27
Attributional tendencies in cultural explanations of M&A performance By: Eero Vaara, Paulina Junni, Riikka M. Sarala, Mats Ehrnrooth, Alexei Koveshnikov This is the accepted version of the following article: Vaara, E., Junni, P., Sarala, R. M., Ehrnrooth, M. and Koveshnikov, A. (2014), Attributional tendencies in cultural explanations of M&A performance. Strategic Management Journal, 35(9), 13021317. doi: 10.1002/smj.2163, which has been published in final form at http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smj.2163. ***©Wiley. Reprinted with permission. No further reproduction is authorized without written permission from Wiley. This version of the document is not the version of record. Figures and/or pictures may be missing from this format of the document. *** Abstract: This paper focuses on managers' attributions of M&A performance. Our analysis indicates that there is a linear association between performance and attributions to cultural differences, which is moderated by prior experience. Furthermore, our results suggest that there is a curvilinear association between performance and attributions to managers' actions, but we found no support for the moderating effect of experience for this association. By substantiating these attributional tendencies, our results contribute to research on M&As and studies on attribution more generally. In particular, our study helps to put cultural differences in perspective and cautions researchers and practitioners alike to avoid simplistic explanations of M&A performance. Keywords: merger | acquisition | culture | attribution | integration Article: INTRODUCTION One of the key debates in research on mergers and acquisitions (M&A) focuses on the role of cultural differences. Scholars have examined the impact of organizational cultural differences on M&A performance (Chatterjee et al., 1992; Stahl, Mendenhall, and Weber, 2005; Stahl and Voigt, 2008) and, in international settings, the impact of national cultural differences (Calori, Lubatkin, and Very, 1994; Chakrabarti, Gupta-Mukherjee, and Jayaraman, 2009; Morosini, Shane, and Singh, 1998; Reus and Lamont, 2009; Weber, Shenkar, and Raveh, 1996). Most of this research tells the same story; cultural differences tend to have a negative impact on performance. Although some of the researchers found a positive impact (Morosini et al., 1998) or argued that cultural differences may provide both sources of value creation and obstacles to integration (Björkman, Stahl, and Vaara, 2007), the fact remains that cultural differences are usually associated by researchers and practitioners alike with disappointment and failure.

Attributional tendencies in cultural explanations of M&A …libres.uncg.edu/ir/uncg/f/R_Sarala_Attributional_2014.pdf · 2016. 9. 13. · In this paper, we want to add to this discussion

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    2

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • Attributional tendencies in cultural explanations of M&A performance

    By: Eero Vaara, Paulina Junni, Riikka M. Sarala, Mats Ehrnrooth, Alexei Koveshnikov

    This is the accepted version of the following article:

    Vaara, E., Junni, P., Sarala, R. M., Ehrnrooth, M. and Koveshnikov, A. (2014), Attributional

    tendencies in cultural explanations of M&A performance. Strategic Management Journal,

    35(9), 1302–1317. doi: 10.1002/smj.2163,

    which has been published in final form at http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smj.2163.

    ***©Wiley. Reprinted with permission. No further reproduction is authorized without

    written permission from Wiley. This version of the document is not the version of record.

    Figures and/or pictures may be missing from this format of the document. ***

    Abstract:

    This paper focuses on managers' attributions of M&A performance. Our analysis indicates that

    there is a linear association between performance and attributions to cultural differences, which

    is moderated by prior experience. Furthermore, our results suggest that there is a curvilinear

    association between performance and attributions to managers' actions, but we found no support

    for the moderating effect of experience for this association. By substantiating these attributional

    tendencies, our results contribute to research on M&As and studies on attribution more

    generally. In particular, our study helps to put cultural differences in perspective and cautions

    researchers and practitioners alike to avoid simplistic explanations of M&A performance.

    Keywords: merger | acquisition | culture | attribution | integration

    Article:

    INTRODUCTION

    One of the key debates in research on mergers and acquisitions (M&A) focuses on the role of

    cultural differences. Scholars have examined the impact of organizational cultural differences on

    M&A performance (Chatterjee et al., 1992; Stahl, Mendenhall, and Weber, 2005; Stahl and

    Voigt, 2008) and, in international settings, the impact of national cultural differences (Calori,

    Lubatkin, and Very, 1994; Chakrabarti, Gupta-Mukherjee, and Jayaraman, 2009; Morosini,

    Shane, and Singh, 1998; Reus and Lamont, 2009; Weber, Shenkar, and Raveh, 1996). Most of

    this research tells the same story; cultural differences tend to have a negative impact on

    performance. Although some of the researchers found a positive impact (Morosini et al., 1998)

    or argued that cultural differences may provide both sources of value creation and obstacles to

    integration (Björkman, Stahl, and Vaara, 2007), the fact remains that cultural differences are

    usually associated by researchers and practitioners alike with disappointment and failure.

    http://libres.uncg.edu/ir/uncg/clist.aspx?id=3981http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smj.2163

  • In this paper, we want to add to this discussion by elucidating the attributional tendencies related

    to cultural differences and M&A performance. In a nutshell, we focus on the way in which

    success or failure is attributed to the actions of managers or to cultural differences. This is an

    important issue for several reasons. Cultural differences may serve as convenient targets for

    attribution—“easy explanations”—compared with other more complex causes of success. Thus,

    the focus on cultural differences alone can unduly simplify the ways in which we conceptualize

    the factors that explain success or failure (King et al., 2004). Moreover, attributions to cultural

    differences can be (mis)used as political tools for self-serving purposes. In particular, managers

    may be tempted to reduce their own responsibility for failure by “blaming” cultural differences;

    the opposite tendency can be expected in successful cases (Vaara, 2002). Methodologically, such

    attributional tendencies may create biases in research with significant implications for our

    knowledge of M&As (Stahl and Voigt, 2008; Teerikangas and Very, 2006).

    To better understand these tendencies, we draw on attribution theory (Heider, 1958;

    Kelley, 1967; Weiner, 1979). Attribution theory provides a useful theoretical basis for analysis of

    explanations of success and failure, and it has also been applied in management studies (Bettman

    and Weitz, 1983; Hayward, 2002; Hayward and Shimizu, 2006; Martinko, Harvey, and

    Dasborough, 2011; Nishii, Lepak, and Schneider, 2008). With a few exceptions (Billett and

    Qian, 2008; Vaara, 2002), research on M&As has, however, focused little attention on

    attributions. In particular, there is scant systematic evidence of the attributional tendencies

    associated with cultural differences.

    This leads us to formulate our research question as follows: Do managers' explanations of M&A

    performance reflect biases toward cultural differences and/or their own actions? In line with

    attribution theory, we first examine whether perceptions of failure lead to increasing attributions

    to cultural differences and whether perceptions of success increase attributions to management's

    actions. We then examine whether these tendencies could be curvilinear instead of linear, which

    would reflect biases in cases of both extreme success and failure. Finally, we focus on the

    question of whether the experience of previous M&As strengthens these tendencies.

    ATTRIBUTION THEORY AND HYPOTHESIS DEVELOPMENT

    The relationship between performance and causal attributions

    Attribution theory aims to understand causal explanations for specific events and phenomena

    (Heider, 1958; Kelley, 1967; Reisenzein and Rudolph, 2008; Weiner, 1979, 2008). It postulates

    that people have an ingrained need to understand and control their environments and thus try to

    develop causal explanations for significant events. The theory argues for a general tendency to

    attribute success to internal causes (people's own actions or abilities, i.e., causes controlled by

    the individual) and, correspondingly, a tendency to explain failure by reference to external

    causes (i.e., causes over which the individual has little control) (Heider, 1958; Kelley, 1971).

  • There are two explanations for such attribution biases. First, the psychological explanation states

    that organizational actors make causal attributions in order to protect their self-esteem, to

    maintain their sense of mastery over their environment, and to reduce cognitive dissonance

    (Bettman and Weitz, 1983; Staw, McKechnie, and Puffer, 1983). Second, the political

    explanation suggests that causal attributions are also utilized to enhance the esteem of actors by

    projecting a favorable self-image to others. For example, executives tend to take credit for

    corporate performance and blame outside events in the event of failure (Bettman and

    Weitz, 1983; Salancik and Meindl,1984). Similarly, board members tend to be divided on the

    basis of whether they are inside or outside board members; the former tend to attribute

    responsibility for negative performance to industry and market conditions, whereas the latter

    attribute it to top management (Schaffer, 2002).

    Management and organizational scholars have used attribution theory in various types of studies

    (Bettman and Weitz, 1983; Hayward, 2002; Hayward and Shimizu, 2006; Jordan and

    Audia, 2012; Mantere et al., 2013; Nishii et al., 2008). However, a recent review makes the point

    that this theory is still underutilized considering its potential and ability to explain important

    sociopsychological phenomena (Martinkoet al., 2011). We argue that this is especially true in

    research on M&As. While attributions have received little attention in studies of M&As, a few

    studies nevertheless provide interesting insights into attributions in general and attributions to

    cultural differences in particular. Hayward and Shimizu (2006) showed that managers are more

    likely to admit failure and divest a target unit when they can do so without incriminating

    themselves. Billett and Qian (2008) provided evidence of self-attribution biases and

    overconfidence in M&As. Their analysis suggested that if managers experienced success in prior

    M&As, they attributed it to their own ability even when it was due to chance; this made them

    overconfident, and could eventually result in negative outcomes in subsequent deals.

    Furthermore, Vaara (2002) demonstrated that managers use a number of discursive strategies to

    deal with the sociopsychological pressures related to success and/or failure; these include

    tendencies to attribute success to one's own actions and failure to cultural differences.

    Hence, although there are good reasons for expecting that the attributions by managers of

    success and failure in M&As are likely to reflect attributional biases, these tendencies need to be

    tested in a systematic way. Accordingly, we first propose that managers emphasize the

    importance of their own actions in successful cases and in turn downplay them in cases of

    failure. Our first hypothesis, therefore, states the following:

    Hypothesis 1a: M&A performance is positively associated with causal attributions to managerial

    agency.

    Second, we expect that managers attribute failure to cultural differences. We thus formulate our

    second hypothesis as follows:

  • Hypothesis 2a: M&A performance is negatively associated with causal attributions to cultural

    differences.

    While attribution research has emphasized the use of self-serving attributions to avert blame and

    to enhance self-confidence, recent studies have questioned the pervasiveness of self-serving

    attributions (Hodkins and Liebeskind, 2003; Lee and Robinson, 2000; Lee and Tiedens, 2001;

    Schlenker, Pontari, and Christopher, 2001; Tomlinson, Dineen, and Lewicki, 2004). For

    instance, in their study of managers' accounts of negative outcomes (a hypothetical salary

    freeze), Lee and Robinson (2000) found that managers made more internal causal attributions to

    factors that were under their control (subject to their own effort and behavior) than to external

    factors that were not under their control (the situation). They argued that, in an organizational

    setting, individuals have multiple motivations that impact causal attributions. On the one hand,

    individuals are motivated to avert blame and to bolster their self-esteem in cases of failure. On

    the other hand, they also wish to project a sense of power and control. Attributing failure to

    internal rather than external factors bestows individuals with a sense of control over the situation,

    and reduces feelings of helplessness (Homsma et al., 2007). The need to project a sense of being

    in control to oneself and to others rather than one of being powerless can lead managers in cases

    of failure to make internal attributions to indicate that they can take charge of the situation and

    intervene in the future to improve the negative situation (Lee and Robinson, 2000; Lee and

    Tiedens, 2001). In contrast, external self-serving attributions can make the account-givers seem

    “deceptive, self-absorbed, and ineffectual; they are viewed as unreliable social participants with

    flawed character” (Schlenker et al., 2001: 15).

    These researchers have also reflected upon whether the nature of self-serving attributions is

    dependent on positions of different status (Lee, 1997; Lee and Robinson, 2000; Lee and

    Tiedens, 2001). More specifically, for actors in high-status roles the motivation to appear

    powerful and in control can be more salient than the need to avert blame, making them more

    likely to attribute failure to internal causes (Lee, 1997; Lee and Robinson, 2000; Lee and

    Tiedens, 2001). Hence, managers, knowing that their actions will be evaluated and assessed, may

    assume greater responsibility for extremely unsuccessful acquisitions. We suggest that the

    tendency to assume responsibility for failure will be strongest in extreme cases of significant

    underperformance, because in these situations the need of managers to project that they are in

    control and can take corrective action is strongest. In this situation, self-serving (external)

    attributions, for example, to cultural differences, are also more likely to be the subject of critical

    examination (Lee and Robinson, 2000), thereby posing the risk that managers will be perceived

    as powerless or ineffectual. Hence, we propose the following curvilinearity hypotheses. These

    represent competing hypotheses for the linear Hypotheses 1a and 2a presented earlier.

    Hypothesis 1b: There will be a U-shaped curvilinear relationship between managerial

    attributions of M&A performance to managerial actions such that attributions to managerial

    agency are highest at extremely high and low levels of performance.

  • Hypothesis 2b: There will be an inverse U-shaped curvilinear relationship between managerial

    attributions of M&A performance to cultural differences such that attributions to cultural

    differences are lowest at extremely high and low levels of performance.

    The moderating impact of acquisition experience

    If and when these attributional tendencies characterize managers' explanations of success and

    failure—either linearly (Hypotheses 1a and 2a) or curvilinearly (Hypotheses 1b and 2b) as

    suggested in the above hypotheses—it is important to examine whether prior experience impacts

    these associations. Although experience can help to provide more nuanced explanations of

    success and failure and thus in principle mitigate biases, insights from attribution research

    suggest that the attributional tendencies may actually strengthen with experience as people learn

    to explain success and failure in particular ways.

    Studies indicate that attributions of success by individuals to their own ability tend to increase

    over time (Bandura, 1997; Schunk, 1994; Weiner, 1992). In particular, prior attributions of

    success to internal factors, such as skills and abilities, have been found to increase

    overconfidence (Duncan and McAuley, 1993; Schunk and Gunn, 1986; Weiner, 1992). For

    example, in educational research, it was found that students' attributions of performance tended

    to become more personally flattering and comforting as the school semester progressed (Arkin,

    Detchon, and Maruyama, 1981). Similarly, it was shown in finance that when analysts and

    managers experienced initial success, they tended to become overconfident in their subsequent

    entry and investment choices (Camerer and Lovallo, 1999; Hilary and Menzly,2006; Malmendier

    and Tate, 2005, 2008). In the M&A context, Billett and Qian (2008) found evidence indicating

    that managers with greater acquisition experience were more likely to attribute M&A success to

    internal factors, which increased their self-attribution biases and caused them to become

    overconfident. This line of reasoning is also supported by studies of learning in organization

    studies. In particular, March and Sutton (1997) argued that managers who make it to the top are

    likely to be biased about their experiences of success; this leads them to be overconfident about

    the impact of their own actions. As managers involved in M&As are usually top managers, they

    may be individuals who are especially likely to attribute success to internal causes; i.e., to

    explain successful acquisition with their own managerial action.

    Concerning external attributions, attributions of poor performance to external factors may also

    increase with experience. This is in line with attribution theory, according to which experience

    increases an individual's awareness about external factors that can potentially impact

    performance, thereby making these external factors more salient (Kelley, 1973). Accordingly,

    Smither et al. (1986) argued that as experience increases, actors develop an appreciation for the

    difficulties in the external environment and as a result make more external attributions; however,

    their results did not support this hypothesis. Mitchell and Kalb (1982) found that experience

    increased the tendency of supervisors to attribute failure to external factors (the work

    environment) because it made the impact of the work environment more salient to the

  • supervisors. Furthermore, managers who experience repeated failures are likely to become

    increasingly defensive and make more external performance attributions in order to protect their

    self-esteem and persuade themselves that they should not be blamed (Brown, 1984). Building on

    the reasoning above, it can be expected that, in cases of poor M&A performance, managers with

    greater acquisition experience will be more likely to attribute failure to external factors because

    these factors are more salient to them and because external attributions protect their personal

    self-esteem and public image. Hence, we propose that managers with experience will be even

    more likely to attribute successes to their own actions and failure to cultural differences.

    Hypothesis 3a: A positive linear association between M&A performance and causal attributions

    to managerial agency will be stronger in acquisitions where the managers have greater prior

    experience.

    Hypothesis 4a: A negative linear association between M&A performance and causal attributions

    to cultural differences will be stronger in acquisitions where the managers have greater prior

    experience.

    The above argumentation assumes a linear relationship between M&A performance and

    attribution effects, in line with Hypotheses 1a and 2a. If the relationship is curvilinear, as

    suggested in Hypotheses 1b and 2b, we would expect experience to moderate the curvilinear

    relationship. We would still predict that, at a high level of performance, experience increases

    attribution to managerial action and decreases attribution to cultural differences because

    managers may become overconfident about their own influence (Billett and Qian,2008;

    Malmendier and Tate, 2005, 2008). However, in contrast to the linear hypotheses, at a low level

    of performance a curvilinear relationship would imply that attributions to manager's actions are

    high and attributions to cultural differences are low as managers take responsibility for cases of

    obvious failure in order to project a sense that they are responsible and in control (Lee, 1997; Lee

    and Robinson,2000; Lee and Tiedens, 2001). Prior research has not explicitly addressed this

    issue, but experience could accentuate this tendency as managers learn that they cannot escape

    taking responsibility for obvious failure. Accordingly, we present the following hypotheses for

    the possible moderating effect of experience in the case of the curvilinear relationship.

    Hypothesis 3b: A positive curvilinear association between M&A performance and causal

    attribution to managerial agency will be stronger in acquisitions where the managers have

    greater prior experience.

    Hypothesis 4b: A negative curvilinear association between M&A performance and causal

    attribution to cultural differences will be stronger in acquisitions where the managers have

    greater prior experience.

    METHODOLOGY

    Sample and data collection

  • Our sample consists of Finnish acquirers and includes domestic and cross-border mergers based

    on a database maintained by the Finnish business magazine Talouselämä between 2001 and

    2004. Mergers were included only if the acquiring firm gained a controlling interest in the

    acquired firm and the acquired firm was valued at EUR 3 million or more. In order to obtain high

    quality responses, acquiring firm CEOs were contacted via telephone and asked to identify

    potential respondents who were involved at the time in the acquisition and were knowledgeable

    about it. Then a survey was mailed to the CEOs, who distributed it to the respondents that had

    been identified in the acquiring and acquired firms. The respondents included CEOs (42.7%), top

    managers (42.7%), and other members of the management group and board members (14.6%).

    The overall response rate was 20 percent, yielding a sample of 92 mergers (51 domestic and 41

    cross-border acquisitions in 22 countries). The cross-border acquisitions included the following

    countries: Austria, Belgium, Canada, China, the Czech Republic, Estonia, France, Germany,

    Great Britain, Hong Kong, Italy, Latvia, Lithuania, Norway, the Netherlands, Poland, Russia,

    Spain, Sweden, Switzerland, and the United States. Ten firms returned responses from multiple

    respondents. Based on the cases from which we received multiple answers, the interrater

    reliability was checked, yielding significant intraclass correlation scores for most cases

    (p 

  • independent variables. Because these relationships are unlikely to be part of the respondents'

    mental models, concerns regarding the existence of common method bias should be alleviated

    (Chang, Witteloostuijn, and Eden, 2010).

    Measures

    Independent variable

    Acquisition performance

    This variable consisted of four items that measured the outcome of the acquisition and the

    integration process. In line with prior studies of M&As, we used managerial evaluations of

    acquisition performance (Birkinshaw et al., 2000; Datta, 1991; Very, Lubatkin, and Calori, 1996;

    Very et al., 1997). This approach is in line with the essence of attribution research where

    perceptions rather than, for example, “objective” assessments are the core of attributional

    analyses (Heider, 1958; Weiner, 2008). First, respondents were asked to indicate how well the

    (1) acquisition, and (2) integration process had succeeded. Second, respondents were asked how

    the (1) acquisition, and (2) integration process had succeeded compared with expectations. The

    scale ranged from 1 = total failure to 7 = great success.

    To examine any consistent bias between objective and subjective performance measures, we

    collected objective performance data. Financial statements were available for 43 publicly listed

    companies, which represented 47.8 percent of our sample. Objective performance, measured as

    the acquirer's ROI after the acquisition (in 2005), correlated positively and significantly with our

    subjective acquisition performance measure. Also, the objective performance measure was

    significantly correlated in the expected direction (negatively) with the measure “attribution to

    cultural differences” and (positively) with “attribution to managers” actions' (Hypotheses 1a and

    2a). This provides further validity for our subjective performance measure.

    Moderating variable

    Personal acquisition experience

    We measured personal acquisition experience by combining the number of acquisitions in which

    the respondent had been personally involved (1) on the acquiring firm side, and (2) on the

    acquired firm side. This sum index represented the person's combined prior experience of

    acquisitions.

    Dependent variables

    Attribution to managers' actions

    We followed the example of previous studies in measuring attributions based on the respondents'

    perceptions (De Michele et al., 1998; Duval and Silvia, 2002; Greenlees et al., 2007; Harvey and

    Martinko, 2009; Schaffer, 2002). Regarding attribution to managers' actions, we therefore asked

  • respondents to rate their perceptions based on the extent to which management's actions (1) had

    affected the outcome of the integration process, and (2) explained the overall success of the

    acquisition. The scale ranged from 1 = not at all to 7 = a great deal.

    Attribution to cultural differences

    Following this logic, we asked respondents about the extent to which they perceived that cultural

    differences (1) had affected the outcome of the acquisition's integration process, and (2)

    explained the overall success of the acquisition. The scale ranged from 1 = not at all to 7 = a

    great deal.

    Control variables

    Organizational cultural differences

    To control for the effect of actual cultural differences on the attributions, we included both

    organizational and national cultural differences in our models. Following the example of

    previous studies (Chatterjee et al., 1992; Weber et al., 1996), we asked managers to report

    organizational cultural differences prior to the acquisition in the following areas: management

    and control, sales and marketing, production, research and development, and company values in

    general. The scale ranged from 1 = no differences to 7 = significant differences.

    National cultural differences

    We controlled for national cultural differences by building a construct of the variance-adjusted

    sum of differences between the two acquisition parties (Kogut and Singh, 1988) based on the

    nine dimensions of the GLOBE practices scores (House et al., 2004).1

    Degree of integration

    To control for the impact of managers' actions in postacquisition integration (Weber et al., 1996),

    we asked respondents to assess the degree of integration between the acquirer and the target in

    the following functions: management and control, sales and marketing, production, research and

    development. The scale ranged from 1 = no integration to 7 = total integration.

    Respondent involvement

    The participation of a respondent in the acquisition and integration decisions may bias his/her

    opinion of the acquisition outcome and the factors that contributed to it (Billett and Qian, 2008).

    We controlled for this by measuring the respondent's involvement in the decision making leading

    to the acquisition and in the integration of the companies. The scale ranged from 1 = not at all to

    7 = yes, as a central decision maker.

    Acquisition size

  • We included the size of the acquisition as a control variable in line with prior acquisition studies

    (Haleblian and Finkelstein, 1999; Morosiniet al., 1998). Acquisition size was measured as the

    target's net sales at the time it was acquired and was reported in the business magazine

    Talouselämä.

    Time elapsed

    Acquisition dynamics can be influenced by the time elapsed since the acquisition. Following the

    approach by Very et al. (1997), we measured the number of years that had passed after the

    acquisition (one to four years). This external measure was based on the information in the

    business magazine Talouselämä.

    RESULTS

    Pretests of the questions with professors and managers supported the face-validity of the

    constructs. To further evaluate the reliability and validity of our items and constructs, we used

    confirmatory factor analysis with partial least squares (PLS) analysis, which is a structural

    equation modeling approach particularly applicable for smaller sample sizes. We followed the

    instructions of Shook et al. (2004) for evaluating the results of the confirmatory factor analysis.

    First, we examined the variable loadings, their t-values and corresponding significance levels (p-

    values) and verified that all of them were significant (see Table 1). Then, we examined the

    reliability of the constructs. Cronbach's alphas all exceeded the commonly used threshold of 0.7.

    However, Cronbach's alphas have several limitations. Hence, following the recommendation of

    Shook et al. (2004), we calculated the composite reliabilities for each construct, all of which

    were above the limit of 0.7, with each indicator reliability above 0.5. To establish convergent

    validity, we calculated the average variance extracted, which exceeded 50 percent (Shook et

    al., 2004). The convergent validity was also supported by examination of an item-to-item

    correlation table that showed that the items correlated highest with other items belonging to the

    same construct. Discriminant validity was assured by calculating the shared variance between

    each pair of constructs and confirming that it was lower than the square root of the average

    variance extracted for each individual construct (Shook et al., 2004).

    Table 1. Item factor loadings, indicator reliability, construct validity, and reliability

    Construct Measurement

    item

    Factor

    loading

    Indicator

    reliability

    t-value Composite

    reliability

    Average

    variance

    Cronbach's

    alpha

    Acquisition

    performance

    Acquisition

    outcome

    0.868 0.753 20.779*** 0.952 0.831 0.933

    Integration

    outcome

    0.918 0.843 49.319***

    Success of the

    acquisition

    0.920 0.846 40.796***

  • compared with

    expectations

    Success of the

    integration

    compared with

    expectations

    0.940 0.883 55.336***

    Attribution to

    managers'

    actions

    Extent to which

    managements'

    actions affect

    integration

    0.928 0.861 23.315*** 0.925 0.861 0.839

    Extent to which

    managements'

    actions explain

    acquisition

    success

    0.928 0.861 26.958***

    Attribution to

    cultural

    differences

    Extent to which

    cultural

    differences affect

    integration

    0.929 0.863 14.826*** 0.885 0.794 0.747

    Extent to which

    cultural

    differences

    explain

    acquisition

    success

    0.852 0.726 7.700***

    Organizational

    cultural

    differences

    Management and

    control

    0.861 0.742 4.314*** 0.894 0.629 0.859

    Sales and

    marketing

    0.808 0.652 4.195***

    Production 0.741 0.549 3.435***

    Research and

    development

    0.737 0.543 3.050***

    Company values

    in general

    0.811 0.658 4.690***

    Degree of

    integration

    Management and

    control

    0.819 0.671 9.738*** 0.938 0.792 0.912

    Sales and 0.889 0.790 10.314***

  • marketing

    Production 0.925 0.855 10.888***

    Research and

    development

    0.922 0.849 12.373***

    Participation Preacquisition

    stage

    0.882 0.778 8.942*** 0.890 0.802 0.755

    Postacquisition

    stage

    0.909 0.826 15.458***

    *** p 

  • integra

    tion

    6.

    Organi

    zation

    al

    cultura

    l

    differe

    nces

    1.

    00

    0

    7.00

    0

    4.8

    17

    0.1

    43

    −0.

    088

    −0.

    10

    0

    0.1

    61

    −0.1

    23

    −0.0

    15

    1

    7.

    Nation

    al

    cultura

    l

    differe

    nces

    0.

    00

    0

    18.8

    53

    2.8

    13

    0.5

    26

    0.1

    70

    0.0

    69

    −0.

    181

    0.04

    1

    −0.0

    99

    −0.

    031

    1

    8.

    Perfor

    mance

    1.

    00

    0

    7.00

    0

    5.2

    24

    0.1

    36

    −0.

    110

    −0.

    00

    5

    −0.

    006

    0.23

    2*

    0.34

    9**

    −0.

    267

    *

    −0.

    08

    1

    1

    9.

    Attrib

    ution

    to

    manag

    ers'

    actions

    1.

    00

    0

    7.00

    0

    3.7

    14

    0.1

    48

    0.0

    22

    −0.

    09

    3

    0.2

    07

    −0.0

    28

    −0.0

    85

    0.1

    81

    −0.

    15

    2

    −0.3

    15**

    1

    10.

    Attrib

    ution

    to

    cultura

    l

    differe

    nces

    3.

    00

    0

    7.00

    0

    5.6

    10

    0.0

    97

    −0.

    228

    *

    0.0

    11

    0.2

    74*

    *

    0.01

    4

    0.26

    1*

    0.1

    20

    −0.

    16

    9

    0.29

    2**

    0.0

    65

    1

    11.

    Square

    d

    perfor

    mance

    1.

    00

    0

    49.0

    00

    28.

    878

    1.2

    84

    −0.

    109

    −0.

    04

    7

    0.0

    87

    0.03

    9

    −0.2

    70*

    *

    0.1

    00

    −0.

    04

    4

    −0.5

    51**

    *

    0.1

    47

    0.1

    36

    1

    12.

    Perfor

    mance

    0.

    00

    490.

    000

    38.

    702

    8.7

    95

    −0.

    032

    0.1

    13

    0.0

    81

    0.86

    3**

    −0.0

    41

    −0.

    093

    −0.

    01

    0.03

    1

    −0.

    05

    −0.

    01

    0.1

    79

    1

  • ×

    acquisi

    tion

    experi

    ence

    0 * 8 3 4

    13.

    Square

    d

    perfor

    mance

    ×

    acquisi

    tion

    experi

    ence

    0.

    00

    0

    343

    0.00

    0

    237

    .58

    4

    60.

    34

    0

    0.0

    15

    0.0

    87

    −0.

    005

    0.89

    3**

    *

    0.02

    9

    −0.

    207

    0.0

    25

    0.32

    1**

    −0.

    07

    2

    −0.

    01

    8

    −0.

    08

    3

    0.86

    3**

    *

    1

    All two-tailed tests. N = 90, missing values were replaced with mean. Pearson's bivariate

    correlations in the table represent standardized beta coefficients. *p 

  • Curvilinear model:

    attribution to

    managers' actions

    Curvilinear model:

    attribution to cultural

    differences

    Curvilinear

    moderation:

    attribution to

    managers' actions

    Linear moderation:

    attribution to cultural

    differences

    Variabl

    es

    Beta t Si

    g.

    VI

    F

    Beta t Si

    g.

    VI

    F

    Beta t Si

    g.

    VI

    F

    Beta t Si

    g.

    VI

    F

    Acquisit

    ion size

    −0.0

    47

    −0.

    563

    0.5

    75

    1.1

    16

    0.01

    8

    0.1

    90

    0.8

    49

    1.1

    16

    −0.0

    49

    −0.

    582

    0.5

    62

    1.1

    18

    0.00

    5

    0.0

    58

    0.9

    54

    1.0

    86

    Time

    elapsed

    0.07

    0

    0.8

    62

    0.3

    91

    1.0

    61

    −0.0

    79

    −0.

    862

    0.3

    91

    1.0

    61

    0.06

    3

    0.7

    60

    0.4

    50

    1.0

    97

    −0.0

    92

    −1.

    025

    0.3

    08

    1.0

    61

    Respon

    dent

    involve

    ment

    0.18

    9*

    2.0

    40

    0.0

    45

    1.0

    87

    0.16

    9

    1.6

    16

    0.1

    10

    1.0

    87

    0.18

    5

    1.9

    83

    0.0

    51

    1.0

    95

    0.17

    2

    1.6

    97

    0.0

    94

    1.0

    84

    Acquisit

    ion

    experien

    ce

    −0.1

    26

    −1.

    297

    0.1

    98

    1.1

    78

    0.08

    4

    0.7

    70

    0.4

    44

    1.1

    78

    −0.0

    35

    −0.

    162

    0.8

    72

    5.8

    21

    0.50

    2*

    2.2

    46

    0.0

    27

    5.1

    57

    Degree

    of

    integrati

    on

    0.16

    4

    1.6

    44

    0.1

    04

    1.1

    91

    0.00

    0

    −0.

    001

    0.9

    99

    1.1

    91

    0.16

    6

    1.6

    52

    0.1

    03

    1.1

    93

    0.03

    1

    0.2

    78

    0.7

    82

    1.1

    92

    Organiz

    ational

    cultural

    differen

    ces

    0.20

    4

    1.8

    84

    0.0

    63

    1.1

    48

    0.06

    7

    0.5

    50

    0.5

    84

    1.1

    48

    0.19

    4

    1.7

    56

    0.0

    83

    1.1

    88

    0.04

    6

    0.3

    89

    0.6

    98

    1.1

    53

    National

    cultural

    differen

    ces

    −0.0

    57

    −0.

    428

    0.6

    69

    1.0

    80

    −0.2

    21

    −1.

    479

    0.1

    43

    1.0

    80

    −0.0

    58

    −0.

    439

    0.6

    62

    1.0

    81

    −0.2

    48

    −1.

    697

    0.0

    94

    1.0

    86

    Perform

    ance

    0.54

    7***

    4.4

    29

    0.0

    00

    1.9

    50

    −0.3

    77**

    −2.

    700

    0.0

    08

    1.9

    50

    0.55

    1***

    4.4

    26

    0.0

    00

    1.9

    56

    −0.4

    25**

    −3.

    464

    0.0

    01

    1.5

    82

    Squared

    perform

    ance

    0.29

    9***

    3.9

    38

    0.0

    00

    1.6

    36

    −0.0

    64

    −0.

    745

    0.4

    59

    1.6

    36

    0.29

    3***

    3.7

    83

    0.0

    00

    1.6

    83

    Squared

    perform

    ance ×

    acquisiti

    −0.0

    70

    −0.

    469

    0.6

    40

    5.7

    94

  • on

    experien

    ce

    Perform

    ance ×

    acquisiti

    on

    experien

    ce

    −0.4

    25*

    −2.

    203

    0.0

    30

    4.7

    64

    R2 0.35

    7

    0.18

    2

    0.35

    9

    0.22

    4

    Adjuste

    d R2

    0.28

    5

    0.09

    0

    0.27

    8

    0.13

    6

    R2 chan

    ge

    0.12

    5

    0.00

    6

    0.00

    2

    0.04

    7

    F 4.94

    1***

    1.40

    9

    4.42

    5***

    2.55

    9*

    All two-tailed tests. N = 90, missing values were replaced with mean. Data in the table represent

    standardized beta coefficients. Dependent variables: attribution to managers' actions, attribution

    to cultural differences. The results are robust to nested models. See the online supporting

    information for the results of the simpler models. *p 

  • To test Hypothesis 4a, we added the interaction term performance × acquisition experience and

    examined its relationship with attribution to cultural differences. The significant improvement in

    the new model (“Linear Moderation” in Table 3) compared with the initial linear model (see

    Table S1) (ΔR2 = 0.047, p 

  • performance and attributions to managers' actions. In other words, managers tend to attribute

    extreme cases of both success and failure to their own actions. Thus, managers are likely to take

    credit for success, which may among other things lead to an illusion of control or to

    overconfidence. Such an illusion of control can be dangerous when managers confront the

    complex challenges of M&A processes (Haspeslagh and Jemison, 1991) and may even partially

    explain the willingness to engage in risky M&As and “merger frenzy” (Billett and Qian, 2008).

    However, our curvilinear findings also suggest that managers do attribute extreme cases of

    failure to their own actions. In such cases, managers may need to project a sense of control both

    to themselves and others (Schlenker et al., 2001). The strength of managerial attributions was,

    however, unaffected by experience.

    By highlighting these attributional tendencies, our analysis has implications for the discussion

    concerning the conceptualization and measurement of cultural differences (Ailon, 2008; Berry,

    Guillén, and Zhou, 2010; Boyacigiller et al., 1996; Harzing, 2003; Shenkar, 2001,2012a,b). On

    the one hand, our results point toward the importance of using and developing more objective

    and comprehensive measures of cultural differences. For example, researchers can use

    multisource and multilevel research designs where (prior) organizational cultures are measured

    by groups of lower-level employees previously employed by the independent organizations. In

    this case, employees' perceptions would be less likely to be biased due to factors related to their

    prestige and/or self-esteem. Cultural differences could then be operationalized as differences

    between the perceptions of these groups. The above could be complemented with analyzing

    differences in perceptions between lower-level employees and higher-level managers. On the

    other hand, our results caution researchers not to rely overly on any static measure of cultural

    differences. In the worst case, the result may be myopia or, in other words, systematic

    overestimation of the impact of culture on different organizational phenomena such as M&A,

    FDI, and entry mode choice (Harzing, 2003). In light of this debate, our analysis provides

    additional grounds for developing new kinds of conceptualizations of cultural dynamics. These

    can include quantitative analyses of convergence or crossvergence in cultural differences (Sarala

    and Vaara, 2010) or qualitative analyses of cultural identity formation (Clark et al., 2010;

    Maguire and Phillips, 2008; Riad and Vaara, 2011; Vaara and Tienari, 2011).

    Our findings also contribute to research on attributions more generally. Applying insights from

    attribution theory to M&As is important per se—and consistent with the recent reviews of

    attribution theory in management studies (Martinko et al., 2011). Furthermore, our findings

    concerning the role of experience and curvilinear relationships may have broader implications

    for research on attributions. While studies of attributions indicate that experience may strengthen

    attributional tendencies, systematic analyses have been scarce. One interpretation of the results is

    that experience may indeed imply learning of a specific kind, i.e., learning to use particular—in

    our case, cultural—language to explain failure. It would be interesting to examine such

    phenomena more closely in future studies; this could include analysis of whether and how

    previous experiences of success or failure make a difference for future attributions. Our findings

  • concerning curvilinearity can in turn complement recent studies suggesting a more complex and

    nuanced understanding of self-serving attributions (Hodkins and Liebeskind, 2003; Lee and

    Robinson, 2000; Lee and Tiedens, 2001; Schlenker et al., 2001; Tomlinson et al., 2004). The

    curvilinear nature of the association of performance and attributions with managers' actions may

    provide a new piece of the puzzle in attribution research—or at least pave the way for new

    studies examining and testing such curvilinearity. Based on our findings, it seems that managers

    may need to project a sense of control both to themselves and others, which requires assuming

    not only credit for success but also responsibility for clear cases of failure.

    Our findings also have practical implications. Practitioners should beware of the attributional

    tendencies that seem to characterize M&As. In particular, there is a need to pay attention to self-

    serving attributions and the resulting illusion of control that could lead to overly risky deals and

    create problems in the management of the integration process. Special attention should be

    focused on how managers may overemphasize the role of cultural differences and even

    deliberately blame cultural differences for failure. At the same time, other causes of integration

    problems might pass unnoticed and be left unaddressed. Furthermore, the “negative”

    connotations of cultural differences may cause the management to overlook the potential value

    embedded in cultural differences (Björkman et al., 2007; Morosini et al., 1998; Reus and

    Lamont. 2009; Stahl and Voigt, 2008) or even shy away from potentially attractive acquisitions

    in the presence of apparent cultural differences.

    Boundary conditions and limitations

    The boundary conditions of our study should be taken seriously when interpreting these findings.

    Our analysis is based on a sample of Finnish companies' acquisitions made during a specific

    period. Thus, our results may be influenced by the characteristics of Finnish firms and this time

    period. For example, the Finnish economy has been historically driven by the pulp and paper and

    metal sectors. It could be that attribution effects are more salient in these types of “traditional”

    industries where Finns consider themselves to be proficient and have a national heritage. While

    we tested our model for some of the peculiarities of the Finnish context (e.g., main traditional

    industries) and found our results to be unaffected, it would be important to examine attributional

    tendencies in other national and cultural contexts (Morris and Peng, 1994). Furthermore, it may

    be that the specific time period emphasizes tendencies that could be different in other

    circumstances. Thus, it would be interesting to compare attributional tendencies for example in

    times of boom and bust.

    Our study is based on top managers' perceptions. While it is important to examine key decision

    makers' interpretations, it is possible that employees, managers of other companies, experts such

    as consultants or financial analysts, or the media might manifest other tendencies. An analysis

    and comparison of various groups' attributional tendencies would be a major issue for future

    research. Such studies could also go further in analysis of agreement and disagreement as well as

    criticality.

  • Our analysis is largely, but not entirely, based on survey data. Although, for example, the

    national cultural difference measures were based on external data, our results may involve a risk

    of common method variance. However, the fact that our tests did not indicate any such bias

    should alleviate this concern. Nevertheless, it would be interesting to compare these results

    against an analysis where some of the measures were operationalized differently or drawn from

    other types of data.

    We relied on cross-sectional, perceptual measures, and thus cannot establish causal direction per

    se, which means that the risk of reverse causality must be kept in mind when interpreting our

    findings. In particular, it is important to focus attention on three key issues related to the role of

    cultural differences in our models. First, one could argue that managers' assessments of cultural

    differences might be affected by the attributional tendencies. The fact that our models include

    not only a measure of organizational cultural differences (based on the managers' own

    assessments) but also national cultural differences (based on external data) should alleviate this

    concern (see also the discussion about common method variance above). Second, one could raise

    the question of whether the results would reflect the actual impact of cultural differences on

    M&A performance; for instance, larger cultural differences would be reflected in poorer

    performance and thus in attributions to cultural differences. To deal with these concerns, we

    systematically controlled for the impact of cultural differences (both organizational and national)

    on the attributional tendencies in all our models. Third, our cross-sectional analysis cannot per se

    establish whether performance affects attributional tendencies or vice versa. In our case, the

    assessments of performance could be influenced by the attributions to cultural differences. The

    fact that the financial performance measure (gathered on the basis of available data) correlated

    significantly and positively with the subjective assessments should at least partially alleviate this

    concern. Moreover, it is usually assumed in attribution research that people first make sense of

    success/failure to be able to then construct explanations (attributions). We think that this is also

    likely to be the usual process in the case of our analysis, especially because the managers first

    responded to questions about performance and only after that to the questions measuring

    attributions. Nevertheless, the exact process of making sense of performance and attributions

    may be more complicated than usually assumed; it may, for instance, be the case that managers

    in “normal circumstances” frame success/failure and develop explanations for it (attributions) in

    a process that involves the mutual reconstruction of both aspects of the association. Thus, we

    underscore the need for future studies using other types of research designs and performance and

    attributional measures to verify our propositions and elaborate on our findings.

    In all, the results of our analysis should be taken seriously by researchers and practitioners alike

    when making sense of performance in M&A and other contexts. While the specific features of

    our sample and other limitations of our study need to be taken into consideration, our analysis

    does indicate that attributional tendencies are likely to play an important role when explaining

    performance, with significant theoretical, methodological, and practical implications. Our study

    has provided intriguing results, but it also gives rise to a number of fascinating new questions

  • and issues that warrant attention in future research on attributions in the M&A and other

    contexts.

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    Eero Vaara is the lead author of this paper; Paulina Junni and Riikka Sarala are listed in

    alphabetical order; Mats Ehrnrooth and Alexei Koveshnikov are also listed in alphabetical order.

    We want to thank Yaakov Weber for his insights when working on an earlier paper on the same

    topic and David Miller for the language review. We also want to express our gratitude to Will

    Mitchell for excellent guidance and helping to develop our initial ideas into a fully fledged

    argument and our reviewers for insightful and constructive comments.

    REFERENCES

    Ailon G. 2008. Mirror, mirror on the wall: culture's consequences in a value test of its own

    design. Academy of Management Review33(4): 885–904.

    Arkin RM, Detchon CS, Maruyama GM. 1981. Causal attributions of high and low achievement

    motivation college students for performance on examinations. Motivation and

    Emotion 5(2): 139–151.

    Bandura A. 1997. Self-Efficacy: The Exercise of Control. Freeman: New York.

    Barnett ML, Salomon RM. 2006. Beyond dichotomy: the curvilinear relationship between social

    responsibility and financial performance. Strategic Management Journal 27(11): 1101–1122.

    Berry H, Guillén MF, Zhou N. 2010. An institutional approach to cross-national

    distance. Journal of International Business Studies41(9): 1460–1480.

    Bettman JR, Weitz BA. 1983. Attributions in the board room: causal reasoning in corporate

    annual reports. Administrative Science Quarterly 28(2): 165–183.

    Billett MT, Qian Y. 2008. Are overconfident managers born or made? Evidence of self-

    attribution bias from frequent acquirers.Management Science 54(6): 1037–1051.

    Birkinshaw J, Bresman H, Håkanson L. 2000. Managing the post-acquisition integration process:

    how the human integration and task integration processes interact to foster value

    creation. Journal of Management Studies 37(3): 395–425.

    Björkman I, Stahl G, Vaara E. 2007. Cultural differences and capability transfer in cross-border

    acquisitions: the mediating roles of capability complementarity, absorptive capacity, and social

    integration. Journal of International Business Studies 38(4): 658–672.

  • Boyacigiller N, Kleinberg MJ, Philips M, Sackmann S. 1996. Conceptualizing culture.

    In Handbook for International Management Research, Punnett BJ, Shenkar O (eds).

    Blackwell: Cambridge, MS; 157–208.

    Brown KA. 1984. Explaining group poor performance: an attributional analysis. Academy of

    Management Review 9(1): 54–63.

    Calori R, Lubatkin M, Very P. 1994. Control mechanisms in cross-border acquisitions: an

    international comparison. Organization Studies 15(3): 361–379.

    Camerer C, Lovallo D. 1999. Overconfidence and excess entry: an experimental

    approach. American Economic Review 89(1):306–318.

    Chakrabarti R, Gupta-Mukherjee S, Jayaraman N. 2009. Mars-venus marriages: culture and

    cross-border M&A. Journal of International Business Studies 40(2): 216–236.

    Chang SJ, Witteloostuijn AV, Eden L. 2010. Common method variance in international business

    research. Journal of International Business Studies 41(2): 178–184.

    Chatterjee S, Lubatkin MH, Schweiger DM, Weber Y. 1992. Cultural differences and

    shareholder value in related mergers: linking equity and human capital. Strategic Management

    Journal 13(5): 319–334.

    Clark SM, Gioia DA, Ketchen J, Thomas JB. 2010. Transitional identity as a facilitator of

    organizational identity change during a merger. Administrative Science Quarterly 55(3): 397–

    438.

    Datta DK. 1991. Organizational fit and acquisition performance: effects of post-acquisition

    integration. Strategic Management Journal12(4): 281–297.

    De Michele P, Gansneder B, Solomon G. 1998. Success and failure attributions of wrestlers:

    further evidence of the self-serving bias.Journal of Sport Behavior 21(3): 242–255.

    Duncan TE, McAuley E. 1993. Social support and efficacy cognitions in exercise adherence: a

    latent growth curve analysis. Journal of Behavioral Medicine 16(2): 199–218.

    Dundas KNM, Richardson PR. 1982. Implementing the unrelated product strategy. Strategic

    Management Journal 3(4): 287–301.

    Duval TS, Silvia PJ. 2002. Self-awareness, probability of improvement, and the self-serving

    bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82(1): 49–61.

    Greenlees I, Stopforth M, Graydon J, Thelwell R, Filby W, El-Hakim Y. 2007. The impact of

    match importance and gender on the team-serving attributional bias among interdependent sports

    team players. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice 11(1):54–65.

  • Haleblian J, Finkelstein S. 1999. The influence of organizational acquisition experience on

    acquisition performance: a behavioral learning perspective. Administrative Science

    Quarterly 44(1): 29–56.

    Harvey P, Martinko MJ. 2009. An empirical examination of the role of attributions in

    psychological entitlement and its outcomes.Journal of Organizational Behavior 30(4): 459–476.

    Harzing A-W. 2003. The role of culture in entry-mode studies: from neglect to

    myopia? In Managing Multinationals in a Knowledge Economy: Economics, Culture and Human

    Resources, Cheng JLC, Hitt MA (eds). Elsevier: Amsterdam; 75–127.

    Haspeslagh PC, Jemison DB. 1991. Managing Acquisitions. Free Press: New York.

    Hayward ML. 2002. When do firms learn from their acquisition experience? Evidence from

    1990–1995. Strategic Management Journal 23(1): 21–39.

    Hayward ML, Shimizu K. 2006. De-commitment to losing strategic action: evidence from the

    divestiture of poorly performing acquisitions. Strategic Management Journal 27(6): 541–557.

    Heider F. 1958. The Psychology of Interpersonal Relations. Wiley: New York.

    Hilary G, Menzly L. 2006. Does past success lead analysts to become

    overconfident? Management Science 52(4): 489–500.

    Hodkins HS, Liebeskind E. 2003. Apology versus defense: antecedents and

    consequences. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 39(4): 297–316.

    Hofstede GH. 1991. Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind. McGraw-Hill: London,

    UK.

    Homsma GJ, Van Dyck C, De Gilder D, Koopman PL, Elfring T. 2007. Overcoming errors: a

    closer look at the attributional mechanism.Journal of Business and Psychology 21(4): 559–583.

    House RJ, Hanges PJ, Javidan M, Dorfman PW, Gupta V. 2004. Culture, Leadership, and

    Organizations. Sage Publications:Thousand Oaks, CA.

    Jordan AH, Audia P. 2012. Self-Enhancement and learning from performance

    feedback. Academy of Management Review 37(2):211–231.

    Kelley HH. 1967. Attribution theory in social psychology. In Nebraska Symposium on

    Motivation, Levine D (ed). University of Nebraska Press: Lincoln, UK; 192–238.

    Kelley HH. 1971. Attribution in Social Interactions. General Learning Press: New York.

    Kelley HH. 1973. The process of causal attribution. American Psychologist 28(2): 107–128.

  • King DR, Dalton DR, Daily CM, Covin JG. 2004. Meta-analyses of post-acquisition

    performance: indications of unidentified moderators. Strategic Management Journal 25(2): 187–

    200.

    Kogut B, Singh H. 1988. The effect of national culture on the choice of entry mode. Journal of

    International Business Studies 19(3):411–432.

    Lee F. 1997. When the going gets tough, do the tough ask for help? Help seeking and power

    motivation in organizations.Organizational Behavior and Human Decision

    Processes 72(3): 336–363.

    Lee F, Robinson R. 2000. An attributional analysis of social accounts: implications of playing

    the blame game. Journal of Applied Social Psychology 30(9): 1853–1879.

    Lee F, Tiedens L. 2001. Who's being served? “Self-serving” attributions in social

    hierarchies. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes 84(2): 254–287.

    Maguire S, Phillips N. 2008. ‘Citibankers’ at citigroup: a study of the loss of institutional trust

    after a merger. Journal of Management Studies 45(2): 372–401.

    Malmendier U, Tate G. 2005. CEO overconfidence and corporate investment. Journal of

    Finance 60(6): 2661–2700.

    Malmendier U, Tate G. 2008. Who makes acquisitions? CEO overconfidence and the market's

    reaction. Journal of Financial Economics 89(1): 20–43.

    Mantere S, Aula P, Schildt H, Vaara E. 2013. Narrative attributions of entrepreneurial

    failure. Journal of Business Venturing 28(4):459–473.

    March JG, Sutton RI. 1997. Organizational performance as a dependent variable. Organization

    Science 8(6): 698–706.

    Martinko MJ, Harvey P, Dasborough MT. 2011. Attribution theory in the organizational

    sciences: a case of unrealized potential.Journal of Organizational Behaviour 32(1): 144–149.

    Mitchell TR, Kalb LS. 1982. Effects of job experience on supervisor attributions for a

    subordinate's poor performance. Journal of Applied Psychology 67(2): 181–188.

    Morosini P, Shane S, Singh H. 1998. National cultural distance and cross-border acquisition

    performance. Journal of International Business Studies 29(1): 137–158.

    Morris MW, Peng K. 1994. Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and

    physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 67(6): 949–971.

  • Nishii LH, Lepak DP, Schneider B. 2008. Employee attributions of the “why” of HR practices:

    their effects on employee attitudes and behaviors, and customer satisfaction. Personnel

    Psychology 61(3): 503–545.

    Podsakoff PM, MacKenzie SB, Lee J-Y, Podsakoff NP. 2003. Common method biases in

    behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. Journal of

    Applied Psychology 88(5): 879–903.

    Reisenzein R, Rudolph U. 2008. The discovery of common-sense psychology. Social

    Psychology 39(3): 125–133.

    Reus TH, Lamont BT. 2009. The double-edged sword of cultural distance in international

    acquisitions. Journal of International Business Studies 40: 1298–1316.

    Riad S, Vaara E. 2011. Varieties of metonymy in media accounts of mergers and

    acquisitions. Journal of Management Studies 48(4):737–771.

    Salancik GR, Meindl JR. 1984. Corporate attributions as strategic illusions of management

    control. Administrative Science Quarterly29(2): 238–254.

    Sarala RM, Vaara E. 2010. Cultural differences, convergence, and crossvergence as explanations

    of knowledge transfer in international acquisitions. Journal of International Business

    Studies 41(8): 1365–1390.

    Schaffer BS. 2002. Board assessments of managerial performance: an analysis of attribution

    processes. Journal of Managerial Psychology 17(2): 95–115.

    Schlenker BR, Pontari BA, Christopher AN. 2001. Excuses and character: personal and social

    implications of excuses. Personality and Social Psychology Review 5(15): 15–32.

    Schunk DH. 1994. Self-regulation of self-efficacy and attributions in academic settings. In Self-

    Regulation of Learning and Performance: Issues and Educational Implications, Schunk

    DH, Zimmerman BJ (eds). Erlbaum: Hillsdale, NJ; 75–99.

    Schunk DH, Gunn TP. 1986. Self-efficacy and skill development: influence of task strategies and

    attribution. Journal of Educational Psychology 79(4): 238–244.

    Shenkar O. 2001. Cultural distance revisited: Towards a more rigorous conceptualization and

    measurement of cultural differences.Journal of International Business Studies 32(3): 519–536.

    Shenkar O. 2012a. Cultural distance revisited: towards a more rigorous conceptualization and

    measurement of cultural differences.Journal of International Business Studies 43(1): 1–11.

    Shenkar O. 2012b. Beyond cultural distance: Switching to a friction lens in the study of cultural

    differences. Journal of International Business Studies 43(1): 12–17.

  • Shook CL, Ketchen DJ, Hult GTM, Kacmar KM. 2004. An assessment of the use of structural

    equation modeling in strategic management research. Strategic Management Journal 25(4): 397–

    404.

    Smither J, Skov RB, Adler S. 1986. Attributions for the poorly performing blackjack dealer: in

    the cards or inability? Personnel Psychology 39(1): 123–139.

    Spector PE. 2006. Method variance in organizational research: truth or urban

    legend? Organizational Research Methods 9(2):221–232.

    Stahl GK, Mendenhall M, Weber Y. 2005. Research in socio-cultural integration in mergers and

    acquisitions. In Mergers and Acquisitions: Managing Culture and Human Resources, Stahl

    GK, Mendenhall ME (eds). Stanford University Press: Stanford, CA;401–411.

    Stahl GK, Voigt A. 2008. Do cultural differences matter in mergers and acquisitions? A tentative

    model for examination. Organization Science 19(1): 160–176.

    Staw BM, McKechnie PI, Puffer SM. 1983. The justification of organizational

    performance. Administrative Science Quarterly 28(4):582–600.

    Teerikangas S, Very P. 2006. The culture-performance relationship in M&A: from yes/no to

    how. British Journal of Management 17(1):31–48.

    Tomlinson EC, Dineen BR, Lewicki RJ. 2004. The road to reconciliation: antecedents of victim

    willingness to reconcile following a broken promise. Journal of Management 30(2): 165–188.

    Vaara E. 2002. On the discursive construction of success/failure in narratives of post-merger

    integration. Organization Studies 23(2):211–248.

    Vaara E, Tienari J. 2011. On the narrative construction of MNCs: an antenarrative analysis of

    legitimation and resistance in a cross-border merger. Organization Science 22(2): 370–390.

    Very P, Lubatkin M, Calori R. 1996. A cross-national assessment of acculturative stress in recent

    European mergers. International Studies of Management and Organization 26(1): 59–86.

    Very P, Lubatkin M, Calori R, Veiga J. 1997. Relative standing and the performance of recently

    acquired European firms. Strategic Management Journal 18(8): 593–614.

    Weber Y, Shenkar O, Raveh A. 1996. National and corporate cultural fit in mergers/acquisitions:

    an exploratory study. Management Science 42(8): 1215–1227.

    Weiner B. 1979. A theory of motivation for some classroom experiences. Journal of Educational

    Psychology 71(1): 3–25.

    Weiner B. 1992. Human Motivation: Metaphors, Theories and Research. Sage: Newbury Park,

    CA.

  • Weiner B. 2008. Reflections on the history of attribution theory and research: people,

    personalities, publications, problems. Social Psychology 39(3): 151–156.

    1. We also calculated an alternative measure of national cultural differences by using Hofstede's

    (1991) and Berry et al.'s (2010) scores. The results did not change regarding the patterns of

    statistical significance or the directional influence, which supported the robustness of our

    measure.

    Supporting Information

    Available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smj.2163

    http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/smj.2163