14

Click here to load reader

Baker Cote - Sport-Specific Practice and the Development - JASP

  • Upload
    43ajf43

  • View
    20

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Cote Baker

Citation preview

  • 12 J. BAKER ET AL.

    12

    JOURNAL OF APPLIED SPORT PSYCHOLOGY, 15: 1225, 2003Copyright ' 2003 by the Association for Advancement of Applied Sport Psychology1041-3200/03 $12.00 + .00DOI:10.1080/10413200390180035

    Sport-Specific Practice and the Developmentof Expert Decision-Making in Team Ball Sports

    JOSEPH BAKER AND JEAN CT

    School of Physical and Health EducationQueens University

    BRUCE ABERNETHY

    School of Human Movement StudiesThe University of Queensland

    The role of sport-specific practice in the development of decision-making expertise in thesports of field hockey, netball, and basketball was examined. Fifteen expert decision-makersand 13 experienced non-expert athletes provided detailed information about the quantity andtype of sport-specific and other related practice activities they had undertaken throughout theircareers. Experts accumulated more hours of sport-specific practice from age 12 years onwardsthan did non-experts, spending on average some 13 years and 4,000 hours on concentratedsport-specific practice before reaching international standard. A significant negative correla-tion existed between the number of additional activities undertaken and the hours of sport-specific training required before attaining expertise, suggesting a functional role for activitiesother than sport-specific training in the development of expert decision-making.

    In their seminal study of chess, Simon and Chase (1973) suggested that a minimum of 10years of preparation was necessary to reach the expert level. Since that time, this 10-year

    Received 24 September 2001; accepted 4 June 2002.During the completion of this research the first author was with the School of Human Movement

    Studies, University of Queensland and the second author was on research leave at the School ofHuman Movement Studies, University of Queensland.

    The authors would like to extend appreciation to Dr. Deborah Hoare, Dr. Ross Smith, and thecoaches and athletes of the Australian national mens basketball, womens netball and mens andwomens field hockey teams for their support and patience during the completion of this study. Theauthors would also like to thank Steve Cobley and other colleagues for helpful comments on an earlierversion of this manuscript. This research was funded by the Australian Sports Commission and astandard research grant from the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC# 410-99-0525).

    Address correspondence to Joseph Baker, School of Physical and Health Education, QueensUniversity, Kingston, ON K7L 3N6. Email: [email protected]

  • 13SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    rule has been shown to also apply to the development of expertise in other domains includingmusic (Ericsson, Krampe, & Tesch-Rmer, 1993; Hayes, 1981; Sosniak, 1985), mathematics(Gustin, 1985), swimming (Kalinowski, 1985), distance running (Wallingford, 1975), tennis(Monsaas, 1985), soccer, and field hockey (Helsen, Starkes, & Hodges, 1998).

    The theory of deliberate practice, developed by Ericsson and colleagues (e.g., Ericsson &Charness, 1994; Ericsson et al., 1993), is predicated on the notion that it is not simply trainingof any type, but rather a minimum of 10 years engagement in deliberate practice that is thenecessary condition for the attainment of expertise. Deliberate practice refers to practice ac-tivities done with the specific instrumental goal of improving performance and which (a) areperformed in a daily, work-like manner; (b) require effort and attention; (c) do not lead toimmediate social or financial rewards; and (d) are frequently not enjoyable to perform. Whilethe notion of deliberate practice was developed through research with musicians, Ericsson andhis colleagues have indicated that it has applicability to the acquisition of expertise in all areasof human endeavor, including sport (Ericsson, 1996; Ericsson et al., 1993).

    To date, research examining the application of the theory of deliberate practice to sport ingeneral and team sports in particular has been limited. Researchers have examined figureskating (Starkes, Deakin, Allard, Hodges, & Hayes, 1996), karate (Hodge & Deakin, 1998),wrestling (Hodges & Starkes, 1996), middle distance running (Young & Salmela, in press),soccer (Helsen et al., 2000; Helsen et al., 1998), and field hockey (Helsen, Starkes, & Hodges,1998). These studies provide support for many of the contentions of Ericsson and his col-leagues, including the strong relationship between accumulated hours of practice and level ofperformance. For instance, expert athletes and musicians performed similar amounts (around25 hours) of deliberate practice per week. However, these studies also indicate that some of theoriginal tenets of the theory of deliberate practice may be too inflexible for direct applicationto sport settings. For example, all of the examinations of deliberate practice performed to datein sport have found that, contrary to one of the key components of Ericssons definition ofdeliberate practice, the athletes systematically rate many specific practice activities as beingvery enjoyable.

    Examination of the application of the theory of deliberate practice to team sport settings hasbeen more difficult. While Ericsson and his colleagues indicated that practice alone was themost important activity for developing musical proficiency, the development of expertise inteam sport settings requires mastery of a wide range of skills extending from very generalelements such as physical fitness to very specific elements such as team strategies. To masterthese components athletes undertake a variety of forms of training, all of which are believed tobe relevant to the improvement of their overall individual and/or team performance. Cross-training (i.e., training using other sports or activities) is routinely used to improve physiologi-cal conditioning (Moran & McGlynn, 1997) whereas skills training is typically done in a verysport-specific manner. Performance in team sports is also dependent upon the cohesive inter-action among members of the sports team unit, thereby necessitating training as a group inaddition to training alone. Therefore, unlike the Ericsson et al. (1993) study of musicianswhere only practice alone was examined, all forms of practice must be considered when exam-ining deliberate practice in team sports (Helsen et al., 2000). In the Helsen et al. (1998, 2000)investigation of deliberate practice in the team sports of field hockey and soccer, they notedthat athletes had performed approximately 10,000 hours of training at the time of data collec-tion and that this accumulated training was a key distinguishing factor between athletes atvarious levels of expertise (i.e., international, national, and provincial level of play). Thisvalue is quite similar to the near 10,000 hours of training indicated by the musicians in theEricsson et al. study.

    Unsurprisingly, the attainment of expertise is much more complex than simply accumulat-

  • 14 J. BAKER ET AL.

    ing a required number of hours of deliberate practice. For instance, key developmental periodsmay occur in the development of sporting expertise where the influence of participation in awide range of activities, and not just the sport of prime interest, is important. Ct and col-leagues (Ct, 1999; Ct, Baker, & Abernethy, in press; Ct, Baker, & Abernethy, 2001;Ct & Hay, 2002), in their investigations of elite Australian and Canadian athletes, haveidentified three distinct stages of sport participation that athletes pass through prior to theattainment of expert performance: the sampling years (512), the specializing years (1315),and the investment years (16+). The sampling years are typified by the athlete participating ina wide range of different sports. It is during this period that skills such as running, jumping,and throwing that are fundamental to performance in many sports are acquired and refined. Inthe specializing years, athletes begin to narrow their focus and participate in fewer activities.In the investment years, athletes become devoted to a single sport by making maximal perfor-mance in this sport their primary life focus. In their study of expert team sport athletes, Helsenet al. (1998) found that at a time around 9 years into their career expert soccer and field hockeyplayers increased both training duration and intensity and they suggested that this was a neces-sary requirement for reaching expert level performance (i.e., international level of competi-tion). Moreover, research on the hierarchy of motor skill development (e.g., Seefeldt, 1980,1982) supports the approach that sport involvement should progress from a focus on funda-mental motor skills in early childhood to more sport-specific skills in adolescence and earlyadulthood. Moreover, the development of fundamental motor skills may be transferable acrosssports and activities that share similar general capacities.

    Most theories of child development (e.g., Piaget, 1962; Vigotsky, 1978) hold that playactivities provide a medium for useful development. In an in-depth analysis of play theories,Sutton-Smith (1997) discussed the different kinds and levels of playful behavior and recog-nized that the various levels of play merit as much attention from researchers as the distinctionbetween play and non-play. Consequently, based on the work of Denzin (1975), Ct andcolleagues (Ct, 1999; Ct et al., in press; Ct & Hay, 2002) differentiated among thevarious kinds of play and suggested the concept of deliberate play as a key component ofchildrens early involvement in sport. Contrary to practice activities where the goal is to im-prove performance, the goal of deliberate play is to have fun. Deliberate play activities have astructure modified from the structure of existing sports and in that sense are also different fromfree play activities (e.g., children playing freely with toy cars). Ct et al. (in press) recentlysuggested an activity scale of involvement in sport ranging from the most elementary activity(i.e., free play) to the most complex (i.e., deliberate practice). The activity-scale levels pro-posed were (a) free play, (b) deliberate play, (c) structured practice, and (d) deliberate practice.Ct and colleagues (Ct et al, in press; Ct & Hay, 2002) also proposed that as the athleteprogresses in sport there is a marked shift from activities focused on pleasurable participationand play during the sampling years to a focus on activities designed to optimize performanceimprovements through practice during the investment years. The virtue of such a scale is thatit is founded on the notion that expertise in sport does not develop from engagement in onlyone type of activity as it has been suggested in music (Ericsson et al., 1993). Ct et al. advo-cated that more attention needed to be focussed on developmental issues and the specificnature of the play and practice activities that athletes engaged in at various stages of theirinvolvement in sport.

    Accordingly, a more comprehensive understanding of the practice base essential for expertperformance, especially team ball sports, requires consideration of not only the sport-specificpractice activities undertaken by players but also the nature and extent of practice and experi-ence accumulated in other related activities. A key characteristic underlying expert perfor-mance in team ball sports is decision-making, that is, the ability to perceive essential informa-

  • 15SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    tion from the playing environment, correctly interpret this information, and then select theappropriate response. While expert superiority in these different components of decision-mak-ing has been consistently demonstrated (see Abernethy, 1991, for a review) little is yet knownabout how such abilities develop. This study examined the quantity and type of both sport-specific and non-sport-specific practice accumulated throughout the careers of expert deci-sion-makers from three different team sports and contrasted these with the practice experi-ences of less accomplished athletes. The purpose of examining the quantity of sport-specificpractice was to (a) ascertain whether this conformed to the general rules for expertise estab-lished in other domains (i.e., 10 years/10,000 hours of training) and (b) determine if the amountof sport-specific practice was a key factor discriminating expert decision-makers from lesserskilled athletes. The purpose of examining the quantity and type of other (non-sport-specific)practice activities undertaken was to (a) identify commonalities in the backgrounds of expertdecision-makers from different sports that may have contributed to their unique expertise and,in particular, (b) determine the extent to which expert athletes participation in other sportswas a distinguishing attribute of their development.

    METHODSParticipants

    Twenty-eight athletes served as participants. The expert group consisted of 15 players cho-sen from the Australian national womens netball team (n = 3), national mens basketball team(n = 4), national mens field hockey team (n = 4), and national womens field hockey team (n= 4). At the time of data collection, each of the teams was highly ranked internationally. Thewomens netball and field hockey teams were world champions, while the mens field hockeyand basketball teams were ranked second and fourth in the world respectively. Each athletewas nominated by their respective national team coaches as being amongst the best decision-makers internationally in their particular sports. All coaches from each respective nationalteam agreed unanimously on the athletes selection for the present study (i.e., athletes chosenfor inclusion in the study were uniformly regarded as expert decision-makers by the entirenational team coaching staff rather than simply by each head coach). Expert decision-makingwas operationally defined to the coaches in terms of superior capabilities to read the playand select the most appropriate option under the pressure of game play rather than in terms ofathleticism or sheer physical skill. The sample of expert decision-makers had a mean age of27.6 years (SD = 4.3) and had been playing their primary sport for an average of 20.7 years(SD = 5.3). These experts played in a variety of different positions on each team with noevidence of potential positional confounds (e.g., selecting only expert point guards in basket-ball and comparing them to centers).

    A sample of 13 non-expert decision-makers (4 from mens basketball and 3 each fromwomens netball, mens field hockey and womens field hockey) provided important com-parative data. The non-expert decision-makers all had greater than 10 years experience in theirsport but had not participated beyond the state or provincial level. The average age for thisnon-expert group was 23.2 yrs (SD = 4.6) with an average involvement of 12.2 years (SD =1.7) in their sport. A general limitation of studies examining the differences between expertsand novices/non-experts is that comparisons are often confounded by years of playing experi-ence. Experts typically have greater experience than non-experts and therefore it is difficult todistinguish whether expert/non-expert differences are a constituent or a by-product of thatexpertise (e.g., see Abernethy, Thomas, & Thomas, 1993). In order to address this issue, ourintent was to obtain athletes who had extensive experience in their sport but who did not reach

  • 16 J. BAKER ET AL.

    the same level of playing proficiency as the expert group. Both expert and non-expert athletesprovided informed consent before participating in the study.

    Procedures

    Each participant completed a structured interview specifically developed to examine thepractice activities of elite athletes (Ct, Ericsson, & Beamer, 2002).1 Interviews were con-ducted one-on-one in a quiet environment with each interview lasting approximately 2 to 3hours. The purpose of the interview was to develop a detailed longitudinal account of eachathletes involvement in sports and other extra-curricular activities. The interview used chartsto systematically record the types of activity that participants engaged in throughout theirdevelopment and evaluated various conditions associated with each activity.

    The initial part of the interview was devoted to the establishment of a comprehensive set ofextra-curricular activities undertaken by the athlete in either a formal or informal way. Thespecific instructional set used to elicit information during this phase of data collection was

    I would like you to focus on the activities that you were involved in when you were young. Iwould like you to list your involvement outside of mandatory school activities, for examplemusic, dance, play, and other domains of activity. I am also interested in your early sportinvolvement. Looking back over your entire life please tell me of any type of activity that youengaged in on a regular basis before you decided to specialize in basketball [or field hockey ornetball]. What musical, sport, play, and artistic activities, if any, were you participating inbefore becoming seriously involved in your main sport? Please list all of these activities, suchas piano, dance, drawing, etc.

    For each of the activities listed, athletes were then asked the following questions:How old were you when you first got started? How long did you keep up the involvement?

    Please tell me of any periods when your involvement was stopped.The second part of the interview assessed the amount of time participants spent in their

    main sport(s) throughout their development. The following instructional set was used to elicitthis information:

    For each of the years listed in the chart can you provide the number of hours per week andnumber of months per year that you were involved in your main sport? This includes practices,games, and specific training activities for your main sport such as organized training, self-initiated training, and individualized instruction.

    This information was categorized as sport-specific training.To examine the quantity and type of practice needed to acquire expert level performance it

    was necessary to define a standard criterion for determining when expert status was achieved.While the identification of such a discrete milestone can be contentious and somewhat arbi-trary the advantage is that it provides a consistent point for the comparison of findings bothwithin and between studies. In this study expertise was defined as the time when the athletes inthe expert decision-making group were first selected to their respective national open-ageteam. The average age for reaching the national open-age team was 18.6 years of age (SD = 2.4years).

    1A copy of the interview guide is available upon request to the second author.

  • 17SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    RESULTSReliability of Retrospective Information

    As the interview task relied extensively on the retrospective recall of the athletes involved,and because of the complexity and depth of information the athletes were required to recall,measures were taken to cross-validate the data provided by the athletes. Previous research hasrelied largely upon the use of one-week training diaries to validate retrospective informationprovided by athletes (Helsen et al., 1998; Hodges & Starkes, 1996). This information, whileproviding an adequate validation of the information provided by the athlete for more recentevents, may not provide an accurate validation of the retrospective recall of activities under-taken much earlier in the athletes development.

    Validation of the number and type of early activities and hours of practice provided by theexpert athletes was done by interviewing a sample of their parents, specifically the parentdeemed by each athlete as most knowledgeable about their sporting career. To examine thecorrespondence between the number of activities reported by the athletes and the number ofactivities reported by their parents (n = 10) the percent agreement (Bahrick, Hall, & Berger,1996) was computed. There was a complete agreement (i.e., 100%) between the total numberof activities reported by the expert athletes and the total reported by the parents. To validatethe athletes self-reported practice hours, a Pearson product moment correlation analysis wasperformed between the number of practice hours per year estimated by the athlete and thecomparable estimate provided by their parent. The correlation coefficient for these two sourceswas r = .59 (p < .05). Reliability of the athletes estimates of hours spent in practice activitieswas also examined through the use of a redundant item built into the interview protocol. Theprotocol elicited information regarding the number of practice hours through two separatequestions. An initial question required athletes to indicate the total number of hours of practicethey performed each week while the second question had the athletes indicate the number ofhours practice spent in each specific activity. The practice hours in each activity were thensummated and compared to the overall training hours estimate provided by the athlete. Thecorrelation between these two estimates was r = .73 (p < .05). Although this coefficient isslightly lower than the one presented by Helsen et al. (1998) this was not surprising given thatthe recall in this study was for events that were, in some cases, up to 30 years in the past.Collectively, these results indicate the data are reasonably valid and reliable.

    Sport-Specific Practice Activities

    The total number of years and hours of sport-specific practice prior to achieving expertperformance (i.e., national open-age team selection) was computed. This information, as wellas the number of other sporting activities in which each athlete participated, is presented inTable 1.

    The number of years spent in practice prior to national team selection ranged from 7 to 20years. All but one of the expert athletes had a minimum of 10 years of involvement beforeachieving expertise (M = 12.9, SD = 2.9). However, the number of hours spent in practice fortheir sport prior to national team selection varied considerably within the sample of expertdecision-makers. The range of practice hours prior to the achievement of expert performanceranged from 600 hours to 6026 hours (M = 3939, SD = 1769.8). Basketball players reportedsignificantly greater sport specific training hours prior to national team selection than the netballand field hockey players, F (2, 12) = 7.9, p < .05.

    Figure 1 presents the distribution of cumulated hours spent in sport-specific practice andpractice hours per week for the expert and non-expert athletes by year of involvement. Figure

  • 18 J. BAKER ET AL.

    2 presents expert and non-expert practice hours per week and involvement in other activitiesby chronological age. Experts accumulated similar hours of sport-specific training to non-experts until approximately 11 years of involvement in their sport. At this point, the rate ofsport-specific practice accumulation by experts escalated dramatically, such that after 15 yearsof sport involvement the number of hours of practice accumulated by experts was nearly twicethat undertaken by the non-experts.

    Figures 1 and 2 provide a comparison of the hours of training per week for the experts andnon-experts according to both year of sport involvement and chronological age. For the yearof sport involvement examination, statistical analyses after 15 years of involvement were notpossible due to an insufficient number of non-expert athletes reporting data after 15 years. Atwo-way repeated measures ANOVA on training hours per week measured at 3-year intervalsrevealed main effects for both level of expertise (F (1, 24) = 6.9, p < .05) and time (each 3-yearepochs; F (1, 24) = 52.9, p < .05), as well as a significant interaction between these two factors(F (1, 24) = 22.3, p < .05). The interaction was attributable to the number of hours of sport-specific practice undertaken by the expert group being greater than that for the non-experts at12 and 15 years after commencing the activity. For the chronological age analysis, a statisticalcomparison of the two skill groups was only possible after 12 years of age because non-ex-perts typically did not begin their involvement in basketball, netball, or field hockey untillater. This analysis revealed a main effect for both level of expertise (F (1, 24) = 6.7, p < .05)and time (F (1, 24) = 14.2, p < .05), as well as a significant interaction between these twofactors (F (1, 24) = 5.3, p < .05). The interaction is attributed to the significant differencebetween hours per week for experts and non-experts after 18 years of age.

    Other Activities

    The number of additional sporting activities (organized and unorganized physical activity)athletes participated in ranged from 3 to 14 activities (M = 8.6, SD = 3.6). Those athletes that

    Table 1Descriptive Statistics for Practice Years, Hours and Number of Other Activities

    Prior to Reaching National Team

    Sport Yrs of Involvement # Practice Hrs # Other Sport Activities

    Basketball players 11.0 (3.2) 5908.5 (104.9) 4.8 (2.2)Range 7 14 5772 6026 3 8

    Netball players 13.3 (1.5) 2260.0 (1479.6) 11.7 (2.1)Range 12 15 600 3440 10 14

    Field hockey players 13.8 (3.0) 3583.3 (1437.0) 9.4 (3.0)Range 10 20 864 5164 5 14

    Females 12.6 (1.5) 2543.4 (1448.5) 11.0 (2.4)Range 10 15 600 4336 7 14

    Males 13.3 (3.8) 5159.5 (903.4) 6.5 (3.1)Range 7 20 3634 6026 3 12

    Entire group 12.9 (2.9) 3939 (1769.8) 8.6 (3.6)Range 7 20 600 6026 3 14

  • 19SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    required fewer hours of sport-specific practice to attain expertise had typically experienced alarger number of other sporting activities, while those athletes that performed the greatestamount of sport-specific practice prior to attaining expertise reported fewer other activities.This observation was confirmed by correlation analysis. A partial correlation between thenumber of prior activities and the number of sport-specific practice hours prior to expertise,while controlling for type of sport, indicated a negative relationship between these two vari-ables (r = .54, p < .05). Engagement in a large number of other sporting activities was asso-ciated with a relatively small number of sport-specific practice hours required before attainingexpert level performance and vice versa.

    Table 2 contains a list of the additional activities that the expert decision-making athletesparticipated in prior to attaining national team selection. Activities were categorized into fourgroups: team sports, individual sports, unorganized sporting activities, and artistic/musicalactivities. The most commonly experienced other team sport activities included basketball,football (including rugby, Aussie Rules, and touch football), cricket and softballsports thatinvolve skills (e.g., working with teammates, anticipating opponents actions) that may betransferable to other similar sports. Athletics (i.e., track and field) was the most commonlyperformed individual sporting activity. Other individual sport activities that were commonlyreported were tennis, squash, and golf. The expert decision-making athletes also reported par-ticipating in a wide range of sport activities that were not organized by an adult (i.e., activitiesthat might constitute deliberate play). These deliberate play activities (such as backyard games,pick-up games) were performed in informal atmospheres that were not part of an organizedsporting system. The most commonly reported deliberate play activities were forms of foot-ball, cricket and cycling. Experts reported minimal participation in artistic/musical activities,with the most common activities being drama, guitar, piano, choir and dance.

    Figure 1. Comparison of expert and non-expert cumulative training hours and training hours perweek by year of involvement.

  • 20 J. BAKER ET AL.

    Figure 2 illustrates the pattern of participation in musical, team and individual sportingactivities, and unorganized activities for each year of involvement. For the expert athletesthere is an increase in the number of activities they participated in until approximately 12 yearsof age. This is followed by a decrease in involvement from 12 to 17 years of age. Finally, atapproximately 17 years of age there is a leveling off of involvement for the expert athletes. Forthe non-experts, a similar pattern is evident for the first few years of involvement, althoughtheir mean involvement in other activities is somewhat less than that undertaken by the expertdecision-makers. As with the experts, non-experts demonstrate an increase in activity involve-ment until approximately 12 years of age. However, after 12 years of age, non-experts main-tain a broader participation in a number of activities while experts begin to decrease theirbroad sports involvement to focus their energies toward their specialist sport and directly re-lated practice activities.

    DISCUSSIONThe data collected in this study suggest that the 10-year rule presented by Simon and

    Chase (1973) remains a good rule of thumb with respect to the minimal sport-specific prac-tice base needed for the development of expertise in team sports. With only one exception, theexpert athletes in the present study were involved in their primary sport for, at least, a mini-mum of 10 years; nearly 13 on average before reaching national, open-age, team selection.Further, the number of accumulated hours of sport-specific practice suggested that this is alsoan important indicator of expertise in sport decision-making, supporting previous findings for

    Figure 2. Comparison of expert and non-expert participation in other activities and training hours perweek by chronological age.

  • 21SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    team sport athletes (Helsen et al., 1998, 2000). However, the number of hours of repeatedsport-specific practice was far short of the 10,000 hours of deliberate practice reported forexpert musicians by Ericsson et al. (1993) and expert soccer and field hockey players byHelsen et al. (1998, 2000), being 4,000 hours, on average.2

    The difference in the number of hours invested in sport specific training (i.e., deliberatepractice) reported by athletes in this study and other studies of athletes in team sports (Helsenet al., 1998, 2000) is likely to be the result of the different time frames used to calculateaccumulated hours of training. The specific criteria used in the current study to define exper-tise (i.e., being chosen for the national open-age team) signified that deliberate practice hourswere counted from first involvement in sport to national team selection (a definite criteria of

    Table 2Additional sporting activities of expert decision-makers

    Activity N Activity N

    Team Sports Unorganized Sporting ActivitiesNetball 1 * Aerobics 1Volleyball 1 Pickup Basketball 1Soccer 3 Squash 1Basketball 8 * Football 7Cricket 5 Cycling 4Softball 6 Cricket 6Football 6 Soccer 2

    Swimming 2Individual Sports Table Tennis 2

    Athletics (track and field) 8 Volleyball 1Tennis 4 Chase 1Karate 1 Badminton 2Running 2 Gymnastics 1Squash 4Golf 4 Artistic/Musical ActivitiesSwimming 3 Organ 1

    Dance 2Drama 4Choir 2Guitar 3Piano 2Trumpet 1Recorder 1Flute 1Crafts 1

    Note. * this number does not include expert athletes from this sport (e.g., basketball n = 8 does not include the 4basketball expert athletes).

    2It is plausible that the expert players reached an elite status because of better practice opportunitiesduring their development. However, in a subsequent analysis it was found that the quality of resourceswas similar between experts and non-experts during the sampling and specializing years. The quality oftraining facilities, equipment, coaching, and social support was significantly better for the expert playerswhen compared to the non-expert players only during the investment years (Abernethy, Ct, & Baker,2002).

  • 22 J. BAKER ET AL.

    expertise attainment in team sports). The metric used to calculate number of hours invested indeliberate practice was the same for all expert players independent of their age at data collec-tion. Because the focus of this study was on the development of expertise and not on themaintenance of expertise (i.e., years on the national team), counting deliberate practice hoursup to national team selection presents an actual account of deliberate practice hours duringdevelopment and is not inflated by the report of older athletes who may have invested a largenumber of deliberate practice hours after reaching an international level of performance. Inthe present study the addition of training done while on the national team, a time frame ac-counted for in Helsen et al. studies (1998, 2000), may have produced an estimate of accumu-lated hours in deliberate practice comparable to the 10,000 hours typically reported in otherstudies.

    An important finding unique to this study is that the range of accumulated hours reportedby the expert athletes was highly variable both within and between sports, suggesting factorsadditional to the total hours of accumulated sport-specific practice influence expertise attain-ment in team sports. Related practice, in other non-sport-specific contexts, emerges as a primecandidate as an additional factor critical to the acquisition of decision-making expertise insport. The involvement of expert athletes in other activities prior to expertise attainment hasnot been examined in other retrospective studies of expert team sport athletes (Helsen et al.1998, 2000) yet examination of other related sporting activities is important as these mayprovide an alternative to early specialization in sport specific deliberate practice activities.

    A significant negative correlation was found between the number of prior sporting activi-ties experienced by the expert decision-makers and the number of hours of sport-specific prac-tice required before national team selection. This suggests that participation in other activitiesmay indeed be a functional element in the development of expert decision-making skill. Expo-sure to practice in other sport settings, especially in generic aspects of pattern recognition anddecision-making, may circumvent the need for, or perhaps partially substitute for, some of themany hours of sport-specific practice needed to become an expert in team ball sports. Perhaps,unlike the domain of music, within which Ericsson et al. (1993) developed their theory ofdeliberate practice, sport expertise in team sports may be sufficiently multi-faceted to permitbeneficial learning to occur through settings other than deliberate, task-specific practice.

    Two of the activities commonly reported as early activities by the expert athletes werefootball (including Aussie Rules, rugby, and touch football) and basketballsports that sharea number of characteristics in common with the sports in which these participants becameexperts. For example, all require repeated, dynamic decision-making during play; all are playedin a confined space that necessitates well-developed pattern recognition and spatial awarenessskills to know where teammates and opponents are at all times; and all require a high degree ofphysical ability for success. The sports of cricket and softball were also commonly reportedand may contribute to developing hand-eye coordination. This may be particularly relevantfor field hockey, another striking game. In addition, athletes also indicated running and cy-cling as common activities in which they were engaged. Participation in these forms of aerobicactivity could provide beneficial cardiovascular effects enhancing performance in sports suchas basketball, netball, and field hockey, which place heavy demands on aerobic energy sys-tems.

    These suppositions suggest that in a practical context, very significant transfer of learningmay take place from one sport to another. Prior research examining transfer of learning wouldsuggest that in all probability transfer occurs more in general cognitive/perceptual capacitiesand physiological adaptations than in the skill-specific execution elements of movement. Fur-thermore, it is likely that this transfer occurs earlier rather than later in the athletes develop-ment (Schmidt & Wrisberg, 2000).

  • 23SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    Perhaps the most significant implication that can be drawn from the present study is thatearly specialization may not be a necessary requirement for expert level performance in deci-sion-making sports. This is an implication consistent with the work of Carlson (1988, 1997),who showed that an all-round sport engagement before adolescence was a key ingredient inthe development of elite tennis players in Sweden, and Hill (1993), who found that diversifiedyouth sport participation was the norm among U.S. professional baseball players. While ex-pert athletes clearly reported more years of involvement in their primary sport than did non-experts, the total hours spent doing sport-specific practice was highly variable. In keepingwith the premises within Cts stages of sport participation model (Ct, 1999; Ct et al., inpress; Ct & Hay, 2002), there was a broad range of activities performed in the samplingyears by experts (age 612), a decrease in other activity involvement in the specializing years(age 1315), followed by devotion to primarily one activity in the investment years (age 16+).Moreover, during the investment years the number of training hours per week for experts wassignificantly higher than those for non-experts.

    The significant negative correlation observed between breadth of sport exposure and thehours of sport-specific practice needed to acquire expertise suggests that it may be possible toaccumulate useful practice from an early age without the need for early sport specializationand a singular focus on sport-specific practice. Indeed, up until the specialization years, ex-perts engaged in as many or more activities additional to their principal sport, than do non-experts. As the existing theory of deliberate practice advocates the relentless accumulation oftask-specific practice as essential for the development of expertise, and hence, by inference,early specialization, it is our contention that modification to this theory is needed in order tofully encapsulate the learning to become an expert that occurs within the domain of team ballsports. Furthermore, we contend that expertise in team sports may be meaningfully investi-gated by examining changes in forms of practice and associated activities throughout the de-velopmental career span of expert athletes.

    Unlike most other study areas, the retrospective study of expertise has a number of uniquelimitations. The study of genuine expertise is necessarily limited by the fact that the number ofexperts is, by definition, small and this makes the establishment of large sample sizes withstrong statistical power extremely difficult. The study of the practice histories of establishedexperts is also necessarily limited by the accuracy with which individuals can recall time spentin activities undertaken up to one or two decades earlier. The current study recognizes thesedifficulties and, while unable to eliminate them, has attempted to work within these constraintsto establish as large a cohort of expert decision-makers as it is feasible to assemble withoutcompromising on the quality criteria used to define an expert and by attempting to cross-validate the retrospective information provided by the athletes with that from other crediblesources. The discussions and conclusions are therefore based on the highest fidelity data ac-cessible to the research group to address the questions of interest. Future research should ex-tend the current findings by examining sports where expert performance occurs at youngerages (e.g., gymnastics, diving, figure skating) to investigate the importance of early special-ization in these domains. Furthermore, domains where the capacities underlying performancemay be more specialized (e.g., chess, music) should be investigated to determine whether thefindings from this study are specific to sports such as basketball, netball, and field hockey orgeneral to all domains of human endeavor.

    REFERENCESAbernethy, B. (1991). Visual search strategies and decision-making in sport. International Journal of

    Sport Psychology, 22, 189210.

  • 24 J. BAKER ET AL.

    Abernethy, B., Ct, J., & Baker, J. (2002). Expert decision making in team sport. Report to the Austra-lian Sports Commission. Brisbane, Australia: University of Queensland.

    Abernethy, B., Thomas, K. T., & Thomas, J. R. (1993). Strategies of improving understanding of motorexpertise. In J. L. Starkes & F. Allard (Eds.), Cognitive issues in motor expertise (pp. 317356).Amsterdam: Elsevier.

    Bahrick, H. P., Hall, L. K., & Berger, S. A. (1996). Accuracy and distortion in memory for high schoolgrades. Psychological Science, 7, 265271.

    Carlson, R. C. (1988). The socialization of elite tennis players in Sweden: An analysis of the playersbackgrounds and development. Sociology of Sport Journal, 5, 241256.

    Carlson, R. C. (1997). In search of the expert sport performer. Science in the Olympic Sport, 1, 113.Ct, J. (1999). The influence of the family in the development of talent in sport. The Sport Psychologist,

    13, 395417.Ct, J., Baker, J., & Abernethy, B. (in press). From play to practice: A developmental framework for the

    acquisition of expertise in team sport. In J. Starkes & K. A. Ericsson (Eds.), The development ofelite athletes: Recent advances in research on sport expertise. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Ct, J., Baker, J., & Abernethy, B. (2001) Stages of sport participation of expert decision-makers inteam ball sports. In A. Papaioannou, M. Goudas, & Y. Theodorakis (Eds.), In the dawn of the newmillenniumProgram and proceedings of the 10th world congress of sport psychology: Vol 3 (pp.150152) Thessaloniki, Hellas: Christodoulidi Publishing.

    Ct, J., Ericsson, K. A., & Beamer, M. (2002). Tracing the development of elite athletes with retrospec-tive interview methods. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Ct, J., & Hay, J. (2002). Childrens involvement in sport: A developmental perspective. In J. M. Silva& D. Stevens (Eds.), Psychological foundations of sport (pp. 484502). Boston: Merrill.

    Denzin, N. K. (1975). Play, games and interaction: The contexts of childhood socialization. The Socio-logical Quarterly, 16, 458478.

    Ericsson, K. A. (1996). The road to excellence: The acquisition of expert performance in the arts andsciences, sports and games. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Ericsson, K. A., & Charness, N. (1994). Expert performance: Its structure and acquisition. AmericanPsychologist, 49, 725747.

    Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Rmer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acqui-sition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100, 363406.

    Gustin, W. C. (1985). The development of exceptional research mathematicians. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.),Developing talent in young people (pp. 139192). New York: Ballantine.

    Hayes, J. R. (1981). The complete problem solver. Philadelphia: Franklin Institute Press.Helsen, W. F., Hodges, N. J., Van Winckel, J., & Starkes, J. L. (2000). The roles of talent, physical

    precocity and practice in the development of soccer expertise. Journal of Sports Sciences, 18, 727736.

    Helsen, W. F., Starkes, J. L., & Hodges, N. J. (1998). Team sports and the theory of deliberate practice.Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 1234.

    Hill, G. M. (1993). Youth participation of professional baseball players. Sociology of Sport Journal, 10,107114.

    Hodge, T & Deakin, J. (1998). Deliberate practice and expertise in the martial arts: The role of context inmotor recall. The Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 20, 260279.

    Hodges, N. J., & Starkes, J. L. (1996). Wrestling with the nature of expertise: A sport-specific test ofEricsson, Krampe and Tesch-Rmers (1993) theory of . International Jour-nal of Sport Psychology, 27, 400424.

    Kalinowski, A. G. (1985). The development of Olympic swimmers. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developingtalent in young people (pp. 139192). New York: Ballantine.

    Monsaas, J. A. (1985). Learning to be a world-class tennis player. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developingtalent in young people (pp. 211269). New York: Ballantine.

    Moran, G. T., & McGlynn, G. H. (1997). Cross-training for sports. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Schmidt, R. A., & Wrisberg, C. A. (2000). Motor learning and performance. Champaign, IL: Human

    Kinetics.

  • 25SPORT-SPECIFIC PRACTICE AND EXPERTISE

    Seefeldt, V. (1980). Developmental motor patterns: Implications for elementary school physical fitness.In C. H. Nadeau, W. R. Halliwell, K. M. Newell, & G. C. Roberts (Eds.), Psychology of motorbehavior and sport1979 (pp. 314323). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Seefeldt, V. (1982). Concept of readiness applied to motor skill acquisition. In R. A. Magill, M. J. Ash,& M. J. Smoll (Eds.), Children in sport (pp. 3137). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Simon, H. A., & Chase, W. G. (1973). Skill in chess. American Scientist, 61, 394403.Sosniak, L. A. (1985). Learning to be a concert pianist. In B. S. Bloom (Ed.), Developing talent in young

    people (pp. 1967). New York: Ballantine.Starkes, J. L., Deakin, J. M., Allard, F., Hodges, N. J., & Hayes., A. (1996). Deliberate practice in sports:

    What is it anyway? In K. A. Ericsson (Ed.), The road to excellence: The acquisition of expertperformance in the arts, sciences, sports and games (pp. 81106). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Sutton-Smith, B. (1997). The ambiguity of play. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Vygotsky, L. S. (Ed.). (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes.

    Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Wallingford, R. (1975). Long distance running. In A. W. Tayler & F. Landry (Eds.), The scientific

    aspects of sport training (pp. 118130). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Young B. W., & Salmela, J. H. (in press). Perceptions of training and deliberate practice of middle

    distance runners. International Journal of Sport Psychology.