67
Motivated Reasoning and Political Parties: Evidence for Increased Processing in the Face of Party Cues Michael Bang Petersen Department of Political Science Aarhus University Denmark Email: [email protected] Martin Skov Danish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance Copenhagen University Hospital Denmark Email: [email protected] Søren Serritzlew Department of Political Science Aarhus University Denmark Email: [email protected] Thomas Ramsøy Decision Neuroscience Research Group Copenhagen Business School Denmark Email: [email protected] Forthcoming in Political Behavior in 2013 Extant research in political science has demonstrated that citizens’ opinions on policies are influenced by their attachment to the party sponsoring them. At the same time, little evidence exists illuminating the psychological processes through which such party cues are filtered. From the psychological literature on source cues, we derive two possible hypotheses: (1) party cues activate heuristic processing aimed at minimizing the processing effort during opinion formation, and (2) party cues activate group motivational processes that compel citizens to support the position of their party. As part of the latter 1

Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Motivated Reasoning and Political Parties:Evidence for Increased Processing in the Face of

Party Cues

Michael Bang PetersenDepartment of Political Science

Aarhus UniversityDenmark

Email: [email protected]

Martin SkovDanish Research Centre for Magnetic Resonance

Copenhagen University HospitalDenmark

Email: [email protected]

Søren SerritzlewDepartment of Political Science

Aarhus UniversityDenmark

Email: [email protected]

Thomas RamsøyDecision Neuroscience Research Group

Copenhagen Business SchoolDenmark

Email: [email protected]

Forthcoming in Political Behavior in 2013

Extant research in political science has demonstrated that citizens’ opinions on policies are influ-enced by their attachment to the party sponsoring them. At the same time, little evidence exists illu-minating the psychological processes through which such party cues are filtered. From the psycho-logical literature on source cues, we derive two possible hypotheses: (1) party cues activate heur-istic processing aimed at minimizing the processing effort during opinion formation, and (2) party cues activate group motivational processes that compel citizens to support the position of their party. As part of the latter processes, the presence of party cues would make individuals engage in effortful motivated reasoning to produce arguments for the correctness of their party’s position. Fol-lowing psychological research, we use response latency to measure processing effort and, in sup-port of the motivated reasoning hypothesis, demonstrate that across student and nationally repres-entative samples, the presence of party cues increases processing effort.

Key words: Public Opinion; Political Parties; Party Cues; Motivated Reasoning; Heuristic Processing

1

Page 2: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Political parties play an important role in the structuring of mass opinion. Research demonstrates

how citizens’ opinions on policies are influenced not only by the content of the specific policies but

also by their attachment to the party sponsoring them. Longitudinal studies show that changes in

partisan discourse on important issues such as race or abortion shape aggregate mass opinion (Lay-

man and Carsey 1998; Zaller 1992). Similarly, a large number of micro-level studies demonstrate

that individuals’ opinions about policy proposals change substantially when they are provided with

information about party positions (see, e.g., Cohen 2003; Kam 2005; Lau and Redlawsk 2001;

Mondak 1993). This does not mean that party sponsorship is the most important, let alone the only,

predictor of citizens’ opinion on policies. Bullock (2011) argues that many studies comparing the

effect of party cues with policy information tend to underestimate the relative effect of policy in-

formation as only sparse information is provided. He also shows in two experiments that with sub-

stantial policy information, “party cues are influential” but that policy information generally matters

at least as much (2011: 512). Nevertheless, party cues remain an important factor for citizen’s opin-

ions. If citizens like the party advocating the policy, they are more likely to agree with it; if they

dislike the party, they are more likely to reject the policy.

Research on this party sponsor effect suggests that it helps citizens form political opinions in the

absence of political knowledge and interest (Kam 2005; Zaller 1992). By delegating their decisions

to ‘like-minded experts’, citizens reduce the costs of collecting information on, for example, the

technical details of the policy and of analysing its effects. In fact, some studies suggest that citizens

can often form just as valid an opinion on the basis of their party’s position as on the basis of a more

careful assessment of the policy itself (Lau and Redlawsk 2006). At the same time, other studies

have argued that this only holds true for policies on which parties hold positions in line with their

2

Page 3: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

normal ideological profile. On other policies, reliance on party positions can result in a biased opin-

ion (Rahn 1993; Cohen 2003; Greitemeyer et al. 2009).

Research on the effects of citizens’ reliance on party positions has provided important insights into

how citizens are able to navigate in modern politics. At the same time, this research has provided

less insight into the psychological processes that these cues rely on and are processed by. Research

on the psychology of opinion formation suggests that two different psychological processes may

explain citizens’ reliance on a source cue such as a party’s position on a policy. The first process,

heuristic processing, minimizes the processing costs involved in opinion formation while the second

process, motivated directional reasoning (or for short, motivated reasoning), invest cognitive ef-

fort in defending valued pre-commitments such as one’s party identification (e.g., by spending ef-

fort to produce convincing arguments for giving into the motivational pull of one’s identification)

(Eagly and Chaiken 1993; Kunda 1990). While a few studies have suggested that motivated reason-

ing drives the processing of party cues (Cohen 2003; Slothuus and de Vreese 2010; Westen et al.

2006), Bullock (2011: 497) sums up the current state of the literature by arguing that party cues “are

widely thought to be processed heuristically”. Yet, until now, studies on party cues in political sci-

ence have not focused directly on the psychological processing of party cues and, hence, have failed

to discern between the different possibilities. This is unfortunate because the two processes are

grounded in different types of motivations and paints very different pictures of citizens’ basic rela-

tion to politics. This is particularly evident in the cases where reliance on the party sponsor results

in a biased opinion. If party sponsor effects originate in heuristic processing, citizens are basically

motivated to hold accurate opinions (Mondak 1993; Lau and Redlawsk 2006), and partisan bias in

opinion formation is just an unfortunate by-product of citizens’ lack of political interest. Hence, if

citizens became more interested or were provided with more valid but still cost-effective cues, the

3

Page 4: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

bias would be reduced. In contrast, if party sponsor effects originate in motivated reasoning, cit-

izens are seen as motivated to be biased, making partisan bias inevitable (Taber and Lodge 2006).

In this article, we utilize research on group psychology and party identification to suggest that cit-

izens engage in motivated reasoning rather than heuristic processing when confronted with novel

policy proposals sponsored by different political parties. Empirically, we report the results of two

tests designed to discern directly between the two psychological processes in the context of party

sponsor effects. We use an experimental design to gauge the psychological dynamics during opinion

formation in the face of party cues, which is innovative in terms of both measurement and sampling.

To discern between heuristic processing and motivated reasoning, we follow extant research and use

response latency (Redlawsk 2002; Cohen 2003; Taber and Lodge 2006). If party cues activate heur-

istic processing, the presence of party cues should decrease the mental effort and time required to

form opinions. If, in contrast, party cues activate motivated reasoning, opinion formation should

under specific circumstances become more mentally effortful and, therefore, prolonged (Matz and

Wood 2005). In terms of sampling, we collected response latencies across two highly diverse

samples. In a laboratory setting, we collected response latencies from a student sample. Outside the

laboratory, we used a web survey to collect response latencies from a representative sample. This

allowed us to ensure the robustness and generalizability of our results to the actual population of

citizens.

Our empirical findings show that the presence of party cues prolongs response times, lending sup-

port to the motivated reasoning account rather than the heuristic processing account. This suggests

that one reason that citizens rely on party positions is a group-based motivation to appear loyal to

4

Page 5: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

the policy line of ‘their’ party. In the conclusion, we discuss the implications of these findings for

understanding citizens’ engagement in party politics.

Party Cues and Modes of Processing

When political parties sponsor a policy, citizens are provided with what is termed a source cue in

the psychological literature on persuasion (Petty and Cacioppo 1981). As mentioned, studies in psy-

chology have identified two modes of psychological processing through which a source cue can be

integrated into an assessment of a persuasive statement: heuristic processing and motivated (direc-

tional) reasoning.

Eagly and Chaiken (1993) define heuristic processing as a mode of processing requiring “less effort

and fewer cognitive resources”, where individuals focus “on that subset of the available information

that enables them to use simple decision rules (…) to formulate their judgments and decisions”. In

the context of political policies, such a decision rule could relate to the party sponsor: If you like the

party, support the policy; if you dislike the party, reject the policy. Heuristic processing is a pro-

cessing strategy designed to achieve an opinion with a satisfactory level of accuracy using a min-

imal level of cognitive effort. Research has demonstrated that heuristic processing is deployed in

situations in which the individual lacks motivation and capacity to perform a more careful assess-

ment of the available information (e.g., Petty et al. 1981; Eagley and Chaiken 1993). Given the high

levels of political ignorance in modern electorates (Lau & Redlawsk 2006; Zaller 1992), many re-

searchers have suggested that heuristic processing plays an important role when citizens rely upon

party cues (Bullock 2011). For example, party cues have been described as the “‘cheapest’ cue

5

Page 6: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

available” (Schafner and Streb 2002: 560), “the most important saving device” (Squire and Smith

1988: 178) and a means to an “obvious cognitive saving” (Lau and Redlawsk 2001: 953).

Where heuristic processing involves minimal effort to arrive at a correct or accurate opinion, direc-

tional motivated reasoning involves the exertion of effort to come up with arguments for why a con-

clusion that is desired for antecedent reasons is indeed the correct one (Kunda 1990). Here, reason-

ing effort operates in the service of motivation to search through memory for arguments, beliefs and

rules that would support the desired conclusion. In the context of party sponsor effects, motivated

reasoning would entail that citizens spend effort to come up with reasons for supporting policies

that are sponsored by a party they like. Importantly, motivation is not always expected to be assisted

by reasoning. Rather, effortful processing is strategically deployed to reach a desired conclusion in

dissonance-provoking situations where an individual holds incongruent beliefs related to the con-

clusion. If motivated reasoning processes influence the processing of party cues, this implies that

reasoning effort should be deployed when individuals are motivated to, for example, support

policies that they do not like because they are sponsored by a party that they do like. Expressing

support for a policy that is attractive in itself and sponsored by the favoured party should, in con-

trast, not require much effort.

Group Psychology and Motivated Processing of Party Cues

While researchers have tended to view party cues as being processed through heuristic processing

(cf. Bullock 2011), several political scientists have, at the same time, argued that partisanship re-

flects a distinct group attachment that is psychologically important to many citizens (Campbell et al.

1960; Lebo and Cassino 2007; Green et al. 2002; Greene 1999). Hence, partisan attachments are –

6

Page 7: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

like other group attachments – formed early in life, transmitted across generations, endure over the

life-span and in the face of significant changes in the environment, and are (to some extent, at least)

independent of other important political predispositions such as core ideological values (Jennings

and Niemi 1974, 1981; Green et al. 2002; Goren 2005). Recent cross-disciplinary studies have

provided additional evidence for the deep-seated nature of party attachments. One study, for ex-

ample, showed that subjects’ testosterone levels were altered as a response to election outcomes

wherein their party suffered defeat, in the same way as when they themselves would suffer an actual

status loss (Stanton et al. 2009). Other studies have shown how party cues are processed by brain

regions associated with affective processes operating below the level of consciousness (Westen et

al. 2004) and that experimental participants, even though substantially influenced by party cues,

think of them as the least influential factor when asked directly (Cohen 2003).

If political parties are psychologically represented as groups, the implication is that whenever a

liked party takes a position on an issue, it is mentally represented as a claim for support from the

group to which one belongs. This would trigger a motivation to provide such support by toeing the

party line and, if necessary, invest cognitive effort in coming up with arguments for the correctness

of doing so. Hence, the studies on the group psychological basis of party identification provide ini-

tial reasons for expecting motivated reasoning processes rather than heuristic processing to be at the

heart of the party sponsor effect.

To provide direct tests of this proposed importance of motivated reasoning, we focus on the pro-

cessing effort underlying opinion formation in the face of party cues. Heuristic processing and mo-

tivated reasoning entail opposite effects of the presence of party cues on the level of processing in-

volved in forming opinions. If party cues are processed using heuristic processing, the availability

7

Page 8: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

of party cues should minimize the effort invested in processing. In contrast, if party cues tap into the

group psychology of citizens, motivated reasoning should in specific circumstances make opinion

formation in the face of party cues more effortful. In particular, motivated reasoning is expected to

kick in when a motivated individual faces dissonance-provoking situations (Kunda 1990). To the

extent that political parties tap into group psychological mechanisms, the presence of party cues

will elicit dissonance and trigger motivated reasoning in at least two situations: when an individual

disagrees with a liked party and when an individual agrees with a liked party on an otherwise dis-

liked policy.

First, citizens who sympathize with a party should feel compelled to meet the party’s claim for sup-

port. Therefore, turning the party down should result in dissonance, giving rise to increased pro-

cessing aimed at resolving the experienced dissonance and to explain away the decision. Hence, we

predict that in the face of party cues, disagreeing (relative to agreeing) with a liked party (relative to

other parties) should, in general, be associated with increased reasoning effort (the Rejection Hy-

pothesis). From the group perspective, this prediction must, by necessity, be valid as signals of

disloyalty should trigger dissonance (Matz and Wood 2005). Still, it might be possible to specify a

similar prediction from a heuristic processing perspective. From that perspective, an increased effort

among those who disagree with an otherwise liked party may reflect a failure to utilize the party cue

rather than reflecting attempts to come up with good arguments for letting down the party (Kam

2005). If the group-oriented interpretation is correct, however, it should be possible to demonstrate

that the added cognitive effort is directly related to concerns about their standing within the group

of party supporters.

8

Page 9: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

The next prediction more directly discerns between the motivated reasoning and the heuristic pro-

cessing perspective. According to the heuristic processing account, citizens should limit cognitive

effort when toeing the party line. In contrast, the motivated reasoning hypothesis implies that under

specific circumstances, individuals are willing to exert effort in order to toe the party line. Research

on small-group psychology shows that individuals psychologically represent group support as an

exchange in which support for the group buys support from the group (Habyarimana et al. 2007;

Yamagishi and Mifune 2008). On this perspective, the costs involved in an act of support become

important. As the costs of support increase, the distress associated with granting it should increase

as well. In the context of political parties, one important cost of support relates to the extent to

which citizens are required to sacrifice other cherished predispositions such as ideological prin-

ciples in order to toe the party line on a specific issue. For example, to the extent that the Demo-

cratic Party adopts a conservative position on an issue, supporters of the Democratic Party are re-

quired to sacrifice their liberal principles in order to agree with the party on this issue. When a liked

party in this way sponsors a party-inconsistent proposal, two things should happen simultaneously:

First, citizens should still, to a significant extent, toe the party line, and studies have indeed demon-

strated that party supporters do follow their party even in situations where a party adopts policies

that are at odds with its normal ideological profile (Cohen 2003; Greitemeyer at al. 2009; Rahn

1993). Second, from a motivated reasoning perspective, agreeing with a liked party on such propos-

als should trigger reasoning effort in order to come up with reasons for bearing the high costs of

support (Cohen 2003). In fact, when these ideological costs become high enough, it might require

more reasoning effort to agree than to disagree with the party. Given this, we predict that, in the face

of party cues, agreeing with a liked party should be experienced as effortful when citizens are re-

quired to sacrifice ideological principles in order to do so (the Costly Support Hypothesis).

9

Page 10: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Previous research has demonstrated that the presence of incongruent information in itself induces

reasoning effort (Huckfeldt et al. 2005; Redlawsk 2002). When a party adopts a party-inconsistent

policy the inconsistency will, in other words, in itself trigger effort. This most likely reflects a form

of learning effect where participants try to integrate the new information with their previous under-

standings. Note, however, how the motivated reasoning perspective moves beyond this observation.

The prediction is not simply that the presence of party-inconsistent proposals triggers effort (i.e.,

that the party inconsistency of the proposal has a major effect on effort) or even that the presence of

party-inconsistent proposals from a liked party triggers effort (i.e., a two-way interaction between

party inconsistency and party attachment). Rather, the prediction here is that agreeing with highly

party-inconsistent proposals from parties is more effortful than disagreeing (which amounts to a

three-way interaction between party inconsistency, party attachment and level of agreement on ef-

fort). In more simple terms, the motivated reasoning perspective predicts that individuals increase

their reasoning effort when they are motivated to abandon ideological beliefs in order to follow

their party’s line. In order to signal loyalty to the group, citizens should nonetheless be willing to

pay this price and, hence, toe the party line even on party-inconsistent proposals.1

The Rejection Hypothesis and the Costly Support Hypothesis describe two mechanisms through

which the presence of party cues can trigger motivated reasoning. When parties claim support by

adopting positions on an issue, citizens’ opinion formation becomes more effortful to the extent that

citizens fail to meet the claim for support or citizens provide the support at high costs. Therefore,

we generally expect that opinion formation is more effortful when party cues are available.

1 To the extent that we are able to provide evidence for this role of agreement with a policy in modulating reasoning effort, this would be very hard to account for in a learning perspective. The unexpectedness of a piece of information (and, hence, the need to learn it) should be independent of whether one agrees or disagrees with that information. At least, if anything, it makes more sense to argue that an incongruent policy from a liked party is more unexpected (and, hence, requires more learning effort) if the individual disagrees with that policy. The motivated reasoning perspective, however, makes the directly opposite prediction: that agreeing with an incongruent policy from a liked party is more effortful.

10

Page 11: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

These predictions follow directly from the motivated reasoning perspective on the party sponsor

effect. In particular, The Costly Support Hypothesis and the expectation that party cues make opin-

ion formation more effortful violate the heuristic processing perspective, as such a processing mode

entails that the presence of party cues makes opinion formation smoother and less difficult. Hence,

empirical support for these predictions will favour the motivated reasoning perspective over the

heuristic processing perspective.

Measurement and Design

Researchers in political science increasingly call for greater reliance on implicit measures (i.e.,

measures that do not require introspection) when dealing with deep psychological processes that

evade conscious thought or, especially relevant in our case, are ridden with social desirability con-

cerns (Burdein et al. 2006; McDermott 2007). To assess effort during opinion formation, we com-

pare response latencies; that is, the time interval between stimulus onset and the individual’s re-

sponse (Fazio 1990). Measuring response latency is a standard implicit method in psychology when

seeking to assess conflict-induced effort (e.g., Payne et al. 1990; van Harreveld et al. 2004) and has

also been used in political science research to measure the level of processing during opinion form-

ation (Bassili 1993, 1995; Huckfeldt et al. 1999; Huckfeldt and Sprague 2000; Huckfeldt et al.

2005; Mulligan et al. 2003). As an implicit measure, response latency has the great advantage of

allowing us to measure effort as it unfolds (i.e., during the formation of opinion) rather than relying

on subjects’ post-hoc rationalizations. Specifically, increased response time is a tell-tale marker of

increased processing and effort during opinion formation: longer response latency implies increased

11

Page 12: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

processing demands. Hence, our basic approach in testing our three predictions is to examine the

effects on response latency of the specified factors.

Using response latency poses other difficulties in terms of sampling, however. On the one hand, it is

important to maximize the reliability of the measurement of response time. This speaks for conduct-

ing laboratory experiments, with all the practical limitations on sample representativeness this

method entails. On the other hand, the theory we propose is general and, hence, it is important to

test whether at least parts of the proposed effects can be traced in all voter segments. This speaks for

prioritizing the external validity of the studies, allowing us to generalize the results to voters in gen-

eral. To solve this quandary, we conducted two separate studies, each maximizing either the reliabil-

ity of measurement or the external validity. Specifically, Study 1 prioritized generalizability and

tested the Rejection Hypothesis using a nationally representative sample with response latency col-

lected over the Internet. Study 2 prioritized reliable measurement and was conducted in a laboratory

with a student sample. In Study 2, the test of the Rejection Hypothesis was replicated and, in addi-

tion, the more fine-grained measurement allowed us to test the Costly Support Hypothesis.

Study 1

Study 1 was designed to provide an externally valid test of the Rejection Hypothesis, and was based

on an experiment embedded in a web survey collected by YouGov Zapera in December 2007 among

1605 Danish citizens aged 18-74 and with a response rate of 61 per cent (RR1). Subjects were re-

cruited from YouGov Zapera’s standing web panel. To ensure national representativeness, quota

sampling on sex, age, and geographical location was used.2

2 See Table A1 in the online appendix for further information on sample representativeness.

12

Page 13: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Given the proposed existence of psychological motivations to meet claims for support from one’s

group, it follows from the Rejection Hypothesis that disagreeing with a liked party is, in general,

experienced as more difficult than agreeing. Testing the Rejection Hypothesis prediction requires

two independent variables: measures of agreement with the proposed policy and measures of the

degree of attachment to the party sponsor. As discussed, response latency is used to gauge the de-

pendent variable, level of effort in forming an opinion on the policy. Given our prediction, we ex-

pected the two independent variables to interact in their effect on response latency such that dis-

agreeing, compared to agreeing, becomes increasingly effortful with higher sympathy for the spon-

sor.

Data and measures

The experiment in Study 1 consisted of two (randomly assigned) conditions manipulating the party

sponsor of a specific policy proposal on which the subjects were asked to state their opinion.3 The

parties used were the Socialist People’s Party (SPP) and the Danish People’s Party (DPP), which are

two major parties of the left- and right-wing blocs, respectively. The policy, which was presented as

sponsored by either of the two parties, was: “The [Socialist People’s Party / Danish People’s Party]

has proposed that local governments test the language skills of all bilingual children and distribute

children with poor Danish skills to schools with fewer bilinguals.” This policy relates to the issue of

ethnic integration. In general, SPP and DPP hold contrasting positions on the issue of integration

but, importantly, the specific policy was chosen so that both parties could convincingly be envi-

sioned proposing it. First, the specific policy can be understood as perfectly consistent with the gen-

eral integration policy of both parties. SPP, which generally holds more lenient positions on integra-

tion issues, can plausibly support this position because language tests and distribution of children

3 To investigate whether the randomization procedure worked, we tested, using Chi2, whether subjects in the two condi-tions differed on gender, age, geographical region, and vote choice. None of the tests revealed any significant differ-ences (p-values ranged from .10 to .75)

13

Page 14: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

with poor Danish skills to other schools can be understood as a policy designed to help these chil-

dren in learning Danish. DPP, which generally holds a stricter position, can plausibly accept the

position because it can be seen as a requirement from society that bilinguals are integrated in ordin-

ary Danish schools. Second, the change in national law allowing for adopting such policies at the

municipal level was supported by DPP. Following that, a SPP mayor has promoted and supported

the policy in the municipality of the Danish capital, Copenhagen. We infer from this that the policy

can plausibly be sponsored by both parties.

To measure agreement with the two parties, subjects where asked “Do you agree or disagree with

the proposal from the [Socialist People’s Party / Danish People’s Party]?” The answer was meas-

ured on a 7-point scale with the respective endpoints labelled “completely disagree” and “com-

pletely agree”. To measure party attachments, we obtained ratings of sympathy toward each party

(“Do you like or dislike the following parties?”) on an 11-point scale with endpoints labelled

“Really dislike the party” and “Really like the party”. These ratings were obtained before exposure

to the experiment. Prior research clearly indicates that such ‘thermometer ratings’ of sympathy are

the most reliable indicators of party attachments in multiparty systems such as the Danish (see

Rosema 2006).4 Following previous research on party sponsor effects (Kam 2005), we use a col-

lapsed measure of party sympathy in the analyses. Specifically, we created a measure, termed Sym-

pathy for Party, tapping the sympathy for the party sponsoring the proposal in the subjects’ specific

experimental condition. Thus, for subjects in the condition in which the policy was proposed by the 4 Some researchers in American politics propose distinguishing between party identification and related measures (see, e.g., Green et al., 2002). However, not only do sympathy scores seem to be more valid indicators of party attachments in multiparty systems (Rosema, 2006), the classic way of measuring party identification (developed as it is in a two-party system) creates other methodological problems. Because of the abundance of parties, it will always be a minority of subjects who explicitly label themselves as identifiers with any two parties. In this respect, sympathy scores are preferable because they allow us to gauge fine-grained differences in levels of attachment between all subjects rather than simply lumping most of our subjects into one big ‘non-identifier’ category. At the same time, using sympathy scores should not make much difference as these measures are highly collinear with measures of party identification. In the survey, we also obtained the classic measure of party identification. 187 subjects identified either with the SPP or DPP. The correlations between sympathy scores and identifying with DPP compared to SPP are -.92 (p < 0.000) in the case of sympathy for SPP and .95 (p < 0.000) in the case of sympathy for DPP.

14

Page 15: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Socialist People’s Party, this measure reflects sympathy for the Socialist People’s Party and, re-

versely, it reflects sympathy for the Danish People’s Party for subjects in the other condition.5

To measure response time, we collected the time lapse – for each subject – from the policy’s appear-

ance on screen to the subject had stated his or her level of agreement and continued to the next

screen. This is unquestionably a noisy measure; most prominently because information needs to

travel back and forth over the web and, hence, will be affected by factors such as the speed of the

subjects’ Internet connection. To reduce noise in the data, we followed prior studies (Petersen et al.

2011; Petersen et al. 2012) and used ranked response latencies rather than the exact response

latency in milliseconds such that the fastest respondent is assigned the lowest value on the ranked

variable, the second fastest the second lowest value and so forth. All measures have been rescaled

between 0 and 1. There is independent evidence that this manoeuvre does in fact reduce measure-

ment noise. Two likely candidates for influencing response latency (age and level of education) do

not correlate in any way with the raw response latency (age: r=.004, p=.86; education: r=-.02, p

= .38) but show correlations with ranked response latency (age: r=.12, p =.000; education: r=-.08,

p=.001).

Results: testing the rejection hypothesis

As we attempt to explain the processing modes underlying party sponsor effects, it is relevant to

start by investigating whether we in fact obtain this in Study 1. In support, we find a correlation

5 This measurement not only follows extant research but is also the most parsimonious way to test our predictions. It could, however, be deemed inadequate for two reasons. First, it builds on the assumption that the effect of sympathy is similar across the two conditions (i.e., in the face of both SPP and the DPP). Second, at the outset, it does not allow us to directly test whether the predicted effects arise from sympathizing specifically with the sponsor or from sympathizing with just any party. Importantly, we deal thoroughly with both issues below (see endnotes 3 and 6). Here, we directly verify that the effects are indifferent across the two conditions and are related to sympathizing specifically with the sponsor.

15

Page 16: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

of .44 (p < 0.000) between party sympathy and level of agreement. Subjects are, in other words,

much more prone to agree with the proposal when it is sponsored by a party they like. According to

the Rejection Hypothesis, one reason is that disagreeing with one’s party (a sign of disloyalty) is

associated with dissonance that requires increased reasoning efforts to explain away. Specifically,

the Rejection Hypothesis states that disagreeing relative to agreeing with a party becomes

increasingly effortful the more subjects like the party sponsor.

To test our hypothesis, we regress response latency on agreement with the party, sympathy for the

party, and the two-way interaction between these measures (see Table 1). As expected, we find a

significant two-way interaction.6 To gauge the substance of this effect, the predicted values together

with 95 per cent confidence intervals are plotted in Figure 1. The two lines in Figure 1 represent the

response of agreeing completely and disagreeing completely with the party, respectively, and show

how the effects of disagreeing relative to agreeing change with changing levels of sympathy for the

party. To estimate the evidence for the Rejection Hypothesis, we need to focus on the difference

between the two lines and, hence, whether disagreeing compared to agreeing becomes more

difficult as the sympathy increases. For disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the response

time increases with increasing sympathy and, in reverse, for agreement with a statement (the black

line), the response time decreases with increasing sympathy. The net outcome is that among those

sympathizing with the party sponsor of a policy, there is a marked difference in response latency

between agreeing and disagreeing, such that disagreeing takes significantly longer than agreeing.

6 Additional analyses demonstrate that this effect of party sympathy is, first, the same across the two parties used in the study (i.e., whether the policy was sponsored by SPP or by DPP) and, second, specifically linked to sympathy for the sponsor rather than sympathy for any party. Specifically, we created a measure of sympathy for the non-sponsor (e.g., sympathy for DDP when SPP sponsored the policy). We then added two three-way interaction terms to the model presented in Table 1 with all lower-order terms between, first, agreement, party sympathy, and party sponsor (to test whether the effect was the same across conditions) and, second, agreement, sympathy for non-sponsor, and party spon-sor (to test whether the effects were specifically linked to sympathy for the sponsor). Consistent with our expectations, only one interaction term emerged as significant from this extended model: the interaction between party sympathy and agreement (F=6.22; p = .01).

16

Page 17: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Hence, in support of the Rejection Hypothesis, disagreeing with a liked party is effortful. The

substantive effect cannot readily be inferred from Figure 1, as it shows ranked latencies rather than

actual latencies in seconds in answers. Response latencies at rank .4 has a response time of 13,440

milliseconds at rank .5 (the median) 14,930, and at .6, it is 16,490 milliseconds. In other words, the

figure shows a substantive effect in the range of 1-2 seconds when comparing latencies for party

sympathizers disagreeing with the statement with party sympathizers agreeing.7

These results support our hypothesis that turning down claims for support from one’s group creates

mental dissonance, which, in turn, facilitates a more effortful opinion formation process, presum-

ably because the voter has to carefully evaluate the alternatives and reconcile to the final outcome.

This result is in line with psychological research on group dynamics (Matz and Wood 2005) and

lends support to the hypothesis that party cues are processed through motivated reasoning pro-

cesses. Still, one could make a case for the Rejection Hypothesis from a heuristic processing per-

spective as the increased effort associated with disagreeing with one’s party could be interpreted as

a failure to utilize the cue at hand and, hence, a more effortful opinion formation process would

ensue (e.g., Kam 2005).

To test this alternative interpretation, we collected a measure for an additional analysis. Hence, the

group psychological account entails that the increased effort induced by disagreeing with a liked

party reflects reasoning related to one’s standing in the group after signs of disloyalty. To directly

test this, we obtained a measure of the subjects’ perceived alignment with supporters of the sponsor-

ing party (subjects indicated on an 11-point scale the extent to which they thought of themselves

and supporters of the party as alike; for the analyses, this variable was recoded to vary between 0-

7 We also tested whether this interaction was robust to the inclusion of key demographic variables and included meas-ures of gender, age, education, and geographical location. If anything, these controls increased the effect of the two-way interaction (after control: F=14.91, p<.001; before control: F=10.41, p=.001).

17

Page 18: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

1). This item was asked right after subjects had indicated whether they agreed or disagreed with the

party and, hence, we were able to directly test how increased effort among those disagreeing with a

liked party relates to perceptions of one’s standing within the group of party supporters. We found a

significant three-way interaction effect between response latency, agreement with the party, and

sympathy for the party on the perceived alignment between oneself and party supporters (F=4.1,

p=.04). For those who disagreed with a liked party, a negative correlation was found between per-

ceived alignment and response latency (b = -.15 for those completely disagreeing with a proposal

sponsored by a party for which they had maximum sympathy; see Table A2 in the online appendix

for the full, specified model). As expected, if effort reflects reasoning related to concerns about

one’s standing in the group after disagreement, increased effort is related to a decrease in perceived

alignment between oneself and other party supporters.

An alternative interpretation of this additional analysis in line with the heuristic perspective could

be that respondents see the policy statements as new information on the parties. Learning from this

new information, they react by seeing themselves less in line with the party. While this interpreta-

tion cannot be entirely ruled out, we do not find it very likely. If the increased effort reflects learn-

ing rather than dissonance, we should expect the effect to be greatest among those most likely to

acquire new information from the policy statements. This should be among those with the lowest

level of initial information, the least politically aware subjects (Kam 2005; Zaller 1992). Hence, we

obtained measures of political awareness using five factual questions about politics and have tested

the existence of a three-way interaction between party sympathy, agreement (cf. the Rejection Hy-

pothesis), and this scale of political awareness on response time. This three-way interaction is

highly insignificant (F=.22; p=.64), implying that the effects exist independently of the political

18

Page 19: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

awareness of the subject. In other words, increased effort is not confined among those most likely to

acquire new information.

In sum, consistent with the Rejection Hypothesis, Study 1 demonstrates that disagreeing with a

liked party induces cognitive effort, and, consistent with the group-oriented interpretation, we have

demonstrated that this increased effort is related to concerns about one’s alignment with other party

supporters.

Study 2

With the Rejection Hypothesis supported, we move on to the Costly Support Hypothesis. It states

that agreeing with a liked party (i.e., using the party cue in the heuristic perspective) elicits effortful

motivated reasoning when the ideological costs of doing so are high. From a group psychological

perspective, providing support for one’s party is psychologically represented as an act of buying

support for oneself, and therefore, decisions for support should be cost sensitive. Furthermore, the

psychological dynamics underlying the Rejection and the Costly Support Hypotheses imply that

opinion formation in the presence of party cues is more effortful than opinion formation in their

non-presence. Quite simply, as entailed by the Rejection and the Costly Support Hypotheses, the

presence of group cues adds a whole range of difficult considerations that must be overcome

through motivated reasoning processes.

These predictions are tested in Study 2 in which response latency, again, is used to measure effort

during opinion formation. To be able to gauge more complicated effects, Study 2 prioritized reliable

measurement so data was collected in a laboratory. The Costly Support Hypothesis is especially

19

Page 20: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

demanding in terms of measures and analyses. In addition to the measures used in testing the Rejec-

tion Hypothesis, the Costly Support Hypothesis requires us to measure the ideological costs in-

volved in agreeing with a liked party. Hence, the Costly Support Hypothesis entails that response

latency is a function of the three-way interaction between these costs, agreement with the party, and

sympathy for the party.

Data and design

79 undergraduate university students from various disciplines were recruited (39 males, mean age =

21.5, std. dev. = 2.57). For participation, subjects earned a lottery ticket. They were told that they

would be presented with a series of policy statements from three sources: (1) The Socialist People’s

Party, (2) The Danish People’s Party, and (3) non-party sources. Specifically, these statements were

presented as “taken from the general public debate among, for example, experts and pundits” (see

below for discussion). Subjects were presented with 94 propositions on individual computer termin-

als using E-Prime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, http://www.pstnet.com/). Each statement ap-

peared on screen for a fixed time of 12 seconds, and subjects were instructed to answer as soon as

they had formed an opinion. The policies related to various policy areas (e.g., redistribution, crim-

inal justice, immigration, the European Union), and the ideological content of the statements varied

in such a way that one half was consistent with left-wing ideology and one half with right-wing

ideology. The policies were presented in a fixed pseudo-randomized order among the three source

conditions. Hence, subjects were exposed to identical statements, and the source of each statement

did not vary between subjects. Provenance was indicated by the logos of the parties and, in the case

of the non-party sources, an exclamation mark (see sample screens in Figure 2). Note that the sym-

bols in terms of overall shape and colours (red and white) are highly similar so response latency

should not be affected by differences in readability.

20

Page 21: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

We obtained the three measures used in Study 1: agreement, party sympathies and response latency.

First, to measure agreement, subjects were asked to either “Agree” or “Disagree” with the policy

statements. Hence, our measure of agreement is a dichotomous variable. Second, we obtained the

subjects’ party affiliation in a pre-experiment paper-and-pencil survey. Again, we used sympathy

ratings formatted and coded as in Study 1. Two subjects failed to provide sympathy ratings of the

focal parties and were removed. Third, response latency in milliseconds was collected by the E-

Prime software that provides millisecond precision timing both in the presentation of stimuli and in

obtaining response latency.

In addition, the test of the Costly Support Hypothesis requires a measure of the ideological costs of

agreeing with a specific statement. In measuring this, we follow a number of previous studies

(Greitemeyer et al. 2009) and focus on whether the statements are consistent or inconsistent with

the normal ideological profile of the party sponsor. Hence, we should expect positive feelings to-

wards a party to, at least in part, reflect a general endorsement of its ideological goals. An individual

sympathizing with a party should, ceteris paribus, be favourable towards typical, that is, party-con-

sistent, statements from the party. Conversely, as the party consistency of statements decreases, a

party follower will increasingly sacrifice his or her normal ideological values to meet the party’s

claim for support. Ratings of party consistency of statements were obtained based on a paper-and-

pencil survey completed by the first 24 subjects after their participation in the experiment. Subjects

were asked the following question. “Below you can find the statements from the [Socialist People’s

Party / Danish People’s Party], which you have just evaluated. Please indicate how typical the state-

ments are for the [Socialist People’s Party / Danish People’s Party]. Answers were recorded for each

statement on a 4-point scale (very typical, typical, less typical, not typical at all). A measure of the

21

Page 22: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

party consistency of each statement attributed to a party was obtained by averaging the answers of

the 24 subjects for each statement and reversing the measure such that higher values correspond to

higher levels of party consistency.

All variables, except response latency measured in milliseconds, vary between 0 and 1. In the ana-

lyses, the data from the subjects’ answers to all propositions was pooled. All analyses have been

conducted with cluster robust standard errors with subject as the cluster variable.8

Results: Testing the Costly Support Hypothesis

As a starting point, we analysed the extent to which we obtained party sponsor effects in Study 2;

that is, whether subjects tended to agree with their party. Our analyses clearly suggest that they do

as we find a positive and significant effect of sympathy for the party on agreement (χ2=31.22; p <

0.000). Consistent with previous studies, (Cohen 2003; Greitemeyer at al. 2009; Rahn 1993) there is

a tendency for subjects to toe the party line even on party-inconsistent proposals. To investigate this,

we used the original division of the statements into left- and right-leaning (see section on research

design). Focusing on the third of the subjects most sympathetic to SPP (the left-wing party), we see

that 65 per cent agree on right-leaning statements when they are attributed to SPP, but only 48 per

cent agree when they are attributed to DPP. This difference is significant (t = 5.07; p < 0.000).

Hence, those sympathetic to the left-wing party are more inclined to support right-wing statements

when these are sponsored by the left-wing relative to the right-wing party. Focusing on the third of

the respondents most sympathetic to DPP, we find that 51 per cent agree with left-leaning state-

8 In addition, we use a number of control strategies to control for differences across the experimental conditions left by the pseudo-randomized design. In testing the Costly Support Hypothesis, we focus only on the two (pooled) conditions with statements from a party. Here, a strong control option is available, and this analysis is controlled for the individual proposition in the form of a series of dummy variables. In this way, all potential differences between propositions have been controlled for. In testing the Mere Presence Hypothesis, we compare the conditions with parties to the condition without parties. The stronger control option is not available because the set of dummy variables is completely collinear with the condition variable because of the pseudo-randomized design. Therefore, to achieve some level of control, we include the length of the individual statements (specifically, their number of characters) as a control variable.

22

Page 23: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

ments when they are attributed to SPP while 62 per cent agree when they are attributed to DPP.

Again, this difference is significant (t=-3.02; p=0.003). Hence, although party sponsorship is only

one among many other factors affecting political attitudes, those sympathetic to the right-wing party

are more inclined to support left-wing statements when these are sponsored by the right-wing party

relative to the left-wing party.

This analysis shows that subjects are willing to sacrifice ideological principles in order to support

their party, but according to the Costly Support Hypothesis, such willingness does not come easily.

The Costly Support Hypothesis predicts that citizens experience dissonance and, hence, have more

difficulties expressing support for their party when they need to sacrifice ideological principles in

order to do so.

In testing the Costly Support Hypothesis, we focus only on the two (pooled) conditions with state-

ments from a party (see below for tests involving the condition without party cues). In the party

conditions, we expect response latency to be a function of three factors: 1) Whether the subject ex-

presses support for a statement from a party, 2) whether the subject feels affiliated with the party

sponsor and, finally, 3) whether the statement is consistent or inconsistent with the ideological pro-

file of the party. Hence, in more detail, we expect response latency to be a function of the three-way

interaction between agreement with the statement, sympathy for the party, and the party consistency

of the statement. Specifically, response latency should increase when subjects agree with a party-in-

consistent statement from a liked party. In Table 2, response time is regressed on the three-way in-

teraction term with all constitutive terms included.

23

Page 24: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

As shown in the table, there is a significant three-way interaction effect on response latency. To in-

terpret this effect, Figure 3 displays the predicted values (together with 95 per cent confidence inter-

vals) holding the party consistency of the statements constant at the maximum of the variables (Fig-

ure 3A) and minimum levels (Figure 3B). Figure 3A replicates the finding in Study 1. On party-

consistent proposals, increasing sympathy for the party is associated with decreasing response

latency for “agree” responses (black line) relative to “disagree” responses (grey line). We find, as in

study 1, that the response latency for party sympathizers is in the range of one or two seconds

longer for respondents disagreeing than for respondents agreeing.9 Hence, when parties advocate

policies in line with their ideological profile, it is effortful for party affiliates to turn down the claim

for support by disagreeing with the policy. This replication across different samples and different

measurement techniques lends important further support to the Rejection Hypothesis.

If the Costly Support Hypothesis is correct, however, we should not expect to find this pattern when

parties advocate party-inconsistent policies and, hence, supporters are required to sacrifice ideolo-

gical principles in order to follow the party. Turning to Figure 3B, we see that something quite dif-

ferent is indeed happening when subjects respond to party-inconsistent statements. On these state-

ments, increasing sympathy is associated with increasing response latency for “agree” responses

(black line) relative to “disagree” responses (grey line). The substantive effect is, for high sympathy

respondents, again in the range of a second or two longer. Hence, when meeting the claim for sup-

port is associated with high ideological costs, agreeing becomes increasingly effortful.10

9 Note that although differences between agreeing and disagreeing respondents are similar in Study 1 and 2, the actual level of the response latency cannot be compared directly. In Study 1, latency is measured in unknown environments (some respondents will be interrupted when completing the web survey), while Study 2 is executed in the lab. In Study 1, latencies included delays caused by Internet traffic; in Study 2, they were recorded precisely by software. Finally, Study 2 had a time limit; Study 1 did not. This resulted in considerably longer response latencies in Study 1 (median = 14,930 ms) compared to Study 2 (median = 5991 ms).10 Previous studies have shown that the presence of incongruent information increases response latency (Redlawsk, 2002; Huckfeldt et al., 2005); we find a similar effect in our data. In a non-interactive model, a main effect of party consistency on response latency is found, such that responses are prolonged when the statement is party-inconsistent (F1,

77 = 7.21, P = 0.009). As discussed in the section on our predictions, this simple effect cannot explain the complicated

24

Page 25: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

These results support the Costly Support Hypothesis.11 If party cues were processed using heuristic

processing, the availability of party cues would decrease processing effort; yet, the current evidence

demonstrates just the opposite, i.e., that following a liked party can be quite effortful. In this re-

spect, it is also important to recall that our subjects do in fact toe the party line on party-inconsistent

proposals despite the associated effort. In other words, our subjects do not agree with parties to min-

imize effort but are rather willing to spend reasoning effort to toe the party line on policies they

would normally disagree with (due to ideological concerns). This provides support for the motiv-

ated reasoning perspective.12

As noted, a final implication of motivated reasoning is that the mere presence of

party cues requires citizens to spend reasoning effort to weigh their group motivation against other

more regular issue-relevant considerations. In direct contrast, the heuristic perspective leads to the

expectation that the presence of party cues minimizes effort because the cues alleviate the need for

reasoning when forming opinions. It turns out that the average response time in the condition

three-way interaction effect entailed by the motivated reasoning perspective.11 Again, additional analyses demonstrate that this interactive effect of party sympathy is the same across the two parties used in the study (i.e., regardless whether the policy was sponsored by SPP or by DPP) and is specifically linked to sympathy for the sponsor rather than sympathy for any party. Following the logic specified in footnote 33, we expanded the model presented in Table 2. Specifically, we added two four-way interaction terms with all lower-order terms between, first, agreement, party sympathy, party consistency of the policy, and party sponsor (to test whether the effect was the same across conditions) and, second, agreement, sympathy for the non-sponsor, party consistency of the policy, and party sponsor (to test whether the effects were specifically linked to sympathy for the sponsor). Consistent with our expectations, the three-way interaction term between agreement, party sympathy, and party consistency of the policy was robust to the inclusion of these extra terms (F=13.50; p = .0004). In addition, neither the three-way interaction term with sympathy for non-sponsor nor the two four-way interaction terms were significant at the .05-level.12 Given the focus of the heuristic perspective on the moderating effects of political awareness, one could perhaps won-der whether the observed sensitivity to the ideological content of the proposals is due to the use of a politically aware student sample. This is, however, not the case. Hence, we obtained the exact same measure of political awareness in Study 2 as in Study 1, and a comparison reveals that the student sample in Study 2 is actually less aware than the aver-age citizen. On a scale from 0 to 1, the mean awareness in our representative sample (Study 1) is .57, while the mean awareness in the student sample (Study 2) is .42. In addition, we have tested whether the predicted effects are moder-ated by the students’ political awareness, but this is not the case. Hence, the four-way interaction term with political awareness is insignificant (p=.62).

25

Page 26: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

without party cues is in fact shorter than the average response in the conditions with party cues.13

We interpret this as further evidence for the motivated reasoning perspective.

Discussion

Our group identities are cherished and therefore become central vehicles for triggering motivated

reasoning (Matz and Wood 2005). For example, claims for support from one’s own group can trig-

ger dissonance and effortful processing if one fails to meet the claim, or if meeting the claim entails

high costs. In this article, we have applied this framework to situations in which political parties

signal their positions on issues. In two studies, we have provided support for the idea that party cues

trigger motivated reasoning. Party cues can substantially influence citizens’ opinions. They do so –

we hypothesize – because they are psychologically represented as group-based claims for support.

In line with the proposed framework, we have demonstrated, first, that turning down a claim for

support from a liked party is effortful (the Rejection Hypothesis). Second, meeting a claim for sup-

port from a liked party is effortful when this entails sacrificing ideological principles (the Costly

Support Hypothesis). Additional tests corroborated the motivated reasoning interpretation of these

effects and, in particular, showed that the mere presence of party cues triggers increased effort in the

moment an opinion is formed.

The empirical findings rest on a methodologically innovative design. First, we followed recent calls

for using implicit measures when investigating psychological processes. Using response latency as

a measure of effort allowed us to sidestep problems associated with relying on subjects’ post-hoc

13 The statements in the no-party condition were labelled by a visual cue of size, colour, and complexity similar to the party cues (see Figure 2). The comparison shows that the availability of party cues significantly prolongs the response time with 213 ms (F1, 77 = 20.39, P < 0.0001). Effort is, in other words, increased rather than decreased in the face of party cues.

26

Page 27: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

rationalizations and gauge effort in the very moment the opinion formation unfolded. In this way,

we ensured that our measures were not tainted by social desirability factors (e.g., reluctance to re-

port that agreeing with ones party was effortful). Second, we solved the inherent sampling problems

associated with achieving reliable implicit measurement by conducting two studies. Study 1 was

conducted over the Internet with a nationally representative sample, thereby maximizing external

validity; Study 2 was conducted in the laboratory using specialized software on a student sample,

thereby maximizing measurement reliability. In both studies, the standard prediction in psychology

on effort and group dynamics, the Rejection Hypothesis, was tested and supported. In addition, the

fine-grained measurement in Study 2 allowed us to test the Costly Support and, hence, demonstrate

the even more complicated effects on effort of the ideological costs of meeting a claim for support.

The marked consistency of the test of the Rejection Hypothesis across studies boosts our confidence

in the basic theoretical framework and its generalizability to the general public and, hence, in the

expectation that also the more complicated dynamics uncovered in Study 2 are general in nature.

This study provides strong evidence that the effect of party sponsorship on opinion formation can-

not be understood fully without considering motivational processes. At the same time, it is import-

ant to note that this finding does not imply that no other factors than party cues affect opinions (Bul-

lock 2011). Neither does it preclude that other processes also play important roles in the way party

cues structure citizens’ opinions. First of all, it is most likely that citizens also use party cues as in-

formation on how to comprehend the content of policy issues as suggested by several political re-

searchers (e.g., Koch 2001; Schaffner and Streb 2002; Squire and Smith 1988). The importance of

this dynamic most likely varies from case to case. In some cases, party cues can provide relevant

information. This is, for example, the case when party endorsements are given to issues closely re-

lated to old political cleavages such as the left-right dimension. Since a political party have a history

27

Page 28: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

and a reputation of promoting certain values, citizens can, with limited investments, get a feel for

the principles underlying a specific policy by knowing which party promotes it and which party

opposes it (see, e.g., Lau and Redlawsk 2006; Petersen et al. 2010). Importantly, such learning dy-

namics might even influence the effort with which citizens process novel policies. As established by

previous research, when parties promote policies that are at odds with their normal stance, citizens

use more effort to decipher the policies (Redlawsk 2002; Huckfeldt et al. 2005).14 For the purpose

of the present article, two observations are important in this regard: a) even when party cues cannot

provide much information, they still affect opinions (Hobolt 2007; Malhotra and Kuo 2009; Malho-

tra and Margalit 2010); b) the effect of these learning dynamics on reasoning effort cannot account

for the full set of results put forth in the present article. As discussed previously, we have not just

demonstrated that the presence of party-inconsistent information triggers effort or that the presence

of party-inconsistent information from a liked party triggers effort. Rather, we have shown that

agreeing with highly party-inconsistent proposals from liked parties is more effortful than dis-

agreeing. That is, in line with a motivated reasoning perspective, individuals increase their reason-

ing effort when they abandon ideological beliefs to toe their party’s line.

Second, a number of individual differences could moderate the effects we have observed. In both

studies, we collected data on a prominent individual differences measure in the literature: political

knowledge or awareness (e.g., Kam 2005; Zaller 1992). One could have suspected that people low

in knowledge would not, for example, notice that some policies were incongruent with the party or

that people high in knowledge were more resistant to toe the party line on incongruent policies. In

neither study, however, do we find effects of political knowledge. This lack of effect is, however,

not entirely unpredictable, as previous studies have found quite different effects of knowledge.

14 In other cases, party cues provide very limited information. This is the case for issues not related to the more general positions of parties. For instance, Hobolt (2007) argues that party cues on issues related to European integration will provide limited information in most European countries.

28

Page 29: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Some studies report greater party cue effects among the least knowledgeable (e.g., Kam 2005),

whereas other studies find the greatest effects among the most knowledgeable (e.g., Slothuus and

De Vreese 2010). One reason for these mixed findings – and the lack of effect that we observe –

could be that political knowledge both measures cognitive and emotional engagement in politics.

For example, while the knowledgeable know more about politics and are, hence, better able to see

the party inconsistency of the policies, they are also more emotionally invested in the parties, and

these cross-cutting pressures could cancel each other out. Recently, Bullock (2011) has found more

consistent effects using a more direct measure of cognitive ability, need for cognition, and future

research should dig deeper into how need for cognition shapes the different processes through

which party cues can influence attitudes.

Finally, it is important to note what we have and what we have not shown. We have provided evid-

ence that motivated reasoning plays a significant role in the processing of party cues. On the basis

of a view dating back to The American Voter (Campbell et al. 1960), we have argued that the motiv-

ation spurring these directional reasoning efforts emerges from group psychological processes; that

is, party attachments are psychologically represented as group attachments, and as a consequence,

people are motivated to appear loyal to their party. While this premise is consistent with previous

research (as we describe in the theory section), we have not provided new independent evidence of

the psychological underpinnings of the motivation here. Given this, we would like to point to new

emerging evidence in this regard. A line of social psychology research has developed an experi-

mental paradigm – called the “who said what?” paradigm – that can be specifically utilized to test

whether cues are processed by psychological mechanisms tailored to process group affiliations

(Kurzban et al. 2001; for a use of this paradigm in political science, see Petersen 2012). By focusing

on attention, these protocols measure the extent to which the availability of a given cue crowds out

29

Page 30: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

attention to alternative cues. To the extent that a specific cue competes exclusively for attention

with cues that previous research has established as group related, this provides solid evidence that

the cue in question is processed by group psychological mechanisms (see Kurzban et al. 2001). By

implication, investigating whether information about individuals’ party affiliation makes subjects

pay less attention to other group cues (e.g., the individuals’ race) without changing their attention to

non-group cues (e.g., the individuals’ sex or age) would be a direct demonstration that party attach-

ments are filtered through distinct group psychological mechanisms. New evidence suggest that this

is indeed the case (Pietraszewski et al., 2012) and provides more direct evidence for the group-

based underpinnings of the motivations that spur motivated reasoning in the context of party polit-

ics.

Which psychological model underlies party sponsor effects has important implications. In contrast

to the heuristic perspective, the motivated reasoning perspective implies that opinion formation in

the face of party cues is not accuracy-oriented (Taber and Lodge 2006). Rather, citizens are, in ef-

fect, motivated to be biased. The group perspective entails, first, that political elites cannot alleviate

partisan bias by providing alternate cues or by facilitating interest in a given topic. Paradoxically, in

their persuasive efforts to win public support for their policies, political parties are, by implication,

at the same time quite powerful and quite constrained. The corroboration of the group perspective

suggests that political parties can shape the opinions of their supporters merely by signalling their

positions, but cannot reach the supporters of opposite parties without having even intellectually

forceful messages distorted. Second, it opens the question of whether it is valid to talk about cit-

izens being ‘led astray’ by their party, or, at least, whether citizens themselves are willing to de-

scribe their own behaviour in these terms. In a traditional democratic perspective, citizens ought to

base their opinions on long-standing values. But, as argued by Green et al. (2002), if political en-

gagement grows out of partisan attachments, citizens might to a much lesser extent share the norm-

30

Page 31: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

ative ideal of value-based opinion formation. To many, it might be more important – psychologic-

ally as well as normatively – to signal loyalty to the party.

Acknowledgements

We thank Lene Aarøe, Jamie Druckman and Rune Slothuus for helpful comments and advice in

preparing this article.

31

Page 32: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

REFERENCES

Bassili, J. N. (1993). Response Latency versus Certainty as Indexes of the Strength of Voting Inten-tions in a Cati Survey. The Public Opinion Quarterly, 57, 54-61.

Bassili, J. N. (1995). On the Psychological Reality of Party Identification: Evidence from the Ac-cessibility of Voting Intentions and of Partisan Feelings. Political Behavior, 17, 339-358.

Bullock, J. G. (2011). Elite Influence on Public Opinion in an Informed Electorate. American Polit-ical Science Review, 105, 496-515. 

Burdein, I., Lodge, M. & Taber, C. (2006). Experiments on the Automaticity of Political Beliefs and Attitudes. Political Psychology, 7, 359-371.

Campbell, A, Converse, P. E., Miller, W. E. & Stokes, D. (1960). The American Voter. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

Cohen, G. L. (2003). Party over Policy: The Dominating Impact of Group Influence on Political Beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 808-822.

Eagly, A. H. and Chaiken, S. (1993). The Psychology of Attitudes. Belmont: Wardsworth Group/Thomson Learning.

Fazio, R. H. (1990). A Practical Guide to the Use of Response Latency in Social Psychological Re-search. In C. Hendrick and M. S. Clark, (Eds.), Research Methods in Personality and Social Psy-chology. Review of Personality and Social Psychology (pp. 74-97). Thousand Oaks: Sage.

Goren, P. (2005). Party Identification and Core Political Values. American Journal of Political Sci-ence, 49, 881-896.

Green, D., Palmquist, B. & Schickler, E. (2002). Partisan Hearts and Minds. New Haven: Yale Uni-versity Press.

Greene, S. (1999). Understanding Party Identification: A Social Identity Approach. Political Psy-chology, 20, 393-403.

Greitemeyer, T., Fischer, P., Frey, D. & Schulz-Hardt, S. (2009). Biased Assimilation: The Role of Source Position. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 22-39.

Habyarimana, J., Humphreys, M., Posner, D. N., & Weinstein, J. M. (2007). Why Does Ethnic Di-versity Undermine Public Goods Provision? American Political Science Review, 101, 709-725.

Hobolt, S. B. (2007). Taking Cues on Europe? Voter Competence and Party Endorsements in Refer-endums on European Integration. European Journal of Political Research, 46, 151-182.

Huckfeldt, R. & Sprague, J. 2000. Political Consequences of Inconsistency: The Accessibility and Stability of Abortion Attitudes. Political Psychology, 21, 57-79 (Special Issue: Response Latency Measurement in Telephone Surveys).

32

Page 33: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Huckfeldt, R., Levine, J., Morgan, W. & Sprague, J. (1999). Accessibility and the Political Utility of Partisan and Ideological Orientations. American Journal of Political Science, 43, 888-911.

Huckfeldt, R., Mondak, J. J., Craw, M. & Mendez, J. M. (2005). Making Sense of Candidates: Par-tisanship, Ideology, and Issues as Guides to Judgment. Cognitive Brain Research, 23, 11-23.

Jennings, K. M. and Niemi, R. G. (1974). The Political Character of Adolescence. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Jennings, K. M. and Niemi, R. G. (1981). Generations and Politics: A Panel Study of Young Adults and Their Parents. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.

Kam, C. D. (2005). Who Toes the Party Line? Cues, Values, and Individual Differences. Political Behavior, 27, 163-182.

Koch, J. W. (2001). When Parties and Candidates Collide: Citizen Perception of House Candidates’ Positions on Abortion. Public Opinion Quarterly, 65, 1-21.

Kunda, Z. (1990). The Case for Motivating Reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 480-498.

Kurzban, R., Tooby, J. & Cosmides, L. (2001). Can Race be Erased? Coalitional Computation and Social Categorization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98, 15387-15392.

Lau, R. R. & Redlawsk, D. P. (2001). Advantages and Disadvantages of Cognitive Heuristics in Political Decision Making. American Journal of Political Science, 45, 951-971.

Lau, R. R. & Redlawsk, D. P. (2006). How Voters Decide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Layman, G. C. & Carsey, T. M. (1998). Why Do Party Activists Convert? An Analysis of Indi-vidual-Level Change on the Abortion Issue. Political Research Quarterly, 51, 723-749.

Lebo, M. J. & Cassino, D. (2007). The Aggregated Consequences of Motivated Reasoning and the Dynamics of Partisan Presidential Approval. Political Psychology, 28, 719-746.

Malhotra, N. & Kuo, A. G. (2009). Emotions as Moderators of Information Cue Use. American Politics Research, 37, 301-326.

Malhotra, N. & Margalit, Y. (2010). Short-Term Communication Effects or Longstanding Disposi-tions? The Public’s Response to the Financial Crisis of 2008. Journal of Politics, 72, 852-867.

Matz, D. C. & Wood, W. (2005). Cognitive Dissonance in Groups: The Consequences of Disagree-ment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 22-37.

McDermott, R. (2007). Cognitive Neuroscience and Politics: Next Steps. In R. Neumann, G. Mar-cus, A. Crigler & M. MacKuen (Eds.), The Affect Effect. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (pp. 375-397).

Mondak, J. J. (1993). Source Cues and Policy Approval: The Cognitive Dynamics of Public Support for the Reagan Agenda. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 186-212.

33

Page 34: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Mulligan, K., Grant, T. J., Mockabee, S. T. & Monson, J. Q. (2003). Response Latency Methodo-logy for Survey Research: Measurement and Modeling Strategies. Political Analysis, 11, 289-301.

Payne, J. W., Bettman, J. R. & Johnson, E. (1990). The Adaptive Decision Maker: Effort and Accur-acy in Choice. In R. M. Hogarth (Ed.), Insights in Decision Making. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 129-153.

Petersen, M. B. (2012). Social Welfare as Small-Scale Help: Evolutionary Psychology and the De-servingness Heuristic. American Journal of Political Science, 56, 1-16.

Petersen, M. B., Slothuus, R., Stubager, R. & Togeby, L. (2010). Political Parties and Value Consist-ency in Public Opinion Formation. Public Opinion Quarterly, 74, 530-550.

Petersen, M. B., Slothuus, R., Stubager, R. & Togeby, L. (2011). Deservingness versus Values in Public Opinion on Welfare: The Automaticity of the Deservingness Heuristic. European Journal of Political Research, 50, 24-52.

Petersen, M. B., Sznycer, D., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2012). Who Deserves Help? Evolutionary Psychology, Social Emotions and Public Opinion about Welfare. Political Psychology, 33, 395-418.

Petty, R. E. and Cacioppo, J. T. (1981). Attitudes and Persuasion: Classic and Contemporary Ap-proaches. Dubuque, Iowa: Wm. C. Brown.

Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T. & Goldman, R. (1981). Personal involvement as a determinant of argu-ment-based persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 134-148.

Pietraszewski, D., Curry, O., Petersen, M. B., Cosmides, L. & Tooby, J. (2012) Politics erases race but not sex: Evidence that signals of political party support engage coalitional psychology. Working paper.

Rahn, W. M. (1993). The Role of Partisan Stereotypes in Information Processing about Political Candidates. American Journal of Political Science, 37, 472-496.

Redlawsk, D. P. (2002). Hot Cognition or Cool Consideration? Testing the Effects of Motivated Reasoning on Political Decision-Making. Journal of Politics, 64, 1021-1044.

Rosema, M. (2006). Partisanship, Candidate Evaluations, and Prospective Voting. Electoral Studies, 25, 467-488.

Schaffner, B. F. & Streb, M. J. (2002). The Partisan Heuristic in Low Information Elections. Public Opinion Quarterly, 66, 559-581.

Slothuus, R. & de Vreese, C. H. (2010). Political Parties, Motivated Reasoning, and Issue Framing Effects. The Journal of Politics, 72, 630-645.

Squire, P. & Smith, E.R.A.N. (1988). The Effect of Partisan Information on Voters in Nonpartisan Elections. The Journal of Politics, 50, 169-179.

34

Page 35: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Stanton, S. J., Beehner, J. C., Saini, E. K., Kuhn, C. M. & LaBar, K. S. (2009). Dominance, Politics, and Physiology: Voters’ Testosterone Changes on the Night of the 2008 United States Presidential Election, PLoS ONE,4:e7543.doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0007543.

Taber, C. S. & Lodge, M. (2006). Motivated Skepticism in the Evaluation of Political Beliefs. American Journal of Political Science, 50, 755-769.

van Harreveld, F., van der Pligt, J., de Vries, N. K., Wenneker, C. & Verhue, D. (2004). Ambival-ence and Information Integration in Attitudinal Judgment. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 431-447.

Westen, D., Blagov, P. S., Harenski, K., Kilts, C. & Hamann, S. (2006). Neural Bases of Motivated Reasoning: An fMRI Study of Emotional Constraints on Partisan Political Judgment in the 2004 U.S. Presidential Election. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 1947-1958.

Yamagishi T. & Mifune, N. (2008). Does Shared Group Membership Promote Altruism?: Fear, Greed, and Reputation. Rationality and Society 20: 5-30.

Zaller, J.R., 1992. The Nature and Origins of Mass Opinion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

35

Page 36: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Table 1. Response latency (rank) as a function of agreement with a party’s policy and sympathy

for the party

ModelIntercept .49***

(.02)Agreement with Party .08

(.04)Sympathy for Party .13*

(.06)Agree Sympathy -.26**

(.08)R2 (adj.) .01

Notes. Notes. N = 1417. Coefficients are OLS-regression estimates with standard errors in paren-

theses. All variables vary between 0 and 1.

*p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001

36

Page 37: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Table 2. Response latency in milliseconds as a function of agreement with a party’s policy, sym-

pathy for the party and the party consistency of the policy

ModelIntercept 8044.55***

(403.07)Agreement with party -651.22

(371.79)Sympathy for party -992.51

(613.57)Party consistency of policy -1360.68*

(536.77)Agree Sympathy 2937.72***

(739.85)Agree Party consistency 885.09

(625.18)Sympathy Party consistency 2767.12**

(917.74)Agree Sympathy Party consistency -5809.38***

(1286.43)R2 (adj.) 0.17

Notes. N = 4644. Only responses on statements attributed to a party are included in the analysis.

Coefficients are OLS-regression estimates with cluster robust standard errors in parentheses. Data is

clustered on subject. The model includes dummy variables for each individual proposition. All vari-

ables vary between 0 and 1, except response latency measured in milliseconds.

*p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001

37

Page 38: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Figure 1. Predicted response latency as a function of agreement with a party’s policy and sym-

pathy for the party

Notes. Predicted values computed from Table 1. Effects displayed for Agreement = 0 (Disagree)

and Agreement = 1 (Agree) and with 95 per cent confidence intervals. Ranked response latency

varies between 0 and 1.

38

Page 39: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Figure 2. The set-up of the experimental interface

Note. For readability, the figures depart slightly from the actual screens in terms of font

size etc. Figure 2A displays a translated sample proposition (“Public transport should be

free of charge”) from condition with the left-wing party, the Socialist People’s Party.

Figure 2B displays a translated sample proposition (“We should cut expenses to the

National Guard”) for the right-wing party, Danish People’s Party. Figure 2C displays a

translated sample proposition from non-party sources (“The EU should adopt common

immigration rules”).

39

Page 40: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Figure 3. Predicted response latency as a function of agreement with a party’s policy, sympathy

for the party and the party consistency of the policy

A B

Notes. Predicted values computed from Table 2. Effects displayed for Party Consistency = 1 (Panel

A) and Party Consistency = 0 (Panel B) and with 95 per cent confidence intervals.

40

Page 41: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Online Appendix

Table A1. Representativeness of Sample for Study 1. Percentages of Sample and Population reported.

Study 1Sample Population

Sexa

Female 49.22 49.89Male 50.78 50.10

Agea

18-29 18.82 19.4030-39 20.25 17.7040-49 19.88 20.8450-59 21.68 18.5260-74 19.38 21.79

Geographical Regiona

Copenhagen Area 25.67 22.01Rest of Sealand and islands 28.66 32.27Jutland 45.67 45.72

Highest Educationa

Elementary School 19.00 31.08High School 6.11 6.08High School (technical) 6.11 2.28Vocational School 25.11 33.04Short Tertiary Education 10.59 4.15Medium Tertiary Education 21.18 12.48Long Tertiary Education 11.28 6.01PhD Degree 0.62 0.36

Votingb

Social Democrats 24.47 25.50Social Liberals 5.29 5.10Conservatives 12.30 10.40Socialist People’s Party 16.07 13.00Christian Democrats 0.86 0.90Danish People’s Party 12.17 13.90Liberals 22.55 26.20New Alliance 3.77 2.80Red/Green Alliance 2.51 2.20

a Information about population proportions from Statistics Denmark (http://www.dst.dk/en). Propor-tions obtained for January 2008. The sample was collected in December 2007.b Population proportions on voting patterns is the actual election results from the general national election for the Danish Parliament (Folketinget) held on November 13 2007. Election result was obtained from the Danish Ministry of the Interior Affairs (http://www.im.dk/English.aspx).

41

Page 42: Citizens toe the party line when forming opinions on …pure.au.dk/portal/files/48906064/Motivated_reasoning_and... · Web viewFor disagreement with a statement (the grey line), the

Table A2. Subjects’ perceived alignment with supporters of a party as a function of agreement with the party’s policy, sympathy for the party and response latency for agreement.

ModelIntercept -.03

(.03)Agreement with party .08

(.05)Sympathy for party .59***

(.07)Response latency .03

(.05)Agree Sympathy .10

(.10)Agree Response latency -.07

(.09)Sympathy Response latency -.18

(.13)Agree Sympathy Response latency .37*

(.18)R2 (adj.) 0.65

Notes. N = 1359. Coefficients are OLS-regression estimates with standard errors in parentheses. All variables vary between 0 and 1.*p < 0.05 **p < 0.01 ***p < 0.001

42