75
Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ T RP C currents Implications for dendritic integration MARCUS PETERSSON Licentiate Thesis Stockholm, Sweden 2010 Akademisk avhandling som med tillstånd av K ungl Tekniska högskolan framlägges till offentlig granskning för avläggande av teknologie licentiatexamen i datalogi fredagen den 29 okt 2010 klockan 13.00 i RB35, AlbaNova, K ungl Tekniska högskolan, Roslagstullsbacken 35, Stockholm. T R I T A -C SC -A 2010:13 • I SSN -1653-5723 • I SR N K T H/ C SC / A –10/ 13-SE • I SB N 978-91-7415-745-1

Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Beyond AMPA and NMDA:Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents

Implications for dendritic integration

MARCUS PETERSSON

LicentiateThesisStockholm, Sweden 2010

Akademisk avhandling sommed tillstånd av Kungl Tekniska högskolan framläggestill offentlig granskning för avläggande av teknologie licentiatexamen i datalogifredagenden29okt 2010klockan13.00i RB35, AlbaNova, Kungl Tekniskahögskolan,Roslagstullsbacken 35, Stockholm.

T RITA -CSC-A 2010:13 • ISSN-1653-5723 • ISRN K TH/ CSC/ A–10/ 13-SE • ISBN 978-91-7415-745-1

Page 2: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA
Page 3: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

iii

Abstract

In order to understand how the brain functions, under normal as well as pathological conditions, itis important to study the mechanisms underlying information integration. Depending on the natureof an input arriving at a synapse, di�erent strategies may be used by the neuron to integrate andrespond to the input. Naturally, if a short train of high-frequency synaptic input arrives, it may bebene�cial for the neuron to be equipped with a fast mechanism that is highly sensitive to inputson a short time scale. If, on the contrary, inputs arriving with low frequency are to be processed, itmay be necessary for the neuron to possess slow mechanisms of integration. For example, in certainworking memory tasks (e. g. delay-match-to-sample), sensory inputs may arrive separated by silentintervals in the range of seconds, and the subject should respond if the current input is identicalto the preceding input. It has been suggested that single neurons, due to intrinsic mechanismsoutlasting the duration of input, may be able to perform such calculations. In this work, I havestudied a mechanism thought to be particularly important in supporting the integration of low-frequency synaptic inputs. It is mediated by a cascade of events that starts with activation of groupI metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGlu1/5), and ends with a membrane depolarization causedby a current that is mediated by canonical transient receptor potential (TRPC) ion channels. Thiscurrent, denoted ITRPC , is the focus of this thesis.

A speci�c objective of this thesis is to study the role of ITRPC in the integration of synapticinputs arriving at a low frequency, < 10Hz. Our hypothesis is that, in contrast to the well-studied,rapidly decaying AMPA and NMDA currents, ITRPC is well-suited for supporting temporal sum-mation of such synaptic input. The reason for choosing this range of frequencies is that neuronsoften communicate with signals (spikes) around 8Hz, as shown by single-unit recordings in be-having animals. This is true for several regions of the brain, including the entorhinal cortex (EC)which is known to play a key role in producing working memory function and enabling long-termmemory formation in the hippocampus.

Although there is strong evidence suggesting that ITRPC is important for neuronal commu-nication, I have not encountered a systematic study of how this current contributes to synapticintegration. Since it is di�cult to directly measure the electrical activity in dendritic branchesusing experimental techniques, I use computational modeling for this purpose. I implemented thecomponents necessary for studying ITRPC , including a detailed model of extrasynaptic glutamateconcentration, mGlu1/5 dynamics and the TRPC channel itself. I tuned the model to replicateelectrophysiological in vitro data from pyramidal neurons of the rodent EC, provided by our ex-perimental collaborator. Since we were interested in the role of ITRPC in temporal summation,a speci�c aim was to study how its decay time constant (τdecay) is a�ected by synaptic stimulusparameters.

The hypothesis described above is supported by our simulation results, as we show that synapticinputs arriving at frequencies as low as 3 - 4Hz can be e�ectively summed. We also show thatτdecay increases with increasing stimulus duration and frequency, and that it is linearly dependenton the maximal glutamate concentration. Under some circumstances it was problematic to directlymeasure τdecay , and we then used a pair-pulse paradigm to get an indirect estimate of τdecay .

I am not aware of any computational model work taking into account the synaptically evokedITRPC current, prior to the current study, and believe that it is the �rst of its kind. We suggestthat ITRPC is important for slow synaptic integration, not only in the EC, but in several corticaland subcortical regions that contain mGlu1/5 and TRPC subunits, such as the prefrontal cortex. Iwill argue that this is further supported by studies using pharmacological blockers as well as studieson genetically modi�ed animals.

Keywords: transient receptor potential, TRP, metabotropic glutamate receptor, mGlu1/5,dendritic integration, synaptic activation, temporal summation, low-frequency, entorhinal cortex,mathematical model, computational neuroscience

Page 4: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA
Page 5: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

v

Acknowledgments

I am grateful to my supervisor Erik for his excellent support. I highly appreciatehaving an available, enthusiastic and sympathetic guide to the world of brain science.

I would like to thank all the friendly current and previous people at the depart-ment, for contributing to a nice and relaxed research environment. Malin and Nalinkindly helped me with TEX, and I have received excellent computer-related advicesfrom David, Johannes, Pradeep and Örjan on several occasions. I am grateful toJeanette for reading and helping me to improve this thesis, to Harriet for alwayssorting out administrative issues, and to Jenny for good scienti�c advices and dis-cussions (especially during the porcine weeks last summer). I am also grateful forthe numerous rewarding research-life discussions I have enjoyed with Iman, Micke,Paweª and others.

I have been highly inspired by many great people who crossed my path in thepast few years, at courses I have taken (especially OCNC 2009), at previous jobs(especially Philips), at lab visits (especially the Mannheim pain lab), and I could notdeny being thankful to Torsten Wiesel for questioning the function of my (poster)synapses.

I also want to thank my good old and not so old good friends for keeping mesane, and my family for always caring and calling.

And thank you Khadeeja, for contaminating me with your happiness and cap-saicin addiction, for reading my thesis, for love and support. Thank you, for movingto the cold and making it warm.

My work was supported by the Swedish Research Council (Grant number: 621-2007-3774) and the National Institutes of Health (Grant number: MH061492-06A2).I have received generous travel support from Stockholm Brain Institute and the Knutand Alice Wallenberg Foundation.

Page 6: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA
Page 7: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Contents

1 Introduction 1

1.1 Scope of this thesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 11.2 Papers included in the thesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 2

2 Biological background 3

2.1 A brief introduction to neuroscience . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32.1.1 Studying the brain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 32.1.2 Brain regions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42.1.3 Neurons . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 6

2.2 Electrical and chemical signaling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62.2.1 The action potential . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 62.2.2 Synaptic integration . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72.2.3 Ion channels . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72.2.4 Metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGlu) and synaptic trans-

mission . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 82.2.5 Second messengers and intracellular signaling . . . . . . . . . 102.2.6 Measuring the electrical properties of neurons . . . . . . . . . 11

2.3 Transient receptor potential (TRP) channels . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122.3.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122.3.2 The origin of the name . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 122.3.3 Expressed in every tissue type . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142.3.4 The TRPC subfamily . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 142.3.5 The (mGlu1/5) receptor activated TRPC current � ITRPC . 15

2.4 Dendrites . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 182.4.1 Dendrites extend the neuronal computation toolbox . . . . . 192.4.2 Dendrites and disease . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 21

3 Models and methods 23

3.1 Review of computational neuroscience . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 233.1.1 The Hodgkin and Huxley model and beyond . . . . . . . . . 233.1.2 Choosing an abstraction level . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 243.1.3 Multicompartmental modeling . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 24

3.2 Model used in this thesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 263.2.1 The Poirazi model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 273.2.2 Extending the Poirazi model by adding ITRPC . . . . . . . . 27

vii

Page 8: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

viii CONTENTS

3.3 Simulations and analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 333.3.1 Synaptic stimulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 333.3.2 Calculating the decay time constant � τdecay . . . . . . . . . 33

3.4 Software used in this thesis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 35

4 Results and discussion 37

4.1 Paper I . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 374.1.1 Main conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 374.1.2 τdecay � a closer look . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 404.1.3 Sensitivity analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 42

4.2 Paper II . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 444.2.1 Main conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 444.2.2 EPSP time course . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 45

4.3 Meta-analysis of in vitro τdecay measurements . . . . . . . . . . . . 464.3.1 Variation of mechanisms among systems studied . . . . . . . 474.3.2 Feature or bug? . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 484.3.3 Conclusion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 48

4.4 Importance of the implemented ITRPC model . . . . . . . . . . . . 49

5 Future work 51

5.1 Model improvements . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 515.2 Additional tests and analysis . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 52

Bibliography 55

Page 9: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Chapter 1

Introduction

1.1 Scope of this thesis

Dendritic integration of synaptic input is essential for information processing inthe human brain. It has recently become clear that complex phenomena of synap-tic integration are abundant and supported by a plethora of non-linear dendriticmechanisms. One of these mechanisms involve an excitatory synaptic current work-ing in parallel with the classical ionotropic AMPA (α-amino-3-hydroxyl-5-methyl-4-isoxazole-propionate) and NMDA (N-methyl-D-aspartic acid) currents. This synap-tic current, denoted ITRPC , will be the focus of this thesis. ITRPC is mediatedby canonical transient receptor potential (TRPC) channels, which are activateddownstream of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGlu1/5). An impor-tant di�erence between the ionotropic currents and ITRPC is that the latter has alonger decay time. Thus, we hypothesized that ITRPC is important for summationof low frequency synaptic inputs. In this thesis I will present further backgroundand arguments in support of the hypothesis.

To test the hypothesis, we constructed a computational model of a pyramidalneuron, and speci�cally tested the dynamics of ITRPC . We did not use a networkmodel, but focused on synaptic integration from the perspective of a single neuron.We limited the study to investigating frequencies that are commonly observed insingle-unit recordings from awake behaving animals (i. e. < 10Hz). Furthermore,we did not include mechanisms downstream of mGlu1/5 other than the activationof TRPC channels, such as presynaptic e�ects or e�ects on other ion channels,in the model. Data for a detailed account of the intracellular signaling networkunderlying ITRPC activation is incomplete, and although we accounted for much ofwhat is known about the activation mechanisms, our model should be consideredfunctional. Consequently, details regarding the underlying biochemical pathways arenot included in the model. It is important to learn more about the ITRPC activationmechanisms, but it should be noted that this is not the aim of this thesis.

Our main source of data is results from electrophysiological recordings of pyra-midal neurons of the entorhinal cortex, done by our experimental collaborators Mo-toharu Yoshida and Michael Hasselmo at Boston University. Therefore, one couldargue that our conclusions may be less relevant for other brain regions. However, it

1

Page 10: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2 CHAPTER 1. INTRODUCTION

is very likely that the conclusions are valid for other brain regions in which ITRPCis present as well, such as the prefrontal cortex, hippocampus, subiculum, anteriorcingulate cortex, amygdala, substantia nigra and cerebellum. In light of this, itis possible that the mechanisms we study play an important role in behavior anddisease, something I will discuss in various parts of this thesis.

I will start by presenting aspects of the biological background (Chapter 2) thatmay be important to know about in order to understand the concepts discussedin subsequent chapters. Following this, I will present the methods I have used totest the hypothesis (Chapter 3). I will focus on describing the model componentsthat I believe are unique to this study, but also discuss more general concepts ofcomputational neuroscience and multicompartment modeling in particular. I willthen discuss the results and importance of the two papers (Chapter 4). Finally, Iwill discuss ways in which the model can be improved and studies that would beinteresting to consider for future work (Chapter 5).

1.2 Papers included in the thesis

Paper I: Low frequency summation of synaptically activated TRP channel medi-ated depolarizations, Petersson M E, Yoshida M, Fransén E, manuscript in review,submitted 2010-04-09

We studied the contribution of mGlu1/5 (group I metabotropic glutamate recep-tor) mediated TRPC (canonical transient receptor potential) currents to synapticintegration in pyramidal neurons, and found that these support summation of low-frequency inputs. We also found that the dynamics of the TRPC decay time constantincreases with increasing stimulus duration and frequency.

In this paper I implemented the model, ran all simulations and did all analysisof simulation results. I conceived the idea of using a pair-pulse protocol for studyingthe dynamics of the model. I contributed to designing the model and to the literaturesearch relating to building and tuning its components. I conducted the meta-analysisconcerning decay times of ITRPC from experimental data. I also did the analysis ofexperimental results included as �gures in the paper and contributed to writing themanuscript.

Paper II: TRPC channels activated by group I mGluR in Entorhinal pyramidalneurons support integration of low frequency (<10 Hz) synaptic inputs, Petersson ME, Fransén E, BMC Neuroscience 2009, 10(Suppl 1):P26 doi:10.1186/1471-2202-10-S1-P26

In this paper, written prior to Paper I, we tuned the model from current injectionexperiments rather than from synaptic stimulation experiments as in Paper I. Oneof the conclusions was that TRPC decay times are signi�cantly longer for currentinjection than from synaptic stimulation experiments. This led us to focus on re-sults from synaptic stimulation when conducting the work for Paper I. Following theresults of Paper I we can conclude that the main �ndings are essentially similar inthe two papers.

In this paper I implemented the model, ran all simulations and did all analysisof simulation results.

Page 11: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Chapter 2

Biological background

In this chapter, my intention is to give the reader an introduction to concepts thatare important for understanding the modeling work of this thesis. After giving abrief introduction to the �eld of neuroscience, I will zoom in on describing what isknown about the electrical and chemical signaling of neurons, with a strong focus onaspects relevant for understanding the methods, results and discussion in subsequentchapters. I will then describe the transient receptor potential (TRP) ion channels,and especially highlight a synaptically evoked current that is mediated by these ionchannels (ITRPC). Finally, I will discuss recent progress in the �eld of studying thefunction of dendrites, since my work may have implications for how these structuresintegrate synaptic inputs.

2.1 A brief introduction to neuroscience

Neuroscience refers to the study of the nervous system. Neuroscientists study thefunction of the nervous system on levels ranging from genes to behavior. Todaymost research groups focus on the brain, but also peripheral functions such as thoseinvolved in pain perception remain to be well understood.

From my point of view, there are two strong driving forces behind the progressof neuroscience, one being curiosity. It is within the human nature to be curious,and it would be safe to say that all people have questions on some level relatingto the mechanisms of sensation, movement, emotion, or cognitive functions such asmemory, intelligence and language. The second driving force is the more speci�cneed to characterize neural disorders, such as addiction, Alzheimer's disease (AD),ADHD (attention de�cit hyperactivity disorder), autism, brain tumors, chronic pain,Down's syndrome, dyslexia, Huntington's disease, major depression, multiple scle-rosis, Parkinson's disease, schizophrenia, seizures and epilepsy, stroke, Tourette syn-drome, and ultimately to �nd ways of restoring normal function.

2.1.1 Studying the brain

The �eld of neuroscience is very broad, and in order to make proper assumptionsanyone who studies it need to be aware of its many levels. For example, psychologists

3

Page 12: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

who study the development of AD need to know something about genetic risk factorssuch as APOE-ε4, and something about how to interpret data from fMRI (functionalmagnetic resonance imaging) studies. Similarly, cellular neurophysiologists need toknow not only about cell physiology, but also something about the role of the neuronin the network it forms a part of, and about the role of that network in behavior. Thework in this thesis involves assumptions on levels ranging from genes to behavior,showing that computational neuroscience is not an exception.

For ethical reasons, there are many restrictions on how the human brain canbe studied with invasive techniques. Some insights can be gained from using non-invasive electromagnetic measurement techniques such as fMRI, EEG (electroen-cephalography) and MEG (magnetoencephalography). Unfortunately, these tech-niques can not give a complete picture of the neural mechanisms underlying a cer-tain behavior, and studying the human brain is hence problematic. Fortunately,the human nervous system has many features in common with less complex animalsthat are easier to study. Important examples of neuroscientists that bene�ted fromworking with animals are Hodgkin and Huxley who studied the giant squid, Hubertand Wiesel who studied cats, and Kandel who studied a sea snail called Aplysia.They were all awarded the Nobel Prize for their discoveries.

The work in this thesis concerns cellular neurophysiology, a sub�eld of neuro-science that to a large extent rely on rodent studies. In cellular neurophysiology,we aim to understand how neurons integrate their input signals and subsequentlytransmit signals to other neurons. For this purpose, I have used computational mod-eling, a technique often used in parallel with experimental techniques. I will give anintroduction to computational modeling in Chapter 3.

2.1.2 Brain regions

Some areas of the human brain have clear functions, such as the visual cortex dealingwith visual information processing, the motor cortex controlling the muscles, or thethalamus serving as a relay station for most incoming information. Other areas,such as the prefrontal cortex, are associated with more than one function and thusare more di�cult to study and understand.

2.1.2.1 Entorhinal cortex

The brain region in focus of this thesis is called the entorhinal cortex (EC). The EC issituated in the medial temporal lobe and serves as an input and output region to thehippocampus (see �gure 2.1), a brain structure with a well-recognized importancefor functions of memory and spatial navigation. Note that, in this thesis, I will notdiscuss the various pathways of the entorhinal-hippocampal network. The intentionof including �gure 2.1 is rather to illustrate the location of EC.

The EC plays a role in olfactory information processing, but in recent yearsmore attention has been given to its role in various aspects of memory and spatialnavigation. Speci�cally, the EC has been shown to be involved in complex spatialnavigation (Frank et al., 2000; Fyhn et al., 2004; Jacobs et al., 2010) and short-termand working memory (Otto and Eichenbaum, 1992; Suzuki et al., 1997; Esclassanet al., 2009).

Page 13: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.1. A BRIEF INTRODUCTION TO NEUROSCIENCE 5

Figure 2.1. The entorhinal cortex (EC) is situated in the medial tempo-ral lobe. (Top) The right human hemisphere, data based on MRI-study (fromHagmann et al., 2008). (Middle) Coronal section of the human brain (fromhttp://www.nia.nih.gov/Alzheimers/Resources/HighRes.htm). (Bottom) The EC serves asthe main interface between the hippocampus and neocortex (rodent schematic anatomy, fromLie et al., 2004).

Page 14: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

6 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

2.1.3 Neurons

The brain is essentially a large network, formed by approximately 100 billion neu-rons. The neuron consists of a soma, dendrites and an axon. A typical neuron, thehippocampal CA1 pyramidal cell (see �gure 3.2), has around 30 000 excitatory and1 700 inhibitory synapses (Megías et al., 2001). There are many types of neurons inthe brain, and although they share many features there are also important di�er-ences. The number of synapses of a single neuron varies between 1 to about 100 000(Purves, 2008), which indicates the complexity of the human brain. In the tradi-tional view, signals arrive at synapses located on the dendrites and travel down tothe soma where they are summed and may or may not trigger an all-or-none signal(the action potential, see section 2.2.1), which is conducted along the axon until itreaches its target, a synapse on another neuron. In recent years, however, a morecomplex view of neuronal signaling has emerged. It is now clear that signals can alsotravel in the opposite direction, from the soma out to the dendrites. People haveeven questioned whether or not there really is a fundamental distinction betweendendrites and axons, since dendrites may release transmitter substances, and axonsmay receive synaptic input (Stuart et al., 2007). In addition, it has been shownthat the dendrites that were classically thought of as passive conductors, can in factperform local computations of incoming signals (Larkum et al., 2009). I will furtherdiscuss dendritic function in section 2.4.

In this thesis I focus on pyramidal neurons, which are abundant in cortical areas.However, the cellular mechanism in focus of my work - mGlu1/5 activated TRPCchannels - is likely to be important also for synaptic integration in other neurontypes. The cerebellar Purkinje cell is one such example, for which much progresshas been made in characterizing the function of mGlu1/5 activated TRPC channels(Hartmann and Konnerth, 2008), the dopaminergic midbrain neuron being another(Bengtson et al., 2004).

2.2 Electrical and chemical signaling

Electrical and chemical signaling in the brain occurs both in parallel and in series.For example, a synaptically transferred signal is typically a combination of chemicaland electrical events occurring in series (e. g. neurotransmitter → transmembranecurrent), while a postsynaptic response can consist of parallel changes in concen-trations of intracellular substances on the one hand, and electrical changes of themembrane on the other. In my work I investigate both electrical and chemical sig-naling, in the current section I will introduce a few phenomena that are essential forunderstanding the modeling results of this thesis.

2.2.1 The action potential

The cell membrane of neurons possess certain properties, such that there is an elec-trochemical gradient across it. The di�erence between the inside and outside poten-tials is called the membrane potential (Vm). The most well de�ned neuronal signalis called the action potential (AP), or simply a �spike�, and appears as a change inVm. This is because initiation of the AP, which occurs in the vicinity of the soma,

Page 15: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.2. ELECTRICAL AND CHEMICAL SIGNALING 7

requires the membrane to be depolarized enough to cross a speci�c threshold. Asynaptic signal which is subthreshold will by itself not evoke an AP and thereforenot reach the synapses of downstream neurons. Nevertheless, the nature of sub-threshold signals is important to study because, for a neuron to decide whether ornot it should generate an AP, it must perform sophisticated subthreshold compu-tations of current and previous synaptic inputs. These computations are normallyreferred to as synaptic integration.

2.2.2 Synaptic integration

According to Gulledge et al. (2005), �synaptic integration is the complex process bywhich synaptic responses interact to promote or inhibit action potential generation�.Similarly, Magee (2000) de�nes it as �the combination of voltage de�ections producedby a myriad of synaptic inputs into a singular change in membrane potential�. Hefurther explains that there are three basic elements involved in the integration: theamplitude of the synaptic signal (excitatory postsynaptic potential, EPSP), spatialsummation and temporal summation (of the EPSPs). In this thesis I study synapticintegration from the perspective of a single neuron. Speci�cally, I study the dynamicsof the fundamental excitable elements of neuronal membranes, the ion channels, witha focus on their role in temporal summation.

2.2.3 Ion channels

Ion channels are pores in the cell membrane that are selective to speci�c ions. Theiropening and closing, and thus the ion �ux, is primarily governed by changes in Vmor concentrations of surrounding molecules. In response to such changes, the ionchannel proteins undergo morphological changes. In the dendrites there are severaltypes of ion channel mediated currents. Many are gated by Vm (e.g. INa, IK,fast,IK,slow, ICa,L, ICa,T , ICa,N , Ih) or calcium (e.g. IKCa,fast, IKCa,slow, ICAN ), whileothers are gated by both Vm and transmitter substances such as glutamate (INMDA,see section 2.2.4). The distribution of these and other dendritic ion channels alongthe dendrites is often non-uniform, and both distribution and function may varyamong di�erent neuron types (Migliore and Shepherd, 2002). Importantly, muchdata is still lacking when it comes to the distribution and function of dendritic ionchannels.

ICAN , listed above, is of special interest in this thesis. It is a calcium acti-vated non-selective current, sometimes denoted INCM (calcium-sensitive nonspeci�ccation current). It is non-selective in the sense that more than one type of ion maypass through it. The molecular counterpart of what mediates the current (the ionchannel/s) could di�er among di�erent cells. In the brain stem it is believed tobe mediated by TRPM (melastatin transient receptor potential) channels (Crowderet al., 2007). In cortical areas, however, there is much evidence pointing to TRPC(canonical transient receptor potential) channels as the main candidate, as I willdiscuss in section 2.3.5. I will devote section 2.3 to describing properties of TRPchannels in general and TRPC channels in particular.

Page 16: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

8 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

2.2.4 Metabotropic glutamate receptors (mGlu) and synaptictransmission

Synaptic transmission is a major form of neuronal communication. Synapses are inprinciple either inhibitory or excitatory. Inhibitory signals decrease, while excitatoryincrease, the probability of an AP being generated. For the majority of excitatorysynapses in the brain, glutamate serves as the neurotransmitter, while inhibitorysynapses are typically GABAergic. Excitatory synapses are in focus of this thesis,though I included inhibitory GABAB (γ-aminobutyric acid) synapses in a controlstudy presented in Paper I (see �gure M4A). The intention was to make sure thatthe EPSP looks normal in a physiologically realistic situation, in which the sum ofboth inhibitory and excitatory synaptic inputs is observed as a somatic membranedepolarization.

Postsynaptic currents at glutamatergic synapses, which are mediated by eitherAMPA, NMDA or metabotropic glutamate (mGlu) receptors (as illustrated in �gure2.2), depolarize the dendritic membrane at the site of synaptic input. The depolar-ization then spreads to other parts of the dendritic tree and may be observed by thesoma as a excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP).

2.2.4.1 Glutamate receptors are either ionotropic or metabotropic

The ionotropic receptors AMPA and NMDA form transmembrane pores and, whenbound to glutamate, allow for ionic current �ows that depolarize the membrane.The AMPA current is relatively fast and gives rise to an EPSP with a decay timeconstant of around 5ms (Destexhe et al., 1994b). The NMDA current is slower, withan EPSP decay time constant of around 150ms.

In contrast to the ionotropic glutamate receptors, the metabotropic glutamatereceptors do not form transmembrane pores and hence can not directly producean EPSP. Instead, they trigger activation of intracellular pathways that may sub-sequently lead to activation of ion channels. Resulting EPSPs are typically slowerthan ionotropic EPSPs.

2.2.4.2 Group I mGlu receptors: mGlu1 and mGlu5

Metabotropic glutamate receptors (abbreviated mGlu according to the InternationalUnion of Pharmacology, see Pin et al.) are abundant in the brain (Fotuhi et al.,1994; Stuart et al., 2007; Olive, 2009; Niswender and Conn, 2010) and have severalimportant functions (Anwyl, 1999; Benarroch, 2008). mGlu receptors are involved inmechanisms relating to e. g. fear (Schulz et al., 2001; Rudy and Matus-Amat, 2009),working memory (Hayashi et al., 2007) and synaptic plasticity (Anwyl, 1999; Clemet al., 2008; Gladding et al., 2009; Niswender and Conn, 2010). Several reviews haveimplicated mGlu receptors as future targets for pharmacological treatment, sincethey may be involved in a range of neurological diseases (Anwyl, 1999; Benarroch,2008; Ferraguti et al., 2008; Olive, 2009; Niswender and Conn, 2010).

mGlu receptor subtypes are divided into three groups: I, II and III. According toKnöpfel and Uusisaari (2008), e�ects following activation of group I are mainly post-synaptic, whereas e�ects of group II and group III are mainly presynaptic (thoughthey are present in postsynaptic structures in some brain regions, see Stuart et al.,

Page 17: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.2. ELECTRICAL AND CHEMICAL SIGNALING 9

Figure 2.2. Schematic drawing of ionotropic (AMPA and NMDA) and metabotropic(mGlu1/5) receptors at an excitatory synapse. At the arrival of a presynaptic action poten-tial, glutamate is released and binds to AMPA, NMDA and mGlu1/5 receptors. mGlu1/5, aswell as TRPC channels, are located peri- and extrasynaptically. The Gαq/11/PLC pathway istriggered by activation of mGlu1/5 activation, or activation of the muscarinic (acetylcholine)M1 receptor. This pathway in turn leads to activation of TRPC channels (although the exactmechanisms of this is unclear), which are also dependent on Ca2+. Sources of Ca2+ thatmay be involved are: voltage gated calcium channels (VGCC), release from the endoplasmicreticulum (via IP3 receptors), and the TRPC channels themselves.

Page 18: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

10 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

Table 2.1. Distribution of TRPC4/5 (left, from Fowler et al., 2007) and mGlu1/5 (right,from Olive, 2009) in the rodent brain.

2007) and will not be discussed in this thesis since I focus solely on postsynapticmechanisms. Group I mGlu receptors, which includes subtypes mGlu1 and mGlu5,couple to the PLC pathway via activation of G-proteins. Mechanisms downstream ofmGlu1/5 activation are diverse and include regulation of NMDA currents, calcium-gated, voltage-gated and inwardly rectifying potassium channels, calcium channels(Knöpfel and Uusisaari, 2008) and TRPC channels (Congar et al., 1997), the latterbeing in focus of this thesis. Unlike AMPA and NMDA receptors, mGlu1/5 recep-tors are not located at the synaptic cleft, but peri- and extrasynaptically (at leastin hippocampal neurons, see Lujan et al., 1996; Mateos et al., 2000), which has ledto discussions about e�ects of �spillover glutamate� in synaptic transmission (seee. g. Knöpfel and Uusisaari, 2008), with synaptic cooperativity as one consequence.mGlu1/5 receptors are present in several di�erent brain regions, see table 2.1 froma review by Olive (2009).

2.2.5 Second messengers and intracellular signaling

Molecular signaling within neurons is important, but harder to study than the elec-trical activity of the membrane. Much of the information within the neuron is carriedby so called second messengers, several of which are important in this thesis. Ex-amples of common second messengers are PIP2 (phosphatidylinositol bisphosphate),DAG (diacylglycerol) and IP3 (inositol trisphosphate), which are all involved in thesignaling following activation of mGlu1/5. Speci�cally, mGlu1/5 leads to activation

Page 19: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.2. ELECTRICAL AND CHEMICAL SIGNALING 11

of G-proteins which in turn activates an enzyme called PLC (phospholipase-C). PLCthen cleaves PIP2 into DAG and IP3.

One of the most important second messengers is the calcium ion (Ca2+), whichhas an impact on nearly every aspect of cellular life (see e. g. Berridge et al., 2003;Clapham, 2007). One interesting fact about Ca2+ is that its intracellular concentra-tion is typically low, 100 nM, compared to the extracellular 2mM (Clapham, 2007).Ca2+ ions may be highly localized, restricted to microdomains. Ca2+ not only playsa crucial role in the activation of TRPC channels, but is also to some extent con-ducted when these channels are open. The role of Ca2+ in TRPC activation will befurther described in section 2.3.

2.2.6 Measuring the electrical properties of neurons

2.2.6.1 In vitro

In vitro, in Latin literally meaning �within the glass�, is a term which in neuroscien-ti�c research usually informs that an experiment was conducted using brain slices orin a cell culture. Two common experimental protocols for studying single neuronsin brain slices are voltage clamp and current clamp, and these are often used incombination with application of pharmacological blockers. Most of the knowledgewe have about ion channels and receptors comes from such studies. The data in�gure 1 of Paper I comes from in vitro experiments performed by our experimen-tal collaborator, who induced synaptic activation of EC neurons, with and withoutmGlu1/5 blockers, and measured the response in the soma in order to study theproperties of the TRPC channel. It is possible to do similar recordings out in thedendrites, but it is relatively di�cult (the diameter of dendritic branches is muchsmaller than that of the soma) and therefore rarely done. Computational modeling,however, with which electrical activity at any part of the neuron can be monitored,can well complement this type of in vitro experiment.

It should also be mentioned that the concentration of Ca2+ can be measured,using optical techniques where Ca2+ is bound to a �uorescent dye. Since Ca2+ playsa very important role as an intracellular messenger, as mentioned in section 2.2.5,it is bene�cial and common to study its dynamics for a range of di�erent researchquestions, including those relating to TRPC channels.

2.2.6.2 In vivo

In vivo, meaning �within the living�, refers to an experiment being performed in aliving animal. In some in vivo experiments, single-unit activity (spiking) is recorded,with the purpose of understanding more about the neural code (Buzsáki, 2004). Thisis however complicated by the fact that neurons are not point sources, but ratherhas a spatio-temporally distributed electrical activity. Furthermore, extracellularrecordings do not reveal much about the underlying single-neuron dynamics, espe-cially in the subthreshold domain. It is therefore exciting to learn about the recentdevelopment of in vivo measurement techniques that gives insights into for exampleCa2+ dynamics and dendritic activity during behavior. A few such studies can bementioned. Murayama and coworkers recently succeeded in using in vivo Ca2+ imag-ing to show that dendritic spikes exist in an awake rat (Murayama et al., 2009), and

Page 20: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

12 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

that dendritic activity correlates with the strength of subsequent hindlimb move-ment (Murayama and Larkum, 2009). Harvey et al. (2009) have been able to recordsubthreshold membrane dynamics in an awake rat performing a visuospatial task ina virtual reality environment, and Lütcke et al. (2010) have reported development ofan optical technique enabling Ca2+ transients to be recorded from individual apicaldendrites in awake and behaving rats. A recent review by Scanziani and Häusser(2009) discusses several aspects of optical techniques, both in vitro and in vivo.

2.3 Transient receptor potential (TRP) channels

2.3.1 Introduction

The transient receptor potential (TRP, often pronounced �trip�) ion channel familyconsists of channels with a great diversity in activation mechanisms and selectivity.There are 28 channel subunit members expressed in humans. A single de�ningfunctional characteristic among TRP channels has not yet been found, but onecan generally describe them as calcium-permeable cation channels with polymodalactivation properties (Ramsey et al., 2006). On the structural level, a commoncharacteristic is that the subunit proteins have six transmembrane segments. TheTRP channels play critical roles in sensory physiology of the peripheral nervoussystem, where they contribute to vision, taste, olfaction, hearing, touch and thermo-and osmosensation (Voets et al., 2004; Venkatachalam and Montell, 2007; Damannet al., 2008; Talavera et al., 2008). Furthermore, they enable individual cells to sensetheir local environment, such as changes in growth cues (TRPC1/3/5), vasculartone (TRPC3/6) and synaptic activity (TRPC1/3/4/5); the latter function beingthe major focus of this thesis. The list of cellular functions that TRP channels giverise to can be made very long, and the importance of their role in calcium signalinghas been particularly emphasized (Clapham et al., 2001; Minke, 2006; Birnbaumer,2009).

2.3.2 The origin of the name

While other cation-selective channels are classi�ed according to their selectivity orligand function, TRP channels are classi�ed as a group on the basis of gene sequencehomology, due to the complexity in their properties (Moran et al., 2004). In analogywith this (and unlike major ion channel families such as the voltage gated sodium,potassium and calcium channels), the TRP family is not named according to theessential function of its members. Rather, the origin of the name is related to howthe family was discovered. In 1969 a Drosophila (fruit �y) mutant was identi�ed thathad a transient rather than sustained response to prolonged illumination (Cosensand Manning, 1969). It was given the name �transient receptor potential� (TRP) byMinke et al. (1975). Several years passed before it was realized that the responsiblegene was actually encoding a transmembrane protein (Montell and Rubin, 1989),or more speci�cally an ion channel (Hardie and Minke, 1992). The TRP channelresearch �eld was only slowly developing during the �rst two decennia (Minke, 2006),but in recent years it has grown substantially as it has become clear that the TRP

Page 21: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.3. TRANSIENT RECEPTOR POTENTIAL (TRP) CHANNELS 13

Figure 2.3. TRPC ion channels, which form one of six major TRP subfamilies (top:from Clapham, 2003), are expressed in a variety of tissue and cell types (bottom left: fromAbramowitz and Birnbaumer, 2009). Genetic deletion experiments show that TRPCs areimportant on the behavioral level (bottom right: from Wu et al., 2010).

Page 22: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

14 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

ion channel family, with its 30 members, is very important for both peripheral andcentral cells of the mammalian nervous system.

2.3.3 Expressed in every tissue type

All known TRP channel types are expressed in the nervous system (Squire, 2009).In fact, TRP channels are widely expressed in all mammalian tissue types, see �gure2.3 (top and lower left). The TRPC channels, which are the focus of this thesis, areexpressed to various extents in di�erent brain regions. Strübing et al. (2001) studiedthe subcellular localization of TRPC1 in embryonic hippocampal neurons and foundthat it was distributed throughout the cell (soma, dendrites and axon), but notat synaptic structures. The subcellular localization in adult neurons is unknownbut suggested to di�er from that in embryonic neurons (Blair et al., 2009). vonBohlen und Halbach et al. (2005) showed that TRPC1 and TRPC5 are ubiquitous inmammalian medial temporal lobe structures and suggested that these proteins maytherefore play an important role in neural plasticity, learning and memory. Fowleret al. (2007) showed that TRPC4 and TRPC5 are expressed in several regions of thebrain (see table 2.1). See also the review by Abramowitz and Birnbaumer (2009) foradditional references on TRPC expression.

2.3.4 The TRPC subfamily

The TRP channels are divided into six subfamilies: TRPC, TRPV, TRPM, TRPA,TRPP, TRPML (channels from a seventh subfamily, TRPN, are not expressed inmammals but only in lower vertebrates and invertebrates). The TRPC (C denoting�canonical� or sometimes �classical�) channels, were the �rst to be discovered. In spiteof this, they are the least characterized (Minke, 2006), as many questions remainregarding their physiological functions and gating mechanisms. Like all members ofthe TRP family, the TRPC subunit proteins assemble as homo- or heterotetramersto form ion channels (Desai and Clapham, 2005; Schaefer, 2005; Villereal, 2006;Venkatachalam and Montell, 2007). It is thought that each subunit contributes toa shared selectivity �lter and ion conducting pore similar to that seen in potassiumchannels (Ramsey et al., 2006). There are seven TRPC subunits, TRPC1-7, thoughTRPC2 is not expressed in humans. Alternative splice variants have been observedfor all subunits except TRPC5 (Li et al., 2005). Based on sequence alignmentsand functional comparisons, TRPC subunits are sometimes grouped into: TRPC1,TRPC2, TRPC3/6/7, TRPC4/5.

Members within a TRPC subunit group tend to form heterotetrameric ion chan-nels together, although channels can consist of proteins from di�erent subunit groupsas well. In 2001, Strübing et al. showed that this may have important func-tional consequences, as they compared the magnitude of single channel conductancesand current-voltage relations between pure TRPC1 and TRPC5 homomeric chan-nels with TRPC1/5 heteromeric channels and found that it di�ered. Muscarinicreceptor-activated TRPC5 homomers had a single channel conductance of 38 pS andan inwardly rectifying current (i. e. the channel passes inward current more easilythan outward current), while TRPC1 homomers did not conduct currents at all.TRPC1/5 heteromers had a single channel conductance of 5 pS, and an outwardly

Page 23: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.3. TRANSIENT RECEPTOR POTENTIAL (TRP) CHANNELS 15

rectifying current similar to that of NMDA channels. The implication of their �nd-ing is quite important, namely that TRPC heteromers have di�erent biophysicalproperties from TRPC homomers, which extends the functional repertoire of TRPCchannels substantially as the number of possible subunit combinations is large. Thishas been pointed out in several reviews, such as: Pedersen et al. (2005); Schaefer(2005); Villereal (2006); Venkatachalam and Montell (2007). Neurons may bene-�t from this TRPC subunit promiscuity, but for scientists studying the resultingcurrents it becomes problematic, which I will discuss further in section 4.3.

Since many questions regarding the functional properties of TRPC channels stillremain to be resolved it is di�cult to give an overview of these, but I will give a fewexamples of what is known. Common to all TRPC channels (in addition to theirstructural similarities) is that:

• They are nonselective cation channels (like all other TRP channels). In otherwords, they can conduct sodium, potassium as well as calcium.

• They are weakly voltage-dependent (Squire, 2009).

• They can be activated downstream of the PLC pathway (Montell, 2005; Ped-ersen et al., 2005).

• They may be located and regulated within spatially con�ned calcium mi-crodomains (Ambudkar, 2006).

• They may play important roles in both sodium (Eder et al., 2005) and calcium(see references in section 2.3.1 above) signaling.

2.3.5 The (mGlu1/5) receptor activated TRPC current � ITRPC

2.3.5.1 Background

As mentioned above, it is known that TRPC currents can be activated in manydi�erent ways. Here, I will focus only on what is known about the aspects relating tothe main mechanism studied in this thesis, i. e. the TRPC mediated current � ITRPC� which is generated in response to synaptic activation of mGlu1/5 (or non-synapticactivation of M1). This current has been known and studied for several years,and has been reported in several regions of the brain: hippocampal CA1 (Congaret al., 1997), entorhinal cortex (Egorov et al., 2002; Fransén et al., 2006; Yoshidaet al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2010), cerebellum (Kim et al., 2003), substantia nigra parscompacta (Bengtson et al., 2004), amygdala (Faber et al., 2006), anterior cingulatecortex (Zhang and Séguéla, 2010), subiculum (Yoshida and Hasselmo, 2009) andprefrontal cortex (Sidiropoulou et al., 2009).

The ITRPC , which is an inward current leading to a depolarization of the mem-brane potential (Vm), has essentially been studied either in its subthreshold orsuprathreshold form of appearance. In its subthreshold, form ITRPC mediates aslow afterdepolarization (sADP) similar to the excitatory postsynaptic potentials(EPSPs) seen in response to AMPA or NMDA activation, described in section 2.2.4above. In its suprathreshold form, ITRPC mediates plateau potentials. During

Page 24: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

16 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

plateau potentials (PPs), which sometimes have a graded nature (Egorov et al.,2002; Fransén et al., 2006; Yoshida et al., 2008), the neuron generates persistenttrains of action potentials at a stable frequency during a long period of time. BothsADPs and PPs have been implicated as being important for proper mnemonic func-tioning, as these two phenomena allow the activity of a neuron to outlast the originalinput stimulus (Egorov et al., 2002; Yan et al., 2009).

ITRPC is activated downstream of Gαq/11, a G-protein pathway which in turn isactivated in response to activation of either the mGlu1/5 or muscarinic M1 receptor,as illustrated in �gure 2.2. Consequently, the current has been studied by activationor inactivation of either mGlu1/5 or M1. Both types of studies are relevant for thework in this thesis, but the mGlu1/5 induced mechanisms are of special interest.It should be mentioned that several names have been used to refer to what is nowbelieved to be a very similar (or identical) current: IsADP (Caeser et al., 1993; Yanet al., 2009), INCM (Shalinsky et al., 2002), ICAN (Fransén et al., 2006), �Alonso-current� (Hasselmo and Stern, 2006), mGluR−EPSC (Reichelt and Knöpfel, 2002).

2.3.5.2 Role of ITRPC in behavior and disease

There is much data indicating that TRPC channels are important in behavior. Ric-cio et al. (2009) studied TRPC5 knock-out mice and observed that fear levels inresponse to aversive stimuli were decreased as compared to control mice. Interest-ingly, they also showed that, in amygdala neurons, the amplitude of mGlu1/5 medi-ated postsynaptic responses were decreased. This is direct support for our workinghypothesis in Paper I and Paper II, namely that mGlu1/5 mediated TRPC currentsare important for synaptic transmission. Taken together, it also implies that thephenomenon we study is important on the behavioral level. The same conclusioncan be made from a study by Sidiropoulou et al. (2009), in which they show thatthe ITRPC mediated depolarization is modulated by dopamine and cocaine experi-ence. They discuss that, as a consequence, ITRPC may be important for cognitivephenomena such as attention, working memory, long-term memory and addiction.Additional support for the role of TRPC channels in working memory comes fromexperimental (Egorov et al., 2002) and modeling (Fransén et al., 2006) studies ofentorhinal pyramidal cells (see review by Hasselmo and Stern, 2006).

Several reviews have stressed the important role that TRPC channels may playin disease (e. g. Abramowitz and Birnbaumer, 2009; Selvaraj et al., 2010). Not onlyTRPC5 has been knocked-out in experiments, but also the other TRPC subtypes(except TRPC7). As can be seen in �gure 2.3 (lower right), these studies haveimplicated that TRPC channels may play important roles in behavior and disease.Other reviews have discussed not only TRPC channels but TRP channels in general,such as Nilius et al. (2007); Venkatachalam and Montell (2007) and all reviewsincluded in the 2007 special issue on �TRP Channels in Disease� of Biochimica etBiophysica Acta (Nilius, 2007).

2.3.5.3 Activation mechanisms

Although the mechanisms for ITRPC activation have been studied for many years,they are still not clear. However, a few recent papers have contributed to severalimportant insights that I will mention here. Key questions involve: What are the

Page 25: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.3. TRANSIENT RECEPTOR POTENTIAL (TRP) CHANNELS 17

molecular mechanisms for ITRPC activation? What is the role of intracellular Ca2+?Which subunits constitute the ion channels?

Yan et al. (2009) showed with a gene knock-out technique that, in cortical pyra-midal cells, Gαq/11 is required for generating the M1R/TRPC mediated sADP, andthe PLC (or more speci�cally, PLCβ1) pathway contributes strongly. Experimentaltechniques involving use of HEK293 (human embryonic kidney) cells are commonfor studying how certain combinations of membrane proteins (like ion channels) be-have when expressed in isolation. In the same study, Yan et al. expressed T-typevoltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC) together with TRPC5 in HEK293 cells andobtained a sADP. Importantly, the sADP was absent if the Ca2+ source (they usedT-type VGCCs, but pointed out that any Ca2+ source may be able to do the job)was excluded, indicating that Ca2+ is required for generating the sADP. When over-expressing TRPC5 or TRPC6 in cortical pyramidal cells they saw an enhancementof the sADP, and thus concluded that ITRPC mediate the sADP.

Zhang et al. (2010) showed, in EC pyramidal cells, that PLC (or more speci�cally,PLCβ) activation is required for generating the ITRPC current via muscarinic recep-tors. They also showed that decrease of PIP2 levels and permissive Ca2+ levels arerequired, and that IP3 receptors may be involved. Further, they found that ITRPCwas reduced after application of TRPC4/5-disrupting peptides, and that the I-V-curve of ITRPC is similar to those of TRPC-heteromers involving TRPC1, TRPC4and TRPC5, indicating that these subunits form the ion channels responsible formediating the ITRPC current. This hypothesis is supported by results from studiesshowing that these three subunits are indeed present in the rodent EC (?Fowleret al., 2007).

Gross et al. (2009) expressed TRPC5 channels in HEK293 cells, and observedthat intracellular Ca2+ either from stores or VGCCs can lead to activation ofTRPC5. They argued that the activation of TRPC5 by calcium is fast (and hencedirect), essential and even su�cient. In contrast with many other studies, theyshowed that inhibition of the PLC pathway does not a�ect this Ca2+-activation.

Furthermore, Blair et al. (2009) used HEK293 cells to express TRPC5 homomers.They showed that Ca2+ plays an important role in controlling the amplitude of ago-nist activated TRPC5 currents, while stressing the fact that Ca2+ a�ects numeroussteps before channel activation (including receptor desensitization, PLC activationand PKC activation), indicating that it is a complex process. Like Yan et al. (2009),they found that increases in Ca2+ by VGCCs (L-type, in this study) can triggerTRPC5 activation.

Kanki et al., 2001 expressed TRPC5 channels in frog eggs (Xenopus oocytes) andshowed that IP3 receptors are important for TRPC5 activation, a result which ispartially supported by the study by Zhang et al. (2010) described above. It shouldbe noted, however, that this is a debated topic (see e. g. reviews by Vazquez et al.,2004; Plant and Schaefer, 2005; Birnbaumer, 2009).

Taken together, these reports suggest that the Gαq/11 pathway (including PLCβ,at least in EC pyramidal cells) is involved, and that an increase in cytosolic Ca2+ isrequired for ITRPC activation. They also suggest that activation of IP3R or VGCCscan contribute to the increase in Ca2+. I EC pyramidal cells, it is likely that ITRPCis mediated by channels formed by TRPC1/4/5 subunits.

Page 26: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

18 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

2.3.5.4 ITRPC is poorly characterized and often absent in experimental

observations

Although shown to be of great importance (see e.g. Batchelor and Garthwaite,1997; Riccio et al., 2009; Sidiropoulou et al., 2009), mGlu mediated currents suchas ITRPC are not always seen in experiments. This may be due to a number ofreasons. In some systems the current does not exist. It may also be the case thatcalcium sensitive potassium channels are activated in response to the calcium in�ux,leading a hyperpolarization that partially or completely occludes the depolarizationcaused by ITRPC . It is furthermore likely that ITRPC is sometimes lumped togetherwith the much more well-recognized INMDA since their time scales partially over-lap. Their sum may then simply be interpreted as and referred to as an �NMDAcurrent�, although �NMDA-like current� or �slow synaptic current� would be moreproper descriptions. ITRPC may also be hard to see in experiments if for examplea low temperature is used, since the probability of synaptic glutamate release isthen reduced, with the consequence that little glutamate reaches perisynaptic andextrasynaptic regions (where mGlu1/5 is situated). An important problem relatedto identifying ITRPC is that it may require activation of VGCCs, in turn requiringdepolarization caused by activation of ionotropic synaptic receptors. VGCCs willnot be su�ciently activated if the synaptic stimulus induced membrane depolariza-tion is too weak. For the same reason, it is problematic to attempt to study ITRPCby simply applying blockers of AMPA and NMDA. To block the TRPC channelsthemselves would be much better for studying their in�uence on synaptic signaling.Such blockers are being developed (Zhang et al., 2010), but are still not widely used.Blocking mGlu1/5 is another option, but since these receptors act on intracellularpathways that have many other downstream targets than TRPC such studies do notgive full insight into the nature of ITRPC . In addition, the relative contribution ofmGlu1 and mGlu5 is still unclear, though it has been argued that the two receptorshave a synergistic e�ect (Gee et al., 2003). Finally, there is also the option to knock-out the genes coding for e. g. TRPC5 channels (Riccio et al., 2009), but in suchstudies it is di�cult to control for the possibility that compensatory mechanismsare triggered or that homomeric TRPC channels are formed in the absence of theTRPC5 partner.

2.4 Dendrites

It has in recent years become clear that dendritic processing is complex, (see e. g.reviews by Segev, 1998; Reyes, 2001; Häusser and Mel, 2003; London and Häusser,2005; Magee and Johnston, 2005; Sidiropoulou et al., 2006; Johnston and Narayanan,2008; Spruston, 2008 and the book devoted to dendrites Stuart et al., 2007). In thissection I will mention a few interesting studies pointing to this complexity, and alsodiscuss spatio-temporal summation of synaptic input. Finally, I will discuss what isknown about the role of dendrites in disease.

Page 27: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.4. DENDRITES 19

2.4.1 Dendrites extend the neuronal computation toolbox

The role of dendrites in neuronal computation was poorly understood for a longtime. One of the �rst important insights came from a modeling study by Agmon-Snir et al. (1998), which demonstrated a function of auditory neurons that couldnot be carried out without nonlinear synaptic integration in the dendrites. Theycompared a point-neuron model with a bipolar-neuron model and showed that de-tection of sound localization was improved in the latter. This auditory neuron issupposed to respond (generate an action potential) only if input arrive both fromthe left and right ear, and achieves this by receiving synaptic inputs from the twoears on di�erent dendritic branches (Segev, 1998; London and Häusser, 2005). Asa result, two inputs from the same ear might not sum enough to exceed spikingthreshold. This is due to shunting inhibition caused by a decrease in electrical driv-ing force for synaptic potentials. Conversely, inputs coming from di�erent ears willexperience less summation sublinearity and may exceed spiking threshold. If a sim-ple bipolar neuron can achieve this kind of algorithmic computation, then what arefor example pyramidal or Purkinje neurons capable of, considering the complexityin morphological structure of these neurons?

In addition to being morphologically complex, dendritic trees are equipped witha plethora of ion channels, as mentioned in section 2.2.3. The presence of theseactive conductances supports phenomena such as back-propagation of action poten-tials, local increases in Ca2+ concentrations and modulation of synaptic potentials(Schiller and Schiller, 2001).

2.4.1.1 Dendritic functional compartmentalization

Katz et al. (2009) recently showed, using electron microscopy in combination withcomputational modeling, that hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons do not integratesynaptic inputs by simply summing them in the soma. Rather, local regenerativespikes are generated in branches of the dendritic tree and these signals may in turnpropagate and undergo computation in a second stage, which is similar to whatLosonczy and Magee (2006) argued for. In line with these results from CA1 neu-rons, Polsky et al. (2004) showed that a similar two-layer computation may exist inneocortical pyramidal neurons, similarly being supported by dendritic spikes. Re-search on these dendritic spikes has increased in recent years, and a study by Larkumet al. (2009) could possibly be considered a landmark study. They argued for a �uni-fying principle� of synaptic integration (in pyramidal cells) that involves spatiallycon�ned regions of the dendritic tree supporting di�erent kinds of dendritic spikes(see also Larkum and Nevian, 2008). According to this hypothesis, sodium spikescan be generated throughout the dendritic tree, whereas initiation of calcium spikesare limited to a zone on the apical trunk, and NMDA spikes can only be initiated atdistal regions of the dendritic tree. An interesting phenomenon emerging from theseproperties is a kind of chain-reaction, as discussed in Williams and Wozny (2009).Inputs arriving at distal parts of the tree may be translated into NMDA spikes,which in turn may induce calcium spikes and �nally an axonal action potential.These studies are very interesting, as they suggest that the neuron process informa-tion not only in the soma, but also in spatially con�ned distal parts of the dendritictree. I recommend reviews by Häusser and Mel (2003) and Magee and Johnston

Page 28: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

20 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

(2005) for further discussions on functional dendritic compartmentalization.Furthermore, it has been suggested that plasticity, being a very important func-

tion of the nervous system, can exist speci�cally for individual dendritic branches(see Frick et al., 2004; Losonczy et al., 2008, and reviews by Frick and Johnston,2005; Sjöström et al., 2008).

2.4.1.2 Dendritic spatio-temporal summation

Under which circumstances does spatial summation occur in a pyramidal cell? Letus �rst look at a few numbers. As discussed above, a number of studies has suggestedthat sections of the dendritic tree (e. g. a branch) may function as computationalsubunits. Larkum et al. (2009) showed that dendritic spikes are local events, whichdo not spread to neighboring tuft branches. In line with this, Polsky et al. (2004)showed that e�ective spatial summation of two adjacent synaptic inputs occurs onlywhen the distance between these is less than 40µm, which suggests that this numbercan be an estimation of the length of a dendritic functional compartment. As anestimate of the density of excitatory inputs to a pyramidal neuron, one can measurethe density of spines. According to Stuart et al. (2007), CA1 neurons are �very spiny�,averaging 2.5 spines per µm, while pyramidal cells of the visual cortex are �muchless spiny�, averaging only about 1.5 spines per µm, which gives us an approximatenumber of 2 spines/µm. The dendritic functional compartment would thus have 80synapses. It has further been suggested that only 10% of the neurons in brain areactive (Shoham et al., 2006), which could mean that only every tenth presynapticterminal actually receives input. Furthermore, it is important to consider that, evenif a neuron generates a spike which propagate to a presynaptic terminal, it is notguaranteed that the signal reaches the postsynaptic terminal, since the probabilityof vesicle release may be low. Borst (2010) has recently suggested that, in contrastto previous estimates of typical release probability which he believes are high, 50%,actual release probability in vivo may be closer to 10%. If we multiply all thesenumbers we get an estimate of how many synaptic inputs arrive at a functionaldendritic compartment: 80 ∗ 0.1 ∗ 0.1 = 0.8 ≈ 1. Admittedly, these numbers maybe in the lower range. Nevertheless it is likely that, under certain circumstances,no spatial summation will occur since only a few synapses receives input. It isof course possible, as hypothesized by Larkum and Nevian (2008), that synapsesare functionally clustered on dendritic branches, meaning that functionally relatedinput may arrive on the same dendritic segment. On the other hand, a recentstudy by Jia et al. (2010) shows that synapses involved in a certain class of synapticinputs (a�erent sensory inputs with the same orientation preference) may be widelydispersed over the dendritic tree, and not grouped together on a speci�c dendriticbranch.

From this reasoning, one may suspect that spatial summation is limited in caseswhen input is spatially sparse. So, what about temporal summation? Can tempo-rally sparse input be summed? Is it even likely that input arrives at low frequencies,in a physiologically realistic situation? The latter question has a relatively straight-forward answer. The code with which neurons communicate is still poorly under-stood, but, as described in section 2.2.6.2, it is actually possible to measure the spikeoutput of single neurons (single-unit activity) in living and behaving animals. Look-

Page 29: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

2.4. DENDRITES 21

ing at single-unit recording data from studies using a wide range of tasks (spatialorientation, working memory, categorization, recognition), recordings from variousbrain regions (hippocampus, entorhinal cortex, prefrontal cortex, amygdala) in var-ious species (rodent, non-human primate, human), one �nds that spike frequenciesduring behavior are typically low, around 8Hz (see Paper I and II for details). Thisindicates that the dendritic branch which receives this input may need to possessmechanisms allowing for temporal summation of such low frequencies. In Chapter 4I will argue that ITRPC is one such mechanism.

2.4.2 Dendrites and disease

Even if studying functional properties of dendrites in the normal brain is excit-ing and rewarding enough, it is also important to recognize all the research thathas associated certain neurological, psychiatric and developmental disorders withchanges in dendrites (Stuart et al., 2007). Alzheimer's disease, schizophrenia andepilepsy are examples of disorders that have been suggested to depend on dendriticabnormalities. Structural changes such as loss of spines, changes in spine size andshape, reduced dendritic branching patterns and shortened dendritic length, haveall been described for di�erent neurological diseases. Unfortunately, there is a lackof understanding of how these structural changes correlate with changes in neuronalfunction such as synaptic integration, plasticity, excitability and �ring behaviors, somuch more research on this is needed. Besides structural changes, disorders relatingto ion channels and receptors are likely to be important. In addition, it can bementioned that work from our laboratory has shown a potential role for a dendriti-cally located fast potassium channel in epilepsy (Fransén and Tigerholm, 2010). Foranyone interested in the topic of dendrites and disease I recommend chapter 20 ofthe also otherwise very interesting book called Dendrites (Stuart et al., 2007), as astarting point.

Page 30: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

22 CHAPTER 2. BIOLOGICAL BACKGROUND

Page 31: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Chapter 3

Models and methods

In this chapter I will, after giving a brief introduction to computational neuroscienceand multicompartmental modeling in particular, review a few studies in which mul-ticompartmental models have been successfully used. I will then describe the detailsof the model I have used for testing our hypothesis. I will also brie�y describe sim-ulation protocols and analysis methods, and �nally list the software that has beenused for this thesis.

3.1 Review of computational neuroscience

In this section I will give a short review of the use and usefulness of models inneuroscience. To begin with it may be worth giving a general re�ection of the role ofcomputational methods in neuroscience. Studies relating to the science of memoryare ubiquitous in the �eld of neuroscience, and much of the work behind this thesishas been devoted to study and re�ect over memory concepts. I came across a bookchapter entitled �Encoding: Models linking neural mechanisms to behavior�, writtenby Hasselmo (Roediger et al., 2007), in which he nicely expresses the importance ofincluding mathematical theories to a larger extent in memory research, though itshould be true for neuroscienti�c studies in general:

�The discussion of memory concepts is useful, but ultimately verbal termsare insu�cient for describing memory function. A full description of anyphysical system requires an e�ective mathematical theory. True under-standing of memory mechanisms will ultimately require a rigorous frame-work, which will only partially map to current colloquial and scienti�cterms describing memory concepts. The mathematical description ofmemory function will be structured according to neural mechanisms andtheir role in behavioral action selection by an agent in an environment.�

3.1.1 The Hodgkin and Huxley model and beyond

The work by Hodgkin and Huxley (1952), for which they received the Nobel Prizein 1963, is considered by many to be among the most important contributions to

23

Page 32: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

24 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

the �eld of neuroscience. Their model was a mathematical description of how twovoltage-dependent membrane conductances can give rise to the action potential phe-nomenon and provided a simple and helpful explanation to what had previouslybeen poorly understood. It is thus a good example of how a mathematical theory ishelpful, or even necessary, to understand and describe certain complex phenomena.The model by Hodgkin and Huxley was the �rst to incorporate the fact that neu-ronal membrane posits separate, ion-selective, voltage-dependent membrane conduc-tances, i.e. ion channels. Their model included Na+ and K+ channels, and althoughsimpli�ed when compared to the biophysical reality, it was possible to simulate thebasic components of an action potential. The concept of time- and voltage-dependentgating parameters n, m and h was introduced, together with descriptions of corre-sponding rate constants α and β. This framework, which I described to some detailin my master thesis (Petersson, 2007), is utilized in or has largely in�uenced mostconductance-based models of today. Ion channel research took a big leap forward,but it is still today not possible to have a perfect model of neither of the two channels,although (hopefully) most models are satisfactory accurate for their purpose.

The classical Na+ and K+ channels are essential for neuronal function, but so aremany other channels that currently lack a satisfactory description. Thus, much moreresearch is required to fully explore the function of ion channels. Many laboratoriestoday use computational models to study ion channels, often in combination withexperimental techniques.

3.1.2 Choosing an abstraction level

When modeling single neuron dynamics a proper level of abstraction must be cho-sen, ranging from detailed compartment models to black-box models (Herz et al.,2006). For several reasons it is desirable to have a reduced complexity. With fewerdimensions it is more feasible to achieve a mathematical understanding of modelingresults. Reduced models are also less computationally demanding and may thereforeallow for more rigorous sensitivity analyses, in turn leading to better understandingof the roles and importance of various parameters.

In this thesis I am studying the in�uence of dendritic ion channel properties onsynaptic integration. It is not obvious which level of abstraction is most suitablefor this purpose, but a starting point is to look at other, similar studies. The onesI have encountered so far commonly use a few hundred compartments, belongingto a category denoted multicompartment models. Multicompartment models arewell-suited for studying the spatiotemporal dendritic integration of synaptic inputs,and allow for modeling of non-uniformly distributed ion channels. I chose to usea multicompartment model published by Poirazi et al. (2003), which I describe insection 3.2 below.

3.1.3 Multicompartmental modeling

Multicompartmental modeling theory, which is based on cable theory, was devel-oped by Rall (1959). This theory provides a discrete solution to the cable equation,by which the membrane potential (Vm) at any point in the dendritic tree can becalculated as a function of time (Johnston and Wu, 1995). Multicompartmental

Page 33: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

3.1. REVIEW OF COMPUTATIONAL NEUROSCIENCE 25

models are typically built in such ways that they capture the passive electrical prop-erties of a neuron, incorporating for example a diameter which changes throughoutthe dendritic tree, a�ecting both the membrane time and length constants (τm andλm). When building a model, the morphological structure of a neuron is �rst ex-tracted using a staining technique so that somatic and dendritic sections can beidenti�ed. Each of these sections are then divided, or discretized, into small com-partments. These compartments are individually modeled as electrical circuits andcoupled with simple resistive components. The electrical circuit of each compart-ment may be simple (passive) and only consist of one resistive and one capacitivecomponent in parallel, mediating the current �owing between the inside and outsideof the cell membrane. In this case, the resistive current represents the �ow of ionsthrough pores (e. g. ion channels) in the membrane, while the capacitive currentrepresents the thin bilipid membrane layer. However, compartments may also bemore complex (active) and contain a multitude of ion channels, ion pumps, recep-tors, synaptic currents, biochemical reactions, etc, and the complexity may varyamong di�erent sections of the neuron.

3.1.3.1 Insights gained using multicompartment models

Multicompartment neuron models have been used for a variety of research questions.They are especially useful for studying aspects of dendritic integration relating tomechanisms such as ion channels, receptors and arising nonlinear phenomena suchas dendritic spikes and backpropagating action potentials. Here I will describe a fewexamples of studies and areas of research I have encountered in which multicom-partmental models have been successfully used.

Passive properties of the dendrites are important for the electrical behavior ofneurons. Thanks to compartment models, several consequences of the detailed ge-ometry of the dendritic tree can be understood (Segev and London, 2000). Forexample, since postsynaptic responses (EPSPs) evoked in distal thin dendrites willexperience a large current sink when traveling to the soma, voltage attenuation isstronger than for an EPSP of similar amplitude traveling in the soma-dendrite di-rection, as shown by Rall and Rinzel (1973). This and other insights gained frommodeling passive (and active) dendritic trees are nicely explained in chapters 13 and14 of the book Dendrites (Stuart et al., 2007). The authors point out that passive(not only active) properties need to be taken into account in order to understandthe mechanisms of dendritic integration.

Deep brain stimulation (DBS) is a neuroprosthetic technique developed to helppatients su�ering from neurological movement and a�ective disorders such as chronicpain, Parkinson's disease, tremor and dystonia. In DBS, electrical stimulation is ap-plied via stimulation electrodes that are implanted into a certain region of the brain,thereby a�ecting the neural activity in such a way that the patient is relieved ofsome symptoms. The most successful application of DBS so far has been to helppatients with Parkinson's disease by stimulating structures in the basal ganglia. Thetechnique works well but side e�ects are common. Since the underlying mechanismsare not fully understood, computational models are used to gain further insights,thereby improving the technique. McIntyre and his group at Case Western Univer-sity contributes to the improvement of DBS techniques by coupling detailed multi-

Page 34: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

26 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

compartment models to models of the electrical �eld induced by the DBS electrode(Miocinovic et al., 2006; McIntyre et al., 2007; Miocinovic et al., 2007). In a recentstudy on Parkinson's patients they showed that side e�ects were reduced if stimulusparameters predicted by computational models were used, as compared with typicalclinically used stimulus parameters (Frankemolle et al., 2010). Their models werepatient speci�c, in the sense that model parameters were individually chosen for eachpatient, based on data from structural imaging, surgical targeting coordinates andmicroelectrode recordings. Their study provides a powerful demonstration of howcomputational models can be used for clinically relevant research questions. Notethat a morphologically detailed model might be necessary for capturing the behaviorof a neuron in the spatially complex electrical �eld that results from a DBS electrode(Herz et al., 2006), as well as for other types of extracellular stimulation techniques.

After developing the CA1 cell model which this thesis is based on, Poirazi et al.(2003) were able to predict that distal dendrites can act as functional computationalsubunits. The outputs of these subunits, which individually summate inputs with asigmoidal activation function, then sum linearly in the soma. The model predictionswere later experimentally con�rmed (Polsky et al., 2004; Losonczy and Magee, 2006),as I have described in section 2.4.1.1.

Theoretical insights from dendritic modeling that has been experimentally vali-dated are mentioned in a review by Segev and London (2000) and include synapticboosting, coincidence detection, the function of spines, background activity, andmotion-sensitive tangential cells. Similarly, a review by Kath (2005) exempli�esthe usefulness of dendritic modeling by discussing a number of studies relating tosynaptic e�cacy, membrane resistance, intracellular resistivity, dependency of back-propagating action potentials on dendritic morphology and ion channel properties,e�ect of dendritic structure on �ring patterns and coincidence detection tuned bydendritic branching patterns, among other things. More examples can be found ina review by Herz et al. (2006), which provides a description of how modeling predic-tions (Jaeger et al., 1997) regarding a net inhibitory synaptic current underlying invivo spike patterns of Purkinje cells were subsequently con�rmed to be true in anexperimental study (Jaeger and Bower, 1999). See also the review by Sidiropoulouet al. (2006) for examples of dendritic modeling studies. Finally, as the distribu-tion of ion channels is often non-uniform along the dendritic tree, thus complicatingthe analysis, modeling is often used to increase the understanding of experimentalresults (see review by Migliore and Shepherd, 2002).

3.2 Model used in this thesis

In order to study how the synaptically activated TRPC current (ITRPC) a�ects den-dritic integration we used a multicompartment model of a pyramidal neuron. Wetuned the ITRPC dynamics to replicate experimental data provided by our collabo-rator, who recorded somatic responses in rodent entorhinal cortex following synapticstimulation, as described in Paper I. In this section I describe and discuss the variouscomponents of the model.

Page 35: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

3.2. MODEL USED IN THIS THESIS 27

3.2.1 The Poirazi model

Development of a detailed compartment model with biophysically realistic propertiesand several ion channels is a large project and outside the scope of this thesis.Instead, I decided to use a morphologically detailed (see �gure 3.2) model developedby Poirazi et al. (2003), which is available for download in the ModelDB onlinedatabase.

The model contains 17 types of ion channels as well as calcium dynamics, whichis of high importance for the mechanisms we study. The details of how the modelwas tuned can be found in the supplementary material published together with thearticle (Poirazi et al., 2003). The model represents what is probably the most well-studied pyramidal neuron: the hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neuron. The authorswanted to construct a state-of-the-art compartment model, taking into account arange of physiological properties. Brie�y, they tuned the internal parameters of theion channels, many of which have a nonuniform distribution along the dendritic tree,both from studies primarily concerned with individual ion channels properties andfrom studies primarily concerned with dendritic physiology and synaptic integra-tion. Several validation studies were performed, including studying the e�ect of thehyperpolarization activated Ih-current on input resistance and steady-state voltagepropagation, and properties of back-propagating and dendritically initiated actionpotentials. The model was also able to capture the spatial summation propertiesin both the apical trunk and thin dendrites that had been found in experiments byCash and Yuste (1999). In addition to this, as I have already described in section3.1.3.1, predictions made with the model have been shown to be valid in subsequentexperimental studies. One important shortcoming of the model, pointed out by theauthors, is that relatively little attention was paid to the properties of the basaldendrites. Therefore it would not be appropriate to use the model for studying theproperties of basal dendrites.

It is a di�cult task to say whether a model is good or bad, and this of coursedepends very much on what it is used for. Nevertheless, based on the work brie�ydescribed above, it should be safe to say that the Poirazi model is well-suited for thepurpose of our study. Furthermore, the Poirazi model has been frequently referredto and cited by several leading scientists in the �eld of dendritic research.

3.2.2 Extending the Poirazi model by adding ITRPC

In order to adjust the Poirazi model to suit the exploration of our research questions,I developed and added three major model components: glutamate concentrationdynamics, a metabotropic glutamate receptor (mGlu1/5) and a transient receptorpotential channel (TRPC). In �gure 3.1 I present these model components in aschematic overview, and in the following sections I will describe them in detail.

3.2.2.1 Glutamate concentration

In the Poirazi model there is no explicit module implemented for modeling the dy-namics of the glutamate concentration released at the synapse. The research regard-ing neurotransmitter concentration pro�les is still relatively incomplete, as pointedout in a recent review by Scimemi and Beato (2009). In Paper II I simply modeled

Page 36: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

28 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

Figure 3.1. Model for ITRPC activation. The Gαq/11/PLC pathway, which is triggered byactivation of mGlu1/5 (or M1R, in the persistent-spiking model), leads to TRPC (ITRPC)activation. Although the mechanisms are not clear, it is believed that this activation partiallydepends on Ca2+ released from stores. Moreover, voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC,opened by depolarization caused by AMPA and NMDA activation) may contribute to furtherincreases in Ca2+, an e�ect we include in the VDA model, but not in the VIA model. Seethe text for details.

Page 37: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

3.2. MODEL USED IN THIS THESIS 29

the glutamate concentration as a square pulse lasting 100ms. As the maximum fre-quency never exceeded 10Hz in that study we did not need to consider summationof glutamate since it follows that triggering of synaptic inputs are always separatedby at least 100ms. From the study in Paper II we realized that the glutamate pulseshape had a signi�cant e�ect on the simulation results, and decided to do a moreaccurate glutamate model for the work in Paper I.

The glutamate model in Paper I is based on data from a recent experimentalstudy indicating that the glutamate concentration following a synaptic input has arather slow decay, several hundred milliseconds, and undergoes summation (Hireset al., 2008). Note that, due to limitations in the temporal resolution, the glutamateconcentration dynamics found in that study might not be suitable when modelingglutamate receptors in the synaptic cleft (AMPA and NMDA), but is more favorableto use when modeling extrasynaptic receptors activated by glutamate spillover, suchas mGlu1/5 (Scimemi and Beato, 2009). I thus designed the glutamate model inPaper I to replicate a slowly decaying glutamate concentration, allowing summationat low frequencies. This model, represented as the �extrasynaptic [Glu]� box in �gure3.1, is described in detail in the method section of Paper I.

3.2.2.2 Metabotropic glutamate receptor

In both Paper I and Paper II we wanted to simulate realistic synaptic activation ofmGlu1/5. An important problem we became aware of was the lack of data availablefor describing the dynamics of the receptor activation. One might be surprised aboutthis, considering how important mGlu1/5 is for neuronal function, and consideringhow well the excitatory ionotropic receptors currents mediated by AMPA and NMDAcan be modeled (Destexhe et al., 1994b). However, the ionotropic receptors aremore straightforward in the sense that they are directly connected to a synapticion channel current. As a consequence, to model the ionotropic synaptic currents,including the chain of events following synaptic stimulation (glutamate release andremoval, receptor binding, channel opening) is relatively simple since it is easy tomeasure the currents. For metabotropic receptors the situation is more complex, asthe corresponding chain of events also involves activation of intracellular pathwaysbefore the current �ow is initiated. This may be one reason why currents mediatedby mGlu are rarely included in compartment models, something I will further discussin section 4.4.

We based our mGlu1/5 model on a GABAB model by Destexhe and Sejnowski(1995). The GABAB receptor is similar to mGlu1/5 in the sense that it is a synapti-cally activated metabotropic receptor which activates G-protein cascades, althoughdownstream pathways and targets are di�erent. The kinetic reactions describingmGlu1/5 activation can be presented in several steps. First, the mGlu1/5 receptorbinds to the transmitter substance T (concentration of glutamate):

R0 + Tk1−→←−k2

R (3.1)

dR

dt= k1 · T ·R0 − k2 ·R (3.2)

Page 38: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

30 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

R0 represents the fraction of receptors that are not bound to glutamate and Rthe fraction of activated receptors. k1 and k2 are rate constants. Since the sum ofR0 and R must be one the equation becomes:

dR

dt= k1 · T · (1−R)− k2 ·R (3.3)

For tuning the rate constants k1 and k2 I used data from a recent study byMarcaggi et al. (2009), which is the �rst study to report a detailed description ofmGlu dynamics. The authors provided a detailed scheme for mGlu1 activation,but our simple model was able to capture the essential dynamics, as described inPaper I. By applying square glutamate concentration pulses they could show that thedeactivation time constant of R is seemingly independent of glutamate concentration(and estimated it to 50ms). This is in line with equation 3.3, which similarly suggeststhe deactivation to be independent of glutamate (when T = 0 at the end of thesquare pulse dR

dt is only dependent on R). The activation time constant, on theother hand, decreases exponentially with glutamate concentration in their study. Tomodel R I therefore had to choose a value of T , preferably as physiologically realisticas possible. Unfortunately a detailed characterization of the spatio-temporal pro�leof synaptically released glutamate does not exist (Scimemi and Beato, 2009). A fewsuggestions of extrasynaptic T exist, however, which I will discuss next.

3.2.2.3 Choosing a value of glutamate concentration

In the synaptic cleft T is in the order of 1mM, but peri- and extrasynaptically (i.e.outside the synaptic cleft, where mGlu1/5 receptors are located) the value is proba-bly smaller. Hires et al. (2008) observed very small T transients, in the order of 1µM.Their technique, however, has a limited temporal resolution (as discussed in theirpaper) and the large initial glutamate transients may not be captured (Marcaggiet al., 2009). Previous mGlu1/5 models have assumed 10µM to be a reasonablevalue, but their argumentation for this is rather loose (Fiala et al., 1996; Steuberand Willshaw, 2004; Steuber et al., 2006). Rusakov and Kullmann (1998) predicted,using a model of extrasynaptic glutamate di�usion, that extrasynaptic glutamatereaches at least 50µM, whereas Dzubay and Jahr (1999) estimated the value to liewithin the range 160 to 190µM. We chose to use 200µM when tuning our model,which is in line with the two lastly mentioned studies and is also a value for whichthe receptor is fully activated (Marcaggi et al., 2009). Furthermore, 200µM is thevalue chosen by Marcaggi et al. for the major part of their kinetic analysis. Finally,an additional argument for physiological values of T being larger than just a few µMis that extracellular glutamate in the intact mammalian brain is thought be in aninterval of 0.5 to 5µM at rest (Featherstone and Shippy, 2008).

3.2.2.4 G-protein mediated cascade as a black box

Next, the mGlu1/5 triggered activation of the G-protein mediated cascade was mod-eled. Although it is known that Gαq/11/PLC activation and PIP2 cleavage is in-volved, kinetic data is lacking for the chain of events taking place between mGlu1/5activation and TRPC activation, so we chose to model this in a black box fashion.

Page 39: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

3.2. MODEL USED IN THIS THESIS 31

We introduced x, the concentration of an unknown metabolite that reaches andactivates TRPC, and calculated it as:

x = C1 ·Rn

Rn + C2(3.4)

We chose to use n = 4 in line with Destexhe and Sejnowski (1995). Values of C1

and C2 were chosen in the TRPC tuning process described in Paper I. As describedin Paper I, x is not independent of Ca2+, and may partly consist of Ca2+ releasedfrom stores. This is illustrated in �gure 3.1 by the arrow drawn from the gray boxto the Ca2+ box.

3.2.2.5 TRPC gating dynamics

Previous models of the TRPC current ITRPC , often referred to as ICAN (calciumactivated non-speci�c cation current), has focused on the role of calcium as activator(Destexhe et al., 1994a; Fransén et al., 2002, 2006), but we have not found anymodel that focused on mGlu1/5 as activator. We therefore developed an mGlu1/5activated ITRPC model, as described in Paper I. In this and the following section, Iwill discuss the various components of that model. In line with how Destexhe et al.(1994a) modeled ICAN , I described ITRPC according to:

ITRPC = g ·m · f(Vm) (3.5)

where g is the maximal conductance and f(Vm) describes the voltage dependence(see the following section). m denotes the gating parameter and is modeled simplyas having two states, open and closed, with rate constants α and β, which is theclassical Hodgkin and Huxley formalism:

openα−→←−β

closed (3.6)

Destexhe et al. (1994a) let β be a constant and α have a dependence on intra-cellular calcium concentration according to:

α = Kα · [Ca]2i (3.7)

Since data is lacking for the exact Ca2+ dependence of ITRPC we simpli�edthe equation by omitting the exponent from equation 3.7. Next, we modi�ed theequation to account for the in�uence of mGlu1/5, by adding the factor x describedin the previous section. Now it is important to note that we (in Paper I) evaluatedtwo models in parallel. The more complex model, in which calcium from voltagedependent calcium channels (VGCCs) a�ects ITRPC , is called the VDA (voltagedependent activation) model. In the VIA (voltage independent activation) model,ITRPC does not depend on calcium from VGCCs, but activation of mGlu1/5 (x > 0)is su�cient for opening the TRPC channel. In both models we assume that themGlu1/5 mediated activation signal x (described in the previous section) producesa shift in the calcium dependence. An argument for assuming a shift is that thevoltage dependence of other TRP channels appears to be shifted in response toimportant activating factors. Speci�cally, the voltage dependence of TRPM8 is

Page 40: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

32 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

shifted by menthol or cooling, and the voltage dependence of TRPV1 is shifted bycapsaicin (the active component of chili pepper) or heating (Voets et al., 2004).Furthermore, the metabolite x induced shift in calcium activation we propose forTRPC activation is in line with how PIP2 has been suggested to a�ect TRPM4(Nilius et al., 2006). There are important di�erences between TRPC and TRPM4channels however. PIP2 activates the latter while it appears to inhibit the former(Otsuguro et al., 2008). The shift in our model is implemented simply by adding xto the Ca2+ concentration:

α = Kα(x+ [Ca]i − Cath) (3.8)

In equation 3.8 we have also added Cath, representing a calcium threshold whichthe sum of x + [Ca]i needs to exceed for the TRPC channel to be opened. Conse-quently, the current is zero at rest. This calcium threshold represents both a calciumbu�er which may become saturated, and a non-linear dose dependency of [Ca]i onthe TRPC channel. [Ca]i in equation 3.8 represents Ca2+ originating from VGCCs.In the VIA model, where we assume that this VGCC mediated Ca2+ is not requiredfor ITRPC activation, we let [Ca]i = Cath so that 3.8 simply becomes:

α = Kαx (3.9)

It should be noted that x in equation 3.9 implicitly includes the increase inintracellular calcium which is mediated by IP3 receptors. This equation is also usedin Paper II, since that ITRPC model is similar to the VIA model described here. Aminor di�erence exists between the VIA model of Paper I and Paper II, however. Inthe latter we assumed [Ca]i > Cath at rest, resulting in non-zero ITRPC at rest.

In �gure 3.1 I illustrate the ITRPC model components described here. The threeboxes marked by a dashed rectangle are not included in the VIA model, but only inthe VDA model. Note the feedback loop present in the VDA model, arising since theITRPC mediated depolarization activates VGCC, which results in increased calcium,in turn activating ITRPC . Furthermore, I have marked the acetylcholine (ACh)activated muscarinic receptor (M1) components with a dotted rectangle. Thesecomponents are not central to this thesis but are worth mentioning since they areimportant for �gure M6 in Paper I, which shows that the cell model is capable ofproducing persistent spiking.

For tuning the dynamics of the TRPC model in Paper I we used data fromsynaptic stimulation experiments. For Paper II, however, we tuned the model afterdata from current injection experiments from which much longer time scales forITRPC activation are observed. Whereas a typical decay rate from a TRPC mediatedEPSP following synaptic stimulation is around 300ms, decay rates following currentinjection can be larger than 3 000ms.

3.2.2.6 TRPC voltage dependence

In 2001 it was reported that heteromeric TRPC channels can have an NMDA-likevoltage dependence (Strübing et al., 2001). The result has been replicated manytimes, and seems to appear only for heteromers (e.g. TRPC1+TRPC5) and notfor homomers (e.g. TRPC5+TRPC5). The NMDA-like voltage dependence was

Page 41: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

3.3. SIMULATIONS AND ANALYSIS 33

originally thought to be caused by a magnesium block, just like for the NMDAchannel. However, experiments have shown (e. g. Faber et al., 2006) that the NMDA-like curve remains even after magnesium is removed, so the mechanism is still notclear. It should also be mentioned that the TRPC voltage dependence has beenindicated to undergo changes during the activation cycles (Obukhov and Nowycky,2008).

In both Paper I and II, I designed the voltage dependence, i. e. the f(Vm) termin equation 3.5, so that the model replicates recent data from ITRPC activation inthe EC (Zhang et al., 2010), as described in Paper I.

3.2.2.7 Modi�ed calcium channels

Studies have indicated that calcium from VGCC subtype CaL contribute to TRPCactivation in EC pyramidal neurons (Egorov et al., 2002). Among the four CaL sub-types (CaL1.1-1.4) CaL1.3 stands out as being activated at lower voltages, whereasthe other three subtypes have higher activation thresholds (Lipscombe et al., 2004).In the original Poirazi model, the CaL channel has a high activation threshold, andhence does not activate for subthreshold synaptic inputs. I therefore shifted the CaLvoltage dependence into a more low-threshold range, as described in Paper I.

3.3 Simulations and analysis

3.3.1 Synaptic stimulation

As described in section 2.2, the electrical properties of neurons can be studied usingelectrophysiological techniques. Our experimental collaborator measured somaticpotentials in EC pyramidal cells in response to synaptic stimulation. To tune theTRPC model, I applied synaptic inputs in the dendritic tree as illustrated in �g-ure 3.2, in order to mimic these experimental recordings. Synaptic stimulationevokes glutamate transients, which couple to ionotropic (AMPA and NMDA) andmetabotropic (mGlu1/5) receptors in parallel, subsequently evoking transmembranecurrents. I then measured the resulting change in somatic membrane potential, nor-mally referred to as an excitatory postsynaptic potential (EPSP), in the soma. Thedetails of the stimulation protocols are explained in Paper I and Paper II. Essentially,I applied pulse trains of varying duration and frequencies in the interval 1-10Hz.

3.3.2 Calculating the decay time constant � τdecay

Since we were interested in studying the e�ects of the TRPC channel on temporalsummation, it was natural to study the decay time of the TRPC current. It istherefore worth mentioning a few words about how the EPSP decay time constantτdecay is calculated. In the strict sense, τdecay can only be calculated if the decaycan be approximated to follow an exponential function, see equation 3.10, whereEPSPmax is the maximal EPSP value.

EPSP (t) = EPSPmax · e(− t

τdecay)

(3.10)

Page 42: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

34 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

Figure 3.2. Model of pyramidal neuron with detailed morphology. In simulations for Paper Iand II synaptic stimulation was applied in a dendritic branch of the apical tree. The mem-brane potential, which is color-coded, was measured in the soma in order to analyze synapticintegration properties. Note that the membrane at the branch receiving synaptic input ismore depolarized than its neighboring branches. The inset �gure (upper right) is a 3D illus-tration of a CA1 pyramidal branch (not from the same study as the full neuron to the left),reconstructed from serial electron microscopy (http://synapses.clm.utexas.edu/index.asp).

Page 43: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

3.4. SOFTWARE USED IN THIS THESIS 35

The EPSP (or sADP) following activation of TRPC channels typically has arelatively slow rise, in the order of 100ms, and a wide peak (see �gure 2 in Paper I).It is therefore not obvious how to measure τdecay. In Paper I we chose to de�neit as the time from the peak to the time where e-1 = 37% of EPSPmax remains.However, this measure was not appropriate to use in the VDA model, as that EPSPconsists of two peaks (see �gure M4A in Paper I). I therefore instead chose to usethe pair-pulse protocol shown in �gure M5 of that paper, to get an indirect measureof τdecay. The rationale for using the pair-pulse protocol is that we can measurewhat actually remains of the signal after the stimulus is gone, which is the purposeof calculating the decay time constant in this study where temporal summation isin focus. Thus, the main advantage of PPP over τdecay is that it is not sensitive tothe shape of the peak.

3.4 Software used in this thesis

All simulations were run with the NEURON (version 7.0) software, developed byHines and Carnevale (2001) for the purpose of studying both neuronal networksand individual neurons with properties including complex branching morphology,multiple channel types, inhomogeneous channel distribution, ionic di�usion, and thee�ects of second messengers. It builds on the cable theory of Rall which I brie�youtlined in section 3.1.3. NEURON is one of the most popular softwares used inthe �eld of computational neuroscience, with more than 700 papers reporting workemploying it as of April 2008, according to Hines et al. (2009).

MATLAB (version R2009a) was used for simulation initiation and analysis. En-gauge Digitizer was used for extracting data from published experiment results. Forwriting I have used the LYX document editor, and for generating �gures I have usedInkscape and GIMP.

Page 44: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

36 CHAPTER 3. MODELS AND METHODS

Page 45: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Chapter 4

Results and discussion

In this chapter, I will �rst discuss the results of Paper I and Paper II. Then, I givea more general discussion about the common theme � the synaptically (mGlu1/5)evoked ITRPC current � and problems encountered when studying it in vitro. Iwill also discuss why it is important to study and include ITRPC in computationalmodeling studies.

4.1 Paper I

In this paper we investigated the hypothesis that TRPC channels are important forintegrating synaptic input arriving at low frequency. I will here discuss the majorconclusions and present additional data relating to decay time constants. I will alsopresent �gures supporting the sensitivity analysis of Paper I.

4.1.1 Main conclusions

4.1.1.1 ITRPC is important for integrating low-frequency synaptic input

As described in Paper I and in section 2.4.1.2, spike frequencies during behavior arelow, around 8Hz, implying that typical synaptic inputs arrive with frequencies in thesame range. With current experimental techniques it is not possible to directly recordthe electrical activity of a dendritic branch from a behaving animal. It is thereforeimportant to use computer simulations for studying synaptic integration. Ionotropicreceptors, including AMPA and NMDA, are well-characterized and incorporated inmost compartmental models. However, as we discuss in Paper I, their kinetics aretypically fast and corresponding currents decay rapidly after each synaptic inputand will hence not allow for e�ective temporal summation of frequencies at or below10Hz, at least not under circumstances where spatial summation is limited (seesection 2.4.1.2). We therefore turned our attention to mGlu mediated currents,and the slow ITRPC in particular. In contrast to the AMPA and NMDA currents,mGlu mediated currents are rarely included in compartmental models and, to myknowledge, a synaptically evoked ITRPC current has never been included.

37

Page 46: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

38 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

With the models I implemented it was possible to study the relative importanceof the two major classes of synaptic currents in supporting integration of synapticinputs. I show in �gures 3 and 8 of Paper I that, in contrast to ionotropic recep-tor currents, the metabotropic receptor current ITRPC is well-suited for allowingsummation of low frequency synaptic inputs.

Even if brain slice experiments show that mechanisms of mGlu1/5 and TRPCmay be important for synaptic function, that is no guarantee that they are importanton a behavioral level. It is therefore important to note that in vivo studies haveindeed shown that mGlu1/5 receptors are important on the behavioral level. Thesein vivo studies have utilized both pharmacological block of mGlu1/5 (Naie andManahan-Vaughan, 2004; Balschun et al., 2006; Hayashi et al., 2007; Mikami et al.,2007; Gregory and Szumlinski, 2008) and mGlu1/5 knock-out (Niswender and Conn,2010). These results are interesting, but note that mGlu1/5 receptors also couple toother channels in addition to TRPC, as mentioned in section 2.2.4. TRPC channelshave been less studied, but as discussed in section 2.3.5.2, a recent TRPC knock-outstudy suggests that ITRPC has strong e�ects on the behavioral level (Riccio et al.,2009). Furthermore, the hypothesis that ITRPC is involved in the integration of lowfrequencies is supported by results from Purkinje cells (Batchelor and Garthwaite,1997). Taken together, these observations suggest that it is important to develop andinclude ITRPC in computational models, in particular when studying low frequencydynamics. Moreover, our �nding that mGlu1/5 activated TRPC channels may beimportant for summation of physiologically relevant frequencies provides a plausibleexplanation to why behavior is a�ected by mGlu1/5 block/knock-out and TRPCknock-out.

4.1.1.2 τdecay depends on frequency and duration of synaptic input

As explained above, we were interested in studying synaptic integration of low fre-quencies, and in particular the contribution of TRPC. Naturally, in order for synap-tic input to be e�ectively summed, some portion of every input-driven signal mustremain until the arrival of the next. For example, if excitatory synaptic input isdelivered at 5Hz, corresponding to an inter-stimulus interval of 200ms, then thedecay time of the post-synaptic depolarization need to be longer than 200ms to al-low for e�ective summation. For the purpose of our project it was therefore naturalto study the decay time constant of the depolarization resulting from activation ofTRPC channels, denoted τdecay (see section 3.3.2 for details).

For both the VIA and VDA model, we measured τdecay of the EPSP following asynaptic stimulus train of varying frequency and number of pulses (see e. g. �gures 4and 9 of Paper I). We found that τdecay increases with increasing stimulation durationand frequency. This result may seem trivial at a �rst glance, since a long pulse trainof high frequency naturally will produce a depolarization with a large amplitude, notonly immediately at the end of the stimulus pulse train, but also after some delay.It should therefore be stressed that a change in τdecay actually re�ects a change inthe decay characteristics � the curve form � of the depolarization. Note that, if thedecay would follow a simple exponential, τdecay would not be a�ected by the increasein depolarization amplitude (resulting from long pulse trains of high frequency).

Page 47: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4.1. PAPER I 39

4.1.1.3 τdecay depends on glutamate concentration

The �nding that τdecay of the TRPC mediated depolarization changes with stimulusparameters was intriguing and led us to further explore the phenomena. As shownin �gure 4C of Paper I, we found a linear relation between τdecay and the maximalglutamate concentration: τdecay = 91.4 + 103 ·max([Glu]). In addition to this, wefound a linear relation between τdecay and the maximal TRPC current max(ITRPC):τdecay = 144 + 4.07 ·103 ·max(ITRPC), ITRPC with unit mA/cm2 (data not shown).It naturally follows that max(ITRPC) is linearly correlated with max([Glu]). Themaximum glutamate concentration appears to be an important determinant of theTRPC current. The fact that glutamate can summate has some importance for ourresults, and will be discussed in the sensitivity analysis below.

4.1.1.4 Pair-pulse protocol suitable for studying τdecay

It is easy to realize the problem of obtaining a direct measurement of τdecay ifthe pulse shape is more complicated than a simple exponential. The shape of anEPSP peak is typically not sharp and may have an unwanted in�uence of the τdecaymeasure, as discussed in section 3.3.2. What we actually want to measure is theability of the pulse train to provide a depolarization that a�ect subsequent pulses.It was therefore natural to attempt to utilize a pair-pulse protocol (PPP, describedin section 3.3.2) in order to get a more indirect measurement of τdecay. PPP is oftenused in studies of short-term potentiation, a phenomenon that has been intensivelystudied for many years. The details of the PPP used in our study was describedin �gure M5 of Paper I. We also introduced ISDR10, de�ned as the largest inter-stimulus delay possible for obtaining more than 10% ampli�cation of a test pulseamplitude when compared to control.

To validate that ISDR10 is a suitable measure of τdecay, we showed in �gure 7Bof Paper I that a linear relationship between ISDR10 and τdecay exists in the VIAmodel. Note that it was even more complicated to directly measure or even de�neτdecay in the VDA model since the EPSP here consists of two peaks, the fast beingunique for the VDA model since it originates from activation of ionotropic receptors.We concluded that the pair-pulse protocol could be successfully applied in our studyof decay time constants.

4.1.1.5 Similar results obtained with VIA and VDA

As described in Paper I there is a discrepancy among experimental studies aiming atunderstanding the activation of TRPC channels. While some studies show that acti-vation of voltage-gated calcium channels (VGCC) is necessary for TRPC activation,others show the opposite. We developed two models, VIA and VDA as described insection 3.2.2.5, representing the two possible cases. Certain di�erences were foundin the results, such as a saturation in both average membrane potential (�gure 8B,Paper I) and relative pulse train ampli�cation RPT (�gure 9B and 9D, Paper I) forfrequencies above 3Hz present only in the VDA model. However, the two modelswere in agreement on the major �ndings, namely those described in sections 4.1.1.1and 4.1.1.2. We thus concluded that the two models essentially give similar results.

Page 48: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

40 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

4.1.1.6 Model veri�cation

As discussed in the paper and in other sections of this thesis, our modeling resultswere in good agreement with experimental data. Speci�cally, we have found that:

• mGlu1/5 activation of TRPC channels allows for summation of low-frequencysynaptic input down to 1 - 2Hz. In entorhinal cortex our experimental collab-orator has observed summation for 2Hz stimulus trains (Yoshida, unpublishedobservations), and Sidiropoulou et al. (2009) showed that 2Hz is su�cient foractivating TRPC in prefrontal cortex. Furthermore, in our model a singlesynaptic stimulus is su�cient for TRPC activation in line with our experi-mental data as described in Paper I, and other studies such as the one byFaber et al. (2006), who studied amygdala pyramidal neurons. Also in thehippocampus it has been shown that one pulse is enough for mGlu1/5 acti-vation (Charpak and Gähwiler, 1991). It should be noted, however, that inother systems it seems like more than one synaptic stimulus pulse is requiredfor ITRPC activation (e. g. Kim et al., 2003).

• ITRPC is modulated by the glutamate concentration. This has also been shownin experiments, e. g. by Reichelt and Knöpfel (2002) who applied a glutamateuptake blocker and observed facilitated activation of the mGlu1/5 mediatedcurrent.

• The amplitude of ITRPC increases with increasing number of inputs, in linewith experimental observations, e. g. Congar et al. (1997); Faber et al. (2006).

• ITRPC kinetics can be modi�ed by stimulus parameters, as discussed above(section 4.1.1.2). This �nding is to some extent supported by experimentaldata, which I will discuss in depth in section 4.3.

• With a modi�ed version of our TRPCmodel it was possible to induce persistentspiking following synaptic input, which has also been shown in experiments,e.g. Egorov et al. (2002). Details about the persistent spiking model can befound in Paper I.

4.1.2 τdecay � a closer look

Much focus in Paper I was devoted to studying τdecay. In this section we will take acloser look at the data and discuss the results.

4.1.2.1 ITRPC decay time constant

As already discussed, we showed that the decay time constant of the ITRPC evokedEPSP, τdecay, depends on the stimulus parameters. However, also the decay timeconstant of ITRPC itself, τdecay, iTRPC , measured at the site of synaptic input, showsthis dependency. In �gure 4.1 (left) I show that τdecay, iTRPC has a similar depen-dence on stimulus parameters as that of the EPSP measured in the soma (see �gure4 in Paper I), but lies in the range 120 - 160ms whereas τdecay lies in the range 195 -250ms. In line with the results discussed in section 4.1.1.3 above, a linear relation

Page 49: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4.1. PAPER I 41

1 1.2 1.4 1.6

120

130

140

150

160

i TR

P τ

−de

cay

(ms)

max([Glu])

0 2 4 6 8 100

50

100

150

i TR

P τ

−de

cay

(ms)

Frequency (Hz)

0 100 200 300 400 5000

50

100

150

i TR

P τ

−de

cay

(ms)

Duration (ms)

23

45

67

8

12

34

56

78

910

0

200

400

600

800

1000

1200

1400

NFrequency (Hz)

ISD

R10

23

45

67

8

12

34

56

78

9100

100

200

300

400

500

600

700

800

NFrequency (Hz)

ISD

R10

Figure 4.1. (Left) ITRPC decay time constant (τdecay,iTRP ) increases with increasingfrequency (top) and stimulus duration (middle). It has a linear dependence on maximumglutamate (bottom). This can be compared to �gure 4 of Paper I, which is similar but withτdecay instead of τdecay,iTRP .(Right) ISDR10 increases with increasing frequency and stimulus duration in both the VIAmodel (top) and the VDA model (bottom), as shown in �gures 7A and 9D of Paper I. In theVDA model ISDR10 does not reach as high values as in the VIA model, but saturates at lessthan 800ms.

Page 50: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

42 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

0 2 4 6 8 100

50

100

150

200

250τ−

deca

y (m

s)

Frequency (Hz)0 2 4 6 8 10

0

0.1

0.2

0.3

0.4

0.5

<V

m>

− V

rest

(m

V)

Frequency (Hz)

Figure 4.2. Sensitivity analysis for the VIA model, varying the maximum TRPC conduc-tance (g). (Left) τdecay increases with increasing frequency. g is set to 50 (blue), 100 (green,identical to �gure 3A of Paper I) and 200% (red) of its original value.(Right) Steady-state summation increases with frequency. g is set to 50 (cross), 100 (circle,identical to �gure 4B of Paper I) and 200% (red) of its original value.

was found also between τdecay, iTRPC and the maximal glutamate concentration:τdecay, iTRPC = 38 + 79 ·max([Glu]), data not shown.

4.1.2.2 ISDR10 versus frequency and duration � 3D-plots

In �gures 7A and 9D of Paper I we showed how the value of ISDR10 changes withnumber of stimulus (N) and frequency (f). ISDR10was plotted versus N , and f wascolor-coded. As an alternative one may choose to plot the same data in 3D, whichI have done in �gure 4.1 (right). The top �gure contains data from the VIA model.One observation is that pulse trains of 1 or 2Hz do not provide enough depolarizationto allow for 10% ampli�cation of the subsequent test pulse. However, it should benoted that the ISDR10 measure is in one sense thresholded at 100ms, since I didnot use inter-stimulus delays ISD < 100ms. A shorter ISD would correspond toa stimulus frequency above 10Hz, which is outside the interval we choose to study(1 - 10Hz) but would probably allow for a 10% ampli�cation. In the VDA model(bottom) ISDR10 does not reach as high values as in the VIA model, but saturatesat less than 800ms.

4.1.3 Sensitivity analysis

In Paper I we presented a sensitivity analysis but did not include a graphical pre-sentation of the data. I will here show examples of data supporting the conclusionsmade there.

4.1.3.1 Varying g in the VIA model

As described in Paper I the steady state summation of Vm, and to a lesser extentthe decay time constant, is a�ected when the maximum TRPC conductance (g) isvaried from 50 to 200% of its original value. The data is presented in �gure 4.2. In

Page 51: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4.1. PAPER I 43

0 2 4 6 8 100

50

100

150

200

250

τ−de

cay

(ms)

Frequency (Hz)0 100 200 300 400 500

0

50

100

150

200

250

τ−de

cay

(ms)

Duration (ms)

0 2 4 6 8 100

0.005

0.01

0.015

0.02

0.025

Max

imal

TR

P c

urre

nt (

mA

/cm

²)

Frequency (Hz)0 2 4 6 8 10

−60.25

−60.2

−60.15

−60.1

−60.05

−60

<V

m>

in s

oma

(mV

)

Frequency (Hz)

Figure 4.3. Sensitivity analysis for the VIA model, using Kglu = 0. (Top) τdecay onlyslightly increases with frequency (left) and duration (right), compared to �gures 4A and 4Bof Paper I, where Kglu = 0.8 (default).(Bottom, left) max(ITRPC) increases less with frequency when Kglu = 0 (cross) compared toKglu = 0.8 (circle, identical to �gure 3A of Paper I). (Bottom, right) Steady-state summationincreases less with frequency when Kglu = 0 (cross) compared to Kglu = 0.8 (circle, solid,identical to �gure 3B of Paper I). Dashed line shows, for comparison, the case of summationwhen only ionotropic receptors are included.

Page 52: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

44 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

the left �gure, which is based on �gure 4A in Paper I, τdecay is plotted versus f forvarying N . There is only a minor variation in τdecay for the three values of g used.In the right �gure, based on �gure 3B in Paper I, it is shown that the steady stateaverage Vm is strongly dependent on the value of g.

4.1.3.2 Disabling glutamate summation

As shown in �gure 4C of Paper I, glutamate summation is essential for the abilityof τdecay to depend on stimulation parameters. In �gure 4.3 I present data fromsimulations where the glutamate summation parameter Kglu is set to zero, resultingin a model of glutamate concentration that reaches the same amplitude for everynew synaptic input, regardless of input history. In the top �gures it can be seenthat τdecay does not vary much with varying f or N . In the bottom �gures it canbe seen that the steady state TRPC current still increases with increasing f, as doesthe resulting average Vm. It should be noted that setting Kglu to zero may not beconsistent with the physiological observation, since it has been shown in at least onestudy that glutamate does undergo summation of frequencies in the range studiedhere (Hires et al., 2008).

4.2 Paper II

The model of Paper II has many features in common with Paper I. Paper I could infact be seen as an extension of Paper II. Di�erences in how the models were tunedexist, however, and the consequences of these will be discussed here. One insightfrom this study was that, as already mentioned, a square glutamate pulse may notbe appropriate to use when simulating ITRPC . Another important insight was thatdecay time constants of ITRPC are longer from experiments using somatic currentinjection and subsequent spike production, than those using synaptic stimulation.

4.2.1 Main conclusions

4.2.1.1 Results consistent with Paper I

The model in Paper II is similar to the VIA model of Paper I but has slower kineticsand a di�erent glutamate model, as was described in Chapter 3. The slower kineticsoriginates from the fact that the model was designed to study the channel dynamicsduring current injection rather than synaptic stimulation, which I discuss in the nextsection. Development of the glutamate model in Paper II preceded that of Paper Iand should be considered less accurate. As was discussed above, an importantconclusion in Paper I is that ITRPC may be able to support summation of low-frequency synaptic input. It is therefore important to note that the same conclusioncan be made from simulations in Paper II, as shown in �gure 1 of that paper. Anotherimportant conclusion from Paper I is that ITRPC decay time constants are a�ectedby stimulus parameters. This phenomenon was also present in the model used forPaper II, though the e�ect is much smaller. A plausible explanation for this di�erenceis that the glutamate did not undergo summation in Paper II. Taken together, weconclude that the results of Paper I are robust.

Page 53: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4.2. PAPER II 45

4.2.1.2 Current injection versus synaptic stimulation

The model in Paper II was tuned from experiments using current injection, whereasthe models in Paper I were tuned from experiments using synaptic stimulation, aswas discussed the previous paragraph and in section 3.2.2.5. Here, I will discussthe consequences. First it should be said that synaptic stimulation represents thephysiological reality of mGlu activated TRPC currents more accurately than currentinjection. Neurons communicate mainly via presynaptic transmitter release (in thiscase glutamate), which activates mGlu1/5 and subsequently TRPC. On the contrary,square pulse current injection in the soma combined with pharmacological (agonist)activation of mGlu1/5 is relatively far from the physiological reality of synapticallyevoked TRPC currents. Nevertheless, current injection provides one way of study-ing transient TRPC currents, and an advantage is that it is experimentally betterconstrained and more frequently used. It may however represent back-propagatingaction potentials. The di�erence in which the two stimulation protocols a�ect ITRPChas previously been studied experimentally in our laboratory with a focus on persis-tent spiking (Yoshida et al., 2008), but when it comes to the subthreshold domain Ihave not encountered any such systematic study. It was therefore highly interestingto �nd, by doing the meta-analysis described in section 4.3 below, that the valuesof τdecay resulting from current injection experiments are clearly longer than thosefrom synaptic stimulation. However, since there were no data points from currentinjection with durations of less than 250ms this should only be taken as an indicativeobservation from available data. In Paper I we focused on relatively short stimuli,typically up to 500 or 1 000ms. It was therefore natural to tune the model fromsynaptic stimulation experiments since that is where most data on this transienttime scale exist. In Paper II on the other hand, we included stimulus durations upto values of 10 000ms.

There may be many explanations as to why current injection yields larger valuesof τdecay than synaptic stimulation. Since ITRPC is strongly dependent on calcium,one explanation could be that the source of calcium from current injection mayhave a slower decay. Also note that a prediction from Paper I is that τdecay isstrongly dependent on the agonist (glutamate, in the case of synaptic stimulation)concentration. In the current injection case, the mGlu1/5 agonist used (e. g. DHPG)may be applied at relatively high concentrations, possibly contributing to long decaytime constants. One can also compare data from synaptic stimulation and currentinjection with data from mGlu1/5 agonist pressure ejection experiments. One suchexperiment was performed by Bengtson et al. (2004), who found that currents frompressure injection had similar amplitudes, but much slower decay kinetics (mean382%) when compared to currents from synaptic stimulation. The authors used100µM DHPG, but it would also be interesting to investigate if a dose-responsee�ect on τdecay appears if the DHPG concentration is varied.

4.2.2 EPSP time course

As described in section 3.2, the components in the model of Paper II are similar tothe VIA model of Paper I, though their kinetics are slower. To illustrate the slowkinetics of this model I present in �gure 4.4 two examples of membrane voltagetraces following synaptic input applied for 2 000ms at a frequency of 2 and 3Hz

Page 54: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

46 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

0 2000 4000 6000 8000−63

−62.8

−62.6

−62.4

−62.2

−62

Vm

(m

V)

time (ms)

2 Hz3 Hz

Figure 4.4. τdecay is longer in model of Paper II, which is tuned from current injectionexperiments. Figure shows Vm trace for synaptic stimulation, frequency of 2 and 3Hz re-spectively, applied for 2 000ms.

respectively. The decay time constants, approximately estimated to 3 000ms, arebarely distinguishable in this case.

4.3 Meta-analysis of in vitro τdecay measurements

Few experimental studies have systematically varied synaptic stimulus parameters(e.g. frequency and duration) in order to study the TRPC current that result frommGlu1/5 activation. It was therefore useful to perform a meta-analysis where datawas taken from several di�erent studies. In �gure 4.5 τdecay is plotted versus stim-ulus duration. Data points are taken both from experiments using current injection(Egorov et al., 2002; Fowler et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2010) and from experimentsusing synaptic stimulation (Congar et al., 1997; Partridge and Valenzuela, 1999;Kim et al., 2003; Bengtson et al., 2004; Faber et al., 2006; Hartmann et al., 2008;Sidiropoulou et al., 2009). These studies were performed on neurons from variousregions of the brain: entorhinal cortex (pyramidal cells), hippocampus (CA1 pyra-midal cells), prefrontal cortex (pyramidal cells), lateral amygdala (pyramidal cells)and cerebellum (Purkinje cells). Di�erent frequencies were used in the synapticstimulation experiments, ranging from 50 to 300Hz, and stimulus durations variedfrom 20 to 6 000ms. On the one hand, one can note that τdecay appears to increasewith increasing stimulus durations, which is in line with the prediction made in bothPaper I and Paper II, thus suggesting that our models predictions are consistent withdata. On the other hand, one may note a large variation in the data set, indicatingthat ITRPC behaves di�erently in di�erent experimental settings, which was alsodiscussed in section 4.1.1 above. In the following two sections I o�er a few possiblesources of this variation. Note, however, that the di�erence in τdecay from currentinjection and synaptic stimulation data was discussed in depth in section 4.2.1.2 andwill hence not be speci�cally discussed here.

Page 55: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4.3. META-ANALYSIS OF IN VITRO τDECAY MEASUREMENTS 47

0 500 1000 1500 20000

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

6000

τ deca

y (m

s)

Stimulus duration (ms)10

210

3

102

103

τ deca

y (m

s)

Stimulus duration (ms)

Figure 4.5. Result of meta-analysis. Values of τdecay from experiments using synapticstimulation (crosses) and current injection (circles) is plotted versus stimulus duration, onnormal (left) and logarithmic (right) scales.

4.3.1 Variation of mechanisms among systems studied

First, some variation can be explained by the simple fact that di�erent studies studydi�erent things. Neurons are speci�cally designed for the task they are involvedin. As a consequence, entorhinal layer III pyramidal neurons are di�erent fromentorhinal layer V neurons and from hippocampal CA1 neurons, and probably evenless related to the neocortical prefrontal neurons, not to mention how di�erent theyare from the neurons of the cerebellum. Also the experimental protocols typicallyvary among studies, with respect to e. g. the species being used (it is often a rat,as in the studies included in 4.5, but sometimes a mouse), synaptic stimulationintensity, type of blocker or the temperature of the brain slice. The temperature,which a�ects the synaptic release probability (Kovalchuk et al., 2000), is typicallyoutside the physiological range. In addition to this, the researchers behind each studyhave di�erent backgrounds and motives, and may therefore use di�erent conditionsor techniques, sometimes without explicitly stating them when describing the study.

The TRPC channel itself comes in di�erent �avors, as the composition of itssubunits is not �x (see section 2.3.4). While TRPC1/4/5 subtypes appears to con-stitute the most important heteromers in the entorhinal cortex, TRPC3/6 appearsto be dominant in the cerebellum. The exact subunit composition is rarely statedin experiments. In line with this, what is often referred to as mGlu1/5 receptoractivation includes activation of either mGlu1 or mGlu5 or both together, and theirindividual contribution and e�ect are not fully explored. In addition, both mGlu1and mGlu5, in turn, undergo alternative splicing. Currently, six splice variants formGlu1 and two for mGlu5 have been identi�ed (Niswender and Conn, 2010). Othercomplicating factors relate to the intermediate mechanisms coupling mGlu1/5 ac-tivation to TRPC activation, such as the PLC pathway. There are several PLCisozymes and although PLCβ is believed to be dominant, other isozymes may playa role (P Séguéla, personal communication). Also the e�ects of PLD (phospholipaseD) remain to be explored.

Furthermore, it is believed that Ca2+ from more than one source can be involvedin TRPC activation, which of course substantially adds to the complexity considering

Page 56: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

48 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

the great complexity in Ca2+ dynamics, with its numerous sources and downstreame�ects. Ca2+ is not in focus as a TRPC activator in this thesis, but it shouldbe noted that ITRPC was classically described as a calcium-activated non-speci�ccurrent (ICAN ) before its molecular identity was discovered. The TRPC channelitself may also conduct Ca2+ ions when open, as shown in �gure 2.2.

4.3.2 Feature or bug?

ITRPC is a�ected by many modulatory substances. TRPC can be activated follow-ing activation of muscarinic acetylcholine (ACh) receptors (see �gure 2.2), whichmediate most of the e�ects of ACh in the brain (Purves et al., 2008). Hasselmoand Stern (2006) proposed the ACh mediated �Alonso current�, later attributed toTRPC channels (Zhang et al., 2010), as the mechanism underlying working memoryfor novel information, but also stressed its role in encoding of long-term memory.While the synaptically activated mGlu1/5 receptors have a transient e�ect on TRPCchannels, ACh could be seen as having e�ect on slower time scales as it appears tobe important for processes of learning and memory (Hasselmo and Bower, 1993;Hasselmo and Giocomo, 2006; Hasselmo, 2006). Furthermore, ACh may be sponta-neously released in slices, which is often not controlled for.

Another mechanism that can a�ect ITRPC on a slow time scale is glutamateuptake. We showed in Paper I that the TRPC dynamics is strongly dependent onmaximal glutamate concentration. Experiments have also shown the in�uence ofglutamate uptake on TRPC (Congar et al., 1997; Reichelt and Knöpfel, 2002). Yetanother example of a modulatory mechanism is receptor tra�cking. It was recentlyshown that the amount of mGlu5 at the cell surface is likely to be regulated in vivo(Wang et al., 2009), indicating subsequent regulation of ITRPC .

Finally, the complex activation of TRPC channels should be described in lightof the family they belong to (TRP). When the TRP research was still in its infancy,Montell et al. (2002) wrote that:

�TRP cation channels display an extraordinary assortment of selectivitiesand activation mechanisms, some of which represent previously unrecog-nized modes for regulating ion channels. Moreover, the biological rolesof TRP channels appear to be equally diverse and range from roles inpain perception to male aggression.�

The observations above, together with the strong links between TRPC and Ca2+

suggest that TRPC channels are important regulators of neuronal function.

4.3.3 Conclusion

To conclude, the observations described here suggest that mechanisms of mGlu1/5mediated TRPC currents are complex. They therefore need to be studied more sys-tematically to be fully understood and appreciated. Considering the complexity inthe activation mechanisms of TRPC channels, I predict that computational model-ing will be an important tool when performing these systematic studies, since it willbe necessary to integrate several functional levels.

Page 57: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

4.4. IMPORTANCE OF THE IMPLEMENTED ITRPC MODEL 49

4.4 Importance of the implemented ITRPC model

I suggested above that computational modeling will be a useful tool for learningmore about the mGlu1/5 mediated ITRPC . In this thesis I have also argued thatthe opposite is true, i. e. that the current may be important to include when build-ing models on varying levels, a discussion I will continue here. Multicompartmentmodels typically do not include e�ects of mGlu activation. The only two exceptionsI have encountered are, �rst, the Purkinje cell work by Steuber and colleagues whostudied the mGlu e�ects on the KCa channel (Steuber and Willshaw, 2004; Steuberet al., 2006), and second, a model of slow calcium oscillations in lamprey spinal cordneurons by Hallen et al. (2004). Neither of these two studies included ITRPC . Froma broader perspective we should also acknowledge the fact that active dendriteshave only been appreciated in the recent 15 years or so (Johnston and Narayanan,2008). Consequently, dendritic models have classically only included passive cableproperties.

Our ITRPC model is probably the �rst of its kind. The idea of implementing itcame to us after realizing from experimental work using mGlu agonist applicationthat the current, which indeed has an important role in persistent spiking, mayalso be observed in its subthreshold form as a depolarizing sADP (Al-Yahya et al.,2003). Since the current is not well-characterized, the process of implementing itrequired a time consuming literature search, including studies of the spatiotemporalglutamate pro�le and the dynamics of the mGlu1/5 receptor. We have so far focusedon using the model for studying the dynamics of ITRPC and for testing the role insynaptic integration in a single cell. We found that it plays an important role in thesummation of inputs arriving at frequencies in a range that is behaviorally relevant.We have however not tested the potential role of ITRPC in a network of neurons,which would be highly interesting to learn about. In the next chapter I will furtherdiscuss what the ITRPC model could be used for in future studies.

Computational modeling studies are good complements to in vitro experiments.Modeling work is relatively cheap, and animals do not have to sacri�ced. In addi-tion, insights can be gained that would be hard to gain from experiments. In thisthesis I have presented results from a large number of simulations, which systemati-cally tested the in�uence of stimulation parameters. One insight was that the decaytime constant τdecay of the ITRPC evoked depolarization depends on the maximalglutamate concentration, and that it increases with increasing stimulus durationand frequency. I demonstrated in �gure 4.3 that the latter phenomena is stronglyreduced if glutamate summation is disabled. A similar test would not be feasiblein an experiment. Furthermore, I evaluated two di�erent ITRPC activation models,based on two di�erent hypotheses. As described in section 4.1.1.5, similar resultswere obtained, except that the VDA model (but not the VIA model) showed sat-uration (of both average steady-state membrane potential and relative pulse trainampli�cation). It would therefore be interesting to conduct an experiment whichmimics the model simulations, and see if the result looks more like the VIA or theVDA model, thereby supporting either model. The work in this thesis also suggeststhat it would be interesting to use a paired-pulse protocol for testing the resultspredicted here in an experimental setting.

Page 58: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

50 CHAPTER 4. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

Page 59: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Chapter 5

Future work

There may be many ways to improve the model I have used for this thesis. Beforetesting and evaluating however, it is hard to know if adding a certain feature or somelevel of complexity actually leads to improvement in understanding or computationalquality. I will in this chapter �rst discuss ways of modifying the model, and thendiscuss additional tests and analysis that could be performed.

5.1 Model improvements

Due to lack of data, the calcium dependence in our ITRPC model is currently phe-nomenological rather than tuned from experimental results. Gross et al. (2009)recently reported that homomeric TRPC5 channels appear to be directly activatedby calcium in a dose-dependent matter. We could tune our model to replicate theirdata, but at least three things are then important to consider. First, the ITRPC westudy is likely to be mediated by heteromeric TRPC1/4/5 channels (Zhang et al.,2010), which have di�erent biophysical properties than TRPC5 homomers (Strübinget al., 2001). Second, they used genetically engineered HEK293 cells, which mayhave di�erent properties than the pyramidal cells that we study. Third, even if weknow the exact calcium dependence of ITRPC the problem remains that we do notknow what the absolute levels of calcium should be in our model cell. The readershould note that we had to modify the L-type VGCC in the Poirazi model to evenget a signi�cant increase in calcium, so this part of our model can be consideredphenomenological as well.

To better represent the dynamics of mGlu1/5 activation, we could in NEURONimplement the 9-state kinetic scheme suggested by Marcaggi et al. (2009), and com-pare it to the current model. It is however important to note that the computationswould then be signi�cantly slowed down, since such a detailed model is compu-tationally demanding. Relating to this, we could also attempt to evaluate e�ectsdownstream of mGlu1/5 activation other than ITRPC (see section 2.2.4.2), althoughthis is challenged by the lack of data.

It would be interesting to add presynaptic e�ects, such a model of presynapticcalcium levels or presynaptically located metabotropic glutamate receptors.

51

Page 60: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

52 CHAPTER 5. FUTURE WORK

As pointed out in this thesis, there is a lack of data regarding the exact mech-anisms of ITRPC activation. It is known that the PLC pathway is involved, butdetails remain elusive. However, experimental laboratories such as those of Séguéla,Clapham and Andrade are working on learning more about the mechanisms, andour model should of course be updated when data becomes available.

The current cell model is rather complex, with its detailed morphology and nu-merous ion channels. It would be interesting to evaluate a reduced model, by de-creasing the number of compartments and ion channels. An advantage would be thatmore sophisticated techniques for analyzing dynamical systems, such as phase planeanalysis, could be used to understand more about the phenomena underlying ourresults. By reducing the morphological complexity one could study the in�uence ofspatial e�ects, and by systematically removing certain ion channels one could studythe in�uence of these.

5.2 Additional tests and analysis

The sensitivity analysis of Paper I was done in order to investigate the in�uence ofparameters thought to be potentially important for the results presented there. Onecould also attempt to do a more rigorous sensitivity analysis by varying a largernumber of parameters, individually or possibly in combinations (Saltelli, 2004).

It would further be interesting to study the e�ect of ITRPC on spatial summa-tion of synaptic inputs. One reason for this is that this synaptically evoked cur-rent is mediated by channels located peri- and extrasynaptically, in contrast to theionotropic synaptic currents which are mediated by channels limited to the synapticcleft. Therefore ITRPC may contribute di�erently to spatial summation than AMPAand NMDA currents, though the e�ect is hard to predict before it is tested. It is alsointeresting that mGlu1/5 receptors are activated by extrasynaptic (or spillover) glu-tamate, and Marcaggi and Attwell (2005) showed that mGlu1-mediated EPSCs inPurkinje cells (probably mediated by TRPC3, see Kim et al., 2003) may be stronglyactivated when synapses located close to each other are activated. They found asynergistic e�ect on this EPSC when activating adjacent synapses. In line with theevidence for compartmentalization of the dendritic tree (see section 2.4.1), this sug-gests that the spatial patterns of synaptic input plays a role in neuronal processing.One may speculate that ITRPC has an interesting contribution to these phenomena,and computational modeling seems like a good tool for learning more about it.

I mentioned in section 4.2.1.2 that synaptic stimulation and current injectionexperiments yield di�erent results regarding the decay time of ITRPC , and suggestedpossible explanations to this. One could take this further and use the model to testideas about why the results of the two di�erent experimental techniques seem todi�er. So far I have only tested the synaptic stimulation protocol, but a currentinjection protocol would be straight-forward to implement by applying a current-clamp at the soma. It would be interesting to compare the two protocols, particularlyto see if the e�ects on dendritic calcium concentration dynamics di�er.

Currently we are investigating an interesting ITRPC - dependent phenomena inour model, which allows the neuron to switch between two di�erent stable sub-threshold Vm levels upon the arrival of synaptic inputs. If a su�ciently strong ex-

Page 61: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

5.2. ADDITIONAL TESTS AND ANALYSIS 53

citatory/inhibitory input is given, the membrane potential switches from its hyper-polarized/depolarized level to its depolarized/hyperpolarized level, and stays thereuntil further inputs arrive. This bistability is an intrinsic mechanism of the neuronsince it does not depend on network e�ects. Preliminary results have been acceptedin abstract form (Petersson and Fransén, 2010).

Page 62: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

54 CHAPTER 5. FUTURE WORK

Page 63: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

Bibliography

Abramowitz, J. and Birnbaumer, L., Physiology and pathophysiology of canon-ical transient receptor potential channels., The FASEB journal, 23(2):297�328,2009.

Agmon-Snir, H., Carr, C. E. and Rinzel, J., The role of dendrites in auditorycoincidence detection., Nature, 393(6682):268�272, 1998.

Al-Yahya, E., Hamel, E., Kennedy, T., Alonso, A. A. and Egorov,

A., Persitent activity in entorhinal cortex neurons induced by muscarinic andmetabotropic glutamate receptor activation and its dependence on trp channels.,in Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 377.5., 2003.

Ambudkar, I. S., Ca2+ signaling microdomains:platforms for the assembly andregulation of TRPC channels., Trends in pharmacological sciences, 27(1):25�32,2006.

Anwyl, R., Metabotropic glutamate receptors: electrophysiological properties androle in plasticity., Brain research. Brain research reviews, 29(1):83�120, 1999.

Balschun, D., Zuschratter, W. and Wetzel, W., Allosteric enhancement ofmetabotropic glutamate receptor 5 function promotes spatial memory., Neuro-science, 142(3):691�702, 2006.

Batchelor, A. M. and Garthwaite, J., Frequency detection and temporallydispersed synaptic signal association through a metabotropic glutamate receptorpathway., Nature, 385(6611):74�77, 1997.

Benarroch, E. E., Metabotropic glutamate receptors: synaptic modulators andtherapeutic targets for neurologic disease., Neurology, 70(12):964�968, 2008.

Bengtson, C. P., Tozzi, A., Bernardi, G. and Mercuri, N. B., Transientreceptor potential-like channels mediate metabotropic glutamate receptor EPSCsin rat dopamine neurones., The Journal of physiology, 555(Pt 2):323�330, 2004.

Berridge, M. J., Bootman, M. D. and Roderick, H. L., Calcium signalling:dynamics, homeostasis and remodelling., Nature reviews. Molecular cell biology,4(7):517�529, 2003.

55

Page 64: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

56 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Birnbaumer, L., The TRPC class of ion channels: a critical review of their roles inslow, sustained increases in intracellular Ca(2+) concentrations., Annual reviewof pharmacology and toxicology, 49:395�426, 2009.

Blair, N. T., Kaczmarek, J. S. and Clapham, D. E., Intracellular calciumstrongly potentiates agonist-activated TRPC5 channels., The Journal of generalphysiology, 133(5):525�546, 2009.

Borst, J. G., The low synaptic release probability in vivo., Trends in neurosciences,33(6):259�266, 2010.

Buzsáki, G., Large-scale recording of neuronal ensembles., Nature neuroscience,7(5):446�451, 2004.

Caeser, M., Brown, D. A., Gähwiler, B. H. and Knöpfel, T., Character-ization of a calcium-dependent current generating a slow afterdepolarization ofCA3 pyramidal cells in rat hippocampal slice cultures., The European journal ofneuroscience, 5(6):560�569, 1993.

Cash, S. and Yuste, R., Linear summation of excitatory inputs by CA1 pyramidalneurons., Neuron, 22(2):383�394, 1999.

Charpak, S. and Gähwiler, B. H., Glutamate mediates a slow synaptic responsein hippocampal slice cultures., Proceedings of the Royal Society, 243(1308):221�226, 1991.

Clapham, D. E., TRP channels as cellular sensors, Nature, 426(6966):517�524,2003.

Clapham, D. E., Calcium signaling., Cell, 131(6):1047�1058, 2007.

Clapham, D. E., Runnels, L. W. and Strübing, C., The TRP ion channelfamily., Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 2(6):387�396, 2001.

Clem, R. L., Celikel, T. and Barth, A. L., Ongoing in vivo experience triggerssynaptic metaplasticity in the neocortex., Science, 319(5859):101�104, 2008.

Congar, P., Leinekugel, X., Ben-Ari, Y. and Crépel, V., A long-lastingcalcium-activated nonselective cationic current is generated by synaptic stimula-tion or exogenous activation of group I metabotropic glutamate receptors in CA1pyramidal neurons., The Journal of neuroscience, 17(14):5366�5379, 1997.

Cosens, D. J. and Manning, A., Abnormal electroretinogram from a Drosophilamutant., Nature, 224(5216):285�287, 1969.

Crowder, E. A., Saha, M. S., Pace, R. W., Zhang, H., Prestwich, G. D.

and Del Negro, C. A., Phosphatidylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate regulates inspi-ratory burst activity in the neonatal mouse preBötzinger complex., The Journalof physiology, 582(Pt 3):1047�1058, 2007.

Damann, N., Voets, T. and Nilius, B., TRPs in our senses., Current biology :CB, 18(18), 2008.

Page 65: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

BIBLIOGRAPHY 57

Desai, B. N. and Clapham, D. E., TRP channels and mice de�cient in TRPchannels., P�ügers Archiv : European journal of physiology, 451(1):11�18, 2005.

Destexhe, A., Contreras, D., Sejnowski, T. J. and Steriade, M., A modelof spindle rhythmicity in the isolated thalamic reticular nucleus., Journal of neu-rophysiology, 72(2):803�818, 1994a.

Destexhe, A., Mainen, Z. F. and Sejnowski, T. J., Synthesis of models for ex-citable membranes, synaptic transmission and neuromodulation using a commonkinetic formalism., Journal of computational neuroscience, 1(3):195�230, 1994b.

Destexhe, A. and Sejnowski, T. J., G protein activation kinetics and spilloverof gamma-aminobutyric acid may account for di�erences between inhibitory re-sponses in the hippocampus and thalamus., Proceedings of the National Academyof Sciences of the United States of America, 92(21):9515�9519, 1995.

Dzubay, J. A. and Jahr, C. E., The concentration of synaptically released glu-tamate outside of the climbing �ber-Purkinje cell synaptic cleft., The Journal ofNeuroscience, 19(13):5265�5274, 1999.

Eder, P., Poteser, M., Romanin, C. and Groschner, K., Na(+) entry andmodulation of Na(+)/Ca(2+) exchange as a key mechanism of TRPC signaling.,P�ügers Archiv : European journal of physiology, 451(1):99�104, 2005.

Egorov, A. V., Hamam, B. N., Fransén, E., Hasselmo, M. E. and

Alonso, A. A., Graded persistent activity in entorhinal cortex neurons., Na-ture, 420(6912):173�178, 2002.

Esclassan, F., Coutureau, E., Di Scala, G. and Marchand, A. R., Acholinergic-dependent role for the entorhinal cortex in trace fear conditioning.,The Journal of Neuroscience, 29(25):8087�8093, 2009.

Faber, E. S., Sedlak, P., Vidovic, M. and Sah, P., Synaptic activation oftransient receptor potential channels by metabotropic glutamate receptors in thelateral amygdala., Neuroscience, 137(3):781�794, 2006.

Featherstone, D. E. and Shippy, S. A., Regulation of synaptic transmission byambient extracellular glutamate., Neuroscientist, 14(2):171�181, 2008.

Ferraguti, F., Crepaldi, L. and Nicoletti, F., Metabotropic glutamate 1receptor: current concepts and perspectives., Pharmacological reviews, 60(4):536�581, 2008.

Fiala, J. C., Grossberg, S. and Bullock, D., Metabotropic glutamate re-ceptor activation in cerebellar Purkinje cells as substrate for adaptive timingof the classically conditioned eye-blink response., The Journal of Neuroscience,16(11):3760�3774, 1996.

Fotuhi, M., Standaert, D. G., Testa, C. M., Penney, J. B. and Young,

A. B., Di�erential expression of metabotropic glutamate receptors in the hip-pocampus and entorhinal cortex of the rat., Brain research. Molecular brain re-search, 21(3-4):283�292, 1994.

Page 66: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

58 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Fowler, M. A., Sidiropoulou, K., Ozkan, E. D., Phillips, C. W. and

Cooper, D. C., Corticolimbic expression of TRPC4 and TRPC5 channels in therodent brain., PLoS ONE, 2(6), 2007.

Frank, L. M., Brown, E. N. and Wilson, M., Trajectory encoding in thehippocampus and entorhinal cortex., Neuron, 27(1):169�178, 2000.

Frankemolle, A. M., Wu, J., Noecker, A. M., Voelcker-Rehage, C.,

Ho, J. C., Vitek, J. L., McIntyre, C. C. and Alberts, J. L., Reversingcognitive-motor impairments in Parkinson's disease patients using a computa-tional modelling approach to deep brain stimulation programming., Brain, 133(Pt3):746�761, 2010.

Fransén, E., Alonso, A. A. and Hasselmo, M. E., Simulations of the roleof the muscarinic-activated calcium-sensitive nonspeci�c cation current INCM inentorhinal neuronal activity during delayed matching tasks., The Journal of Neu-roscience, 22(3):1081�1097, 2002.

Fransén, E., Tahvildari, B., Egorov, A. V., Hasselmo, M. E. and Alonso,

A. A., Mechanism of graded persistent cellular activity of entorhinal cortex layerv neurons., Neuron, 49(5):735�746, 2006.

Fransén, E. and Tigerholm, J., Role of A-type potassium currents in excitabil-ity, network synchronicity, and epilepsy., Hippocampus, 20(7):877�887, 2010.

Frick, A. and Johnston, D., Plasticity of dendritic excitability., Journal of neu-robiology, 64(1):100�115, 2005.

Frick, A., Magee, J. and Johnston, D., LTP is accompanied by an enhancedlocal excitability of pyramidal neuron dendrites., Nature neuroscience, 7(2):126�135, 2004.

Fyhn, M., Molden, S., Witter, M. P., Moser, E. I. and Moser, M.-B. B.,Spatial representation in the entorhinal cortex., Science, 305(5688):1258�1264,2004.

Gee, C. E., Benquet, P. and Gerber, U., Group I metabotropic glutamatereceptors activate a calcium-sensitive transient receptor potential-like conductancein rat hippocampus., The Journal of physiology, 546(Pt 3):655�664, 2003.

Gladding, C. M., Fitzjohn, S. M. and Molnár, E., Metabotropic glutamatereceptor-mediated long-term depression: molecular mechanisms., Pharmacologicalreviews, 61(4):395�412, 2009.

Gregory, M. L. and Szumlinski, K. K., Impoverished rearing impairs work-ing memory and metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 expression., Neuroreport,19(2):239�243, 2008.

Gross, S. A. A., Guzmán, G. A. A., Wissenbach, U., Philipp, S. E. E., Zhu,

M. X. X., Bruns, D. and Cavalié, A., TRPC5 is a Ca2+-activated channelfunctionally coupled to Ca2+-selective ion channels., The Journal of biologicalchemistry, 284(49):34423�34432, 2009.

Page 67: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

BIBLIOGRAPHY 59

Gulledge, A. T., Kampa, B. M. and Stuart, G. J., Synaptic integration indendritic trees., Journal of neurobiology, 64(1):75�90, 2005.

Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Meuli, R., Honey, C. J.,

Wedeen, V. J. and Sporns, O., Mapping the structural core of human cerebralcortex., PLoS biology, 6(7):e159+, 2008.

von Bohlen und Halbach, O., Hinz, U., Unsicker, K. and Egorov, A. V.,Distribution of TRPC1 and TRPC5 in medial temporal lobe structures of mice.,Cell and tissue research, 322(2):201�206, 2005.

Hallen, K., Huss, M., Kettunen, P., Elmanira, A. and Hellgrenko-

taleski, J., mGluR-Mediated calcium oscillations in the lamprey: a computa-tional model, Neurocomputing, 58-60:431�435, 2004.

Hardie, R. C. and Minke, B., The trp gene is essential for a light-activated Ca2+channel in Drosophila photoreceptors., Neuron, 8(4):643�651, 1992.

Hartmann, J., Dragicevic, E., Adelsberger, H., Henning, H. A., Sumser,

M., Abramowitz, J., Blum, R., Dietrich, A., Freichel, M., Flockerzi,

V., Birnbaumer, L. and Konnerth, A., TRPC3 channels are required forsynaptic transmission and motor coordination., Neuron, 59(3):392�398, 2008.

Hartmann, J. and Konnerth, A., Mechanisms of metabotropic glutamatereceptor-mediated synaptic signaling in cerebellar Purkinje cells., Acta physio-logica, 195(1):79�90, 2008.

Harvey, C. D., Collman, F., Dombeck, D. A. and Tank, D. W., Intra-cellular dynamics of hippocampal place cells during virtual navigation, Nature,461(7266):941�946, 2009.

Hasselmo, M. E., The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory., Currentopinion in neurobiology, 16(6):710�715, 2006.

Hasselmo, M. E. and Bower, J. M., Acetylcholine and memory., Trends inneurosciences, 16(6):218�222, 1993.

Hasselmo, M. E. and Giocomo, L. M., Cholinergic modulation of cortical func-tion., Journal of molecular neuroscience : MN, 30(1-2):133�135, 2006.

Hasselmo, M. E. and Stern, C. E., Mechanisms underlying working memoryfor novel information., Trends in cognitive sciences, 10(11):487�493, 2006.

Häusser, M. and Mel, B., Dendrites: bug or feature?, Current opinion in neu-robiology, 13(3):372�383, 2003.

Hayashi, K., Yoshihara, T. and Ichitani, Y., Involvement of hippocam-pal metabotropic glutamate receptors in radial maze performance., Neuroreport,18(7):719�723, 2007.

Herz, A. V., Gollisch, T., Machens, C. K. and Jaeger, D., Modeling single-neuron dynamics and computations: a balance of detail and abstraction., Science,314(5796):80�85, 2006.

Page 68: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

60 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Hines, M. L. and Carnevale, N. T., NEURON: a tool for neuroscientists., TheNeuroscientist, 7(2):123�135, 2001.

Hines, M. L., Davison, A. P. and Muller, E., NEURON and Python., Frontiersin neuroinformatics, 3, 2009.

Hires, S. A. A., Zhu, Y. and Tsien, R. Y., Optical measurement of synapticglutamate spillover and reuptake by linker optimized glutamate-sensitive �uores-cent reporters., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the UnitedStates of America, 105(11):4411�4416, 2008.

Hodgkin, A. L. and Huxley, A. F., A quantitative description of membranecurrent and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve, The Journal ofPhysiology, 117(4):500�544, 1952.

Jacobs, J., Kahana, M. J., Ekstrom, A. D., Mollison, M. V. and Fried,

I., A sense of direction in human entorhinal cortex, Proceedings of the NationalAcademy of Sciences, 107(14):6487�6492, 2010.

Jaeger, D. and Bower, J. M., Synaptic control of spiking in cerebellar Purk-inje cells: dynamic current clamp based on model conductances., The Journal ofNeuroscience, 19(14):6090�6101, 1999.

Jaeger, D., De Schutter, E. and Bower, J. M., The role of synaptic andvoltage-gated currents in the control of Purkinje cell spiking: a modeling study.,The Journal of Neuroscience, 17(1):91�106, 1997.

Jia, H., Rochefort, N. L., Chen, X. and Konnerth, A., Dendritic organi-zation of sensory input to cortical neurons in vivo, Nature, 464(7293):1307�1312,2010.

Johnston, D. and Narayanan, R., Active dendrites: colorful wings of the mys-terious butter�ies., Trends in neurosciences, 31(6):309�316, 2008.

Johnston, D. and Wu, S. M., Foundations of cellular neurophysiology, MITPress, 1995, ISBN 0262100533.

Kanki, H., Kinoshita, M., Akaike, A., Satoh, M., Mori, Y. and Kaneko,

S., Activation of inositol 1,4,5-trisphosphate receptor is essential for the openingof mouse TRP5 channels, Molecular Pharmacology, 60(5):989�998, 2001.

Kath, W. L., Computational modeling of dendrites., Journal of neurobiology,64(1):91�99, 2005.

Katz, Y., Menon, V., Nicholson, D. A., Geinisman, Y., Kath, W. L. and

Spruston, N., Synapse distribution suggests a two-stage model of dendritic in-tegration in CA1 pyramidal neurons., Neuron, 63(2):171�177, 2009.

Kim, S. J. J., Kim, Y. S. S., Yuan, J. P., Petralia, R. S., Worley, P. F.

and Linden, D. J., Activation of the TRPC1 cation channel by metabotropicglutamate receptor mGluR1., Nature, 426(6964):285�291, 2003.

Page 69: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

BIBLIOGRAPHY 61

Knöpfel, T. and Uusisaari, M., Modulation of excitation by metabotropic glu-tamate receptors., Results and problems in cell di�erentiation, 44:163�175, 2008.

Kovalchuk, Y., Eilers, J., Lisman, J. and Konnerth, A., NMDA receptor-mediated subthreshold Ca(2+) signals in spines of hippocampal neurons., TheJournal of neuroscience, 20(5):1791�1799, 2000.

Larkum, M. E. and Nevian, T., Synaptic clustering by dendritic signalling mech-anisms., Current opinion in neurobiology, 18(3):321�331, 2008.

Larkum, M. E., Nevian, T., Sandler, M., Polsky, A. and Schiller, J.,Synaptic integration in tuft dendrites of layer 5 pyramidal neurons: a new unifyingprinciple., Science, 325(5941):756�760, 2009.

Li, S., Gosling, M. and Poll, C., Determining the functional role of TRPC chan-nels in primary cells., P�ügers Archiv : European journal of physiology, 451(1):43�52, 2005.

Lie, D. C., Song, H., Colamarino, S. A., Ming, G.-l. L. and Gage, F. H.,Neurogenesis in the adult brain: new strategies for central nervous system dis-eases., Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology, 44(1):399�421, 2004.

Lipscombe, D., Helton, T. D. and Xu, W., L-type calcium channels: the lowdown., Journal of neurophysiology, 92(5):2633�2641, 2004.

London, M. and Häusser, M., Dendritic computation., Annual review of neuro-science, 28(1):503�532, 2005.

Losonczy, A. and Magee, J. C., Integrative properties of radial oblique dendritesin hippocampal CA1 pyramidal neurons., Neuron, 50(2):291�307, 2006.

Losonczy, A., Makara, J. K. and Magee, J. C., Compartmentalized dendriticplasticity and input feature storage in neurons, Nature, 452(7186):436�441, 2008.

Lujan, R., Nusser, Z., Roberts, J. D., Shigemoto, R. and Somogyi, P.,Perisynaptic location of metabotropic glutamate receptors mGluR1 and mGluR5on dendrites and dendritic spines in the rat hippocampus., The European journalof neuroscience, 8(7):1488�1500, 1996.

Lütcke, H., Murayama, M., Hahn, T., Margolis, D. J., Astori, S., Zum

Alten Borgloh, S. M. M., Göbel, W., Yang, Y., Tang, W., Kügler, S.,

Sprengel, R., Nagai, T., Miyawaki, A., Larkum, M. E., Helmchen, F.

and Hasan, M. T., Optical recording of neuronal activity with a genetically-encoded calcium indicator in anesthetized and freely moving mice., Frontiers inneural circuits, 4, 2010.

Magee, J. C., Dendritic integration of excitatory synaptic input., Nature reviews.Neuroscience, 1(3):181�190, 2000.

Magee, J. C. and Johnston, D., Plasticity of dendritic function., Current opinionin neurobiology, 15(3):334�342, 2005.

Page 70: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

62 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Marcaggi, P. and Attwell, D., Endocannabinoid signaling depends on thespatial pattern of synapse activation., Nature neuroscience, 8(6):776�781, 2005.

Marcaggi, P., Mutoh, H., Dimitrov, D., Beato, M. and Knöpfel, T., Op-tical measurement of mGluR1 conformational changes reveals fast activation, slowdeactivation, and sensitization., Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciencesof the United States of America, 106(27):11388�11393, 2009.

Mateos, J. M., Benítez, R., Elezgarai, I., Azkue, J. J., Lázaro, E., Os-

orio, A., Bilbao, A., Doñate, F., Sarría, R., Conquet, F., Ferraguti,

F., Kuhn, R., Knöpfel, T. and Grandes, P., Immunolocalization of themGluR1b splice variant of the metabotropic glutamate receptor 1 at parallel �ber-Purkinje cell synapses in the rat cerebellar cortex., Journal of neurochemistry,74(3):1301�1309, 2000.

McIntyre, C. C., Miocinovic, S. and Butson, C. R., Computational analysisof deep brain stimulation., Expert review of medical devices, 4(5):615�622, 2007.

Megías, M., Emri, Z., Freund, T. F. and Gulyás, A. I., Total number anddistribution of inhibitory and excitatory synapses on hippocampal CA1 pyramidalcells., Neuroscience, 102(3):527�540, 2001.

Migliore, M. and Shepherd, G. M., Emerging rules for the distributions ofactive dendritic conductances., Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 3(5):362�370, 2002.

Mikami, A., Masuoka, T., Yasuda, M., Yamamoto, Y. and Kamei, C.,Participation of cholinergic system in memory de�cits induced by blockade ofhippocampal mGlu(1) receptors., European journal of pharmacology, 575(1-3):82�86, 2007.

Minke, B., TRP channels and Ca2+ signaling., Cell calcium, 40(3):261�275, 2006.

Minke, B., Wu, C. and Pak, W. L., Induction of photoreceptor voltage noise inthe dark in Drosophila mutant., Nature, 258(5530):84�87, 1975.

Miocinovic, S., Noecker, A. M., Maks, C. B., Butson, C. R. and McIn-

tyre, C. C., Cicerone: stereotactic neurophysiological recording and deep brainstimulation electrode placement software system., Acta neurochirurgica. Supple-ment, 97(Pt 2):561�567, 2007.

Miocinovic, S., Parent, M., Butson, C. R., Hahn, P. J., Russo, G. S.,

Vitek, J. L. and McIntyre, C. C., Computational analysis of subthalamicnucleus and lenticular fasciculus activation during therapeutic deep brain stimu-lation., Journal of neurophysiology, 96(3):1569�1580, 2006.

Montell, C., The TRP superfamily of cation channels., Science's STKE,2005(272), 2005.

Montell, C., Birnbaumer, L. and Flockerzi, V., The TRP channels, a re-markably functional family., Cell, 108(5):595�598, 2002.

Page 71: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

BIBLIOGRAPHY 63

Montell, C. and Rubin, G. M., Molecular characterization of the Drosophilatrp locus: a putative integral membrane protein required for phototransduction.,Neuron, 2(4):1313�1323, 1989.

Moran, M. M., Xu, H. and Clapham, D. E., TRP ion channels in the nervoussystem., Current opinion in neurobiology, 14(3):362�369, 2004.

Murayama, M. and Larkum, M. E., Enhanced dendritic activity in awake rats.,Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America,106(48):20482�20486, 2009.

Murayama, M., Pérez-Garci, E., Nevian, T., Bock, T., Senn, W. and

Larkum, M. E., Dendritic encoding of sensory stimuli controlled by deep corticalinterneurons., Nature, 457(7233):1137�1141, 2009.

Naie, K. and Manahan-Vaughan, D., Regulation by metabotropic glutamatereceptor 5 of LTP in the dentate gyrus of freely moving rats: relevance for learningand memory formation., Cerebral cortex, 14(2):189�198, 2004.

Nilius, B., TRP channels in disease., Biochimica et biophysica acta, 1772(8):805�812, 2007.

Nilius, B., Mahieu, F., Prenen, J., Janssens, A., Owsianik, G., Vennekens,

R. and Voets, T., The Ca2+-activated cation channel TRPM4 is regulated byphosphatidylinositol 4,5-biphosphate., The EMBO journal, 25(3):467�478, 2006.

Nilius, B., Owsianik, G., Voets, T. and Peters, J. A., Transient receptorpotential cation channels in disease., Physiological reviews, 87(1):165�217, 2007.

Niswender, C. M. and Conn, P. J., Metabotropic glutamate receptors: physi-ology, pharmacology, and disease., Annual review of pharmacology and toxicology,50:295�322, 2010.

Obukhov, A. G. and Nowycky, M. C., TRPC5 channels undergo changes in gat-ing properties during the activation-deactivation cycle., Journal of cellular physi-ology, 216(1):162�171, 2008.

Olive, M. F., Metabotropic glutamate receptor ligands as potential therapeuticsfor addiction., Current drug abuse reviews, 2(1):83�989, 2009.

Otsuguro, K.-i., Tang, J., Tang, Y., Xiao, R., Freichel, M., Tsvilovskyy,

V., Ito, S., Flockerzi, V., Zhu, M. X. and Zholos, A. V., Isoform-speci�cinhibition of TRPC4 channel by phosphatidylinositol 4,5-bisphosphate, Journalof biological chemistry, 283(15):10026�10036, 2008.

Otto, T. and Eichenbaum, H., Complementary roles of the orbital prefrontal cor-tex and the perirhinal-entorhinal cortices in an odor-guided delayed-nonmatching-to-sample task., Behavioral neuroscience, 106(5):762�775, 1992.

Partridge, L. D. and Valenzuela, C. F., Ca2+ store-dependent potentiationof Ca2+-activated non-selective cation channels in rat hippocampal neurones invitro., The Journal of physiology, 521 Pt 3:617�627, 1999.

Page 72: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

64 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Pedersen, S. F. F., Owsianik, G. and Nilius, B., TRP channels: an overview.,Cell calcium, 38(3-4):233�252, 2005.

Petersson, M. and Fransén, E., Role of TRPC channels in dendritic integrationand subthreshold membrane potential plateaus, in Soc. Neurosci. Abstr., 246.1,2010.

Pin, J.-P., Acher, F., Conn, P. J., Duvoisin, R., Ferraguti, F., Flor,

P. J., Hampson, D., Johnson, M. P., Monn, J., Nakanishi, S., Nicoletti,

F., Schoepp, D. D. and Shigemoto, R., Metabotropic glutamate receptors,IUPHAR database (IUPHAR-DB), Accessed on 2010-08-24.

Plant, T. D. and Schaefer, M., Receptor-operated cation channels formedby TRPC4 and TRPC5., Naunyn-Schmiedeberg's archives of pharmacology,371(4):266�276, 2005.

Poirazi, P., Brannon, T. and Mel, B. W., Arithmetic of subthreshold synapticsummation in a model CA1 pyramidal cell., Neuron, 37(6):977�987, 2003.

Polsky, A., Mel, B. W. and Schiller, J., Computational subunits in thindendrites of pyramidal cells., Nature neuroscience, 7(6):621�627, 2004.

Purves, D., Neuroscience, Sinauer, 4th edition, 2008, ISBN 0878936971.

Rall, W., Branching dendritic trees and motoneuron membrane resistivity., Exper-imental neurology, 1:491�527, 1959.

Rall, W. and Rinzel, J., Branch input resistance and steady attenuation forinput to one branch of a dendritic neuron model., Biophysical journal, 13(7):648�687, 1973.

Ramsey, I. S., Delling, M. and Clapham, D. E., An introduction to TRPchannels., Annual review of physiology, 68:619�647, 2006.

Reichelt, W. and Knöpfel, T., Glutamate uptake controls expression of a slowpostsynaptic current mediated by mGluRs in cerebellar Purkinje cells., Journalof neurophysiology, 87(4):1974�1980, 2002.

Reyes, A., In�uence of dendritic conductances on the input-output properties ofneurons., Annual review of neuroscience, 24:653�675, 2001.

Riccio, A., Li, Y., Moon, J., Kim, K.-S. S., Smith, K. S., Rudolph, U.,

Gapon, S., Yao, G. L. L., Tsvetkov, E., Rodig, S. J., Van't Veer, A.,

Meloni, E. G., Carlezon, W. A., Bolshakov, V. Y. and Clapham, D. E.,Essential role for TRPC5 in amygdala function and fear-related behavior., Cell,137(4):761�772, 2009.

Roediger, H. L., Dudai, Y. and Fitzpatrick, S. M., Science of memory :concepts, Oxford University Press, 2007, ISBN 0195310446.

Rudy, J. W. and Matus-Amat, P., DHPG activation of group 1 mGluRs in BLAenhances fear conditioning., Learning & memory, 16(7):421�425, 2009.

Page 73: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

BIBLIOGRAPHY 65

Rusakov, D. A. and Kullmann, D. M., Extrasynaptic glutamate di�usion inthe hippocampus: ultrastructural constraints, uptake, and receptor activation.,The Journal of neuroscience, 18(9):3158�3170, 1998.

Saltelli, A., Sensitivity analysis in practice : a guide to assessing scienti�c models,Wiley, 2004, ISBN 0470870931.

Scanziani, M. and Häusser, M., Electrophysiology in the age of light., Nature,461(7266):930�939, 2009.

Schaefer, M., Homo- and heteromeric assembly of TRP channel subunits.,P�ügers Archiv : European journal of physiology, 451(1):35�42, 2005.

Schiller, J. and Schiller, Y., NMDA receptor-mediated dendritic spikes andcoincident signal ampli�cation., Current opinion in neurobiology, 11(3):343�348,2001.

Schulz, B., Fendt, M., Gasparini, F., Lingenhöhl, K., Kuhn, R. and Koch,

M., The metabotropic glutamate receptor antagonist 2-methyl-6-(phenylethynyl)-pyridine (MPEP) blocks fear conditioning in rats., Neuropharmacology, 41(1):1�7,2001.

Scimemi, A. and Beato, M., Determining the neurotransmitter concentrationpro�le at active synapses., Molecular neurobiology, 40(3):289�306, 2009.

Segev, I., Sound grounds for computing dendrites., Nature, 393(6682):207�208,1998.

Segev, I. and London, M., Untangling dendrites with quantitative models., Sci-ence, 290(5492):744�750, 2000.

Selvaraj, S., Sun, Y. and Singh, B. B., TRPC channels and their implicationin neurological diseases., CNS & neurological disorders drug targets, 9(1):94�104,2010.

Shalinsky, M. H., Magistretti, J., Ma, L. and Alonso, A. A., Muscarinicactivation of a cation current and associated current noise in entorhinal-cortexlayer-II neurons, Journal of neurophysiology, 88(3):1197�1211, 2002.

Shoham, S., O'Connor, D. H. and Segev, R., How silent is the brain: is therea "dark matter" problem in neuroscience?, Journal of comparative physiology,192(8):777�784, 2006.

Sidiropoulou, K., Lu, F.-M. M., Fowler, M. A., Xiao, R., Phillips, C.,

Ozkan, E. D., Zhu, M. X., White, F. J. and Cooper, D. C., Dopaminemodulates an mGluR5-mediated depolarization underlying prefrontal persistentactivity., Nature neuroscience, 12(2):190�199, 2009.

Sidiropoulou, K., Pissadaki, E. K. K. and Poirazi, P., Inside the brain of aneuron., EMBO reports, 7(9):886�892, 2006.

Page 74: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

66 BIBLIOGRAPHY

Sjöström, P. J., Rancz, E. A., Roth, A. and Häusser, M., Dendritic ex-citability and synaptic plasticity., Physiological reviews, 88(2):769�840, 2008.

Spruston, N., Pyramidal neurons: dendritic structure and synaptic integration.,Nature reviews. Neuroscience, 9(3):206�221, 2008.

Squire, L. R., Encyclopedia of neuroscience, Elsevier Academic Press, 2009, ISBN0080446175.

Steuber, V. and Willshaw, D., A biophysical model of synaptic delay learn-ing and temporal pattern recognition in a cerebellar Purkinje cell., Journal ofcomputational neuroscience, 17(2):149�164, 2004.

Steuber, V., Willshaw, D. and Van Ooyen, A., Generation of time delays:simpli�ed models of intracellular signalling in cerebellar Purkinje cells., Network,17(2):173�191, 2006.

Strübing, C., Krapivinsky, G., Krapivinsky, L. and Clapham, D. E.,TRPC1 and TRPC5 form a novel cation channel in mammalian brain., Neuron,29(3):645�655, 2001.

Stuart, G., Spruston, N. and Häusser, M., Dendrites, Oxford UniversityPress, 2007, ISBN 0198566565.

Suzuki, W. A., Miller, E. K. and Desimone, R., Object and place memoryin the macaque entorhinal cortex., Journal of neurophysiology, 78(2):1062�1081,1997.

Talavera, K., Nilius, B. and Voets, T., Neuronal TRP channels: thermome-ters, path�nders and life-savers., Trends in neurosciences, 31(6):287�295, 2008.

Vazquez, G., Wedel, B. J., Aziz, O., Trebak, M. and Putney, J. W., Themammalian TRPC cation channels., Biochimica et biophysica acta, 1742(1-3):21�36, 2004.

Venkatachalam, K. and Montell, C., TRP channels., Annual review of bio-chemistry, 76(1):387�417, 2007.

Villereal, M. L., Mechanism and functional signi�cance of TRPC channel mul-timerization., Seminars in cell & developmental biology, 17(6):618�629, 2006.

Voets, T., Droogmans, G., Wissenbach, U., Janssens, A., Flockerzi, V.

and Nilius, B., The principle of temperature-dependent gating in cold- andheat-sensitive TRP channels., Nature, 430(7001):748�754, 2004.

Wang, H., Westin, L., Nong, Y., Birnbaum, S., Bendor, J., Brismar, H.,

Nestler, E., Aperia, A., Flajolet, M. and Greengard, P., Norbin is anendogenous regulator of metabotropic glutamate receptor 5 signaling., Science,326(5959):1554�1557, 2009.

Williams, S. R. and Wozny, C., Neuroscience: The chain reaction of dendriticintegration., Current biology, 19(20):R956�R957, 2009.

Page 75: Beyond AMPA and NMDA: Slow synaptic mGlu/ TRPC currents - DiVA

BIBLIOGRAPHY 67

Wu, L.-J. J., Sweet, T.-B. B. and Clapham, D. E., International Union ofBasic and Clinical Pharmacology. LXXVI. Current progress in the mammalianTRP ion channel family., Pharmacological reviews, 62(3):381�404, 2010.

Yan, H.-D. D., Villalobos, C. and Andrade, R., TRPC channels mediate amuscarinic receptor-induced afterdepolarization in cerebral cortex., The Journalof neuroscience, 29(32):10038�10046, 2009.

Yoshida, M., Fransén, E. and Hasselmo, M. E., mGluR-dependent persistent�ring in entorhinal cortex layer III neurons., The European journal of neuro-science, 28(6):1116�1126, 2008.

Yoshida, M. and Hasselmo, M. E., Persistent �ring supported by an intrinsiccellular mechanism in a component of the head direction system., The Journal ofneuroscience, 29(15):4945�4952, 2009.

Zhang, Z., Reboreda, A., Alonso, A., Barker, P. A. and Séguéla, P.,TRPC channels underlie cholinergic plateau potentials and persistent activity inentorhinal cortex., Hippocampus, 2010.

Zhang, Z. and Séguéla, P., Metabotropic induction of persistent activity inlayers II/III of anterior cingulate cortex., Cerebral cortex, 2010.

Master thesis

Computational Modeling of Deep Brain Stimulation. Marcus E. Petersson. MScthesis, Linköping University, 2007. Published on http://urn.kb.se/resolve?urn=urn:nbn:se:liu:diva-9512