25
This article was downloaded by: [University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign] On: 17 April 2013, At: 13:47 Publisher: Taylor & Francis Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Spatial Cognition & Computation: An Interdisciplinary Journal Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hscc20 Effects of Basic Path Properties on Human Path Integration Xiaoang Wan a , Ranxiao Frances Wang b & James A. Crowell b a Tsinghua University, Beijing, China b University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA Accepted author version posted online: 23 Apr 2012.Version of record first published: 14 Jan 2013. To cite this article: Xiaoang Wan , Ranxiao Frances Wang & James A. Crowell (2013): Effects of Basic Path Properties on Human Path Integration, Spatial Cognition & Computation: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13:1, 79-101 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2012.678521 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable

bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    1

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

This article was downloaded by: [University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign]On: 17 April 2013, At: 13:47Publisher: Taylor & FrancisInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH,UK

Spatial Cognition &Computation: AnInterdisciplinary JournalPublication details, including instructions forauthors and subscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/hscc20

Effects of Basic Path Propertieson Human Path IntegrationXiaoang Wan a , Ranxiao Frances Wang b & James A.Crowell ba Tsinghua University, Beijing, Chinab University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign,Champaign, Illinois, USAAccepted author version posted online: 23 Apr2012.Version of record first published: 14 Jan 2013.

To cite this article: Xiaoang Wan , Ranxiao Frances Wang & James A. Crowell (2013):Effects of Basic Path Properties on Human Path Integration, Spatial Cognition &Computation: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13:1, 79-101

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13875868.2012.678521

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes.Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan,sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone isexpressly forbidden.

The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make anyrepresentation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up todate. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae, and drug doses should beindependently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable

Page 2: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand, or costs or damageswhatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connectionwith or arising out of the use of this material.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 3: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Spatial Cognition & Computation, 13:79–101, 2013

Copyright © Taylor & Francis Group, LLC

ISSN: 1387-5868 print/1542-7633 online

DOI: 10.1080/13875868.2012.678521

Effects of Basic Path Properties on

Human Path Integration

Xiaoang Wan,1 Ranxiao Frances Wang,2 and James A. Crowell2

1Tsinghua University, Beijing, China2University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, Illinois, USA

Abstract: We investigated how path integration performance can be influenced by

five basic path properties in a Virtual Reality Cube. Participants performed path-

completion tasks in hallway paths with up to 12 segments. Distance information was

visual, whereas turning angles were specified through vision and body senses. The

ridge regression analyses suggested that path integration was affected by the number

of segments, overall path length/turning angles, and the correct homing distance.

Moreover, an un-correlation paradigm showed that path completion performance might

be affected by participants’ expectations for the correct homing distance of different

paths. Implications on models of path integration were discussed.

Keywords: path integration, virtual reality, path properties, spatial updating

1. INTRODUCTION

Moving animals can integrate information regarding self-motion, such as

velocity and acceleration information, to estimate their current position and

orientation relative to the starting point of their travel, a phenomenon re-

ferred to as path integration (Etienne, 1992; Gallistel, 1990; Mittelstaedt

& Mittelstaedt, 1982). A variety of species have been observed to show

path integration ability, including insects (Collett & Collett, 2000; Müller

& Wehner, 1988; Wehner & Srinivasan, 1981), birds (Regolin, Vallortigara,

& Zanforlin, 1995; von Saint Paul, 1982), and mammals (Etienne, 1992;

Mittelstaedt & Mittelstaedt, 1982) including humans (Klatzky et al., 1990;

Loomis et al., 1993).

Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan,

Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences, Tsinghua University, Beijing,

China 100084. E-mail: [email protected]

79

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 4: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

80 X. Wan et al.

Humans’ path integration ability has been directly measured in path

completion tasks (e.g., Klatzky et al., 1990; Loomis et al., 1993), or in-

directly assessed in other spatial tasks (e.g., Allen, Kirasic, Dobson, Long,

& Beck, 1996; Passini, Proulx, & Rainville, 1990). In the path completion

tasks, participants are asked to directly return to the starting point of their

travel without the aid of direct perceptual cues of the paths after they have

traveled along several segments, made several turns at the intersections of

the segments, and arrived at the end of the paths. When there are only two

segments in the outbound paths, this task can be also referred to as a triangle

completion task.

Humans can use different types of information regarding self-motion to

perform path completion tasks, including purely internal information from

the vestibular, proprioceptive and efferent systems (Klatzky et al., 1990;

Loomis et al., 1993; Wiener, Berthoz, & Wolbers, 2011), purely external

information such as optic flow (Ellmore & McNaughton, 2004; Péruch, May,

& Wartenberg, 1997; Riecke, van Veen, & Bülthoff, 2002; Wiener & Mallot,

2006), or a mixture of both (Kearns, Warren, Duchon, & Tarr, 2002; Wan,

Wang, & Crowell, 2010).

Previous studies have suggested that human path completion performance

can be affected by at least five basic properties of the outbound paths, includ-

ing the number of segments, overall path length and overall turning angle,

and the correct distance and direction responses participants need to make to

return to the origin. First, Klatzky and colleagues (1990) demonstrated that

blind or blindfolded sight participants showed longer reaction times (RTs) and

greater turning errors when the number of segment within each outbound path

increased from one to three. However, In Wiener and Mallot (2006)’s study,

participants were passively and visually guided along 2-, 3-, 4-, and 5-segment

paths in virtual environments and then pointed to the direction of the origin,

with the overall travel distance and overall turning angles being constant for

every outbound path. Interestingly, participants showed longer RTs to point

to the origin for 2- and 3-segment trials than for 4- and 5-segment trials. They

also showed larger unsigned pointing errors for 2-segment paths than for 3-,

4-, and 5-segment paths, although this effect was eliminated when researchers

excluded those 2-segment trials in which the correct turning angle was 140

degrees. In short, participants showed longer RTs for paths containing fewer

segments than paths containing more segments. These results suggested that

the number of segments, overall path length, and overall turning angles might

interact to influence human path integration.

In addition, Wiener and colleagues (2011) showed that the influence of

overall path length on path completion may be modulated by task demands.

They asked their participants to perform triangle completion tasks but giving

them different instructions. Specifically, they asked some participants to re-

member the configuration of the outbound path and to calculate the homing

vectors on the basis of their representation of the path, so these partici-

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 5: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 81

pants were instructed to perform configural updating. Other participants were

asked to continuously update the location of the starting point, so they were

instructed to perform continuous updating. Also, although all the outbound

paths were 2-segment paths, the overall length of the paths were manipulated

as short or long (8.3 or 15.3 m on average, respectively). Accordingly,

participants performing continuous updating showed shorter RTs to turn to

face the origin than those performing configural updating. On the other hand,

participants performing configural updating showed more accurate homing

performance than those performing continuous updating for long paths, but

the two groups showed comparable performance for short paths.

Human path completion performance has also been shown to depend on

the Euclidean distance between the starting and ending points of the paths,

which is referred to as the correct homing distance, and the turn participants

need to make to return to the origin, which is referred to as the correct homing

angle (Fujita, Klatzky, Loomis, & Golledge, 1993; Loomis et al., 1993;

Klatzsky et al., 1997, 1999). For example, Loomis and colleagues (1993)

guided blind or blindfolded sighted participants to perform the triangle path

completion task. They varied the length of each segment (2, 4, or 6 m) and the

inner angle between the two segments (60, 90, or 120 degrees) to obtain 27

different path configurations with varied correct homing distance and correct

homing angles. They found that, when the correct homing distance/angle

increased, the observed response distance/angle also increased, but with a

slope smaller than 1 and a positive intercept. These results suggested that

participants overestimated the small values and underestimated the large

values for both distance and direction responses.

However, two issues remained unsolved and more studies are needed

to examine the influence of path properties on human path completion per-

formance. First, to understand the mechanism of human path integration,

one needs to have a comprehensive picture on which factors affect people’s

performance. The number of segments, overall path length, overall turning

angle, correct homing distance, and correct turning angle may all influence

path completion performance. These factors are generally correlated with

each other, and therefore can confound the analyses unless all of them

are taken into consideration simultaneously. However, in previous studies,

researchers primarily focused on the effects of the number of segments

without controlling both overall path length/turning angle and correct homing

distance/turning angle at the same time (Klatzky et al., 1990; Wiener &

Mallot, 2006). In the current study, we included all five factors in our analyses

to examine how they affect path integration. Second, most of previous studies

focus on relatively short outbound paths which contained no more than

5 segments, whereas path integration is quite common with long paths in

real situations. Our goal is to examine the effects of basic path properties

on human path integration under more general situations by using paths

containing 2 to 12 segments.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 6: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

82 X. Wan et al.

2. EXPERIMENT 1

In this experiment, we manipulated the number of segments to examine how

path completion can be influenced by the number of segments, overall path

length, overall turning angle, correct homing distance, and correct turning

angle. Participants traveled through 2-, 4-, 8-, and 12-segment paths and then

attempted to directly return to the starting point of each outbound path.

2.1. Methods

2.1.1. Participants. Fifteen students (4 female) from the University of Illinois

at Urbana-Champaign (UIUC) participated in this experiment. Participants ei-

ther were paid or received course credits to fulfill an introductory psychology

class requirement. They all reported to have normal or corrected-to-normal

visual acuity and normal stereo vision.

2.1.2. Apparatus. The experiment took place in a Virtual Reality (VR) Cube

at the UIUC. Driven by a cluster of computers, the Cube produces stereo-

scopic imagery at 60 frames per second per eye on all six surfaces, each

of which is a 3 m � 3 m rear-projection screen. In other words, this VR

Cube is a 3 m � 3 m � 3 m cubic room in which participants could

make body rotations freely, but their translational movements are restricted

by the size of the room. Participants’ position and orientation in the VR

Cube were tracked by Ascension MotionStar Wireless tracking system. A

Stereographics LCD shutter-glass system was used to provide active stereo,

and binocular images were produced on the basis of each individual’s inter-

ocular distance measured before the experiment started. An Intel Wireless

Series Gamepad was used by the participants to interact with the virtual

environments.

2.1.3. The Virtual Environment. Hallway mazes were used, and each maze

consisted of 2, 4, 8, or 12 segments. As shown in Figure 1, outbound and

homing paths were specified by brown-and-white ceramic pattern and sandy

yellow rocky pattern, respectively. These patterns were selected to make the

seams of the VR Cube invisible. Each hallway was 1 m wide, 2.2 m high, 2

or 3 m long. Both the beginning and the end of each hallway were in circular

shapes with 0.6-m radius, and the circular end of the N hallway was also

the circular beginning of the .N C 1/ hallway. The turning angle at each

intersection of two connecting hallways was clockwise 60 deg, clockwise

120 deg, counterclockwise 60 deg, or counterclockwise 120 deg.

When an outbound path was generated by the computer, the length of

each hallway and the turning angle at each intersection was randomly chosen

from all the options mentioned above with the constraint that there should

be no cross-over between any two hallways. When participants traveled in

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 7: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 83

Figure 1. Examples of virtual displays used in this study. (A) During the outbound

travel, a hallway appeared, and the participants pressed a button to drive along it. (B)

During the response stage, a 1,000-m long hallway appeared in the direction that the

participants had selected, and they pressed a button to drive along it until they judged

to be at the origin of the outbound path (color figure available online).

these mazes, they obtained information about distance purely through optic

flow, as they kept their bodies still and pressed a button on the game pad to

drive along each hallway at a constant speed of 1.5 m per second. In contrast,

they obtained information about rotation through both optic flow and body

senses, as they actually rotated their bodies at their own speed when they

needed to make a turn. It should also be noted that only one segment was

presented at a time. It is possible that participants attempted to track localized

features to help with their self-motion estimation when they traveled along

one segment, but no localized features could function as landmarks to allow

for landmark-based navigation in the current study.

2.1.4. Procedure. At the beginning of each trial, a hallway was presented in

a random direction, and participants were instructed to physically remain still

and to press a button to drive along. When they arrived at the end of this

hallway, the next hallway was presented. A red arrow pointing to the left or

right appeared in front of the participants to indicate the direction of the next

hallway, and they were instructed to physically turn around their bodies to

face it. This process was repeated until they arrived at the end of the outbound

path. At that time, the response stage started immediately accompanied by the

wallpaper change, and participants were asked to directly return to the origin

of the path. They first made a direction response by turning around their

bodies to face the origin and pressed a button. Then a 1000-m long hallway

appeared in the selected direction, and they made a distance response by

pressing a button to drive and stopping when they judged to be at the origin.

Each participant was encouraged to complete as many trials as possible in 30

minutes. For every four trials they needed to finish one of each 2-, 4-, 8-, and

12-segment path, while the order of these trials was randomly determined.

As a result, participants completed 27 to 60 trials.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 8: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

84 X. Wan et al.

Figure 2. An illustration of the path completion task. Participants start from H ,

travel along four segments to arrive at A, and then attempt to directly return to H .

To successfully return to H , they should turn ˇ degrees (unsigned correct turning

angle) and travel for distance c (correct distance). However, their actual responses

might be to turn ˇr degrees, travel for distance cr , and arrive at a new location

Hr . We assessed the accuracy of their path completion by calculating position errors

(Euclidean distances between H and Hr ), unsigned distance errors .jcr � cj/, signed

distance errors .cr � c/, unsigned direction errors .jˇr � ˇj/, and signed direction

errors .ˇr � ˇ/.

2.1.5. Data Analysis. Participants’ RTs to make the direction responses and

their direction responses as well as distance responses were recorded. We used

five parameters to characterize each outbound path, including the number of

segments, overall path length, overall turning angle, correct distance, and

correct turning angles. As shown in Figure 2, we also used six measures

to assess path completion performance. We assessed overall path completion

performance by the RTs and position errors (Euclidean distances between the

actual origins and observed endpoints of the distance responses). Longer RTs

and/or greater position errors indicate poorer path completion performance,

whereas shorter RTs and/or smaller position errors indicate better path com-

pletion performance.1 The accuracies of direction responses were assessed by

unsigned and signed difference between the response angles and the correct

angles, referred to as the unsigned direction errors and the signed direction

1Given the potential tradeoffs between RT and accuracy, the difference in

performance may manifest itself in either RT or accuracy or both depending on

whether the participants emphasized on speed, or accuracy, or in-between, therefore

the exact theoretical meaning of the effects in these two types of measures is difficult

to assess.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 9: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 85

errors, respectively. The accuracies of distance responses were assessed by

unsigned and signed difference between the response distance and correct

distance, referred to as the unsigned distance errors and the signed distance

errors, respectively. For the unsigned direction and distance errors, greater

values indicate lower accuracies and smaller values indicate higher accuracies.

For the signed distance and direction errors, positive values indicate overes-

timation and negative values indicate underestimation. Note that the signed

direction and distance errors measure the systematic biases and are reflected

in the accuracy measures (unsigned direction and distance errors), which

include contributions from both these systematic errors (constant errors) and

variable errors.

2.2. Results

The results of this experiment are shown in Figure 3. To examine which

factors have influence on the path completion performance, we used five

independent variables, including the number of segments, overall path length,

overall turning angle, correct homing distance, and correct turning angles, to

predict the RTs and different types of path completion errors. As can be seen

in Table 1, these five independent variables were highly correlated with each

other. To minimize the influence of multicollinearity on the results in ordinary

multivariate regression, we performed ridge regressions (Hoerl & Kennard,

1970), examined the ridge trace and chose the smallest biasing constant k for

each model when all the regression coefficients stopped changing signs from

Table 1. Intercorrelations between number of segments, overall path length, overall

turning angle, correct homing distance, and correct turning angle in Experiments 1

and 2

1. 2. 3. 4. 5.

Experiment 1 .n D 15/

1. Number of segments — .999** .998** .976** �.738**

2. Overall path length — .997** .976** �.724**

3. Overall turning angle — .969** �.737**

4. Correct homing distance — �.674**

5. Correct turning angle —

Experiment 2 .n D 15/

1. Number of segments — .994** .985** 0 �.723**

2. Overall path length — .976** .032 �.718**

3. Overall turning angle — �.025 �.674**

4. Correct homing distance — .165

5. Correct turning angle —

Note. **indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:01 level.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 10: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

86 X. Wan et al.

Figure 3. The results of Experiments 1. The RTs and position errors are shown in

Panels A and B, respectively. Unsigned and signed direction errors are shown in Panels

C and D, respectively. Unsigned and signed distance errors are shown in Panels E

and F, respectively.

positive to negative or vice versa and ridge trace became smooth and steady.2

The data showed that the number of segments, overall path length, and overall

turning angle could predict the position errors, R2 D :61, F.5; 54/ D 17:18,

p < :001, and unsigned direction errors, R2 D :22, F.5; 54/ D 3:01, p <

:05. Moreover, the number of segments, overall path length, overall turning

2Given the high correlations among these factors, their independent contributions

may only be separated partially, even with the ridge regression analysis.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 11: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 87

angle, and correct homing distance could predict the unsigned distance errors,

R2 D :37, F.5; 54/ D 6:35, p < :001. The regression coefficients in these

ridge regression models are shown in Table 2.

The significant positive regression coefficients indicate that position er-

rors, unsigned direction errors, and unsigned distance errors increased when

the number of segments, overall path length, or overall turning angle in-

creased. Moreover, the unsigned distance errors also increased when the cor-

Table 2. Ridge regression models chosen for Experiment 1

Dependent

variable

Independent

variable k b ˇ t p

RTs Number of segments .35 �.002 �.01 .38 .71

Overall path length �.002 .04 .97 .34

Overall turning angle 0 0 — —

Correct homing distance �.01 �.09 1.37 .18

Correct homing angle .002 .07 .71 .48

Position errors Number of segments .10 .20 .15 3.37 **

Overall path length .14 .26 5.15 ***

Overall turning angle .003 .19 3.16 **

Correct homing distance .16 .12 1.15 .26

Correct homing angle �.03 �.08 .82 .41

Unsigned direction Number of segments .45 .53 .10 3.25 **

errors Overall path length .27 .13 3.94 ***

Overall turning angle .007 .11 3.26 **

Correct homing distance .004 .001 .01 .99

Correct homing angle �.13 �.10 1.17 .25

Unsigned distance Number of segments .20 .08 .10 2.72 **

errors Overall path length .04 .14 3.26 **

Overall turning angle .001 .10 2.17 *

Correct homing distance .13 .17 2.04 *

Correct homing angle �.02 �.10 .95 .34

Signed direction Number of segments .20 .63 .10 2.23 *

errors Overall path length .34 .14 2.72 **

Overall turning angle .01 .09 1.53 .13

Correct homing distance �1.25 �.21 2.13 *

Correct homing angle �.05 .03 .24 .81

Signed distance Number of segments .15 �.10 �.08 1.59 .12

errors Overall path length �.01 �.02 .36 .71

Overall turning angle 0 �.004 .08 .94

Correct homing distance �.21 �.19 1.55 .13

Correct homing angle �.04 �.14 .94 .15

Note. *indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:05 level.

**indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:01 level.

***indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:001 level.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 12: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

88 X. Wan et al.

rect homing distance increased. In contrast, the RTs, signed direction errors,

and signed distance errors could not be well predicted by our multivariate

regression models, all R2 < :07, all F s < 1.3 However, as can be seen in

Table 2, we found significant individual regression coefficients for the signed

direction errors (positive for the number of segments and overall path length

but negative for the correct homing distance), although the overall model

was not significant. Furthermore, note the mean unsigned direction errors for

2-, 4-, 8-, and 12-segment paths were 28 deg, 46 deg, 52 deg, and 56 deg,

respectively, all of which were smaller than the unsigned direction errors at

the chance level (90 deg), all ts > 5, suggesting that participants’ performance

was above chance level.

2.3. Discussion

In this experiment, we varied the number of segments to be 2, 4, 8, or 12 and

examined how path completion were influenced by the number of segments,

overall path length, overall turning angle, correct homing distance, and correct

turning angle. First, we found that position errors, unsigned direction errors,

and unsigned distance errors increased when the number of segments, overall

path length, or overall turning angle increased. These results suggest that

path integration was impaired by the increase in the number of segments

and the length of the trip (overall path length and overall turning angle).

Second, participants’ RTs were not affected by any of the five variables.

Third, we found that unsigned distance errors increased when the correct

homing distance increased. Specifically, when participants were further away

from the origin of the paths, they showed less accurate distance responses,

but we did not find such patterns for position errors or unsigned direction

errors.4 Last, participants’ systematic biases (signed distance and direction

errors) could not be well predicted by the ridge regression models.

Note that the five basic properties of the outbound path were quite highly

correlated, as they do in some real life situations such as city-block navigation.

Experiment 1 addressed this intrinsic problem by taking the correlation as is

and using statistical tools (e.g., ridge regression) to disentangle their effects.

This statistical approach has the advantage of preserving the characteristics

of the task in its natural condition, but has the disadvantage of limited power

3There was a marginally significant negative correlation between the signed

distance errors and the correct homing distance .p D :06/, as shown in Figure 3.

However the ridge regression data suggested that the correct homing distance did not

have independent contribution to the signed distance errors.4Note that in this experiment, position errors could not be well predicted by

the correct homing distance. Position errors are affected by both distance errors and

angular errors, but the relationship is not simple addition. Thus, an increase in either

distance errors or in direction errors, or in both, does not always result in an increase

in position errors.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 13: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 89

when the correlation is high. Thus, Experiment 2 was conducted by taking

the alternative approach of un-correlating the number of segment and the

correct homing distance in the experimental design. This approach examined

only a subset of special trial types, but with much higher power. Together

the two approaches may provide complimentary and converging evidence on

the effects of various basic path properties on path integration performance.

3. EXPERIMENT 2

In this experiment, we manipulated the number of hallways and correct

homing distance to make these two variables uncorrelated, and then examined

how the number of segments, overall path length, overall turning angle,

correct homing distance, and correct turning angle influence path completion.

Participants traveled through 4- or 8-segment paths and then attempted to

directly return to the starting point of each outbound path, while the Euclidean

distance between the starting and ending points of each path might be 3, 6,

or 9 meters.

3.1. Methods

Fifteen UIUC students (9 female) participated in this experiment. None of

them participated in Experiment 1. The methods of this experiment were the

same as those of Experiment 1 except the following. First, a 2 (number of

segments, 4 or 8) � 3 (correct homing distance, 3, 6, or 9 m) orthogonal

design was used in this experiment. That is, we only used 4- and 8-segment

outbound paths for which the correct distance was fixed to 3, 6, or 9 m.

Although the length of each hallway was 2 or 3 m and there was no cross-

over between any two hallways as in Experiment 1, the turning angles at each

intersection in this experiment ranged from 30 to 150 degrees, clockwise or

counterclockwise, to make it possible to generate the desired path types.

A pair of 4-segment and 8-segment paths with constant correct distance is

shown in Figure 4. Second, a random order of the 6 trial types was generated,

and this process repeated for as many times as needed to produce a random

sequence of trials for each participant. All participants were encouraged to

complete as many trials as possible in 40 minutes, and eventually completed

22 to 95 trials.

3.2. Results

The results of this experiment are shown in Figure 5. As can be seen in

Table 1, the number of segments, overall path length, overall turning angle,

and correct turning angle were highly correlated with each other, whereas

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 14: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

90 X. Wan et al.

Figure 4. An illustration of a pair of 4-segment and 8-segment outbound paths in

Experiment 2. In the former case, participants start from H , travel along four segments

to arrive at A4; whereas in the latter case, they start from H , travel along eight

segments to arrive at A8. These two paths generate the same correct distance, that is,

c4 (Euclidean distance between H and A4) is equal to c8 (Euclidean distance between

H and A8).

none of them was correlated with correct homing distance. To minimize

the influence of multicollinearity on the results in ordinary multivariate re-

gression, we performed ridge regressions on all the measures as we did in

Experiment 1. The data showed that position errors could be predicted by the

number of segments, overall path length, and overall turning angle, R2 D :22,

F.5; 84/ D 4:76, p < :001. Unsigned direction errors could be predicted by

the number of segments, overall path length, overall turning angle, and correct

homing distance, R2 D :33, F.5; 84/ D 8:31, p < :001. Unsigned distance

errors could be predicted by the number of segments and correct homing

distance, R2 D :11, F.5; 84/ D 1:99, p D :088. Moreover, signed distance

errors could also be predicted by the number of segments and correct homing

distance, R2 D :25, F.5; 84/ D 5:55, p < :001. In contrast, the RTs and

signed direction errors could not be well predicted by our ridge regression

models, both R2 < :08, both F s < 1.45, p > :21, although some of the

individual coefficients were significant as shown in Table 3. Taken together,

the significant positive regression coefficients indicate that position errors and

unsigned direction errors increased when the number of segments, overall path

length, or overall turning angle increased; the significant negative regression

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 15: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 91

Figure 5. The results of Experiments 1. The RTs and position errors are shown in

Panels A and B, respectively. Unsigned and signed direction errors are shown in

Panels C and D. Unsigned and signed distance errors are shown in Panels E and F.

Error bars show standard errors of the mean.

coefficients indicate that unsigned direction errors, unsigned distance errors,

and signed distance errors decreased when correct homing distance increased.

To examine whether participants were sensitive to the correct homing

distance, we further performed a 2 (number of segments, 4 or 8) � 3 (correct

distance, 3, 6, or 9 m) Analyses of Variances (ANOVAs) on the observed

distance responses. The main effect of the number of segments was signif-

icant, F.1; 14/ D 11:20, p < :01, suggesting that participants made shorter

distance responses for 4-segment paths (7.93 m) than for the 8-segment paths

(9.87 m). The main effect of correct homing distance was also significant,

F.2; 28/ D 6:92, p < :01. Planned pair-wise comparisons with Bonferroni

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 16: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

92 X. Wan et al.

Table 3. Ridge regression models chosen for Experiment 2

Dependent

variable

Independent

variable k b ˇ t p

RTs Number of segments .20 �.004 �.01 .23 .82

Overall path length �.007 �.05 .89 .38

Overall turning angle �.001 �.13 1.88 .06

Correct homing distance .03 .08 .94 .35

Correct homing angle �.002 �.05 .49 .62

Position errors Number of segments .25 .27 .17 4.34 ***

Overall path length .11 .17 3.78 ***

Overall turning angle .003 .13 2.40 *

Correct homing distance .001 .0004 .01 .996

Correct homing angle .01 .07 .84 .40

Unsigned direction Number of segments .50 1.17 .09 3.37 **

errors Overall path length .64 .13 4.36 ***

Overall turning angle .02 .10 2.83 **

Correct homing distance �3.18 �.31 5.24 ***

Correct homing angle �.001 �.001 .02 .98

Unsigned distance Number of segments .50 .14 .10 2.88 **

errors Overall path length .04 .07 1.90 .06

Overall turning angle .001 .06 1.40 .17

Correct homing distance �.19 �.15 2.18 *

Correct homing angle .00 �.001 .01 .99

Signed direction Number of segments .35 .80 .05 1.23 .22

errors Overall path length .26 .04 .93 .36

Overall turning angle .01 .03 .66 .51

Correct homing distance �2.75 �.19 2.48 *

Correct homing angle �.003 .002 .02 .98

Signed distance Number of segments .10 .30 .15 2.27 *

errors Overall path length .03 .03 .40 .69

Overall turning angle .001 .04 .39 .69

Correct homing distance �.68 �.40 4.63 ***

Correct homing angle 0 �.005 .04 .96

Note. *indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:05 level.

**indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:01 level.

***indicates significance at the ˛ D 0:001 level.

corrections for multiple tests showed that participants made smaller distance

responses for 3 m condition (8.14 m) and 6 m condition (8.94 m) than for

9 m condition (9.63 m), both ts > 2.60, p < :062, although their distance

responses for 3 m and 6 m conditions were comparable, t.14/ D 1:93,

p D :23. The interaction effect of these two factors was not significant,

F.2; 28/ D 1:18, p D :32. To sum up, participants were sensitive to the

difference between short (3 m and 6 m) and long (9 m) correct homing

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 17: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 93

distances, but they were not sensitive to the difference between 3 m and

6 m conditions. In addition, the mean unsigned direction errors for 4- and

8-segment trials were 54 deg and 71 deg, respectively, both of which were

smaller than the unsigned direction errors at chance level (90 deg), both ts >

4), suggesting that participants’ direction responses were well above chance

level.

3.3. Discussion

In this experiment, we systematically manipulated the number of segments (4

or 8) and correct homing distance (3, 6, or 9 m) to make them uncorrelated,

and once again examined how path integration can be influenced by the

number of segments, overall path length, overall turning angle, correct homing

distance, and correct turning angle. First, we found that position errors and

unsigned direction errors increased when the number of segments, overall

path length, and overall turning angle increased. Second, participants’ RTs

could not be well predicted by the ridge regression models which include

these five variables. Both of these results are consistent with what we found

in Experiment 1. Third, we found that unsigned distance errors increased

when the number of segments increased, but they could not be predicted by

the increase in overall path length and overall turning angle as in Experiment

1. Instead, as can be seen in Figure 5, unsigned distance errors decreased

when the correct homing distance increased. This pattern was different from

that in Experiment 1 in which unsigned distance errors increased when correct

homing distance increased. Last, we found that participants were sensitive to

the difference between short (3 m and 6 m) and long (9 m) correct homing

distances, but they were not sensitive to the difference between 3 m and 6 m

conditions. The theoretical implications of these results and the underlying

mechanisms were further discussed next.

4. GENERAL DISCUSSION

In this study, we examined the contributions of the number of segments,

overall path length, overall turning angle, correct homing distance, and correct

turning angle to human path completion performance. In Experiment 1,

participants traveled along 2-, 4-, 8-, and 12-segment paths and then attempted

to directly return to the origin of the path. The five basic path properties

were highly correlated and ridge regressions were used to partially assess the

contributions of each path property. In Experiment 2, participants traveled

along 4- and 8-segment paths and then attempted to directly return to the

origin, with the correct homing distance for each path being orthogonally

manipulated as 3, 6, or 9 m. The number of segments, overall path length

and overall turning angle still correlated with each other as in Experiment

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 18: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

94 X. Wan et al.

1, but the correct homing distance was uncorrelated with the number of

segments, overall path length, or overall turning angle.

In both experiments, we consistently found two results. First, position

errors and unsigned direction errors increased when the number of segments,

overall path length, and overall turning angle increased. The effect of the

number of segments on unsigned direction errors we found for paths contain-

ing up to 12 segments appeared to be similar with that for paths containing

1 to 3 segments reported by Klatzky et al. (1990) and Ruddle, Payne, and

Jones (1998). On the other hand, Wiener and Mallot (2006) reported their

participants showed larger unsigned pointing errors for 2-segment paths than

for 3-, 4-, and 5-segment paths, although this effect was eliminated when

researchers excluded those 2-segment trials in which the correct turning angle

was 140 degrees. As Wiener and Mallot’s (2006) study indicated, the reason

for the discrepancy might be that the overall path length/turning angles were

kept constant for all the paths in their study but the correct distance/turning

angle were not controlled, whereas the number of segments was highly

correlated to overall path length/turning angle in all other studies, including

our study, and the correct distance was controlled in our Experiment 2.

Second, our results showed that participants’ RTs could not be predicted

by the increase in the number of segments, overall path length, or overall

turning angle. This finding was inconsistent with Klatzky and colleagues’

(1990) findings that their participants showed increased RTs when the number

of segments increased in nonvisual path completion tasks with short paths

containing 1 to 3 segments. Also, our finding was inconsistent with Wiener

and Mallot’s (2006) findings that their participants showed longer RTs to

point to the origin for paths containing fewer segments than paths containing

more segments in visual homing task. Note that they used 2- to 5-segment

paths with the overall path length being kept constant as 18 m, whereas we

used greater range for the number of segments (2 to 12) and for the overall

path length (4 to 36 m). The discrepancy in the RT results might be due to that

different ranges of path length, different ranges of the number of segments,

and different perceptual cues were used in different studies, which could

affect the strategies used by the participants (e.g., configural vs. continuous

updating, see discussion). Due to the large methodological difference among

these studies, the exact cause of the discrepancy is unclear and need further

investigation.

Our findings allowed us to further compare two broad classes of models

that might explain the mechanisms of human path integration. The first

class of models is the configural updating models. In general, the configural

updating models assume that path completion is performed on the basis

of detailed representation of the configuration of the paths. For example,

the Encoding-Error Model proposed that humans sense the pathways and

construct the representation of the outbound path (encoding), calculate the

homing vector (spatial reasoning), and execute the homing vector to return to

the origin (Fujita et al., 1993; Klatzky et al., 1997, 1999; Loomis, Klatzky,

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 19: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 95

Golledge, & Philbeck, 1999). In addition, the Encoding-Error Model assumed

that systematic errors come solely from the encoding stage, with no systematic

errors in the spatial reasoning or response execution stages. The model pro-

vided a remarkably accurate account of the systematic errors in the triangle-

completion task of Loomis et al. (1993).5 However, the model did not do

well when the path increased to 3-legs, and Fujita et al. (1993) suggested

that additional errors sources than the encoding stage may contribute to the

path completion performance of longer trips. Other studies have suggested

that the mental spatial reasoning phase of the path completion task also

introduce systematic errors (Gramann, Müller, Eick, & Schönebeck, 2005;

Riecke, 2008; Riecke et al., 2002).

The second class of models is the nonconfigural models, for example,

the continuous path integration model (Merkle, Rost, & Alt, 2006). These

models proposed that navigators keep track of the spatial relationship between

themselves and the origin of the outbound path during their movements,

without having a detailed representation of the structure of the outbound

paths. Although there is indeed evidence that humans are able to obtain and

maintain the representation of the outbound paths as Loomis and colleagues

(1993) suggested, this evidence of path configuration memory came from

paths with only 1 to 3 segments. As the number of segments increases,

the demand on memory to reconstruct the whole path configuration also

increases, and it is not clear the human working memory capacity is capable

of supporting such a strategy for longer and more complex paths. Thus, the

nonconfigural path integration is a much more economical process, and might

be the more prevalent mode of path integration in general.

The two classes of models make similar predictions on the effects of the

five basic path properties on path completion errors. Both classes of models

predict that path completion errors may increase when the number of seg-

ments, overall path length, and overall turning angles increase. Specifically,

the configural updating models predict that more segments and increased

length of the trip (overall path length and overall turning angles) might lead

to more misperceptions of the segments and turns, more complex internal

representations, and more complex calculations in the spatial reasoning stage,

all of which might generate greater path integration errors shown in our study.

The nonconfigural updating models generally assume that errors occur at each

step of movement and accumulate over time. For example, a nonconfigural

5Their mathematical modeling actually does not require the assumption of

configural updating. For example, a moment-to-moment continuous updating model

can produce the encoding error functions for both translation and rotation equally

well, by assuming an initial overestimation when translation or rotation starts (which

determines the intercept) and a systematic underestimation in each (infinitesimal) step

of updating afterwards during a given segment of translation or angle of turning.

Therefore the modeling results are consistent with both configural and nonconfigural

updating.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 20: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

96 X. Wan et al.

model may assume that each segment or turn of the path is one “step,”

therefore as the number of segments increases, the number of updating steps

also increases, which may lead to more updating errors. Moreover, errors in

the estimation of the displacement or turning vectors of each step may depend

on the size of these vectors, therefore as the overall trip length increases, the

errors may also increase. Thus, a nonconfigural model can also predict that

the accuracies of path completion should decrease with more segments and

increased length of the trip. Finally, the response execution process is common

to both classes of models, therefore any assumptions about errors resulting

from the execution stage can be adopted by both classes of models to explain

the experimental data.

Although both classes of models can potentially accommodate similar

patterns of path completion errors, specific models within each class may

not be consistent with our data. Thus, the present findings put constraints on

the future developments of specific quantitative models of path integration.

That is, these models need to account for the influence of the four basic path

properties (i.e., the number of segments, overall path length/turning angles,

and correct homing distance) on both signed and unsigned path completion

errors. For example, the original Error-Encoding Model proposed by Fujita

and colleagues (1993) only addressed the systematic errors reflected in signed

direction errors and signed distance errors, therefore additional assumptions

regarding other error sources are needed to predict the effects with unsigned

direction errors and unsigned distance errors found in our experiments. For

example, assumptions about the size of the random errors for both distance

and angle encoding, whether they are constant or depend on the segment

length and angle of turn (and if so, is the function linear or nonlinear),

whether they depend on the length of the trip, and so on, are needed to

model the effects of unsigned errors.

Similarly, a strict, moment-by-moment vector summation model assumed

that errors occur at each moment of outbound trip and accumulate over time,

so it predicts the effects of the overall path length/turning angle, but may have

difficulty explaining the effects of the number of segments. That is, assume

a small random error (possibly with nonzero mean to allow for systematic

biases) occurs at each integration step of translation and rotation. The total

accumulated error (either from translation or rotation) is the sum of the errors

in each step during the whole trip. If the integration step is sufficiently small

and occurs numerous times during each segment of translation and each

angle of turn, then the total number of integration steps is solely determined

by the length of the trip (total path length and total rotation angle). Although

some of these errors may cancel out, in general the longer the trip, the more

integration steps, and thus the larger the errors accumulated. In contrast, it

is unclear how an increase in the number of segments would increase the

accumulated errors in this model because it does not affect the number of

integration steps, nor the size of the errors in each step. The only potential

influence of segments is by affecting the possible error-cancellation due to

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 21: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 97

the configuration of the path. However, this effect is very complicated and in

general more segments don’t necessarily lead to more errors.

On the other hand, different classes of models make different predictions

on the effects of the number of segments on the RTs. That is, the configural

model predicts that the RTs should increase when the number of segments

increases, because the process of finishing constructing the whole path struc-

ture and reasoning process occurs after the whole outbound trip is completed.

Thus, the more segments the path has, the longer it should take to compute

the return vector (e.g., Loomis et al., 1993).6 In contrast, the continuous

models predict that RTs should remain constant for paths of different length

and segment number, because the homing vector is calculated and updated

during each step of the outbound trip, and the only computation that needs

to be done before making a response is the updating of the last step, which

should remain largely constant. Thus, both classes of models can explain the

error data in our experiments, but our RT data are more consistent with the

nonconfigural models.

Although Wiener and colleagues (2011) suggested that participants may

use configural updating for “simple paths” and continuous updating for “com-

plex paths,” it should be noted that our participants did not know the number

of segments a path contained until arriving at the end of the outbound paths.

Therefore, it is difficult for them to rely on the number of segments to decide

beforehand which updating strategy to use in our experiments. Nonetheless,

task demand in our experiments might have biased them more toward a

representation of the homing vector without the representation of the paths.

For one, our participants knew at the beginning that they would only need

to directly return to the origin of the trip. For another, because we used

paths containing up to 12 segments, it was more economical to perform

continuous updating to complete the task, as configural updating require very

complex internal representation of the paths which may increase working

memory load. Although the current study cannot rule out the possibility that

participants might begin every path using a configural strategy and then switch

to a continuous updating strategy when their working memory capacity was

exceeded by the accumulation of segments, it is a more complex strategy

(involving two different mechanisms that need to be combined) than the

single, continuous updating strategy, thus it is not clear why participants

would use the complex strategy instead of the simpler one if they knew they

would travel along many segments (a majority of the trials in Experiment 1

and all trials in Experiment 2 had no less than 4 segments).

6Although it is conceivable to build configural representations for sub-sections

of the path along the way (e.g., every 2–3 segments), the resulting vectors of these

sub-configurations still need to be integrated in the end, so there should still be an

increase in RT (although maybe smaller than the full-configural model) as the number

of segments increase, unless these vectors are added up along the way, in which case

the model becomes very much like a large-step continuous updating model.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 22: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

98 X. Wan et al.

We also found some interesting difference between Experiments 1 and 2

regarding the independent effects of correct homing distance on unsigned and

signed distance errors. In Experiment 1, correct homing distance increased

when the number of segments increased as a consequence of random path

construction, and an increase in the correct homing distance led to an increase

in the unsigned distance errors but not the signed distance errors. However,

in Experiment 2, correct homing distance was forced to be uncorrelated with

the number of segments, and an increase in the correct homing distance led

to a decrease in both the unsigned and signed distance errors, suggesting that

participants’ systematic bias (overestimation) decreased when correct homing

distance increased. One possible reason for these results is that path comple-

tion performance might be affected by participants’ intuitive expectation on

the correct homing distance for paths of different length/number of segments.

Specifically, Experiment 1 simulated a more natural situation where correct

homing distance increased with path length, and Experiment 2 used special

paths with many segments but short correct homing distance, leading to high

systematic bias in these unusual paths. Thus, the current study may also

provide converging evidence that human path integration may be affected by

people’s nonsensory factors, including expectation as implied by the current

study, knowledge of the environment (Philbeck, Klatzky, Behrmann, Loomis,

& Goodridge, 2001; Philbeck & O’Leary, 2005; Rieser, 1999), and previous

navigation experience (Petzschner & Glasauer, 2011; Tcheang, Bülthoff, &

Burgess, 2011).

It is also very impressive how our participants completed the path com-

pletion task for 12-segment paths and continuously updated their homing

vector after such long trips, although their path integration was subject to

error accumulation, and their path integration system may not be suitable to

support long distance travels (Souman, Frissen, Sreenivasa, & Ernst, 2009).

The physical rotations our participants made during travel might make crit-

ical contributions to their path completion performance. Without physical

movements (especially rotations), people easily became disoriented in VR

navigation tasks (e.g., Klatzky, Loomis, Beall, Chance, & Golledge, 1998).

It should also be noted that the current study was conducted in the VR Cube,

and it cannot be ruled out the possibility that some participants might use

the real environment (Cube) to orient themselves in the real environment

(virtual maze), as Kelly and colleagues (2008) showed that the geometry

of the surrounding environment can prevent complete disorientation. On the

other hand, the different textures used for outbound and homing paths might

affect speed and distance perception and therefore increase the difficulty to

estimate homing distances.

In conclusion, compared to previous studies focusing on outbound paths

that contained no more than 5 segments, we examined the effects of path

properties on human path integration under more general situations with paths

containing 2 to 12 segments. We examined multiple important path properties

simultaneously, and the Virtual Reality Cube we used allowed participants

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 23: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 99

to make physical rotations in both outbound and homing trips to facilitate

their path completion performance. Results of the current study suggested

that human path completion performance was impaired by increased number

of segments, increased trip length, and increased correct homing distance.

Our findings also provide preliminary evidence that human path completion

performance might be affected by people’s expectations.

AUTHOR NOTES

This work was supported by the SRF for ROCS from SEM to X. Wan and

NSF Grant BCS 03-17681 to R. F. Wang. Some of the data was presented at

the 47th Annual Meeting of the Psychonomics Society, Houston, TX, USA.

We thank Hank Kaczmarski and the Integrated Systems Laboratory at the

University of Illinois for technical support.

REFERENCES

Allen, G. L., Kirasic, K. C., Dobson, S. H., Long, R. G., & Beck, S.

(1996). Predicting environmental learning from spatial abilities: An in-

direct route. Intelligence, 22, 327–355.

Collett, T. S., & Collett, M. (2000). Path integration in insects. Current

Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 757–762.

Ellmore, T. M., & McNaughton, B. L. (2004). Human path integration by

optic flow. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 4, 255–273.

Etienne, A. S. (1992). Navigation of a small mammal by dead reckoning and

local cues. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23, 1–8.

Fujita, N., Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., & Golledge, R. G. (1993). The

encoding-error model of pathway completion without vision. Geograph-

ical Analysis, 25, 295–314.

Gallistel, C. R. (1990). The organization of learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT

Press.

Gramann, K., Müller, H. J., Eick, E., & Schönebeck, B. (2005). Evidence

of separable spatial representations in a virtual navigation task. Journal

of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 31,

1199–1223.

Hoerl, A., & Kennard, R. W. (1970). Ridge regression: Biased estimation for

nonorthogonal problems. Technometrics, 1, 55–67.

Kearns, M. J., Warren, W. H., Duchon, A. P., & Tarr, M. J. (2002). Path

integration from optic flow and body senses in a homing task. Perception,

31, 349–374.

Kelly, J. W., McNamara, T. P., Bodenheimer, B., Carr, T. H., & Rieser, J. J.

(2008). The shape of human navigation: How environmental geometry is

used in the maintenance of spatial orientation. Cognition, 109, 281–286.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 24: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

100 X. Wan et al.

Klatzky, R. L., Beall, A. C., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., & Philbeck, J. W.

(1999). Human navigation ability: Tests of the encoding–error model of

path integration. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 1, 31–65.

Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Beall, A. C., Chance, S. S., & Golledge,

R. G. (1998). Spatial updating of self-position and orientation during

real, imagined, and virtual locomotion.Psychological Science, 9, 293–

298.

Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., & Golledge, R. G. (1997). Encoding spatial

representations through nonvisually guided locomotion: Tests of human

path integration. The Psychology of Learning, 37, 41–84.

Klatzky, R. L., Loomis, J. M., Golledge, R. G., Cicinelli, J. G., Doherty, S.,

& Pellegrino, J. W. (1990). Acquisition of route and survey knowledge

in the absence of vision. Journal of Motor Behavior, 22, 19–43.

Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., Cicinelli, J. G., Pellegrino,

J. W., & Fry, P. A. (1993). Nonvisual navigation by blind and sighted:

Assessment of path integration ability. Journal of Experimental Psychol-

ogy: General, 122, 73–91.

Loomis, J. M., Klatzky, R. L., Golledge, R. G., & Philbeck, J. W. (1999).

Human navigation by path integration. In R. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding

behavior: Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 125–151).

Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Merkle, T., Rost, M., & Alt, W. (2006). Egocentric path integration models

and their application to desert arthropods. Journal of Theoretical Biology,

240, 385–399.

Mittelstaedt, H., & Mittelstaedt, M. L. (1982). Homing by path integration.

In F. Papi & H. G. Wallraff (Eds.), Avian navigation (pp. 290–297). New

York, NY: Springer.

Müller, M., & Wehner, R. (1988). Path integration in desert ants, Cataglyphis

fortis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 85, 5287–

5290.

Passini, R., Proulx, G., & Rainville, C. (1990). The spatio-cognitive abilities

of the visually impaired population. Environment and Behavior, 22, 91–

116.

Péruch, P., May, M., & Wartenberg, F. (1997). Homing in virtual environ-

ments: Effect of field of view and path layout. Perception, 16, 301–

311.

Petzschner, F. H., & Glasauer, S. (2011). Iterative Bayesian estimation as

an explanation for range and regression effects: A study on human path

integration. Journal of Neuroscience, 31, 17220–17229.

Philbeck, J. W., Klatzky, R. K., Behrmann, M., Loomis, J. M., & Goodridge,

J. (2001). Active control of locomotion facilitates nonvisual navigation.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Perfor-

mance, 27, 141–153.

Philbeck, J. W., & O’Leary, S. (2005). Remembered landmarks enhance the

precision of path integration. Psicológica, 26, 7–24.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13

Page 25: bInterdisciplinary Journal Computation: An Spatial CognitionCorrespondence concerning this article should be addressed to Xiaoang Wan, Department of Psychology, School of Social Sciences,

Path Integration & Path Properties 101

Regolin, L., Vallortigara, G., & Zanforlin, M. (1995). Detour behaviour in

the domestic chick: Searching for a disappearing prey or a disappearing

social partner. Animal Behaviour, 50, 203–211.

Riecke, B. E. (2008). Consistent left-right reversals for visual path integration

in Virtual Reality: More than a failure to update one’s heading? Presence:

Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 17, 143–175.

Riecke, B. E., van Veen, H. A. H. C., & Bülthoff, H. H. (2002). Visual homing

is possible without landmarks: A path integration study in virtual reality.

Presence, 11, 443–473.

Rieser, J. J. (1999). Dynamic spatial orientation and the coupling of rep-

resentation and action. In R. G. Golledge (Ed.), Wayfinding behavior:

Cognitive mapping and other spatial processes (pp. 168–190). Baltimore,

MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.

Ruddle, R. A., Payne, S. J., & Jones, D. M. (1998). Navigating large scale

“desktop” virtual buildings: Effects of orientation aids and familiarity.

Presence: Teleoperators and Virtual Environments, 7, 179–192.

Souman, J. L., Frissen, I., Sreenivasa, M. N., & Ernst, M. O. (2009). Walking

straight into circles. Current Biology, 19, 1538–1542.

Tcheang, L., Bülthoff, H., & Burgess, N. (2011). Visual influence on path

integration in darkness indicates a multimodal representation of large-

scale space. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108,

1152–1157.

von Saint Paul, U. (1982). Do geese use path integration for walking home?

In F. Papi & H. G. Wallraff (Eds.), Avian navigation (pp. 298–307). New

York, NY: Springer.

Wan, X., Wang, R. F., & Crowell, J. A. (2010). The effect of active selection

in human path integration, Journal of Vision, 10(11), 1–11.

Wehner, R., & Srinivasan, M. V. (1981). Searching behavior of desert ants,

genus Cataglyphis (Formicidae, Hymenoptera). Journal of Comparative

Physiology, 142, 315–318.

Wiener, J. M., Berthoz, A., & Wolbers, T. (2011). Dissociable cognitive

mechanisms underlying human path. Experimental Brain Research, 208,

61–71.

Wiener, J. M., & Mallot, H. (2006). Path complexity does not impair visual

path integration. Spatial Cognition and Computation, 6, 333–346.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Uni

vers

ity o

f Il

linoi

s at

Urb

ana-

Cha

mpa

ign]

at 1

3:47

17

Apr

il 20

13