144
Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere without the permission of the Author.

Brain injury and discrimination : the effects of visibility and … · 2016. 5. 27. · proportion of people with brain injury) may be more likely to experience discrimination. In

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere without the permission of the Author.

  • BRAIN INJURY AND DISCRIMINATION: THE EFFECTS OF VISIBILITY AND PERCEPTIONS OF

    DANGEROUSNESS AND RESPONSIBILITY

    Lynette A Foster BScHon, MSc

    Department of Psychology Massey University

    Wellington New Zealand

    This thesis is submitted as a partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctorate of Clinical Psychology

    28 February 2012

  • ii

    TABLE OF CONTENTS

    List of Tables ............................................................................................................... iv

    List of Figures ............................................................................................................... v

    Abstract ........................................................................................................................ vi

    Candidates Declaration ............................................................................................ viii

    Acknowledgements....................................................................................................... ix

    Chapter 1: Introduction .............................................................................................. 1

    Chapter 2: Brain Injury .............................................................................................. 8

    Definition and severity .............................................................................................. 8

    Prevalence and causes ............................................................................................... 9

    Misconceptions about brain injury .......................................................................... 11

    Secondary complications ......................................................................................... 12

    Chapter 3: The Visibility of a Disability .................................................................. 19

    Research on visibility effects .................................................................................. 22

    Chapter 4: Stigma ...................................................................................................... 25

    Stereotypes, prejudice and discrimination .............................................................. 25

    Stigma ...................................................................................................................... 26

    Relationship between stigma, stereotyping and prejudice ................................. 29

    Stigma and brain injury ........................................................................................... 30

    Chapter 5: Danger and Responsibility Models and familiarity and

    knowledge about how to interact ............................................................................... 32

    Familiarity with individuals with brain injury ......................................................... 32

    Lack of knowledge about how to interact ............................................................... 34

    Responsibility model ............................................................................................... 36

    Weiner’s Attribution Theory ............................................................................... 38

    Danger model .......................................................................................................... 41

    Chapter 6: Article One - Do visible scars influence people’s predictions of

    other people’s willingness to socialise with an adolescent with brain injury? ..... 44

    Abstract ................................................................................................................... 46

    Introduction ............................................................................................................. 47

    Visible signs of disability .................................................................................... 48

    Familiarity with brain injury .............................................................................. 49

  • iii

    The current research ........................................................................................... 50

    Method ..................................................................................................................... 51

    Results ..................................................................................................................... 55

    Discussion ............................................................................................................... 58

    Appendix A ............................................................................................................. 63

    References ............................................................................................................... 64

    Chapter 7: Article Two - Brain injury and discrimination: Two competing

    models – perceptions of responsibility and dangerousness ..................................... 67

    Abstract ................................................................................................................... 69

    Introduction ............................................................................................................. 70

    Responsibility model ........................................................................................... 71

    Danger model ..................................................................................................... 73

    Populations ......................................................................................................... 74

    Visibility .............................................................................................................. 75

    Other factors which may influence willingness to interact with survivors

    of brain injury ..................................................................................................... 75

    The current research ........................................................................................... 77

    Study one ................................................................................................................. 78

    Study two ................................................................................................................. 84

    Discussion ............................................................................................................... 95

    Appendix A ........................................................................................................... 103

    References .............................................................................................................. 105

    Chapter 8: Overall Discussion ................................................................................ 109

    Article One ............................................................................................................ 109

    Article Two ............................................................................................................ 110

    Discussion and clinical implications ..................................................................... 112

    Limitations and future research ............................................................................. 116

    Conclusion ............................................................................................................. 118

    Appendix A – Photos ............................................................................................. 119

    References ................................................................................................................. 122

  • iv

    LIST OF TABLES

    Table 1. Study one conditions 79

    Table 2. Study two conditions 85

    Table 3. Pearson’s correlation matrix for the Danger Model 88 Table 4. Pearson’s correlation matrix for Responsible Model 92

  • v

    LIST OF FIGURES

    Figure 1. Weiner’s attribution model showing a direct relationship 38 between responsibility and willingness to assist and the

    relationship mediated by anger and pity

    Figure 2. Danger model showing a direct relationship 42 between perceptions of dangerousness and willingness to assist, and fear mediating this relationship

    Article One Figure 1. Two way interaction between Participant (male and female)

    and Adolescent (male and female). Standard error bars are also shown. 57

    Figure 2. Two way interaction between Photo (scar and no scar) and

    Participant (male and female). Standard error bars are also shown. 57

    Article Two Figure 1a. Fear mediating the relationship between perceptions of 89

    dangerousness and willingness to socialise Figure 1b. Moderated mediation - fear mediating the relationship 90

    between perceptions of dangerousness and willingness to socialise and Photo (scar/no scar) moderating the mediated relationship

    Figure 2a. Pity and anger mediating the relationship between 93

    responsibility and willingness to socialise Figure 2b. Moderated mediation – pity and anger mediating the 95

    relationship between perceptions of responsibility and willingness to socialise and Photo (scar/no scar) moderating the mediated relationship

  • vi

    ABSTRACT The aim of the research outlined in the following pages was to examine the

    impact that the visible signs of brain injury and perceptions of dangerousness and

    responsibility have on participants’ willingness to socialise with adolescent survivors of

    brain injury. The research has two articles, and Article Two has two studies.

    In Article One, participants were shown a picture of either an adolescent male or

    female, with or without a head scar, with a brief vignette advising that the adolescent

    had sustained a brain injury. Participants reported that others would be more willing to

    socialise with the adolescents with a scar, than the adolescents with no scar, and female

    participants reported that others would be more willing to socialise with the female

    adolescent, than the male adolescent.

    Article Two used a similar paradigm to Article One. Study one of Article Two

    replicated Article One and added an additional factor, knowledge about how to interact,

    as a factor influencing willingness to socialise. Results showed that participants with

    more knowledge about how to interact with survivors were more willing to socialise,

    than participants with less knowledge. Study two examined whether describing the

    adolescents as dangerous or responsible for contributing to, or causing their brain

    injury, would influence willingness to socialise. Results showed strong support for a

    danger model, where perceptions of dangerousness were mediated by fear. When the

    adolescents’ were described as dangerous, fear increased and subsequent willingness to

    socialise decreased.

    To a lesser extent, support was found for a responsibility model. Perceptions of

    being responsible (falling and sustaining a significant injury to the head after drinking

    too much alcohol) increased anger but anger in turn did not impact willingness to

  • vii

    socialise. Being described as not responsible (as a result of a brain tumour) increased

    pity, but again there was no relationship between pity and willingness to socialise.

    This information is useful for rehabilitation professionals assisting adolescents’

    re-integration back into the community post injury. Informing survivors that people’s

    attitudes may change once visible signs of injury heal may be relevant when managing

    expectations of how others may treat them. It may also be useful to discuss how others

    may perceive them when they have contributed to causing their current condition or are

    perceived as dangerous. Finally, knowledge about how to interact may be useful for

    policy makers when designing campaigns to reduce discrimination.

  • viii

    Candidate’s Declaration

    I, Lynette Foster, candidate for the degree of Doctor of Clinical Psychology at Massey University Wellington, do hereby certify that:

    1. The papers and thesis contained herein comprise entirely my original work towards the degree,

    2. This work has not been submitted to any other university or institution for a higher degree,

    3. The thesis including papers is less than 65,000 words in length, excluding tables, references and appendices,

    4. Ethics approval for the research was obtained from: Study One - Massey University Ethics Committee: Southern B, Application 10/62. Study Two - Massey University Ethics Committee: Southern B, Application 11/13.

    ........................................................................................

    Lynette A Foster

  • ix

    Acknowledgements

    I would like to thank my primary supervisor Professor Janet Leathem for her

    support and guidance while completing my thesis and my second supervisor Dr Steve

    Humphries for his statistical advice. Their knowledge and support was much

    appreciated and needed throughout this time. I would also like to thank Associate

    Professor Paul Jose for his assistance with structural equation modelling and for

    listening to my attempts to understand structural equation modelling.

    On a personal note, I am very appreciative of the support, assistance and

    nurturance from my partner, Neil de Wit, and two children, Jessica and Katie. They

    were very encouraging during this time and acknowledged and celebrated my

    accomplishments and listened to my many challenges and stories throughout my

    journey. I would also like to acknowledge and thank my friends for listening and being

    there for me, particularly Robyn Burton and Fran McDonald who listened to my newly

    learnt theories and ideas! Finally, my journey was made all the more enjoyable as a result of the amazing support and nurturance from fellow clinical students, especially Lucia Munoz Larroa,

    Lauren Callow and Veena Sothieson. Also I would like to acknowledge Dr Maree Hunt

    for her tireless assistance and support when starting this journey.

    Contribution of author to project: The author was responsible for most aspects of this research, including the formulation of research questions and data collection in Wellington, data analysis and interpretation and write up of the papers and thesis.

  • 1

    CHAPTER 1

    INTRODUCTION The ideas for this research evolved from research that I had conducted at

    Masters level. The focus of that research had been to examine the effect that visible

    markers of brain injury had on the attributions made by the general public of an

    adolescent’s undesirable behaviour. Results showed that when the brain injury was

    visible, people attributed the behaviours equally to brain injury and adolescence. In

    contrast, when the injury was not visible, people attributed the behaviours more to

    adolescence (Foster, 2010; McClure, Buchanan, McDowall, & Wade, 2008; McClure,

    Devlin, McDowall, & Wade, 2006). These results suggest that more consideration was

    given to the brain injury as the cause of the undesirable behaviour when visible signs of

    brain injury were present, than when they were not.

    Thinking about the implications of McClure et al.’s. (2008; 2006) findings and

    my Masters research and extensive reading in the areas of attribution theory, stigma and

    brain injury, raised questions about whether the visibility of a brain injury would

    influence discriminatory behaviours. Specifically, would the visibility of a brain injury

    influence people’s willingness to socialise with adolescents who had sustained a brain

    injury. If people were more likely to excuse behaviour when a survivor of brain injury

    had a visible marker of injury (McClure, et al., 2008), then perhaps they would be more

    likely to socialise with or assist a survivor when a visible marker of injury was present.

    By comparison, survivors without a visible marker (and this applies to a large

    proportion of people with brain injury) may be more likely to experience discrimination.

    In support of this contention, Zahn (1973) found that young people with disabilities had

    better interpersonal relationships when their disability was visible than when it was not.

  • 2

    Willingness to socialise and visibility effects have not been specifically studied

    in terms of brain injury before, although the work of Linden and colleagues has gone

    some way towards this. Linden and colleagues found that there was more willingness to

    help and less prejudicial attitudes when adult survivors of brain injury had acquired

    their injury through organic or external means (e.g., brain haemorrhage) than when it

    was self-initiated or sustained by internal means (e.g., started a fight after drinking

    alcohol) (Redpath & Linden, 2004), and when the injury was not their fault (Linden,

    Hanna, & Redpath, 2007).

    While these studies go some way towards the examination of stigma and brain

    injury, they do not examine the impact visible signs of brain injury have on attitudes.

    Furthermore, they do not identify how ‘blame’ or perceptions of responsibility lead to

    prejudicial attitudes and helping behaviour. Is there a direct relationship between the

    responsibility for a brain injury and attitudes, or is this relationship mediated or

    moderated by other factors? Examining mediating and moderating factors is essential

    when attempting to understand the processes of discrimination and the current research

    will address this, as well as the impact the visibility of a brain injury has on people’s

    willingness to socialise with survivors.

    Related to this are findings from the disability literature associating the visible

    markers of disability with more prejudice and discrimination than when there are no

    visible markers of disability (Latner, Stunkard, & Wilson, 2005; Matthews &

    Harrington, 2000). However, typically, the literature examines physical disabilities and

    prejudice and discrimination. Considerable research examining prejudice,

    discrimination and stigma has also been conducted in the mental health field and it

    seemed appropriate to examine brain injury in a similar manner. There is considerable

    rationale for this. Some people believe that brain injury is associated with madness and

  • 3

    altered personality (Simpson, Mohr, & Redman, 2000) and others describe survivors of

    brain injury as mentally disabled (Linden & Boylan, 2010). Furthermore, complications

    resulting from a brain injury can include mania, post traumatic stress disorder (Kim et

    al., 2007) and depression (Tsaousides, Ashman, & Seter, 2008). Finally, aggression is

    associated with brain injury (Dooley, Anderson, Hemphill, & Ohan, 2008; Kim, et al.)

    and mental illness (Hiday, 1997), and both conditions are potentially hidden illnesses

    which may only become apparent to others during interactions.

    Research examining mental illness and stigma has often employed Weiner’s

    attribution model when attempting to identify the processes involved in stigmatisation

    of individuals with mental illness. Weiner’s (1995) causal attribution theory proposes

    that individuals make cognitive appraisals regarding the controllability of a person’s

    stigmatising condition and try to identify whether the person was responsible or not for

    causing their condition. A strong belief regarding responsibility elicits emotional

    responses such as anger or pity, which in turn influence behavioural responses

    (willingness to help or interact). For example, when the cause of a mental illness is self-

    inflicted (illegal drug use) reported beliefs about responsibility are higher, than when

    the cause is accidental or organic (as a result of an accident) (P. W. Corrigan,

    Markowitz, Watson, Rowan, & Kubiak, 2003). Higher beliefs about responsibility are

    associated with increased feelings of anger and less willingness to assist and lower

    beliefs about responsibility are associated with increased pity and more willingness to

    assist (Weiner, 1995).

    Support for Weiner’s attribution theory is found in the mental illness literature

    which shows that responsibility for causing an illness impacts emotions, and emotions

    subsequently influence behaviour and attitudes (P. W. Corrigan et al., 2002; Martin,

  • 4

    Pescosolido, & Tuch, 2000; Reisenzein, 1986; Weiner, Perry, & Magnusson, 1988).

    The current research employs Weiner’s model within a brain injury perspective.

    There are a number of factors which potentially influence the willingness of the

    general public to interact or assist survivors of brain injury. A danger model has been

    developed to explain aspects of stigma in the mental illness literature (P. W. Corrigan,

    2000; P. W. Corrigan, et al., 2003; P. W. Corrigan, et al., 2002). According to this

    model, when an individual with a mental illness is perceived as dangerous, fear

    increases and people are more likely to withhold assistance and avoid encounters, than

    when they are not perceived as dangerous (P. W. Corrigan, et al., 2003). The danger

    model may be relevant when examining brain injury and discrimination as research

    shows that students fear survivors of brain injury more than the general public does,

    describing survivors as violent, unpredictable and aggressive (Linden & Crothers,

    2006). Qualitative research also found that the public reported that survivors of brain

    injury would have difficulties with aggression, frustration, and irritation and emotion

    regulation (Linden & Boylan, 2010). Based on these findings the current research

    examined perceptions of dangerousness as a factor influencing willingness to socialise

    with survivors of brain injury.

    Other factors which may influence the willingness of the general public to

    interact with survivors of brain injury are: 1. people’s level of familiarity with

    individuals with brain injury, and 2. knowledge about how to interact with them.

    Albrecht, Walker and Levy (1982) found that this uncertainty about how to interact

    influenced people’s willingness to socialise with individuals with stigmatising

    conditions (conditions ranging from diabetes and cancer to mental illness and drug

    abuse). In preparing for the current research only one study was found that examined

    familiarity with individuals with brain injury and discrimination; where it was found

  • 5

    that people’s familiarity (measured by asking a yes/no question) had no impact on

    prejudice and discriminatory attitudes (Linden, et al., 2007). However, the mental

    illness literature shows considerable support for a familiarity hypothesis. People with

    higher levels of contact or familiarity with individuals with mental illness report less

    stigma, relative to people with low levels of contact (P. W. Corrigan, Edwards, Green,

    Diwan, & Penn, 2001; P. W. Corrigan, Green, Lundin, Kubiak, & Penn, 2001; P. W.

    Corrigan, et al., 2003). Accordingly it seemed important to also consider the amount of

    familiarity (based on the amount of individual contact with survivors of brain injury)

    and knowledge about how to interact as factors influencing willingness to socialise with

    survivors of brain injury as part of the planned research.

    Finally, there is a lack of research examining willingness to socialise with

    adolescents who have sustained brain injuries. Adolescence is a time of self doubt and

    some adolescents fear being judged about how they dress, behave or talk, and are

    preoccupied with differences between themselves and others (Miller & Sammons,

    1999). The effects of navigating adolescence may be exacerbated when an adolescent

    sustains a brain injury. Accordingly, identifying factors which may impact this group is

    an important step towards developing strategies to assist with rehabilitation.

    In summary, there is little research examining brain injury and discrimination

    and none found examining how and under what conditions adolescent survivors of brain

    injury experience discrimination. For example, do perceptions of dangerousness and

    responsibility influence stigmatising attitudes by impacting emotional responses (fear,

    anger and pity)? Furthermore, there is a lack of research exploring the effects that the

    visible signs of brain injury have on the willingness of other people to socialise with

    adolescent survivors of brain injury, and the effect the visibility of an injury has on

    perceptions of dangerousness and responsibility. Research examining these processes is

  • 6

    essential as it may assist in identifying factors that are important for successful

    community integration post brain injury. This, then, became the focus of the current

    research, which will be reported as two articles.

    Article One examined whether visible markers of brain injury influenced

    people’s predictions of the extent to which other people would be willing to socialise

    with adolescent survivors of brain injury. The effect of the gender of the observer

    (participants) and adolescent with brain injury were also examined. Familiarity with

    individuals with brain injury was examined as it was expected that participants’ level of

    familiarity would influence their predictions of others’ willingness to socialise. Article

    One asked participants their opinion about other people’s willingness to socialise to

    minimise social desirability effects as the author collected data in public locations.

    Article One builds on previous research in two ways. It examined the impact the

    visibility of a brain injury had and the impact this has for adolescents, as opposed to

    adults.

    Article Two has two studies. Study one replicated Article One and contributed to

    the literature by including a lack of knowledge about how to interact as a factor

    influencing discriminatory behaviour. Study two examined the effect visible markers of

    brain injury have on people’s willingness to socialise with survivors of brain injury

    employing Weiner’s full responsibility model and a danger model. Pity and anger were

    examined as mediators between the relationship of personal responsibility for a brain

    injury and willingness to socialise. Visible markers of brain injury were examined as

    moderators of this relationship. Fear was examined as a mediator in the relationship

    between perceptions of dangerousness and willingness to interact, as research shows

    that people fear individuals with brain injury (Linden & Boylan, 2010; Linden &

  • 7

    Crothers, 2006). Again, visible markers of brain injury were examined as moderators of

    this relationship.

    Article Two built on and extended previous research in three ways. It examined

    mediating (pity and anger) and moderating (visible signs of injury) variables in the

    responsibility model. It examined a danger model in relation to brain injury for the first

    time, and included visibility effects as a moderator of this relationship. Finally, it

    examined a lack of knowledge about how to interact as a factor influencing willingness

    to socialise.

    Chapter two provides definitions of brain injury and traumatic brain injury (TBI)

    and information about prevalence and causes. Chapter two also includes common

    misconceptions about brain injuries, secondary complications that may arise following a

    brain injury and community re-integration aspects of rehabilitation. Chapter three

    discusses visible and invisible disabilities and related research. Chapter four focuses on

    stigma and provides definitions for stereotyping, prejudice, discrimination and stigma

    and how these concepts are related in relationship to brain injury. Chapter five discusses

    other factors (perceptions about responsibility and dangerousness, familiarity and

    knowledge about how to interact) which may influence discriminatory responses. These

    chapters are followed by Article One and Article Two chapters and an overall

    discussion chapter.

  • 8

    CHAPTER 2

    BRAIN INJURY

    Definition and severity

    Brain injuries are common and are caused by a variety of factors such as

    infections, brain tumours, drugs, alcohol, hypoxia (lack of oxygen to the brain) and

    solvents (Brain Injury New Zealand, 2011; Kelly, Brown, Todd, & Kremer, 2008). A

    traumatic brain injury (TBI) is caused by an external trauma to the brain by either an

    object penetrating the skull (open head injury) or a blunt force trauma to the head with

    no penetration (closed head injury) (Lucas & Addeo, 2006). Road traffic accidents,

    falls, sporting accidents, assaults and being hit by something are common causes of TBI

    (Lucas & Addeo; McKinlay et al., 2008). The cause of a TBI varies as does the

    subsequent severity of the injury.

    There are a number of ways to ascertain the severity of a TBI including the time

    frame of posttraumatic amnesia (PTA: memory of events immediately following the

    trauma), Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) and loss of consciousness (LOC) (Lucas &

    Addeo, 2006). When PTA is less than 1 hour the injury is rated as mild, 1 to 24 hours is

    rated as moderate and when PTA exceeds 24 hours the injury is rated as severe (Lucas

    & Addeo). The resulting classification system, mild, moderate or severe typically

    corresponds with the level of recovery, with longer PTA associated with poorer

    recovery (Lucas & Addeo).

    The GCS measures best verbal response, eye-opening and motor responses

    (Teasdale & Jennett, 1974). Scores range from 3 to 15 points, with 13 points and above

    indicating mild TBI, 9 to 12 points indicating moderate TBI and 8 or less indicating

    severe injury. As well as indicating the severity of an injury, the GCS also indicates

    likely prognosis; the higher the score the better the prognosis.

  • 9

    A TBI is classified as mild when LOC is 30 minutes or less and moderate to

    severe TBI when LOC exceeds 30 minutes. The three measures of TBI severity

    discussed above overlap with regard to assessing deficits and the measure selected is

    based on the patient population, health professionals skill set and specific setting (i.e.,

    whether it is in a doctor’s office, hospital or at the scene of the accident) (J. D. Corrigan,

    Selassie, & Orman, 2010).

    Prevalence and causes

    Traumatic brain injury is quoted as one of the leading causes of disability and

    death for children, young adults and the elderly in the United States, where there are

    more than 5.3 million people living with a TBI related disability (Centers for Disease

    Control and Prevention, 2011). In New Zealand 90 people sustain a TBI every day, and

    24,000 are diagnosed as having experienced concussion (classified as a mild TBI) every

    year (Brain Injury New Zealand, 2011). The implications of these statistics are rarely

    fully understood in terms of the financial cost to society and the subsequent cognitive,

    emotional, sensory and motor impairments which impact survivors’ recovery and

    quality of life. Furthermore, unlike physical disabilities, which are mostly visible to

    others, TBI often has no outward sign of disability making it difficult for others to

    notice and accommodate subsequent deficits and difficulties experienced by survivors.

    In the United States 506 per 100,000 people sustain a TBI each year and 269 per

    100,000 in Finland (see review of epidemiology by J. D. Corrigan, et al. (2010)). The

    figure is considerably higher in New Zealand with 1750 per 100,000 people sustaining a

    TBI each year (McKinlay, et al., 2008). The reported statistics vary as a result of

    methodological differences. The type of data used for analysis, methods of calculating

    and the source of the data vary among researchers (J. D. Corrigan, et al.). Data may be

    obtained only from hospital admissions or include accident and emergency and doctors

  • 10

    visits. Research using self-report measures find higher incidences but may suffer from

    inaccuracies due to memory and reporting deficits (Body & Leathem, 1996). Research

    reporting lower incident rates of 100-300 per 100,000 per annum (Cassidy et al., 2004)

    and 269-506 per 100,000 (J. D. Corrigan, et al.) included only hospital admissions or

    visits whereas McKinlay et al.’s research included all medical assistance sought for

    TBI. McKinlay et al. found that 30% of all cases were admitted to hospital and the

    majority were seen by general practitioners or accident and emergency departments. As

    a result of the varying methodology practices it is difficult to accurately estimate actual

    prevalence; nonetheless, TBI is considered a significant health problem worldwide.

    The New Zealand statistics reported above were from a Christchurch cohort

    study involving 1265 participants. This study found that 31.6% of the population

    suffered at least one TBI that required medical assistance by the time they were 25 years

    of age (McKinlay, et al., 2008). The highest rates of TBI were in the 15 to 20 age group

    and males had higher rates (38%) than females (24%) (McKinlay, et al.). These gender

    and age statistics with males, adolescents and young adults being more likely to suffer a

    TBI than females and older adults (excluding the elderly) are similar to overseas studies,

    e.g., Cassidy et al. (2004); J. D. Corrigan et al. (2010). The elderly are another at risk

    group, with several studies showing that after approximately 70 years of age the

    incidents rates of TBI increase with advancing age due to falls and motor vehicle

    accidents (Burns & Hauser, 2003).

    Falls are reported as the most common source of injury up to 14 years of age in

    New Zealand and after 15 years of age rugby, assault, motor vehicle accidents and being

    hit with an object are reported as the most common causes of injury (McKinlay, et al.,

    2008). Other countries (United States, Australia and France) report similar statistics,

  • 11

    with road traffic accidents and falls accounting for the majority of TBI (Burns &

    Hauser, 2003; Cassidy, et al., 2004; J. D. Corrigan, et al., 2010).

    In summary, TBI is one of the leading causes of disability and death worldwide

    with road traffic accidents and falls accounting for the majority of injuries. Adolescents

    and young adults are one of the most at risk groups with males twice as likely as

    females to sustain a TBI. Therefore, research is required to identify appropriate

    rehabilitation and education strategies to assist this high risk group, with an emphasis on

    males. Furthermore, the “invisible” nature of TBI needs to be considered when planning

    research to understand the impact visible and invisible injuries have on recovery.

    Finally, the high prevalence rates and the lack of outward signs of disability have

    resulted in TBI being described as the “invisible epidemic” (Centers for Disease Control

    and Prevention, 2011). Brain injuries are classified as a brain injury or a TBI dependent

    upon the cause of the injury; however, from this point forward, for ease of reading, all

    injuries will be referred to as a brain injury, except when discussing prevalence and

    cause statistics.

    Misconceptions about brain injury

    A brain injury can be a debilitating condition impacting all aspects of a

    survivor’s life. Survivors must focus not only on their recovery but also contend with

    misconceptions and beliefs held by the public and others about the recovery process and

    outcomes of brain injury. The public, family members of survivors of brain injury and

    health professionals hold a number of misconceptions about brain injury which may

    impact attention and care considerations. The public believe that brain injuries heal as

    quickly as physical injuries, and once survivors receive treatment and are discharged

    from hospital they are physically and mentally well (Swift & Wilson, 2001). Further,

    the public believe that the recovery from a brain injury is primarily influenced by the

  • 12

    amount of effort expended by a survivor and that complete recovery is possible in most

    instances (Gouvier, Prestholdt, & Warner, 1988; Hux, Schram, & Goeken, 2006).

    There is a lack of awareness of the time frames involved in recovery and a lack

    of understanding that further injury is more likely due to survivors’ vulnerability (as a

    result of cognitive or physical difficulties (Chamberlain, 2006)). Furthermore, the public

    and non-expert health professionals fail to appreciate that the ability to perform

    common tasks varies from day to day as a result of fatigue and sometimes undesirable

    behaviours resulting from a brain injury persist (Swift & Wilson, 2001). These

    misconceptions may impede survivors’ recovery efforts. As a result of these common

    misconceptions about injury, which are held in Britain (Chapman & Hudson, 2010) and

    the United States (Hux, et al., 2006), inferences of ‘malingering’ are also implied by the

    public and health professionals (Chamberlain).

    It is possible that misconceptions about brain injury endure and ‘malingering’ is

    implied as a result of the ‘invisibility’ of brain injuries. Even though people are aware

    of a person’s brain injury, there may be no ongoing salient mark of injury to remind

    them of it (as with physical disabilities). Finally, common misconceptions, inferences of

    malingering and a lack of visible signs of brain injury may result in survivors receiving

    insufficient care and attention when seeking treatment and assistance with community

    re-integration. The current research examines community re-integration and how some

    common attitudes and beliefs held by the public may impact survivors’ recovery efforts.

    Secondary complications

    A number of potential deficits and problems can develop post injury. They may

    develop as a result of brain damage sustained directly from the brain injury and/or

    develop as survivors learn to live with a brain injury and contemplate their life post

    injury and the effects the injury will have on their life. Survivors may experience

  • 13

    memory, attention and executive functioning problems (Draper & Ponsford, 2008) as

    well as behavioural and personality difficulties (Willer, Johnson, Rempel, & Linn,

    1993). Furthermore, depression (Tsaousides, et al., 2008), post traumatic stress disorder

    and mania (Kim, et al., 2007) may develop as a result of the trauma experienced or

    difficulties adjusting to subsequent disabilities. Severe brain injury may result in the

    development of epilepsy which may occur months or years following the initial injury

    (Lowenstein, 2009). Survivors may experience social difficulties and have trouble

    obtaining employment post injury; they may be dependent on others and have

    difficulties with community re-integration (Brooks, Campsie, Symington, Beattie, &

    McKinlay, 1987; Kelly, et al., 2008; Sloan, Winkler, & Anson, 2007; Winkler,

    Unsworth, & Sloan, 2006). The following sections discuss these factors and others

    which may impact survivors’ community re-integration efforts post injury.

    Cognitive Outcomes

    The subsequent type and level of cognitive impairment following a brain injury

    varies depending upon the severity of the injury, the location of injury and the time

    since injury (Dikmen, Machamer, Winn, & Temkin, 1995; Draper & Ponsford, 2008;

    Stratton & Gregory, 1994). Cognitive impairments may include difficulty with

    attention, cognitive flexibility, concentration, memory, orientation, language, executive

    functioning, motor functioning and self-awareness (Dikmen, et al., 1995; Millis et al.,

    2001). Impairments may improve over time, deteriorate further or not change at all, with

    memory, attention, executive functioning and processing speed being more likely to

    remain impaired five to ten years post injury (Draper & Ponsford; Millis, et al.).

    Children and adolescents can experience similar cognitive impairments as

    discussed above post injury, although the severity of the injury does not always

    correlate with the severity of subsequent impairments (as can be the case for adults as

  • 14

    well) (Conklin, Salorio, & Slomine, 2008; Middleton, 2001; Wassenberg, Max,

    Lindgren, & Schatz, 2004). A number of factors such as premorbid functioning, age and

    the developmental level of the child influence subsequent problems experienced

    (Middleton). Suggestions that plasticity may facilitate normal development in children

    is not always supported as areas in the brain that are currently developing rapidly may

    be more vulnerable to damage, whereas areas that are already fully developed may incur

    a temporary loss and then recover (Middleton). For these reasons, it is essential

    professionals do not underestimate the effect mild injuries can have on children

    (Middleton). Finally, children and adolescents may appear highly intelligent but exhibit

    poor executive functioning skills, such as meta-representation, monitoring system

    difficulties, lack of insight, attention, organisation and orientation which influence their

    ability to plan and following through on goals (Middleton).

    Emotional and behavioural outcomes

    Common behavioural and emotional manifestations following brain injury for

    adults include verbal and physical aggression, inappropriate social behaviour (e.g.,

    social awkwardness, non-compliance, behaving in embarrassing ways, excessive

    talking, bossiness, withdrawal, intrusive or prying, attention seeking and lighting fires),

    lack of initiative, perseveration and repetitive behaviour and inappropriate sexual

    behaviour (predominantly sexual talk) (Brooks, et al., 1987; Kelly, et al., 2008). Both

    Brookes et al. and Kelly et al.’s research involved survivors with moderate to severe

    brain injuries and Kelly found that the most common difficulties reported were verbal

    aggression, inappropriate social behaviour and lack of initiative.

    Further, research with adult survivors of moderate to severe injuries supports the

    aggression findings (Baguley, Cooper, & Felmingham, 2006) and shows that survivors

    report more verbal aggression than uninjured individuals, and people with spinal cord

  • 15

    injuries (Dyer, Bell, McCann, & Rauch, 2006). An interesting finding in Dyer et al.’s

    research was that there was no difference in physical aggression between the three

    groups examined (brain injury, no injury and spinal cord injury). This in turn, suggests

    that survivors of brain injury are no more physically aggressive than the general public,

    a distinction that may not be recognised by the public.

    For children and adolescents, it is often behavioural problems that trigger a

    referral for treatment (Middleton, 2001). Social disinhibition is commonly reported by

    others, which may be a result of reduced insight into the needs and feelings of others

    and inflexibility regarding play and activities (Middleton). Also adolescents and

    children (aged 7 to 15 years) may not be aware that their subsequent deficits impact

    their social functioning (Jacobs, 1993). This may arise as the children and adolescents

    have not been sufficiently informed about the relationship between their injury and

    subsequent behavioural difficulties (Middleton). Increased aggression, destructiveness,

    impulsivity and overactivity are common problems reported by parents post injury

    (McGuire & Rothenberg, 1986). Additional research supports the finding that children

    and adolescent survivors experience increased aggression post injury (as reported by

    parents) (Cole et al., 2008; Dooley, et al., 2008).

    Psychosocial outcomes and community re-integration

    Assisting survivors’ return to the community following a brain injury is an

    essential component of rehabilitation and has been identified as a primary goal for

    survivors (Sander, Clark, & Pappadis, 2010). The emphasis is typically on ‘full

    community integration’ which involves education, vocational, recreational, social and

    community activities (Sander, et al.). The challenges involved in this goal are

    considerable, particularly considering the possible subsequent difficulties and deficits

    post injury. In support of this contention, research showed that the severity of an injury

  • 16

    and subsequent disability and challenging behaviour (loss of emotional control,

    disinhibition, impulsivity, frequent mood changes and irritability) predicted low

    community integration (Sloan, et al., 2007; Winkler, et al., 2006). Furthermore over

    time paid care facilitating active engagement reduces or ceases and gratuitous care

    increases, leaving the responsibility to family and friends (Sloan, et al.).

    Obtaining employment post injury is one of the aspects of community

    integration that survivors have difficulty with. Kelly et al. (2008) found that 80% of

    adult survivors (N = 190) with moderate injuries were unemployed. High

    unemployment is reported in a number of studies, and chronic unemployment

    contributes to the development of low self esteem and depression (Morton & Wehman,

    1995). A lack of employment reduces the chances of social and leisure activities often

    available when working. Another potential change for survivors is a reduction or

    cessation of leisure activities such as sports and membership of clubs due to physical,

    emotional or behavioural changes. Again, this reduces the chances for socialisation and

    peer support.

    A lack of employment as well as behavioural, cognitive and emotional

    difficulties impact current relationships and impede the development of new

    relationships (Morton & Wehman, 1995). Existing friendships and networks may

    dissolve or reduce significantly increasing reliance on family members for social and

    emotional support. Physical difficulties (wheelchair use, difficulties in eating and

    personal care) may also reduce chances for socialisation and increase reliance on family

    members and prompt realisations about lost independence. The social isolation and

    dependence on family members impacts self esteem and increases the likelihood of

    depression (Morton & Wehman). A lack of socialisation also reduces the chances for re-

    learning and practicing social skills with peers (Morton & Wehman).

  • 17

    Socialising with peers is especially poignant for the group most at risk of

    sustaining a brain injury (15-24 years), as this is a time when they are separating from

    parents and family and joining others with similar outlooks and developing their own

    identity, which is intricately tied up with peer acceptance and support (Erikson, 1970;

    Miller & Sammons, 1999). As a result of subsequent behavioural, emotional and

    physical difficulties children and adolescents often lose friends and have difficulty

    developing new friendships (Middleton, 2001).

    The potential number and range of subsequent changes and difficulties post

    injury understandably impact on survivors’ attempts to re-integrate back into

    communities and friendships. Although anger management and training are often

    included in rehabilitation strategies, there is a lack of suitable courses or resources to

    assist with other prominent difficulties, such as inappropriate social behaviour, which

    impact on re-integration (Kelly, et al., 2008). To understand the effects aggression has

    on survivors’ community re-integration efforts, the current research examines the

    impact perceptions of dangerousness have on people’s willingness to interact with

    adolescent survivors of brain injury.

    In conclusion, brain injuries occur frequently for young people aged 15-24 at a

    time in their life when they are separating from their parents and family and trying to fit

    in with peers. Being accepted by others is crucial as is appearance and how others

    perceive them. Understanding the public’s attitudes towards this group provides useful

    information when assisting re-integrating back into society. For adolescents, as for all

    survivors of brain injury, the subsequent cognitive, emotional, behavioural and social

    outcomes post injury may result in undesirable or negative attitudes from others. This

    creates an unhelpful cycle where undesirable sequelae contribute to a lack of friendships

    and socialisation, impacting well being. Furthermore, understanding how visible signs

  • 18

    of injury (in this instance a scar) influence attitudes is particularly relevant for

    adolescents who may perceive a scar to be an added disadvantage, as it distinguishes

    them as different from others. The following chapter will discuss visible disabilities and

    the impact the visibility has on attitudes and outcomes.

  • 19

    CHAPTER 3

    THE VISIBILITY OF A DISABILITY

    Whether a disability is visible or not greatly influences the way a person is

    viewed or defined by others (Crocker, Major, & Steele, 1998). An absence of outward

    signs of disability implies an absence of disability and accordingly the individual is

    placed into categories based on gender, culture, life experiences, expectations and

    stereotypes (Miller & Sammons, 1999). Outward signs of disability, such as mobility , a

    missing limb, speech impediment or facial disfiguration prompt a disabled

    categorisation, which in turn influences expectations, i.e., people generally expect less

    from individuals categorised as disabled than they do for the able bodied (Stone, 2005).

    A common misconception about survivors of brain injury is that you can tell that

    they have had a brain injury (Linden & Boylan, 2010). This may occur as we are taught

    that we can easily distinguish who belongs in which category (disabled or absence of

    disability) by visual or auditory cues (Stone, 2005). However, these perceptions are

    often unfounded as it is estimated that 40% of people with disabilities have no visible

    sign of disability (Asch, 1984). Whether these individuals experience discrimination

    once people become aware of their disability, and if so whether they experience

    discrimination in the same way or level as individuals with visible disabilities, is

    unclear. The current research addresses this distinction.

    Several studies have reported the detrimental social outcomes experienced by

    people with disabilities. Examples include discrimination in obtaining and maintaining

    employment relative to able bodied persons. Ravaud, Madiot, and Ville (1992), for

    example, sent unsolicited job applications to over 2000 French companies describing a

    person with or without a disability, and with high or moderate qualifications. The highly

    qualified able bodied applicant was 1.78 times more likely to receive a favourable

  • 20

    response than the highly qualified disabled applicant, and the moderately qualified able

    bodied applicants were 3.2 times more likely to receive a favourable response than the

    moderately qualified disabled applicant.

    Once people with disabilities obtain employment they report more overt and

    subtle discrimination as well as less procedural justice and lower levels of job

    satisfaction than able bodied employees (Snyder, Carmichael, Blackwell, Cleveland, &

    Thornton, 2010). Furthermore, non-physically disabled employees report more subtle

    and procedural discrimination and less perceived organisational support than employees

    with physical disabilities (Snyder, et al.). This last finding may highlight a distinction

    between visible and non visible disabilities, although it is difficult to infer this as Snyder

    et al. did not mention the specific disability of the non-physically disabled.

    Discrimination is also reported by students in higher education environments.

    Students with physical disabilities report feeling marginalised, different and distanced

    from other students as a result of waiting outside emergency exits for entry and sitting at

    the front of the class (Holloway, 2001). Further research shows that students with

    mobility disabilities have less choice about which university or course they select, due

    to a lack of access and the necessity to remain close to medical facilities and family

    support (Hadjikakou, Polycarpou, & Hadjilia, 2010).

    When a person has no visible signs of disability, discrimination is less likely to

    occur, unless the disability is revealed. Individuals with non visible disabilities may be

    reluctant to disclose their disability as disclosure may prompt unwanted sympathy,

    judgements about believability and impact existing relationships with others (Matthews

    (1994) as cited in Matthews & Harrington, 2000). Furthermore, disclosure may result in

    others being less inclined to accommodate the disability, as the person may look healthy

    and happy (McClure, 2011). Nondisclosure of a disability is also used for impression

  • 21

    management whereby individuals with non visible disabilities want to be seen “only” as

    a person (Matthews & Harrington, 2000). Concealing the disability also eliminates the

    possibility of being labelled disabled, thereby maintaining a “normal status” between

    the parties involved (Matthews & Harrington). As a result of possible consequences and

    the associated discrimination, individuals with non visible disabilities spend

    considerable time and effort concealing their disability from others (Matthews, (1994)

    as cited in Matthews & Harrington).

    Therefore, from a discrimination perspective, in some instances having a non

    visible disability may be considered advantageous. However, there is a downside. The

    effort involved in concealing non visible disabilities may produce secondary effects

    such as increased anxiety due to the effort involved and worry about being discovered

    (Matthews & Harrington, 2000). Furthermore, people with non visible disabilities

    experience more emotional problems than people with visible disabilities (Ireys, Gross,

    Werthamer-Larsson, & Kolodner, 1994).

    Brain injury is a disability which may or may not result in visible signs of

    neurological impairment. There may be noticeable thought, speech, motor or facial

    impairments or there may be no outward signs of impairment at all. In some instances

    survivors may initially have visible signs of brain injury in the form of bandages or

    scars but over time these disappear, leaving the survivor with no outward sign of brain

    injury, in spite of there often being significant non-visible injury, e.g., impairment of

    cognition and interpersonal function. Road traffic accidents and assaults are two

    common causes of TBI which may result in physical injuries (McKinlay, et al., 2008).

    Initially, the injury may be obvious to others (bandages, scars); however, these signs of

    injury may disappear or heal over time, leaving the survivor with no obvious outward

  • 22

    sign of disability. Brain tumours or haemorrhages are another type of brain injury that

    may produce similar effects.

    To assist with effective rehabilitation it is essential to understand the public’s

    likely response to survivors of brain injury both with and without outward visible signs

    of disability. This information is particularly relevant when assisting adolescents’

    rehabilitation following a brain injury as adolescence is a time of self doubt about

    appearance, looks and how they talk (Miller & Sammons, 1999), and this group (15-20

    year olds) has one of the highest incident rates of TBI (McKinlay, et al., 2008). The

    current research explores these issues and examines whether the public’s willingness to

    socialise varies dependent upon whether an adolescent survivor has a visible sign of

    injury or no visible sign of injury.

    Research on visibility effects

    McClure and colleagues examined visibility effects in relation to brain injury

    and attributions. They found that when a male adolescent survivor of brain injury had

    no visible signs of injury participants attributed his undesirable behaviours more to his

    adolescence than his brain injury (McClure, et al., 2008; McClure, et al., 2006). In

    contrast, when he had a visible sign of injury (head scar) participants attributed his

    undesirable behaviours equally to his adolescence and brain injury. This finding

    suggests that more consideration was given to the brain injury as the cause of the

    undesirable behaviours when a visible sign of injury was present. McClure and

    colleagues’ research examined the impact of visible signs of injury on attributions, but

    there is a lack of research examining visible signs of brain injury and discrimination.

    The current research considers this by examining the extent to which the visible

    sign of brain injury (in this instance a scar) impacts on the willingness of other people to

    socialise with an adolescent survivor of brain injury. The scar was chosen to represent a

  • 23

    visible sign of brain injury for a number of reasons. Road traffic accidents, falls and

    assaults are three common causes of TBI for adolescents (McKinlay, et al., 2008), often

    resulting in visible signs of injury to the head or face which may or may not disappear

    over time. Similarly, brain tumours may result in a scar as a result of surgical

    interventions which may later heal. A wheelchair was considered as an alternative

    outward sign of injury but not chosen as this was considered to be more representative

    of a physical injury as opposed to a brain injury. Facial asymmetry, speech and motor

    impairments were not chosen as they would have been difficult to photograph and they

    did not necessarily represent a visible sign that would disappear or heal over time.

    Finally, portraying adolescents who had actually suffered a brain injury was not

    considered due to ethical concerns. Members of the public may have recognised the

    adolescent but were not aware of their injury, thereby potentially exposing the

    adolescent to stigma. If the adolescent had no visible sign of brain injury, then that

    would have to be imposed on their photo, which may be distressing for the adolescent.

    Conversely, we would have to digitally alter adolescents’ photos that had facial scars

    which may cause distress as the photo without scars may remind the adolescent about

    their appearance prior to their injury.

    In conclusion, people with visible and non visible disabilities face a number of

    issues and difficulties but these factors vary depending on the visibility of their

    disability. There is considerable literature examining physical disabilities, mental

    illness, brain injury and discrimination but no research was found simultaneously

    examining brain injury, visibility effects, adolescents and discrimination. The current

    research therefore examines the effect the visibility of a brain injury has on people’s

    willingness to socialise with adolescents who have sustained a brain injury. The

    following chapter discusses stigma and related factors as the stigma concept provides a

  • 24

    conceptual understanding of the visibility distinction when related to managing

    disclosure and outcomes (discrimination).

  • 25

    CHAPTER 4

    STIGMA

    People with disabilities routinely experience discrimination relative to people

    without disability even when there is no outward suggestion that they deserve less

    opportunity (with regard to employment or renting a house), support or friendship

    (Miller, Parker, & Gillinson, 2004). Social psychology provides a conceptual

    framework when examining stigma and the related concepts of stereotyping, prejudice

    and discrimination (Crocker, et al., 1998; Hamilton & Sherman, 1994). These concepts

    and their inter-relatedness are discussed in the following sections.

    Stereotypes, prejudice and discrimination

    Stereotypes are collectively agreed upon ideas or beliefs that are held about

    social groups that can be quickly accessed (Hamilton & Sherman, 1994). Stereotyping

    occurs when labels link a person to particular characteristics of a social group

    (Goffman, 1963). Stereotypes are also described as cognitive schemas (Hamilton &

    Sherman) which influence encoding, storing and retrieval of group-based information

    (Biernat & Dovidio, 2000). Being aware of stereotypes is not necessarily negative; it is

    only when a stereotype is agreed to and an emotional response developed toward it that

    prejudice is implied (Biernat & Dovidio).

    Prejudice, then, is a possible outcome of stereotyping and has been described as

    “an aversive or hostile attitude toward a person who belongs to a group, simply because

    he belongs to that group, and is therefore presumed to have the objectionable qualities

    ascribed to the group” (Allport, 1958, p. 7). Prejudice, therefore, has an evaluative

    component incorporating a cognitive (determining stereotype) and affective response

    (danger, fear) to beliefs (stereotypes). Discrimination flows on from prejudice and is the

  • 26

    behavioural response to prejudicial attitudes (e.g., not employing or renting a house to a

    person as a result of prejudiced attitudes) (Link & Phelan, 2001).

    Stigma

    Stigma is a more complex construct which may include all or some aspects of

    the processes discussed above at any one time. Based on the constructs discussed above,

    stigma occurs when labels (e.g., mentally ill) link an individual to stereotypical beliefs

    which prompt prejudiced attitudes, which are then acted upon (discrimination).

    However, there are more aspects related to the stigma construct than labeling,

    stereotyping, prejudice and discrimination. Additional aspects include different types

    and dimensions of stigma and factors influencing the development of stigma. The

    following paragraphs discuss some well known definitions of stigma and discuss

    different types of, and dimensions to, stigma.

    Stigma was defined by Goffman several decades ago as “an attribute that links a

    person to an undesirable stereotype, leading other people to reduce the bearer from a

    whole and usual person to a tainted, discounted one” (Goffman, 1963, p. 11). Although

    this definition is still relevant today, it does not include the consequences of being

    stigmatised and the context and process of stigma. For instance, it is suggested that

    stigma occurs when a power imbalance exists, that is, when more powerful groups have

    the power to stigmatise other less powerful groups (Link & Phelan, 2001), and when

    someone possesses an attribute that is devalued in one social context (Crocker, et al.,

    1998). These additional factors suggest that stigma is a social construction which

    changes dependent upon culture and power balances (Dovidio, Major, & Crocker,

    2000).

    When considering these additional factors, stigma can be defined as a social

    construction; a distinguishing mark causes devaluation by others which occurs when a

  • 27

    power imbalance allows labeling, stereotyping, exclusion, status loss and discrimination

    (Dovidio, et al., 2000; Link & Phelan, 2001; Link & Phelan, 2006). This last definition

    conveys a fairly succinct description of stigma but it is important to bear in mind that

    not all components described are required at any one time for stigmatisation to occur

    and in the literature any of the components are interchangeable with the term stigma.

    Finally, the rationale behind the stigma concept is that when others are labeled, creating

    a “them and us” situation, and linked to stereotypes (which are typically undesirable

    attributes) a justification exists for discriminatory behaviour (Link & Phelan, 2006).

    A number of different types and dimensions of stigma will be discussed below.

    Goffman (1963) differentiated between visible and invisible stigmatising conditions

    using the terms discredited and discreditable stigmas. A person with a visible stigma is

    stigmatised and discredited by others based on their visible sign of difference (ethnicity,

    disfiguration). A person with no visible sign of stigma is discreditable but not yet

    discredited. In these circumstances illness or disability can only be inferred based on

    other signals such as labels, behaviours or psychiatric symptoms. Individuals with

    invisible conditions face a dilemma when deciding whether or not to disclose their

    condition as there are advantages and disadvantages either way.

    Goffman (1963) further classified stigma according to condition; tribal

    identities, blemishes of individual character and abominations of the body. Tribal

    stigmas are common stereotypes or group-based inferences about ethnicity, gender and

    age that are constantly activated as a result of repeated exposure and are therefore

    quickly accessed and retrieved (Biernat & Dovidio, 2000). Furthermore, tribal stigmas

    are consensually held and culturally transmitted within groups (Biernat & Dovidio).

    Individual character blemishes may involve similar processes as tribal stigma and

    include stigmatising conditions such as mental illness, where common stereotypical

  • 28

    beliefs include notions that people with mental illness are inferior and require coercive

    handling, also that they need to be cared for and are socially threatening (Angermeyer &

    Matschinger, 2004; Biernat & Dovidio; Holmes, Corrigan, Williams, Canar, & Kubiak,

    1999).

    Abominations of the body stigmas (disfigurement, some medical conditions,

    cerebral palsy) are generally less common and therefore not as readily activated and

    may invoke an emotional response first, as opposed to stereotypical knowledge, due to

    the visible nature of the condition (Biernat & Dovidio, 2000). Tribal and abominations

    of the body stigmas are typically visible stigmas whereas individual character stigmas

    can be visible or non visible.

    Jones et al. (1984) identified six dimensions of stigma, these include;

    “concealability” (the visibility of the condition), “course of the mark” (will the

    condition become more salient or deteriorate over time), “disruptiveness” (level of

    impact on relationships), “aesthetics” (physical manifestation of stigma), “origin”

    (cause of condition) and “peril” (perceived danger to others). Concealability, aesthetics

    and disruptiveness are closely related to Goffman’s (1963) concepts of visibility,

    abominations and individual character blemishes, respectively. The cause of a

    stigmatising condition and perceived danger are concepts that have been well

    researched with regards to the impact they have on discriminatory and prejudiced

    attitudes.

    Researchers argue that the most important aspects of stigma are the visibility

    and controllability or cause of a stigmatising condition (Crocker, et al., 1998). Visible

    stigmas activate schemas about stigmatising conditions which influence attitudes

    (Crocker, et al.) and perceived controllability has been shown to influence attitudes

    (Weiner, et al., 1988).

  • 29

    Finally, three types of stigma have been described in the literature; public

    stigma, self stigma and courtesy stigma. Public stigma refers to stereotypical, prejudiced

    and discriminatory practices directed by the general public towards the stigmatised (P.

    W. Corrigan & Wassel, 2008). Self stigma develops when stigmatised individuals

    internalise public stigma and apply the stereotype personally (P. W. Corrigan &

    Wassel). Goffman (1963) also coined the term “courtesy stigma”, where individuals

    who are associated with stigmatised individuals experience stigma. While these three

    types of stigma clearly identify groups impacted by stigma, the term ‘stigma’ in itself is

    a stigmatising term and several researchers suggest using discrimination instead. Using

    the term discrimination places the focus of research on the perpetrators of

    discrimination and changing factors associated with this, as opposed to focusing on

    changing factors associated with the recipients of stigma (Link & Phelan, 2001; Sayce,

    1998). The brief descriptions discussed above show that stigma, stereotyping and

    prejudice are closely related constructs. To further understand their connectedness,

    related aspects of stigma, stereotyping and prejudice are discussed.

    Relationship between stigma, stereotyping and prejudice

    Stigma and stereotyping are related as stereotyping and stigma often co-occur

    but in some instances stigmatisation occurs without prior stereotyping (Biernat &

    Dovidio, 2000). For example, tribal and character blemish stigmas are more likely to

    invoke strong stereotypical beliefs and subsequent stigmas associated with these groups

    but abominations of the body (e.g., disfigurements) are more likely to produce affective

    reactions initially which impact stigma avoidance directly without necessarily invoking

    stereotypical knowledge (Biernat & Dovidio).

    Stigma and prejudice are related as stigma (discrediting mark) most likely leads

    to prejudice (cognitive and affective response) but stigma is more encompassing and

  • 30

    includes additional factors such as character and identity attributions and individual and

    group reactions to the stigmatised (Dovidio, et al., 2000). Also, stigma is socially

    defined which infers that stigmatising “marks” vary over time and culture (Dovidio, et

    al.). Some researchers suggest that stigma and prejudice are similar constructs and that

    the difference in definitions results primarily from the populations studied (Stuber,

    Meyer, & Link, 2008). A review of 18 models of prejudice and stigma found support

    for this claim (Phelan, Link, & Dovidio, 2008). The review found that stigma

    researchers focus on individuals who are stigmatised as a result of “unusual” conditions

    (mental illness, facial disfigurement and HIV) as well as the stress resulting from the

    stigma or stress in maintaining vigilance (Stuber, et al.). Prejudice researchers focus on

    class, race, age and gender divisions, and structural forms of discrimination (Stuber, et

    al.). Finally, it is suggested that the term stigma can replace a number of the constructs

    discussed (labeling, stereotyping, prejudice and discrimination) and the processes can

    occur in varying degrees (Link & Phelan, 2001).

    Stigma and brain injury

    Some groups of people may be subjected to multiple forms of stigma. For

    instance, an adolescent with brain injury may be stigmatised due to their age (tribal),

    stigmatised as a result of their brain injury (blemish of character) and/or stigmatised as a

    result of facial disfigurement (abomination). Survivors’ of brain injury may internalise

    public stigma and develop self stigma, resulting in them believing stereotypical

    information about their injury or illness, impacting recovery. Furthermore, survivors’ of

    brain injury may experience stigma in different ways depending on whether they have

    visible markers of injury or not, and whether they choose to disclose or not disclose

    their injury.

  • 31

    When identifying who might experience different types of stigmatising

    conditions (tribal, character blemishes and abominations of the body), it is possible that

    when a scar or facial disfigurement is present an emotional response is the first reaction

    (abominations of the body), but when there is no physical sign of stigma, stereotypical

    or common beliefs are invoked (character blemish). The current research explores this

    distinction to ascertain whether the public’s willingness to socialise with adolescent

    survivors of brain injury varies dependent upon whether their injury is visible (facial

    disfiguration; abomination stigma) or non visible (character blemish stigma).

    In conclusion, the current research explores public stigma and focuses on the

    effect the visibility of a brain injury (character blemish and abomination stigmas) has on

    the willingness of others to socialise with adolescents who have sustained a brain injury.

    The cause of the brain injury is also examined as perceptions of responsibility may

    influence people’s willingness to socialise with survivors and perceptions of

    dangerousness are examined as this may also influence others willingness to socialise.

    The following chapter discusses danger and responsibility models of stigma and other

    factors that may impact on people’s willingness to socialise.

  • 32

    CHAPTER 5

    DANGER AND RESPONSIBILITY MODELS

    AND FAMILIARITY AND KNOWLEDGE ABOUT HOW TO

    INTERACT

    There are potentially a multitude of factors which may influence peoples’

    willingness to socialise with adolescent survivors of brain injury. Previous chapters

    discussed visibility, stereotyping, prejudice, stigma and the gender of the adolescent

    survivor as possible contributing factors. The current chapter will consider other factors

    purported to influence discrimination. The degree of injury or resulting disability,

    ethnicity, education, socio economic status, whether an individual has an authoritarian

    /coercion-segregation attitude and even the time of day may all influence discriminatory

    responses. However, the current research is focusing on people’s level of familiarity or

    contact with individuals’ with brain injury, causal attributions regarding the cause of a

    brain injury, perceptions of dangerousness and a lack of knowledge about how to

    interact with survivors of brain injury.

    These factors were selected as they were shown to have influenced

    discriminatory behaviours in relation to mental illness and/or brain injury or were

    discussed as possible reasons why people would be reluctant to socialise with

    individuals with disabilities (lack of knowledge about how to interact). These concepts

    are discussed below.

    Familiarity with individuals with brain injury

    Familiarity with brain injury can be defined as the amount of knowledge

    someone has about brain injury and/or the amount of contact they have had with

    individuals with brain injury (Holmes, et al., 1999). Familiarity with individuals with

    mental illness has been examined using the level-of-contact report developed by

  • 33

    Holmes et al. The measure includes 12 items describing varying levels of personal

    contact such as: “I have watched a movie or television show in which a character

    depicted a person with mental illness”, “a friend of the family has a severe mental

    illness” and, “I have a severe mental illness” (Holmes, et al., p. 450). Respondents

    endorse statements they have experienced and the highest level of contact is used as the

    final indicator of degree of level of contact.

    Using the level-of-contact report, the mental illness literature finds that people

    who have experienced more contact with individuals with mental illness are less likely

    to engage in stereotyping and discrimination (P. W. Corrigan, et al., 2003). Further

    research exploring the public’s perceptions of adults with mental illness found that

    greater familiarity with individuals with mental illness resulted in lower perceptions of

    dangerous (P. W. Corrigan, Green, et al., 2001). A similar pattern occurred with regard

    to endorsing prejudicial attitudes (authoritarian and benevolence) (P. W. Corrigan,

    Edwards, et al., 2001). However, there was an exception to these findings; adolescents

    who had more contact with individuals with mental illness were more likely to endorse

    dangerous perceptions and stigmatising attitudes (P. W. Corrigan et al., 2005).

    Furthermore adolescents who were more familiar with peers with mental illness were

    more likely to view them as more dangerous than adolescents with less familiarity.

    Relative to the mental illness literature there is little research examining

    familiarity with individuals with brain injury and the impact the familiarity has on

    discriminatory behaviour. The only study found that specifically examined familiarity,

    brain injury and discrimination reported that participants’ contact with individuals with

    brain injury had no effect on prejudicial and discriminatory attitudes (Linden, et al.,

    2007). However Linden et al.’s research measured familiarity using a yes/no question

    with limited statistical power (P. W. Corrigan, Green, et al., 2001).

  • 34

    Related research examining brain injury, familiarity, visible signs of injury and

    attributions found that familiarity with brain injury did influence attributions. When

    shown a photo of an adolescent male with a head scar who was described as sustaining

    a brain injury, participants with more familiarity attributed his undesirable behaviours

    (angers quickly, lacks motivation and self confidence and sleeps a lot) more to his brain

    injury than his adolescence, than people with less familiarity, who attributed the

    behaviours more to his adolescence (Foster, 2010). Furthermore, when the adolescent

    was depicted with no scar there was no difference in attributions between the groups,

    suggesting that the scar acted as a moderator of the relationship between familiarity and

    attributions (Foster). The familiarity measure used in Foster’s study differed from that

    used by Linden et al. (2007) (who used a categorical measure asking participants if they

    had had contact with people with brain injury – a yes or no response). Foster used a

    modified version of a nine-point level-of-contact scale developed by Holmes et al.

    (1999), which was then converted into high and low familiarity.

    Lack of knowledge about how to interact

    Another factor which may influence people’s willingness to socialise with

    adolescent survivors of brain injury is a lack of knowledge about how to interact with

    survivors. This topic has received little attention in the mental illness and brain injury

    literature. One study, examining stigma and pharmacy students’ beliefs about

    individuals with schizophrenia from six countries, found that perceptions that people

    with schizophrenia were “difficult to talk to” was one of three factors, along with

    unpredictability and perceptions of dangerous that predicted higher social distance (Bell

    et al., 2010). Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer, and Rowlands (2000) found similar results

    when examining mental disorders and the public’s attitudes. Participants felt it was

    difficult to talk to people with any of the disorders described (drug and alcohol

  • 35

    addiction, eating disorder, panic attacks, severe depression, schizophrenia and

    dementia), and described people with alcohol and drug addiction and schizophrenia as

    dangerous and unpredictable.

    Albrecht, Walker, and Levy (1982) found that being responsible for a

    stigmatising condition (conditions ranging from diabetes and cancer to mental illness

    and drug abuse) did not explain the willingness, or lack of willingness to socially

    interact. To understand why being responsible did not account for this, they asked

    participants why they thought other people did not want to socialise with individuals

    with stigmatising conditions. Ambiguity in the social interaction was cited; a lack of

    knowledge about how to interact or respond to people with stigmatising conditions and

    also apprehension about things they did not understand and new things. Also, threats to

    social and physical well being, physical offensiveness, moral weakness and feelings of

    guilt for being healthy were cited.

    Albrecht et al. (1982) used open-ended questions in the third person to minimise

    social desirability effects and to negate the need to explain responses on a forced choice

    Likert scale or something similar. Questions included: “Some people do not like to be

    around a person who is physically disabled (or who has a social disability such as

    alcoholism or some other addiction). Why do you think they feel this way?” Their

    sample consisted of 150 managers enrolled in further education. The sample was all

    white college graduates, mostly married and urban dwellers. This limited sample may

    explain the differences found in the responsibility results which contradict P. W.

    Corrigan et al. (2003) and others research.

    As identified by Albrecht et al. (1982) some people are unsure about how to

    respond or act in the presence of people with disabilities or mental illness and others

    believe that it is difficult to talk to them (Bell, et al., 2010). Perhaps they were raised in

  • 36

    a time when people with disabilities were in special classrooms, schools or were

    institutionalised (Miller & Sammons, 1999). Perhaps they are scared of saying the

    wrong thing or looking in the wrong place or using the wrong language (Miller, et al.,

    2004). Further, the brain’s automatic response to differences, whether it is disability,

    appearance, illness or potential threat can prompt uncertainty, anxiety or avoidance

    behaviours (Miller & Sammons). Automatic reactions were essential early survival

    skills, however these, a lack of socialisation and being told “not to stare” by parents and

    others may all contribute to feelings of discomfort about differences and stop people

    engaging with people with disabilities.

    We learn about how to respond to differences by watching others, and new

    encounters with individuals with differences facilitate new learning and a broadening of

    comfort zones. However, only recently have institutions become outmoded in some

    countries, resulting in people with disabilities and mental illnesses living in the

    community. New encounters that are unfamiliar prompt comparison with expected

    norms or ideals as we try to understand and accommodate differences (Miller &

    Sammons, 1999). Some people may have had little contact with individuals with

    disabilities and mental illness; therefore, it is not surprising that they may be anxious

    and uncertain about how to respond to differences. The current research will explore

    this concept by examining knowledge about how to interact as a factor influencing

    willingness to socialise.

    Responsibility Model

    Causal Attributions; effects of being responsible or not responsible for causing a brain

    injury

    Attribution theory was identified as a suitable theoretical model to examine

    causal attributions and discriminatory attitudes towards survivors of brain injury. It was

  • 37

    chosen because it views people as active thinkers who evaluate events by looking for

    the cause of a situation and then use this causal information, which may be incomplete,

    in whatever manner they deem appropriate (Jones et al., 1972). Furthermore, attribution

    theories specifically examine the rules or processes people use when they evaluate the

    cause of events or behaviour (Jones, et al.). Therefore, attribution theory examines

    causal attributions (responsible or not responsible) and mediating factors which may

    influence that relationship. Mediating variables potentially explain how one variable

    leads to another variable (Baron & Kenny, 1986). For instance, it would be possible to

    determine whether there is a direct relationship between responsibility and willingness

    to socialise or w