54
Chap.12 Communication 鄭鄭鄭 (Ayo) 鄭鄭 鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭 鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭 鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭 鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭 + 鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭鄭

Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Chap.12 Communication

鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授國立台南大學 環境與生態學院

生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Page 2: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology2

Chap.12 communication

Introduction Communication

• Defined as the transfer of information from a signaler to a receiver

Fig. 12.1 vervet ( 長尾黑顎猴 ) alarm calls Fig. 12.2 clever house

Communication and honesty Communication solves problems

How to coordinate group foraging How to find and secure a mate How to warn others about predators

Page 3: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology3

Fig. 12.1 vervet alarm calls

Vervets give different alarm calls depending on what type of predator has been sighted (A) here we see vervets standing up after

hearing a “chutter” alarm call indicating that a snake has been spotted.

When a leopard (B) is detected, vervets will give a “barking” alarm call and (C) climb up trees for safety.

Page 4: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology4

Page 5: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology5

Fig. 12.2 clever house

Clever house was thought capable of incredible mental feats.

In fact, clever house was picking up very subtle cues from the individual who asked him a question.

Page 6: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology6

Page 7: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology7

Communication and honesty

Selection is thought to favor the most economical way to share information.

The natural selection should favor less exaggerated signals– which was referred to as “conspirational whispers”( 共謀低語 ).

This is because signaling often involves some costs, and natural selection should favor minimizing these costs during conspirational whispers and hence reducing the conspicuousness( 惹人注目 )of the communication itself. For example, the case of ptarmigans ( 雷鳥 ) .

(Fig. 12.3) Both male and female rock ptarmigans are a

stark white against the snow of winter.

Page 8: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology8

the case of ptarmigans ( 雷鳥 )

Once spring arrive, females molt quickly, while males stay white, and as such the males are very conspicuous against the dark brown background (Fig. 12.3). Such conspicuousness attracts females, but it also attracts predators.

As the breeding season progresses, rather than molting, males soil their plumage to become less conspicuous. Males communicate information to females via

coloration, but when the period during which communication is useful comes to an end, they shut down the communication system in a cost-efficient manner– by soiling their white plumage.

Page 9: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology9

Page 10: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology10

Communication and honesty in toads

Selection should favor females paying attention only to those cues that are in fact true indicators of larger size.

The case in toads, in whom deep croaks can only be produced b large males because of the design of their vocal system. (Fig. 12.4) Female toads use the frequency of males’ calls to

gauge their potential mate’s size. Honesty is also possible when traits are not

impossible, but merely very costly, to fake. (handicap principle).

Page 11: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology11

Page 12: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology12

Communication solves problems

How to coordinate group foraging Food calls in birds Honeybees and the waggle dance Chemical and vibrational communication in

foraging ants How to find and secure a mate

Birdsong Aquatic insects

How to warn others about predators Meerkats ( 狐獴 ) Alarm calls

Page 13: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology13

Food calls in birds

Colonial breeding cliff swallows live in nests that serve as “information centers”.

Using both playback experiments and provision experiments (putting food out to entice birds), Brown and his team found that cliff swallows gave off “squeak( 吱吱叫 ) ” calls, which alerted conspecifics that a new food patch– often a swarm of insects– had been found (Table 12.1).

Page 14: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology14

The mean number of birds and squeak calls heard two minutes before and two minutes after insects were flushed by foraging birds.

Page 15: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology15

Under certain conditions, ravens emit a loud “yell” upon uncovering a new food source, such yells attract other birds.

Page 16: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology16

Fig.12.6 yellers are hungry

In ravens, “yelling” is often associated with foraging– in particular, calling others to a food bonanza.( 富礦帶 ) Immature ravens yell

progressively more as a function of hunger.

hungry birds call more often than satiated( 飽足的 ) birds.

Page 17: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology17

Captured birds to a location where there a new prey item that researchers had placed into the environment.

Birds that had learned the location of prey acted as leaders.

Fig. 12.7 Raven recruitment. The line denotes when the number of ravens that

knew of the prey source equals the number of ravens at the prey source the next day.

Green points indicate statistically significant recruitment

Orange points indicate that recruitment was not statistically significant.

Page 18: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology18

Page 19: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology19

Honeybees and the waggle dance

Fig. 12.8 bee foraging.

Honeybee foraging involves a complex communication system, including waggle dances.

This dance, along with other informational cues, gives bees in a hive information about the relative position of newly found food sources.

Page 20: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology20

(A) Imagine a patch of flowers that is 1500 meters from a hive, at an angle 40 degrees to the right of the sun.

(B) when a forager returns, the bee dances in a figure eight pattern. In this case, the angle between a bee’s “straight run” and vertical line is 40 degrees.

Page 21: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology21

(C) The length of the straight run portion of the dance translates into distance from the hive to the food source.

Page 22: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology22

Round dance Sickle dance

Waggle dance

When resources are close to the hive, honeybee foragers tend to use round dance.

When resource are at greater distances, sickle dance

When food is very far from a hive, waggle dance

Page 23: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology23

The number of “figure eight” circuits in a waggle dance when bees were raised at a temperature of 36 or 32℃ ℃

Bees from the 32 ℃treatment were less likely to use the waggle dance when they returned to the hive than were bees from other treatments.

Bees from 32 ℃treatment made significantly fewer “circuits”.

Bees raised in the 36 ℃treatment fared much better in individual learning tasks.

Page 24: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology24

Foraging ants (leaf-cutter ants)

Leaves must first be cut, then carried to the nest, ground up, chewed and treated with enzymes, placed into a “fungus garden”, and subsequently cultivated.

Two chemicals are particularly important, methyl 4-methylpyrrol-2-carboxylate and 3-ethyl-2,5 methylphrazine. These substance are produced in the poison

gland and used to recruit fellow workers to foraging sites.

Recruitment pheromones are incredibly powerful.

Page 25: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology25

Leaf-cutter ants can ravage ( 毀滅 ) foliage in their path. The ants don’t attack all the leaves, however, but instead they often strip some leaves to the stalk, were leaving other leaves untouched.

Page 26: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology26

Fig. 12.13 stridulating communication. A schematic of a leaf-cutter ant cutting a leaf and stridulating its gaster up and down. the vibrations were being sent along the length of a leaf in a long series of vibrational “chirps”.

While only 40% of the ants stridulated when cutting tough leaves, the number increased to 70% when the leaves were tender, and to almost 100% when either type of leaf was dipped in sugar water.

Page 27: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology27

Page 28: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology28

Fig. 12.14 minim workers hitchhiking on a leaf

These minim workers cannot cut leaves, but they are often found hitchhiking rides on leaves on the backs of leaf-cutters. Minim protect these other leaf-cutting ants

from attack by parasitic flies. Hitchhiking minims apparently use the

vibrational cues created by stridulating leaf-cutting nestmates to locate the leaf-cutters.

The stridulating signals emitted by leaf-cutters are used in numerous contexts. One such venue is between leaf-cutters and

minims, who use these signals to eventually hitch rides on cut leaves that are carried on leaf-cutters’ backs.

Page 29: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology29

Page 30: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology30

How to find and secure a mate

The role of (1) vocal communication (birdsong) (2) tactile communication (insects)

Birdsong: In most species of songbirds, males don’t

just learn a single song, they learn many different songs.

• For example, the song sparrow sings about ten different songs, the western marsh wren sings more than a hundred songs, and the brown thrasher sings an incredible thousand different songs.

Page 31: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology31

Cowbirds

The role of repertoire size in the mating success of male brown-headed cowbirds.

Male cowbirds posses between two to eight different perched songs. Earlier studies indicated a correlation

between the size of the perched song repertoire and mating success.

Whether male repertoire size had an effect on female mate choice? Five different song treatments

Page 32: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology32

Five different song treatments

1. Smaller repertoire: s single perched song sung three times in succession by a male Santa Barbara cowbird.

2. --- by a male Ventura cowbird.3. Larger repertoire: three different

perched songs sung in quick succession by a male Santa Barbara cowbird.

4. -- by a male Ventura cowbird.5. A control: from a different species

(song sparrow)

Page 33: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology33

Copulation solicitation displays (CSDs)

Youtube: http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=TQP91a-rhLw

The length of time that a female displayed ritualized “copulation solicitation displays” (CSDs) to different songs was recorded and used as a measure of female choice. Female cowbirds displayed longer CSDs when

they heard cowbird versus song sparrow songs. Females show a marked increase in CSD times

when exposed to males with large song repertoires (Fig.12.15)

Female had longer CSDs when they were exposed to three different songs than to the same song played three times.

Page 34: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology34

Page 35: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology35

Phylogenetic studies of birdsong

Sexual selection operates more strongly in polygynous mating systems and in systems that are sexually dimorphic (Fig. 12.16) Across 17 species of blackbirds, the

maximum note length of songs increased as the size difference between males and females increased.

There is strong competition for access to females, natural selection acts strongly on the many components that make up male song.

Page 36: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology36

Page 37: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology37

Birdsong from a proximate perspective

Birdsong is incredibly diverse in terms of structure, pattern, tempo, frequency, and repertoire size. However, the vocal organ used in birds, the

syrinx, varies little between different species. How is it possible that morphological

invariance in the syrinx can translate into great diversity in birdsong? The syrinx has two compartments—left and

right– and that the two sides of a bird’s brain can control these compartment independently.

Page 38: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology38

Table 12.2 the different ways to sing

Independent bilateral phonation: Operate both sides of the syrinx independently without one side being dominant. (Brown thrasher and gray catbirds)

Unilateral dominance: Have one side of the syrinx dominate song generation. (Canaries)

Alternating lateralization: Alternate which side of the syrinx dominates during a song. (Brown-headed cowbirds)

Sequential lateralization: Have one side of the syrinx dominate for certain frequencies, and the other side dominate for the remainder of the frequencies used in a song. (Northern cardinal)

Page 39: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology39

Page 40: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology40

Ripple ( 波紋 ) communication and mate choice

In water striders, ripples are usually produced by an up-and-down movement of the legs, with both right and left legs in synchrony and in constant contact with the water surface.

The water striders produce different patterns of ripples for different kinds of behaviors, including signals for calling mates, courtship, copulation, post-copulation, sex discrimination, mate guarding, spacing, territoriality, and food defense.

Page 41: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology41

Page 42: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology42

Problem: how to warn others about predators

Woodpecker and chickadee( 山雀 ) alarm calls

Development, learning, and alarm call communication in meerkats

Alarm calls as deceptive communication Male vervets ( 長尾黑顎猴 ) Dishonest alarm calls in

swallows? (Fig. 12.20) Alarm calls in Richardson’s

squirrels (Fig. 12.21)meerkats

Page 43: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology43

While downy woodpeckers don’t give alarm calls when they are paired with same-sex partners, they emit such alarm calls when they are paired with a member of the opposite sex

Page 44: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology44

Development, learning, and alarm call communication in meerkats

Behavior observation indicates, compared to adults, Pups were initially more likely to respond to

alarm calls in the presence of less dangerous or nondangerous predators, and they were more likely to ignore alarm calls emitted in the presence of dangerous predators.

Pups don’t react as appropriately to alarm calls as adults.

Fig. 12.19 age differences in reaction to alarm calls.

Page 45: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology45

Page 46: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology46

Page 47: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology47

Alarm calls as deceptive communication

Because alarm calls are a powerful form of communication– failure to listen might lead to death– natural selection should favor paying close attention to such calls.

Nonetheless, these same selection pressures set up the possibility of using alarm calls in a deceptive manner. Using alarm calls in a deceptive manner is

probably the exception rather than the rule.

Page 48: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology48

Vervets ( 長尾黑顎猴 )

Verbets sometimes use deceptive predator alarm calls during some intergroup encounters – encounters that can lead to serious aggression between group members.

Male verbets five an alarm call when encountering a new troop, even though no predator is in the vicinity.

It was almost always a low-ranking male who gave the call.

Page 49: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology49

Dishonest alarm calls in swallows?

Male swallow may emit false alarm calls when they see their mate engaing in an extrapair copulation.

These alarm calls break up extrapair matings.

When females were absent during egg laying, the males who returned to their nests almost always gave false alarm calls.

Male swallow who emitted false alarm calls during possible periods of extrapair copulations. (Fig. 12.20)

Page 50: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology50

Page 51: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology51

Alarm calls in Richardson’s squirrels

When alarm calls become less reliable, natural selection should favor paying less and less attention to them.

Two treatment (juveniles of squirrels) (Fig. 12.22) Heard a recorded alarm call and then saw

a predator (true alarm calls) Heard a recorded alarm call. But no

predator (false alarm calls)

Page 52: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology52

Page 53: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo 2010 Ethology53

Page 54: Chap.12 Communication 鄭先祐 (Ayo) 教授 國立台南大學 環境與生態學院 生態科學與技術學系 環境生態研究所 + 生態旅遊研究所

Ayo NUTN website:http://myweb.nutn.edu.tw/~hycheng/

問題與討論