42
(AQA A) AS Psychology Unit 2: PSYA2 – Biological Psychology, Social Psychology and Individual Differences

Biological Psychology, Social Psychology and … · (AQA A) AS Psychology Unit 2: PSYA2 – Biological Psychology, Social Psychology and Individual Differences

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

(AQA A) AS Psychology

Unit 2: PSYA2 – Biological Psychology, Social Psychology

and Individual Differences

Key Study Title Relates to:

Brady (1959) Stress-related illness and the immune system

Kiecolt-Glaser (1984) Stress-related illness and the immune system

Cohen (1993) Stress-related illness and the immune system

Rahe et al (1970) Life changes

DeLongis et al (1982) Daily Hassles

Marmot et al (1997) Workplace stress

Friedman and Rosenman (1959) Personality Factors, including Type A behaviour.

Key Study Title Relates to:

Sherif (1935) Types of conformity

Asch (1951) Explanations of why people conform

Milgram (1974) Obedience

Variations on Milgram (1974) Explanations of why people obey

Bickman (1974) Obedience and pressures to obey authority.

Zimbardo (1973) Implications for social change of research into social influence

Clark (1998) Internalisation

Gamson, Fireman & Rytina (1982) Explanations of independent behaviour

Key Study Title: Relates to:

Rosenhan and Seligman (1989) Definitions of abnormality: Failure to function

Jahoda (1958) Definitions of abnormality: deviation from ideal mental health.

Watson and Rayner (1920) Key features of the behavioural approach to psychopathology

Elkin et al (1989) Psychological and Biological therapies

Davidson et al (2004) Psychological and Biological therapies

Stress-related illness and the immune system

Brady (1958)

A classic study by Joseph Brady linked high levels of stress to

increased hormone production and the development of ulcers. In

an early study he placed monkeys in “restraining chairs” and

conditioned them to press a lever. They were given shocks every

20 seconds unless the lever was pressed in the same time period.

This investigation came to an abrupt halt when many of the

monkeys suddenly died. Post-mortem examination showed that

the monkeys had raised gastro-intestinal hormone levels and that

ulcers were the cause of death. The ulcers were not due to the

restraint because other monkeys had been kept in restraining

chairs for up to 6 months with no deaths occurring. The question was, were the ulcers due the

electric shocks or the stress?

To test this Brady and his colleagues used yoked controls. One monkey, called the “executive”, was

responsible for controlling the lever while a second monkey received the shocks but had no control

over the lever. Therefore only the “executive” monkey received the psychological stress of having to

press the lever but both monkeys received the shocks.

After 23 days of 6 hours on, 6 hours off schedule the executive monkey died due to a perforated

ulcer. Initially Brady thought that the stress might be related to the reinforcement schedule. He tried

various routines, such as 18 hours on and 6 hours off, or 30 minutes on, 30 minutes off. However, no

monkeys died from ulcers. He then tested the stomachs of executive monkeys on a 6 hour on, 6

hour off schedule, and found that stomach acidity was greatest during the rest period.

Brady concluded that it was clearly stress, not the shocks, that created the ulcers, and that the

greatest danger occurred when the sympathetic arousal stopped and the stomach was flooded with

digestive hormones, a parasympathetic rebound.

Task: Re-write this key study in less than 30 words – using a cue card to restrict you. On the reverse of the cue card, find two strengths and two weaknesses of the study. Remember to State, Explain and Apply each point.

Evaluation of Brady (1959)

This study became very well known and for a long time confirmed a common belief that too

much stress at work led to ulcers. The findings were supported in a study by Weiner et al

(1957) using army recruits. Prior to basic training, the soldiers were tested and classed as

oversecretors or undersecretors of digestive enzymes. After 4 months of stressful training,

14 % of the oversecretors had developed ulcers whereas none of the undersecretors had.

This suggests not only that the same principles do apply to humans but also that individual

differences may be important because not all of the oversecretors developed ulcers

One criticism made of Brady’s study in general was that the monkeys were not randomly

selected, the “executive” was chosen because it was faster at learning an avoidance

response. This may of course have parallels with the human world.

A more serious problem was raised in the

research by Marshall et al (1985). They found

strong evidence of another cause of stomach

ulcers, a bacterium called Helicobacter pylori. It

might be that stress has some influence on the

development of ulcers but the danger was that

people assumed this was the only cause. Marshall

was so convinced that the bacterium was at fault

that he deliberately gave himself the infection –

and developed ulcers.

To what extent do you think it is

reasonable to generalise from

research on monkeys to human

behaviour?

What ethical objections could be

raised in connection with this

study?

Stress-related illness and the immune system

Kiecolt-Glaser (1984)

Kiecolt-Glaser (1984) studied human responses to stress by using a naturally occurring situation –

examinations. The researchers took blood samples from 75 1st-year medical students (49 males and

26 females), all of whom were volunteers. The samples were taken one month before their final

examination (the baseline sample) and again on the first day of their final examinations, after the

students had completed two of the examinations. This was the “stress samples” taken when the

students’ stress levels should be at their highest.

Kiecolt-Glaser et al found that natural killer cell

activity declined between the two samples,

confirming other research findings that stress is

associated with a reduced immune response.

The volunteers were also assessed using

behavioural measures. On both occasions they

were given questionnaires to assess psychiatric

symptoms, loneliness and life events. This was

because there are theories which suggest that all

3 are associated with increased levels of stress.

Kiecolt-Glaser et al found that immune responses

were especially weak in those students who

reported feeling most lonely, as well as those

who were experiencing other stressful life events

and psychiatric symptoms such as depression or

anxiety.

This means that there were 2 key findings from

this study. One was that stress was associated with a lowered immune response in humans. The

second was that there were a number of different sources of stress and factors that moderate it.

Evaluation of Kiecolt-Glaser (1984)

There are many strengths to this study. First of all, it was a natural experiment which means

that there can be fewer ethical objections but, at the same time, it does mean it was not a

true experiment. At best we can say that stress and immune response are negatively

correlated (as one increases, the other decreases), we cannot say that one caused the other.

The advantage of this study, again related to the choice of independent variable (exam

stress), is that it was a long-term form of stress. In previous studies with human participants,

stress had been artificially induced, for example by restricting sleep for several days

(Palmblad et al, 1979). This is likely to produce a different kind of stress to that experienced

naturally.

Stress-related illness and the immune system

Cohen (1993)

Cohen et al investigated the role of general life stress on vulnerability to the common cold virus.

Three hundred and ninety-four participants completed questionnaires on the number of stressful life

events they had experienced in the previous year. They also rated their degree of stress and their

level of negative emotions such as depression. The three scores were combined into what Cohen et

al called a stress index.

The participants were then exposed to the common cold virus, leading to 82% becoming infected

with the virus. After seven days the number whose infection developed into clinical colds were

recorded. The findings were that the chance of developing a cold, i.e. failing to fight off the viral

infection, was significantly correlated with stress index scores.

Cohen et al concluded that life stress and negative emotions reduce the effectiveness of our immune

system, leaving participants less able to resist viral infections.

Evaluation of Cohen (1993)

Methodological issues

This was an indirect study in that there were no direct measures of immune function.

However it is supported by Evans and Edgington (1991) who found that the probability of

developing a cold was significantly correlated with negative events in the preceding days.

It did measure health outcomes (development of clinical colds), showing a relationship

between life stress and illness. This can be compared with studies that use measures of

immune function rather than illness outcomes.

There was no direct manipulation of the independent variable (the stress index), and so a

cause and effect relationship cannot be confirmed.

This study does not tell us which element of the stress index is most important.

Ethical issues

Clinical studies in psychology and medicine are always

covered by rigorous ethical considerations. All applications

are reviewed by professional committees, who consider a

variety of issues.

Participants should be in good health with no illnesses or

infections prior to the study.

Participants should be able to give fully informed consent,

with debriefing afterwards.

During the study participants should be constantly

monitored to check for any reactions to the viral challenge.

The scientific value of the study should be balanced against

any psychological or physical distress to participants.

Life changes as a source of stress

Rahe (1970)

The aim of this study was to investigate whether scores on the Holmes and Rahe Social

Readjustment Rating Scale (SRRS) were correlated with the subsequent onset of illness.

2,500 male American sailors were given the SRRS to assess how many life events they had

experienced in the previous 6 months. The total score on the SRRS was recorded for each

participant. Then over the following six-month tour of duty, detailed records were kept of each

sailor’s health status. The recorded number of Life Change Units were correlated with the sailors’

illness scores.

There was a positive correlation of +0.0118 between Life Change scores and illness scores. Although

the positive correlation was small (a perfect positive correlation would be +1.00), it did indicate that

there was a meaningful relationship between Life Change Units and health (this is often referred to

as a statistically significant correlation). As Life Change Units increased, so did the frequency of

illness.

The researchers concluded that as Life Change Units were positively correlated with illness scores,

experiencing life events increased the chances of stress-related health breakdown. As the

correlation was not perfect, life events cannot be the only factor in contributing to illness.

The study does not take into account individual differences in reactions to stress.

A correlation does not imply causality nor the direction of any effect; depression or anxiety may not be caused

by life events, since depressed or anxious people may bring about life events such as separation or divorce

The sample was restricted to male US Navy personnel; therefore it was ethnocentric (Americans only) and

androcentric (males only). This reduces the validity of the study and makes it difficult to generalise to other

populations.

Daily Hassles

DeLongis et al (1982)

Most people experience major life events very infrequently. Therefore a better measure of stress

might look at the stresses and strains of daily life. These are called “hassles”. DeLongis et al (1982)

compared the two measures: a life events scale and their own hassles scale, to see which was a

better predictor of later health problems. They also considered the effects of “uplifts”. Uplifts are

events that make you feel good. How does that affect health?

Participants were asked to complete four questionnaires once a month for a period of a year:

Hassles scale (117 hassles, such as: concerns about weight, rising prices, home maintenance,

losing things, crime and physical appearance).

Uplifts scale (135 uplifts, such as: recreation, relations with friends, good weather, job

promotion).

Life events questionnaire (24 major events).

A health status questionnaire consisting of questions on overall health status, bodily

symptoms, and energy levels.

There were 100 participants from around San Francisco area, aged between 45 and 64. They were

predominantly well educated and had high income. The findings were that both the frequency and

intensity of hassles were significantly correlated with overall health status and bodily symptoms.

Daily uplifts had little effect on health. They also found no relationship between life events and

health during the study, although there was a relationship for life events recorded for the 2 ½ years

before the study.

Task: With a learning partner, try to find 3 strengths

of this study and 3 weaknesses of this study. Write

the points using the State, Explain and Apply

framework. When you are ready write your point on

the whiteboard.

How might the sample of respondents have biased the results obtained in the study? Why might daily hassles be a better measure of stress than life events?

Evaluation of DeLongis et al (1982)

This approach managed to avoid the problem of the relevance of major life events but it still

overlooked many chronic “ongoing” sources of stress, such as poor housing, low incomes, strains of

family life, unsatisfying work and so on. Health is probably affected by all three: hassles, chronic

situations, and life changes.

One problem with the scales used in this study was that the same hassle can be experienced in

different ways by different people or by the same person on different occasions. The same factors

that are a hassle to one person may actually be uplift to another, or the same factor might mean

different things to the same person on different occasions. For example, a traffic jam may

sometimes give you time to relax, whereas at other times it seems highly stressful. For this reason

DeLongis, Folkman and Lazarus (1988) later produced a single “Hassles and Uplifts Scale” where

respondents could indicate the strength of a factor either as a hassle or an uplift. In other words,

when asked to indicate how they felt about “sex” they could rate it as a 0 1 2 3 as a hassle or 0 1 2 3

as an uplift.

A further problem concerns the original sample which was of people aged over 45, this sample bias

may have affected the results. Khan and Patel (1996) found that older people tended to have less

severe, and fewer, hassles than younger people.

Workplace Stress

Marmot et al (1997)

Most people would agree that work is a source of considerable stress, and some would agree that

this stress may lead to illness. The question for researchers is to find out what aspects of the

workplace are the most important sources of stress.

Marmot et al sought to test the job-strain model. This model proposes that the workplace creates

stress and illness in two ways: (1) high demand and (2) low control. Marmot et al suggested that this

could be tested in context of civil service employees where the higher grades would experience high

job demand, and low-grade civil servants would experience low job control.

Civil service employees (men and women) working in London were invited to take part in this study.

A total of 7,372 people agreed to answer a questionnaire and be checked for signs of cardiovascular

disease. About 5 years later each participant was reassessed. For each participant the following

information was recorded: signs of cardiovascular disease (e.g. ischemia or chest pains), presence of

coronary risk factors (e.g. smoking), employment grade (a measure of the amount of job demand

an individual experienced), sense of job control (measured by questionnaire) and amount of social

support (also measured by a questionnaire).

At the end of the study it was found that participants in the higher grades of the civil service had

developed the fewest cardiovascular problems. Participants in the lower grades expressed a weaker

sense of job control and also had poorest social support. It was also found that cardiovascular

disease could in part be explained in terms of risk factors such as smoking (i.e. people who

developed cardiovascular disease were more likely to be smokers and be overweight).

The main conclusion is that low control appears to be linked to cardiovascular disorder, whereas

high job demand is not linked to stress and illness. This does not fully support the job-strain model

because it does not show that high demand is linked to illness, but lack of control does appear to be

linked to stress and illness.

Can the findings be explained in terms of socio-

economic status?

We can classify individuals in terms of their socio-

economic status (SES) – a measure of the kind of job

they do (skilled or unskilled), how much money they

have, the kind of house they live in and so on. People

who are of low SES are more likely to smoke, live in

more stressful environments and have poorer diets –

all factors linked to cardiovascular problems. This

means that low-grade civil servants may have more

cardiovascular problems than high-grade civil servants

because of factors related to low SES rather than

because they lack job control.

However, Marmot et al argue that other studies have demonstrated that lack of control

does increase stress. If low control is generally a source of stress, it may well be an

important factor of stress in the workplace in addition to other SES factors.

Biased sample

The conclusions of this study are biased on the sample studied- civil servants. The

response s of such individuals may not be typical of all adults as they are urban

dwellers who are probably quite job oriented and ambitious, in contrast with rural

inhabitants whose jobs may play a less significant role in their lives. One study found

that ambitious individuals were more affected by workplace stressors (Caplan et al

1975). Therefore, we might conclude that not everyone will be affected by low control

in the way that these individuals were.

Personality factors, including Type A behaviour

Friedman and Rosenman (1974)

The Western Collaborative Group Study was set up in the 1960’s to test Friedman and Rosenman’s

hypothesis that type A individual were more likely to develop heart disease than type B. The type A

behaviour pattern was initially assessed by means of the structured interview. This assessment

procedure makes use of two kinds of information: (1) the answers given to questions asked during

the interview, and (2) the individual’s behaviour, including aspects of his or her way of speaking (e.g.

loudness and speed of talking). The individual’s tendencies towards impatience and hostility are

assessed by the interviewer deliberately interrupting the person being interviewed from time to

time. The type A behaviour pattern has also been assessed by various self-report questionnaires

(e.g. Jenkins Activity Survey).

On the basis of these measures the participants were classed as A1 (Type A), A2 (not fully type A),

X (equal amounts of A and B), and B (fully type B). Nearly 3,200 healthy men aged between 39 and

59 living around San Francisco were assessed in 1960. Eight and half years later 257 of the men in

the original sample had developed coronary heart disease (CHD).

The findings were striking – 70% of those with CHD had been assessed as Type A, nearly twice as

many as Type Bs. This remained so, even when account was taken of various other factors (e.g.

blood pressure, smoking, obesity) which are known to be associated with heart disease.

One of the limitations of the Western Collaborative Group Study was that it was not clear which

aspect of the Type A behaviour pattern was most closely associated with heart disease. This issue

was addressed by Matthews et al (1977). They re-analysed the data from the Western Collaborative

Group Study, and found that coronary heart disease was most associated with the hostility

component of Type A.

Why is type A (or its hostility component) associated with heart disease? As Ganster et al (1991)

pointed out, it has often been assumed that “chronic elevations of the sympathetic nervous system

(in type As) lead to deterioration of the cardiovascular system”. Ganster et al put their participants

into stressful situations and recorded various physiological measures, including blood pressure and

heart rate. Only the hostility component of Type A was associated with high levels of physiological

reactivity. These findings, when combined with those of Matthews et al (1977), suggest that high

levels of hostility produce increased activity within the sympathetic nervous system, and this plays a

role in the development of coronary heart disease.

Evaluation of Friedman and Rosenman (1974) These findings were confirmed in another large-scale longitudinal study which involved both men and women, the Framingham Heart study (Haynes et al, 1980). However, some researchers have failed to find any relationship between type A and coronary heart disease. This has led a number of psychologists to doubt the importance of the Type A behaviour pattern as a factor in causing heart disease. However, Miller et al (1991) reviewed the literature, and found that many of the negative findings were obtained in studies using self-report measure of type A behaviour. Studies using the Structured Interview with initially healthy populations reported a mean correlation of +0.33 between type A behaviour and coronary heart disease, supporting the initial finding of a moderate relationship between the two variables. One problem for any study is that as soon as an individual knows they are at risk of CHD they may engage in behaviours which modify that risk, such as giving up smoking or taking more exercise. This may create lower long-term correlations between Type A and CHD. The evidence has been applied successfully, for example Friedmand, Tordoff and Ramirez (1986) reported on the Recurrent Coronary Prevention Project which aimed to modify type A behaviour and so reduce CHD in participants who had experienced a heart attack. At a five year follow-up, those participants who had taken part in a behaviour modification did have fewer second heart attacks than those who received counselling or no treatment.

Types of conformity

Sherif (1935)

The first major study of conformity was carried out by

Muzafer Sherif. He made use of what is known as the

autokinetic effect. If we look at a stationary spot of light in

a darkened room, then very small movements of the eyes

make the light seem to move. In Sherif’s key condition, the

participants were first of all tested one at a time, and then

in small groups of three. They were asked to say how much

the light seemed to move, and in what direction. Each

participant rapidly developed his or her own personal norm.

This norm was stable, but it varied considerably between

individuals. When three individuals with very different

personal norms were then put together into a group, they

tended to make judgements that were very close to each

other. The fact that a group norm rapidly replaced the

personal norms of the members of the group indicates the

influence of social influence.

Sherif (1935) also used a condition in which individuals

started the experiment in groups of three, and then were

tested on their own. Once again, a group norm tended to

develop within the group. When the members of the group

were then tested on their own, their judgements

concerning the movement of the light continued to reflect

the influence of the group.

Evaluation of Sherif (1935)

There are three major

limitations with Sherif’s

research. First, he used a

very artificial situation, and

it is not clear how relevant

his findings are for most

everyday situations.

Second, there was no

“correct” answer in his

situation. It is not very

surprising that individuals

rely on the judgment of

others when they have no

clear way of deciding what

judgments to make. Third,

conformity effects can be

assessed more directly by

arranging for all but one of

the participants in an

experiment to give the

same judgement, and then

seeing what effect this has

on the remaining

participant. This was done

by Jacobs and Campbell

(1961) using the

autokinetic effect and they

found strong evidence of

conformity.

Explanations of why people conform

Asch (1951)

Asch’s aim was to see if participants would yield to majority social influence and give incorrect

answers in a situation when the correct answers were always obvious.

Seven male, student participants looked at two cards: the test card showed one vertical line; the

other card showed three vertical lines of different length. The participants’ task was to call out, in

turn, which of these three lines was the same length as the test line. The correct answer was always

obvious. All participants, except one, were accomplices of the experimenter. The genuine

participants called out his answer last but one. Accomplices gave unanimous wrong answers on 12 of

the 18 trials. These 12 trials were called the critical trials. In total, Asch used 50 male college

students as naïve, genuine participants in this first study.

Participants conformed to the unanimous incorrect answer on 32% of the critical trials. This might

not strike you as a very high figure but remember the correct answer was always obvious. 74% of

participants conformed at least once. 26% of participants never conformed. Some of these

‘independent’ participants were confident in their judgements. More often, however, they

experienced tension and doubt but managed to resist the pressure exerted by the unanimous

majority. During post-experimental interviews, some conforming participants claimed to have

actually seen the line identified by the majority as the correct answer. Others yielded because they

could not bear to be in a minority of one and risk being ridiculed or excluded by the group. Most

participants who had conformed, however, experienced a distortion of judgement: they thought

that their perception of the lines must be inaccurate and for that reason they yielded to the majority

view.

Even in unambiguous situations, there may be a strong group pressure to conform, especially if the

group is a unanimous majority. However, after interviewing the participants, Asch concluded that

people go along with the views of other for different reasons. Some people experience normative

social influence and feel compelled to accept the mistaken majority’s norms or standards of

behaviour to avoid being rejected. Others experience informational social influence and doubt their

own judgements – they want to be seen as correct.

Evaluation of Asch (1951)

All the participants were male college students and so a very limited sample.

The time and place when the research was carried out might have affected the

findings. In the 1950s the USA was very conservative, involved in an anticommunist

witch-hunt against anyone who was thought to hold left-wing views (this became

known as ‘McCarthyism’, named after the senator who spearheaded the witch-

hunt) and its educational institutions were more hierarchal than they are today.

Since all the participants were male and college students, it might not be valid to

generalise the findings to a wider population. The way to check this is to rerun the

study using different types of participants. Do you think that male college students

are typical, or more or less conformist than the general population?

TASK:

With a learning partner, find two strengths and two limitations of Asch’s method.

State, explain and apply every point (say why it is good or bad). Furthermore,

explain in your limitations how Asch’s experiment could have been improved.

Obedience

Milgram (1974)

Stanley Milgram (1974) advertised in a local paper for men to take part in an experiment concerning

memory and learning, to be conducted at the prestigious Yale University in America. 40 men, aged

between 20 and 50 volunteered. They were paid $4.50 simply for turning up; payment did not

depend on staying the study. When participants arrived they were told that there would be two

participants, a “learner” and a “teacher”. The experimenter drew lots to see which participant would

take which part. At this point you should know that this experiment was “single blind”. The

participant was not told the true details of the research. The truth was that the other participant was

in fact a confederate of the experimenter, and the “experimenter” was also a confederate. The true

participant always ended up being given the role of the “teacher”.

The “teacher” was told to give electric shocks to the “learner” every time the wrong answer was

given, and the shock intensity was increased each time. In fact, the apparatus was arranged so that

the learner never actually received any shocks, but the teacher did not realise this. At 180 volts, the

learner yelled “I can’t stand the pain”, and by 270 volts the response had become an agonised

scream. The maximum intensity of shock was 450 volts. If the teacher was unwilling to give shocks,

the experimenter urged him to continue, saying such things as “it is absolutely essential that you

should continue.”

Milgram (1974) asked 14 psychology students to predict what participants would do. They

estimated that no more than 3% of the participants would go up to 450 volts. In fact, about 65% of

Milgram’s participants gave the maximum shock. One of the most striking cases of total obedience

was that of Pasqual Gino, a 43-year-old water inspector. Towards the end of the experiment, he

found himself thinking, “Good God, he’s dead. Well, here we go, we’ll finish him. And I just

continued all the way through to 450 volts”. Other participants found the experience very distressing.

They were seen to “sweat, tremble, stutter, bite their lips, groan and dig their finger-nails into their

flesh”. Milgram reported that three participants had “full-blown uncontrollable seizures”.

At the end of the experiment all participants were debriefed by being told the actual nature of the

study. They were introduced to the “learner” and assured they he had experienced no actual shocks.

They were told that their behaviour was entirely normal and, when interviewed later by

questionnaire, 74% said they had learned something of personal importance. Only one person

expressed regret about having taken part.

Evaluation of Milgram (1974)

The main text offers a discussion of some of the key criticisms made of this study. The main issues

relate to experimental and ecological validity, and the ethical concerns raised by the study. Criticism

can also be positive, and the findings of this research have had a powerful influence on subsequent

empirical research and psychological theory.

A key feature of the experiment was that the authority, in this case, was “unjust”. The fact that

participants obeyed during the initial stages is neither surprising nor objectionable. The fact that they

continued to obey is surprising. Milgram (1974) said that authority-agent relations are the simple

machinery of social routine. What is significant about some situations, such as his experiment, is that

sometimes authority makes unreasonable requests and then people ought not to obey.

Explanations of why people obey

Variations on Milgram (1974)

Milgram (1974) carried out several variations on his basic experiment. He found that there were two

main ways in which obedience to authority could be reduced:

1. Increasing the obviousness of the learner’s plight. This was studied by comparing

obedience in 4 situations differing in the obviousness of the learner’s plight (the % of

participants who were totally obedient is shown in brackets):

Remote feedback: the victim could not be heard or seen (66%)

Voice feedback: the victim could be heard but not seen (62%)

Proximity: the victim was only 1 metre away from the participant (40%)

Touch-proximity: this was like the proximity condition; expect that the participant had to

force the learner’s hand onto the shock plate (30%)

2. Reducing the authority or influence of the experimenter:

Staging the experiment in a run-down office building rather than at Yale University (48%)

Orders by telephone rather than having experimenter sitting close to the participant (20.5%).

The effect of distance may help to explain why it is less stressful to kill people by dropping

bombs from a plane than by shooting them at close range.

The experimenter was an ordinary member of the public rather than a white-coated

scientist (20%)

Giving the participant a confederate who refused to give shocks (10%)

Evaluation of Milgram’s variations

These variations give use greater insight into the

conditions under which people will obey unjust

requests. They also show us something about

independent behaviour because in many situations the

majority of participants behaved independently.

There were ethical criticisms made of the original study

by Milgram. One of Milgram’s defences was that he

could not anticipate how many participants would obey

and therefore suffer the stress associated with knowing

what they did. In these later variations Milgram could

not use this defence

Obedience and pressures to obey authority

Bickman (1974)

Leonard Bickman (1974) tested the ecological validity of Milgram’s work by conducting an

experiment in a more realistic setting. In this study three male experimenters gave orders to 153

randomly selected pedestrians in Brooklyn, New York. The experimenters were dressed in one of

three ways: a sports coat and tie, a milkman’s uniform, or guard’s uniform that made them look like

a police officer. The experimenter gave 1 of 3 orders:

Pointing to a bag on the street, “Pick up this bag for me.”

Nodding in the direction of a confederate “This fellow is over parked at the meter but

doesn’t have any change. Give him a dime.”

Approaching the participant at a bus stop, “Don’t you know you have to stand on the other

side of the pole? This signs says ‘No standing’”.

Bickman found that participants were most likely to obey the experimenter dressed as a guard than

the milkman or civilian. This supports one of the variations of Milgram’s findings, that obedience can

be related to the amount of perceived authority.

Who would you obey?

Evaluation of Bickman

Field experiments may have increased ecological validity and mundane realism but the cost is

decreased control. They also raise more ethical concerns because informed consent cannot be

sought and it is difficult to debrief participants without alerting others to the experiment. Bickman’s

study differs from Milgram’s in one important way. The orders were not quite so unreasonable;

therefore obedience was more understandable.

Implications for social change of research into social influence

Zimbardo (1973)

This study is relevant to understanding obedience as well as conformity. It also demonstrates the

power of social situations on people’s behaviour. Specifically, Zimbardo was testing (and trying to

find evidence against) the dispositional hypothesis. According to this, the dehumanising effects of

the prison system are due to prisoners’ antisocial attitudes and behaviour, and guards’ sadism and

insensitivity. Participants were carefully selected for not displaying any of these characteristics. They

were randomly assigned to the role of prisoner or guard. This meant that any antisocial behaviour or

sadism they showed was caused by the prison conditions, not by participants’ personal

characteristics. A mock (simulated) prison was deliberately created (in the basement of the Stanford

University psychology department). Hence, the study is often called the ‘prison simulation

experiment’ (or the ‘Stanford prison study’). Zimbardo wished to create a prison-like environment,

which was as psychologically real as possible.

Participants were recruited though newspaper advertisements asking for male student volunteers

for a two-week study of prison life. From 75 volunteers, 24 were selected. They were judged to be

emotionally stable, physically healthy, and of ‘normal to average’ personality. They also had no

history of psychiatric problems, and had never been in trouble with police. They were all white,

middle-class students from across the US. Those allocated to the prisoner role were arrested by local

police. They were charged with a felony, read their rights, searched, handcuffed, and taken to the

police station to be ‘booked’. After being fingerprinted, each prisoner was taken blindfold to the

basement prison. On arrival, they were stripped naked and issued with a loose-fitting smock. Their

ID number was printed on the front and back, and they had a chain bolted around one ankle. They

wore a nylon stocking to cover their hair, were referred to by number only, and were allocated to

6 x 9ft ‘cells’ (three to a cell). The guards wore military-style khaki uniforms and silver reflector

sunglasses (making eye contact impossible). They carried clubs, whistles, handcuffs, and keys to the

cells. There were guards on duty 24 hours a day, each working eight hour shifts. They had complete

control over the prisoners, who were confined to their cells around the clock – except for meals,

toilet privileges, head counts and work.

An initial ‘rebellion’ by the prisoners was crushed. After this, they began to react passively as the

guards stepped up their aggression. They began to feel helpless and no longer in control of their lives.

Every guard at some time or another behaved in an abusive, authoritarian way. Many seemed to

really enjoy the new-found power and control that went with the uniform. After less than 36 hours,

one prisoner had to be released because of uncontrolled crying, fits of rage, disorganised thinking,

and severe depression. Three others developed the same symptoms, and were released on

successive days. Another prisoner developed a rash over his whole body. They became demoralised

and apathetic, and started to refer to themselves (and others) by their numbers. Zimbardo intended

the experiment to run for two weeks. But it was abandoned after just six days, because of the

prisoners’ pathological reactions.

Zimbardo rejected the dispositional hypothesis. They argued that their findings supported the

situational hypothesis. This claims that it’s the conditions of prisons (physical, social and

psychological) that are ‘to blame’, not the characteristics of prisoners and guards. So, anyone given

the role of guard or prisoner would probably behave as Zimbardo’s participants did. A brutalising

atmosphere, like the mock prison, produces brutality. If the roles had been reversed, the prisoners

would have abused their power in just the same way. It’s the prison environment that makes people

act in ‘typical guard’ or ‘typical prisoner’ ways.

Evaluation of Zimbardo (1973)

High ecological validity. Both the environment and the behaviour (of guards and prisoners)

were ‘realistic’, and the findings can be applied to real prisons.

Mere role-playing. Participants were acting out their prior expectations about how guards

and prisoners should behave (based on TV, movies etc). In other words, they were

conforming, but this wasn’t so much yielding to group pressure as trying to be a ‘typical’

guard or prisoner.

The reality of roles. They may have been role-playing at the start. But participants were

soon taking their roles very seriously indeed – they became ‘real’.

Relevant to social influence. The experiment relates to various forms of social influence.

These include both conformity and obedience.

Informed consent. Participants signed an informed consent form before the experiment

began. They were told a great deal about the experiment – its purpose, what was likely to

happen and so on. But they weren’t told about how they’d be arrested.

Zimbardo’s loss of objectivity. Zimbardo should have stopped the experiment long before

6 days. Zimbardo admitted that he became too involved in his role as prison supervisor. An

outsider had to remind him that he was also the psychologist in charge of the study!

Practical consequences. The experiment had practical implications for the welfare of

prisoners.

Internalisation

Clark (1998)

Clark carried out a series of studies using the 1954 film 12 Angry Men in which a single juror (the

actor Henry Fonda) believes that a defendant is innocent of killing his father and sets out to convince

the rest of the jury that the young man is innocent. Participants were asked to play the role of jurors

and to make up their minds about the guilt or innocence of the young man.

Clark wanted to test two different predictions in these studies:

That the minority could exert its influence through the information presented and the

persuasive nature of the minority’s arguments

That the minority could influence the majority through changes in behaviour or ‘defections’.

Seeing other people change their view can have a powerful effect on the individual’s own

beliefs.

In the first study, Clark used 220 psychology students, 129 women and 91 men. The participants

were given a four-page booklet with a summary of the plot of 12 angry men. This booklet contained

evidence for the defendant’s guilt:

That he had purchased and used a rare knife from a local store.

That he had been seen by two eyewitnesses, one an old man who claimed he had heard the

defendant say ‘I am going to kill you’, and the other a woman who had been in the

apartment opposite, who identified the defendant as the murderer.

Clark varied whether or not the students were given information about Henry Fonda’s defence and

the counter arguments. He found that a minority juror only led people to change their minds when

they could provide counter-evidence to the charge. If they did not provide evidence, people did not

move from the majority position. This supports his claim that the information given by the minority

is important.

In another study, Clark focused on the impact of behaviour, or people defecting to the minority

position. Student participants were given a three-page summary of the jury’s discussion in the film.

This contained the main counter arguments presented by the minority juror played by Henry Fonda.

These were:

That he (Henry Fonda) had been able to produce in court an identical ‘rare’ knife, which he

had bought from a nearby junk shop.

That the man could not have seen or heard the murder as his old age and disabilities meant

it took him too long to get to the window in the apartment

That the old woman could not have seen the defendant as she had very bad eyesight and

was not wearing glasses.

Clark presented different scenarios to the students in which he showed varying numbers of

defectors (people changing their behaviour to adopt the ‘not guilty’ position) from one to six. Clark

asked the students to use a nine-point scale to give their opinion of whether or not the man was

guilty. He found that participants were influenced by the number of defectors to the ‘not guilty’

position. When they heard that four or seven jurors had changed their mind to agree with Henry

Fonda they were more likely to adopt the ‘not guilty’ position themselves. Seven defectors had no

more influence than four. Clark argued that after four people had changed their minds; a ‘ceiling of

influence’ is reached, meaning that more defectors do not produce more influence. The findings of

this study support Clark’s view that minorities can influence people to change their views through

changing their own behaviour.

Evaluation of Clark (1998) Methodological issues

Clark’s study provides a very good example of how research into social psychology has changed over the last 30 years. In contrast to Asch and Moscovici, who used laboratory experiments to investigate conformity, Clark’s participants were asked to play the role of jurors. The task was a simulation of a realistic situation in which social influence takes place – that of jury decision making.

The costs of making an error for participants in this research study were much lower

than in real-life jury service, where it is likely that decisions would be accompanied by much more soul searching. It is questionable how far the results of this role-play can be generalised to real-life jury service.

Ethical issues

In this study, participants were not misled as to the nature of the task and were subjected to little by way of stress or discomfort during the role play. This means that the role play is much more ethically acceptable than previous experimental studies, which have involved stress and deception.

Explanations of independent behaviour

Gamson, Fireman and Rytina (1982)

Gamson et al wished to set up a situation in which participants were encouraged to rebel against an

unjust authority. The researchers placed an advert in the local papers in a town in Michigan, US,

asking for volunteers to take part in a paid group discussion on ‘standards of behaviour in the

community’. Those who responded were asked to attend a group discussion at a local Holiday Inn.

When they arrived they were put into groups of nine and met by a consultant from a fictional human

relations company called Manufacturers Human Relations Consultants (MHRC). The young man

explained that MHRC was conducting research for an oil company, which was taking legal action

against a petrol station manager. They argued that the manager had been sacked because his

lifestyle was offensive to the local community. In contrast, the manager argued that he had been

sacked for speaking out on local television against high petrol prices.

Participants were asked to take part in a group discussion about the sacking, and this was filmed. As

the discussion unfolded, it became apparent that the participants’ own views were irrelevant and

that the HR Company wanted them to argue in favour of the sacking. At a number of points during

the discussion, the cameraman stopped filming and instructed different members of the group to

argue in favour of the oil company’s decision to sack the manager. Finally, the participants were

asked to sign a consent form allowing the film to be shown in a court case.

Of the 33 groups tested by Gamson, 32 rebelled in some way during the group discussion. In 25 out

of the 33 groups the majority of group members refused to sign the consent form allowing the film

to be used in court. Nine groups even threatened legal action against MHRC. Rebellion against

authority in this context involved challenging two well-established social norms in the situation, the

norm of obedience and the norm of commitment, both of which participants had engaged in by

agreeing to take part in the study.

Evaluation of Gamson, Fireman and Rytina (1982)

Methodological issues

Gamson’s research had a high level of realism. While the situation itself was

rather unusual, participants’’ behaviour was likely to be free from demand

characteristics as they were unaware they were participating in a research

study.

It was difficult to separate the many factors that may have led to disobedience

in this study. Rebellion could have been influenced by a number of factors

including the high costs involved of being seen to lie on film in court as well as

the group nature of the decision. Both of these are likely to have contributed

to the high level of disobedience shown.

Ethical issues

A number of ethical issues were involved in this study. Participants were

deceived as to the nature of the exercise and did not give their fully informed

consent. In addition, the experience was exceedingly stressful for those who

took part.

7 features of abnormality

Rosenhan and Seligman (1989) suggest that the most suitable approach to defining mental

abnormality may be to identify a set of abnormal characteristics. Each of these on its own may not

be sufficient to cause a problem but, when several are present, then they are symptomatic of

abnormality. The fewer of the seven features of abnormality displayed by individuals in their

everyday lives, the more they can be regarded as normal. This approach allows us to think in degrees

of normality and abnormality, rather than simply making judgements about whether a behaviour or

a person is abnormal or not.

SUFFERING – Most abnormal individuals report that they are suffering, and so the presence

of suffering is a key feature of abnormality. However, it is not adequate on its own because,

for example, nearly all normal individuals grieve and suffer when a loved one dies. In

addition, some abnormal individuals (e.g. psychopaths or those with anti-social personality

disorder) treat other people very badly but do not seem to suffer themselves.

MALADAPTIVENESS – Maladaptive behaviour is behaviour that prevents an individual from

achieving major life goals such as enjoying good relationships with other people or working

effectively. Most abnormal behaviour is maladaptive in this sense. However, maladaptive

behaviour can occur because of an absence of relevant knowledge or skills as well as

because of abnormality.

VIVIDNESS & UNCONVENTIALITY OF BEHAVIOUR – Vivid and unconventional behaviour is

behaviour that is relatively unusual. The ways in which abnormal individuals behave in

various situations differ substantially from the ways in which we would expect most people

to behave in those situations. However, the same is true of non-conformists.

UNPREDICTABLITY AND LOSS OF CONTROL – Most people behave in a fairly predictable and

controlled way. In contrast, the behaviour of abnormal individuals is often very variable and

uncontrolled, and is inappropriate. However, most people can sometimes behave in

unpredictable and uncontrolled ways. IRRATIONALITY AND INCOMPREHENSIBILITY – A common feature of abnormal behaviour is

that it is not clear why anyone would choose to behave in that way. In other words, the

behaviour is irrational and incomprehensible. However, behaviour can seem

incomprehensible simply because we do not know the reasons for it. For example, a

migraine may cause someone to behave in ways that are incomprehensible to other people. OBSERVER DISCOMFORT – Our social behaviour is governed by a number of unspoken rules

of behaviour. These include maintaining reasonable eye contact with other people and not

standing too close to other people. Those who see these rules being broken often

experience some discomfort. Observer discomfort may reflect cultural differences in

behaviour and style rather than abnormality. For example, Arabs like to stand very close to

other people, and this can be disturbing to Europeans. VIOLATION OF MORAL AND IDEAL STANDARDS – Behaviour may be judged to be abnormal

when it violates moral standards, even when many or most people fail to maintain those

standards. For example, religious leaders have sometimes claimed that masturbation is

wicked and abnormal, in spite of the fact that it is widespread.

Evaluation of Rosenhan and Seligman (1989)

One of the greatest problems with the seven features of abnormality proposed by

Rosenhan and Seligman is that most of them involve making subjective judgements.

Behaviour that causes severe discomfort to one observer may have no effect on

another observer, and behaviour that violates one person’s moral standards may be

consistent with another person’s moral standards. (an example might be

vegetarianism).

Another problem with some of the proposed features of abnormality is that they

can also apply to people who are non-conformists or who simply have their own

idiosyncratic style.

This issue was addressed in the introduction to the 3rd revised version of the

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM III-R), published in 1987.

“Neither deviant behaviour, e.g. political, religious, or sexual, nor conflicts

that are primarily between the individual and society are mental disorders

unless the deviance or condition is a symptom of a dysfunction

(i.e. impairment of function) in the person.”

This does support the concept of “dysfunction”

that is subjective but in a way that allows us to

view the experience of mental disorder from the

point of view of the person experiencing it.

TASK:

With a learning partner, search for

two strengths and two weaknesses

of this definition of abnormality.

Allow time for you to state your

point, explain what the point means

and then for you to apply your point

(say WHY it is a strength or a

weakness).

You can also suggest a way in which

this definition of abnormality could

be improved.

The characteristics of mental health

We define physical health in terms of the presence of healthy behaviours, such as normal body

temperature and normal skin colour. Why not do the same for mental illness? We can consider

abnormality as the absence of normality. In fact, Marie Jahoda (1958) argued that the concepts of

abnormality and normality were useless because they rely on the identification of a reference

population. She suggested that it was preferable to identify the criteria for positive mental health

and then look at the frequency of their distribution in any population.

Jahoda wrote a report for the Joint Commission on Mental Illness and Health. She drew on the views

of others and tried to identify common concepts that were used when describing mental health. In

her report she proposed that there were six categories that clinicians typically related to mental

health.

SELF-ATTITUDES – High self-esteem and a strong sense of identity are related to mental

health.

PERSONAL GROWTH – The extent of an individual’s growth, development or self-

actualisation is important. These criteria are not concerned with one’s self perception but

more with what a person does over a period of time.

INTERGRATION – This is a “Synthesising psychological function”, the extent to which the

above two concepts are integrated. It can be assessed in terms of the individual’s ability to

cope with stressful situations.

AUTONOMY –The degree to which an individual is independent of social influences and able

to regulate his or herself.

PERCEPTION OF REALITY – Many clinicians identify the link with reality as a prime factor in

mental healthiness. This includes being free from “need-distortion” (an individual’s need to

distort their perception of reality) and demonstrating empathy and social sensitivity.

ENVIRONMENTAL MASTERY – The extent to which an individual is successful and well-

adapted. This includes the ability to love, adequacy at work and play, adequacy in

interpersonal relations, efficiency in meeting situational requirements, capacity for

adaptation and adjustment, and efficient problem solving.

TASK:

Which of the above categories might be affected

by cultural bias? In what way might they be

affected? With a learning partner, give an

example of a cultural bias for each category.

How might the “environmental mastery”

category be criticised?

Evaluation of Jahoda (1958)

This approach has the benefit of being positive. It seeks to identify the characteristics that

people need to be mentally healthy rather than identifying the problems (i.e. ill health). As

such it could be translated into useful therapeutic aims – goals to set during treatment.

However, it may not provide useful criteria for identifying what constitutes abnormality.

When we use signs of physical health to identify physical ill health, we rely on fairly objective

measurements, such as blood pressure. The same is not true when we are using

psychological concepts. A psychological scale that measures a person’s self-esteem can

never be an objective measurement.

A further problem is that any set of values is inevitably culture-bound; related to the specific

historic period and the views of a particular society. Nobles (1976) wrote about the

extended concept of self in African people. He claimed that they have a sense of “we”

instead of the Western “me”. This means that they value co-operation whereas Westerners

value independence and autonomy, as reflected in Jahoda’s list of mentally healthy

behaviours.

Different cultures may view the themes of The Simpsons in different ways.

TASK: In what way does the above article make Marie Jahoda a more credible source?

Key features of the behavioural approach to psychopathology

Watson and Rayner (1920)

In one of the most celebrated if unethical studies in psychology, Watson and Rayner (1920)

classically conditioned an 11-month old child, since known as Little Albert, to fear fluffy animals.

They did this by pairing presentation of a tame white rat with a sudden loud noise. The noise caused

fear, an unconditional reflex equivalent to salivation in Pavlov’s experiment, while the rat was the

equivalent of the bell. Eventually Albert was conditioned to associate the rat with fear. Little Albert

also became afraid of other fluffy objects similar to the white rat such as a rabbit and white dog; this

is known as stimulus generalisation.

Methodological issues

We should also note that this was a single case study and there was no systematic and objective

measure of any signs of ‘fear’; instead Watson and Rayner relied on general verbal descriptions.

Ethical issues

Besides the serious ethical issue of scaring a young child and causing psychological harm,

Watson and Rayner did not de-condition Little Albert as his mother, with Watson and Rayner’s

knowledge, removed him from the research programme.

Biological and psychological therapies

Elkin et al (1989)

This was a study across several treatment centres. In all 240 patients with depression were treated

with either CBT, psychotherapy or anti-depressant drugs. There was also a placebo control group.

Treatment lasted for 16 weeks. The findings were:

There was a large placebo effect of 35-40%.

All therapies were significantly more effective than placebo, and overall had similar levels of

effectiveness

Drugs tended to be the most effective therapy for severe depression.

The individual therapist was a significant factor in the effectiveness of psychotherapy.

Across all treatment groups, 30-40% of patients did not respond to therapy. It is a common

finding in studies of effectiveness that no treatment is ever 100% effective.

The conclusions were that drugs, CBT and psychotherapy are all more effective that placebo in

treating depression. Also note that the follow-up was only 16 weeks. Ideally patients should be

followed up for 6-12 months, as there is evidence that the therapeutic effect of CBT in anxiety

conditions is longer lasting than the effect of drugs (Bechdolf et al 2006)

Biological and psychological therapies

Davidson et al (2004)

295 patients with generalised social anxiety (fear of social situations) were treated either with CBT,

with the SSRI antidepressant fluoxetine, or with combined CBT + fluoxetine.

The findings were:

The overall placebo effect was 19%

All therapies were effective over and above the placebo effect, and after 14 weeks there

were no differences between the therapy groups. The combined therapy was not superior to

either therapy alone.

40-50% of patients did not respond to therapy.

The conclusions were that drugs and CBT are equally effective in treating social anxiety, and

combining them does not improve their effectiveness. However many patients do not respond to

either treatment.

Studies comparing treatments – methodological issues

Elkin and Davidson are not conventional experiments. They do have an independent

variable (the different treatments) and a dependent variable (the effect of the

treatment, measured by improvement).

The separate patient groups to be given the different treatments should be

matched in terms of severity of the disorder. It would also be desirable if they

were matched on other characteristics, such as age, gender, socioeconomic

status. This is rarely possible, but these factors have been shown to influence,

for instance, depression.

The length of the study should be sufficient for treatment effects to be

observable. Even with drugs effects can take weeks to develop. Ideally

observation should continue for at least a year to check that any improvement

is sustained and not temporary.

There should be a non-treated group to control for the specific effects of

treatment. With drugs this is a placebo group given a non-active substance they

think is the drug. For psychological therapies it is more difficult. Often an

‘interaction’ condition is used where participants talk to the therapist but there

is no attempt to apply scientific techniques such as CBT or free association. This

controls for the effects of being given attention by a therapist.

Measurement of improvement should be consistent and thorough across the

groups. Questionnaires can be given to participants, and there should also be

ratings of clinical improvement by qualified staff; these staff should not know

what treatment group the participants is in to prevent bias and investigator

effects.

Studies comparing treatments – ethical issues

These studies should be subject to the same ethical criteria as psychological

experiments. For instance, a major issue in the study of psychological disorders is

informed consent; people with disorders may be less able to understand the full

consequences of a treatment. Avoidance of psychological harm is also important,

and debriefing should be carried out.

An issue specific to studies evaluating treatments is that a non-treated control

group is used. If treatments are effective, the control group is denied help. As a

control group is essential to measure improvement this is unavoidable, and is

justified by the hope of identifying the most effective treatment.

The picture that emerges is that in terms of general effectiveness, no one

therapy if consistently the best and this is particularly the case for

depression and general anxiety. However there are other considerations

that can help decide the most appropriate treatment.

For accessibility and speed of action, i.e. how easy is it to provide

treatment and how quickly it works, drug therapy stands out. Drugs are

readily available for most disorders, and although it usually takes a few

days for the therapeutic effect to appear, this is faster than for CBT,

behavioural and psychodynamic therapies.