CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    1/12

    LIFE COURSE EPIDEMIOLOGY

    Maternal pre-pregnancy overweight and

    obesity, and child neuropsychologicaldevelopment: two Southern Europeanbirth cohort studiesMaribel Casas,1,2,3* Leda Chatzi,4 Anne-Elie Carsin,1,2,3 Pilar Amiano,3,5,6 Monica Guxens,1,2,3

    Manolis Kogevinas,1,2,3,7 Katerina Koutra,4 Nerea Lertxundi,7,8 Mario Murcia,3,9

    Marisa Rebagliato,3,9,10 Isolina Riano,11 Clara L Rodrguez-Bernal,3,9 Theano Roumeliotaki,4

    Jordi Sunyer,1,2,3,12 Michelle Mendez1,2,3,13 and Martine Vrijheid1,2,3

    1Centre for Research in Environmental Epidemiology (CREAL), 08003 Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain, 2IMIM (Hospital del Mar

    Medical Research Institute), 08003 Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain, 3CIBER Epidemiologa y Salud Publica (CIBERESP), Instituto de

    Salud Carlos III, Melchor Fernandez Almagro 3-5, 28029 Madrid, Spain, 4Department of Social Medicine, Faculty of Medicine,

    University of Crete, PO Box 2208, Heraklion, 71003, Crete, Greece, 5Public Health Division of Gipuzkoa, San Sebastian, Basque

    Health Department, Basque Region, Spain, 6Basque Health Research Institute (BIODONOSTIA), Po. Doctor Begiristain, s/n 20014

    Donostia, Spain, 7National School of Public Health, Athens, Greece, 8University of Basque Country (UPV-EHU), Campus Gipuzkoa,

    Tolosa Hiribidea, 70, 20018 San Sebastian, Spain, 9Centre of Public Health Research (CSISP), Avenida Cataluna, 21, 46020 Valencia,

    Spain, 10Public Health Board, Conselleria de Sanitat, Avenida Cataluna, 21, 46020 Valencia, Spain, 11Servicio de Pediatria, Hospital

    San Agustn, Camino de Heros, 6, 33401 Aviles, Asturias, Spain, 12Pompeu Fabra University, Placa de la Merce, 10-12, 08002

    Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain and 13Department of Nutrition, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Chapel Hill, North

    Carolina, USA

    *Corresponding author. Centre for Research in Environmental Epidemiology, 08003 Barcelona, Spain. E-mail: [email protected]

    Accepted 8 January 2013

    Background Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity may be associated with impairedinfant neuropsychological development; however, there are fewstudies and it is unclear if reported associations are due to intra-uterine mechanisms.

    Methods We assessed whether maternal pre-pregnancy overweight and obes-ity were associated with cognitive and psychomotor developmentscores (mean 100 15) of children aged 1122 months in two birthcohorts: Environment and Childhood (INMA, Spain; n 1967) andMother-Child (RHEA, Greece: n 412). Paternal body mass index(BMI) was used as a negative control exposure.

    Results The percentage of overweight and obese mothers was 18% and 8%,respectively, in INMA and 20% and 11% in RHEA, respectively.

    Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity was associated with reduced infantcognitive development scores in both INMA (score reduction: 2.72;95% CI: 5.35, 0.10) and RHEA (score reduction: 3.71; 95% CI:8.45, 1.02), after adjusting for socioeconomic variables and paternalBMI. There was evidence in both cohorts of a dose-response relation-ship with continuous maternal BMI. Paternal overweight/obesity wasnot associated with infant cognitive development. Associations withpsychomotor scores were not consistent between cohorts, and werestronger for paternal than maternal BMI in RHEA.

    Published by Oxford University Press on behalf of the International Epidemiological Association

    The Author 2013; all rights reserved.

    International Journal of Epidemiology2013;42:506517

    doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002

    506

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    2/12

    Conclusions This study in two birth cohorts with moderately high obesity preva-lence suggests that maternal pre-pregnancy obesity is associatedwith reduced child cognitive development at early ages. This asso-ciation appears more likely to be due to maternal than sharedfamily and social mechanisms, but further research is needed todisentangle a direct intrauterine effect from other maternal con-

    founding factors.Keywords Body weight, overweight, obesity, child development, cognition,

    psychomotor performance.

    IntroductionObesity is increasing worldwide and its prevalence isparticularly high in Central, Eastern and SouthernEurope;1 in Southern Europe prevalence can reach30%.1 Obesity is well characterized as a systemic in-flammatory condition.2 There is evidence that suchinflammation during pregnancy may be transferredto the offspring brain, decrease neurogenesis and in-crease oxidative stress, changes that can result in cog-nitive disruption.3,4 Few epidemiological studies5-12

    have examined evidence of neuropsychological effectsin children associated with maternal obesity and ofthese, only six evaluated cognitive function5,8-12 andtwo evaluated psychomotor development.5,10 Fourstudies reported maternal pre-pregnancy obesity[body mass index (BMI) 530 kg/m2] t o b e relatedto an increased risk of lower cognitive function5,10,11

    and an increased risk of intellectual disability.9 Thesestudies did not find any associations with maternaloverweight5,10,11 or motor ability.5,10 One study

    showed a decrease in cognitive performance withincreasing continuous BMI.12 An analysis of twoEuropean cohorts observed no consistent associationbetween maternal pre-pregnancy overweight/obesitycombined and child behavioural problems and meas-ures of cognitive abilities.8 Challenges for future stu-dies include having adequate sample sizes toseparately assess maternal overweight and obesityand disentangling the role of intrauterine mechan-isms from those of confounding variables linked tomaternal weight status.

    The mechanisms underlying the potential effects ofmaternal pre-pregnancy overweight and obesity onchild neurodevelopment are still unknown. Besides

    the potential effects on brain development fromanti-inflammatory processes, high levels of leptin, ahormone synthesized by adipose tissue and associated

    with obesity,13 have been associated with gestationaldiabetes. Gestational diabetes can delay brain maturityand induce neurobehavioural abnormalities in off-spring.14 Thyroid dysfunction, as well as nutrient defi-ciencies such as folic acid or vitamin D, all morecommon in obese women,15,16 have also been impli-cated as potential mechanisms linking maternal over-

    weight/obesity and child neurodevelopment.17,18

    Further, some environmental neurotoxicants, such aspolychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and mercury (Hg),can bio accumulate in adipose tissue. Rodriguez et al.7

    proposed that adipose tissue of overweight and obesewomen may contain higher levels of neurotoxins thatwill negatively affect foetal brain development.

    This study aimed to assess whether maternal

    pre-pregnancy overweight and obesity were associatedwith neuropsychological development of children be-tween 1122 months in two countries with moderatelyhigh obesity prevalence (Spain and Greece). We usedindividual neuropsychological tests, assessed theimpact of possible intermediate factors and assessedthe association between paternal overweight and obes-ity with childrens neuropsychological development, asa negative control exposure.19,20 We hypothesized that adirect maternal intrauterine mechanism would producestronger associations than an indirect paternal effect.This approach has previously been used to disentangleintrauterine influences from social and other confound-ing variables forprenatal smoking19 and maternal over-

    weight/obesity.8

    MethodsStudy participants

    The population-based birth cohort study INMA(INfancia y Medio AmbienteEnvironment andChildhood) recruited pregnant women during the firsttrimester of pregnancy from four Spanish regions(Asturias, Gipuzkoa, Sabadell and Valencia) between2004 and 2008, following a common protocol.21

    Inclusion criteria were: age 516 years, singleton preg-nancy, no assisted conception, intention to deliver

    at the reference hospital and no communication prob-lems.21 A total of 2644 mothers were recruited and2226 children undertook the neuropsychological testat around 14 months of age. After the exclusion of pre-term births, neuropsychological tests of uncertain val-idity due to less than optimal cooperation of the child,basic pathologies (i.e. epilepsy, plagiocephaly) and ageat examination 523 months, 1967 mother-child pairs

    were included in the analyses (Table 1).The RHEA (Mother-Child) Study in Crete (Greece)22

    enrolled pregnant women during 20072009 at the

    MATERNAL OBESITY AND CHILD NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT 507

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    3/12

    time of the first major ultrasound examination at 1013 weeks of gestation if they were: resident in thestudy areas,416 years of age and without communi-cation handicaps. The cohort enrolled 1606 mothersand a random subgroup of 612 children performedthe neuropsychological test at around 18 months ofage. After excluding multiple pregnancies, pretermbirths, subjects with missing information on maternalBMI, infants with basic pathologies (pervasive devel-opmental disorder and hydrocephalus) and tests ofuncertain validity, a total of 412 mother-child pairs

    were included in the analysis (Table 1).

    Maternal and paternal overweight andobesity

    Maternal height was measured and pre-pregnancy

    weight reported by the mother at the first trimestervisit; these were used to calculate pre-pregnancy BMI(kg/m2). Reported pre-pregnancy weight was highlycorrelated with measured weight at 12 weeks of preg-nancy in INMA (r 0.96; P < 0.0001). Paternal weightand height were reported by the mother at the firsttrimester visit and used to calculate paternal BMI.Maternal and paternal BMI were grouped accordingto the World Health Organization categories for under-

    weight (

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    4/12

    childrens birthweight, gestational age and gender wasobtained from clinical records. Data on nursery attend-ance, main childminder, childs weight and height, ma-ternal employment status after birth and breastfeedingduration were collected when infants were around 14and 9 months old in INMA and RHEA, respectively(Supplementary Table 1, available as Supplementary

    data at IJE online). Childrens BMI-for-age z-scoreswere calculated and children with BMI z-scores51.035, corresponding to the 85th percentile, were clas-sified as overweight.27 All questionnaires were con-ducted face to face by trained interviewers.

    In INMA, 25-OH vitamin D3 was determined in thefirst trimester plasma using a high-performance liquidchromatography assay (BioRAD kit28). First-trimesterserum thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH) was deter-mined using a solid-phase, time-resolved sandwichfluoroimmunoassay (AutoDELFIA, PerkinElmer Lifeand analytical Sciences, Wallac Oy, Turku, Finland)using a lanthanide metal europium (Eu) label.First-trimester serum C-reactive protein (CRP), a sensi-

    tive marker for systemic inflammation,2 was deter-mined by turbidimetric assay using a Hitachi modularanalyser system (Roche Modular DPP, Hitachi Ltd,Tokyo, Japan) (this last measure was only available inthe Gipuzkoa and Sabadell regions). Total PCBs (118,153, 138, 180) were also measured in maternalfirst-trimester serum in Gipuzkoa, Sabadell andValencia using a 96-well solid-phase extraction discplate procedure gas chromatograph as described else-

    where.29 Total Hg was measured in cord blood in allINMA regions by thermal decomposition, amalgam-ation and atomic absorption spectrometry usinga single-purpose LECO AMA-254 analyzer.30 In RHEA,TSH was measured in first-trimester maternal serum bythe IMMULITE 2000 immunoassay system (SiemensHealthcare Diagnostics, IL, USA).

    Statistical analysis

    Multiple imputation of missing values for the covari-ates was performed using chained equations where 25completed datasets were generated, separately forINMA and RHEA cohorts, and analysed using thestandard combination rules for multiple imput-ations.31 Complete case analyses were also conductedfor all models.

    Multivariate linear regression models were used toassess the association between maternal pre-

    pregnancy overweight and obesity, and infant cogni-tive and psychomotor development scores. First, allmodels were adjusted for childs gender, as well asfor INMA regions (Model 1). In a second model, aset of primary socioeconomic variables were included:maternal and paternal education, maternal country ofbirth and maternal and paternal social class (INMAonly) (Model 2). In a third model, other potentialconfounders were included: maternal and paternalage, breastfeeding duration, maternal smoking, ma-ternal employment status during pregnancy and

    after birth, parity, nursery attendance and main child-minder (Model 3). These variables were selected apriori from the literature based on their potential toconfound the association between maternal pre-pregnancy BMI and child cognitive development.5,79

    A fourth model included paternal BMI categories(Model 4). Standardized effect size (Cohens d) was

    calculated dividing theb coefficients by one SD (mag-nitude of effect size: 0.20 small, 0.50moderate,0.80 large).32 We repeated Model 3 using BMI ascontinuous measure. We also repeated Models 13using paternal rather than maternal overweight andobesity as the main exposure of interest.

    Sensitivity analyses were performed to assess the ro-bustness of our results (all based on Model 3). Weassessed the role of the following possible intermediatefactors: vitamin D, dietary folate intake, folate supple-mentation, TSH, CRP, PCBs and total Hg, gestational

    weight gain, birthweight, gestational age and childsBMI after birth, by including them separately inModel 3 and evaluating if they changed the coeffi-

    cients for maternal obesity. Further, we excluded ma-ternal and paternal social class covariates from theINMA models to assess the potential residual con-founding due to socioeconomic factors in RHEA.Finally, we excluded mothers who did not live withthe father to evaluate the validity of maternal reportof paternal BMI. All statistical analyses were conducted

    with Stata 12.0 statistical software (Stata Corporation,College Station, TX, USA).

    ResultsThe percentage of overweight and obese mothers was

    18% and 8%, respectively, in INMA and 20% and 11%,respectively, in RHEA (Table 2). The percentage ofoverweight and obese fathers was higher: 44% and12%, respectively, in INMA and 47% and 25%, re-spectively, in RHEA (Supplementary Table 4, availableasSupplementary data at IJE online). The prevalenceof maternal overweight and obesity was higher inlower educational levels (INMA and RHEA) andlower social classes (INMA) (Table 2). Paternal obes-ity was more prevalent in lower educational andsocial class categories in INMA, but in RHEA there

    was little difference between paternal educationallevels (Supplementary Table 4, available asSupplementary data at IJE online). In both cohorts,

    younger and older mothers were more likely to beoverweight, whereas younger mothers were morelikely to be obese (Table 2). Paternal overweight wasmore prevalent in older fathers in INMA but in

    younger fathers in RHEA; the prevalence of obesefathers was relatively similar between age categoriesin both cohorts. Smokers and mothers who breastfedless were more likely to be obese. Confounding pat-terns were similar between overweight and obesemothers; only obesity was more strongly associated

    with lower educational levels than overweight.

    MATERNAL OBESITY AND CHILD NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT 509

    http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1
  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    5/12

    Table 2 Percentage of overweight and obese mothers by covariate categories

    Characteristics

    INMA RHEA

    TotalN

    Overweightn (%)

    Obesen (%) P-valuea

    TotalN

    Overweightn (%)

    Obesen (%) P-valuea

    N 1967 361 (18.4) 150 (7.6) 412 82 (19.9) 44 (10.7)

    Childs gender 0.404 0.225

    Female 986 18.5 6.5 201 16.9 12.4

    Male 980 18.4 8.8 211 22.8 9.0

    Missing (%) 1 (0.1) 0

    Maternal age 0.074 0.068

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    6/12

    In both cohorts, cognitive development scores werehigher among females, firstborns, children whosemothers were not the main childminder, and mothers

    who worked either during pregnancy or after birth.

    In INMA, children with higher psychomotor develop-ment scores were more likely to not attend nurseryschool, to have shorter breastfeeding duration, to havesiblings and to have younger parents. In RHEA,

    Table 2 Continued

    Characteristics

    INMA RHEA

    TotalN

    Overweightn (%)

    Obesen (%) P-valuea

    TotalN

    Overweightn (%)

    Obesen (%) P-valuea

    N 1967 361 (18.4) 150 (7.6) 412 82 (19.9) 44 (10.7)

    Parity 0.006 0.249

    0 1124 16.4 6.5 181 17.1 8.8

    1 841 21.1 9.2 219 20.6 12.8

    Missing (%) 2 (0.1) 12 (2.9)

    Maternal smoking 0.369 0.126

    Never smoke 896 18.8 7.5 237 17.7 11.0

    Quit early pregnancy 730 16.6 7.5 92 28.3 6.5

    During pregnancy 315 21.0 8.9 82 17.1 14.6

    Missing (%) 26 (1.3) 1 (0.2)

    Maternal employment statusduring pregnancy

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    7/12

    psychomotor development scores were higher amongchildren who were breastfed longer, whose parentshad higher educational levels, whose mothers wereolder and those whose mothers worked during preg-nancy or after birth (Supplementary Table 5, availableas Supplementary data at IJE online).

    Children of obese mothers had lower cognitive de-velopment scores than those of normal weightmothers in INMA (3.37 points, 95% CI: 5.90,0.83) and RHEA (6.00 points, 95% CI: 10.75,1.24) in the unadjusted Model 1 (Table 3 andFigure 1). Adjustment for socioeconomic variables(Model 2) attenuated these coefficients, especially in

    RHEA. After further adjustment (Model 3), the coef-ficient for maternal obesity was 2.69 (95% CI:5.27, 0.11) in INMA and 3.92 (95% CI: 8.60,0.76) in RHEA (Table 3). These associations corres-ponded to a Cohens d of 0.18 (95% CI: 0.35,0.01) in INMA and of0.26 (95% CI: 0.57, 0.05)in RHEA, indicating a small effect size. Maternalpre-pregnancy overweight was associated with a

    very small reduction in cognitive scores in INMAbut not in RHEA (Table 3). In both cohorts weobserved decreasing cognitive scores with increasing

    continuous BMI (INMA: 0.17 per kg/m2; 95%CI: 0.34, 0.01; RHEA: 0.26 per kg/m2; 95% CI:0.57, 0.06). When analysing very obese mothers(BMI 433 kg/m2) separately, these tended to showthe lowest cognitive scores compared with normal

    weight mothers, particularly in INMA (4.12; 95%CI: 7.64, 0.61) (RHEA [3.97; 95% CI: 9.72,1.79]). Receptive communication scores in RHEA

    yielded similar results to those shown for cognitivescores; expressive communication scores had no asso-ciation with obesity (data not shown).

    Coefficients for psychomotor scores in INMA andfine, but not gross, motor scores in RHEA showed

    small reductions in obese and overweight motherscompared to normal weight mothers (Table 3), and

    we observed trends for declining scores with continu-ous BMI (INMA: 0.18; 95% CI: 0.34, 0.01; RHEAfine motor: 0.28; 95% CI: 0.60, 0.03; RHEA grossmotor: 0.07; 95% CI: 0.44, 0.29). Adjusting forpaternal BMI did not greatly influence the coefficientsfor cognitive or psychomotor development scores(Table 3 [Model 4] and Figures 1 and 2). Paternaloverweight and obesity were not associated withchild cognitive or psychomotor development in

    Table 3 Association (b coefficient with 95% confidence interval) of maternal pre-pregnancy BMI with cognitive andpsychomotor development scores main analysis with multiple imputed dataa (cognitive: INMA N 1866, RHEAN 397;psychomotor: INMA N 1869, RHEA N 397)

    CohortMaternal

    BMI N

    Model 1b Model 2c Model 3d Model 4e

    b 95% CI b 95% CI b 95% CI b 95% CI

    Cognitive development scores

    INMA Normal 1360 ref ref ref ref

    Overweight 359 1.14 2.87, 0.59 0.95 2.69, 0.79 0.88 2.63, 0.88 0.88 2.64, 0.88

    Obese 147 3.37 5.90, 0.83 2.93 5.49, 0.36 2.69 5.27, 0.11 2.72 5.35, 0.10

    RHEA Normal 271 ref ref ref ref

    Overweight 82 0.17 3.86, 3.52 0.64 2.97, 4.25 1.34 2.32, 5.00 1.41 2.26, 5.08

    Obese 44 6.00 10.75, 1.24 4.20 8.88, 0.49 3.92 8.60, 0.76 3.71 8.45, 1.02

    Psychomotor development scores

    INMA Normal 1363 ref ref ref ref

    Overweight 358 1.36 3.11, 0.38 1.49 3.25, 0.27 1.61 3.38, 0.16 1.68 3.45, 0.10

    Obese 148 1.51 4.06, 1.04 1.57 4.15, 1.01 1.85 4.45, 0.75 2.10 4.74, 0.54

    RHEA,fine

    motor

    Normal 271 ref ref ref ref

    Overweight 82

    3.49

    7.07, 0.09

    2.60

    6.18, 0.97

    2.23

    5.88, 1.42

    1.94

    5.59, 1.71Obese 44 2.97 7.58, 1.64 1.51 6.15, 3.13 1.67 6.33, 3.00 1.09 5.80, 3.62

    RHEA,grossmotor

    Normal 271 ref ref ref ref

    Overweight 82 2.01 2.15, 6.17 3.16 1.00, 7.32 3.49 0.69, 7.67 4.00 0.18, 8.18

    Obese 44 2.88 8.24, 2.49 0.72 6.12, 4.67 0.40 5.75, 4.94 0.73 4.66, 6.12

    Ref, reference population.aExcluding underweight mothers.bModel 1: adjusted for childs gender and region (only in INMA).cModel 2: model 1 plus maternal education, paternal education, maternal country of birth and maternal and paternal social class(only in INMA).dModel 3: model 2 plus maternal age, paternal age, breastfeeding duration, maternal smoking, maternal employment status duringpregnancy and after birth, parity, nursery attendance and main childminder.eModel 4: model 3 plus paternal BMI.

    512 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY

    http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1
  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    8/12

    INMA (Figures 1 and 2 and Supplementary Table 7,available as Supplementary data at IJE online). InRHEA, however, children of overweight and obesefathers had reduced fine and gross psychomotorscores (Figure 2andSupplementary Table 7, availableas Supplementary data at IJE online). Complete case

    analyses yielded similar results for both maternal andpaternal models (Supplementary Tables 6 and 8,available as Supplementary data at IJE online).

    Sensitivity analyses showed no changes in coeffi-cients for cognitive development associated withmaternal obesity after additionally adjusting for

    Figure 2 Association of maternal and paternal pre-pregnancy obesity on child psychomotor development scores.aUnadjusted (Model 1 of Table 3 and Supplementary Table 7, available as Supplementary data at IJE online): modelincluding childs gender and region (only in INMA).bWith mutual adjustment (Model 4 ofTable 3and Supplementary Table 7, available asSupplementary dataat IJE online):

    model including childs gender, region (only in INMA), maternal education, paternal education, maternal country of birth,maternal and paternal social class (only in INMA), maternal age, paternal age, breastfeeding duration, maternal smoking,maternal employment status during pregnancy and after birth, parity, nursery attendance, main childminder, and paternalBMI

    Figure 1 Association of maternal and paternal pre-pregnancy obesity on child cognitive development scores.aUnadjusted (Model 1 of Table 3 and Supplementary Table 7, available as Supplementary data at IJE online): modelincluding childs gender and region (only in INMA).bWith mutual adjustment (Model 4 ofTable 3and Supplementary Table 7, available asSupplementary dataat IJE online):model including childs gender, region (only in INMA), maternal education, paternal education, maternal country of birth,maternal and paternal social class (only in INMA), maternal age, paternal age, breastfeeding duration, maternal smoking,maternal employment status during pregnancy and after birth, parity, nursery attendance, main childminder, and paternalBMI

    MATERNAL OBESITY AND CHILD NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT 513

    http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1
  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    9/12

    gestational weight gain, gestational age at birth orchild BMI z-score after birth. However, includingbirthweight in the model led to a substantial reduc-tion in the coefficient in INMA (from2.69 to 3.14)

    (Table 4). Adjusting for dietary folate intake, folatesupplementation, circulating levels of vitamin D, ma-ternal TSH, total Hg and PCBs levels did not changethe cognitive development coefficient linked to obesity(Table 4). After excluding maternal and paternalsocial class from the INMA models, coefficientsslightly decreased, particularly in paternal BMImodels (mental: from 0.56 to 0.85; psychomotor:from 0.09 to 0.26) (Supplementary Table 9, availableas Supplementary data at IJE online). Excludingmothers who did not live with the father, the direc-tion of the associations remained similar to the mainanalyses (data not shown).

    DiscussionMaternal pre-pregnancy obesity was associated witha reduction in childrens cognitive developmentscores at 12 years of age. This association was con-sistent in the Spanish and Greek cohorts, althoughthe smaller sample size of the latter resulted inlower statistical power. Maternal pre-pregnancy over-

    weight was associated with reduced cognitive devel-opment scores only in INMA. In both cohorts there

    was evidence of a dose-response with continuousBMI. Associations between maternal overweight andobesity and infant psychomotor scores were weakerand less consistent between the two cohorts.

    Paternal BMI was not associated with infant cognitiveor psychomotor development in INMA; however, inRHEA, paternal obesity was more strongly associated

    with infant fine and gross motor scores than was ma-ternal obesity.

    Comparing maternal and paternal weight status, weobserved substantially stronger associations for mater-nal than paternal obesity with cognitive scores, espe-cially in the INMA cohort. In the INMA cohort, bothmaternal and paternal BMI were strongly and consist-ently associated with socioeconomic factors suggest-ing that, at least in this cohort, paternal BMI wasa good indicator for a common social environment.In RHEA, however, paternal BMI was less strongly

    and consistently associated with education and wewere not able to adjust for social class, so here thepaternal adjustment is less likely to account for acommon social environment. In both cohortsthough, associations with maternal obesity wererobust to adjustment for paternal BMI, both usingmultiple imputation and complete case analyses.Overall, these results suggest that, for cognitive devel-opment, a maternal mechanism (be it a direct intra-uterine or an indirect mechanism) is more likely to beresponsible for these findings than an indirect effect

    Table 4 Cognitive development scores for obese mothers (Model 3): intermediate factors (b coefficient with 95% confi-dence interval)

    Characteristics

    INMAb RHEAb

    N b 95% CI N b 95% CI

    Obese mothersa 147 2.69 5.27, 0.11 44 3.92 8.60, 0.76

    Intermediate factors

    Gestational weight gain 147 2.72 5.34, 0.11 44 3.68 8.39, 1.03

    Childs birthweight 147 3.14 5.72, 0.57 44 4.23 8.91, 0.45

    Childs gestational age 147 2.82 5.39, 0.26 44 3.99 8.68, 0.70

    Childs BMI after birth 147 2.62 5.20, 0.04 44 4.01 8.72, 0.70

    Dietary folate intake 147 2.65 5.23, 0.06 44 3.82 8.52, 0.89

    Folate supplementation 147 2.69 5.28, 0.11 44 3.96 8.64, 0.73

    Vitamin D (maternal plasma) 147 2.59 5.17, 0.02

    TSH (maternal serum)d 147 2.84 5.54, 0.15 44 4.03 8.73, 0.67

    Total Hg (cord blood) 147 2.67 5.25, 0.09

    Sub-sample

    CRP (maternal serum)c 64 3.55 4.30, 2.80

    PCBs (cord blood)d 98 3.18 6.37, 0.02

    TSH, thyroid-stimulating hormone; Hg, mercury; CRP, C-reactive protein; PCBs, polychlorinated biphenyls.aCompared with normal weight mothers (INMA: 1360; RHEA: 271).bModel 3: adjusted for childs gender, region (only in INMA), maternal education, paternal education, maternal country of birth,maternal and paternal social class (only in INMA), maternal age, paternal age, breastfeeding duration, maternal smoking, maternalemployment status during pregnancy and after birth, parity, nursery attendance and main childminder.cCRP only available in INMA: Gipuzkoa and Sabadell (Model 3 for obese mothers: 3.47; 95%CI: 7.27, 0.34).dPCBs available in INMA, Gipuzkoa, Sabadell and Valencia (Model 3 for obese mothers: 3.24; 95%CI: 6.12, 0.37).

    514 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY

    http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1
  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    10/12

    through factors that act in the shared family andsocioeconomic context (socioeconomic class, smoking,exercise patterns) or paternal factors (genetics).

    Adjustment for paternal BMI is unlikely to adjustfor indirect factors that are mainly determined bythe mother: even though we adjusted for maternalemployment status, main childminder and nursery at-

    tendance during the first year of life, residual con-founding by postnatal factors, such as diet andother factors related to postnatal care, cannot beruled out. It should also be noted that confoundingstructures for some variables were somewhat differentin the two cohorts: in RHEA, maternal BMI was lessassociated with maternal employment status, breast-feeding and nursery attendance than in INMA. Thesimilarity in the main results for cognitive develop-ment in these two cohorts with different confoundingstructures adds some support to a causal effect ofmaternal BMI on child cognition. Paternal BMI wasreported by the mothers and thus was probably esti-mated with more error than maternal BMI; this mayhave resulted in attenuated associations with paternalBMI. However, after excluding those women who didnot live with the father, results did not change. Also,the cohorts did not collect information on some otherpotential confounders, such as parental intelligence,mental health and home environment. To further ad-dress the confounding issues and establish causalityof the observed association with more certainty, largerstudies could use genetic variants that are reliablyassociated with maternal adiposity as instrumental

    variables (i.e. Mendelian randomization studies ormultivariate twin design33,34).

    Children of overweight mothers showed a small re-

    duction in cognitive development scores (only inINMA). In both cohorts however, we observeddecreasing cognitive scores with increasing continu-ous maternal BMI, with the greatest decreases in chil-dren of very obese mothers; therefore, it seemsprobable that there is an impact across the entireBMI range. Furthermore, in spite of the differencesobserved in educational levels between overweightand obese mothers, the reduction in cognitive scores

    was stronger in obese than in overweight mothers inthe unadjusted model (Table 3, Model 1). This makesit unlikely that such differences in educational levels

    were responsible for the non-significance of results inoverweight mothers.

    Concerning psychomotor development, maternalBMI was associated with somewhat reduced scoresin INMA and RHEA. However, in RHEA, but notINMA, this association was stronger for paternal

    than for maternal BMI. The inconsistencies observedbetween the cohorts with respect to paternal over-

    weight and obesity make it difficult to assess whether

    the weak associations observed between maternalBMI and psychomotor development are due tochance, indirect familial and paternal influences orother mechanisms.

    Only six other studies have examined the associ-ation between maternal pre-pregnancy overweightand obesity and child cognitive development;5,8-12

    two of them also assessed psychomotor develop-ment.5,10 Neggers et al.5 found that children of obesemothers had lower IQ and nonverbal scores at age5 years in a small (n 355) population with high

    levels of obesity (44%) and low mean score for gen-eral IQ (82.1). A large US study (n 6850) showedthat children of obese mothers had lower mental de-

    velopment scores at age 2 years.10 In both studies noassociation was observed with maternal overweight.Furthermore and consistently with our inconclusivefindings for psychomotor development, neither mater-nal obesity nor maternal overweight were associated

    with childrens gross motor ability scores. In anothertwo studies, Heikura et al.9 and Tanda et al.11 reportedthat pre-pregnancy obesity, not overweight, was asso-ciated with offspring mild intellectual disability (IQ5070) at age 11 and lower Peabody individualachievement test (PIAT) at age 57 years, respectively.

    An analysis within the Millenium Cohort Study(n 19 517) recently reported a significant decreasein cognitive scores with increasing continuous BMI(age 5: b0.075; age 7: b0.17).

    12 Finally,Brion et al.8 found no consistent associations betweenmaternal overweight/obesity, as a combined variable,and maternal reports on child cognitive abilities at 30months to 8 years in two European cohorts (ALSPACand Generation R) with about 20% of overweight/obese mothers. There were no associations with pa-ternal BMI. In ALSPAC, however, a strong negativeassociation was observed between maternal over-

    weight/obesity and general IQ assessed by a psycholo-gist at age 8 years. These findings reinforce the

    potential importance of conducting standardized as-sessments by trained psychologists rather than mater-nal reports. These studies and our findings alsosuggest that it may be important to distinguish over-

    weight and obese mothers and to evaluate associ-ations across the range of pre-pregnancy BMIs;combining these categories may obscure effects if as-sociations are observed only in a substantially smallersubset of mothers who are obese. Child age at testadministration may also explain some differences be-tween previous studies and ours. Children in ourstudy were younger, which may have the advantagethat they were less influenced by additional postnatalfactors such as schooling. However, the Bayley test

    has shown low predictive value for later cognition35

    and the associations reported here need to be fol-lowed up at older ages.

    In this study we examined the impact of adjustingfor several plausible biological intermediates.However, none of the factors explored seemed toexplain the association between maternal obesityand child cognition. Obese mothers from ourstudy did not have elevated levels of TSH or lowerlevels of vitamin D compared with normal weightindividuals, as reported in other studies;15,16 this

    MATERNAL OBESITY AND CHILD NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT 515

  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    11/12

    may explain why we did not observe a mediatingeffect. Though levels of CRP were higher in obesemothers (mean: 11.39 6.95 mg/l) compared with nor-mal weight mothers (mean: 5.65 7.27 mg/l), this in-dicator of systemic inflammation did not seem toexplain the association between maternal obesityand the reduction in cognitive scores. However, only

    one inflammatory indicator was available and only ina sub-sample (n 894). The inclusion of additionalmarkers of inflammatory processes such as certaininterleukins, cytokines or hormones involved in theregulation of energy balance processes such as leptinhormone36 would be of great interest in determiningpossible mechanisms.

    The strengths of this study lie in its prospectivedesign, its use of two independent cohorts for repli-cation, its adjustment for paternal BMI, and the qual-ity of neuropsychological tests: the BSID is recognisedinternationally as one of the most comprehensivetools to assess cognitive and psychomotor abilities ofchildren starting as young as 1 month old.25 Different

    versions of the Bayley test were used, however, inINMA and RHEA, requiring caution in comparing re-sults between cohorts.

    ConclusionsThis study in two Southern European birth cohorts

    with moderately high obesity prevalence suggeststhat maternal pre-pregnancy obesity is associated

    with reduced child cognitive development at earlyages; this association appears more likely to be dueto maternal than shared family and social mechan-

    isms. Findings for psychomotor development wereinconclusive. Further research is needed to disentan-gle an intrauterine effect from other maternal con-founding factors, for example by using markers ofthe inflammatory processes or genetic variants thatare reliably associated with maternal adiposity.

    Supplementary DataSupplementary data are available at IJE online.

    Funding

    This work was supported by grants from the SpanishMinistry of Health-Instituto de Salud Carlos III(Red INMA G03/176, CB06/02/0041, FIS-PI041436,FIS-PI081151, FIS-PI042018, FISPI09/02311, FIS-PI06/0867, FIS-PS09/00090, FIS-FEDER 03/1615, 04/1509,04/1112, 04/1931, 05/1079, 05/1052, 06/1213, 07/0314,09/02647); Generalitat de Catalunya (CIRIT 1999SGR00241); Conselleria de Sanitat Generalitat Valenciana;Universidad de Oviedo; Obra social Cajastur;Department of Health of the Basque Government(2005111093, 2009111069); the Provincial Governmentof Gipuzkoa (DFG06/004, DFG08/001); Fundacion RogerTorne (INMA Project); European project: EU FP7-2008-ENV-1.2.1.4 Envirogenomarkers; and the Greek

    Ministry of Health (Program of Prevention of obesityand neurodevelopmental disorders in preschool children,in Heraklion district, Crete, Greece (2011-2014) (RHEAstudy). European projects: EU FP6-2003-Food-3-ANewGeneris, EU FP6. STREP Hiwate, EU FP7 ENV.2007.1.2.2.2. Project No 211250 Escape, EU FP7-HEALTH-2009 Proposal No 241604 CHICOS, EU FP7-ENV-2008 Proposal No 226285 ENRIECO (INMAProject and RHEA study). All these sources of fundinghave confirmed their agreement.

    AcknowledgementsThe authors would particularly like to thank all the

    cohort participants for their generous collaboration. Afull roster of the INMA Project Investigators can befound at http://www.proyectoinma.org/presentacion-inma/listado-investigadores/listado-investigadores.html .

    Conflict of interest: None declared.

    KEY MESSAGES

    Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity may be associated with impaired infant neuropsychologicaldevelopment; however, there are few studies and it is unclear if reported associations are due tointrauterine mechanisms rather than to social factors or indirect pathways linked to maternal weight

    status.

    Using data from two birth cohorts with moderately high obesity prevalence, children of obesemothers had a reduction in cognitive development scores at 12 years of age.

    This reduction in cognitive developmental scores appears more likely to be due to maternal thanshared family and social mechanisms.

    Further research is needed to disentangle a direct intrauterine effect from other maternal confound-ing factors.

    516 INTERNATIONAL JOURNAL OF EPIDEMIOLOGY

    http://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1http://www.proyectoinma.org/presentacion-inma/listado-investigadores/listado-investigadores.htmlhttp://www.proyectoinma.org/presentacion-inma/listado-investigadores/listado-investigadores.htmlhttp://www.proyectoinma.org/presentacion-inma/listado-investigadores/listado-investigadores.htmlhttp://www.proyectoinma.org/presentacion-inma/listado-investigadores/listado-investigadores.htmlhttp://ije.oxfordjournals.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1093/ije/dyt002/-/DC1
  • 8/14/2019 CASAS ET AL 2012_Obesity neurodevelopment.pdf

    12/12

    References1 Berghofer A, Pischon T, Reinhold T, Apovian CM,

    Sharma AM, Willich SN. Obesity prevalence from aEuropean perspective: a systematic review. BMC PublicHealth 2008;8:200.

    2 Das UN. Is obesity an inflammatory condition? Nutrition2001;17:95366.

    3 Tozuka Y, Kumon M, Wada E, Onodera M, Mochizuki H,Wada K. Maternal obesity impairs hippocampal BDNFproduction and spatial learning performance in youngmouse offspring. Neurochem Int 2010;57:23547.

    4 Bilbo SD, Tsang V. Enduring consequences of maternalobesity for brain inflammation and behavior of offspring.FASEB J 2010;24:210415.

    5 Neggers YH, Goldenberg RL, Ramey SL, Cliver SP.Maternal prepregnancy body mass index and psycho-motor development in children. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand2003;82:23540.

    6 Rodriguez A. Maternal pre-pregnancy obesity and risk forinattention and negative emotionality in children. J ChildPsychol Psychiatry 2010;51:13443.

    7 Rodriguez A, Miettunen J, Henriksen TB et al. Maternal

    adiposity prior to pregnancy is associated with ADHDsymptoms in offspring: evidence from three prospectivepregnancy cohorts. Int J Obes (Lond) 2008;32:55057.

    8 Brion MJ, Zeegers M, Jaddoe V et al. Intrauterine effectsof maternal prepregnancy overweight on child cognitionand behavior in 2 cohorts. Pediatrics 2011;127:e20211.

    9 Heikura U, Taanila A, Hartikainen AL et al. Variations inprenatal sociodemographic factors associated with intel-lectual disability: a study of the 20-year interval betweentwo birth cohorts in northern Finland. Am J Epidemiol2008;167:16977.

    10 Hinkle SN, Schieve LA, Stein AD, Swan DW,Ramakrishnan U, Sharma AJ. Associations between ma-ternal prepregnancy body mass index and child neurode-velopment at 2 years of age. Int J Obes (Lond) 2012;36:131219.

    11 Tanda R, Salsberry PJ, Reagan PB, Fang MZ. Theimpact of prepregnancy obesity on childrens cognitivetest scores. Matern Child Health J 2012; doi:10.1007/s10995-012-0964-4.

    12 Basatemur E, Gardiner J, Williams C, Melhuish E,Barnes J, Sutcliffe A. Maternal prepregnancy BMI andchild cognition: a longitudinal cohort study. Pediatrics2013;131:5663.

    13 Hendler I, Blackwell SC, Mehta SH et al. The levels ofleptin, adiponectin, and resistin in normal weight, over-weight, and obese pregnant women with and withoutpreeclampsia. Am J Obstet Gynecol 2005;193:97983.

    14 Ornoy A. Growth and neurodevelopmental outcome ofchildren born to mothers with pregestational and gesta-tional diabetes. Pediatr Endocrinol Rev 2005;3:10413.

    15 Kouidhi S, Berhouma R, Ammar M et al. Relationship ofThyroid Function with Obesity and Type 2 Diabetes inEuthyroid Tunisian Subjects. Endocr Res 2012;10:53143.

    16 Bodnar LM, Catov JM, Roberts JM, Simhan HN.Prepregnancy obesity predicts poor vitamin D status inmothers and their neonates. J Nutr 2007;137:243742.

    17 Haddow JE, Palomaki GE, Allan WC et al. Maternal thy-roid deficiency during pregnancy and subsequent neuro-psychological development of the child. N Engl J Med1999;341:54955.

    18 Handel AE, Disanto G, Ramagopalan SV. Visceral obesityand brain volume. Ann Neurol 2010;68:77071.

    19 Davey Smith G. Assessing intrauterine influences on off-spring health outcomes: can epidemiological studies yieldrobust findings? Basic Clin Pharmacol Toxicol 2008;102:24556.

    20 Lipsitch M, Tchetgen TE, Cohen T. Negative controls: atool for detecting confounding and bias in observationalstudies.Epidemiology 2010;21:38388.

    21 Guxens M, Ballester F, Espada M et al. Cohort Profile: theINMA-INfancia y Medio Ambiente(Environment andChildhood) Project. Int J Epidemiol 2012;41:93040.

    22 Chatzi L, Plana E, Daraki V et al. Metabolic syndrome inearly pregnancy and risk of preterm birth. Am J Epidemiol2009;170:82936.

    23 Koutra K, Chatzi L, Roumeliotaki T et al. Socio-demographic determinants of infant neurodevelopmentat 18 months of age: Mother-Child Cohort (RheaStudy) in Crete, Greece. Infant Behav Dev 2012;35:4859.

    24 Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant Development. 2nd edn. SantAntonio TX: Psychological Corporation, 1993.

    25 Bayley N.Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development. 3rdedn. Sant Antonio TX: Psychological Corporation, 2005.

    26 Rasmussen KM, Yaktine AL. Weight Gain During Pregnancy:Reexamining the Guidelines. Washington, DC: Institute ofMedicine and National Research Council of the NationalAcademies, 2009.

    27 de Onis M, Garza C, Onyango AW, Rolland-Cachera MF.Les standards de croissance de LOrganisation mondialede la sante pour les nourrissons et les jeunes enfants[WHO growth standards for infants and young children].Arch Pediatr 2009;16:4753.

    28 Instruction manual BIO-RAD. 25(OH)-vitamin D3 byHPLC. Munchen, Germany: Laboratories GmbH, 2003.

    29 Goni F, Lopez R, Etxeandia A, Millan E, Amiano P. Highthroughput method for the determination of organochlor-ine pesticides and polychlorinated biphenyls in humanserum. J Chromatogr B Analyt Technol Biomed Life Sci

    2007;852:1521.30 Ramon R, Murcia M, Aguinagalde X et al. Prenatal mer-

    cury exposure in a multicenter cohort study in Spain.Environ Int 2011;37:597604.

    31 Rubin DB. Multiple imputation for non-response in surveys.New York: John Wiley & Sons, 1987.

    32 Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences.2nd edn. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,1988.

    33 Rutter M. Proceeding From Observed Correlation toCausal Inference. Perspect Psychol Sci 2007;2:37795.

    34 Academy of Medical Sciences Working Group. Identifyingthe Environmental Causes of Disease: How Should We DecideWhat to Believe and When to Take Action?London: Academyof Medical Science, 2007; http://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/

    download.php?file/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdf(17 December 2012, date last accessed).

    35 McGrath E, Wypij D, Rappaport LA, Newburger JW,Bellinger DC. Prediction of IQ and achievement at age8 years from neurodevelopmental status at age 1 yearin children with D-transposition of the great arteries.Pediatrics 2004;114:e57276.

    36 Paz-Filho G, Wong ML, Licinio J. The procognitive effectsof leptin in the brain and their clinical implications. Int JClin Pract 2010;64:180812.

    MATERNAL OBESITY AND CHILD NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL DEVELOPMENT 517

    http://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdfhttp://www.acmedsci.ac.uk/download.php?file=/images/publicationDownload/SummaryR.pdf