App a Caldwell

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    1/18

    Human Intelligence

    Human Intelligence

    Historical Survey of Human Intelligence Research and Measurement

    Allison Caldwell

    Walden University

    August 20th, 2009

    1

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    2/18

    Abstract

    The study of human intelligence and its measurement has been one of the mostconsistently successful areas of research and practice over the last century. Sinceits inception, a few lines of investigation have endured such as the nature of

    intelligence and whether it is primarily based in biology or is more affected by

    environmental and developmental dynamics. The structure of intelligence asmultivariate and hierarchical, and the single dominant general intelligenceg

    factor, is examined within its historical context and in light of contemporary

    research. A survey is provided of the major intelligence theorists and developersof cognitive testing batteries from Sir Frances Galton through to present day

    psychologists and practitioners.

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    3/18

    Human Intelligence

    Historical Survey of Human Intelligence Research and Measurement

    The study and measurement of human intelligence has been one of the most consistently

    important and successful areas of research and practice since the birth of psychology just over

    a century ago (Geary, 2004). This success is largely due to the steady emphasis on statistics, and

    the development of quantitative data that has been part of the evolution of research in this area of

    study. A major line of investigation and debate has been the nature and origin of human

    intelligence. The questions that have been foundational to the study of intelligence concern

    whether it is constituted by heritable traits that are biologically based, or if it is more affected by an

    environmental and developmental dynamic. Another line of inquiry is whether there is a single

    dominant factor through which human intelligence can be defined, or are there multiple factors

    through which human cognition is structured, and how are these factors measured (Kamphaus,

    Winsor, Rowe, & Kim, 2005). The progression and refinement of these issues are seen through the

    historical context of intelligence theories and methods of measuring human cognition. What

    follows is a brief survey focusing on the major players involved in this work over the last 100

    years.

    Foundations of Research on Intelligence

    The history behind the study of individual differences in intelligence dates back to the work

    of Sir Francis Galton in the latter half of the 19th century. Galton contributed a great deal to the

    psychology of intelligence; his work was foundational to intelligence theory, statistics, and

    cognitive measurement (McIntosh & Dixon, 2005). The cousin of Charles Darwin, Galton was

    greatly influenced by Darwinian Theory. Primary assumptions on which the theory of evolution

    3

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    4/18

    was based included the concept of natural selection; that humankind was in a constant process of

    adaptation and change, with dominant, ascendant characteristics being passed on to the next

    generation through heredity (Jensen, 2002). In this regard, Darwin held that the strongest genes

    promoting intelligent, survivalist behavior would be carried over to subsequent generations,

    resulting in the evolution of human cognition. The effect of this heritable process is a great deal of

    variation within the species of humankind. It is this disparity of differences in human intelligence

    that became the focus of much of Galtons research (Jensen).

    The nature versus nurture debate sparked by Darwins work was carried over to Galtons

    research on mental abilities and has permeated theories of intelligence since that time. The

    dispute between intelligence as an innate, inborn faculty, or whether it can be developed, has been

    on-going. Galton believed in the genetic origin of human mental abilities, and put forward a

    definition of fixed intelligence (McIntosh & Dixon, 2005, p.505) based on heredity. Much of

    his research focused on this concept. Given his interest in statistics, Galton systematized studies

    on heritable intelligence within families, measuring what he called general intellectual power

    (Geary, 2004, p. 254). This led to the beginning of the eugenics movement, and the ranking of

    human beings based on their abilities and characteristics, with the underlying idea of procreation of

    humanitys brightest and best (Simonton, 2003).

    It is worthy of note that as a result of his intelligence studies within families, Galton made

    significant and lasting contributions to psychology through the discovery of measures of bivariate

    correlations, analysis of variance, and the concept of regression (Jensen, 2002). He discovered

    statistical correlations based on how family members were generationally similar to one another

    (Jensen). He also found that excessively different heritable traits tended to move towards a more

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    5/18

    Human Intelligence

    normal or average expression in subsequent generations, statistically moving closer to a central

    means (Seligman, 2002).

    Galton is credited with developing one of the first measures of intelligence (McIntosh &

    Dixon, 2005). He theorized that intelligence was developed through sensory experience

    (Wasserman & Talky, 2005). He, therefore, developed a measure of intelligence based on

    quantifying sensory acuity, reasoning that the intellect would be able to gain more from experience

    if a persons sensory acuity was high (Thorndike, 1997).

    James McKeen Cattell was an American psychologist who studied under Wilhelm Wundt

    in Germany, later collaborating with Galton at Cambridge before returning to the United States.

    Highly influenced by Galton, Cattell brought home with him the idea of assessing intelligence

    through the senses rather than through the higher mental processes (Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005,

    p. 5). Like Galton, Cattell also held a firm belief in statistics and the quantitative scientific method

    as a means of strengthening the science of psychology. He developed a mental testing battery

    based on many of Galtons own tests and established a testing program for students at Columbia

    University (Wasserman & Tulsky). This program was short lived, however, when experimental

    results did not support correlations between Cattells mental tests (McIntosh & Dixon, 2005, p.

    4) and levels of student achievement. The assumption held by Galton and Cattell as to the sensory

    nature of human intelligence was incorrect but their belief in some underlying measure of intellect

    would come to predominance in the study of human cognition.

    5

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    6/18

    Development of Test Theory

    In 1904, Charles Spearman, a graduate student, conducted research using a new statistical

    technique called factor analysis. Using this mathematical formula, Spearman found correlations

    between many different tests that measured intellectual ability, all of which were strongly related

    to one central correlate; an individuals global intelligence capacity, which was termed theg factor

    (Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005). Borrowing from the principles of physics, and in a flashback to the

    explanations of Galton and Cattell, Spearman explained theg factoras an individuals amount of

    general mental energy (Spearman, 1927, p. 137, as quoted in Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005).

    Spearman published his research in a paper entitled, General Intelligence, Objectively Determined

    and Measuredwhich theorized that all cognition has in common the foundationalg factor

    (Wasserman & Tulsky); a central, defining measure of a persons intelligence. The fundamental

    importance ofg, as opposed to a multivariate view of intelligence factors, immediately became a

    topic of intense debate, and has remained so to the present day. Spearmansg factoris a

    psychological discovery of phenomenal importance, and has guided much of the development of

    human intelligence theories since its inception (Wasserman & Tulsky). A major query concerning

    theg factorthat has remained is whether it just a psychometric, statistical occurrence, or are there

    biological or psychological foundations to this finding (Johnson & Bouchard, 2005). A second

    important discovery from Spearmans original work was that human intelligence factors could be

    presented in a hierarchical fashion, withgas the overriding and dominant factor to which all else

    correlated (Geary, 2004; Reynolds & Keith, 2007). This observation by Spearman was a

    foreshadowing of the premise on which much contemporary intelligence theory is based (Geary).

    A major influence on contemporary intelligence research occurred with the development of

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    7/18

    Human Intelligence

    the Cattell-Horn Gf-Gc theory. A student of Spearman, Raymond B. Cattell was a psychologist

    and scientist believing in a mostly heritable view of human cognition. Like his predecessors,

    Cattell had a strong statistical bent, and held to empirically based factor-analytic studies in his

    research. John Horn was a prominent student of Cattells. Together, Cattell and Horn developed a

    theory that divided Spearmansg factorinto two distinct areas, each of which was made up of

    multiple interacting factors of intelligence (McGrew, 2005). Fluid Intelligence (Gf) was theorized

    to have a biological basis, free from the influences of learning and acculturation, but in essence

    constituting an ability to learn (Geary, 2004, p. 256). Crystallized intelligence (Gc) is more

    related to acquired skills such as language knowledge and vocabulary, having a direct relation to

    experience, acculturation, and education (Geary). By its definition, Gf-Gc theory takes into

    account biological and hereditary factors, as well as environmental influences such a learning and

    experience. Gf-Gc theory is based on a hierarchical multi-factor view of intelligence, with

    correlations evident between higher level factors of intelligence to lower stratum measures

    (McGrew, 2005). Due to the correlations found in this hierarchy, Cattell and Horn minimized the

    need and importance of theg factor. Indeed, Horn did not believe that ag factorwas necessary at

    all in intelligence models or theories (Horn & Blankson, 2005; Kvist & Gustafsson, 2008).

    A second major theoretical high point occurred in 1993 with the publication of John B.

    Carrolls Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor Analytic Studies (Carroll, 1993). Based

    on an amalgamation of decades of research and over 460 factor-analytic studies of human

    intelligence, Carroll put together a comprehensive framework of human cognitive ability (Johnson

    & Bouchard, 2005). Carrolls Three-Stratum Theory presents three levels of cognitive ability, with

    7

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    8/18

    theg factorat the top of the hierarchy due to its overriding influence and correlation with lower

    stratum factors (Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005; Carroll, 2005). The second stratum contains 8 or

    more factors, with greater than 65 lower levels abilities identified in the third stratum (Wasserman

    & Tulsky).

    The presentation of Carrolls Three-Stratum theory was strikingly similar to the Cattell-

    Horn Gf-Gc hypothesis (McGrew, 2009), with the marked exception that Carroll included the g-

    factor as a third level, and overarching element of human intelligence, believinggto be a

    necessary component in the description of cognitive ability (McGrew). These two theories were

    placed under one common umbrella because of their complementary strengths, to make what is

    known as the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory of cognitive abilities. This amalgamated theory

    represents an open-ended view of intelligence, and a model for the study of cognition that could be

    built upon, and added to, with on-going research (McGrew). Recent research has examined the

    hypothesis that Cattell-Horns fluid intelligence (Gf) is equivalent to theg factoras defined by

    Carroll with positive, but not conclusive, results (Blair, 2006; Gignac, 2006; Kvist & Gustafsson,

    2008). The concept of fluid intelligence however is clearly and strongly correlated with general

    intelligence (Arendasy, Hergovich & Sommer, 2008).

    Development of Testing Instruments

    The bridge between theories of intelligence and practical application is seen in the

    measures and testing instruments created to quantify human cognition. Two events in the early

    20th century had a major impact on the development of intelligence testing batteries. The first was

    the largely international requirement for the availability of general public education. The second

    was the strong desire to establish psychology as a scientific discipline by emphasizing empirical

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    9/18

    Human Intelligence

    research and quantitative data to support its growth and development (Thorndike, 1997).

    A major breakthrough in intelligence testing came in 1904 when the French

    government asked Alfred Binet to develop a test to identify children who may be in need of

    alternative educational environments due to a lower level of cognitive functioning (McIntosh &

    Dixon, 2005). Binet disagreed with the approach taken by Galton and James McKeen Cattell that

    quantified sensory acuity, and instead focused on the creation of an assessment of higher mental

    functions such as working memory, judgment and reasoning (Geary, 2004, p. 257), as well as

    motor and perceptual skills. Binet defined intelligence largely in terms of an individuals capacity

    for good judgment (Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005). His scales were more theoretically in line with

    the later work of R. B. Cattell and Horn, since they were hierarchically organized. Binet

    recognized three levels of intelligence, which was truly revolutionary at the time (Wasserman &

    Tulsky, p. 3), but he also questioned the practical need for a focus on theg factorin explanations

    of cognitive abilities (von Mayrhauser, 1989).

    The major goal of Binets work was to measure childrens level of intelligence to

    determine their probable success in school or their need for alternative education (Geary, 2004).

    He did this by devising a system whereby a childs ability level was equated with age. The

    problem with this system however was that a child of 7 and a child of 11 could both be determined

    to be functioning at the level of a 9 year old, but it offered no further specification of their

    cognitive performance. This difficulty was solved in 1914 however by Wilhelm Stern who

    developed the Intelligence Quotient (IQ) which basically divided the mental age, proposed by

    Binet, by the childs chronological age, multiplied by 100 (Boake, 2002).

    9

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    10/18

    Binets work was cut short by his death in 1911 but other psychologists continued to build

    on the firm foundation that had been laid (Boake, 2002). Foremost among them was Lewis

    Terman. Along with a number of colleagues from Stanford University, Terman authored a U.S.

    revision of the Binet-Simon scales, titled the Stanford-Binet. The revised battery met with wide-

    spread and on-going acclaim, quickly becoming the new standard for intelligence tests

    (Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005, p. 11). The solid reputation of this measure was largely the result of

    Termans high scientific standards, and his methodically sound norm sampling (Wasserman &

    Tulsky). A major advantage of the Stanford-Binet was its incorporation of Sterns 1914

    Intelligence Quotient (IQ), which fell in line with the foundational belief in Galtons theory that a

    central measure of intelligence provided an essential gauge of success in many different areas of

    life (Simonton, 2003). Indeed, numerous studies have shown a significant correlation between IQ

    and otherg factorscores, and levels of academic, job and career success (Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett,

    S. A., & Ones, D. S., 2004; Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J., 2004).

    Complimenting Binets pragmatic approach, intelligence testing in America took a

    practical turn and gained a substantial foothold through the efforts of Robert Mearns Yerkes,

    president of the American Psychological Association (APA), and his work with the U.S. Army

    during World War I. Yerkes chaired a committee on the Psychological Examination of Recruits,

    which included Lewis Terman and other prominent U.S. psychologists. The goal of the committee

    was the development of group intelligence tests to screen army applicants regarding their fitness

    for service in different levels of the military (Boake, 2002). Rather than focusing on a general

    level of cognitive ability, such as would be measured by theg factor, these assessments were more

    practical in nature, measuring intellectual aptitude (von Mayrhauser, 1989, p. 67) and specific

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    11/18

    Human Intelligence

    abilities (p. 63) required for different jobs. Through the military testing program, mental testing

    followed the functionalist approach, adding a pragmatic perspective that took into account

    developmental and environmental aspects of intelligence, rather than embracing a predominantly

    hereditarian view based on IQ and theg factor(von Mayrhauser).

    A young doctoral student named David Wechsler worked with Yerkes as an army

    psychological examiner during World War I, and later continued studies with James McKeen

    Cattell and Charles Spearman (Wasserman & Tulsky, 2005). Wechsler developed one of the most

    successful cognitive testing batteries of the last half century, the revisions of which constitute the

    most widely used intelligence test today (Zhu & Weiss, 2005). A master at synthesizing existing

    tests (Wasserman & Tulsky, p. 12), Wechlser used the Army tests, and other assessments, to

    develop his Wechsler Scales. He defined intelligence as an individuals global capacity to act

    purposefully and to think rationally (Wechsler, 1944, p. 3) in relation to their environment. Two

    major innovations Wechsler made to intelligence testing included dividing subtests that measured

    theg factorinto Verbal and Performance scales. This allowed for differential diagnoses based on

    an individuals strengths and deficits in specific intelligence factors (Zhu & Weiss). He also

    developed the Deviation IQ, which permits ranking of an individuals level performance relative to

    their age group (Wechsler).

    The Woodcock Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities is one of the most recent

    assessments to be developed for measuring the factors of human intelligence (Schrank, 2005). The

    theoretical foundation for this battery of tests is the Cattell-Horn-Carroll (CHC) theory of cognitive

    abilities, which is recognized as one of the most valid contemporary representations of a multi-

    11

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    12/18

    factor theory of intelligence (McGrew, 2005). The marrying of the Cattell-Horn Gf and Gc theory

    and the Carroll Three-Stratum hierarchical model of human intelligence in the Woodcock Johnson

    III includes a measure of theg factorwhich is expressed as the general intellectual ability score.

    When examined strictly on its own, theg factorholds limited meaning. The strength of this

    assessment battery, and the significance of the correlatingg, is found in the examination of the

    cluster scores indicative of the levels of cognitive skills; the broad abilities that make up the g

    score (Schrank). It is also essential to consider the next level of narrow abilities that make up the

    cluster scores to determine the cognitive strengths and needs (Davidson & Downing, 2000), and to

    make practical recommendations that will be of assistance to the individual being tested, based on

    the purpose for testing (Reeve, 2004). This assessment battery is representational of a new era in

    bridging cognitive theory to the practice of applied measurement of intelligence (McGrew, p.

    139).

    Contemporary Research and Theory

    Contemporary research and theory regarding intelligence and measurement of cognitive

    abilities is very much a reflection of the issues embedded within its history. The seminal,

    structural model of cognitive abilities proposed by J. B. Carroll in 1993, provided an empirically

    based framework of intelligence, and the multi-factor and hierarchical nature of human cognition

    has gained widespread acceptance (Alfonso, Flanagan & Radwan, 2005; McGrew, 2009). The idea

    that intelligence is generated by a general unified brain capacity, commonly known as theg factor,

    has been a matter of controversy since it was first proposed by Charles Spearman in 1904 (Geary,

    2004). The existence of the g factor has been firmly established both from a theoretical

    (Anderson, 2001,) and a statistical psychometric perspective through factor analysis studies

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    13/18

    Human Intelligence

    (Johnson, Nijenhuis & Bouchard, 2008; Floyd, Shands, Rafael, Bergeron & McGrew, 2009), but

    its practical relevance is still under examination. Some contemporary theorists, such as J. L. Horn,

    whose combined work with Cattell and Carroll laid the foundation for the Carroll-Horn-Cattell

    (CHC) model of cognitive abilities, fundamentally disagrees that the concept of ag factoris

    necessary (Kvist & Gustafsson, 2008). Johnson & Bouchard (2005) challenged this position and

    found that theg factorwas an important aspect to all areas of intelligence in their examination of

    three prominent theories of intelligence, namely Carrolls three-strata, CHC and Vernons verbal-

    perceptual model.

    Johnson , Bouchard & Krueger (2004) provided support for the existence of theg factor,

    and its strong correlation among 3 cognitive testing batteries, using heterogeneous populations. In

    a successful second replication study, Johnson, Nijenhuis & Bouchard (2008) corroborated this

    evidence, finding consistency in measurement of ag factorusing 5 different cognitive assessments

    within a variety of populations. This demonstrated that the existence of theg factoris consistent

    across the five testing batteries, being strongly generalized, and holding a significant place in

    relation to the correlation of intellectual abilities.

    As a psychological or biological construct (vanderMaas, Dolan, Grasman, Wicherts,

    Huizinga & Raijmakers, 2006) the origin and nature ofgas a dominant, pervasive factor of general

    intelligence is still debated in the realm of theoretical cognitive science (Johnson, Nijenhuis &

    Bouchard, 2008). Two current explanations that attempt to hypothesize answers to this line of

    inquiry are Cattells investment theory and a dynamical model of general intelligence

    (vanderMass, et al; Kvist & Gustafsson, 2008). Investment theory proposes that a general

    13

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    14/18

    intelligence exists, that it is developed in children through the process of maturation, affected by

    such things as effort, experience, and the degree to which one applies themselves to learning (Kvist

    & Gustafsson). This general intelligence, which Cattell theoretically equates with fluid

    intelligence (Gf), is a causal agent accounting for variation in individual differences, because it has

    a general involvement in the acquisition of a variety of knowledge, abilities and skills (Kvist &

    Gustafsson). The dynamical model is similarly based on development, and the beneficial

    interactions between cognitive processes (vanerMass et al., p. 842). This is explained as a

    positive manifold effect (p. 855) whereby the strong correlation between intelligence factors is

    produced through maturation and development in a dynamic process that takes into account

    genetic, neurological, and environmental factors and their reciprocal causation (p. 857) in the

    growth of intelligence (Hambrick, Pink, Meinz, Pettibone & Oswald, 2008). Next steps for

    contemporary theoretical models of intelligence include making use of factor-analysis statistical

    methods to both scrutinize their validity, as well as to enhance and widen theoretical frameworks

    (Johnson & Bouchard, 2005; Keith, 2005; McGrew, 2009).

    Individual differences in human cognition have been an area of study for over a century,

    dating back to the time of Sir Frances Galton. The multivariate and hierarchical nature of factors

    that make up intelligence has been well established, as has the psychometric, statistical notion of

    an overriding general intelligence factor that correlates with other factors that are part of cognition.

    Strong correlational evidence also exists regarding the connection between and among intelligence

    factors at different levels of the hierarchy by making use of factor analysis statistics (Davidson &

    Downing, 2000; Keith, 2005). The origin and nature of intelligence is still specifically undefined,

    although contemporary theories reflect an approach to research that is open-ended, considering a

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    15/18

    Human Intelligence

    variety of causal and dynamic factors in the development of intelligence, including biology,

    neurology, environment, and affective influences. The evolution of mental testing is also on the

    verge of a new era, whereby assessment is being developed on the foundation of intelligence

    theory (McIntosh & Dixon, 2005). As we examine intelligence through a multidimensional lens,

    and seek to measure it with instruments that reflect open multi-factor theory, we will gain greater

    insights into its origin and nature, and more global benefits of this increase in knowledge,

    translated into practical advantages.

    References

    Alfonso, V. C., Flanagan, D. P., & Radwan, S. (2005). The impact of the Cattell-Horn-Carroll

    Theory on test development and interpretation of Cognitive and academic abilities. InFlanagan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev.

    ed.) (pp. 185202). New York: Guilford Press.

    Anderson, M. (2001). Annotation: Conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Child Psychologyand Psychiatry, 42(3), 287-298.

    Arendsay, M. E., Hergovick, A., & Sommer. M. (2008). Investigating the g-saturation ofvarious stratum-two factors using automatic item generation. Intelligence, 36, 574-583.

    Blair, C. (2006). How similar are fluid cognition and general intelligence? A developmentalneuroscience perspective on fluid cognition as an aspect of human cognitive ability.

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 109-160.

    Boake, C. (2002). From the Binet-Simon to the Wechsler-Bellevue: Tracing the history ofintelligence testing. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 24(3), 383-

    405.

    Carroll, J. B. (1993).Human cognitive abilities: A survey of factor analytic studies. New York:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Carroll, J. B. (2005). The three-stratum theory of cognitive abilities. In Flanagan, D. P., &

    Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev. ed.) (pp. 69-76). New

    15

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    16/18

    York: Guilford Press.

    Davidson, J. E., & Downing, C. L. (2000). Contemporary models of intelligence. In R. J.

    Sternberg (Ed.),Handbook of intelligence (pp. 34-52). New York: CambridgeUniversity Press.

    Floyd, R. G., Shands, E. I., Rafael, F. A., Bergeron, R., & McGrew, K. (in press). Thedependability of general-factor loadings: The effects of factor-extraction methods, test

    battery composition, test battery size, and their interactions. Intelligence.

    Doi:10.1016/j.intell.2009.05.003

    Geary, D. C. (2004). The origin of mind: Evolution of the brain, cognition, and general

    intelligence. Washington, D.C. American Psychological Association.

    Gignac, G. E. (2006). Evaluating subtest g saturation levels via single trait-correlated

    uniqueness (STCU) SEM approach: Evidence in favor of crystallized subtests as the best

    indicators of g. Intelligence, 34, 29-46.

    Hambrick, D. Z., Pink, J. E., Meinz, E. J., Pettibone, J. C., & Oswald, F. L. (2008). The roles of

    ability, personality, and interests in acquiring current events knowledge: A longitudinalstudy. Intelligence, 36, 261-278.

    Horn, J. L., & Blankson, N. (2005). Foundations for better understanding of cognitive abilities.

    In Flanagan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev.ed.) (pp. 41-68). New York: Guilford Press.

    Jensen, A. R. (2002). Galtons legacy to research on intelligence. Journal of Biosocial Science,34, 145-172.

    Johnson, W., & Bouchard, T. J. (2005). The structure of human intelligence: It is verbal,perceptual, and image rotation (VPR), not fluid and crystallized. Intelligence, 33(4), 393-

    416.

    Johnson, W., te Nijenhuis, J., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2008). Still just 1 g: Consistent results fromfive test batteries. Intelligence, 36 81-95.

    Kamphaus, R. W., Winsor, A. P., Rowe, E. W., & Kim, S. (2005). A history of intelligence test

    interpretation. In Flanagan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectualassessment (Rev. ed.) (pp. 23-38). New York: Guilford Press.

    Keith, T. Z. (2005). Using confirmatory factor analysis to aid in understanding the constructs

    measured by intelligence tests. In Flanagan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.),

    Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev. ed.) (pp. 581-614). New York: Guilford

    Press.

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    17/18

    Human Intelligence

    Kuncel, N. R., Hezlett, S. A., & Ones, D. S. (2004). Academic performance, career potential,

    creativity, and job performance: Can one construct predict them all? Journal of

    Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1), 148-161.

    Kvist, A. V., & Gustafsson, J. E. (in press). The relation between fluid intelligence and the

    general factor as a function of cultural background: A test of Cattells investment theory,Intelligence.

    McGrew, K. S. (2005). The Cattell-Horn-Carroll theory of cognitive abilities. In D.P. Flanagan,

    & P. I. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues

    (pp. 136-181)., 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press.

    McGrew, K. S. (2009). CHC theory and the human cognitive abilities project: standing on the

    shoulders of the giants of psychometric intelligence research. Intelligence, 37, 1-10.

    McIntosh, D. E., & Dixon, F. A. (2005). In D.P. Flanagan, & P. I. Harrison (Eds.),Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp.504-520)., 2nd ed.

    New York: Guilford Press.

    Reeve, C. L. (2004). Differential ability antecedents of general and specific dimensions of

    declarative knowledge: More than g. Intelligence, 36, 621-652.

    Reynolds, M. R., & Keith, T. Z. (2007). Spearmans law of diminishing returns in hierarchicalmodels of intelligence for children and adolescents. Intelligence, 35, 267-281.

    Schmidt, F. L., & Hunter, J. (2004). General mental ability in the world of work: Occupationalattainment and job performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86(1),

    162-173.

    Schrank, F. A. (2005). Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. In Flanagan, D. P.,

    & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev. ed.) (pp. 3-22).

    New York: Guilford Press.

    Seligman, D. (2002). Good breeding. National Review, 54(1), 53-54.

    Simonton, D. K. (2003). Francis Galtons hereditary genius: Its place in the history andpsychology of science. In R. J. Sterberg (Ed.), The anatomy of impact: What makes the

    great works of psychology great(pp. 3 18). American Psychological Association:

    Washington, D.C.

    Thorndike, R. M. (1997). The early history of intelligence testing. In Flanagan, D. P., Genshaft,

    17

  • 8/2/2019 App a Caldwell

    18/18

    J. L., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment(pp. 3-16). NewYork: Guilford Press

    vanderMaas, H. L. J., Dolan, C.V., Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wicherts, J. M., Huizinga, H. M., &Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A dynamical model of general intelligence: The positive manifold of

    intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113, 842861.

    Von Mayrhauser, R. T. (1989). Making intelligence functional: Walter Dill Scott and applied

    psychological testing in World War 1. Journal of the History of Behavioral Sciences, 25,

    60-72.

    Wasserman, J. D., & Tulsky, D. S. (2005). A history of intelligence assessment. In Flanagan, D.

    P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev. ed.) (pp. 3-22).

    New York: Guilford Press.

    Wechsler, D. (1944). The measurement of adult intelligence (3rd ed.) Baltimore: Williams &

    Wilkins.

    Zhu, J., & Weiss, L. (2005). The Wechsler Scales. In Flanagan, D. P., & Harrison, P. L. (Eds.),

    Contemporary intellectual assessment (Rev. ed.) (pp. 297-324). New York: GuilfordPress.