7
This article was downloaded by: [Dalhousie University] On: 07 October 2014, At: 01:41 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjrl20 Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television Kirstin J. Hough a & Philip G. Erwin a a Manchester Metropolitan University , Manchester, England Published online: 02 Apr 2010. To cite this article: Kirstin J. Hough & Philip G. Erwin (1997) Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television, The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 131:4, 411-415 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223989709603528 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content.

Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

This article was downloaded by: [Dalhousie University]On: 07 October 2014, At: 01:41Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

The Journal of Psychology:Interdisciplinary and AppliedPublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/vjrl20

Children's Attitudes TowardViolence on TelevisionKirstin J. Hough a & Philip G. Erwin aa Manchester Metropolitan University , Manchester,EnglandPublished online: 02 Apr 2010.

To cite this article: Kirstin J. Hough & Philip G. Erwin (1997) Children's AttitudesToward Violence on Television, The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary andApplied, 131:4, 411-415

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00223989709603528

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all theinformation (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform.However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make norepresentations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness,or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and viewsexpressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, andare not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of theContent should not be relied upon and should be independently verified withprimary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for anylosses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages,and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly orindirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of theContent.

Page 2: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found athttp://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Dal

hous

ie U

nive

rsity

] at

01:

41 0

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 3: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

The Journal of Psychology, 1997, 131(4), 41 1 4 1 5

Children’s Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

KIRSTIN J. HOUGH PHILIP G. ERWIN

Manchester Metropolitan University Manchester; England

ABSTRACT. Children’s attitudes toward television violence were studied. A 47-item questionnaire collecting attitudinal and personal information was administered to 3 16 children aged 1 1 to 16 years. Cluster analysis was used to split the participants into two groups based on their attitudes toward television violence. A stepwise discriminant func- tion analysis was performed to determine which personal characteristics would predict group membership. The only significant predictor of attitudes toward violence on televi- sion was the amount of television watched on school days (p c .05), but we also found that the impact of other predictor variables may have been mediated by this factor.

VIOLENCE IS A COMMON CHARACTERISTIC of many television pro- grams. On British television, for example, an estimated average of one violent scene occurs every 16 min (Barlow & Hill, 1985). At schools, many educators have reported a marked increase in children’s aggression both on the playground and in the classroom during the past decade (Barlow & Hill, 1985). It is not uncommon for these two observations to be juxtaposed and used as a basis for speculations concerning causal relationships.

Cashmore (1 994) wrote that the focus of at least 1,000 research publications has been the possibility of a link between television violence and actual violence; in over three quarters of these publications, the authors have claimed that such a link does indeed exist and have provided various interpretations of its mecha- nisms. Nonetheless, others have managed to conclude that there is no connection at all. On balance, it seems likely that any relationship that may exist between watching television violence and perpetrating actual violence is likely to be a complex one, and a number of contributing factors must be considered.

Address correspondence to Philip G. Erwin, Department of Psychology and Speech Pathology, Manchester Metropolitan University, Elizabeth Gaskell Campus, Hathersage Road, Mancheste,: MI3 OJA, England.

41 I

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Dal

hous

ie U

nive

rsity

] at

01:

41 0

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 4: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

4 I2 The Journal of Psychology

Repeated exposure to television violence has been implicated as a major factor in the gradual desensitization of individuals to such scenes (Cline, Croft, & Courrier, 1973). It has been argued that this desensitization, in turn, may weaken some viewers’ psychological restraints on violent behavior (such as guilt and fear of retaliation) and their fear of social disapproval (Doob &Wood, 1972). Turner, Hesse, and Peterson-Lewis (1986) concluded that the balance of evidence supports the notion that watching television results in a long-term increase in aggression in boys but not in girls, even though (a) boys and girls watch similar amounts of television (Van Evra, 1990) and (b) girls recall the content of the programs as well as, or better than, boys (Field & Anderson, 1985). A possible explanation for any gender differences may lie in the types of programs preferred by boys and girls. Boys typically prefer violent pro- grams; girls prefer nonviolent ones. Girls are also reported to like violence less, approve of it less, and see it as less realistic; they are more frightened and dis- tressed by television violence, respond to it more emotionally, and watch it in a more involved and less detached way than boys do (Van Evra, 1990).

Regardless of the types of television programs preferred, children who watch a lot of television tend to see more violent programs than children who watch relatively less television (Wiegman, Kuttschreuter, & Baarda, 1992). Chil- dren who spend more time watching television, especially violent programs, are also more likely to show later aggression, restlessness, and a belief in a “scary world” (Singer, Singer, & Rapacynski, 1984).

A common and important point raised when the potentially harmful effects of violent television programs are discussed is that any significant negative effects tend to be specific and selective to those already predisposed to violence. A number of studies have shown that those young viewers who are more aggres- sive state that they watch or prefer to watch more violent programs (Wober, 1988). Cross-cultural research indicates that this finding appears to hold true in almost every country studied (Huesman & Eron, 1986).

Given the potential impact of television programs on children, the control of their viewing is of major concern. Such control is often regarded as the parents’ responsibility, although evidence suggests that relying on parents is often a poor safeguard. Parental control over children’s viewing habits appears to have been declining for several decades (Barlow & Hill, 1985). Rubinstein (1983) found that although there was much parental concern about the levels of sex and vio- lence on television, parents exerted a relatively low level of control over what their children watched.

In addition, when parents have placed restrictions on what children can watch, the children have found many ways of getting around these limitations. Typical strategies have included watching on another television in a bedroom, sneaking downstairs and watching through a door, or watching at a friend’s house (Barlow & Hill, 1985).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Dal

hous

ie U

nive

rsity

] at

01:

41 0

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 5: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

Hough &Erwin 413

Overall, existing research has shown a complex interrelationship of person- al characteristics, patterns of media usage, and attitudes toward screen violence. Our aim in this study was to clarify this relationship. Using multivariate tech- niques, we assessed the attitudes of secondary school children toward violence on television and determined how those attitudes related to the children’s per- sonal characteristics and media usage.

Method

Participants

The participants were 3 16 children (156 girls and 160 boys) attending four local, single-sex schools: a boys’ grammar school, a boys’ secondary modern school, a girls’ grammar school, and a girls’ secondary modem school. Entrance tests for the academically selective grammar schools are administered at age 11, and children who do not gain entrance to these institutions attend secondary modem schools. Three classes of pupils from each school participated, from years 7 (1 1- to 12-year-olds), 9 (13- to lCyear-olds), and 11 (15- to 16-year- olds). All members of each class participated. The majority (93.7%) of the chil- dren’s parents had occupations that were classified, according to the Registrar General’s classification of occupations (Reid, 1989), as professional or skilled (manual and nonmanual).

Materials

We constructed a 47-item questionnaire that consisted of two parts, each preceded by its own printed instructions. In Part 1, 17 questions collected basic personal details (age, school grade, sex, parents’ occupations) and information about the respondents’ television and video viewing facilities, preferences, and habits. The remaining 30 items concerned attitudes toward violence on televi- sion. These items were based on verbatim statements extracted from semi- structured interviews previously conducted with three 11- to 12-year-olds who did not participate in the current study. All attitude questions were responded to on 5-point rating scales.

Procedure

Each class in each school was tested separately. Two research assistants con- ducted the testing sessions. The questionnaires were distributed, and the instruc- tions for Part 1 were read aloud. Any questions were answered in a nondirective manner. The children then completed Part 1 of the questionnaire in silence, after which the instructions for Part 2 were read aloud, and any further questions were iinswered. Part 2 of the questionnaire was then completed in silence.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Dal

hous

ie U

nive

rsity

] at

01:

41 0

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 6: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

414 The Journal of Psychology

Results

Thirteen of the questionnaires contained unusable or incomplete responses. Because these problems appeared to be spread evenly across the groups, we omitted those questionnaires from subsequent analyses, leaving 303 question- naires completed by 155 boys and 148 girls.

We used a principal-components analysis with varimax orthogonal rotation to reduce the number of attitude items on the questionnaire. With Kaiser’s (1960) criterion, nine factors with eigenvalues greater than 1.0 were extracted. Those factors accounted for 53.7% of the variance on the attitude questions.

On the basis of their responses to the attitude items, we divided the partici- pants into two groups. The groups were constructed by using the SPSS Quick Cluster program and the factor scores for each participant on each of the attitude factors. Group 1, representing liberal attitudes toward violence on television, consisted of 183 children. Group 2 (120 children) contained children with more extreme views about violence on television. Though generally positive toward the media, these children were aware of and wary of violence on television, and they were generally more in favor of social control of the media. To confirm the adequacy of the clustering procedure, we conducted a series of nine univariate analyses of variance to test the differences between the cluster centers for each attitude factor. The cluster centers for the nine factors were significantly differ- ent, F(1, 301) > 3.81, p c .05.

Finally, we used the 17 items in Part 1 of the questionnaire as predictor vari- ables in a stepwise discriminant function analysis. We conducted the analysis to identify which of the children’s personal characteristics predicted their attitudes toward violence on television. One discriminant function was extracted, r( 1, N = 303) = 4.30, p c .05. The amount of television watched on school days was the largest predictor variable and was entered into the discriminant analysis at Step 1 (Wilks’s lambda = .99, p c .05). No other variable achieved significance; thus, the function was determined solely by this primary variable, which had a unitary correlation with the function.

The overall pattern of results showed that the children in Group 1, those with more liberal attitudes toward screen violence, reported watching less television on an average school day (an average of 2.7 hr) than their more concerned coun- terparts (Group 2), who watched an average of 3.1 hr per day.

Discussion

Although we examined a substantial number of personal characteristics, we found that the only variable that predicted children’s attitudes toward violence on television was the number of hours spent watching television on school days. However, a preliminary exploration of the relationships among the predictor vari- ables had shown a number of intercorrelations indicating that the impact of the

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Dal

hous

ie U

nive

rsity

] at

01:

41 0

7 O

ctob

er 2

014

Page 7: Children's Attitudes Toward Violence on Television

Hough & Erwin 4 I5

other eight variables may have been mediated by the number of hours of televi- sion viewed on school days.

If, as suggested by Wiegman et al. (1992), children who watch more television are being vicariously exposed to more violence, control could focus on reducing either the absolute quantity of exposure or the violent content of programs. It is not surprising that the children in Group 2, who viewed a great deal more television than those in Group 1, had a higher sense of personal risk and suspicion and were more likely to perceive the world as a scary place (Singer et al., 1984).

It is encouraging that a substantial number of those children recognize that television has an impact on them and that there may be a need for some form of social control of television programming or watching. Somewhat less encouraging are reports that parental control over children’s viewing has decreased over the past several decades and that those children who are less subject to parental control (and more able to avoid it) may be the ones who need it most (Barlow & Hill, 1985).

REFERENCES

Barlow, G., & Hill, A. (1985). Video violence and children. London: Hodder and

Cashmore, E. (1994). . . . and then there was television. London: Routledge. Cline, V. B., Croft, R. G., & Courrier, S. (1973). Desensitization of children to television

violence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27, 360-365. Doob, A. N., & Wood, L. E. (1972). Catharsis and aggression: Effects of annoyance and

retaliation on aggressive behavior. Journal of Personaliry and Social Psychology, 22,

Field, D. E., &Anderson, D. R. (1985). Instruction and modality effects on children’s tele- vision attention and comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 91-100.

Huesman, L. R., & Eron, L. D. (1986). Television and the aggressive child: A cross- national comparison. New York: Erlbaum.

Kaiser, H. F. (1960). The application of electronic computers to factor analysis. Educa- tional and Psychological Measurement, 20, I4 1 - 15 1 .

Reid, I . ( I 989). Social class diferences in Britain. Glasgow: Fontana. Rubinstein, E. A. ( 1 983). Television and behavior. American Psychologist, 38, 820-825. Singer, J. L., Singer, D. G., & Rapacynski, W. S. (1984). Family patterns and television

viewing as predictors of children’s beliefs and aggression. Journal of Communication,

Turner, C. W., Hesse, B. W., & Peterson-Lewis, S. (1986). Naturalistic studies of long-

Van Evra, J. (1990). Television and child development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Wiegman, 0.. Kuttschreuter, M., & Baarda, B. (1992). A longitudinal study of the effects

of television viewing on aggressive and prosocial behaviour. British Journal of Social

Wober, M. (1988). The extent to which viewers watch violence-containing programs. Cur-

Stoughton.

156- 162.

34, 73-89.

term effects of television violence. Journal ofsocial Issues, 42, 51-73.

Psychology, 31, 147-164.

rent Psychology, 7, 76-92.

Received June 7, 1996

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Dal

hous

ie U

nive

rsity

] at

01:

41 0

7 O

ctob

er 2

014