thetement reported in the articles. Given the considerableity of the interventions it was decided not to statisti-e data of the studies in order to perform a quantitativesis.

    included

    the results of the selection process are shown. A total oftudies were selected for further analysis; twenty-sevenght clinical studies. The clinical studies will be discussedOf the twenty-seven RCTs, twenty-one interventionuded a population of healthy older adults (Bailey et al.,k et al., 2008; Berry et al., 2010; Borella et al., 2010;d Cavallini, 2009; Buiza et al., 2008; Buschkuehl et al.,on et al., 2008; Cavallini et al., 2010; Fairchild and0; Hastings and West, 2009; Klusmann et al., 2010;et al., 2010; Mozolic et al., 2011; Noice and Noice,mond et al., 2011; Slegers et al., 2008; Smith et al.,

    et al., 2008; Tranter and Koutstaal, 2008; West et al.,six intervention studies included a population of peo-CI (Barnes et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010b; Kinsella

    ; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011; Wagner et al.,es 1 and 2 present the characteristics of the interven-ms (for healthy older adults population and MCI) andd 4 present the characteristics of the included RCTs,utcome measures and effects.

    haracteristics

    ber of healthy older adults in the experimental groups 13 (Buschkuehl et al., 2008) to 242 (Smith et al., 2009).ge of the study populations ranged from 63.5 (Cavallini

    to 80.2 years (Noice and Noice, 2009). All studies were in an experimental setting. A group-based interven-ed in ten studies (Bottiroli and Cavallini, 2009; Buiza; Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Carlson et al., 2008; Cavallini; Klusmann et al., 2010; McDougall et al., 2010; Noice

  • J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275 267

    Table 2Intervention characteristics of studies in MCI population (n = 6).

    Study Aim of intervention Intervention Duration Total hours ofintervention

    Control group

    Barnes et al. 1005 da6 w

    Jean et al. (2 6 se45 m

    Kinsella et a 1.5 5 w

    Troyer et al. 10 s120

    Tsolaki et al. 60 s90 m3 se

    Wagner et a 7 se90 m

    and Noice, number of set al., 2010;Fairchild an2011; Slege2008) and studies inclvention (BoWest, 2009

    The numied from 11mean age o2008) to 78ies, the inteet al., 2009et al., 2008)(Kinsella et et al., 2008)vention (Baeffectivenesately after (Kinsella etassessment

    The totaas can be seanalysis to icould be prtiveness of of signicanmeasures. Winterventiothat in the ship betwethe interven

    scrip

    le 1 sed

    moryput (2009) To improve auditoryprocessing speed and accuracy

    Computer-based cognitivetraining developed by PositScience Corporation

    010b) To re-learn face-nameassociations

    Training in face-nameassociations using botherrorless learning and spacedretrieval paradigm andpsycho-educational sessionson memory

    l. (2009) Memory rehabilitation, topractice memory strategies foreveryday problems

    Practice in memory strategies(internal and external),strategies for improvingorganisational and attentionalskills and general copingstrategies

    (2008) Multidisciplinary group-basedintervention to changeeveryday memory behavior

    Practice and application ofmemory strategies andeducation about lifestyle:nutrition, recreation andrelaxation

    (2011) Holistic cognitiverehabilitation program to coverthe needs and deciencies ofpatients with MCI

    Practice of attention andparameters of executivefunction; enhancement ofmental imagery, episodic,semantic, autobiographical andvisual memory;relaxation techniques

    l. (2008) To analyze everyday situationswith memory problem andtrain specic strategies

    Practicing prospective memoryand structured processing ofnew information related toreal-life situations

    2009; Talib et al., 2008; West et al., 2008), the sametudies used an individually based intervention (Bailey

    Basak et al., 2008; Berry et al., 2010; Borella et al., 2010;d Scogin, 2010; Mozolic et al., 2011; Richmond et al.,rs et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2009; Tranter and Koutstaal,one study used both (Hastings and West, 2009). Five

    3.3. De

    Tabperforming meby comuded follow-up data, up till one year after the inter-rella et al., 2010; Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Hastings and; Slegers et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2009).ber of people with MCI in the intervention groups var-

    (Jean et al., 2010b) to 104 (Tsolaki et al., 2011). Thef the study populations ranged from 53.7 (Wagner et al.,

    years (Kinsella et al., 2009). In four (out of six) stud-rventions were carried out in a clinical setting (Barnes; Kinsella et al., 2009; Tsolaki et al., 2011; Wagner. A group-based intervention was used in four studiesal., 2009; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011; Wagner, whereas two studies used an individually based inter-rnes et al., 2009; Jean et al., 2010b). In two studies thes of the intervention was not only assessed immedi-the intervention but also after three or four months

    al., 2009; Troyer et al., 2008). In one study a follow-up was planned (Wagner et al., 2008).l hours of intervention varied widely between studies,en in Tables 1 and 2. We performed a linear regressionnvestigate whether the effectiveness of the interventionedicted by the total hours of intervention. The effec-an intervention was calculated by dividing the numbert outcome measures by the total number of outcomee found no signicant effect between total hours of

    n and intervention effect (R2: 0.016, p = 0.54), indicatingincluded studies there was no dose response relation-en total hours of intervention and the effectiveness oftion.

    2008; Richmtask (Borellof memory2010; Taliblessons (Faies aimed atraining proa memory theory (Mccognitive fu(Klusmann tive cognititraining (Buet al., 2008this, single ing metacoby a real-timby training intelligence2008). Ten seleven usedtion of an insessions (Bo

    Table 2 in people wory performKinsella et aet al., 2008 min/dayys/weekeeks

    50 h More passive computeractivities

    ssions ofin

    4.5 h Training in face-nameassociations using errorlesslearning paradigm andpsycho-educational sessionson memory

    h/weekeeks

    7.5 h Waiting list

    essions of min

    20 h Waiting list

    essions ofin

    ssion/week

    90 h Waiting list

    ssions ofin

    10.5 h No training

    tion of interventions

    hows the characteristics of the interventions that werein healthy older adults. Eleven studies aimed at improv-

    performance; four studies addressed working memoryerized training (Berry et al., 2010; Buschkuehl et al.,

    ond et al., 2011) or training of the categorization span

    a et al., 2010); four studies provided training of the use strategies (Bottiroli and Cavallini, 2009; Cavallini et al.,

    et al., 2008), accompanied with psycho-educationalirchild and Scogin, 2010) and the other three stud-t improving memory self-efcacy by a multifactorialgram (Hastings and West, 2009; West et al., 2008) or

    training program derived from Banduras self-efcacyDougall et al., 2010). Six studies aimed at improvingnctioning in general by providing a computer courseet al., 2010; Slegers et al., 2008), a plasticity-based adap-ve training (Smith et al., 2009), a multifactorial cognitiveiza et al., 2008), a community-based program (Carlson) or an acting course (Noice and Noice, 2009). Besidesstudies aimed at improving learning abilities by train-gnitive skills (Bailey et al., 2010), executive functioning

    e strategy game (Basak et al., 2008), attentional skillsa selective attention task (Mozolic et al., 2011) and uid

    by novel stimulating activities (Tranter and Koutstaal,tudies used an active control condition, where the other

    a waiting list or no training control group. The dura-tervention varied between 3 hourly sessions up till 180rella et al., 2010; Buiza et al., 2008).

    shows the characteristics of the interventions assessedith MCI. Most interventions aimed at improving mem-ance by training memory strategies (Jean et al., 2010b;l., 2009; Troyer et al., 2008; Tsolaki et al., 2011; Wagner), accompanied with psycho-education on memory

  • 268J.

    Reijnders

    et al.

    / A

    geing Research

    Review

    s 12 (2013) 263 275

    Table 3Study characteristics of intervention studies in healthy older adults (n = 21).

    Study Sample size(EG/CG)

    Mean age(year)

    Intervention Follow-up Outcome measures of interest Signicant effect forexperimental groupd

    Bailey et al. (2010) 29/27 Range 6089 Metacognitive trainingat home

    Post-intervention Self-paced memory taskExperimenter-paced memory task

    +0

    Basak et al. (2008) 20/20 69.1/70.0 Strategy-basedreal-time videogame

    Post-intervention Operation spanTask switchingN-back taskVisual short-term memoryRavens Advanced Progressive MatricesStopping taskFunctional eld of viewAttentional blinkEnumerationMental rotation

    0++++0000+

    Berry et al. (2010) 15/15 71.9 Perceptualdiscrimination training

    Post-intervention Trained perceptual taskUntrained perceptual taskWM/delayed recognition paradigm NIWM/delayed recognition paradigm IS

    +++0

    Borella et al. (2010) 20/20 69.0/69.2 Verbal WM training Post-interventionand 8 monthsfollow up

    Visuospatial WM (dot matrix task)Short-term memory (digit span)Inhibition (stroop color task)Processing speed (pattern comparisontask)Fluid intelligence (Catells Culture Fair)

    ++++ (also at FU)+ (also at FU)

    Bottiroli and Cavallini(2009)

    21/23 66.2/66.1 Computer-basedmemory training

    Post-intervention Figure list recognitionWord list recognitionFace-name learningPlace-word learningPaired associated recall

    +++++

    Buiza et al. (2008) 85/68/85a 74.4 Cognitive therapyEG1: Cognitiveintervention andwell-beingEG2: cognitive therapy

    Post-intervention Immediate memoryRecent logic execution memoryShort-term memoryWorking memoryLearning potentialVisomanual coordinationMotor executionPhonetic uencyCapacity for abstractionPraxis

    + (for EG1)00+ (for EG1)+ (for EG1)0000+ (for EG1)

    Buschkuehl et al.(2008)

    13/19 80.1 Computerized WMtraining

    Post-interventionand 1 year FU

    Visual free recallVerbal free recallDigit spanBlock span

    +00+

    Carlson et al. (2008) 70/58 70.1/68.4 Community-basedprogram

    At 4 and 8 months Word list memoryTrail Making TestRey-Osterrieth Complex gure testPsychomotor speed

    +++0

    Cavallini et al. (2010) 27/24/29a 65.8/63.566.0

    Memory trainingEG1: training inmnemonicsEG2: training inmnemonics andinstructions

    Post-intervention Associate learning (practiced task)List learning (practiced task)Text learningPlace learningName-face learningGrocery list learning

    + (EG1 = EG2)+ (EG1 = EG2)+ (EG2 > EG1)000

  • J. Reijnders

    et al.

    / A

    geing Research

    Review

    s 12 (2013) 263 275

    269

    Fairchild and Scogin(2010)

    28/25 72.4 Memory enhancementprogram

    Post-intervention Names and Faces taskAppointment Keeping taskMultifactorial Memory QuestionnaireObject Misplacement taskTarget Complaints Methodology

    +0+++

    Hastings and West(2009)

    99/45/41a 70.9 Memory trainingEG1: group-based EG2:self help

    Post-intervention(only performancemeasures) and 1month FU

    Memory Self-Efcacy QuestionnaireMetamemory in Adulthood (3 subscales)-locus of control-anxiety-achievementName recallStory recallShopping list recall

    + (for EG1 at FU)

    + (EG1 and EG2, at FU)00+ (EG1 and EG2, also at FU)+ (EG1 and EG2, also at FU)0

    Klusmann et al. (2010)e 92/91/76a 73.6 Computer course (EG1)and physical exercisecourse (EG2)

    Post-intervention Rivermead Behavioral Memory TestFree and Cued Selective Reminding TestVerbal uencyStroop testTrail Making TestSubjective ratings of-memory-concentration-exibility-physical health-physical well-being

    + (EG1 and EG2)+ (due to worsening in CG)00+ (due to worsening in CG)

    + (EG1)+ (EG1)0+ (EG2)0

    McDougall et al. (2010) 135/130 75 Memory training At 2, 6, 14 and 26months

    Mini-Mental State ExamHopkins Verbal Learning Test revisedBrief Visuospatial Memory TestRivermead Behavioral Memory TestDirect Assessment of Functional StatusMemory Self-Efcacy QuestionnaireSpielberger State-Trait Anxiety InventoryCenters for Epidemiologic Studies ScaleMetamemory in Adulthood

    + (only at 2 months)0000000+

    Mozolic et al. (2011) 33/33 69.4 Modality specicattention training

    Post-intervention Selective attention task (cross-modal)Selective attention task (within-modality)Multisensory integration taskSymbol Digit Modalities TestWalk and talk paradigmLetter n-back testStroop testTrail Making TestHopkins verbal learning testProle of Mood StatesHealth status Questionnaire

    +0+++000000

    Noice and Noice (2009) 42/40/40b 80.2/82.7/81.6 Multi-modal activitiesin college acting classesEG: acting courseCG1: no interventionCG2: singing course

    Post-intervention Word list recallDelayed word list recallCategory FluencyDigit SpanStory Recall TaskProblem SolvingSelf-reported Personal Growth

    Memory Controllability InventoryLifestyle Activities Questionnaire

    ++0+ (only EG compared toCG1)+0 (improved in EG andCG2)00

  • 270J.

    Reijnders

    et al.

    / A

    geing Research

    Review

    s 12 (2013) 263 275

    Table 3 (Continued)

    Study Sample size(EG/CG)

    Mean age(year)

    Intervention Follow-up Outcome measures of interest Signicant effect forexperimental groupd

    Richmond et al. (2011) 21/19 66 Verbal and spatial WM training Post-intervention Reading spanDigit span forwardDigit span backwardRavens Advanced Progressive MatricesTest of everyday attentionCVLT: total correctCVLT: repetitionsCVLT: intrusionsSelf-reports: memorySelf-reports: attention

    +00000+0++

    Slegers et al. (2008)f 60/49/55/40c Range 6475 Computer training and InternetUsageEG: training and interventionCG1: training, no interventionCG2: no training, no interventionCG3: no contact

    Post-intervention and 12 monthsFU

    Short-Form Health Survey (SF-36)Loneliness questionnaireSymptom Check List 90Development and activity (self-report)Instrumental Activities of Daily LivingBelief in External Control ScaleMastery ScaleComputer useVisual verbal learning testMotor choice reaction time testLetter digit substitution testConcept shifting testStroop color word testCognitive Failure Questionnaire

    00000000000000

    Smith et al. (2009) 242/245 75.6/75.0 Computerized cognitive training Post-intervention and 3 months FU RBANS (auditory memory and attention)Rey Auditory Verbal Learning TestRivermead Behavioral Memory TestDigit span backwards testLetter-number sequencingProcessing speedCognitive Self-Report Questionnaire

    ++ (also at FU)0++ (also at FU)+ (also at FU)+

    Talib et al. (2008) 11/12 67.8/67.3 Memory training Post-intervention Prose recallList recallWAIS digit symbolNumber of underlined words in short textCategorization measure

    +0+0+

    Tranter and Koutstaal(2008)

    22/22 67.8 Mentally stimulating activities Post-intervention Catells Culture Fair (uid intelligence)WAIS-R blocks (Spatial perceptual test)

    ++

    West et al. (2008) 42/42 69.9 Group-based memory training Post-intervention at week 6 andweek 9

    Memory Self-Efcacy QuestionnaireMetamemory in Adulthood (3 subscales)-locus of control-anxiety-achievementName recallStory recallShopping list recallDigit symbol substitution testStrategy checklisteffective strategy use

    +

    +00++00+

    EG, experimental group; CG, control group; N, number of patients; FU, follow-up; WM, working memory; NI, no interference task; IS, interrupting stimulus task; RBANS, Repeatable Battery for the Assessment of NeuropsychologicalStatus; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test.

    a Two experimental groups.b Two control groups.c Three control groups.d +, positive effect for experimental group; 0, no difference between groups; , negative effect for experimental group.e Subjective measures reported in Klusmann et al. (2011).f Objective outcome measures reported in Slegers et al. (2009).

  • J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275 271

    Table 4Study characteristics of intervention studies in MCI population (n = 6).

    Study Sample size(EG/CG)

    Mean age(year)

    Intervention Follow-up Outcome measures of interest Signicant effect forexperimental groupa

    Barnes et al.

    Jean et al. (2

    Kinsella et a

    Troyer et al.

    Tsolaki et al.

    Wagner et a

    MCI, mild cognMMQ, Multifa

    a +, positiveb Improved

    (Jean et al., 2tional skillsaimed at imcognitive tran active colist or no trvaried betw2011).

    3.4. Outcom

    In the tour analysiobjective menteen studafter the intfollowing taing memoret al., 2010;et al., 2011;(Bottiroli anand Cavallinory and lea(Carlson et (2009) 22/25 74 Computer-basedauditoryprocessing speedtraining

    Post-intervention

    010b) 11/11 68.5 Training inface-nameassociations

    Post-interventionat 1 and 4 weeks

    l. (2009) 22/22 78 Strategy training(memory,attention, andorganisationalskills)

    Post-interventionat 2 weeks and 4months

    (2008) 24/24 75.4 Memory strategiestraining andlifestyle education

    Post-interventionand 3 months

    (2011) 104/72 67 Cognitiverehabilitation

    Post-interventionprogram

    l. (2008) 33/40 53.7 Behavioral analysisand memorystrategy training

    Post-interventionand follow upplanned

    itive impairment; EG, experimental group; CG, control group; N, number of patients; RBActorial Memory Questionnaire; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment.

    effect for experimental group; 0, no difference between groups; , negative effect for exin both groups.

    010b), lifestyle (Troyer et al., 2008), or practice of atten- (Kinsella et al., 2009; Tsolaki et al., 2011). One studyproving processing speed by using a computer-basedaining program (Barnes et al., 2009). Two studies usedntrol condition, where the other four used a waitingaining control group. The duration of an interventioneen 5 and 20 weeks (Kinsella et al., 2009; Tsolaki et al.,

    e measures and effects

    wenty-one RCTs in healthy older adults included ins, the most commonly used outcome measure wasemory performance (twenty studies) (Table 3). In sev-ies, memory performance was signicantly improvedervention. Signicant differences were observed on thesks; self-paced memory task (Bailey et al., 2010), work-y tasks (Basak et al., 2008; Berry et al., 2010; Borella

    Buiza et al., 2008; Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Richmond Smith et al., 2009; Talib et al., 2008), recognition taskd Cavallini, 2009), face-name learning tasks (Bottirolii, 2009; Fairchild and Scogin, 2010), immediate mem-rning potential (Buiza et al., 2008), word list recallal., 2008; Cavallini et al., 2010; Noice and Noice, 2009),

    name recallrecall (CavaNoice, 2009ioral Memothe AssessmRey Auditostrategy usies, measurve studiesswitching (et al., 2010)Complex Fities Test (Met al., 2011)In three stuwere obserTranter andces (Basak on attentionsensory intfor AssessmIn two studwas observstudy a meRBANSCalifornia verbal learning testBoston naming testTrial making testVerbal uencyDesign uencySpatial span

    0000000

    Face-name associationsCalifornia verbal learning testDementia Rating ScaleMini Mental State ExaminationRivermead Behavioral Memory TestSelf-esteem scaleMultifactorial Memory Questionnaire

    0b

    000000b

    Prospective memoryMMQ-abilityMMQ-strategyMMQ-contentmentStrategy knowledge repertoire

    +000+

    Memory ToolboxMultifactorial Memory QuestionnaireImpact rating scaleLifestyle importanceName recallNumber recallWordlist recall

    +000000

    Mini Mental State ExaminationMontreal Cognitive Assessment

    ++Functional Cognitive Assessment ScaleMoCA (verbal memory)Rey-sterrieth Complex Figure TestMoCA (Clock drawing)Functional Rating Scale of Symptomsof Dementia

    +++++

    Appointment testLogical memory ILogical memory IIMemory assessment clinicsquestionnaire

    +0++

    NS, Repeatable Battery for Assessment of Neuropsychological Status;

    perimental group.

    (Hastings and West, 2009; West et al., 2008), story/textllini et al., 2010; Hastings and West, 2009; Noice and; Talib et al., 2008; West et al., 2008), Rivermead Behav-ry Test (Klusmann et al., 2010), Repeatable Battery forent of Neuropsychological Status (Smith et al., 2009),

    ry Verbal Learning Test (Smith et al., 2009), memorye (Talib et al., 2008; West et al., 2008). In eight stud-es of executive functioning were included, of which

    showed signicant effects on the following tasks; taskBasak et al., 2008), Stroop Color Word Task (Borella, Trail Making Test (Carlson et al., 2008), Rey-sterriethgure Test (Carlson et al., 2008), Symbol Digit Modali-ozolic et al., 2011), walk and talk paradigm (Mozolic, uency and problem solving (Noice and Noice, 2009).dies, signicant improvement on (uid) intelligence testved; Catells Culture Fair (Bottiroli and Cavallini, 2009;

    Koutstaal, 2008), Ravens Advanced Progressive Matri-et al., 2008). In two studies, signicant improvemental tasks were observed; selective attention task, multi-

    egration task (Mozolic et al., 2011), Repeatable Batteryent of Neuropsychological Status (Smith et al., 2009).ies, signicant improvement in speed of processinged (Borella et al., 2010; Smith et al., 2009). In oneasure of general cognitive functioning (Mini Mental

  • 272 J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275

    State Examination) was included as an outcome measure and sig-nicantly improved by the intervention (McDougall et al., 2010).Subjective measures of cognitive performance were included inve studies. Signicant effects were observed for the Multifacto-rial MemorMisplacemeEfcacy QueMetamemoet al., 2010tionnaire (Sfunctioninget al., 2010et al., 2008)

    All six Rused objectfour studiesinterventioon the follomemory st2008), verbet al., 2011memory (Wfunctioningscale was s(Tsolaki et afunctioningformance, mthe Montrethe intervenmeasure of Memory Quon this scasure of dailDementia)

    3.5. Method

    The meassessed. Tincluded st(Table 5). Thological quathe lowest qseven catemethods-ramation. In methods-rthan 50% of

    3.6. Non-ra

    Eight noolder adults2010) and 2008; HampLondos et a

    In healthmemory pmemory trtransfer tasTwo studieSchmiedek ronment inand workinreports as obrain traini

    Nintendo Wii and reading sessions which showed practice-relatedimprovement but no signicant transfer effects.

    In people with MCI, two studies focused on improving adap-tive behavior and functional ability. Greenaway et al. (2008)

    d imppored ced acreas

    focg or

    effeos eon ptioneme, and

    cussi

    s systerved thas aspce, eence

    bethetere mce aimilaa montiove oategt al., ntio-eduoging muehfocusest, 2unctre, aing

    ddit, theetwesed ing (>e and thaformtentiann eposiermer, tal excompprov

    anclitatiy Questionnaire (Fairchild and Scogin, 2010), Objectnt task (Fairchild and Scogin, 2010), Memory Self-stionnaire (Hastings and West, 2009; West et al., 2008),ry in Adulthood (Hastings and West, 2009; McDougall; West et al., 2008), and Cognitive Self-Report Ques-mith et al., 2009). In two studies, measures of daily

    (Direct Assessment of Functional Status (McDougall) and Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (Slegers) were included, but no signicant effect were observed.CTs in the MCI population included in our analysisive memory performance as an outcome measure: in

    memory performance signicantly improved after then (Table 4). Signicant improvements were observedwing tasks; prospective memory (Kinsella et al., 2009),rategy knowledge (Kinsella et al., 2009; Troyer et al.,al memory (Montreal Cognitive Assessment) (Tsolaki), appointment test (Wagner et al., 2008) and logicalagner et al., 2008). In two studies measures of executive

    were included; the functional cognitive assessmentignicantly improved by the intervention in one studyl., 2011). In three studies, a measure of general cognitive

    was included. In only one study general cognitive per-easured with the Mini Mental State Examination and

    al Cognitive Assessment was signicantly improved bytion (Tsolaki et al., 2011). In three studies, a subjectivecognitive performance was included, the Multifactorialestionnaire, although no signicant effects were foundle. In one study a signicant improvement on a mea-y functioning (Functional Rating Scale of Symptoms ofwas observed (Tsolaki et al., 2011).

    ological quality of study

    thodological quality of the twenty-seven RCTs washe percentage of the Consort items reported in theudies ranged from 16% to 73%, with an average of 44%e study of Smith et al. (2009) had the highest method-lity (73%), while the study of Wagner et al. (2008) haduality (16%). The Consort statement can be divided into

    gories: title/abstract; introduction; methods-trial;ndomization; results; discussion; and other infor-ve of these categories (title/abstract, methods-trial,andomization, results, and other information) less

    the items was reported.

    ndomized controlled trials

    n-RCTs were included; three clinical studies in healthy (Ackerman et al., 2010; Li et al., 2008; Schmiedek et al.,ve clinical studies in people with MCI (Greenaway et al.,stead et al., 2008; Joosten-Weyn Banningh et al., 2008;

    l., 2008; Ozgis et al., 2009).y older adults, one study aimed at improving working

    erformance by computerized training. The workingaining showed improvements on practiced and nearks but no effects on far transfer tasks (Li et al., 2008).s aimed at improving cognitive functioning in general.et al. (2010) provided an internet-based training envi-cluding tasks of perceptual speed, episodic memory,g memory. They used only self-report evaluationutcome measures. Ackerman et al. (2010) provided ang environment including training sessions with the

    showeory suprovidincreasand instudiestraininrelatedof Londbilitatioccupaimprovmance

    4. Dis

    Thitive inshowevariouformanintelligparisonof the outcomformanused strated interveobjectiory strTalib eof attepsychoand ScworkinBuschkgrams and Wutive fmeasuimprov2008).

    In astudiesonly baddresity ratipositivshoweof perand atKlusmhad a the RivHowevphysicMCI, a no imperformrehabiprovement in functional ability by the use of a mem-t system and Joosten-Weyn Banningh et al. (2008)ognitive behavioral group therapy which resulted incceptance and marital satisfaction in people with MCIed awareness in their caregivers. The results of twousing on memory improvement (by memory strategy

    spaced retrieval memory training) showed practice-cts (Hampstead et al., 2008; Ozgis et al., 2009). The studyt al. (2008) examined the effect of an established reha-rogram designed to improve cognitive performance,al performance, and quality of life. The results showednts in cognitive processing speed, occupational perfor-

    some domains of quality of life (Londos et al., 2008).

    on

    tematic review, evaluating the effectiveness of cogni-ntions in healthy older adults and people with MCI,t cognitive interventions can be effective in improvingects of objective cognitive functioning; memory per-xecutive functioning, processing speed, attention, uid, and subjective cognitive performance. A critical com-ween different intervention studies is difcult becauseogeneity of the intervention programs and the choseneasures. Most intervention studies used memory per-s the primary outcome measure, but only few studiesr tests to measure memory functioning which frus-re quantitative meta-analysis on these studies. Differentn programs proved to be effective in improving eitherr subjective memory performance; training of mem-ies (Bottiroli and Cavallini, 2009; Cavallini et al., 2010;2008; Wagner et al., 2008), accompanied with practicenal skills (Kinsella et al., 2009; Tsolaki et al., 2011),cational lessons on memory and/or lifestyle (Fairchild, 2010; Troyer et al., 2008); computerized training ofemory task (Berry et al., 2010; Borella et al., 2010;l et al., 2008; Richmond et al., 2011); and training pro-ing on memory self-efcacy or metacognition (Hastings009; McDougall et al., 2010; West et al., 2008). Exec-ioning was mostly included as a secondary outcomend only two intervention programs aimed directly atexecutive functioning (Basak et al., 2008; Carlson et al.,

    ion to the heterogeneity of the included intervention methodological quality of the studies differed vastly:en 16% and 73% of the quality criteria items weren the studies. When assessing studies with a high qual-60% of the Consort quality criteria), we observed bothd negative results. The study of Smith et al. (2009)t computerized cognitive training led to improvementance generalizing to untrained measures of memoryon and subjective everyday functioning. The study oft al. (2010) showed that a 6-month computer course

    tive affect on memory performance (measured withead Behavioral Memory Test) in healthy older adults.he same improvement was achieved with a 6-monthercise course (Klusmann et al., 2010). In people withuter-based auditory processing speed training showed

    ements on memory, executive, or general cognitivee (Barnes et al., 2009). However, a 5-week memoryon program focusing on practicing memory strategies

  • J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275 273

    Table 5Quality assessment of included studies using the Consort statement 2010.

    Title, abstract(max. 2)

    Introduction(max. 2)

    Methods-trial(max. 9)

    Methods-randomization(max. 8)

    Results(max. 10)

    Discussion(max. 3)

    Other information(max. 3)

    Total (%) score(max. 37)

    Barnes et al.Jean et al. (2Kinsella et aTroyer et al. Tsolaki et al.Wagner et aBailey et al. Basak et al. (Berry et al. (Borella et al.Bottiroli and

    (2009)Buiza et al. (Buschkuehl

    (2008)Carlson et alCavallini et aFairchild and

    (2010)Hastings and

    (2009)Klusmann etMcDougall eMozolic et aNoice and NRichmond eSlegers et alSmith et al. (Talib et al. (2Tranter and

    (2008)West et al. (2

    Total for cat 11

    did show imknowledge

    Anotheraddressed igroup receiWith an actattention alooking at tan active comeasures ofunctioning(Borella et a2010; McDo2009; RichmTranter andincluded anthese two s2009; Jean interventioconcluded ton various ationing in h

    An impowhether obfunctioningtioning, or lA number oalization tofunctioningCavallini, 20Richmond e(ADL) meas

    ant From (2009) 2 2 5 5 010a) 1 2 5 4 l. (2009) 2 2 5 5 (2008) 1 2 4 4

    (2011) 1 2 2 2 l. (2008) 0 2 3 0 (2010) 0 1 4 12008) 0 2 5 1 2010) 0 1 4 2

    (2010) 0 2 4 2 Cavallini 1 1 4 2

    2008) 1 1 3 0et al. 0 2 3 1

    . (2008) 1 2 5 2 l. (2010) 0 1 3 2

    Scogin 0 2 1 3

    West 0 2 4 1

    al. (2010) 2 2 6 6t al. (2010) 0 2 4 3 l. (2011) 1 2 4 4 oice (2009) 0 2 4 2 t al. (2011) 0 2 4 2 . (2008) 1 2 5 4 2009) 1 2 5 7 008) 0 2 3 1 Koutstaal 0 2 3 1

    008) 0 2 2 1

    egory (%)15 (28%) 49 (91%) 104 (43%) 68 (31%)

    provements on prospective memory task and strategyrepertoire (Kinsella et al., 2009).

    signic2009). aspect of methodological quality, not specicallyn the Consort Statement items, is whether the controlved an active control intervention or no intervention.ive control condition the effects of, for example, socials part of group meetings can be controlled for. Whenhe RCTs in the healthy older adults, ten RCTs includedntrol condition. These studies did show an effect onf cognitive functioning, more specically executive, speed, attention, and subjective memory functioningl., 2010; Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Fairchild and Scogin,ugall et al., 2010; Mozolic et al., 2011; Noice and Noice,ond et al., 2011; Slegers et al., 2008; Smith et al., 2009;

    Koutstaal, 2008). In the MCI population, two studies active control group. The interventions executed intudies did not show any signicant effect (Barnes et al.,et al., 2010b). In conclusion, evaluating the results ofn studies with an active control condition, it may behat cognitive interventions can have a positive effectspects of both objective and subjective cognitive func-ealthy older adults, but not in people with MCI.rtant question that was raised in earlier reviews isserved training effects on various aspects of cognitive

    generalize to untrained tasks or overall cognitive func-ead to functional improvement in daily life situations.f studies specically addressed the question of gener-

    untrained task or to subjective experience of cognitive (Berry et al., 2010; Borella et al., 2010; Bottiroli and09; Buschkuehl et al., 2008; Fairchild and Scogin, 2010;t al., 2011; Smith et al., 2009). Activities of Daily Livingures were included in three intervention studies and a

    evidence foand daily liinterventiostill needs t

    Anotherthe rootingFollow-up They showeremained apositive effafter 1 monory and spepositive effmonths (Boup did notSlegers et aing effects c

    In compet al. (2011ies were incmore elabogating the However, oearlier reviSimard, 201reviews, is ting to the Cquality of into accounand interim5 3 2 24 (65%)5 2 1 20 (54%)6 3 1 24 (65%)6 2 0 19 (51%)2 1 0 10 (27%)1 0 0 6 (16%)3 3 1 13 (35%)6 3 1 18 (49%)4 1 1 13 (35%)3 3 0 14 (38%)4 1 0 13 (35%)

    0 2 1 8 (22%)4 3 1 14 (38%)

    5 3 1 19 (51%)2 2 1 11 (30%)6 3 1 16 (43%)

    3 3 1 14 (38%)

    6 3 1 26 (70%)5 2 2 18 (49%)4 3 1 19 (51%)6 2 1 17 (46%)3 3 1 15 (41%)5 3 1 21 (57%)8 3 1 27 (73%)3 1 1 11 (30%)5 3 1 15 (41%)

    3 3 1 12 (32%)

    3 (42%) 64 (79%) 24 (30%) 44%

    effect on ADL was observed in one study (Smith et al., these data it can be concluded that there is very little

    r generalization effects to overall cognitive functioningfe situations. The issue whether the effects of cognitivens generalize to improvement in everyday life activitieso be addressed more explicitly in future research.

    important question is whether there is evidence of of training effect after the training period has ended.data were available in seven out of twenty-six RCTs.d that positive effects on memory strategy knowledge

    fter 3/4 months (Kinsella et al., 2009; Troyer et al., 2008),ects on memory self-efcacy and recall tasks preservedth (Hastings and West, 2009), positive effects on mem-ed preserved after 3 months (Smith et al., 2009), andects on speed and uid intelligence preserved after 8rella et al., 2010). Two studies including a 1-year follow

    nd any signicant effects (Buschkuehl et al., 2008;l., 2008). From these data it can be concluded that train-an be preserved at least for a couple of months.arison to the earlier reviews of Papp et al. (2009), Martin) and Tardif and Simard (2011), more intervention stud-luded in this review. This could be the results of usingrated search terms or an increase in studies investi-effect of cognitive interventions in the last few years.ur conclusions are in line with the conclusions of theseews (Martin et al., 2011; Papp et al., 2009; Tardif and1). A strength of this study, in comparison with earlierhe methodological quality rating we performed accord-onsort Statement 2010 for RCTs. The methodological

    the twenty-seven RCTs differed considerably. Takingt that some items (such as blinding, serious harms,

    analysis) are less applicable for non-pharmacological

  • 274 J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275

    interventions, it seems clear that substantial improvement in termsof study quality can still be made in future intervention studies. Inthis review we included intervention studies from both healthyolder adults and MCI populations. Although a clear differentiationbetween thmal versus cannot be cwith MCI bolder adultsis that due outcome mmeta-analyble publicathe results

    4.1. Conclu

    The reseffective intioning; mespeed, atteformance. Astudies is dtion prograthe heterogodological qthe effects oeveryday lifmore explica core set othe effectivobjective andomains (emeasures tshould be pthe methodimproved btained in thdesign and follow up a

    Source of f

    This stunition (NIBResearch (N

    Conict of

    We decl

    References

    Ackerman, P.Lcise practi753766.

    Bailey, H., Dunimprove o

    Barnes, D.E., eimpairmenand Assoc

    Basak, C., et acognitive d

    Berry, A.S., et aolder adul

    Borella, E., et aand maint

    Bottiroli, S., Ccomputer-

    sample of older adults. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. SectionB, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition 16 (4), 401418.

    Buiza, C., et al., 2008. A randomized, two-year study of the efcacy of cognitiveintervention on elderly people: the Donostia Longitudinal Study. InternationalJournal of Geriatric Psychiatry 23 (1), 8594.

    ehl, Mance

    M.C., utive 6), 793i, E., eg Clin9. Heag/Hee, J.K.,stigatts. Ag.L., 2

    ), Brai

    ay, M cognentiaead, Bace-nt inves8838, E.C.,d mem594.et al.itive atric Pet al.,impaibilitaWeynpatien

    other740.

    , G.J., eised c7307n, V.,cogniof Ger688.n, V., lts froogy, Dition t al., maintE., et itive Other., et alropsycM., etpeoplews 1all.JJr.der ad, J.L., ic arobiol., Noicemeng, Neu., Renpectivgy 55 V., Waintervctionsd, L.Lhologek, F.rams.K.F., Aelinesicine K., vausage rolledal Scieese two populations can be made with regard to nor-pathological aging, the intervention programs appliedlearly differentiated and the question whether peopleenet from other intervention programs than healthy

    still needs to be answered. A limitation of this reviewto the heterogeneity of the intervention programs andeasures, we were not able to conduct a quantitativesis. Besides this, the selected search terms and possi-tion bias are two aspects which could have inuencedour review.

    sions

    ults show evidence that cognitive training can be improving various aspects of objective cognitive func-mory performance, executive functioning, processingntion, uid intelligence, and subjective cognitive per-

    critical comparison between different interventionifcult because of the heterogeneity of the interven-ms and the chosen outcome measures. In addition toeneity of the included intervention studies, the meth-uality of the studies differed widely. The issue whetherf cognitive interventions generalize to improvement ine activities is still unresolved and needs to be addresseditly in future research. For future research, inclusion off outcome measures would be necessary to compare

    eness of different cognitive intervention programs. Bothd subjective outcome measures for specic cognitive

    .g. memory, executive functioning) and ecological validhat show improvements in daily cognitive functioningart of this core set of outcome measures. Besides this,ological quality of future intervention studies should bey specically addressing the quality control items con-e Consort criteria. In particular, the description of trialrandomization should be reporting more accurately andssessments should be included.

    unding

    dy was funded by the National Initiative Brain & Cog-C) and the Netherlands Organization for ScienticWO).

    interest

    are that there are no conicts of interest.

    ., Kanfer, R., Calderwood, C., 2010. Use it or lose it? Wii brain exer-ce and reading for domain knowledge. Psychology and Aging 25 (4),

    losky, J., Hertzog, C., 2010. Metacognitive training at home: does itlder adults learning? Gerontology 56 (4), 414420.t al., 2009. Computer-based cognitive training for mild cognitivet: results from a pilot randomized, controlled trial. Alzheimer Disease

    iated Disorders 23 (3), 205210.l., 2008. Can training in a real-time strategy video game attenuateecline in older adults? Psychology and Aging 23 (4), 765777.l., 2010. The inuence of perceptual training on working memory ints. PLoS One 5 (7), e11537.l., 2010. Working memory training in older adults: evidence of transferenance effects. Psychology and Aging 25 (4), 767778.avallini, E., 2009. Can computer familiarity regulate the benets ofbased memory training in normal aging? A study with an Italian

    Buschkuform

    Carlson,exec48 (

    CavallinAgin

    CBS, 200Haa

    Fairchildinveadul

    Glisky, E(Ed.FL.

    GreenawmildDem

    Hampstfor fpilo(5),

    Hastingsbase586

    Jean, L., cognGeri

    Jean, L., tive Reha

    Joosten-for cant731

    Kinselladom(7),

    Klusmanand nal 680

    KlusmanresucholCogn

    Li, S.C., eand

    Londos, cognand

    Lustig, CNeu

    Martin, and Revi

    McDougin ol

    MozolicspecNeu

    Noice, HretirAgin

    Ozgis, Sprostolo

    Papp, K.tive dire

    RichmonPsyc

    Schmiedprog

    Schulz, guidMed

    Slegers, net contSoci., et al., 2008. Impact of working memory training on memory per-in oldold adults. Psychology and Aging 23 (4), 743753.et al., 2008. Exploring the effects of an everyday activity program onfunction and memory in older adults: experience corps. Gerontologist801.t al., 2010. Promoting transfer in memory training for older adults.ical and Experimental Research 22 (4), 314323.lth and Care in Figures 2009. Centraal Bureau voor de Statistiek, Den

    rlen, The Netherlands. Scogin, F.R., 2010. Training to Enhance Adult Memory (TEAM): anion of the effectiveness of a memory training program with oldering and Mental Health 14 (3), 364373.007. Changes in cognitive function in human aging. In: Riddle, D.n Aging: Models, Methods, and Mechanisms. CRC Press, Boca Raton,

    .C., et al., 2008. A behavioral rehabilitation intervention for amnesticitive impairment. American Journal of Alzheimers Disease and Others 23 (5), 451461..M., et al., 2008. Explicit memory training leads to improved memoryame pairs in patients with mild cognitive impairment: results of atigation. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society 1489.

    West, R.L., 2009. The relative success of a self-help and a group-ory training program for older adults. Psychology and Aging 24 (3),

    , 2010a. Cognitive intervention programs for individuals with mildimpairment: systematic review of the literature. American Journal ofsychiatry 18 (4), 281296.

    2010b. Efcacy of a cognitive training programme for mild cogni-rment: results of a randomised controlled study. Neuropsychologicaltion 20 (3), 377405.

    Banningh, L.W., et al., 2008. A cognitive behavioural group therapyts diagnosed with mild cognitive impairment and their signi-s: feasibility and preliminary results. Clinical Rehabilitation 22 (8),

    t al., 2009. Early intervention for mild cognitive impairment: a ran-ontrolled trial. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 8036.

    et al., 2010. Complex mental and physical activity in older womentive performance: a 6-month randomized controlled trial. The Jour-ontology. Series A, Biological Sciences and Medical Sciences 65 (6),

    et al., 2011. Activity experiences shape perceived tness trajectories:m a 6-month randomized controlled trial in older women. Neuropsy-evelopment, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and18 (3), 328339.2008. Working memory plasticity in old age: practice gain, transfer,enance. Psychology and Aging 23 (4), 731742.al., 2008. Effects of a goal-oriented rehabilitation program in mildimpairment: a pilot study. American Journal of Alzheimers Disease

    Dementias 23 (2), 177183.., 2009. Aging, training, and the brain: a review and future directions.hology Review 19 (4), 504522.

    al., 2011. Cognition-based interventions for healthy older peoplee with mild cognitive impairment. Cochrane Database of Systematic, CD006220., G., et al., 2010. The SeniorWISE study: improving everyday memoryults. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing 24 (5), 291306.et al., 2011. A cognitive training intervention improves modality-ttention in a randomized controlled trial of healthy older adults.ogy of Aging 32 (4), 655668.e, T., 2009. An arts intervention for older adults living in subsidizedt homes. Neuropsychology, Development, and Cognition. Section B,ropsychology and Cognition 16 (1), 5679.

    dell, P.G., Henry, J.D., 2009. Spaced retrieval signicantly improvese memory performance of cognitively impaired older adults. Geron-(2), 229232.lsh, S.J., Snyder, P.J., 2009. Immediate and delayed effects of cogni-entions in healthy elderly: a review of current literature and future. Alzheimers Dementia 5 (1), 5060.., et al., 2011. Working memory training and transfer in older adults.y and Aging 26 (4), 813822., et al., 2010. Cognitive enrichment in old age web-based training

    GeroPsychology 23 (2), 5967.ltman, D.G., Moher, D., 2010. CONSORT 2010 statement: updated

    for reporting parallel group randomized trials. Annals of Internal152 (11), 726732.n Boxtel, M.P., Jolles, J., 2008. Effects of computer training and Inter-on the well-being and quality of life of older adults: a randomized,

    study. Journals of Gerontology. Series B, Psychological Sciences andnces 63 (3), 176184.

  • J. Reijnders et al. / Ageing Research Reviews 12 (2013) 263 275 275

    Slegers, K., van Boxtel, M., Jolles, J., 2009. Effects of computer training and internetusage on cognitive abilities in older adults: a randomized controlled study. AgingClinical and Experimental Research 21 (1), 4354.

    Smith, G.E., et al., 2009. A cognitive training program based on principles of brainplasticity: results from the Improvement in Memory with Plasticity-based Adap-tive Cognitive Training (IMPACT) study. Journal of the American GeriatricsSociety 57 (4), 594603.

    Talib, L.L., et al., 2008. Cognitive training increases platelet PLA2 activity in healthyelderly subjects. Prostaglandins Leukotrienes and Essential Fatty Acids 78 (45),265269.

    Tardif, S., Simard, M., 2011. Cognitive stimulation programs in healthy elderly: areview. International Journal of Alzheimers Disease, Article ID: 378934.

    Teixeira, C.V., et al., 2012. Non-pharmacological interventions on cognitive functionsin older people with mild cognitive impairment (MCI). Archives of Gerontologyand Geriatrics 54 (1), 175180.

    Tranter, L.J., Koutstaal, W., 2008. Age and exible thinking: an experimental demon-stration of the benecial effects of increased cognitively stimulating activity on

    uid intelligence in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology, Development, andCognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition 15 (2), 184207.

    Troyer, A.K., et al., 2008. Changing everyday memory behaviour in amnestic mildcognitive impairment: a randomised controlled trial. Neuropsychological Reha-bilitation 18 (1), 6588.

    Tsolaki, M., et al., 2011. Effectiveness of nonpharmacological approaches in patientswith mild cognitive impairment. Neurodegenerative Diseases 8 (3), 138145.

    Valenzuela, M., Sachdev, P., 2009. Can cognitive exercise prevent the onset of demen-tia? Systematic review of randomized clinical trials with longitudinal follow-up.American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry 17 (3), 179187.

    Wagner, S., et al., 2008. Does a cognitive-training programme improve the per-formance of middle-aged employees undergoing in-patient psychosomatictreatment? Disability and Rehabilitation 30 (23), 17861793.

    West, R.L., Bagwell, D.K., Dark-Freudeman, A., 2008. Self-efcacy and memory aging:the impact of a memory intervention based on self-efcacy. Neuropsychology,Development, and Cognition. Section B, Aging, Neuropsychology and Cognition15 (3), 302329.

    Cognitive interventions in healthy older adults and people with mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review1 Introduction2 Methods3 Results3.1 Studies included3.2 Study characteristics3.3 Description of interventions3.4 Outcome measures and effects3.5 Methodological quality of study3.6 Non-randomized controlled trials

    4 Discussion4.1 Conclusions

    Source of fundingConflict of interestReferences