23
Author's personal copy Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults Arielle Borovsky a,b,, Jeffrey L. Elman a,c , Anne Fernald b a Center for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA b Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA c Department of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA article info Article history: Received 25 November 2011 Revised 27 January 2012 Available online 23 May 2012 Keywords: Sentence comprehension Individual differences Language development Eye-tracking Visual-world paradigm Language processing abstract Adults can incrementally combine information from speech with astonishing speed to anticipate future words. Concurrently, a grow- ing body of work suggests that vocabulary ability is crucially related to lexical processing skills in children. However, little is known about this relationship with predictive sentence processing in chil- dren or adults. We explore this question by comparing the degree to which an upcoming sentential theme is anticipated by combining information from a prior agent and action. 48 children, aged of 3 to 10, and 48 college-aged adults’ eye-movements were recorded as they heard a sentence (e.g., The pirate hides the treasure) in which the object referred to one of four images that included an agent- related, action-related and unrelated distractor image. Pictures were rotated so that, across all versions of the study, each picture appeared in all conditions, yielding a completely balanced within- subjects design. Adults and children quickly made use of combina- tory information available at the action to generate anticipatory looks to the target object. Speed of anticipatory fixations did not vary with age. When controlling for age, individuals with higher vocabularies were faster to look to the target than those with lower vocabulary scores. Together, these results support and extend cur- rent views of incremental processing in which adults and children make use of linguistic information to continuously update their mental representation of ongoing language. Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. 0022-0965/$ - see front matter Ó 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.005 Corresponding author at: Center for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive MC 0526, La Jolla, CA 92093-0526, USA. Fax: +1 858 822 5097. E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Borovsky). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417–436 Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect Journal of Experimental Child Psychology journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jecp

Borovsky Elman Fernald-JECP2012

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

hdsjhdjsdjhsjdhsjhdjshdjshds sd s ds s c s cs cs ds fd ef d df df

Citation preview

  • Author's personal copy

    Knowing a lot for ones age: Vocabulary skill and not ageis associated with anticipatory incremental sentenceinterpretation in children and adults

    Arielle Borovsky a,b,, Jeffrey L. Elman a,c, Anne Fernald baCenter for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USAbDepartment of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USAcDepartment of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA

    a r t i c l e i n f o

    Article history:Received 25 November 2011Revised 27 January 2012Available online 23 May 2012

    Keywords:Sentence comprehensionIndividual differencesLanguage developmentEye-trackingVisual-world paradigmLanguage processing

    a b s t r a c t

    Adults can incrementally combine information from speech withastonishing speed to anticipate future words. Concurrently, a grow-ing body of work suggests that vocabulary ability is crucially relatedto lexical processing skills in children. However, little is knownabout this relationship with predictive sentence processing in chil-dren or adults. We explore this question by comparing the degree towhich an upcoming sentential theme is anticipated by combininginformation from a prior agent and action. 48 children, aged of 3to 10, and 48 college-aged adults eye-movements were recordedas they heard a sentence (e.g., The pirate hides the treasure) in whichthe object referred to one of four images that included an agent-related, action-related and unrelated distractor image. Pictureswere rotated so that, across all versions of the study, each pictureappeared in all conditions, yielding a completely balanced within-subjects design. Adults and children quickly made use of combina-tory information available at the action to generate anticipatorylooks to the target object. Speed of anticipatory xations did notvary with age. When controlling for age, individuals with highervocabularies were faster to look to the target than those with lowervocabulary scores. Together, these results support and extend cur-rent views of incremental processing in which adults and childrenmake use of linguistic information to continuously update theirmental representation of ongoing language.

    2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.

    0022-0965/$ - see front matter 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.005

    Corresponding author at: Center for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive MC 0526,La Jolla, CA 92093-0526, USA. Fax: +1 858 822 5097.

    E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Borovsky).

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

    Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

    Journal of Experimental ChildPsychology

    journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate/ jecp

  • Author's personal copy

    Introduction

    One of the challenging aspects of real-time spoken language comprehension is that languagemust be processed incrementally and at a relatively fast speed. The meaning of a sentence evolvesas it unfolds, and sentential meaning cannot generally be inferred from any single word alone. Inlight of these challenges, it has been hypothesized that listeners use a strategy of continually gen-erating expectancies about upcoming referents. Although this hypothesis remains controversial,over the past two decades a growing body of computational evidence suggests that prediction isa powerful mechanism for learning (Elman, 1990; Misyak, Christiansen, & Bruce Tomblin, 2010;Rodriguez, Wiles, & Elman, 1999), and a large number of empirical studies using behavioral andneurophysiological techniques have provided support for expectancy generation in sentence com-prehension in adults.

    Much less is known about the role of predictive processing during childhood. There are strong sug-gestions that prediction may underlie childrens ability to segment continuous streams of auditory in-put into word-sized chunks (Estes, Evans, Alibali, & Saffran, 2007; Saffran, Aslin, & Newport, 1996).Nor is it known what factors might inuence individual childrens abilities to engage in expectancygeneration, although one likely candidate is vocabulary size. For the purposes of this study, we wereparticularly interested in the relationship between vocabulary and predictive processing in sentences.Vocabulary size is highly associated with speed of comprehension in looking tasks (Fernald, Perfors, &Marchman, 2006; Marchman & Fernald, 2008). Moreover, there are a number of proposals in the lan-guage development literature that highlight the relationship between lexical and grammatical devel-opment. These suggest that vocabulary knowledge may serve an important role in language tasks thatrequire meaning and structure to be parsed and interpreted across multiple words, as in online sen-tence comprehension. For example, early growth of the lexicon has been argued to be the critical foun-dation from which basic grammar emerges (Bates & Goodman, 1997; Marchman & Bates, 1994). Thisproposal is supported by many observations that development of the lexicon and early morphosyntac-tic competence is strongly correlated, with lexical development preceding grammatical development.This relationship has been noted in cross-sectional and longitudinal studies (Dale, Dionne, Eley, &Plomin, 2000; Fenson, Dale, Reznick, Bates, & Thal, 1994) and across many languages, includingIcelandic (Thordardottir, Ellis Weismer, & Evans, 2002), Italian (Caselli, Casadio, & Bates, 1999), andHebrew (Maital, Dromi, Abraham, & Bornstein, 2000), in bilingual language acquisition (Marchman,Martnez-Sussmann, & Dale, 2004), and in atypical language development (Moyle, Ellis Weismer,Evans, & Lindstrom, 2007). The idea that sentence processing may be linked to lexical knowledge isalso supported by a number of grammatical theories that rmly root syntactic competence in thelexicon, including Combinatory Categorical Grammar (Steedman & Baldridge, 2006), Head-drivenPhrase Structure Grammar (Pollard & Sag, 1994), and Lexical-Functional Grammar (Bresnan, 2001).Therefore, a central goal of this study was to extend our understanding of the development of antic-ipatory sentence processing mechanisms by directly comparing the online processing of transitivesentence comprehension in 3- to 10-year-old children with that in adults and by testing the degreeto which sentence interpretation and prediction skills are associated with differences in vocabularylevel.

    Much of the evidence for predictive processing in sentential comprehension in adults involvesinferences made from measurement of eye movements in response to language while viewing avisual scene. As objects are mentioned, visual attention is directed toward the spatial location ofthe object in the scene (Cooper, 1974; Tanenhaus, Spivey-Knowlton, Eberhard, & Sedivy, 1995), of-ten before the word is even complete (Allopenna, Magnuson, & Tanenhaus, 1998; Dahan, Magnu-son, & Tanenhaus, 2001). More dramatically, eye gaze may be directed toward objects even beforethey are mentioned (Altmann & Kamide, 1999; Altmann & Mirkovic, 2009; Kamide, Altmann, &Haywood, 2003). Thus, eye gaze appears to capture moment-to-moment changes in the compre-hension of language. Results from numerous studies using this technique (known as the visualworld paradigm [VWP] in the adult literature), and others suggest that adults continuously updatetheir mental representation of ongoing events in the sentence and use cues from many differentsources. These include semantic features (Federmeier & Kutas, 1999), event-level expectations (Bic-

    418 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    knell, Elman, Hare, McRae, & Kutas, 2010; Hald, Steenbeek-Planting, & Hagoort, 2007; Kamideet al., 2003; Metusalem, Kutas, Hare, McRae, & Elman, 2010), prosodic cues (Salverda, Dahan, &McQueen, 2003), phonological information (DeLong, Urbach, & Kutas, 2005; VanBerkum, Brown,Zwitserlood, Kooijman, & Hagoort, 2005), verb tense markers (Altmann & Kamide, 2007), grammat-ical and biological gender (Arnold, Eisenband, Brown-Schmidt, & Trueswell, 2000; Lew-Williams &Fernald, 2010; Tanenhaus, Magnuson, Dahan, & Chambers, 2000; Wicha, Moreno, & Kutas, 2004),verb selectional restrictions (Altmann & Kamide, 1999), pronominal adjectives (Sedivy, Tanenhaus,Chambers, & Carlson, 1999), verb structural biases (Trueswell, Tanenhaus, & Kello, 1993), and ref-erential restrictions (Tanenhaus et al., 1995). Crucially, this updating mechanism is proactive. It notonly reacts and integrates to information it receives but also actively generates expectationsfor plausible sentence continuations.

    A concrete example of this process is illustrated in a series of studies reported by Kamide et al.(2003), which yielded some of the earliest and most convincing support for active prediction duringadult sentential comprehension. In their Study 2, participants viewed simple visual scenes containingmultiple agents and objects with similar affordances, including drinkable objects such as BEER andMILK. Participants then heard sentences such as The man will drink the beer while their eye move-ments to this scene were simultaneously recorded. After hearing the verb drink but before beer, par-ticipants xated on the image of the BEER more than the image of the MILK, indicating that their eyemovements reected more than a simple lexical association between the verb drink and the upcomingobject; rather, it reected predictions motivated by a combination of the prior agent and verb. Appro-priate controls also established that the effect was not the result of lexical associations between manand beer or due to visual saliency of BEER over MILK.

    This result demonstrates that expectancy generation may reect integration from multiple cues. Inthis case, those cues were the agent and the verb in combination. Moreover, the integration draws onreal-world knowledge regarding which objects are likely patients of a verb given the agent carryingthe action denoted by the verb (for related ndings, see also Bicknell et al., 2010; Hare, Jones, Thom-son, Kelly, & McRae, 2009; Matsuki et al., 2011).

    Children also have been found to be able to integrate information from multiple sources to com-prehend sentences in real time. However, children are not always sensitive to the same cues as adults.For example, children under 5 or 6 years of age seem to show difculty in interpreting referential orextralinguistic cues to meaning in a visual scene (Kidd & Bavin, 2005; Snedeker & Trueswell, 2004;Trueswell, Sekerina, Hill, & Logrip, 1999). Despite these limitations, children still do use many sourcesof information with great skill and speed. Both young children and adults can call on their understand-ing of the syntactic and semantic meanings of verbs (Altmann & Kamide, 1999; Fernald, Zangl, Portillo,& Marchman, 2008; Nation, Marshall, & Altmann, 2003), adjectives (Fernald, Thorpe, & Marchman,2010), and article grammatical gender (Lew-Williams & Fernald, 2010) to correctly predict an upcom-ing referent before it is spoken.

    For example, Fernald and colleagues (2008) have shown that children as young as 26 months areable to generate anticipatory looks to an object (e.g., cookie) on hearing a verb Eat the . . ., much likeadults (Altmann & Kamide, 1999). Moreover, electrophysiological and neuroimaging data suggest thatthe neural mechanisms underlying predictive skills such as noting incongruities in labelobject pair-ings are present by the second year of life (Friedrich & Friederici, 2004, 2005; Mills, Coffey-Corina, &Neville, 1997; Travis et al., 2011). Taken together, these discoveries suggest that the skills adults use toswiftly and efciently understand rapidly spoken sentences are in place from the earliest moments oflanguage acquisition.

    As was noted above, it seems that such integration and prediction skills are both a marker and pre-dictor of language skill and growth, especially in relation to vocabulary. Vocabulary skill has an impor-tant relation to processing speed, as children with larger vocabularies are also quicker to comprehendspoken words. Accuracy and speed in online lexical comprehension at 25 months of age correlateswith earlier measures of vocabulary growth between 12 and 24 months (Fernald et al., 2006). Evenmore striking, these early differences in the speed of lexical processing at 25 months have long-termconsequences for language acquisition and are correlated with language outcomes 6 years later(Marchman & Fernald, 2008). Differences in language ability are also associated with more complexmeasures of predictive ability. For example, the speed with which 26-month-olds correctly predict

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 419

  • Author's personal copy

    the upcoming object of a verb in a sentence such as Eat the cookie is also associated with concurrentvocabulary size (Fernald et al., 2008).

    It is well established that dramatic differences in vocabulary size between individuals can beobserved from across the earliest stages of learning during infancy through adulthood (Fensonet al., 1994; Verhaeghen, 2003). Children typically begin to produce their rst words at around theend of their rst year, and vocabulary expands rapidly throughout childhood. However, the timelineand trajectory of this growth varies considerably (Fenson et al., 1994). Whereas some children mayspeak their rst words at 12 months and may know as many as 550 words by 24 months, others mightnot begin to speak for another half year and might know only 50 words by 2 years (Fenson et al.,1994). It should be emphasized that children at both ends of this lexical learning spectrum are stillconsidered within the normal range despite this tremendous initial variability.

    These differences become even more sizable by the time children begin school. For instance,some preliterate 5- or 6-year-olds in rst grade will have an expressive vocabulary of as manyas 5000 words, whereas others may produce only half as many words (Beck & McKeown, 1991).Across the school years, the average student will learn thousands of new words per year, amount-ing to several new words a day (Anglin, 1993; Graves, 1986). However, vocabulary growth inschool is considerably slower for children who begin with smaller vocabularies (White, Graves,& Slater, 1990).

    Finally, vocabulary growth does not stop after childhood, and vocabulary size differences continuethroughout the lifespan (Verhaeghen, 2003). For instance, differences in vocabulary between individ-uals who have attended college and those who have not can be signicant. One study estimated thevocabulary knowledge between adults with a high school education and those with a college degree todiffer by 5000 word families, which is a measurement of word knowledge that counts a root word plusits inected forms and derivations as a single family (Zechmeister, Chronis, Cull, DAnna, & Healy,1995). Variability in vocabulary size is notable even in college studentsa population that has beenat least partially selected by performance on tests of linguistic prociency in entrance exams (Martino& Hoffman, 2002).

    Differences in early vocabulary size have important impacts on later language outcomes andschool achievement. Early vocabulary growth not only is a building block for acquiring grammat-ical skills (Bates & Goodman, 1999; Marchman & Bates, 1994) but also is at least partially relatedto language and reaching achievement outcomes many years later (Cunningham & Stanovich,1997; Marchman & Fernald, 2008). Vocabulary size has also been shown to associate with a num-ber of other linguistic and cognitive abilities in school-age children, including phonological workingmemory (Gathercole & Baddeley, 1989), phonological awareness (Metsala, 1999), and reading com-prehension (Cunningham & Stanovich, 1997; Stahl & Nagy, 2006). A particularly strong relationshipbetween vocabulary level and reading comprehension has been noted since the 1920s (Whipple,1925; see Nation, 2009, for a review). However, early vocabulary size alone is not enough to reli-ably detect which individuals will be most at risk for later language and learning disabilities, asnoted by numerous investigators who have tried, and largely failed, to accurately identify childrenwho will receive later diagnoses of language impairments solely from infant measures of vocabu-lary (Ellis Weismer, 2007; Paul, 1996; Rescorla, 2000; Whitehurst & Fischel, 1994). In sum, vocab-ulary and linguistic processing speed seem to have an important relationship during earlychildhood, although it is likely that the relationship between the two and later outcomes maybe more complex.

    Taken together, these ndings motivate a need for a deeper and more detailed understanding ofchildrens predictive abilities in language comprehension and for a better understanding of the rela-tionship of this ability to other markers of language skill such as vocabulary. The goal of the currentstudy was to address these gaps by asking three specic questions. First, we asked whether the resultsof prior studies that nd evidence for linguistic prediction across one or two words scale up to morecomplex language processing tasks that use more challenging stimuli. Advancing our understanding ofhow words are understood in sentences that require children to calculate more complex multiwordcontingencies is necessary if we are to fully understand how humans can swiftly and efciently under-stand complex and novel multiword utterances, which is arguably one of the dening characteristicsof language (Hockett, 1960). Second, to better understand the developmental trajectory of this skill,

    420 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    we examined the development of this skill across a wide range of ages. In the current study, we in-cluded participants from 310 years of age as well as adults. Finally, because vocabulary appears tobe an important marker of processing speed in young children, we asked whether this relationshipalso exists in performance on this experimental task.

    To answer these questions, we built on the task used by Kamide and colleagues (2003) describedabove. Several modications were made to this task, and extensive norming was conducted to ensurethat both adults and children could comprehend the images and sentences. One important adaptationinvolved the layout of the visual display. Rather than provide visual scenes that included the sententialagents as was done in the original study by Kamide and colleagues, we used a four-alternative forced-choice display in which participants were asked to indicate which image corresponded to the sen-tence-nal target. The purpose of this modication was twofold. First, it served to reduce the visualcomplexity of the visual scenes, thereby increasing the likelihood that we would get clean andmotivated looks to the target items from the children. Second, it allowed us to compare therelative activation and anticipation of candidate target meanings in response to the agent andaction as the sentence unfolds by controlling the relationship of the distractor items to words inthe sentence.

    Each visual scene consisted of the target and three types of distractors: an object that wasassociated with the agent (agent-related), an object that was associated with the action (action-related), and an object that was unassociated with either the agent or action (unrelated). Becausethe target was by denition associated with both the agent and action, this meant that two objectswere associated with the action (target and agent-related pictures) and two objects were associatedwith the agent (target and action-related pictures). Only the target was appropriate given both theagent and action.

    This design was chosen because it also allowed us to study in some detail the potential comprehen-sion strategies that children and adults might employ as they interpreted the sentence in real time.One type of possible strategy would involve a staged elimination of potential sentential targets asthe sentence unfolds. Consider the sample sentence The pirate hides the treasure, with a visual dis-play consisting of TREASURE (target), a (PIRATE) SHIP (agent-related distractor), BONE (action-relateddistractor), and CAT (unrelated distractor). After hearing pirate, we might expect looks to the twoobjects that are associated with the agent (SHIP and TREASURE). When the action (hides) is heard, thismakes one member of the initial cohort (SHIP) unlikely, and one might expect subsequent looksonly to the target (TREASURE). This approach is analogous to the strategy proposed by the Cohortmodel of word recognition (Marslen-Wilson, 1987), in which the beginning of a word activates allwords that are consistent with that beginning; as the rest of the word is spoken, subsequent cueseliminate inconsistent members of the cohort until only one wordthe correct wordremainsactive.

    An alternative strategy is suggested by the TRACE and Merge/Shortlist models of speech perception(McClelland & Elman, 1986; Norris, 1994). In those models, subsequent cues may both eliminatemembers of the initial cohort and activate newwords that are consistent with later cueseven if thosenew words are inconsistent with the initial sound of the word. Thus, as the beginning of a target wordsuch as beaker is heard, listeners might initially activate not only beaker but also words with similaronsets such as beetle and bee. When the second syllable is heard, this might eliminate beetle and bee.But TRACE also predicts that new words that are consistent with that second syllable, such as speaker,will become active. The strategy makes it possible to recover from initial errors in perception or pro-duction and, in fact, seems to provide a better t for the empirical data (Allopenna et al., 1998). In thecase of transitive sentence processing, after hearing the agent and action in The pirate hides . . .example, we might then expect xations not only to the target object (TREASURE) but also to the itemthat is locally consistent with the action (BONE). There is increasing evidence that adults sententialcomprehension may be at least partially inuenced by local coherence effects analogous to thosepredicted by TRACE for word recognition (Kukona, Fang, Aicher, Chen, & Magnuson, 2011; Kuperberg,2007; Tabor, Galantucci, & Richardson, 2004). The extent to which such local strategies areapplicable sentence processing is debated, and whether children use different strategies than adultsis unknown.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 421

  • Author's personal copy

    Method

    Adult participants

    A total of 48 native English-speaking college students (30 women and 18 men) between 18 and28 years of age (M = 21.4 years) took part in this study in return for course credit. An additional 12 par-ticipants took part and received credit but were excluded from analysis: 11 for signicant exposure toother languages during childhood and 1 for receiving prior speech therapy. Participants had normal orcorrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing, and they reported no history of diagnosis or treat-ment for cognitive, attentional, speech, or language issues.

    Child participants

    A total of 48 monolingual English-learning children (23 girls and 25 boys) between 3 and 10 yearsof age (M = 6.2 years) were recruited from families in the surrounding metropolitan region of San Die-go, California, in the southwestern United States. Children either had previously participated in childlanguage research or had answered yers and ads posted in the community. An additional 5 childrenalso participated but were excluded from analysis: 2 for inattentiveness during testing (failing to lookor respond on a majority of trials), 2 for receiving speech therapy, and 1 for being not typicallydeveloping (diagnosis of PDDNOS [pervasive developmental disordernot otherwise specied]). Chil-dren received a toy in return for their participation, and their families also received $10 compensationfor time and travel. For children who were included in analysis, their parents reported them to havenormal hearing and vision and to be primarily hearing English at home. Parental report also indicatedthat all children were typically developing, without signicant birth histories and with no recent orchronic ear infections and no diagnosis or treatment for other language, speech, motor, or cognitiveissues.

    Stimuli

    Eight sets of image/sentence quartets were selected for the study. Sentence quartets were con-structed by crossing two agents with two actions to create four sentences following the design prin-ciples described in the Introduction. Images corresponded to the sentential themes in the quartet (seeFig. 1 for an illustration). For example, one quartet crossed the agents (pirate and dog) with actions(hides and chases) to result in the following four sentences:

    1. The pirate hides the treasure.2. The pirate chases the ship.3. The dog hides the bone.4. The dog chases the cat.

    The target object images for these four sentences would be TREASURE, SHIP, BONE, and CAT,respectively, and participants would see all four images concurrently. For any individual sentence,each image would correspond to one of the four image conditions (target, agent-related,action-related, or unrelated). In a sentence such as The pirate hides the treasure, TREASUREwould be the target image, SHIP would be the agent-related image, BONE would be the action-related image, and CAT would be the unrelated distractor. Across each quartet, each image wouldappear in each condition once, thereby yielding a completely balanced design (see Fig. 1. for fur-ther illustration). Thus, each word and each image serves as its own control across multiple listsin which the role of the word and object changes. In this way, intrinsic differences between sal-ience or attractiveness of words and objects were exactly balanced across lists. Visual imageswere selected to be typical exemplars of target items and were photo-realistic images. Imageswere then edited to t within a 400 400-pixel square, and objects were placed on a lightbackground.

    422 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    Norming

    Prior to the study, we conducted two norming experiments to evaluate whether our stimuli wouldbe familiar to young children. First, to ensure that the images and their labels were known and recog-nizable, we asked 3- and 4-year-olds (who did not participate in the nal experiment) at preschools toselect a named image when it appeared along with the other images in each quartet. The images wererecognized with a very high level of accuracy (>95%) on this task. Second, we normed these sentence/image pairs to make sure that children would have the requisite world knowledge to pick the correcttarget if given only the agent and action in an ofine picture selection task. Preschoolers (34 years ofage) were asked to judge which would be the likely target picture in each image quartet when givenonly the agent and action such as Which one would a pirate chase? or Which one does a dog hide?Performance on this task was also very high (90%). Quartets were not selected for the eye-trackingstudy if any single image in the quartet was not appropriately selected by at least 70% of preschoolerstested. We began the norming process with 12 sentence/image quartets, but 4 did not meet our nor-ming criteria for nal selection in the study, yielding 8 quartets.

    The auditory stimuli were recorded by a female native English speaker (A.B.) in a child-directedvoice and sampled at 44,100 Hz on a single channel. Each sentence was edited using Praat audio

    Fig. 1. Illustration of stimuli and conditions in the experimental task.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 423

  • Author's personal copy

    editing software (Boersma, 2001). First, the onsets of each word (article 1 [Art1], agent, action, article 2[Art2], and target) were marked and the word duration was normalized to the mean duration of allwords in the same sentential position. In cases where actions included a particle such as jump into,the action duration was marked to include the duration of the verb and particle. The mean intensityof all sentences was then normalized to 70 dB. This arrangement yielded 32 sentences of equal length,in which the onset of each word was standardized across all sentences. This was done to ensure thatparticipants were given the same amount of time in each sentence to use the agent and action infor-mation to anticipate the target. The sentential word durations were as follows: Art1, 134 ms; agent,768 ms; action, 626 ms; Art2, 141 ms; target, 630 ms. The nal experimental sentences were thenjudged by several listeners who were unfamiliar with the goals of the study to sound natural. Thesound les were presented to participants via headphones so that the same stimulus was presentedto each ear. The experimental sentences are listed in the Appendix.

    In any one version of the study, participants heard 16 of 32 sentences. Each quartet of four objectswas seen twice, with two of the four possible sentences for each set being presented. Across all ver-sions the position of each object was presented with equal frequency in each quadrant, and in eachversion the target image appeared in each quadrant an equal number of times.

    Procedure

    Experimental taskParticipants were seated in a comfortable chair in front of a 17-inch LCD display, and the

    eye-tracker was focused and calibrated. Stimuli were presented using a PC computer running EyeLinkExperiment Builder software (SR Research, Mississauga, Ontario, Canada). Participants were told thatthey would be seeing pictures and listening to sentences, and they were told to either point to(children) or click on using the mouse (adults) the picture that goes with the sentence. It wasexpected that participants would point to or click on the sentence-nal object, which was the targetof the action, and indeed this is what participants did. They were given one practice trial before start-ing the study. Before the start of the experimental trials, a manual 5-point calibration and validationroutine was performed using a standard black-and-white 20-point bulls-eye image. Before each trial,participants were shown the same centrally located bulls-eye image, which they were instructed toxate on before starting the trial. This dot also served as a drift correction dot before each trial. Onceparticipants had xated on this location, the experimenter began the trial.

    Participants were shown the set of four images for 2000 ms before sentence onset, and the imagesremained on the screen after sentence offset until participants had selected an image from the arraywith the mouse or by pointing. Recalibration of the eye-tracker was performed between trials if nec-essary, although this was rarely needed. Participants were given a break halfway through the study.This portion of the experiment lasted 510 min.

    Eye movement recordingEye movements were recorded using an EyeLink 2000 remote eye-tracker with remote arm cong-

    uration (SR Research) at 500 Hz. Because the physical size of our adult and child participants varied con-siderably, we individually adjusted the position of the display using a remote arm conguration of theeye-tracker such that the display and eye-tracking camera were placed 580620 mm from each partic-ipants face. Head and eye movements were automatically tracked by the eye-tracker system with theuse of a target sticker afxed to the participants forehead. This arrangement allowed for stable trackingevenduring somemovement and shiftingof theparticipants position relative to the camera anddisplay.

    Fixations were recorded in each trial from the onset of the images until the participant or experi-menter clicked on or pointed to the selected picture. The recorded eye movements were automaticallyclassied as saccades, xations, and blinks using the eye-trackers default threshold setting. Ofine,the data were binned into 10-ms intervals, over which subsequent analyses were performed.

    Ofine measurementsAfter the completion of the eye-tracking task, participants were administered two ofine language

    measures: the Peabody Picture Vocabulary TestVersion 4 (PPVT-4; Dunn & Dunn, 2007) and the

    424 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    Sentence Completion subtest of the Comprehensive Assessment of Spoken Language (CASLSC; Car-row-Woolfolk, 1999). The PPVT-4 is a norm-referenced test designed to estimate receptive vocabularyfor standard American English in participants between 2 and 90 years of age. The test contains 220items arranged in order of increasing difculty with age-appropriate starting points. Each item hasfour colorful illustrations arranged in a multiple-choice format. The participants task is to selectthe picture considered to correspond to the best meaning of a stimulus word presented orally bythe examiner. The CASLSC test contains 60 items of increasing difculty with various age-appropriatelevels. In each item, the examiner reads a sentence, leaving off the nal word. The participants task isto provide any semantically and syntactically appropriate word to t the context.

    Results

    Behavioral accuracy

    To ensure that adults and children understood the sentences and task, we rst examined theiraccuracy to select the correct target picture. Accuracy was very high on the task in both groups,and very few errors were made in selecting the correct target picture. There were 4 incorrect re-sponses across adults (99.5% correct), and there were 20 incorrect responses across children (97.4%correct). All analyses below were conducted, and all results are reported, with these incorrect re-sponses removed.

    Time course measurements

    To examine the time course of incremental sentence processing in adult and child groups, we cal-culated the mean proportion of time spent xating to the target, agent-related, action-related, andunrelated images at each 10-ms time window across all adult participants (Fig. 2A) and children(Fig. 2B). Furthermore, to inspect the data for potential developmental and vocabulary-related differ-ences in processing, we split the child participants into older (6 years 5 months [6;5]10;0) and youn-ger (3;06;3) age groups (Fig. 3) and divided adults and children into high and low vocabulary groupsaccording to a median split of age-normalized PPVT (Fig. 4) and age-normalized CASLSC scores(Fig. 5). Relevant demographics and characteristics of children in each median split group are reportedin Table 1.

    These time course plots illustrate several interesting xation patterns. Most prominently visible isthe robust acceleration of (anticipatory) xations to the target object that begins as the verb is spoken.In addition, looks to the agent-related and action-related objects also increase after the agent and ac-tion onset, respectively. Notably, the timing at which xations to the target object diverge from looksto other items, especially the agent-related item, visibly varies between age and vocabulary groups.

    Fig. 2. Time course of xating to target and competitors interest areas (with SE bars) during the sentence in all adults (A) andall children (B) plotted in 10-ms time bins.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 425

  • Author's personal copy

    Time course of looks to targetWe initially characterized the timing of these apparent differences in xation timing between

    groups by conducting point-by-point t tests at each 10-ms bin between mean xation proportionsto the target object and xations to the agent-related item because this was the second most highlyxated object in each group. To minimize the possibility that differences measured by these multiplet test comparisons might have arisen by chance, we report the earliest time at which a minimum of

    Fig. 3. Proportion of time xating to each interest area in 10-ms time bins (with SE bars) from sentence onset to sentence offset,for older children (A) and younger children (B).

    Fig. 4. Time course of xations to target and distractor interest areas (with SE bars) during the sentence plotted in 10-ms timebins for adults and children in high and low vocabulary median split groups.

    426 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    ve subsequent and consecutive one-tailed t tests with an alpha level of p < .05 indicate a signicantpositive difference between xations to the target and other items.

    According to these analyses, the time at which xations to the target diverge varies considerablybetween adults (1220 ms) and children (1420 ms). Although these differences are smaller betweenolder and younger children (1410 and 1460 ms, respectively). Adults with higher and lower (age-normalized) vocabulary scores show some difference in target divergence times (1210 and 1300 ms,respectively),1 and these values are very similar for high and low sentence completion groups (1220and 1300 ms, respectively). The target divergence times are quite marked between children with higher

    Fig. 5. Time course of xations to target and distractor interest areas (with SE bars) during the sentence plotted in 10-ms timebins for adults and children in high and low sentence completion median split groups.

    Table 1Demographics of children in all median split groups.

    Group Age range (years;months) Mean age (years;months) Sex

    Older children 6;510;0 8;0 (2.5) 13 F, 11 MYounger children 3;06;3 4;3 (2.9) 9 F, 15 MHigh vocabulary 3;09;2 6;5 (4.7) 12 F, 12 MLow vocabulary 3;010;0 5;11 (6.0) 10 F, 14 MHigh sentence completion 3;09;2 6;4 (4.6) 13 F, 11 MLow sentence completion 3;010;0 5;11 (6.1) 9 F, 15 M

    Note. Standard errors are reported in parentheses. F, female; M, male.

    1 These results are identical when using raw vocabulary and sentence completion scores. Age-normalized scores are reported tomaintain standardized analysis procedures between adults and children.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 427

  • Author's personal copy

    (age-normalized) vocabularies (1220 ms) and those with lower (age-normalized) vocabularies(1520 ms). Relatively smaller but still sizable differences in target divergence were also found on the sen-tence completion measure in children (high: 1320 ms; low: 1510 ms).

    This preliminary inspection and description of the time course between groups indicated that therewere differences in the timing of anticipatory looking to the target. Because these differences arerevealed by a measure of the overall proportion of time spent xating on the object, there are a num-ber of ways in which these timing differences might have arisen. For one, it is possible that they aredue to a difference in the speed in initial looking to the target object. Alternatively, the mean durationor number of individual xations might vary. In the next set of analyses, we investigated these variouspossibilities.

    Timing of initial looksTo examine whether the observed timing differences arose from differences in the speed at which

    each group initially looked to the target, we measured the latency of the initial saccades that landed onthe target object after the action onset. Children initially xated to the target more rapidly than adults,t(94) = 2.057, p = .04. Older children were signicantly faster than younger children to xate on thetarget, t(46) = 2.28, p = .03, as were individuals with higher age-normalized vocabularies, t(94) =2.24, p = .03, whereas individuals with higher sentence completion scores were marginally faster toxate on the target, t(94) = 1.80, p = .07.

    Duration of lookingNext, we asked whether group differences in looking to the target might be driven by variation in

    the average individual xation time. This was measured by calculating the total time spent xating onthe target during the period from action word onset to target word offset and dividing this by the totalnumber of xations generated to the target image during this period. There were no group differencesin any age, vocabulary, or sentence completion comparison examined with this measure.

    Number of xationsFinally, we asked whether the total number of xations to the target varied between groups by

    measuring the overall number of xations to the target from action word onset to target offset. Chil-dren with higher vocabulary scores, t(46) = 3.298, p = .0019, and sentence completion scores,t(46) = 3.350, p = .0016, made a greater number of xations to the target object than their lower scor-ing peers. There were no other group effects for adults or across age groups.

    Associations among anticipatory xations, age, and measures of linguistic ability

    The above analyses explore group differences acrossmeasures that capture looking behaviors acrosstime windows that precede and follow the onset of the target word, that is, using eye movement mea-sures that are not solely anticipatory. Our primary interest, however, was to understand the interrela-tionships among patterns of anticipatory xations, age, and linguistic ability. To investigate thisquestion, we operationally dened anticipatory looking as the mean proportion of time spent xatingon the target object from the onset of the action word to the target word, that is, over the portion of thesentence duringwhich the necessary information to generate anticipatory looks was available. Becausewe are only interested in xations that were generated after the onset of the action, xations wereexcluded from analysis if their preceding saccade was initiated prior to action onset.

    With these measurements, we rst sought to determine whether participants xations to the tar-get region signicantly differed from xations to other regions during the same period. Consequently,we conducted several planned comparisons to compare the difference in xation proportion betweenthe target region and (a) the agent-related distractor, (b) the action-related distractor, and (c) theunrelated distractor. If eye movements were driven by combinatorial integration of meanings fromboth the sentential agent and action, then we should expect to see that differences between looksto the target and any other region should be signicantly greater than zero. That is, if participantsare indeed anticipating the upcoming target word before it is spoken, then we would expect partici-pants to xate to the target image with greater magnitude than other objects before the target itself is

    428 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    spoken. This is in fact what we found in all but two groups: children with lower sentence completionscores and children with lower vocabulary scores. These comparisons are summarized in Table 2.

    Table 3 shows associations between online measures of mean proportion of anticipatory xationsto the target object and ofine measures of age and (age-normalized) linguistic ability. Even thoughvocabulary and sentence completion were highly intercorrelated, r(96) = .83, p < .0001, only vocabu-lary was signicantly associated with the online measure of anticipatory xation, r(96) = .27,p = .0086.

    To further explore the relationship between the experimental and ofine measures, we enteredage, vocabulary, and sentence completion measures into a multiple regression model. Again, the onlyvariable to explain signicant variance in this model was vocabulary, r(96) = .33, p < .001. Neither agenor sentence completion scores explained signicant variance in our experimental measure whenthey were entered into the model singly or in combination with other factors. Because the sentencecompletion measure was highly intercorrelated with the vocabulary measure and it failed to accountfor signicant variance in task performance, we dropped this factor from subsequent analyses.

    We further investigated the possibility that the relationship between vocabulary and sentence pro-cessing might potentially interact with age by carrying out an additional (2 2) analysis of variance(ANOVA) with factors of age (adult or child) and a median split of age-normalized vocabulary scores(high or low). This analysis found a signicant effect of vocabulary, F(1,95) = 11.23, p = .0012, but notof age or Age Vocabulary (Fs < 1). To rule out that the general lack of age effects might be driven bythe inclusion of the adult participants in our analyses, we then conducted an identical ANOVA withonly the children included. In this case, two age groups were determined by a median split of olderand younger children (younger range = 3;06;3; older range = 6;510;0). Again, there was a main

    Table 2T-scores for difference between xation proportions in anticipatory time window to target image minus distractors.

    Target agent-related Target action-related Target unrelated

    All adults 7.14*** 8.34*** 8.74***

    High vocabulary adults 6.44*** 7.38*** 6.66***

    Low vocabulary adults 3.81** 4.68*** 6.00***

    High sentence completion adults 5.64*** 6.53*** 5.87***

    Low sentence completion adults 4.55*** 5.26*** 7.09***

    All children 2.50** 7.5*** 8.3***

    Older children 1.90** 6.72*** 7.05***

    Younger children 1.59# 4.24** 4.85***

    High vocabulary children 5.00*** 9.80*** 6.90***

    Low vocabulary children .552 3.06* 5.05***High sentence completion children 4.05** 9.99*** 6.81***

    Low sentence completion children .190 3.09** 5.01***

    * p < .01.** p < .001.*** p < .0001.# p = .062.

    Table 3Pearson correlation coefcients (r) between online measures of mean proportion of anticipatoryxations to target object, age, and ofine measures of language ability.

    Anticipatoryxations

    Vocabulary Sentencecompletion

    Age

    Anticipatoryxations

    .27* .17 .012

    Vocabulary .27* .83** .004Sentence completion .17 .83** .0001Age .012 .004 .0001

    Note. N = 96 (48 children and 48 adults).* p < .01.** p < .0001.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 429

  • Author's personal copy

    effect of vocabulary, F(1,47) = 6.84, p < .01, but not of age or Age Vocabulary (Fs < 1). This samepattern also holds when age and vocabulary are entered as continuous factors into a multiple regres-sion model with children only: vocabulary, F(1,47) = 9.35, p = .0038, age and Age Vocabulary(Fs < 1). Similarly, a median split of adults alone into higher and lower scoring vocabulary groupsyields signicant effects of vocabulary both for median splits of age-normalized vocabulary scores,F(1,47) = 3.99, p = .05, and for raw PPVT, F(1,47) = 5.99, p = .02. As continuous variables, adult rawPPVT scores signicantly associate with anticipatory xations, r(47) = .30, p = .04, but age-normalizedPPVT scores do not.

    Time course of xations to action-related item

    The time course in Figs. 25 suggests that participants in all groups generated additional looks tothe action-related distractor at varying time points after hearing the verb. To quantify the timing ofwhen these xations diverged from that of looks to the distractor items, we conducted point-by-pointt tests to determine the time window where the mean xation proportions to the action-related dis-tractor exceeded those to the unrelated distractor. As above, only time windows at which a minimumof ve consecutive time points reached signicance on this t test measure are reported. This analysisrevealed that children and adults in all examined groups xated on the action-related item in greatermagnitude than on the unrelated item, as reported in Table 4.

    Discussion

    In this investigation, we measured the relationships among anticipatory xations during a simplespoken sentence comprehension task, age, and linguistic ability. To our knowledge, this study is therst to show an association between vocabulary knowledge and incremental sentence interpretationin both adults and children. The study also replicates and extends a number of prior ndings in theincremental sentence processing literature. As reported in previous studies (Altmann & Kamide,1999; Kamide et al., 2003), adults in this study launched anticipatory xations to the target item start-ing at the action prior to the target. The timing with which these eye movements diverged from looksto other object regions was rapid in adults (1210 ms)approximately 300 ms after the onset of theaction. Given that it is generally considered to take approximately 200 ms to launch a saccade (cf.Altmann, 2011; Haith, 1993), the speed at which these xations diverged indicates that adults wereintegrating information from the combined agent and action with remarkable speed.

    We began by asking three questions. The rst was whether the predictive behaviors found in chil-dren in simple looking tasks scale up to more complex language processing in which listeners mustintegrate cues over multiple words. The answer is an unequivocal yes. Curiously, the anticipatory look-ing behaviors seem to be relatively larger than those seen by Kamide and colleagues (2003). Webelieve that much of this difference simply reects methodological differences in the visual display

    Table 4Summary of xation divergence times for target and action-related items.

    Comparison Target vs. agent-related (ms) Action-related vs. unrelated (ms)

    All adults 1220 14301490, 15202240All children 1420 14802370Older children 1410 15302400Younger children 1460 14402280High vocabulary adults 1210 16501990Low vocabulary adults 1310 13101470, 15102250High vocabulary children 1220 15102350Low vocabulary children 1520 15702290High sentence completion adults 1220 15102050, 21502230Low sentence completion adults 1300 16202170High sentence completion children 1320 14902340Low sentence completion children 1520 15702290

    430 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    and response demands between these two studies. Kamide and colleagues used a more naturalisticvisual scene that included representations of both potential agents, whereas we presented a simplerfour-choice array in order to reduce the distractibility of the scene for our younger participants. Inaddition, whereas we asked our participants to select the item that represented the sentential object,Kamide and colleagues simply asked participants to attend to the visual display and sentences. As aresult, we measured numerically larger xation proportions to all items in the experiment, includingthe distractor. Even with these differences, the pattern of ndings remains similar across the two stud-ies. It is clear that these methodological adaptations were successful in our younger participants. Thechildren understood and easily performed the task given that they made few response errors and gen-erated anticipatory eye movements with speeds that rivaled those of the adult participants. Yet, itshould be noted that some minor differences between children and adults did exist. The time courseof looking to the agent-related and target items early in the sentence did appear to be slightly differentbetween the child and adult groups. Because every item in our experimental design was completelybalanced across conditions, it is unlikely that this was due to visual properties of our stimulus. How-ever, it may represent a potential age-related strategy to look back at the agent-related item beforeselecting the target.

    Our second question was how and whether such abilities vary across childhood. The (nonantic-ipatory) looking behaviors noted in Fernald, Pinto, Swingley, Weinberg, and McRoberts (1998)showed clear increases in speed between 15 and 24 months of age. We expected similar increasesin this study. Although the children were older (310 years), the task was considerably more chal-lenging. However, here we were surprised to nd that age did not predict performance consideringchildren either alone or in comparison with adults. Strikingly, the children with the highest vocab-ulary levels performed nearly as quickly as adults with high vocabulary scores. This pattern is nota-ble because high vocabulary children were younger (average age = 6;5, range = 3;09;2) and hadlower raw PPVT scores on this task (mean PPVT score = 160, range = 113206) than their college stu-dent counterparts (mean PPVT score = 213, range = 208219), who are at least partially selected byhaving the requisite language ability to gain admission to the university. One potential caveat of thisnding, however, concerns the nature of our stimuli. Because these sentences were spoken usingchild-directed prosody, there is some possibility that adults, who are less familiar with this typeof speech than children, may have been slowed to some extent on this task. Nonetheless, even whenconsidering only child performance, the effects of age were not as notable as those of vocabularyskill (corrected for age).

    These results address our third question, which was what role one measure of linguistic abilityvocabulary skillmight play in predicting anticipatory looking skills. Vocabulary has been shown tostrongly predict nonanticipatory looking in other tasks with very young children (Hurtado, Marchman,& Fernald, 2008). Similarly, other measures of linguistic ability, such as reading comprehension inschool-aged children (Nation et al., 2003) and self-rating of second language prociency in college-aged adults (Chambers & Cooke, 2009), also seem to correlate with some aspects of predictive process-ing. In our tasks, we observed a complementary nding, where the participants age-normalized scoreon a test of receptive vocabulary is a better predictor of anticipatory looking than the age of the par-ticipant. That is, having a large vocabulary for ones age is important in this task. This result comple-ments the earlier ndings regarding vocabulary size per se and contributes to the appreciation that thevarious relationships among age, vocabulary, and speed of linguistic processing are somewhat com-plex, with the potential for multiple mediating factors. Because we failed to nd age-related changesin processing on our task, our result is consistent with the interpretation that processing speed itselfmay contribute independently to vocabulary learning rather than the other way around (Hurtadoet al., 2008). The relationship between vocabulary and online sentence comprehension is a topic thatmerits further study. It is also noteworthy that another measure of language ability, sentence comple-tion, was not associated with performance on this task even though sentence completion is itselfhighly correlated with vocabulary. This nding emphasizes the need to more nely determine whatprocessing subskills and specic linguistic knowledge support predictive processing in sentencecomprehension.

    Successful anticipatory performance on our task required not only an awareness of the semantic tor association between the verb and the correct upcoming target but also a more complex calculation

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 431

  • Author's personal copy

    of higher order contingencies among the agent, action, and patient. For instance, in the sentence Thepirate chases the ship, the associated visual display contains two objects that are potentially chase-able (SHIP and CAT) but only one that is likely to be chased by the pirate. The denition of likelihoodhere specically invokes knowledge of real-world events and situations. The current data indicatethat, as seems true for adults (Bicknell et al., 2010; Hald et al., 2007; Kamide et al., 2003; Matsuki,McRae, Hare, & Elman, 2008; Matsuki et al., 2011; Metusalem et al., 2012), 3- to 10-year-olds are ableto rapidly integrate multiple cues during sentence processing and use real-world knowledge of eventsand situations to guide their expectancies regarding upcoming referents.

    Finally, we comment on the strategy that listeners appeared to be using during this integrative pro-cess. The sentences in our design began with a cue (the agent) that potentially activated two visualitems: the target and the agent-related distractor. As we noted above, one strategy, consistent withthe Cohort model (Marslen-Wilson, 1987; Marslen-Wilson & Welsh, 1978), might be for listeners touse the subsequent cue (the action) to eliminate the agent-related distractor from this initial cohort,leaving active only the cohort. In this case, looks to the target should increase at mention of the action(which they did), but looks to all other distractors should decrease, including the initially activatedagent distractor.

    In fact, what appeared to happen is that listeners used a strategy more consistent with the TRACEmodel (McClelland & Elman, 1986). In TRACE, early cues activate an initial cohort of items that may bepruned by subsequent cues that are inconsistent with it. However, TRACE also allows subsequent cuesto activate new items that are locally consistent with the new cues even if they are inconsistent withthe earlier cues. This might seem like a nonoptimal strategywhy entertain hypotheses that oneknows are not likely to be correct?but it does allow for recovery under imperfect listening conditionsin which the initial word onset was not processed correctly.

    Indeed, this second strategy appears to best correspond to what listeners actually do when theyidentify words (Allopenna et al., 1998) and seems to resemble what our participants did when pro-cessing sentences. Rather than only narrowing the range of targets they considered, listeners showeda willingness to predict targets that were locally consistent with the action even though these targetswere not expected given the already mentioned agent. Thus, looks to the action-related target (e.g.,CAT) increased at the verb when participants heard The pirate chased . . . In fact, there is increasingevidence in the adult sentence processing literature that locally coherent syntactic and semantic inter-pretations are temporarily activated (Kukona et al., 2011; Tabor et al., 2004). We suggest that thisstrategy may be advantageous when encountering unexpected outcomes during language compre-hension. Rather than entirely discounting less expected possibilities, adults and children allow forthe possibility that the unexpected may happenalthough these less probable outcomes are weightedaccordingly.

    Acknowledgments

    We express our sincere gratitude to the families and children throughout the San Diego region whoparticipated in this research. We also thank Sarah Creel and Heather Pelton for assisting with two priornorming studies leading up to the research and thank Kim Sweeney and Alex Huynh for assisting withdata collection. In addition, we thank the editor, an anonymous reviewer, and Gerry Altmann for theirhelpful comments and suggestions. This work was funded by grants received from the National Insti-tutes of Health (HD053136 to J.E. and DC010106 to A.B.).

    Appendix

    List. of 32 sentences designed for the study

    The reman rides the truck.The girl rides the bike.The reman tastes the hamburger.The girl tastes the candy.

    432 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    The shark swims in the ocean.The boy swims in the pool.The shark catches the sh.The boy catches the ball.

    The pirate hides the treasure.The dog hides the bone.The pirate chases the ship.The dog chases the cat.

    The baby drinks the milk.The woman drinks the water.The baby wears the diaper.The woman wears the dress.

    The cat catches the bird.The frog catches the y.The cat jumps onto the couch.The frog jumps into the pond.

    The monkey jumps through the trees.The dolphin jumps through the waves.The monkey eats the banana.The dolphin eats the sh.

    The baby eats the cookie.The cow eats the grass.The baby sleeps in the crib.The cow sleeps in the barn.

    The boy ies the kite.The pilot ies the airplane.The boy wears the t-shirt.The pilot wears the helmet.

    References

    Allopenna, P. D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1998). Tracking the time course of spoken word recognition using eyemovements: Evidence for continuous mapping models. Journal of Memory and Language, 38, 419439.

    Altmann, G. T. M. (2011). Language can mediate eye movement control within 100 milliseconds, regardless of whether there isanything to move the eyes to. Acta Psychologica, 137, 190200.

    Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (1999). Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference.Cognition, 73, 247264.

    Altmann, G. T. M., & Kamide, Y. (2007). The real-time mediation of visual attention by language and world knowledge: Linkinganticipatory (and other) eye movements to linguistic processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 502518.

    Altmann, G. T. M., & Mirkovic, J. (2009). Incrementality and prediction in human sentence processing. Cognitive Science, 33,583609.

    Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in ChildDevelopment, 58(10, Serial No. 238).

    Arnold, J. E., Eisenband, J. G., Brown-Schmidt, S., & Trueswell, J. C. (2000). The rapid use of gender information: Evidence of thetime course for pronoun resolution from eyetracking. Cognition, 76, B13B26.

    Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1997). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia, andreal-time processing. Language and Cognitive Processes, 12, 507584.

    Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (1999). On the emergence of grammar from the lexicon. In B. MacWhinney (Ed.), The emergence oflanguage (pp. 2979). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Beck, I. L., & McKeown, M. G. (1991). Conditions of vocabulary acquisition. In R. Barr, M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, & P. D. Pearson(Eds.). Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp. 789814). New York: Longman.

    Bicknell, K., Elman, J. L., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Kutas, M. (2010). Effects of event knowledge in processing verbal arguments.Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 489505.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 433

  • Author's personal copy

    Boersma, P. (2001). Praat, a system for doing phonetics by computer. Glot International, 5, 341345.Bresnan, J. (2001). Lexical functional syntax. Malden, MA: Blackwell.Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment of spoken language. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Caselli, C., Casadio, P., & Bates, E. (1999). A comparison of the transition from rst words to grammar in English and Italian.

    Journal of Child Language, 26, 69111.Chambers, C. G., & Cooke, H. (2009). Lexical competition during second-language listening: Sentence context, but not

    prociency, constrains interference from the native lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, andCognition, 35, 10291040.

    Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye xation by the meaning of spoken language: A new methodology for the real-timeinvestigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 84107.

    Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 yearslater. Developmental Psychology, 33, 934945.

    Dahan, D., Magnuson, J. S., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2001). Time course of frequency effects in spoken-word recognition: Evidencefrom eye movements. Cognitive Psychology, 42, 317367.

    Dale, P. S., Dionne, G., Eley, T. C., & Plomin, R. (2000). Lexical and grammatical development: A behavioural genetic perspective.Journal of Child Language, 27, 619642.

    DeLong, K. A., Urbach, T. P., & Kutas, M. (2005). Probabilistic word pre-activation during language comprehension inferred fromelectrical brain activity. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 11171121.

    Dunn, D. M., & Dunn, L. M. (2007). Peabody picture vocabulary test. Minneapolis, MN: NCS Pearson.Ellis Weismer, S. (2007). Typical talkers, late talkers, and children with specic language impairment: A language endowment

    spectrum? In R. Paul (Ed.), The inuence of developmental perspectives on research and practice in communication disorders: AFestschrift for Robin S. Chapman. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Elman, J. (1990). Finding structure in time. Cognitive Science, 14, 179211.Estes, K. G., Evans, J. L., Alibali, M. W., & Saffran, J. R. (2007). Can infants map meaning to newly segmented words? Psychological

    Science, 18, 254260.Federmeier, K. D., & Kutas, M. (1999). A rose by any other name: Long-term memory structure and sentence processing. Journal

    of Memory and Language, 41, 469495.Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., & Thal, D. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of

    the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, Serial No. 242).Fernald, A., Perfors, A., & Marchman, V. A. (2006). Picking up speed in understanding: Speech processing efciency and

    vocabulary growth across the 2nd year. Developmental Psychology, 42, 98116.Fernald, A., Pinto, J. P., Swingley, D., Weinberg, A., & McRoberts, G. W. (1998). Rapid gains in speed of verbal processing by

    infants in the 2nd year. Psychological Science, 9, 228231.Fernald, A., Thorpe, K., & Marchman, V. A. (2010). Blue car, red car: Developing efciency in online interpretation of adjective

    noun phrases. Cognitive Psychology, 60, 190217.Fernald, A., Zangl, R., Portillo, A. L., & Marchman, V. (2008). Looking while listening: Using eye movements to monitor spoken

    language comprehension by infants and young children. In I. Sekerina, E. Fernandez, & H. Clahsen (Eds.), Developmentalpsycholinguistics: On-line methods in childrens language processing (pp. 97135). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.

    Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2004). N400-like semantic incongruity effect in 19-month-olds: Processing known words inpicture contexts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 14651477.

    Friedrich, M., & Friederici, A. D. (2005). Lexical priming and semantic integration reected in the event-related potential of 14-month-olds. NeuroReport, 16, 653656.

    Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary inchildren: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200213.

    Graves, M. F. (1986). Vocabulary learning and instruction. Review of Educational Research, 13, 4989.Haith, M. M. (1993). Future-oriented processes in infancy: The case of visual expectations. In C. Granrud (Ed.), Visual perception

    and cognition in infancy (pp. 235264). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Hald, L. A., Steenbeek-Planting, E. G., & Hagoort, P. (2007). The interaction of discourse context and world knowledge in online

    sentence comprehension. Evidence from the N400. Brain Research, 1146, 210218.Hare, M., Jones, M., Thomson, C., Kelly, S., & McRae, K. (2009). Activating event knowledge. Cognition, 111, 151167.Hockett, C. D. (1960). The origin of speech. Scientic American, 203, 8896.Hurtado, N., Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Does input inuence uptake? Links between maternal talk, processing speed,

    and vocabulary size in Spanish-learning children. Developmental Science, 11, F31F39.Kamide, Y., Altmann, G. T. M., & Haywood, S. (2003). The time-course of prediction in incremental sentence processing:

    Evidence from anticipatory eye movements. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 133156.Kidd, E., & Bavin, E. L. (2005). Lexical and referential cues to sentence interpretation: An investigation of childrens

    interpretations of ambiguous sentences. Journal of Child Language, 32, 855876.Kukona, A., Fang, S.-Y., Aicher, K. A., Chen, H., & Magnuson, J. S. (2011). The time course of anticipatory constraint integration.

    Cognition, 119, 2342.Kuperberg, G. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of language comprehension: Challenges to syntax. Brain Research, 1146,

    2349.Lew-Williams, C., & Fernald, A. (2010). Real-time processing of gender-marked articles by native and non-native Spanish

    speakers. Journal of Memory and Language, 63, 447464.Maital, S. L., Dromi, E., Abraham, S., & Bornstein, M. H. (2000). The Hebrew Communicative Development Inventory: Language

    specic properties and cross-linguistic generalizations. Journal of Child Language, 27, 4367.Marchman, V. A., & Bates, E. (1994). Continuity in lexical and morphological development: A test of the critical mass hypothesis.

    Journal of Child Language, 21, 339366.Marchman, V. A., & Fernald, A. (2008). Speed of word recognition and vocabulary knowledge in infancy predict cognitive and

    language outcomes in later childhood. Developmental Science, 11, F9F16.

    434 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Author's personal copy

    Marchman, V. A., Martnez-Sussmann, C., & Dale, P. S. (2004). The language-specic nature of grammatical development:Evidence from bilingual language learners. Developmental Science, 7, 212224.

    Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word recognition. Cognition, 25, 71102.Marslen-Wilson, W. D., & Welsh, A. (1978). Processing interactions and lexical access during word recognition in continuous

    speech. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 2963.Martino, N. L., & Hoffman, P. R. (2002). An investigation of reading and language abilities of college freshmen. Journal of Research

    in Reading, 25, 310318.Matsuki, K., Chow, T., Hare, M., Elman, J. L., Scheepers, C., & McRae, K. (2011). Event-based plausibility immediately inuences

    on-line language comprehension. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 37, 913934.Matsuki, K., McRae, K., Hare, M., & Elman, J. (2008). On-line dynamic combination of concepts during language comprehension.

    Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology [Revue Canadienne de Psychologie Experimentale], 62, 282.McClelland, J. L., & Elman, J. L. (1986). The TRACE model of speech perception. Cognitive Psychology, 18, 186.Metsala, J. L. (1999). The development of phonemic awareness in reading-disabled children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20,

    149158.Metusalem, R., Kutas, M., Urbach, T., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J.L. (2012). Generalized event knowledge activation during

    online sentence comprehension. Journal of Memory and Language. doi.10.1016/j.jml.2012.01.001.Metusalem, R., Kutas, M., Hare, M., McRae, K., & Elman, J. L. (2010). Generalized event knowledge activated during on-line

    language comprehension. In S. Ohlsson & R. Catrambone (Eds.), Proceedings of the thirty-second annual meeting of theCognitive Science Society (pp. 10581063). Austin, TX: Cognitive Science Society.

    Mills, D. L., Coffey-Corina, S., & Neville, H. J. (1997). Language comprehension and cerebral specialization from 13 to 20 months.Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 397445.

    Misyak, J. B., Christiansen, M. H., & Bruce Tomblin, J. (2010). Sequential expectations: The role of prediction-based learning inlanguage. Topics in Cognitive Science, 2, 138153.

    Moyle, M. J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J. L., & Lindstrom, M. J. (2007). Longitudinal relationships between lexical and grammaticaldevelopment in typical and late-talking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50, 508528.

    Nation, K. (2009). Reading comprehension and vocabulary: Whats the connection? In R. K. Wagner, C. Schatschneider, & C.Phythian-Sence (Eds.), Beyond decoding: The behavioral and biological foundations of reading comprehension (pp. 176194).New York: Guilford.

    Nation, K., Marshall, C. M., & Altmann, G. T. M. (2003). Investigating individual differences in childrens real-time sentencecomprehension using language-mediated eye movements. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 314329.

    Norris, D. (1994). Shortlist: A connectionist model of continuous speech recognition. Cognition, 52, 189234.Paul, R. (1996). Clinical implications of the natural history of slow expressive language development. American Journal of

    SpeechLanguage Pathololgy, 5, 521.Pollard, C., & Sag, I. A. (1994). Head-driven phrase structure grammar. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Rescorla, L. (2000). Do late talking toddlers turn out to have languages and reading difculties a decade later? Annals of Dyslexia,

    50, 87102.Rodriguez, P., Wiles, J., & Elman, J. L. (1999). A recurrent neural network that learns to count. Connection Science, 11, 540.Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274, 19261928.Salverda, A. P., Dahan, D., & McQueen, J. M. (2003). The role of prosodic boundaries in the resolution of lexical embedding in

    speech comprehension. Cognition, 90, 5189.Sedivy, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., Chambers, C. G., & Carlson, G. N. (1999). Achieving incremental semantic interpretation through

    contextual representation. Cognition, 71, 109147.Snedeker, J., & Trueswell, J. (2004). The developing constraints on parsing decisions: The role of lexical biases and referential

    scenes in child and adult sentence processing. Cognitive Psychology, 49, 238299.Stahl, S. A., & Nagy, M. E. (2006). Teaching word meanings. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Steedman, M., & Baldridge, J. (2006). Combinatory categorical grammar. In K. Brown (Ed.). Encyclopedia of language and

    linguistics (Vol. 2, pp. 610622). Oxford, UK: Elsevier.Tabor, W., Galantucci, B., & Richardson, D. (2004). Effects of merely local syntactic coherence on sentence processing. Journal of

    Memory and Language, 50, 355370.Tanenhaus, M. K., Magnuson, J. S., Dahan, D., & Chambers, C. (2000). Eye movements and lexical access in spoken-language

    comprehension: Evaluating a linking hypothesis between xations and linguistic processing. Journal of PsycholinguisticResearch, 29, 557580.

    Tanenhaus, M. K., Spivey-Knowlton, M. J., Eberhard, K. M., & Sedivy, J. C. (1995). Integration of visual and linguistic informationin spoken language comprehension. Science, 268, 16321634.

    Thordardottir, E. T., Ellis Weismer, S., & Evans, J. L. (2002). Continuity in lexical and morphological development in Icelandic andEnglish-speaking 2-year-olds. First Language, 22, 328.

    Travis, K. E., Leonard, M. K., Brown, T. T., Hagler, D. J., Curran, M., Dale, A. M., et al (2011). Spatiotemporal neural dynamics ofword understanding in 12- to 18-month-old infants. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 18321839.

    Trueswell, J. C., Sekerina, I., Hill, N. M., & Logrip, M. L. (1999). The kindergarten-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processingin young children. Cognition, 73, 89134.

    Trueswell, J. C., Tanenhaus, M. K., & Kello, C. (1993). Verb-specic constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects oflexical preference from garden-paths. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19,528553.

    VanBerkum, J. J. A., Brown, C. M., Zwitserlood, P., Kooijman, V., & Hagoort, P. (2005). Anticipating upcoming words in discourse:Evidence from ERPs and reading times. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 31,443467.

    Whipple, G. (Ed.). (1925). The twenty-fourth yearbook of the National Society for the Study of Education: Report of the NationalCommittee on Reading. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing.

    Verhaeghen, P. (2003). Aging and vocabulary scores: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 18, 332339.

    A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436 435

  • Author's personal copy

    White, T. G., Graves, M. F., & Slater, W. H. (1990). Growth of reading vocabulary in diverse elementary schools: Decoding andword meaning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 281290.

    Whitehurst, G. J., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Early developmental language delay: What, if anything, should a clinician do about it?Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 613648.

    Wicha, N. Y. Y., Moreno, E. M., & Kutas, M. (2004). Anticipating words and their gender: An event-related brain potential study ofsemantic integration, gender expectancy, and gender agreement in Spanish sentence reading. Journal of CognitiveNeuroscience, 16, 12721288.

    Zechmeister, E. B., Chronis, A. M., Cull, W. L., DAnna, C. A., & Healy, N. A. (1995). Growth of a functionally important lexicon.Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 201212.

    436 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012) 417436

  • Corrigendum

    Corrigendum to Knowing a lot for ones age: Vocabularyskill and not age is associated with anticipatoryincremental sentence interpretation in children and adults[Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 112 (2012)417436]

    Arielle Borovsky a,b,, Jeffrey L. Elman a,c, Anne Fernald baCenter for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USAbDepartment of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USAcDepartment of Cognitive Science, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA

    children (B) plotted in 10-ms time bins.

    DOI of original article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.01.005 Corresponding author at: Center for Research in Language, University of California, San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093, USA.

    Fax: +1 858 822 5097.E-mail address: [email protected] (A. Borovsky).

    Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114 (2013) 371373

    Contents lists available at SciVerse ScienceDirect

    Journal of Experimental ChildPsychology

    journal homepage: www.elsevier .com/locate/ jecp0022-0965/$ - see front matter 2012 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved.Portions of Figs. 25 were incorrectly illustrated and labeled in the issue. The corrected gures aregiven below.

    Fig. 2. Time course of xating to target and competitor interest areas (with SE bars) during the sentence in all adults (A) and allhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2012.09.010

  • Fig. 3. Proportion of time xating to each interest area in 10-ms time bins (with SE bars) from sentence onset to sentence offsetfor older children (A) and younger children (B).

    Fig. 4. Time course of xations to target and distracter interest areas (with SE bars) during the sentence plotted in 10-ms timebins for adults and children in high and low vocabulary median split groups.

    372 A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114 (2013) 371373

  • A. Borovsky et al. / Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 114 (2013) 371373 373The authors would like to apologize for any inconvenience caused.

    Fig. 5. Time course of xations to target and distracter interest areas (with SE bars) during the sentence plotted in 10-ms timebins for adults and children in high and low sentence completion median split groups.

    corr.pdfCorrigendum to Knowing a lot for ones age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults [Journal of Experimental Chil