Brain-2005-Hubel y Wiesel SUPER

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/30/2019 Brain-2005-Hubel y Wiesel SUPER

    1/4

  • 7/30/2019 Brain-2005-Hubel y Wiesel SUPER

    2/4

    brilliance; and with that kind of intuitive brilliance fancy

    methodology was unnecessary. Before Hubel and Wieselstarted their work, most believed that the visual cortex

    would be activated by light, as such. Failure to achieve results

    with diffuse light stimulation was a trigger for the construction

    of more sophisticated apparatus. In Germany, Richard Jung

    and his colleagues spent much time and effort constructing

    elaborate machines to activate the cells with diffuse light.

    Jung was later to recount that he might have discovered the

    orientation-selective cells during his 5 years work on the

    visual cortex if he had used a stick instead of the quantifying

    machine. Hubel and Wiesel had themselves used a now

    outmoded approach to stimulate the visual system, when

    that chance insertion of a slide into the projector elicited a

    wild response from a cell. Their concern from then on wasto simplify themachinery andmake the cells a gooddeal easier

    to study.For them,justas importantas stubbornness, ingetting

    results, was almost certainly the simplicity, the looseness, of

    our methods of stimulation. The incredible crudeness of our

    firstslide projectors . . . andour refusal to wastetime bothering

    withmeasuring intensities, rates of movement andso on . . . all

    worked in our favor. The term loose is curiously out of

    place, given how much valuable information was obtained

    with it, and how little from sophisticated stimuli. From then

    on they seemed to have developed a lasting contempt for fancy

    machinery, Hubel once boasting that the best use they could

    make of their computer was to heat the laboratory. How could

    one avoid this disdain when papers, published as late as 1977,

    using sophisticated methodology and advanced computer

    analysis, did not improve much on the precision with which

    the properties of cells were described, and provided no

    new insights whatsoever? How can one admire the detailedand tedious measurement of the orientational preferences of

    cells that arenot orientation-selective, as some have tried to do

    with the directionally selective cells of V5?

    There is perhaps a little overmodesty in the description of

    their work as being hypothesis-free,or what many would today

    describe pejoratively as a sort of fishing expedition. Even if

    true, this was a major fishing expedition with an ineluctable

    and beautifully exploited logic; and the catch was astonishing.

    It is no wonder that every chance was seized avidly. Once

    orientation-selective cells were discovered (by chance) in

    the visual cortex of an animal (the cat) which has two eyes

    and a retinotopically organized primary visual cortex, the rest

    followed easily, or so this collection of papers makes it seem.The succession of questions can be summarized as follows:

    How many orientations are represented within any given

    retinotopically defined position of the visual field? How are

    cells responding to different orientations organized with

    respect to one another? Are all orientations represented for

    each eye? How are the orientation columns and the ocular

    dominance columns organized with respect to each other?

    What is the overall organization of the two sets of columns

    in the context of the topographic map within V1? Given that

    visual deprivation early in life leads to lifelong blindnessa

    topic widely discussedsincethe time of John Locke butput ona

    scientific and clinical basis by the publication of Marius von

    Sendens bookSpace and Sight(1962)it was natural to wantto study the effects of visual deprivation on the specificity of

    visual cells in the cortex. Here, two eyes were better than one,

    since it was possible to deprive one eye, or both, or neither. It

    would,of course, bestretching thingstoo much to claim that the

    decision to work on a two-eyed animal was taken by chance

    but, even if this accident is allowed, they still usedthe two eyes

    in a most imaginative way. Buriedwithin this setof questionsis

    some kind of implicit hypothesis. And the authors were well

    aware, right from the start, that their undertaking was a major

    one and that they were on to some striking findings. Why else,

    astheytellus, wouldthey project to writea book asearly asthe

    1960s, well before their work came to maturity? Why else,

    too, would they have been so aware of potential competitors,jealously guarding their results and projected experiments?

    It is likely that their apprenticeship prepared Hubel and

    Wiesel, if not for the accidents and lucky breaks, then at

    least for how to proceed once the significance of the accident

    had been appreciated. The biographical sketches detail their

    brilliant intellectual surroundings. The catalogue of names is a

    veritable aristocratic roll call: Bernard Katz, William Rushton,

    E. D. Adrian, John Eccles, Alan Hodgkin, Andrew Huxley,

    Edwin Land, Michael Fuortes and Francis Crick. Indeed they

    regard themselves as the scientific great-grandsons of Charles

    David H. Hubel

    Torsten N. Wiesel

    Book review 1227

  • 7/30/2019 Brain-2005-Hubel y Wiesel SUPER

    3/4

    Sherrington, a lineage of which the latter would no doubt

    have approved. No room here for the more modest in ability,

    who might nevertheless come up with an interesting question

    or two. Towering above all in inspiration to them was Steve

    Kuffler, thegrandmaestro of neurobiologyin the1950sand the

    1960s, who had surrounded himself with brilliant investigat-

    ors. Clearly, the atmosphere at Johns Hopkins, and later atHarvard Medical School, was highly congenial and deeply

    inspiring. Clearly, too, the focus was on the problem rather

    than on thoughtless measurement. Hubel recounts how he was

    once advised by Kuffler to stick in some quantification, so as

    to soothe the scientific conscience of the mindless measurers,

    making the paper seem more scientific and therefore better

    suited for publication. He laments that their work would be

    unpublishable today, since referees, under the guise of a

    scientific rigour that is both coarse and imperceptive, would

    demand a host of detailed measurements. In one paper, they

    write of the long time taken to produce a graph using a com-

    puter, adding: We concluded that for both speed and for pre-

    cision it is hard to beat judgments based on the human ear.Certainly [the curves] could not have been obtained with com-

    puter averaging methods before the authors reached the age

    of mandatory retirement. It is difficult to deny that their work

    would have suffered greatly if they had been working in the

    present era of white-coated, rubber-gloved and dispassionate

    scientific honesty, one that, distrustful of human judgement,

    confides all measurements to a computer that supposedly

    does not suffer from human bias. Yet it is also interesting

    and instructive to compare the results that Hubel and Wiesel

    obtained when they judgedthe response specificitiesof cells by

    ear (itself not a negligible measuring device) with the host of

    papers that make generous use of the apparently neutral

    computer-generated orientationselectivity index, the colourselectivity index, and many other indices. After a quarter of a

    century of rigorous application, the latter have revealed noth-

    ing or little that is interesting or new. The authors themselves

    make the point, perhaps unwittingly, in the commentaries that

    follow each paper. Advances there have been, they seem to be

    saying, but nothing to compare with the original discoveries.

    It is, no doubt, this poverty of results, compared to the rich-

    ness of the original findings, that has led DavidHubel to regard

    modern technologies, and the mathematical pitchforks asso-

    ciated with them, with such disdain. The Epilogue of the book

    has him wandering sadly through the debris left by the much-

    vaunted computational approach which, one cannot but agree,

    has contributed little that is of importance to understandinghow the brain works. He disapproves, as indeed do I, of linear

    systems analysis and apparently shares my horror for the

    current craze for multiplexing, the belief that few if any

    cells in the visual brain are highly specialized and that most

    of them aremultipurpose. He hasnothing but praisefor thefact

    that the major textbook of molecular biology, The Molecular

    Biology of the Gene, contains noequationsand that no one has

    tried to fit a protein molecule to a Gabor function. Before

    anyone dismisses these views too hastily, it is worth con-

    sidering how much information the crude techniques used

    by Hubel and Wiesel gave, and how little, by contrast, these

    fancy approaches have provided. The evidence is all there in

    this book.

    How did such a collaboration last for so many years and

    why did it eventually end? The first question is relatively easy

    to answer. One successful set of experiments led to another,

    generating an unstoppable momentum. Both authors wereintoxicated enough by their results to persevere with one

    experiment and then plan and push on with the next. Several

    factors aided them in this. Steve Kuffler was evidently some

    kind of Lorenzo de Medici, content to see brilliant work

    progress in his department, dispensing advice in an avuncular

    way and not anxious, as so many are today, to add his name

    to the papers and thus get hiscut of the glory. The relationships

    established in the laboratory were evidently friendly enough

    for the authors to say nothing more than that they were some-

    times mixed with resentments and varieties of complexes.

    Crucially, both were spared the curse of administration

    one that so many academics, in spite of their protestations,

    love. After one year administering the Physiology Departmentat Harvard, Hubel had had more than he could take when

    the installation of a pencil sharpener necessitated at least

    two elaborate meetings. They were also working at a time

    when cats were even more plentiful than monkeys and not

    much more expensive. Editors were more relaxed and referees

    kinder. Nor were the grant-giving committees quite as for-

    bidding as today. The authors state that their kind of work

    could notbe done today.Sadly,theyare right.Theirlaboratory,

    small in size and shared with others, encouraged continual

    dialogue and discussionone of the characteristics of great

    laboratories such as the Laboratory of Molecular Biology in

    Cambridge, UK, or the Bell Laboratories, USA. One gets the

    impression that there wasalsoa feelingof self-sufficiency. Oneof the authors great strengthsbut in the longer term also a

    weaknesswas their understandable indifference to work

    done by others, perhaps encouraged by Steve Kufflers oft-

    repeated question: Do you want to be a consumer or a produ-

    cer?For example, even in this book, andafterso many years of

    discussion, they still believe, erroneously, that I mapped area

    V5 of theowl monkeyin 1980, thus gettingboth thespeciesand

    the date wrong, though I should be grateful that they got my

    name right! They arenot even aware of where V5 is,describing

    it as lying in the anterior lunate sulcus, whereas it is actually

    located some distance away, in the posterior bank of the

    superior temporal sulcus. This is surprising, since V5 is now

    one of the most studied visual areas after V1 and is comingclose to surpassing the latter. But all this presumably matters

    little to David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel, since the work was

    not undertaken by them! It is said that on one occasion, when

    asked whether he intended to respond to criticisms made of

    theirworkinapaper,Hubelrepliedthat,todoso,hewouldhave

    to read the paper, which was more than he was prepared to do.

    By the time they parted company in 1980, the authors had

    succeeded in charting the anatomy and physiology of area V1

    of the brain in greater detail than ever before, and making it

    the best understood area of the cerebral cortex. They had used

    1228 Book review

  • 7/30/2019 Brain-2005-Hubel y Wiesel SUPER

    4/4

    almost all techniques available to them; charted ocular

    dominance and orientation columns and looked at their ana-

    tomy; established with greater precision than ever before the

    details of how the fibres from the lateral geniculate nucleus

    terminate in the visual cortex; established the concept of the

    column in the visual cortex firmly; shown that selectivities are

    established at birth and are susceptible during a critical periodafter that; and, above all, with their loose methods had intro-

    duced new and very high standards of evidence into cortical

    neurobiology which fewhave managed to emulate. Why, then,

    did this flourishing scientific collaboration end? Their work

    had been mainly on area V1, or area 17, and with so much

    achieved it seemed as if V1 was well enough understood, and

    theworkthusbegantorunoutofsteam.Indeed,asearlyas1968

    they had thought of V1 as sufficiently well characterized that

    despite the large areas still unexplored, in broad outline the

    function of area 17 isprobably nowrelativelywellunderstood.

    It was to areas beyond V1 that one should turn ones attention,

    or sotheyimpliedwhenthey wrotein thesame paper Howthis

    information [in area17] is used at later stages in the visual pathis far from clear, and represents one of the most tantalizing

    problems for the future.

    In his autobiographical sketch, Torsten Wiesel writes with

    disarming honesty that there may have been a more profound

    reason that our partnership came to an end. The additional

    demands on our time [came] when we were investigating

    the properties of cells of higher visual areas beyond the pri-

    mary visual cortex, an exploration that each of us eventually

    regarded as a failure . . . The two naturalists, who for so

    long had journeyed together with a seemingly inexhaustible

    sense of wonder, were unaccustomed to the frustrations that

    are the daily bread of so much scientific research . . . what

    developed between us, our special bond and private dialogues,took place while we carried out our experiments. When these

    explorations stalled, when the wonder faded, so did our col-

    laboration. Perhaps it was necessary for each of us to turn to

    new partners and questions.

    It is indeedthrough the exploration of the highervisual areas

    that their concept of hierarchy began to be questioned.

    That concept was largely the result of concentrating on

    orientation-selective cells and ignoring other categories of

    cell. They managed to identify different levels of complexity

    in thefunctional properties of orientation-selective cells, andit

    is this that led them to the hierarchical concept of visual physi-

    ology. Ironically, it was adherence to thishierarchical doctrine

    that made them misinterpret, or not interpret, the functions of a

    visualareain thecat that isequivalent to area V5 inthe monkey,the ClareBishop area, in an article that they describe as not

    one of our favorite papers. Hubel was later to repudiate this

    exclusive hierarchical concept implicitly, but never explicitly.

    The implicit repudiation came with the work he did with

    Margaret Livingstone, which showed that their earlier model

    of V1 was incorrect; that there was a functional segregation

    within V1,much as hadbeen predictedfrom thedemonstration

    of functional specialization among the higher visual areas of

    the brain, beyond V1, and which receive their input from V1.

    It is a pity that Hubel and Wiesel ignored the concepts of

    functional specialization and parallelism, established by work

    in the higher visual areas. It is a pity, too, that in gazing into the

    future through the past David Hubel (the main author of thecommentaries of this book) has nothing to say about this

    functional specializationperhaps because it does not figure

    in their collaboration. It is, in my view, a major oversight, but

    it is an oversight that one might want to forgive, much as

    one would forgive Einstein for reputedly ignoring quantum

    mechanics. When one has finished rereading, or reading for

    the first time, the extraordinary output that has resulted from

    this legendary collaboration, one decides pretty much to

    forgive such blemishes. Neuroscience should rejoice that,

    during a mere 25 years, its world was enriched not only by

    a wealth of knowledge but also by new standards of evidence

    and elegance of methodology which have left a permanent

    imprint.

    Semir Zeki

    Laboratory of Neurobiology

    University College London

    London, UK

    doi:10.1093/brain/awh507

    Book review 1229