666

Circuitos Medulares Ingles

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • This page intentionally left blank

  • THE CIRCUITRY OF THEHUMAN SPINAL CORD

    Studies of human movement have proliferated in recent

    years, and there have been many studies of spinal path-

    ways in humans, their role in movement, and their dys-

    function in neurological disorders. This comprehen-

    sive reference surveys the literature related to the con-

    trol of spinal cord circuits in human subjects, showing

    how they can be studied, their role in normal move-

    ment, and how they malfunction in disease states. The

    distinguished authors each bring to the book a life-

    times research and practice in neuroscience, motor

    control neurobiology, clinical neurology and rehabili-

    tation. Chapters are highly illustrated and consistently

    organised, reviewing, for each pathway, the experimen-

    tal background, methodology, organisation and con-

    trol, role during motor tasks, and changes in patients

    with CNS lesions. Each chapter concludes with a helpful

    resume that can be used independently of the main text

    to provide practical guidance for clinical studies. This

    is therefore the last word on the role of the spinal cord

    in human motor control. It will be essential reading for

    research workers and clinicians involved in the study,

    treatment and rehabilitation of movement disorders.

    Emmanuel Pierrot-Deseilligny is Professor of Rehabil-

    itation and Clinical Neurophysiology at the Hopital de

    la Salpetrie`re, University of Paris.

    David Burke is Professor and Dean of Research and

    Development at the College of Health Sciences, Univer-

    sity of Sydney.

  • THE CIRCUITRYOF THE HUMANSPINAL CORD

    Its Role in Motor Controland Movement Disorders

    Emmanuel Pierrot-DeseillignyHopital de la Salpetrie`re

    and

    David BurkeUniversity of Sydney

  • cambridge university pressCambridge, New York, Melbourne, Madrid, Cape Town, Singapore, So Paulo

    Cambridge University PressThe Edinburgh Building, Cambridge cb2 2ru, UK

    First published in print format

    isbn-13 978-0-521-82581-8

    isbn-13 978-0-511-12544-7

    Cambridge University Press 2005

    2005

    Information on this title: www.cambridge.org/9780521825818

    This publication is in copyright. Subject to statutory exception and to the provision ofrelevant collective licensing agreements, no reproduction of any part may take placewithout the written permission of Cambridge University Press.

    isbn-10 0-511-12544-5

    isbn-10 0-521-82581-4

    Cambridge University Press has no responsibility for the persistence or accuracy of urlsfor external or third-party internet websites referred to in this publication, and does notguarantee that any content on such websites is, or will remain, accurate or appropriate.

    Published in the United States of America by Cambridge University Press, New York

    www.cambridge.org

    hardback

    eBook (EBL)eBook (EBL)

    hardback

  • Contents

    Preface page xvAcknowledgements xixList of abbreviations xxi

    1 General methodology 1The monosynaptic reflex : H reflex andtendon jerk 1

    Initial studies 1Underlying principles 2Basic methodology 4Limitations related to mechanisms

    acting on the afferent volley of thereflex 11

    Pool problems related to theinputoutput relationship in themotoneurone pool 16

    Normative data and clinical value 20Critique: limitations, advantages and

    conclusions 21The F wave 21

    Underlying principles and basicmethodology 21

    Characteristics of the F wave 22F wave as a measure of excitability of

    motoneurones 23Clinical applications 24Conclusions 24

    Modulation of the on-going EMG activity 24Underlying principles and basic

    methodology 24Changes in the on-going EMG and in

    the H reflex need not be identical 26

    v

  • vi Contents

    Post-stimulus time histograms (PSTHs)of the discharge of single motor units 28

    Underlying principles 29Basic methodology 29Assessment of the timing of the

    changes in firing probability 32Assessment of the size and significance

    of the peaks and troughs in the PSTH 34Critique: limitations, advantages and

    conclusions 36Unitary H reflex 37

    Underlying principles and basicmethodology 37

    Significance of changes in CFSproduced by conditioning stimuli 38

    Critique: limitations, advantages andconclusions 39

    Stimulation of the motor cortex 39EMG responses evoked by cortical

    stimulation 39Electrical stimulation 40Magnetic stimulation 42Critique: advantages, limitations,

    conclusions 44Spatial facilitation 45

    Underlying principles 45Spatial facilitation judged in the PSTH

    of single units recordings 46Spatial facilitation judged from

    monosynaptic test reflexes 47Conclusions 48

    Coherence analysis between EMG/EMGor EEG/EMG signals 48

    Cross-correlation 48Coherence techniques 48

    General conclusions 49Methods 49Development 49

    Resume 49Monosynaptic reflex 49F wave 52Modulation of the on-going EMG 53Post-stimulus time histograms

    (PSTHs) of the discharge of singlemotor units 53

    Unitary H reflex 54

    Stimulation of the motor cortex 55Spatial facilitation 56Coherence analysis in EMG/EMG or

    EEG/EMG signals 56References 56

    2 Monosynaptic Ia excitation andpost-activation depression 63Background from animal experiments 64

    Initial findings 64Pathway of monosynaptic Ia excitation 64Distribution of heteronymous

    monosynaptic Ia excitation 65The stretch reflex 66

    Methodology 66Underlying principles 66Homonymous monosynaptic Ia

    excitation 66Heteronymous monosynaptic Ia

    excitation 70Range of electrical thresholds of Ia

    afferents when stimulating usingsurface electrodes 77

    Organisation and pattern of connections 79Homonymous monosynaptic Ia

    excitation 79Heteronymous monosynaptic Ia

    excitation in the lower limb 81Heteronymous monosynaptic Ia

    excitation in the upper limb 83Developmental changes in

    heteronymous Ia connections 86Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 87

    Homonymous monosynaptic Iaexcitation. Stretch reflexresponses 87

    Heteronymous monosynaptic Iaexcitation 92

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 95

    Methodology 95Peripheral neuropathies,

    mononeuropathies and proximalnerve lesions 95

    Spasticity 96

  • Contents vii

    Post-activation depression at the Iaafferent-motoneurone synapse 96

    Background from animal experiments 96Functional significance 97Methodology 97Post-activation depression in spastic

    patients 99Conclusions 100

    Role of monosynaptic Ia excitation innatural motor tasks 100

    Changes in monosynaptic Ia excitationin patients 101

    Resume 101Importance of studies of Ia

    connections 101Background from animal

    experiments 101Methodology 102Organisation and pattern of

    connections 103Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 104Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 105Post-activation depression at the

    Ia-motoneurone synapse 106References 106

    3 Muscle spindles and fusimotor drive:microneurography and othertechniques 113Background from animal experiments 113

    Initial investigations 113Current views of spindle structure and

    function 114 (skeleto-fusimotor) neurones 117

    Methodology 117Discredited techniques 117Acceptable techniques 119Critique of the tests to study muscle

    spindle afferent discharge andfusimotor drive 126

    Organisation and pattern of connections 127Background fusimotor drive 127Effects of cutaneous afferents on

    fusimotor neurones 127

    Corticospinal volleys 130Effects of muscle vibration on human

    muscle spindles 130Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 131

    Reflex reinforcement by remotemuscle contraction: the Jendrassikmanoeuvre 131

    Effects of voluntary effort on fusimotordrive to the contracting muscle 133

    Possible role of the fusimotor systemduring normal movement 136

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 138

    Spasticity 139Parkinsons disease 140

    Conclusions 141Resume 142

    Background from animal experiments 142Methodology 142Critique of the tests to study fusimotor

    drive 143Organisation and pattern of

    connections 143Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 144Changes in fusimotor activity in

    patients 145References 145

    4 Recurrent inhibition 151Background from animal experiments 151

    Initial findings 151General features 151Input to Renshaw cells 152Projections of Renshaw cells 153Conclusions 154

    Methodology 154Using homonymous antidromic motor

    volleys is an invalid technique inhumans 154

    The paired H reflex technique toinvestigate homonymous recurrentinhibition 155

    Methods for investigatingheteronymous recurrent inhibition 161

  • viii Contents

    Organisation and pattern of connections 169Homonymous recurrent projections to

    motoneurones 169Heteronymous recurrent projections

    to motoneurones in the lower limb 169Heteronymous recurrent projections

    to motoneurones in the upper limb 170Recurrent inhibition of interneurones

    mediating reciprocal Ia inhibition 171Corticospinal suppression of recurrent

    inhibition 173Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 173

    Recurrent inhibition of motoneuronesof a muscle involved in selectivecontractions 173

    Recurrent inhibition duringcontraction of the antagonisticmuscle 180

    Recurrent inhibition of antagonisticmuscles involved in co-contraction 180

    Heteronymous recurrentinhibition and heteronymous Iaexcitation 183

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 184

    Spasticity 184Patients with other movement

    disorders 187Conclusions 187

    Changes in recurrent inhibition innormal motor control 187

    Changes in recurrent inhibition andpathophysiology of movementdisorders 188

    Resume 188Background from animal experiments 188Methodology 188Organisation and pattern of

    connections 190Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 191Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 192References 192

    5 Reciprocal Ia inhibition 197Background from animal experiments 197

    Initial findings 197General features 198Projections from Ia interneurones 199Input to Ia interneurones 199Presynaptic inhibition 200Conclusions 201

    Methodology 201Underlying principles 201Inhibition of various responses 201Evidence for reciprocal Ia inhibition 204Critique of the tests to study reciprocal

    Ia inhibition 208Organisation and pattern of connections 209

    Pattern and strength of reciprocal Iainhibition at rest at hinge joints 209

    Absence of true reciprocal Iainhibition at wrist level 211

    Cutaneous facilitation of reciprocal Iainhibition 214

    Descending facilitation of reciprocal Iainhibition 215

    Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 217

    Voluntary contraction of theantagonistic muscle 217

    Reciprocal Ia inhibition directed tomotoneurones of the active muscle 223

    Reciprocal Ia inhibition duringco-contraction of antagonisticmuscles 225

    Changes in reciprocal Ia inhibitionduring postural activity 227

    Changes in reciprocal Ia inhibitionduring gait 227

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 229

    Methodology 229Spasticity 229Patients with cerebral palsy 233Patients with hyperekplexia 233Patients with Parkinsons disease 233Changes in non-reciprocal group I

    inhibition at wrist level 234

  • Contents ix

    Conclusions 234Role of reciprocal Ia inhibition in

    motor tasks 234Changes in reciprocal Ia inhibition and

    pathophysiology of movementdisorders 235

    Resume 235Background from animal experiments 235Methodology 235Organisation and pattern of

    connections 236Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 237Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 238References 239

    6 Ib pathways 244Background from animal experiments 244

    Initial findings 244Golgi tendon organs and Ib afferents 245General features 245Projections of Ib afferents 246Input to Ib interneurones 247Contraction-induced Ib inhibition 248Presynaptic inhibition and

    post-activation depression 248Reflex reversal during fictive

    locomotion 248Methodology 249

    Ib inhibition 249Evidence for Ib inhibition 252Oligosynaptic group I excitation 255Critique of the tests to reveal Ib effects 255

    Organisation and pattern of connections 256Pattern and strength of Ib inhibition 256Oligosynaptic group I excitation 258Convergence of Ia afferents onto

    interneurones mediating Ibinhibition 260

    Effects of low-threshold cutaneousafferents 261

    Facilitation of Ib inhibition by jointafferents 263

    Effects from nociceptive afferents 265

    Descending effects 265Multiple convergence onto common

    interneurones 265Conclusions: necessity for

    convergence of multiple inputs 267Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 267

    Suppression of Ib inhibition tovoluntarily activatedmotoneurones 268

    Ib inhibition directed tomotoneurones not involved in thevoluntary contraction 272

    Changes in Ib inhibition duringwalking 273

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 275

    Ib inhibition 275Ib excitation in spastic patients 277

    Conclusions 279Role of changes in Ib inhibition during

    motor tasks 279Changes in Ib pathways and the

    pathophysiology of movementdisorders 279

    Resume 279Background from animal

    experiments 279Methodology 280Organisation and pattern of

    connections 280Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 281Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 282References 283

    7 Group II pathways 288Background from animal experiments 288

    Initial findings 288Muscle spindle secondary endings and

    group II afferents 289Synaptic linkage 289Projections from group II

    interneurones 291

  • x Contents

    Excitatory inputs to group IIinterneurones 291

    Inhibitory control systems 292Methodology 293

    Underlying principles 293Stretch-induced homonymous

    group II excitation of leg and footmuscles 293

    Electrically induced heteronymousgroup II excitation 293

    Evidence for muscle group II excitation 297Critique of the tests used to reveal

    group II actions 299Organisation and pattern of connections 302

    Peripheral pathway 302Central pathway of group II excitation 303Distribution of group II excitation 304Convergence with other peripheral

    afferents 305Peripheral inhibitory input to

    interneurones co-activated by group Iand II afferents 307

    Corticospinal control of peripheralfacilitation 307

    Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 310

    Voluntary contractions 310Postural tasks 312Changes in group II excitation

    during gait 314Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 320

    Peripheral neuropathies 320Spasticity 320Parkinsons disease 326

    Conclusions 326Role of group II pathways in natural

    motor tasks 326Changes in group II excitation and

    pathophysiology of movementdisorders 328

    Resume 328Background from animal experiments 328Methodology 328

    Organisation and pattern ofconnections 330

    Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 331

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 331

    References 332

    8 Presynaptic inhibition of Ia terminals 337Background from animal experiments 337

    Initial findings 337General features 337Inputs to PAD interneurones 339Selectivity of the control of presynaptic

    inhibition 339Conclusions 340

    Methodology 340Discrepancy between the variations in

    the on-going EMG and those in theH reflex 340

    Activating PAD INs by a conditioningvolley to assess their excitability 340

    Background presynaptic inhibitioninferred from Ia facilitation of theH reflex 345

    Techniques using single motor units 346Conclusions 347

    Organisation and pattern ofconnections 347

    Projections on Ia terminals directed todifferent motoneurone types 347

    Organisation in subsets with regard tothe target motoneurones of Iaafferents 348

    Peripheral projections to PADinterneurones 348

    Corticospinal projections 350Vestibulospinal projections 353Tonic level of presynaptic inhibition of

    Ia terminals 353Weak sensitivity of stretch-evoked Ia

    volleys to presynaptic inhibition 354Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 355

  • Contents xi

    Ia terminals on lower limbmotoneurones involved in voluntarycontractions 355

    Ia terminals directed to motoneuronesof inactive synergistic muscles 359

    Presynaptic inhibition of Ia terminalsduring contraction of antagonisticmuscles 360

    Presynaptic inhibition of Ia terminalsduring contraction of remotemuscles 361

    Changes in presynaptic inhibition ofIa terminals on upper limbmotoneurones 362

    Changes in presynaptic inhibitionduring upright stance 363

    Changes in presynaptic inhibitionduring gait 365

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 367

    Methodology 367Spasticity 368Changes in presynaptic inhibition in

    Parkinsons disease 371Changes in presynaptic inhibition

    of Ia terminals in patients withdystonia 371

    Conclusions 372Role of changes in presynaptic

    inhibition of Ia terminals in normalmotor control 372

    Changes in presynaptic inhibition andpathophysiology of movementdisorders 373

    Resume 373Background from animal experiments 373Methodology 374Organisation and pattern of

    connections 375Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 376Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 377References 378

    9 Cutaneomuscular, withdrawal andflexor reflex afferent responses 384Background from animal experiments 385

    Initial findings 385Cutaneous responses mediated

    through private pathways 385FRA reflex pathways 388Conclusions 391

    Methodology 391Underlying principles 391Stimuli 391Responses recorded at rest 394Modulation of motoneurone

    excitability 396Critique of the tests to study cutaneous

    effects 396Organisation, connections andphysiological implications of withdrawalreflexes 399

    Afferent pathway of withdrawalreflexes 399

    Central pathway of early withdrawalresponses 400

    Functional organisation of earlywithdrawal reflexes 401

    Late withdrawal responses 407Interactions between different inputs

    in withdrawal reflex pathways 411Changes in withdrawal reflexes

    during motor tasks 412Organisation, connections andphysiological implications ofcutaneomuscular reflexes evoked bynon-noxious stimuli 414

    The different responses 414Afferent conduction 418Central pathway of short-latency

    responses occurring at spinallatency 418

    Central pathway for long-latencyeffects 421

    Projections of cutaneousafferents to different types ofmotoneurones 424

  • xii Contents

    Pattern and functional role of earlyresponses 427

    Changes in patients and clinicalimplications 432

    Complete spinal transection 433Upper motoneurone lesions other

    than those due to a complete spinaltransection 433

    Grasp reflex 436Parkinsons disease 436Peripheral neuropathies 437Diagnostic uses 437

    Conclusions 438Role of cutaneous reflexes in motor

    control 438Changes in cutaneous reflexes in

    patients 438Resume 439

    Background from animalexperiments 439

    Methodology 440Withdrawal reflexes 441Cutaneomuscular reflexes evoked by

    non-noxious stimuli 442Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 444References 445

    10 Propriospinal relay for descendingmotor commands 452The cervical propriospinal system 452Background from animal experiments 452

    The propriospinal system in the cat 452Conflicting results in the monkey 454

    Methodology 455Propriospinally mediated excitation

    produced by peripheral volleys 455Cutaneous suppression of

    descending excitation 458Rostral location of the relevant

    interneurones with respect tomotoneurones 459

    Organisation and pattern ofconnections 460

    Excitatory inputs to propriospinalneurones 460

    Inhibition of propriospinal neuronesvia feedback inhibitoryinterneurones 463

    Interaction between excitatory andinhibitory inputs 467

    Organisation of the cervicalpropriospinal system 468

    Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 471

    Evidence for propriospinaltransmission of a part of thedescending command 471

    Propriospinally mediated facilitationof motoneurones during voluntarycontraction 474

    Functional implications: role of thepropriospinal relay in normal motorcontrol 476

    Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 479

    Patient with a discrete lesion of thespinal cord at the junction C6C7spinal level 479

    Stroke patients 481Patients with Parkinsons disease 484

    Conclusions 485Role of propriospinal transmission of

    a part of the descending command 485Changes in propriospinal

    transmission of the command inpatients 485

    Resume 486Background from animal

    experiments 486Methodology 486Organisation and pattern of

    connections 487Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 488Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 489The lumbar propriospinal system 490Background from animalexperiments 490Methodology 491

    Underlying principle 491

  • Contents xiii

    Non-monosynaptic excitation ofvoluntarily activated single motorunits 491

    Non-monosynaptic excitation ofcompound EMG responses 493

    Rostral location of the relevantinterneurones 493

    Organisation and pattern ofconnections 494

    Peripheral excitatory input toexcitatory lumbar propriospinalneurones 494

    Peripheral inhibitory inputs tolumbar propriospinal neurones 496

    Peripheral inhibition ofmotoneurones 497

    Corticospinal control 498Motor tasks and physiologicalimplications 500

    Propriospinally mediated changes inthe quadriceps H reflex during weakcontractions 500

    Modulation of the on-going EMGduring different motor tasks 502

    Functional implications 502Studies in patients and clinicalimplications 503

    Spasticity 503Patients with Parkinsons disease 503

    Conclusions 505Resume 505

    Background from animalexperiments 505

    Methodology 505Organisation and pattern of

    connections 505Motor tasks and physiological

    implications 506Studies in patients and clinical

    implications 506References 506

    11 Involvement of spinal pathwaysin different motor tasks 511Isometric tonic contractions 512

    Fusimotor drive 512

    Cutaneomuscular responses 514Suppression of transmission in

    inhibitory pathways 514Conclusions 515

    Flexionextension movementsinvolving hinge joints 515

    Afferent discharges accompanying avoluntary flexionextensionmovement 515

    Excitation of active motoneurones 516Control of different features of the

    movement 517Recruitment of different types of

    motor units 518Inhibition of antagonists 519Timing of the different effects 520Different strategies for proximal and

    distal movements 521Conclusions 522

    Movements involving ball joints 522Different organisation of the

    human spinal circuitry at wristlevel 522

    Non-reciprocal group I inhibitionduring wrist movements 524

    Changes in presynapticinhibition on Ia terminals on wristmotoneurones 526

    Other spinal pathways possiblyinvolved in wrist movements 526

    Co-ordinated activation of varioussynergies 527

    Where are motor synergies laiddown? 527

    Synergies based on hardwiredspinal connections 528

    Cervical propriospinal system 529State-dependent modulation of

    sensory feedback 530Motor learning 530

    Co-contractions of antagonists at thesame joint 531

    Control of spinal pathways duringco-contraction of antagonists 531

    Control of the decreased inhibitionbetween antagonists 533

  • xiv Contents

    Joint stiffness 533Control of the stretch reflex at hinge

    joints 534Control of the excitation at ball joints 534Conclusions 535

    Maintenance of bipedal stance 535Normal quiet standing 535Unstable postural tasks

    requiring prolonged musclecontractions 537

    Responses to fast transientperturbations of stance 538

    Gait 542Characteristics of human walking 542Changes in transmission in spinal

    pathways during normal walking 545Reactions to external perturbations 547Running, hopping, landing 550

    References 550

    12 The pathophysiology of spasticityand parkinsonian rigidity 556Spasticity 556

    What is spasticity? What is it not? 557Spasticity and animal decerebrate

    rigidity are unrelated 560

    Possible spinal mechanismsunderlying the pathophysiologyof spasticity at rest 560

    Why do spinal pathwaysmalfunction? 571

    Changes in the intrinsicproperties of muscles fibres(contracture) 572

    Changes in spinal pathways duringmovements in spasticity 573

    Pathophysiology of spasticity aftercerebral lesions 575

    Pathophysiology of spasticity afterspinal lesions 580

    Conclusions 582Parkinsons disease 582

    Possible mechanisms underlyingParkinsonian rigidity 582

    Transmission in spinal pathwaysat rest 584

    Alterations of transmissionin spinal pathways during motortasks 589

    Conclusions 592References 592

    Index 601

  • Preface

    Spinal mechanisms in the control of movement. Inthe 19101920s Paul Hoffmann demonstrated thatpercutaneous electrical stimulation of the posteriortibial nerve in human subjects produced a synchro-nised response in the soleus muscle with the samecentral delay as the Achilles tendon jerk. This land-mark study long preceded Lloyds identification ofthe corresponding pathway in the cat (1943). Subse-quently, much of the primary knowledge about thespinal circuitry has come from animal experiments,but human studies have retained a unique role: theability to shed direct light on how spinal mechanismsare used in the control of voluntary movement. Inthe 19401950s, many spinal pathways were an-alysed in reduced animal preparations with regardto their synaptic input and to their projections toother neurones.

    Modern views about spinal pathways began toemerge when Anders Lundberg and colleaguesshowed in the 1960s and 1970s that, in the cat,each set of spinal interneurones receives extensiveconvergence from different primary afferents anddescending tracts, and that the integrative functionof spinal interneurones allows the motoneuronesto receive a final command that has been updatedat a premotoneuronal level. Methods have nowbeen developed to enable indirect but neverthelessvalid measurements of spinal interneuronal activ-ity in human subjects, and these techniques havedemonstrated reliability, particularly when congru-ent results are obtained with independent meth-ods. Their use has allowed elucidation of howthe brain modulates the activity of specific spinal

    xv

  • xvi Preface

    interneurones to control movement. This, togetherwith the abnormalities of motor control resultingfrom lesions in the central nervous system (CNS) andthe underlying pathophysiology of movement disor-ders, is the subject of this book.

    Over recent years, reappraisal of the role of directcortico-motoneuronal projections in higher pri-mates including humans has led to the view thatthe control of movement resides in the motor cor-tical centres that drive spinal motoneurone pools toproduce the supraspinally crafted movement. Thisview belies the complex interneuronal machinerythat resides in the spinal cord. It is a thesis of thisbook that the final movement is only that part ofthe supraspinally derived programme that the spinalcord circuitry deems appropriate. While the capacityof the spinal cord to generate or sustain even simplemovements, particularly in human subjects, is lim-ited, the influence that it plays in shaping the finalmotor output should not be underestimated. Therecent recording by Eberhard Fetz and colleaguesfrom spinal interneurones during, and before, volun-tary movement in the awake monkey well illustratesthis role of the spinal cord. A goal of rehabilitation ofpatients with upper motor neurone lesions shouldbe to harness the residual motor capacities of thespinal cord and, for this to occur, the informationin this book is critical. The techniques described inthis book will also allow assessment in patients ofwhether any regeneration is appropriate.

    Studying motor control in human subjects. Therehas been an explosion of studies on human move-ment and of the dysfunction that accompanies dif-ferent neurological disorders, and the prime ration-ale for this book is to summarise the literaturerelated to the control of spinal cord circuitry inhuman subjects. Of necessity, only some interneu-ronal circuits can be studied reliably in human sub-jects, and no one book can provide a completeoverview of the role of spinal circuitry in normal andpathological movement: there are no data for themany circuits that cannot yet be studied in humansubjects, let alone the cat. This book is intended toprovide a comprehensive account of (i) how somewell-recognised and defined circuits can be stud-

    ied, (ii) how they are used in normal movement, and(iii) how they malfunction in disease states.

    It is as well to retain some reservations about con-clusions of studies in human subjects: (i) All studieson human subjects are indirect and cannot be con-trolled as rigorously as in the cat. (ii) Some path-ways cannot be explored quantitatively, becausetheir effects are contaminated by effects due to otherafferents (e.g. effects due to group II afferents arealways contaminated by group I effects whatevertesting method is used). (iii) For methodologicalreasons (stability of the stimulating and recordingconditions), isometric voluntary contractions havebeen the main motor tasks during which changes intransmission in spinal pathways have been investi-gated. However, recent technological advances nowallow the investigation of spinal pathways duringnatural movements, including reaching and walking.(iv) With transcranial magnetic stimulation of themotor cortex, it is possible to investigate the corti-cospinal control of spinal interneurones, but thereare little data for other descending controls frombasal ganglia and the brainstem, other than vestibu-lar projections. (v) In patients, spinal circuitry hasusually been explored under resting conditions,but the functionally important deficits may appearonly during attempted movements (reinforcementof spasticity during movement, dystonia).

    Methodological advances. The H reflex has servedmotor control well but, over the last 30 years, othertechniques have been developed to allow more accu-rate probing of spinal pathways in human subjects,providing data that can validate and extend the find-ings from H reflex studies. As a result, knowledge ofthe role of spinal pathways in normal and pathologi-cal motor control has increased greatly, and this pro-vides a further motivation for this book. For example,the use of post-stimulus time histograms has allowedthe investigation of single motoneurones in humansubjects, the technique of spatial facilitation allowsthe exploration of the convergence of different vol-leys on spinal interneurones, and transcortical stim-ulation of the motor cortex allows the corticospinalcontrol of spinal pathways to be investigated. Thisbook details this newer knowledge for the use of

  • Preface xvii

    those who have an interest in the subject but whohave not had time to read the rapidly accumulatingoriginal literature. Inevitably, there will be inconsis-tencies in conclusions from studies on intact humansubjects who can respond to a stimulus. Greatervalidity comes from using a number of independenttechniques to demonstrate the same finding, as isemphasised in the following chapters. Inconsistentor irreproducible findings can lead to controversyabout the nature and the functional role of a specificpathway in normal subjects and in patients, and suchinconsistencies are presented, and the validity of themethod(s) used to explore that pathway is addressed.Possible future directions for the research arediscussed.

    Organisation of individual chapters. The differ-ent spinal pathways for which there are reliable andnon-invasive methods of investigation are consid-ered with, for each pathway:

    (i) A brief background from animal experiments.Human investigations are indirect and it iscrucial to know the essential characteristicsof each pathway described in animal experi-ments with recordings from motoneuronesand/or interneurones. Caution should alwaysbe taken in extrapolating from data obtainedin reduced preparations (anaesthetised, decer-ebrate or spinalised animals) to awake intacthuman subjects, but the validation of a tech-nique for exploring a given pathway may requirecontrols only possible in animal experimentsand is more credible when there is a close anal-ogy with animal experiments.

    (ii) A critical description of the available method(s)that have been used to explore the relevant path-ways selectively. Methodological details allow-ing the reader to use reliable methods aredescribed.

    (iii) The organisation and descending control (inparticular corticospinal) of these pathways inhuman subjects. The basic organisation of eachpathway may well be the same in humans andcats, but the strength of the projections of indi-vidual spinal pathways on different motoneu-rone pools and their descending control have

    been the subject of phylogenetic adaptationsto different motor repertoires. For the humanlower limb, more elaborate reflex assistance isrequired for bipedal stance and gait. That therehas been this phylogenetic adaptation arguesthat spinal pathways have a functional rolein human subjects and are not evolutionaryrelics.

    (iv) The changes in transmission in these pathwaysduring various motor tasks. How spinal reflexpathways are used in motor control cannot bededuced from experiments on reduced ani-mal preparations. It requires experiments per-formed during natural movements, as can bedone in humans. This has been one major con-tribution of human studies to the understandingof motor control physiology. Thus, even thoughmany of the conclusions are speculative, thisbook gives a large place to the probable func-tional implications of the described changes intransmission in spinal pathways during move-ment.

    (v) Changes in transmission in these pathways inpatients with various lesions of the CNS. Thishas provided new insights about the patho-physiology of the movement disorder in thesepatients.

    Overall organisation of the book. The generalmethodologies that are used for investigating path-ways are considered in a first chapter with, for eachmethod, its advantages and its disadvantages. Thereis a risk that starting with a technical chapter woulddissuade the non-specialist reader from delving fur-ther into the book. This initial chapter is useful tounderstand fully the particular techniques used forthe investigation of the different pathways, but itis not essential for comprehension of the followingchapters.

    For those who wish to know how methods andconcepts have evolved over the years and why someinterpretations were erroneous even if, at the time,influential, the methods are described in detail, withtheir limits and caveats, and the results obtained andtheir interpretation(s) are critically evaluated in eachchapter. Because human studies are fraught with

  • xviii Preface

    technical difficulties, much space has been allotedto methods and potential pitfalls.

    For those who want to get to the gist of the mat-ter reasonably quickly each chapter terminates witha resume of its salient points. The resumes can beused on their own without reference to the detailedtext. They give a practical recipe on the choice of theappropriate technique and its proper use in routineclinical studies, together with data on the possiblefunctional role of the particular pathway in motorcontrol and in the pathophysiology of movementdisorders.

    The final two chapters summarise and synthesisethe changes in transmission in spinal pathways dur-ing movement and how these changes contribute tomotor control, and spinal mechanisms underlyingspasticity and motor impairment in patients withParkinsons disease. In these chapters, the physio-logical (Chapter 11) and pathophysiological (Chap-ter 12) roles of different spinal pathways, consideredin the previous chapters, are presented with anotherapproach:(i)howdifferentmotortasksarecontrolledby spinal pathways (Chapter 11); (ii) how these path-ways contribute to motor disorders (Chapter 12).

  • Acknowledgements

    This book is dedicated to Evelyne and Katre. It wouldnot have been possible if our wives had not appre-ciated the importance for us of bringing togetherin a single volume the accumulated knowledge onspinal mechanisms in the control of movement.They have encouraged, supported and tolerated us,understanding even when we were unreasonable,putting life on hold so that we could work.

    We are greatly indebted to Paolo Cavallari,Jean-Michel Gracies, Hans Hultborn, Lena Jami,Stacey Jankelowitz, Elzbieta Jankowska, DominiqueMazevet, Leonor Mazie`res, Jens Nielsen, Uwe Proskeand Marco Schieppati who have given generously oftheir time to read and comment on drafts of variouschapters. Above all, particularly special thanks go toPaolo, Lena and Leonor who read the entire text.

    Genevie`ve Bard and Mary Sweet have labouredlong and hard in getting the text into presentableorder, and we are grateful for their friendship, loy-alty and meticulous attention to detail over our manyyears of association.

    Finally, the studies summarised in the bookrepresent the intellectual activity of collaborators,colleagues, students and staff. We are grateful toeveryone who contributed to these studies, andto our colleagues and their publishers who haveallowed us to reproduce Figures from their papers.Finally, the authors would like to thank INSERM andNH&MRC for support of their work.

    xix

  • Abbreviations

    5-HT 5-hydroxytryptophanACh acetylcholineAff. affectedAHP afterhyperpolarisationAPB abductor pollicis brevisBi bicepsCFS critical firing stimulusCo FRA contralateral FRACPN common peroneal nerveCS or (Cort. sp.) corticospinalCUSUM cumulative sumCut cutaneousDesc. descendingDPN deep peroneal nerveECR extensor carpi radialisED extensor digitorumEDB extensor digitorum brevisEDL extensor digitorum longusEHB extensor hallucis brevisEHL extensor hallucis longusEMG electromyogramEPSP excitatory post-synaptic potentialErect sp erector spinaeExc excitatoryFCR flexor carpi radialisFCU flexor carpi ulnarisFDB flexor digitorum brevisFDI first dorsal interosseusFDS flexor digitorum superficialisFHB flexor hallucis brevisFN femoral nerveFPL flexor pollicis longus

    xxi

  • xxii List of abbreviations

    FRA flexion reflex afferentGlut Max (or Glut) gluteus maximusGM gastrocnemius medialisGS gastrocnemius-soleusGTO Golgi tendon organH hamstringsIN interneuroneInhib. inhibitoryIPSP inhibitory post-synaptic

    potentialISI inter-stimulus intervalL-Ac L-acetylcarnitineLC (or Loc. coer). locus coeruleusMC musculo-cutaneousMEP motor evoked potentialMLR medium-latency responseMN motoneuroneMRI magnetic resonance imagingMT motor thresholdMVC maximal voluntary

    contractionNA noradrenalineNRM nucleus raphe magnusPAD primary afferent depolarisationPer Brev peroneus brevis

    PL peroneus longusPN propriospinal neuronePs psoasPSP post-synaptic potentialPT perception thresholdPTN posterior tibial nerveQ quadricepsRC Renshaw cellRect Abd rectus abdominisRS or (Ret. Sp). reticulo-spinalRubr. sp. rubro-spinalSLR short-latency responseSol soleusSPN superficial peroneal nerveSSEP somatosensory evoked potentialStim. stimulusTA tibialis anteriorTFL tensor fasciae lataeTMS trans cranial magnetic stimulationTN tibial nerveTri triceps brachiiUnaff. unaffectedVI vastus intermediusVL vastus lateralisVS vestibulo-spinal

  • 1General methodology

    The following chapters discuss methods that allowthe selective investigation of different spinal path-ways. Whatever the pathway investigated, its activa-tion produces changes in the excitability of spinalmotoneurones, the final common path in themotor system. A prerequisite for any investigationof changes in the spinal circuitry in human sub-jects is therefore to be able to assess changes inmotoneurone excitability quantitatively, using validreproducible methods. Several non-invasive meth-ods have been developed, and these are consideredin this chapter with their advantages and disadvan-tages. All are, of course, indirect, and valid conclu-sions can only be obtained if congruent results areobtained with different methods relying on differentprinciples. All may be, and many have been, usedin studies on patients, but here the methodologyshould be simple and rapid.

    This initial chapter is technical and non-specialistreaders could bypass it, referring back if they needto clarify how results were obtained or understandthe advantages and limitations of a particular tech-nique. However, the chapter is required reading forthose who want to understand fully the particulartechniques used for the different pathways and howto use those techniques.

    The monosynaptic reflex: H reflexand tendon jerk

    The monosynaptic reflex forms the basis of the firsttechnique available to investigate spinal pathways

    in animals and humans. The principle is based onthe apparent simplicity of the monosynaptic projec-tion of Ia afferents to homonymous motoneurones.Subsequent studies have shown that the so-calledmonosynaptic reflex is not as simple as was initiallythought. We will consider successively: (i) the initialfindings; (ii) the principles underlying the mono-synaptic reflex testing method; (iii) the basicmethodology of the H reflex; (iv) limitations relatedto mechanisms which can change the size of thereflex by altering its afferent volley; (v) pool prob-lems related to the inputoutput relationship withinthe motoneurone pool.

    Initial studies

    Animal studies

    The monosynaptic reflex depends on the projec-tion of muscle spindle Ia afferents to homonymousmotoneurones and was used in the early 1940s asa tool for investigating changes in excitability ofthe motoneurone pool (Renshaw, 1940; Lloyd, 1941).When used as a test reflex, the monosynaptic reflexallows one to assess the effect on the motoneu-rone pool of conditioning volleys in peripheral affer-ents or descending tracts. During the 1940s andearly 1950s this method was used to reveal impor-tant features of the input to spinal motoneurones.Intracellular recordings later allowed more detailedanalysis of the synaptic input to motoneurones inanimals (see Baldissera, Hultborn & Illert, 1981), but

    1

  • 2 General methodology

    MN

    Tendontap

    Electricalstimulation

    Ia afferent

    Presynapticinhibition

    Fig. 1.1. Sketch of the pathway of the monosynaptic reflex. Ia afferents from muscle spindle primary endings (dotted line) have

    monosynaptic projections to motoneurones (MNs) innervating the corresponding muscle (homonymous MNs). The H reflex is

    produced by electrical stimulation of Ia afferents, and bypasses muscle spindles. The tendon jerk is elicited by a tap that stretches

    muscle spindles and therefore also depends on the sensitivity to stretch of primary endings, a property that may be altered by the

    activity of efferents (however, see Chapter 3, pp. 11718). The pathway of presynaptic inhibition of Ia terminals (see Chapter 8) is

    represented.

    interestingly this greater precision did not change themain conclusions that had emerged from the experi-ments employing the monosynaptic reflex. This sug-gests that the monosynaptic reflex method producesreliable results.

    Human studies

    Percutaneous electrical stimulation of the posteriortibial nerve produces a synchronised response in thesoleus muscle (Hoffmann, 1918, 1922). This becameknown as the Hoffmann reflex or H reflex (Magladery& McDougal, 1950). Magladery et al. (1951a) showedthat the first motoneurones discharging in the Hreflex do so at a latency consistent with a mono-synaptic pathway (see Chapter 2). After the pioneerinvestigations of Paillard (1955), the H reflex, whichis the equivalent of the monosynaptic reflex in ani-mal studies, became the main tool in many motorcontrol investigations and diagnostic studies per-formed on human subjects (for reviews, see Schiep-pati, 1987; Burke et al., 1999; Pierrot-Deseilligny &Mazevet, 2000).

    Underlying principles

    The monosynaptic reflex arc

    Pathway

    Ia fibres from muscle spindle primary endings havemonosynaptic excitatory projections to motoneu-rones innervating the muscle from which the affer-ents emanate (homonymous projections, Fig. 1.1).This pathway is responsible for the tendon jerk (seeChapter 2). The H reflex is produced by electricalstimulation of Ia afferents, which have a lower elec-trical threshold than motor axons, particularly forstimuli of relatively long duration (see p. 6).

    The H reflex, tendon jerk and short-latencyspinal stretch reflex

    These are all dependent on monosynaptic excitationfrom homonymous Ia afferents. However, the affer-ent volleys for these reflexes differ in many respects(cf. Chapter 3): (i) the electrically induced afferent

  • The monosynaptic reflex 3

    Test reflex alone:

    Test reflex conditioned by an excitatory input

    (a)

    (b)

    Test reflex conditioned by an inhibitorory input(c)

    MNs

    MNs

    MNs

    Test EPSP

    Conditioning + test EPSPs

    Conditioning IPSP + test EPSP

    Fig. 1.2. Principles of the monosynaptic reflex. (a) Orderly recruitment of motoneurones (MNs) by a given Ia input: the size of the

    monosynaptic Ia EPSP (upper row) decreases as MN size increases (lower row). The dotted horizontal line represents the threshold

    for discharge of the MNs. Only the smallest MNs (black) are fired by the test Ia volley, and the excitability of subliminally excited

    MNs decreases from the smallest to the largest (as indicated by the decreasing tone of grey). (b) Facilitation by an excitatory input.

    There is summation of the conditioning (thin lines) and test (thick lines) EPSPs. As a result, MNs which had just failed to discharge

    in the control reflex are raised to firing threshold and the size of the reflex is increased. (c) Inhibition by an inhibitory input. There is

    summation of the conditioning IPSP (thin line) and of the test EPSP (the test EPSP is also reduced by changes in the membrane

    conductance, see p. 27). As a result, MNs which had just been recruited in the control reflex cannot be discharged, and the size of the

    reflex is reduced. Note that the excitability of the MNs in the subliminal fringe of excitation is also modified by the conditioning

    input. Modified from Pierrot-Deseilligny & Mazevet (2000), with permission.

    volley for the H reflex bypasses muscle spindlesand produces a single synchronous volley in groupIa and Ib afferents; (ii) the tendon tap producesa highly dynamic stretch, which activates mainlymuscle spindle primary endings and elicits a pro-longed discharge in Ia afferents; (iii) the short-latency Ia spinal stretch reflex is overlapped bya medium-latency response due to a group IIvolley from muscle spindle secondary endings (seeChapter 7).

    The orderly recruitment of motoneurones inthe monosynaptic reflex

    Figure 1.2(a) shows that, in the cat, the size of the testIa excitatory post-synaptic potential (EPSP) evokedin individual motoneurones by a given afferent vol-ley is larger in small motoneurones supplying slowmotor units than in large motoneurones supplyingfast units. As a result, motoneurones are recruited inan orderly sequence by the Ia input, from the smallest

  • 4 General methodology

    to the largest, according to Hennemans size prin-ciple (see Henneman & Mendell, 1981). Motoneu-rones contributing to the human H reflex arerecruited in a similar orderly sequence from slow tofast motor units (Buchthal & Schmalbruch, 1970).This orderly recruitment of motoneurones is pre-served when they receive a variety of excitatory andinhibitory inputs (though not all, see pp. 1820), suchthat facilitation will initially affect those motoneu-rones that just failed to discharge in the control reflex(dark grey motoneurones in Fig. 1.2(b)) and inhibi-tion will affect those that had just been recruitedinto the control reflex (largest black motoneurones inFig. 1.2(a)).

    Principles of the monosynapticreflex method

    In the control situation, the test Ia volley elicitedby stimulation of constant intensity causes somemotoneurones to discharge producing the controltest reflex (black motoneurones in Fig. 1.2(a)) andcreates EPSPs in other motoneurones which therebybecome subliminally excited (grey motoneurones inFig. 1.2(a)). If motoneurones are now facilitated bya subthreshold conditioning volley, motoneuronesthat had just failed to fire in the control reflex will dis-charge when the conditioning and test EPSPs sum-mate (Fig. 1.2(b)). The size of the test reflex willincrease. By contrast, if motoneurones receive condi-tioning inhibitory post-synaptic potentials (IPSPs),the test Ia volley will not be able to discharge themotoneurones that had been recruited last intothe control reflex, and the size of the test reflexwill be decreased (Fig. 1.2(c)). The method allowsone to distinguish between: (i) conditioning stimuliwithout effect on the excitability of motoneurones;(ii) those which evoke only subliminal excitationof the motoneurones when applied alone; and (iii)those which inhibit motoneurones. A variant ofthe method is to compare the amplitude of thereflex in two situations (e.g. natural reciprocal inhi-bition of the reflex with respect to rest during

    voluntary contraction of the antagonistic muscle,cf. Chapter 5).

    Basic methodology

    H reflexes cannot be recorded with equal ease in dif-ferent motor nuclei (cf. Chapter 2). In most healthysubjects at rest, H reflexes can usually be recordedonly from soleus (Hoffmann, 1918), quadriceps(Gassel, 1963), hamstrings (Magladery et al., 1951a)and flexor carpi radialis (FCR) (Deschuytere, Rosselle& DeKeyser, 1976). However, when a weak voluntarycontraction is used to potentiate the reflex by raisingmotoneurone excitability close to firing threshold,H reflexes can be recorded from virtually all limbmuscles, if the parent nerve is accessible to elec-trical stimulation (cf. Burke, Adams & Skuse, 1989;Chapter 2).

    General experimental arrangement

    Subjects posture

    The subject should be comfortably seated in an arm-chair with the examined limb loosely fixed in a posi-tion avoiding stretch of the test muscle (see Hugon,1973; Burke et al., 1999). Thus, the lower limb is com-monly explored with the hip semi-flexed (120), theknee slightly flexed (160) and the ankle at 110 plan-tar flexion. The upper limb is explored with the shoul-der in slight abduction (60), the elbow semi-flexed(110), and the forearm pronated and supported bythe arm of the chair. In patients, recordings can beperformed supine, again avoiding stretch on the testmuscle.

    Awareness

    The state of awareness of the subject may modify theamplitude of the H reflex, often in an unpredictableway. The H reflex increases during alertness, at leastwhen the level of attention is high (Bathien & Morin,1972). Task demands can induce variations in the

  • The monosynaptic reflex 5

    (i )

    Stimulus intensity (mA)

    Size

    of r

    espo

    nses

    (mV)

    H reflexM wave

    0

    1

    2

    3

    4

    5 10 15 20 25 30

    (a) (d)

    X Y Z X Y ZX Y ZX Y Z

    (b) (c)

    (e)(f )(g)(h)

    1 mV

    10 ms

    Fig. 1.3. Recruitment curve of the H and M waves in the soleus. (a)(h) Sample EMG responses ((e)(h)) and sketches of the

    corresponding volleys in Ia afferents and motor axons ((a)(d)) when the stimulus intensity is progressively increased. MNs

    discharged by the Ia volley are black, muscle fibres activated by the H reflex are speckled and those activated by the M wave are

    hatched. (a) and (e), stimulation (9 mA) activates Ia afferents only and causes MN X to fire in the H reflex. (b) and (f ), stronger

    stimulation (12 mA) activates more Ia afferents and this causes MNs X and Y to fire in the H reflex, which increases in size. It also

    elicits a motor volley in the axon of MN Z and an M wave appears in the EMG. The antidromic motor volley in MN Z does not

    collide with the reflex response, because this MN does not contribute to the reflex. (c) and (g), even stronger stimulation (15 mA)

    causes MNs X and Y to fire in the H reflex and elicits a motor volley in the axon of MNs Z and Y: as a result, an M wave appears

    in the muscle fibres innervated by MN Y. The antidromic motor volley collides with and eliminates the reflex volley in the axon of

    MN Y, and the H reflex decreases. (d) and (h), yet stronger stimulation (30 mA) produces Mmax, and the H reflex is eliminated by

    collision with the antidromic motor volley. The vertical dashed line in (e)(g) indicates the latency of the H reflex. (i), the amplitudes

    of the H reflex () and of the M wave () are plotted against stimulus intensity. Modified from Pierrot-Deseilligny & Mazevet (2000),with permission.

    H reflex related to the particular characteristics ofthe mental effort required by the task itself (Brunia,1971). In practice, H reflexes should be recorded ina quiet room, and the influence of the mental effortinvolved in a difficult motor task should be taken intoaccount. Conversely, the H reflex decreases duringthe early stages of sleep and is abolished during REMsleep (Hodes & Dement, 1964).

    Recording the H reflex

    Recording

    Reflexes generally appear in the EMG as triphasicwaveforms, particularly with soleus where the elec-trodesarenotoverthemotorpoint(cf.Fig. 1.3(e)(g)).

    (i) Bipolar surface electrodes are commonly placed1.52 cm apart over the corresponding muscle belly

  • 6 General methodology

    for recording H and tendon reflexes. For the quadri-ceps the best place is on the anterior aspect of thethigh, 510 cm above the patella over the vastusintermedius. In the forearm, a selective voluntarycontraction can be used as a first step to focus thereflex response on the desired motoneurone pool,because during the contraction the reflex dischargecan be obtained at lower threshold in the contractingmuscle.

    (ii) Monopolar recordings, with an active elec-trode over the mid-belly of the muscle and a remoteelectrode over its tendon, have been recommendedto minimise the effects of changes in geometry ofthe muscle during voluntary contraction (Gerilovsky,Ysvetinov & Trenkova, 1989). However, these changesare adequately taken into account if the reflex isexpressed as a percentage of the maximal M wave(see p. 8) measured under the same conditions. Inaddition, the more distant the remote electrode, theless likely is the recorded activity to come from onlythe muscle underlying the active electrode.

    Measurement

    (i) Reflex latency is measured to the first deflectionof the H wave from baseline, not to the first positivepeak of the commonly triphasic waveform (see thevertical dashed line in Fig. 1.3(e)(g)).

    (ii) In practice it makes little difference whether theamplitude or the surface area of the reflex is assessedor whether amplitude is measured for the negativephase only or from negative peak to the followingpositive peak. Whichever way the H reflex is mea-sured, the same method should be used for the max-imal M wave, Mmax (see p. 8), and the amplitudeof the H reflex must be expressed as a percentage ofMmax.

    Cross-talk

    Pick up of the EMG potentials from an adjacent mus-cle can occur if there is spread of the test stimulus electrical to another nerve (H reflex), or mechanicalto another muscle (tendon jerk) (see Hutton, Roy &Edgerton, 1988). Even if this does not occur, it can

    still be difficult to be certain that a surface-recordedEMG potential comes exclusively from the under-lying muscle rather than a synergist (e.g. responseselicited in the FCR and finger flexors after mediannerve stimulation). In addition, responses evokedby a conditioning stimulus may also contaminatethe test reflex, e.g. the H reflex in the antagonistFCR when studying reciprocal inhibition from wristflexors to wrist extensors. Muscle palpation mayhelp recognise inadvertent activation of inappropri-ate muscles. Another simple way of ensuring thatthe reflex response originates from the muscle overwhich it is recorded is to check that it increases dur-ing a selective voluntary contraction of that muscle.

    Stimulation to elicit the H reflex

    H reflexes are produced by percutaneous electricalstimulation of Ia afferents in the parent nerve. Thetechnique is now well codified (see Hugon, 1973;Burke et al., 1999).

    Duration of the stimulus

    The diameter of Ia afferents is slightly larger thanthat of motor axons and their rheobasic threshold islower, such that it is generally possible, particularly insoleus, to evoke an H reflex with stimuli below motorthreshold (1 MT). The strengthduration curvesfor motor axons and Ia afferents differ and, as aresult, the optimal stimulus duration for eliciting theH reflex is long (1 ms; see Paillard, 1955; Panizza,Nilsson & Hallett, 1989). The stimulus intensity forthe threshold H reflex then approaches rheobasefor low-threshold Ia afferents, approximately 50% ofrheobase for motor axons (Lin et al., 2002).

    Unipolar and bipolar stimulation

    The best method for ensuring that Ia afferents areexcited at lower threshold than motor axons involvesplacing the cathode over the nerve and the anodeon the opposite side of the limb, so that currentpasses transversely through the nerve. The soleusand quadriceps H reflexes are commonly evoked bymonopolar stimulation of the posterior tibial nerve

  • The monosynaptic reflex 7

    (cathode in the popliteal fossa, anode on the anter-ior aspect of the knee) and the femoral nerve (cath-ode in the femoral triangle, anode on the poster-ior aspect of the thigh), respectively. However, inareas where there are many nerves, bipolar stimula-tion may avoid stimulus encroachment upon othernerves: the median nerve (FCR) is so stimulated atthe elbow. The same applies to the stimulation ofthe deep peroneal branch of the common peronealnerve (tibialis anterior) at the fibular neck and ofthe sciatic nerve (hamstrings) at the posterior aspectof the thigh. It is generally stated that the cathodeshould then be placed over the nerve with anode dis-tal (or lateral) to avoid the possibility of anodal block.However, there is little evidence that this is really aproblem in practice.

    Frequency of stimulation

    Because of post-activation depression (seeChapter 2), there is reflex attenuation as stim-ulus rate is increased above 0.1 Hz. This attenuationrequires at least 10 s to subside completely, butits effects are sufficiently small after 34 s to allowtesting at 0.20.3 Hz. Use of these frequencies consti-tutes a compromise between reflex depression andthe necessity to collect a large number of responsesbecause of reflex variability. During a backgroundcontraction of the tested muscle, the attenuationwith increasing stimulus repetition rate is reducedor even abolished (cf. Chapter 2).

    Magnetic stimulation

    The H reflex may also be evoked by magnetic stimu-lation of the parent nerve (or nerve root) and appearswith the same latency as with electrical stimulation(Zhu et al., 1992). One advantage of magnetic stim-ulation is the ease with which an H reflex can beelicited from deep nerves, such as the sciatic nerve inthe thigh or the sacral nerve roots, which are difficultto access with percutaneous electrical stimulationunless needle electrodes are inserted (Abbruzzeseet al., 1985). However, with magnetic stimulation, thethreshold for the H reflex is usually higher than that

    for the M wave. This difference is probably due tothe extreme brevity (0.05 ms) of the effective stim-ulus produced by magnetic stimulation, a stimulusduration that favours motor axons with respect to Iaafferents (Panizza et al., 1992).

    H and M recruitment curve

    The recruitment curve

    As the intensity of the electrical stimulus to the pos-terior tibial nerve is increased, there is initially a pro-gressive increase in amplitude of the soleus reflexdue to the stronger Ia afferent volley (Fig. 1.3(a), (b),(e), (f )). When motor threshold is reached, the short-latency direct motor response (M wave) appears inthe EMG due to stimulation of motor axons ((b) and(f )). Further increases in the intensity of the test stim-ulus cause the M wave to increase and the H reflexto decrease ((c) and (g)). Finally, when the directmotor response is maximal, the reflex response iscompletely suppressed ((d) and (h)). This is becausethe antidromic motor volley set up in motor axonscollides with and eliminates the H reflex response(Hoffmann, 1922, Fig. 1.3(d)). Note that, when it firstappears in the EMG, the M response involves axonsof the largest motoneurones (e.g. MN Z in Fig. 1.3(b)and (f )), which have a high threshold for recruit-ment into the H reflex. Because they are not acti-vated in the reflex, stimulation of these motor axonsdoes not interfere with the reflex response. The vari-ations of the H and M responses with the test stimu-lus intensity can be plotted as the recruitment curveof Fig. 1.3(i). Because of the orderly recruitment ofmotoneurones (see pp. 34), the sensitivity of thereflex to facilitation and inhibition depends on thelast motoneurones recruited by the test volley (aslong as the reflex is not on the descending limb ofthe recruitment curve, see below).

    Maximal M wave (Mmax)

    Mmax is evoked by the stimulation of all motor axonsand provides an estimate of the response of theentire motoneurone pool. This estimate is actually an

  • 8 General methodology

    overestimate, because the necessarily strong stimu-lus will produce EMG activity in synergists in addi-tion to the test muscle. Accordingly, the Mmax follow-ing median nerve stimulation at the elbow comesfrom the FCR, finger flexors and pronator teres.Ignoring this issue, Mmax should always be measuredin the same experiment with the same recordingelectrode placement because: (i) comparing it withthe reflex response provides an estimate of the pro-portion of the motoneurone pool discharging in thereflex; (ii) expressing the reflex as a percentage ofMmax enables one to control for changes in mus-cle geometry due to changes in muscle length orcontraction; (iii) expressing the test reflex as a per-centage of Mmax allows the investigator to be surethat the test reflex remains within the linear rangeof the input/output relationship for the motoneu-rone pool (i.e. between 10 and 60% of Mmax for thesoleus H reflex, see pp. 1618).

    The test reflex should not be on the descendinglimb of the recruitment curve

    This is because the component of the H reflex seenin the EMG is generated by low-threshold motoneu-rones, which are insensitive to excitation or inhibi-tion. Small motoneurones innervating slow motorunits are first recruited in the H reflex (see pp. 34),whereas electrical stimulation will first activatemotor axons of large diameter from high-thresholdmotoneurones. As a result, on the descending limbof the recruitment curve, the reflex response seen inthe EMG will be produced by small motoneurones,in which the collision in motor axons has not takenplace. The reflex response in the fastest motor unitsof the H reflex, i.e. those that were last recruitedinto the reflex and are thus sensitive to excitationand inhibition, will be eliminated by collision withthe antidromic motor volley (Fig. 1.3(c) and (g)) (seePierrot-Deseilligny & Mazevet, 2000).

    Monitoring the stability of the stimulationconditions

    If the H reflex is performed during a manoeuvrethat can alter the stimulating conditions (e.g. musclecontraction, stance or gait), it is necessary to ensure

    that changes in the test H reflex are not due to achange in the position of the stimulating electrode.The reproducibility of a M wave can then be usedto monitor the stability of the stimulation. To thatend, stimulation should be adjusted to produce asmall M wave in addition to the H reflex. If there isneed for a test response without a M wave, the sta-bility of stimulation can be monitored by alternatingthe test stimulus with a stimulus evoking a M wavethrough the same electrode. This procedure raisesquestions about the acceptable range of variability ofthe M wave in such studies. Some authors have used arange of 10% of Mmax, but this is a large range whencompared to the changes expected in the H reflex,and changes not exceeding 10% of the recorded Mwave, not10% of Mmax, are recommended. It shouldbe realised that, during experiments involving a vol-untary or postural contraction of the tested muscle,there will be changes in axonal excitability unrelatedto stability of the stimulating conditions (Vagg et al.,1998), and there will inevitably be some variability inthe M wave from trial to trial.

    Recruitment curves in other muscles

    The recruitment curves for the quadriceps and FCR Hreflexes are similar. However, the threshold of the Mand H responses of FCR and quadriceps are generallycloser than in soleus.

    Tendon jerk

    In proximal muscles (e.g. biceps and triceps brachii),the H reflex is difficult to record at rest without theM wave, and it then appears merged into the endof the M wave. For routine testing, it may be moreconvenient to test the excitability of these motoneu-rone pools using tendon reflexes. An electromag-netic hammer (such as a Bruel and Kjaer shaker,Copenhagen, Denmark) will produce reproducibletransient tendon percussion. In healthy subjects atrest, a tendon jerk reflex can be elicited in the soleus,quadriceps, biceps femoris, semitendinosus, bicepsand triceps brachii, FCR, extensor carpi radialis(ECR) and the masseter. Use of the tendon jerk intro-duces two complications.

  • The monosynaptic reflex 9

    Delay due to the tendon tap

    The tendon tap introduces a delay, and in the soleus,the afferent volley for the tendon jerk will reachmotoneurones 5 ms later than the electricallyinduced volley producing the H reflex (cf. Chapter 2).An estimate of the central delay of the effect of a con-ditioning volley on a test tendon jerk may be obtainedby comparing the first interstimulus interval (ISI) atwhich this effect occurs to the first ISI at which aheteronymous monosynaptic Ia volley delivered tothe same nerve facilitates the tested motoneurones(see Mazevet & Pierrot-Deseilligny, 1994). An exam-ple would be the group Ia projection from median-innervated forearm muscles to biceps and tricepsmotoneurones.

    Fusimotor drive

    The amplitude of the reflex response produced bytendon percussion may depend on the level of drivedirected to muscle spindle primary endings of thetested muscle (see Fig. 1.1). Accordingly, it has beenargued that differences in the behaviour of H and ten-don jerk reflexes reflect the involvement of drive inthe tendon jerk (e.g. see Paillard, 1955). This belief hasbeen called into question because H and tendon jerkreflexes differ in a number of other respects, as dis-cussed in detail in Chapter 3. Of greater importancecould well be the effects on the spindle response topercussion of the thixotropic properties of intrafusalfibres (see Chapter 3).

    Random alternation of control andconditioned reflexes

    In most investigations, the monosynaptic reflex isused as a test reflex to assess the effect of condition-ing volleys on the motoneurone pool. The size of thereflex is compared in the absence (control reflex) andin the presence (conditioned reflex) of the condition-ing volley. Control and conditioned reflexes shouldbe randomly alternated, because: (i) this avoids thepossibility of the subject voluntarily or involuntarilypredicting the reflex sequence; and (ii) regular alter-nation produces erroneously large results (Fournier,

    Katz & Pierrot-Deseilligny, 1984), possibly due topost-activation depression (see Pierrot-Deseilligny& Mazevet, 2000).

    Estimate of the central delay of a conditioningeffect. Time resolution of the method

    It is essential to estimate the central delay of an effectin order to characterise the neural pathway activatedby a conditioning stimulus as mono-, di-, or poly-synaptic. This can be done by comparing the earliestconditioning-test interval at which the test reflex ismodified with the interval estimated for the simulta-neous arrival of the conditioning and test volleys atspinal level. The greater the difference between thesetwo values the longer the intraspinal circuit, andthis could be because more synapses are involved.It should be noted that the H reflex method under-estimates the true central delay. For example, despitethe extra 0.8 ms due to the interneurone interposedin the pathway of disynaptic reciprocal Ia inhibition,the earliest conditioning-test interval with inhibitioncorresponds to the simultaneous arrival of the twovolleys at spinal level (Chapter 5). This is due to tworeasons (Fig. 1.4).

    PSPs in individual motoneurones

    The rise time of the EPSP is sufficiently long that thedischarge of the last recruited motoneurones evokedby the monosynaptic input will not occur before thearrival of a disynaptic IPSP. This is so even though thesynaptic delay at the interneurone delays the onsetof the IPSP by 0.51.0 ms relative to the beginningof the monosynaptic EPSP (see Matthews, 1972, andFig. 1.4(a)). In addition, an EPSP elicited by a condi-tioning volley entering the spinal cord after the testvolley may summate with the decay phase of the testIa EPSP and cause the motoneurone to discharge ata too early ISI.

    Motoneurones do not discharge at the same timein the test reflex

    Even in the cat there is 0.5 ms between the firing of thefirst and last recruited motoneurones contributing to

  • 10 General methodology

    0.8 ms

    EPSPIPSP

    Spike

    Stimulation

    Stimulation

    (a)

    (b) Individual spikescontributing to the H reflex

    IPSP

    PSPs in an individual MN

    Fig. 1.4. A disynaptic IPSP can inhibit the monosynaptic reflex. (a) Post-synaptic (PSP) potentials in an individual motoneurone

    (MN): when volleys eliciting a monosynaptic Ia EPSP and a disynaptic IPSP enter the spinal cord simultaneously, the rise time of the

    EPSP in individual MNs allows the spike in the last recruited MNs to be inhibited by the IPSP, even though the latter does not begin

    until 0.51.0 ms after the beginning of the EPSP. (b) MNs contributing to the H reflex do not discharge simultaneously in the test

    reflex. Thus, a disynaptic IPSP elicited by a conditioning volley entering the spinal cord at the same time as the test monosynaptic

    Ia volley may inhibit the last spikes (thin interrupted lines) contributing to the monosynaptic reflex discharge, while the first

    spikes (thick continuous lines) are not modified. Adapted from Matthews (1972) (a), and Araki, Eccles & Ito (1960) (b), with

    permission.

    the monosynaptic reflex (Araki, Eccles & Ito, 1960).In human subjects, where the afferent pathway islonger and the conduction velocity of Ia afferentsslower, this interval has been estimated at 1.5 msfor the quadriceps H reflex (Fournier et al., 1986)and 2 ms for the soleus H reflex (Burke, Gandevia& McKeon, 1984). There are differences in the rise-times of mechanically and electrically evoked EPSPs(10 ms for tendon percussion; 2 ms for the elec-trically evoked volley), but this is not obvious in thereflex EMG potentials because the axons of the lastrecruited motoneurones have a more rapid conduc-tion velocity than those first recruited. Figure 1.4(b)shows that, because of the desynchronisation at

    spinal level, the last individual spikes contributing tothe monosynaptic test reflex discharge can be inhib-ited by a disynaptic IPSP elicited by a conditioningvolley entering the spinal cord at the same time asthe monosynaptic test volley.

    The recovery cycle of the H reflex

    The recovery cycle of the H reflex investigates thetime course of the changes in the H reflex after aconditioning reflex for conditioning-test intervals upto 12 s. Such studies were in vogue in the 19501960s (Magladery et al., 1951b, 1952; Paillard, 1955).However, the recovery cycle is no longer employed,

  • The monosynaptic reflex 11

    because it results from too many phenomena to beof practical use. Factors that could alter the test reflexinclude changes in excitability of Ia afferents (seebelow), post-activation depression (cf. pp. 1314),presynaptic inhibition of Ia terminals activated bythe conditioning volley (cf. Chapter 8), afterhyperpo-larisation and recurrent inhibition of motoneurones(cf. Chapter 4), muscle spindle receptor unloadingby the conditioning twitch (cf. Chapter 3), Golgitendon organ activation by the conditioning twitch(cf. Chapter 6), and effects mediated by long loops(Taborkova & Sax, 1969).

    When the conditioning and test volleys involve thesame population of afferents, the conditioning dis-charge will change the excitability of the stimulatedafferents for 100 ms, and this is an additional com-plicating factor. Figure 1.5 shows the recovery cycleof the H reflex after a conditioning volley that wassubthreshold for the H reflex even during contrac-tion, comparing the results obtained with thresholdtracking ((a), see below) with those of conventionalamplitude tracking (b). There is an initial period ofdecreased excitability, corresponding to refractori-ness, followed by a period peaking at 78 ms corre-sponding to supernormality and a final phase cor-responding to late subnormality. These changes inexcitability are those of the stimulated peripheralnerve axons (Chan et al., 2002), and this finding indi-cates that two identical stimuli delivered to a nervewill not excite the same population of afferent axonswhen the interval between them is

  • 12 General methodology

    10

    5

    0

    - 5

    -10

    -15

    180

    120

    60

    0

    - 601 10 100 1000

    Conditioning-test interval (ms)

    Nor

    ma

    lised

    am

    plitu

    de (%

    of co

    ntrol)

    Nor

    ma

    lised

    thre

    shol

    d (%

    of co

    ntrol)

    Rest

    Contraction

    (a)

    (b)

    Threshold tracking

    Amplitude tracking

    Fig. 1.5. The recovery cycle of the H reflex following a single subthreshold conditioning stimulus. The soleus H reflex was

    conditioned by a weak stimulus to the posterior tibial nerve (65% of the unconditioned test stimulus, subthreshold for the H reflex

    during contraction). Data representing the deviation from the unconditioned value (horizontal dashed line), using threshold

    tracking (a) and amplitude tracking (b) at rest () and during tonic soleus voluntary contractions () are plotted against theconditioning-test interval. In (a) the intensity of the test stimulus was altered to keep the test H reflex constant: an increase in

    excitability would therefore require less current. In (b), the test stimulus was constant: an increase in excitability would therefore

    increase the amplitude of the test H reflex. Note the logarithmic scale for the x-axis. Mean data SEM for six subjects. Adapted fromChan et al. (2002), with permission.

    Alterations in the excitability of Ia afferents

    Repetitive activation of cutaneous afferents (Kiernanet al., 1997) and natural activity of motor axons (Vagget al., 1998) produce axonal hyperpolarization andthereby a significant reduction in the excitability of

    the active axons. The extent of hyperpolarizationdepends on the impulse load, but can be prominent.For example, with motor axons, contractions last-ing only 15 s increase threshold by 1020%, i.e. afterthe contraction, the stimulus had to be increasedby 1020% to activate the same number of axons

  • The monosynaptic reflex 13

    0 4 8 12

    (a)

    Soleus contraction

    TA contraction

    Passive stretch

    Tendon tap

    H reflex

    (b)

    Conditioning-test interval (s)

    Con

    ditio

    ned

    refle

    x (%

    of co

    ntrol)

    0

    25

    50

    75

    100

    MN

    Ia afferent

    Homosynaptic depressionat the synapse Ia-MN after

    repetitive activation

    Fig. 1.6. Post-activation depression produced by different ways of activating the Ia afferent-MN synapse repetitively. (A) Sketch of

    the pathway (the grey area indicates the Ia afferent-motoneurone [MN] synapse). (b) The recovery of the soleus H reflex (expressed

    as a percentage of its control value) after various conditioning stimuli, plotted against the conditioning-test interval: preceding H

    reflex (), subliminal tendon tap (), passive dorsiflexion of the ankle (), voluntary contraction of the tibialis anterior () or of thesoleus (). Modified from Crone & Nielsen (1989) and Hultborn et al. (1996), with permission.

    (Vagg et al., 1998). This issue is probably impor-tant for group Ia afferents and the H reflex, becausethe excitability of the afferents will decrease dur-ing a voluntary contraction if there is a fusimotor-driven increase in discharge from the active muscle(cf. Chapter 3). As a result, the reflex response to afixed stimulus could change, independently of theother contraction-related changes (presynaptic inhi-bition of Ia afferents, post-activation depression,motoneurone excitability). Additionally, the contrac-tion will activate Ib afferents and thereby reducetheir excitability to electrical stimulation. This wouldreduce the number of Ib afferents in the afferentvolley and the extent to which they limit the size ofthe H reflex (see pp. 1416).

    Presynaptic inhibition of Ia terminals

    Ia terminals mediating the afferent volley of themonosynaptic reflex are subjected to presynapticinhibition accompanied by primary afferent depo-larisation (PAD). Changes in presynaptic inhibitionof Ia terminals can cause major changes in the ampli-tude of the H reflex, and the possibility that a changein presynaptic inhibition accounts for a change inthe amplitude of the H reflex must therefore always

    be considered. Several methods have been devel-oped to assess presynaptic inhibition of Ia termi-nals in human subjects, as described in detail inChapter 8.

    Post-activation depression

    A different presynaptic mechanism limitingmonosynaptic reflexes is post-activation depressionat the Ia fibre-motoneurone synapse, probably dueto reduced transmitter release from active Ia affer-ents, a phenomenon which is described in detailin Chapter 2 (pp. 96100). Post-activation depres-sion occurs when (and only when) the conditioningstimulus or manoeuvre activates the very afferentsresponsible for the test response. H reflex depressionhas been reported to occur following a preceding Hreflex (Magladery & McDougal, 1950), a subliminaltendon tap (Katz et al., 1977), passive dorsiflexionof the ankle (Hultborn et al., 1996), and voluntarycontraction of soleus or stretch of soleus producedby contraction of tibialis anterior (Crone & Nielsen,1989; see also Wood, Gregory & Proske, 1996). Theeffects of this phenomenon can be profound, asillustrated in Fig. 1.6, showing the time course

  • 14 General methodology

    of the recovery of the soleus H reflex after thesemanoeuvres. In all cases, there was dramatic reflexdepression at short intervals (12 s), with gradualrecovery over 10 s. The depressive effects of thestimulus rate on reflex size are generally takeninto consideration in reflex studies, but the samecannot be said for the post-activation depressionoccurring under other circumstances. It is likelythat misinterpretations have arisen because thisphenomenon was neglected in studies comparingchanges in the test reflex during or after a voluntarycontraction. In addition, when the effects of aconditioning volley are compared at rest and duringcontraction, post-activation depression may alsoalter the transmission through the conditioningpathway (e.g. see Chapter 5, p. 221), though notall afferent inputs are similarly affected (see Lamyet al., 2005; Chapter 7, p. 310).

    Contribution of oligosynaptic pathwaysto the H reflex

    Limitation of the size of the H reflex

    In soleus, when the intensity of the test stimulus isincreased, the amplitude of the H reflex commonlyreaches its peak before the antidromic volley set up inmotor axons collides with and annihilates the reflexresponse (see p. 7). Thus, there is a limitation to thesize of the H reflex independent of the collision withthe antidromic volley in motor axons. Taborkova &Sax (1968) demonstrated that, in normal subjects, thepercentage of soleus motoneurones activated in theH reflex by maximal stimulation of Ia afferents rangesfrom 24 to 100, usually 50%. In the homogeneoussoleus, this implies the existence of factors limitingthe size of the reflex.

    Curtailment of the compound EPSP by anoligosynaptic IPSP

    The first motoneurones discharging in the H reflexdo so at a latency consistent with a monosynapticpathway (Magladery et al., 1951a). However, basedon estimates from post-stimulus time histograms(PSTHs) of the discharge of single motor units, it

    has been argued that the duration of the compoundgroup I EPSP underlying the H reflex is so short(some 12 ms) that the monosynaptic Ia compo-nent of the EPSP must be curtailed by oligosynap-tic inhibition, and that this would help limit the sizeof the H reflex (Burke, Gandevia & McKeon, 1984).Transmission in two disynaptic inhibitory path-ways could truncate the monosynaptic Ia excitation:(i) Ib inhibitory interneurones activated by the groupI test volley produce autogenetic inhibition with anonset 0.7 ms after the onset of the facilitation dueto the group Ia monosynaptic EPSP in motoneu-rones (Pierrot-Deseilligny et al., 1981; Chapter 6,pp. 2535); (ii) Renshaw cells are activated by thereflex discharge of low-threshold motoneurones(Chapter 4, p. 159) and could produce recurrent inhi-bition that would prevent the discharge of higher-threshold motoneurones.

    Disynaptic limitation of the group Ia excitation

    Recent experimental evidence for a disynaptic lim-itation of the group Ia excitation that is the basisof the H reflex has been provided for the quadri-ceps (Marchand-Pauvert et al., 2002). The evidenceis as follows. At rest and during weak contractions ofquadriceps stimulation of the deep peroneal nerveproduces a late facilitation of the quadriceps Hreflex with a central delay of 612 ms (Fig. 1.12(c),), but this is suppressed during a contractionof 1020% maximum voluntary contraction (MVC)(Fig. 1.12(c), , and thick line in Fig. 1.7(b)). However,the corresponding facilitation of the on-going EMG isnot suppressed (Fig. 1.12(b), ). Such a discrepancyraises the possibility of an inhibitory mechanism ga-ting the afferent volley of the test reflex, the nature ofwhich was clarified in experiments involving PSTHsof single motor units in quadriceps (Fig. 1.7(c)(f )).Panel (c) shows the peak of homonymous group Iexcitation evoked by femoral stimulation, panel (d)the weak facilitation at around 27 ms elicited by sep-arate stimulation of the deep peroneal nerve, and(e) the significant reduction of the femoral excitationon combined stimulation. Suppression on combinedstimulation when the stimuli by themselves produce

  • The monosynaptic reflex 15

    (e) - ((c)+(d))

    (a)

    IaIb

    Ib IN

    Femoralnerve

    Q MN

    DPN

    0

    100

    200

    300

    400

    16 20 24 28 32

    DPN + H

    Control

    H re

    flex

    (%

    of ba

    ckgro

    und E

    MG)

    Latency (ms)

    (b)

    0

    3

    0

    3

    0

    3

    -2

    0

    2

    24 25 26 27 28

    Latency (ms)

    FN 0.8 x MT

    (c)

    (d)DPN

    2 x MT

    DPN + FN16 ms ISI

    Difference

    Num

    ber o

    f cou

    nts

    (% of

    numb

    er of

    trigge

    rs)(e)

    (f )

    Fig. 1.7. Evidence for suppression of the H reflex by disynaptic autogenetic inhibition from afferents in the test volley. (a) Sketch of

    the presumed pathway: when facilitated by the deep peroneal nerve (DPN) volley, Ib inhibitory interneurones (IN) co-activated

    by Ia and Ib afferents in the femoral nerve (FN) test volley truncate the monosynaptic EPSP in the last recruited quadriceps (Q)

    motoneurones (MN). (b) The rectified and averaged Q H reflex (20 sweeps, 5 kHz sampling rate) during a contraction (10% of MVC),

    showing control responses (thin line) and conditioned responses (thick line, stimulation of the DPN at 2 MT, 13 ms ISI). (c)(e)PSTHs of the discharge of a single unit in vastus lateralis (after subtraction of the background firing, 0.2 ms bin width, quadriceps

    contraction 20% MVC). (c) Stimulation of FN by itself (0.8 MT); (d) the DPN by itself (2 MT), and (e) both nerves, the DPNpreceding the FN stimulus by 16 ms. (f ) The suppression of the FN group I excitation, calculated as (e) ((c) + (d)). The number ofcounts in each bin is plotted against the latency after FN stimulation (even in (d) where only DPN stimulation was given). Note the

    lack of suppression in the initial bins of the femoral group I excitation (the dashed and dotted vertical lines highlight the onset of

    the femoral peak at 24.6 ms and the suppression at 25.4 ms, respectively). Adapted from Marchand-Pauvert et al. (2002), with

    permission.

    facilitation reflects convergence (see p. 47) of the twovolleys onto common inhibitory interneurones (asin the wiring diagram in Fig. 1.7(a)). The suppres-sion spared the first 0.8 ms of the femoral group Iexcitation. This is consistent with disynaptic inhi-bition elicited by the test group I volley. Because ofthe synaptic delay at the interneurone, the inhibitoryinput would reach the motoneurone after the directmonosynaptic Ia input, and there should be nochange in the bins of the histogram appropriate for

    this interneuronal delay. Thus, post-synaptic inhi-bition due to afferents in the test volley should notaffect the onset of the femoral excitation, and initialsparing should be demonstrable, as it was.

    Can the results obtained for the quadricepsbe generalised?

    So far, evidence for a limitation of the H reflex by disy-naptic inhibition elicited by the test group I volley

  • 16 General methodology

    has only been demonstrated for the quadriceps.However, the limitation should be more pronouncedfor the soleus than for the quadriceps, because thedegree of desynchronisation of the reflex dischargeis more marked in the former (see p. 10). This pre-sumably reflects the longer afferent pathway of thesoleus H reflex, which would allow greater disper-sion of the afferent volley and thereby a greater influ-ence on the reflex discharge from Ib afferents acti-vated by the test stimulus. It is therefore probablethat soleus H reflexes are also truncated by disynapticinhibitory activity. This limitation could contributeto the absence of H reflex at rest in muscles suchas tibialis anterior and extensor carpi radialis (seeChapter 2, p. 81).

    Recurrent inhibition

    There is so far no experimental evidence for recurrentinhibition elicited by the discharge of low-thresholdmotoneurones preventing the discharge of higher-threshold motoneurones, and it is probable that thepeak of recurrent inhibition occurs too late to curtailsignificantly the test H reflex (see Chapter 4).

    Consequences for the use of the H reflex

    The sensitivity of the H reflex to di- or oligosynap-tic inhibition by afferents in the test volley limits thevalue of H reflex studies. Motoneurones recruited lastinto the reflex will be most dependent on pathwayswith interposed interneurones, and the changes inthe reflex, e.g. during movement, are largely deter-mined by the recruitment of these motoneurones. Itis possible to test for an oligosynaptic limitation oft