ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    1/60

    Author's Accepted Manuscript

    Extinction of aversive eliciting functions as ananalog of exposure to conditioned fear: Does

    it alter avoidance responding?

    Carmen Luciano, Sonsoles Valdivia-Salas,Francisco J. Ruiz, Miguel Rodrguez-Valverde,Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Michael J. Dougher,Francisco Cabello, Vanessa Snchez, YvonneBarnes-Holmes, Olga Gutierrez

    PII: S2212-1447(13)00011-2DOI: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.05.001Reference: JCBS18

    To appear in: Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science

    Received date: 17 January 2012Revised date: 10 April 2013Accepted date: 1 May 2013

    Cite this article as: Carmen Luciano, Sonsoles Valdivia-Salas, Francisco J. Ruiz,Miguel Rodrguez-Valverde, Dermot Barnes-Holmes, Michael J. Dougher,Francisco Cabello, Vanessa Snchez, Yvonne Barnes-Holmes, Olga Gutierrez,Extinction of aversive eliciting functions as an analog of exposure toconditioned fear: Does it alter avoidance responding?, Journal of ContextualBehavioral Science, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.05.001

    This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted forpublication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of

    the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, andreview of the resulting galley proof before it is published in its final citable form.Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered whichcould affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journalpertain.

    www.elsevier.com/locate/jcbs

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    2/60

    1

    Extinction of aversive eliciting functions as an analog of exposure to

    conditioned fear: Does it alter avoidance responding?

    Carmen Luciano (1), Sonsoles Valdivia-Salas (1), Francisco J. Ruiz (1)

    Miguel Rodrguez-Valverde (2), Dermot Barnes-Holmes (3), Michael J. Dougher (4),

    Francisco Cabello (5), Vanessa Snchez (1), Yvonne Barnes-Holmes (3), & Olga

    Gutierrez (6).

    (1) Universidad de Almera

    (2) Universidad de Jan

    (3) National University of Ireland

    (4) University of New Mexico

    (5) Universidad de Murcia

    (6) Universidad de Barcelona

    Running head: Altering avoidance responding

    Address corresponding to: Carmen Luciano, Ph.D., Ed. A. Facultad Psicologa,

    Universidad Almera, 04120 Almera, Spain. Email: [email protected] Phone: 34-950-

    015260

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    3/60

    2

    Running head: Altering avoidance responding

    Extinction of aversive eliciting functions as an analog of exposure to

    conditioned fear: Does it alter avoidance responding?

    Abstract

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    4/60

    3

    Exposure techniques rely on the assumption that the extinction of the classically

    conditioned response (i.e., fear) will result in the disruption of limiting forms of

    avoidance behavior, both directly trained and derived/indirectly established. This report

    presents translational research that attempts to test this assumption in laboratory

    conditions in two experiments with human volunteers. The procedure in both

    experiments included six phases: (1) conditional discrimination training for the

    formation of two 6-member equivalence classes; (2) classical conditioning of elicited

    responses to Class 1 (A1/B1) and Class 2 (A2/B2) members in the white context,

    followed by conditioning of avoidance/approach responses to Class 1/Class 2 members,

    respectively, in thegreen context; (3) test for the transfer of avoidance/approach

    functions and of eliciting respondent functions to D1/F1 and D2/F2 in thegreen

    context; (4) extinction of classically conditioned responses to A1/B1 in the white

    context; (5) test of the effects of respondent extinction on avoidance responding to the

    A, B, D, and F stimuli in thegreen context; and (6) test of derived symmetry and

    equivalence relations. Results show that after successful respondent extinction in the

    white context, only 33.3% participants stopped showing avoidance behavior in the

    green context, and that respondent elicitation was reinstalled during the test (Phase 5).

    In Phase 4 of Experiment 2, in addition to undergoing respondent extinction,

    participants were instructed that the white and green contexts were similar. Results

    show that after successful respondent extinction in the white context during Phase 4,

    only 10% participants stopped showing avoidance behavior in the green context, and

    that respondent elicitation was almost eliminated during the test (Phase 5). We discuss

    these findings and their applied implications.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    5/60

    4

    Key words: aversive conditioning, respondent extinction, equivalence relations,

    avoidance, transfer and transformation of functions, skin conductance, human fear.

    Extinction of aversive eliciting functions as an analog of exposure to conditioned fear:

    Does it alter avoidance responding?

    Traditionally, conditioning-based approaches to the explanation of anxiety

    disorders have assumed that a history of direct aversive conditioning is necessary for the

    acquisition of fear and avoidance responses (Barlow, 2002). Although clinical data

    indicate that fears often emerge in the absence of any identifiable aversive conditioning

    (e.g., Rachman, 1977, 1991), and recent research in derived relational responding (see

    Dymond & Roche, 2009) and associative learning (Field, 2006) has identified ways in

    which fear and avoidance can be learned indirectly, it could be said that the assumption

    still holds in general terms, with some aversive conditioning experience needed at some

    point in the genesis of anxiety.

    Conditioning-based approaches have led to the design of exposure techniques,

    widely used in behavior therapy for the treatment of anxiety disorders (e.g., Barlow,

    2002; Deacon & Abramowitz, 2004; Marks, 1981). Exposure therapy is based on the

    assumption that repeated exposure to the feared object or event (conditioned stimulus),

    produces the extinction of the aversively conditioned responses (i.e., fear) and, hence,

    the reduction of their behavioral outcome, namely avoidance (e.g., Craske &

    Mystkowski, 2006; Mowrer, 1960). In fact, preventing avoidance is the ultimate goal of

    exposure therapy, as this behavioral process is considered a critical factor in the

    etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders (e.g., Barlow, 2002; Forsyth, Eifert, &

    Barrios, 2006; Hayes, 1976; Hayes, Wilson, Gifford, Follette, &Strosahl, 1996).

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    6/60

    5

    During the last 15 years, research on the extinction of conditioned fear has

    focused on the conditions in which exposure treatments work (e.g., Hermans, Craske,

    Mineka, & Lovibond, 2006), showing that the introduction of inhibitory CSs as safety-

    signals (e.g., the presence of the therapist during exposure) or that the use of safety

    behaviors such as avoidance have a deleterious effect on fear extinction (e.g., Lovibond,

    Davis, & OFlaherty, 2000; Lovibond, Mithcell, Minard, Brady, & Menzies, 2009).

    Also, it has been shown that the extinction of conditioned fear responses is context

    sensitive; that is, that a change of context typically produces renewal of already

    extinguished fear responses (Neumann & Longbottom, 2008; Vansteenwegen, Dirikx,

    Hermans, Vervliet, &Eelen, 2006), turning fear extinction into a difficult target. Besides

    the observed difficulties in obtaining fear extinction, somewhat surprisingly the central

    assumption underlying exposure treatments remains untested in laboratory conditions.

    That is, there is no laboratory evidence that the extinction of fear responses in the same

    context in which they were conditioned will alter subsequent avoidance responding in a

    context in which there is an actual opportunity to avoid.

    Research on relational responding during the last decades is successfully

    addressing some of the limitations of traditional conditioning approaches to the

    acquisition of fear and avoidance (see Dymond & Roche, 2009; Forsyth et al., 2006).

    Specifically, there is evidence that a stimulus may acquire eliciting and avoidance

    functions indirectly by virtue of its relation to another stimulus whose eliciting and

    avoidance functions were acquired by direct conditioning. For instance, Dougher,

    Auguston, Markham, Greenway, and Wulfert (1994) demonstrated that after training

    and testing for two four-member equivalence classes (A1-B1-C1-D1 and A2-B2-C2-

    D2) and pairing B1 to electric shocks, most participants showed higher skin

    conductance to C1 and D1 than to C2 and D2 (for similar results, see Rodrguez-

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    7/60

    6

    Valverde, Luciano, & Barnes-Holmes, 2009). Even more interestingly, Dougher,

    Hamilton, Fink, and Harrington (2007) found higher skin conductance to a non-

    conditioned stimulus (say C) than to an aversively conditioned stimulus (say B), by

    virtue of the derived relation of comparison previously established between both stimuli

    (B is less than C). A similar transfer of function effect has been observed with

    respondent extinction. Dougher et al. (1994) exposed participants to aversive

    conditioning by pairing several elements of the same equivalence class (B1, C1, and

    D1) with shock. Then, only one of the elements underwent extinction (i.e., was

    presented repeatedly without shock). As a result, the remaining members of the class

    failed to elicit responses in a subsequent test.

    Avoidance-evoking functions may also transfer across members of the same

    relational network. By using similar procedures to those in Dougher et al. (1994),

    Auguston and Dougher (1997) trained avoidance responding in the presence of an

    aversive conditioned stimulus (B1 paired with shock) and then observed that other

    members of the same equivalence class (C1 and D1) evoked avoidance responding

    although they had never been directly paired with shock. Also, Roche, Kanter, Brown,

    Dymond, and Fogarty (2008) showed that the extinction of avoidance responding in the

    presence of one element of an equivalence class transferred to other elements of the

    same class. In summary, data show that respondent elicitation and extinction, as well as

    avoidance-evoking functions and operant extinction of avoidance, may transform

    according to equivalence and non-equivalence relations. These results attest how

    importantly verbal processes are involved in human conditioning (e.g., De Houwer,

    2009; Lovibond, 2006). To date, however, no study has addressed the impact of

    respondent extinction of conditioned fear on avoidance behavior, either directly trained

    or acquired by relational means. Indeed, to our knowledge, no published study has

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    8/60

    7

    analyzed the transfer of both respondent elicitation and avoidance-evoking functions

    simultaneously (in the same task and with the same participants). Published work has

    focused on either one or the other.

    The present study attempts to fill this gap, with two goals: first, to design an

    experimental analogue of the acquisition and derived transfer of both respondent fear-

    elicitation and avoidance-evoking functions by adapting well-known laboratory

    procedures within the research area of derived relational responding; second, and most

    importantly, to examine whether respondent fear extinction will reduce the likelihood of

    subsequent avoidance responding (as an analogue of exposure techniques). Two

    experiments were conducted with electric shocks as unconditioned aversive stimulation

    during conditioning phases. In contrast to what was done in previous studies, we

    measured both elicited (skin conductance) andoperant (avoidance and approach)

    responses throughout the procedure. In Experiment 1, we tested whether respondent

    extinction in the same context in which fear responses had been acquired would lead to

    the alteration of avoidance behavior in a different context (in which avoidance had been

    trained). Given the low impact of this procedure on avoidance responding, in

    Experiment 2 we trained a relation of similarity between the context in which

    respondent extinction occurred and the context in which avoidance responses were

    available.

    EXPERIMENT 1

    Method

    Participants

    Eighteen undergraduates (13 females; age range = 19-25) attending different

    courses (e.g., introductory psychology, maths, law) at Universidad de Almera

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    9/60

    8

    participated in the experiment. None of them had previous experience with the

    procedures employed in this study. They were recruited through in-class and on-campus

    flyer announcements, and each of them received 10 Euro for participation. At the

    beginning, all participants read and signed a consent form informing them that they

    would receive mild shocks and that they were free to discontinue participation at any

    time without having to give up the 10 Euro they received in return. Upon completion of

    the tasks, participants were fully debriefed.

    Setting, Apparatus, and Stimuli

    The setting, apparatus, and stimuli involved in this series of experiments were

    almost identical to those in Rodrguez-Valverde et al. (2009); thus the following

    description will mainly focus on their different features. The experiment was run in a

    laboratory consisting of two adjacent rooms (an experimental cubicle and an

    observation room) with a two-way mirror for participant observation. All visual stimuli

    were presented on an HP nx9010 laptop computer (15 in color screen). Skin

    conductance was measured and recorded according to the constant voltage technique

    (0.5V) of exosomatic recording (see Dawson, Schell, & Filion, 1990) through a

    computerized physiological recording system (BIOPAC Instruments) with non-

    polarizable Ag/AgCl finger electrodes attached to the palmar side of the distal phalanx

    of the first and third fingers of the participants nondominant hand. An isolated square-

    wave stimulator (Laffayette 82415-IS) was used for the delivery of constant voltage

    electric shocks (450 ms duration) through two disposable adhesive round electrodes

    attached to the inner surface of the participants non-dominant arm (see Rodrguez-

    Valverde et al., 2009, p. 88).

    The visual stimuli were 18 black shapes, each framed in a square white

    background (see Figure 1), presented on a general black background. The size of the

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    10/60

    9

    stimuli was 88 cm2. For ease of communication each stimulus was designated with an

    alphanumerical label (e.g., A1, A2, A3). Participants never saw these labels.

    Procedure

    All procedures were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee for

    Research with Human Participants at Universidad de Almeria. Upon arrival at the

    laboratory and completion of the consent procedures, participants were escorted to the

    experimental room for the administration of the computer tasks. Experiment 1 consisted

    of six phases, all conducted in one session that lasted 150 to180 min approximately (see

    Figure 2). Participants were run individually.

    Phase 1: Conditional discrimination training. Participants were presented with a

    card containing the following instructions:

    During this task, a sample symbol will appear at the top of the computer screen

    followed by three more symbols along the bottom. Your job is to select the

    correct symbol at the bottom given the one at the top by using the keyboard:

    press the Z key to select the symbol on the left, the V key to select the symbol in

    the middle, and the M key to select the symbol on the right. When your selection

    is correct, the word Correct will appear on the screen. When your selection is

    incorrect, the word Incorrect will appear on the screen. Your job is to produce

    as many correct selections as possible.

    The experimenter then asked the participant to summarize what she would have

    to do during the task. If the participant did not describe her task correctly, the

    experimenter repeated the instructions and asked again. Once participants understood

    the instructions, the experimenter left the room and the task commenced. An arbitrary

    linear matching-to-sample (MTS) procedure was employed to establish two 6-member

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    11/60

    10

    equivalence classes (Class 1: A1-B1-C1-D1-E1-F1, and Class 2: A2-B2-C2-D2-E2-F2).

    We utilized 6 members per class so as to have a sufficient number of related stimuli to

    test the indirect or derived functions, whereas the linear conditional discrimination

    procedure was used in order to rule-out second or higher-order associative conditioning

    as possible explanations (e.g., Smyth, Barnes-Holmes, & Forsyth, 2006).

    The trained relations for both classes were A-B, B-C, C-D, D-E, and E-F. On

    any given trial, one sample stimulus (e.g., A1) appeared centred in the top third of the

    computer screen. Two seconds later, three comparison stimuli (e.g., B1, B2, and B3)

    appeared in the lower third of the screen with one in the middle, one on the left side and

    the other one on the right side. A third set of six stimuli designated with number 3 (i.e.,

    A3, B3, C3, etc.) was used as incorrect comparisons in the procedure, with no explicitly

    trained relations amongst them. The location of the comparison stimuli varied randomly

    across trials. Participants selection cleared the screen and produced the written

    feedback Correct or Incorrect. The feedback remained on the screen for 2 s, and an

    inter-trial interval (ITI) of 2 s preceded the next trial.

    The training sequence proceeded as follows. Each new relation (starting with

    A1-B1) was trained until the participant emitted two consecutive correct responses.

    Training with the same relational pair in Class 2 (e.g., A2-B2) followed until two

    consecutive correct responses were produced. Subsequently, both relational pairs (e.g.,

    A1-B1 and A2-B2) were presented in random order in blocks of four trials (two per

    relational pair), until completion of one block with 100% correct selections. This same

    sequence was repeated with the remaining relational pairs from each class (i.e., B1-C1,

    B2-C2, C1-D1, C2-D2, D1-E1, D2-E2, E1-F1, and E2-F2). Blocks of mixed trial-types

    (with equal number of trials for Class 1 and Class 2 relations) were interspersed as

    follows. After B-C training was completed, 4-trial blocks containing A-B and B-C

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    12/60

    11

    relations (one trial per relational pair) were presented until production of two

    consecutive blocks with 100% correct responses. After completion of C-D training, 16-

    trial blocks containing four A-B, four B-C, and eight C-D trials were presented until

    production of one block with 100% correct selections. After completion of D-E and E-F

    training, 4-trial blocks containing D-E and E-F relations (one per relational pair) were

    presented until production of two consecutive blocks with 100% correct selections. This

    was followed by the presentation of 6-trial blocks containing the C-D, D-E and E-F

    relations (one trial per relational pair) until two blocks with 100% correct responses

    were completed. Finally, blocks containing all the trained relations (i.e. A-B, B-C, C-D,

    D-E, and E-F) in random order were presented until completion of three consecutive 10-

    trial (one per relation) blocks with 100% correct selections.

    Phase 2: Respondent and avoidance/approach conditioning with A and B

    stimuli. During this phase, A1 and B1 served as CSs+ (i.e., were followed by shock),

    and A2 and B2 served as CSs- (i.e., they were followed by points, exchangeable upon

    experiment completion for university canteen vouchers). Skin conductance responses

    (SCRs, measured in S) and avoidance responses served as dependent variables.

    Respondent conditioning. This part of Phase 2 started with a shock setup

    procedure in order to select the shock level that would be used as unconditional

    stimulation. Participants were fitted with the SCR recording and shock delivery

    electrodes (see Setting, Apparatus, and Stimuli). They were told that the purpose of this

    stage was to select an uncomfortable but not painful shock level to be used during this

    and subsequent phases of the experiment. The shock generator was set to 20V (levels

    ranged from 10 to 100V), and a brief shock was administered. If the participants did not

    rate the shock as uncomfortable, the shock voltage was gradually increased in 20V steps

    until the participants reported that the dispensed shock was uncomfortable but not

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    13/60

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    14/60

    13

    conductance to stabilize. During the ITI the screen remained black. A total of two 4-trial

    (1 per stimulus, in random order) blocks were presented.

    Avoidance/Approach training. Once the previous 8 conditioning trials were

    completed the experimenter entered the experimental cubicle and read aloud the

    following instructions:

    From now on, you will at times have the opportunity to avoid the shock by using

    the keyboard. Likewise, if you want to keep accumulating points, you will have

    to use the keyboard. Those opportunities will be signalled by a change in the

    appearance of the white circle located at the top left of the screen. At times, it

    will progressively turn green. When the circle is completely green, then you can

    avoid the shock by pressing the Q key with your free hand, and you can keep

    accumulating points by pressing the P key. These keys will only be operative

    when the circle is completely green. If you press before the circle has completely

    turned green, the computer will count that as an error and you will have to start

    over. Note as well that the keys will be operative for a very limited time after the

    circle is fully green. This is a long phase and you have a limited number of Q

    presses available, so use them only when you are sure that the shock will be

    delivered. Not all stimuli are followed by shock, so it is important to respond in

    accordance with what you have learned in the previous phase and with what you

    will be learning in this one. Once again, it is extremely important to sit as still

    as possible because any movement will disrupt the measurement of

    physiological responses. Also, remember that if you find yourself becoming

    upset at any time and would like to end your participation, you can call out and

    I will stop the procedure. Do you have any questions?

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    15/60

    14

    Once the experimenter ensured participants understanding of the instructions,

    participants pressed the space bar and the computer screen remained blank for 2 min for

    the stabilization of skin conductance level (SCL). Then, the avoidance/approach

    training started.

    A typical operant trial was as follows: An A or B stimulus was presented on the

    screen for 8 s. During the first 4 s, the circle at the top left of the screen remained white,

    and neither the avoidance nor the approach key was functioning (see Figure 3). During

    the last 4 s (transition interval) the circle was progressively filled with green and both

    keys remained inoperative. The transition interval was intended to signal the upcoming

    availability of the operant response once the colour transition was complete. During the

    8 s interval, changes in skin conductance level were measured. Once the circle was

    completely green, the avoidance and approach keys were operative for 1 s. During this

    time, participants could avoid shock in the presence of A1 and B1 by pressing the Q key

    (i.e., the avoidance response). Shock was delivered if they pressed no key or pressed the

    wrong one (P). Likewise, they could earn points in the presence of A2 and B2 by

    pressing P (i.e., the approach response). If a participant failed to emit the approach

    response, then the message Number of Points: 0 was displayed for 1.5 s. After that,

    the screen went blank for a 25-35 s ITI and a new trial began.

    Training started with one 16-trial block containing 4 respondent conditioning

    trials (one per stimulus; A1, B1, A2, B2) intermixed with 12 operant (avoidance and

    approach) conditioning trials (3 per stimulus) presented in random order. Respondent

    conditioning trials were intermixed in order to avoid the extinction of conditioned

    physiological responses likely acquired during the first part of this phase. A block was

    considered correct when participants produced avoidance responses on all A1/B1 trials

    (and not on any A2/B2 trial) as well as when they produced approach responses on all

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    16/60

    15

    but two (maximum) A2/B2 trials (and never on A1/B1 trials). We adopted a less strict

    criterion for the approach response because our main target was the acquisition and

    extinction of aversive functions. After one correct block, participants were presented

    with a second identical 16-trial block. Participants who failed to meet criterion in the

    first block of mixed trials went through an extra respondent conditioning block (4 trials)

    before the second block of mixed trials. Participants who failed at this second block too,

    were dropped from further participation.

    Phase 3: Test for the transfer of respondent and avoidance/approach functions

    to D and F stimuli. Phase 3 began immediately after Phase 2 and it only included

    operant trials. These were identical to the ones presented in Phase 2, with the sole

    exception that instead of A and B, D and F stimuli were used. Participants were first

    presented with one 4-trial block with D1 and D2 (2 trials per stimulus in random order).

    If they failed to emit the avoidance and/or approach responses (as expected according to

    the purportedly established equivalence relations), then consequences were delivered as

    with the A and B stimuli (i.e., shock was administered after D1 and the zero-point

    written feedback was displayed after D2). A 4-trial block with F1 and F2 (2 trials per

    stimulus in random order) followed. The criterion for the transfer of avoidance was that

    participants produced avoidance responses on all F1 trials andapproach responses on

    all F2 trials. Participants who met the transfer criterion proceeded to the next phase;

    participants who did not, were dropped from further participation.

    Phase 4: Respondent extinction with A1 and B1. This phase started immediately

    after Phase 3. As with respondent conditioning trials in Phase 2, A or B stimuli

    appeared in the middle of the screen for 6 s in the presence of the white circle. The

    difference was that during this phase neither A1 nor B1 were followed by shock, while

    A2 and B2 were still followed by points. The application of the extinction procedure

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    17/60

    16

    only with A1 and B1 was intended to have a within-subject control of changes in

    responding in the next phase of the experiment (i.e., to see if participants would stop

    showing avoidance by not pressing the Q key in the presence of Class 1 stimuli, but

    would still press the P key to obtain points in the presence of Class 2 stimuli).

    Participants were presented with two 12-trial blocks (3 trials per stimulus in random

    order).The extinction criterion was established as follows: the average difference in skin

    conductance between Class 1 and Class 2 members during the last three trials of each

    class should be less than 0.05 S. However, it was not possible to assess whether

    participants had achieved this criterion until the whole procedure had finished.

    Accordingly, all participants in Phase 4 proceeded to Phase 5.

    Phase 5: Critical Test. Test for avoidance/approach functions after respondent

    extinction. The purpose of this phase was to examine the effect of respondent extinction

    with A1 and B1 on avoidance responding to these and other arbitrarily related

    (equivalent) stimuli (D1 and F1). This phase started immediately following Phase 4.

    Trials had the same format as the operant trials in Phase 2 (avoidance/approach

    training), with the difference that shock was never presented, regardless of the

    participants responses. First, a 2-trial block with the B stimuli was presented (first B1

    and then B2; this sequence was maintained across participants). If participants produced

    avoidance responses (i.e., pressed the Q key) in the presence of B1they were

    immediately re-exposed to respondent extinction (two 4-trial blocks with A and B

    stimuli, with the same format as in Phase 4). Subsequently they were presented with a

    new 2-trial block with B1 and B2 to test for the avoidance response functions of B1.

    This sequence of re-exposure to respondent extinction was repeated up to three times if

    participants kept avoiding in the presence of B1. After that, they were dropped from

    further participation. On the contrary, if participants did not show avoidance in the

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    18/60

    17

    presence of B1, they continued to a 2-trial block with A stimuli (first A2 and then A1,

    with this sequence maintained across participants). As with the B stimuli, avoidance (in

    this case with A1) entailed re-exposure to respondent extinction up to a maximum of

    three times, and non-avoidance granted access to the next block (one 4-trial block with

    the sequence B2, A1, B1, A2).

    Only participants who did not show avoidance with A1 nor with B1 proceeded

    to the test with D, E, and F stimuli. Those participants who had passed the transfer of

    avoidance/approach test in Phase 3 (i.e., who had pressed the avoidance key on the first

    D1 trial) were presented with the following test sequence: D1, D2, F1, E1, F2, E2.

    Those participants who had not passed the transfer test in Phase 3 (and thus received

    shock after the first presentation of D1) were presented with the following test

    sequence: F1, F2, E1, E2, D2, D1. As with A1 and B1, no shock was delivered.

    Phase 6: Equivalence test. Mutual and combinatorial relations were tested using

    the same trial format as in conditional discrimination training (Phase 1), with the

    difference that no feedback was provided in any trial. The experimenter read aloud the

    following instructions:

    As in a previous phase, you will see one stimulus at the top of the screen and

    then three stimuli at the bottom. Please look at the stimulus at the top and then

    choose one stimulus from the bottom by clicking on it with the mouse. There is

    always a correct answer, but this time the computer will not tell you whether

    your choice is correct or not. Answer according to what you have learned in a

    previous phase and try to accumulate as many correct responses as possible.

    The twenty possible combinatorial relations per class (i.e. 10 transitive and 10

    equivalence relations: A-C, C-A, A-D, D-A, A-E, E-A, A-F, F-A, B-D, D-B, B-E, E-B,

    B-F, F-B, C-E, E-C, C-F, F-C, D-F, F-D) were first tested in a 40-trial block (one per

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    19/60

    18

    relation, in random order). The test finished upon production of a minimum of 34

    correct responses. Otherwise, a test for symmetry relations (five relations per class) was

    presented next in one 10-trial block (one trial per relation in random order). Upon

    production of at least eight correct responses, a new block of 40 combinatorial test trials

    was presented. Participants failing to achieve the criteria in either the symmetry or the

    second combinatorial block were deemed as not passing the equivalence test. After that,

    the experiment finished.

    Skin conductance response quantification

    Response amplitude was the parameter selected for quantification according to

    the following criteria: the largest increase in SCL (measured in Siemens [S]) was

    calculated for each trial in Phases 2 to 5. This variation was measured from the point of

    response onset to the highest SCL value within the following time periods:

    (1) For Phases 2, 3, and 5, the 4-s period during which the circle transitioned

    from white to green. Pilot work showed that once the avoidance contingencies were in

    place, autonomic activation mainly took place during this period, and not during the

    first 4-s interval (white circle).

    (2) For Phase 4, during the 6-s period in which A and B stimuli were displayed

    on the screen (always in the presence of the white circle), only if the response onset

    point started at least 0.5 s after visual stimulus onset.

    Negative variations in SCL during these measurement intervals were quantified

    as zero.

    Data Analysis

    Individual and group analyses of the data were conducted to examine the effect

    of respondent extinction on avoidance responding, both directly trained and acquired by

    derived means. The primary datum for individual analyses was the percentage of

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    20/60

    19

    participants that met the specified criterion in each phase. Our main focus was on

    individual adjustment to the successive manipulations performed through the

    experiment, respondent extinction being the most important one. This reduced the

    number of participants whose performance was suitable for analysis through the last

    test. Specifically, during Phase 4 (respondent extinction), we only considered data from

    participants who (1) had learned during Phase 2 (respondent and avoidance/approach

    conditioning) to produce avoidance responses to elements of Class 1 and approach

    responses to elements of Class 2 andalso had shown higher elicited SCRs to elements

    of Class 1 than to elements of Class 2; and (2) during Phase 3 (Transfer Test) had

    shown transfer of the avoidance/approach functions to Class 1/Class 2 members,

    respectively. Likewise, in Phase 5 (Critical Test) we only analysed the performance of

    participants for whom respondent extinction had been effective during Phase 4, in order

    to examine the impact of successful respondent extinction on subsequent avoidance

    responding. A detailed description of the specific achievement criteria and of the

    number of participants whose data were analysed in each phase is presented in the

    Results and Discussion section.

    As mentioned previously in the procedure section, participants who did not meet

    the operant criterion were dropped from further participation. It is important to note,

    however, that the SCR data were available to the experimenters only upon completion

    of the experimental tasks. The analysis of SCR data showed that some participants who

    had met the operant criterion and thus completed all phases, had not met the respondent

    conditioning or extinction criteria. Accordingly, their performance was not included in

    the analyses. All the available individual data are presented in Appendix 1.

    As to the analysis of group data, we calculated both the average number of

    avoidance responses and the average SCRs to the elements of Class 1 and Class 2 in

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    21/60

    20

    each phase, for phases 2 to 5. All participants who produced avoidance and approach

    responses correctly as expected according to the experimental design across phases (i.e.,

    phases 2, 3, and 5) entered the analysis, regardless of whether or not they also met the

    respondent conditioning or extinction criteria. This was intended to tracking the

    synchronicity between elicited arousal responses and avoidance responding. One-

    sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov tests were conducted on all pertinent SCR and avoidance

    response datasets to determine if the data fulfilled the normality assumption. Where the

    normality assumption was met, related samples T tests were conducted on avoidance

    responses and on SCRs in order to establish comparisons between classes within the

    same phase, and within classes across phases. Otherwise, the non-parametric Wilcoxon

    Signed-Rank Test was conducted for this purpose. In each case, Cohens d was

    calculated for statistically significant results in order to determine the effect size of

    successive manipulations. Following Cohens (1988) guidelines, .2, .5, and .8 were used

    as thresholds to define small, medium and large effects, respectively.

    Results and Discussion

    Conditional discrimination training(Phase 1)

    All 18 participants met the training criterion. The total number of trials to

    criterion varied across participants from 117 trials (P12) to 301 trials (P4) (see

    Appendix 1, Phase 1).

    Respondent and Avoidance/Approach Conditioning with A1 and B1 Stimuli (Phase 2)

    The criterion to determine if respondent conditioning was acquired for each

    participant was: larger SCRs to A1 and B1 (CSs+) than to A2 and B2 (CSs-) in more

    than half of the conditioning trials, with an average difference of at least 0.05 S

    between Class 1 and Class 2 stimuli. This criterion (adapted from Rodrguez-Valverde

    et al., 2009) had to be achieved in any of the avoidance conditioning blocks, each of

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    22/60

    21

    which had 12 trials (six avoidance and six approach trials). As previously mentioned,

    the operant conditioning criterion was that participants consistently pressed the

    avoidance key in the presence of A1/B1 and the approach key in the presence of A2/B2.

    Seventeen out of 18 participants (see Figure 4, Phase 2, and Appendix 1) met the

    avoidance/approach criterion (94.4%). Of these, 14 (82.3%) met the respondent

    conditioning criterion. Figure 5 (upper and lower graphs) shows that the percentage of

    avoidance responses to Class 1 stimuli (M= 97.22, SD = 2.78) was significantly larger

    than to Class 2 stimuli (M= 2.77, SD = 7.00;Z= -3.83,p< .000, d= 16.03); and that the

    average SCR to Class 1 stimuli (M= .61, SD = .59) was significantly larger than to

    Class 2 stimuli (M= .12, SD = .14; t= 4.13,p= .001, d= 1.13) for those participants

    who met the avoidance/approach criterion (N= 17).

    Transfer of Respondent and Avoidance/Approach functions to D and F(Phase 3).

    The criterion to determine the occurrence of transfer relied entirely on the first

    exposure to each test stimulus. The rationale for adopting this criterion has been

    presented elsewhere (see Rodrguez-Valverde et al., 2009, pp. 96-97).

    Transfer of Avoidance/Approach functions.Of the 14 participants who showed

    both avoidance/approach and respondent conditioning during Phase 2, 11 (78.6%)

    produced avoidance/approach responses during F1 and F2 presentations. Six of them

    (54.5%) also showed transfer with D1 and D2: P17, P1, P10, P12, P5, and P16 (see

    Figure 4, phase 3, and Appendix 1).

    Statistical analyses (Figure 5, upper graph) showed that the percentage of

    avoidance responding (pressing Q) to the first presentation of a Class 1 stimulus (M=

    78.57, SD = 32.21) was significantly larger than to the first presentation of a Class 2

    stimulus (M= 10.71, SD = 28.95;Z= -3.15,p = .002, d= 2.21).

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    23/60

    22

    Transfer of respondent elicitation functions. The criterion to determine that

    transfer had occurred was similar to that established for respondent conditioning: larger

    SCRs to D1 and F1 than to D2 and F2, respectively, during the first presentation of each

    stimulus (adapted from Rodriguez-Valverde et al., 2009). Six participants out of the 11

    (54.5%) who showed transfer of avoidance/approach functions also showed transfer of

    respondent elicitation functions.

    Statistical analyses revealed that SCRs to Class 1 stimuli (M= .90, SD = .93)

    were larger (marginally significant) than to Class 2 stimuli (M= .36, SD = .39; t= 2.16,

    p = .056, d= .76) for those participants who met the transfer of avoidance/approach

    criterion (N= 11).

    Respondent Extinction with A1 and B1 (Phase 4)

    The criterion for respondent extinction was that the difference in average SCRs

    between Class 1 and Class 2 stimuli was less than 0.05 S during the last three trials per

    class. Nine out of the 10 participants who showed transfer of avoidance/approach

    functions during Phase 3 (data from P12 were lost during the extinction phase) met the

    respondent extinction criterion (see Appendix 1, Phase 4).

    Statistical analyses showed that SCRs to Class 1 stimuli (M= .039, SD = .056)

    did not differ from those to Class 2 stimuli (M= .036, SD = .054; t= -.15,p = .89)

    during the last three trials (see Figure 5, lower graph). No participant emitted

    avoidance/approach responses during extinction trials.

    Critical Test: Effect of Respondent Extinction with A1 and B1 on Avoidance/Approach

    (Phase 5)

    Only three (P4, P5, and P6) out of the nine (33.3%) participants who had shown

    extinction of elicited SCRs to A1 and B1did not show avoidance responding in the

    presence of these stimuli during the critical test. The other six still produced avoidance

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    24/60

    23

    responses when presented with A1 and B1 (and four of them showed larger SCRs to

    these stimuli too) in Phase 5. Approach responding to A2 and B2 remained intact for all

    participants (see Figure 4 and Appendix 1, phase 5. Detailed data from all participants

    are in Appendix 1).

    Besides, in what regards the stimuli whose avoidance-evoking functions had

    been indirectly acquired (i.e., D1, E1, and F1), only the data from two of the three

    participants were available (P6 quit before the end of the experiment). Neither P4 nor

    P5 showed avoidance responding in the presence of D1, F1, and E1.

    Overall, statistical analyses revealed significant differences between the

    percentages of avoidance responses produced by the nine participants to Class 1 (M=

    66.67, SD = 50) and Class 2 stimuli (M= 0, SD = 0;Z= -2.5,p = .014, d= 1.89, see

    Figure 5). SCRs during the first test trial were significantly larger to Class 1 stimuli (M

    = .72, SD = .72) than to Class 2 stimuli (M= .12, SD = .17; t= 2.61,p = .031, d= 1.15).

    A significant difference was also found between SCRs to Class 1 stimuli during

    respondent extinction (Phase 4, last three trials in the presence of the white circle) and

    SCRs to Class 1 stimuli during the Critical Test (Phase 5, first stimulus presentation in

    the presence of the green circle) for all nine participants (N= 9, t= -2.89,p = .02, d=

    1.34). The corresponding within-class comparison with Class 2 stimuli yielded no

    significant changes across phases (t= -1.15,p = .28).

    It is important to note that the Critical Test was programmed so as to drop

    participants who continued avoiding in the presence of B1. As a result, we missed a

    substantial amount of data regarding avoidance/approach responses and elicited SCRs

    to the derived stimuli D, E, and F; the same applies to the equivalence class formation

    data collected in Phase 6. Only the data for P4 and P5, who had survived to this phase,

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    25/60

    24

    were collected. In both cases, the equivalence criterion was met within the first block of

    trials.

    These results indicate that respondent extinction of conditioned SCRs to A1 and

    B1 in the white context had very little effect on avoidance responding in the green

    context. The analyses also show that more than half of the participants who ran through

    the Critical Test showed fear renewal (measured as larger SCRs to Class 1 stimuli)

    when moved from the white to the green context. Approach responses and small SCRs

    to Class 2 stimuli, however, remained constant across contexts. This is consistent with

    previous studies showing that respondent extinction is contextually controlled and that

    conditioned fear is susceptible to the renewal effect when a change in context occurs

    (Hermans et al., 2006; Vansteenwegen et al., 2006). It is likely that in the present

    experiment, for most participants, the functions acquired by the white context during

    respondent conditioning and extinction were different or opposite to the functions

    acquired by the green context during avoidance conditioning. This might explain why

    respondent extinction of SCRs in the white context did not generalize to the green

    context for many participants, and why avoidance was still in place for most

    participants. The participants informal post-task reports seem to support this

    conclusion. For instance, some of the participants who continued to show avoidance

    responding in the critical test said that they had pressed the key because that was what

    they had done before in the green context. Others said that even though they were pretty

    sure that the shock would not be delivered, they were pressing just in case.

    With the same goal of Experiment 1, in Experiment 2 we trained a relation of

    similarity between the white (i.e., respondent conditioning and extinction) context and

    the green (i.e., avoidance) context, in order to examine whether this might facilitate

    preventing avoidance responding upon a history of respondent extinction.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    26/60

    25

    EXPERIMENT 2

    As mentioned above, it is likely that most participants in Experiment 1 produced

    avoidance responses during the Critical Test because the extinction and the avoidance

    contexts had different functions, which probably resulted in a fear renewal effect.

    Experiment 2 was designed to prevent such difference between contexts by training a

    relation of similarity between the white and the green circle (i.e., the extinction and the

    avoidance context, respectively). Extrapolating to the applied arena, this might be

    analogue to instructing an anxious client that the context where they are undergoing

    exposure therapy and the context where they usually show pathological forms of

    avoidance are similar, so that similar reactions would be expected in both cases.

    Additionally, in Experiment 2 all participants who got to the Critical Test went

    on to complete the experiment, regardless of their performance in the first trials of the

    Critical Test (i.e., whether or not they had avoided in the presence of B1 or A1).

    Method

    Participants

    Twenty-five undergraduates (13 females; age range = 18-26) were recruited,

    welcomed, compensated for participation, and debriefed as in Experiment 1.

    Procedure

    The procedures employed in Experiment 2 were almost identical to those in

    Experiment 1, with two exceptions: (a) a procedure for assessing and training a relation

    of similarity between the white and the green contexts was introduced in Phase 4; and

    (b) unlike in Experiment 1, all participants who got to the Critical Test (Phase 5)

    completed it, including test trials with the directly conditioned stimuli (i.e., A and B)

    and with the derived stimuli (i.e., D, E, and F),and also completed the test for the

    formation of equivalence classes.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    27/60

    26

    Phase 4. Assessment and training of a similarity relation between the green and

    white contexts. After the test for the transfer of respondent and avoidance/approach

    functions with D and F Stimuli (Phase 3), there was an assessment and subsequent

    training of the similarity relation between the green and the white contexts. The

    assessment included both open-ended and close-ended questions. First, there were four

    open-ended questions: the computer screen showed A1 or B1 or A2 or B2 in the

    presence of either the green or the white circle, with the stimuli arranged as they

    appeared during conditioning trials (Experiment 1, Phases 2 and 3). The experimenter

    then asked: What came to mind immediately upon seeing this? and wrote down the

    participants responses. The close-ended format assessment was next. The experimenter

    asked participants to pay attention to the screen and to select the most appropriate

    response option by clicking on it with the mouse, and then left the experimental room.

    Figure 6 shows the typical arrangement of stimuli on the computer screen during any

    given close-ended assessment trial. At the top third of the screen, a message reminded

    participants to select the most appropriate response option according to their experience

    in previous phases. Below, two pictures were displayed in the middle of the screen. One

    of them depicted either A1 or B1 in the presence of the white circle, and the other one

    depicted the same stimulus in the presence of the green circle (see Figure 6). The

    response options (they are similar, they are opposite, or neither) were located

    below, along the lower third of the screen (with their position varying randomly across

    trials). Participants selections cleared the screen for 2 s, and were followed by the start

    of a new trial. The close-ended assessment consisted of two trials, one per stimulus (i.e.,

    A1 and B1). After that, participants underwent training of a similarity relation between

    the green and the white circles. The trial format was the same as during close-ended

    assessment, with two exceptions. First, an initial message instructed participants to keep

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    28/60

    27

    selecting one of the three options and to accumulate as many consecutive correct

    responses as possible. Second, correct selections (i.e., they are similar) cleared the

    screen and produced the written feedback Correct for 2 s; incorrect selections

    produced the feedback Wrong. Training proceeded in blocks of two trials (one per

    stimulus, A1 and B1, in random order) until participants produced two consecutive

    blocks with 100% correct responses.

    Once the similarity training was over, participants underwent respondent

    extinction with A1 and B1 as in Experiment 1. In this case, however, upon termination

    of the first 12-trial extinction block, and before the second 12-trial extinction block,

    similarity blocks (two trials per stimulus: A1 and B1) were inserted in order to retrain

    the similarity relation between the green and white contexts. They were repeatedly

    presented until the participant responded correctly on all trials across two consecutive

    blocks.

    Phase 5: Critical Test. Effect of Respondent Extinction with A1 and B1 (plus

    context similarity training) on Avoidance/Approach. This test was conducted as in

    Experiment 1, with the only difference that both the elicited SCRs and

    avoidance/approach responses to theD, F, and E stimuli were tested regardless of

    whether participants had previously shown avoidance responses to the directly

    conditioned A1 and B1 in this same test (see Experiment 1, Phase 5, for sequence

    details).

    Results and Discussion

    Conditional Discrimination Training

    All 25 participants met the training criterion. The number of trials necessary to

    reach the criterion varied from 89 (P12) to 300 trials (P16) (see Appendix 2, Phase 1).

    Respondent and Avoidance/Approach Conditioning with A1 and B1 stimuli

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    29/60

    28

    As shown in Figure 7 (see also Appendix 2, Phase 2), 24 participants (96%) met

    the avoidance/approach criterion. That is, they avoided both A1 and B1, and approached

    both A2 and B2 (see criterion in the Results section of Experiment 1). Of these 24, 15

    (62.5%) showed differential SCR conditioning (larger SCRs to A1 and B1 than to A2

    and B2).

    Figure 8 (upper and lower graphs) shows that the percentage of avoidance

    responses to Class 1 stimuli(M= 98.66, SD = 3.12) was significantly larger than that to

    Class 2 stimuli (M= 3.33, SD = 8.83; Z = -4.58, p < .000, d = 14.43); also, that SCRs to

    Class 1 stimuli (M= .11, SD = .13) were significantly larger than those to Class 2

    stimuli (M= .04, SD = .05;Z= -3.94,p< .000, d= .71) for participants who met the

    avoidance/approach criterion (N= 24).

    Transfer of Respondent and Avoidance/Approach Functions to D and F

    As in Experiment 1, only the results of the participants who met both the

    avoidance/approach and respondent criteria during the conditioning phase (15 out of 24)

    were analysed; however, the results for the nine remaining participants who achieved

    the operant criterion are detailed in Appendix 2.

    Transfer of avoidance/approach functions. Eleven out if 15 participants (73.3%)

    produced avoidance responses to F1 and/or D1, and approach responses to F2 and/or D2

    (P3, P5, P6, P16, P17, P21, P22, and P25, with both the F and D stimuli) (see Figure 7

    and Appendix 2).

    Statistical analyses (Figure 8, upper graph) showed that the percentage of

    avoidance responding to the first presentation of Class 1 stimuli (D1 and F1;M= 85.71,

    SD = 30.56) was significantly larger than toClass 2 stimuli (D2 and F2;M= 3.57, SD =

    13.36;Z= -3.36,p = .001, d= 3.48).

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    30/60

    29

    Transfer of respondent elicitation functions. A shown in Figure 7, five of the 11

    participants (45.8%) who had shown transfer of avoidance functions also showed larger

    SCRs to the first presentation of D1 and F1 relative to the first presentation of D2 and

    F2. However, statistical analysis yielded no significant differences between SCRs to

    Class 1 stimuli (M= .15, SD = .10) and to Class 2 stimuli (M= .11, SD = .13; t=.92,p

    = .38). That is, transfer of avoidance and approach responses to non-directly

    conditioned Class 1 and Class 2 stimuli occurred even in the absence of a differential

    pattern of elicited arousal responses between classes.

    As in Experiment 1, the data for all participants who met the operant criteria

    (avoidance/approach responding) are presented in Appendix 2.

    Assessment and Training of Similarity between Green and White Contexts, and

    Respondent Extinction with A1 and B1

    The results obtained in the assessment of the relation between the white and

    green contexts showed great variability across participants. After training the similarity

    relation between contexts, however, all 11 participants selected the option they are

    similar (100% correct, see criterion). As for respondent extinction with A1 and B1, all

    but one of the 11 participants (90.9%) who had shown transfer of avoidance/approach

    functions met the respondent extinction criterion (see Appendix 2, Phase 4). That is, the

    difference between average SCR to Class 1 and Class 2 stimuli was less than .05 S

    during the last three trials per class.

    Statistical analyses showed that SCRs to Class 1 stimuli (M= .038, SD = .067)

    did not differ from those to Class 2 stimuli (M= .034, SD = .042; t= .28) during the last

    three trials for these 10 participants (see Figure 8).

    Critical Test: Effect of Respondent Extinction with A1 and B1 on Avoidance/Approach

    and Respondent functions

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    31/60

    30

    As shown in Figure 7 (see also Appendix 2, phase 5), only one (P8) out of the 10

    participants (10%) who had shown extinction of conditioned SCRs to A1 and B1

    stopped showing avoidance responding in the presence of the same stimuli during the

    Critical Test. The remaining 9 participants still showed avoidance responding when

    presented with A1 and B1. However, only one participant (P25) showed larger SCRs to

    A1 and B1 than to A2 and B2. As in Experiment 1, the approach response to the stimuli

    in Class 2 remained intact for all participants.

    As for the test for avoidance/approach responding to the D, F, and E stimuli,

    participants showed an identical pattern of responding to that observed with A and B

    (see Figure 7). The available data (one participant quit before the end, and data from

    another two participants were lost; see Appendix 2 for details) show that the only

    participant who had not shown avoidance responding with A1 and B1 (P8), also did not

    with D1, F1, and E1; the six participants who showed avoidance responding in the

    presence of A1 and B1, also did in the presence of D1, F1, and E1. All participants

    produced approach responses in the presence of D2, F2 and E2, and none of them

    showed larger SCRs to Class 1 stimuli than to Class 2 stimuli.

    Statistical analyses revealed that the percentage of avoidance responses to A1

    and B1 (N= 10; Class 1:M= 90, SD = 31.62) was significantly larger than that to A2

    and B2 (M= 0, SD = 0;Z= -3,p = .003, d= 4.03). Likewise, the percentage of

    avoidance responses to F1, D1, and E1 (N= 7: Class 1:M= 85.71%, SD = 37.79) was

    significantly larger than that to F2, D2, and E2 (M= 0, SD = 0;Z= -2.5,p = .014, d=

    3.24) (see Figure 8, upper and lower graph). Also, SCRs to A1 and B1 (N= 10;M=

    .033, SD = .047) did not differ from those to A2 and B2(M= .018, SD = .014; t= .81,p

    = .45); likewise, SCRs to D1, F1, and E1 (N= 7;M= .027, SD = .019) did not differ

    from those to D2, F2, and E2 (M= .042, SD = .045; t= -1.31,p = .24). Lastly, no

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    32/60

    31

    significant differences between phases (Phase 4, respondent extinction: last three trials

    in the presence of the white circle, vs. Phase 5, Critical Test: first stimulus presentation

    in the presence of the green circle) were found for SCRs to Class 1 stimuli (t= -1.09,p

    = .318). In other words, there was no renewal of elicited SCRs when changing from the

    extinction context (i.e., white circle) to the avoidance context (i.e., green circle).

    Although data from participants who did not show respondent conditioning in

    Phase 2 were not considered in the analyses, it is interesting to note that six out of the

    eight who were exposed to the Critical Test (see Appendix 2) produced avoidance

    responses to both A1 and B1, and to the derived stimuli D1, E1, and F1 (although they

    still showed no evidence of respondent conditioning in this test).

    While a comparison of these data with those obtained in Experiment 1 is feasible

    only for the first stimulus presentation per class in the Critical Test (note that

    participants who produced avoidance responses to the first B1 presentation during the

    Critical Test in Experiment 1 were not tested any further), we may conclude that

    training the similarity relation between the white and the green context served to

    maintain extinction of SCRs in the green context, from the first to the last trial, for all

    but one of the 10 participants who made it to the Critical Test (see Appendix 2). That is,

    the similarity instruction prevented fear renewal when moving from the white to the

    green context. Most importantly, however, although participants did not show elicited

    SCRs to the test stimuli, they still produced avoidance responses in their presence.

    Given that in Experiment 2 all stimuli (either with direct or derived functions) were

    tested, it can be concluded that avoidance behavior remained intact in the absence of

    any noticeable increases in arousal (as measured by SCRs) for all participants but one.

    This seems consistent with clinical literature that has regarded avoidance, rather than

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    33/60

    32

    fear, as the key component in the maintenance of anxiety disorders (e.g., Hayes, 1976;

    Hayes et al., 1996; Forsyth et al., 2006; Powers, Smits, & Telch, 2004).

    Equivalence Test

    All participants but one (whose data were not relevant to the main analysis) who

    were tested for equivalence passed successfully (see Appendix 2, Phase 6).

    General Discussion

    The main purpose of this study was to examine the impact of respondent

    extinction on avoidance responding. Neither of the extinction protocols tested in

    Experiments 1 and 2 made a significant impact on avoidance. Before addressing the

    discussion of this finding, we will turn to commenting the results relating to the

    acquisition and transfer of fear and avoidance.

    Acquisition and transfer of fear and avoidance.

    Conditional discrimination training. All 43 participants (18 from Experiment 1

    and 25 from Experiment 2) met the conditional discrimination training criteria

    established for the formation of two six-member classes. As with similar experiments

    with smaller stimulus classes (e.g., Auguston & Dougher, 1997; Dymond, Roche,

    Forsyth, Whelan, & Rhoden, 2007; Roche et al., 2008; Rodriguez-Valverde et al.,

    2009), we observed substantial variability in the length of conditional discrimination

    training. It is worth noting that despite the large number of relations participants had to

    learn, training was relatively short. This was probably due to the use of an orderly

    sequence of trials in which each new relation was trained only after the participant had

    met the mastery criterion for the previous relation or set of relations being trained.

    Conditioning and transfer of respondent elicitation and avoidance-evoking

    functions. Taking the total amount of participants in both experiments as a whole, we

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    34/60

    33

    found that during conditioning 41 out of 43 participants met the avoidance/approach

    criterion, and that 29 of those also met the respondent conditioning criterion. In

    addition, a good number of participants (33 out of 41) showed transfer of

    avoidance/approach functions to non-directly conditioned stimuli (that is, showed

    avoidance to D1 or/and F1 and approach responses to D2 or/and F2), but only half of

    them (16) showed transfer of respondent elicitation (i.e., showed larger SCRs to

    D1/F1than to D2/F2). It is worth noting that the use of a differential conditioning

    procedure wherein Class 1 stimuli acquired aversive functions while Class 2 stimuli

    acquired appetitive functions, served methodological control purposes and was very

    effective in establishing clearly distinct responses for each class during conditioning and

    transfer. For instance, no participant produced approach responses to Class 1 stimuli,

    and there were almost no avoidance responses to Class 2 stimuli; besides, only minimal

    elicited SCRs to Class 2 stimuli were observed. Overall, these findings show that

    avoidance responding is easily acquired and that avoidance-evoking functions easily

    transfer to stimuli with no direct aversive conditioning history. More interestingly, they

    show that avoidance responding may occur and transfer in the absence of noticeable

    conditioned fear responses to the stimuli being avoided.

    These results replicate previous findings on the transfer of respondent elicitation

    and avoidance-evoking functions (e.g., Augustson & Dougher, 1997; Dougher et al.,

    1994; Dymond et al., 2007; Roche et al., 2008; Rodrguez-Valverde et al., 2009) and

    add to them in several respects. First, as in Auguston and Dougher (1997), a strict

    criterion was established to determine whether transfer had occurred, namely,

    performance on the first stimulus presentation of each class. The demonstration of

    transfer during the first test trial with any stimulus is an important proof of derived

    effects, because it is obtained prior to the participant experiencing any contingencies

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    35/60

    34

    (either experimentally implemented or inferred by the participant) with that very

    stimulus. Second, transfer of functions was assessed before the equivalence test, which

    rules out the impact of contiguity and second-order conditioning as potential

    explanations for transfer effects (see Dymond & Rehfeldt, 2000).

    The third and critical way these results add to previous findings relates to the

    fact that this study included the simultaneous measurement of both respondent and

    operant responses throughout the procedure. Although transfer of respondent extinction

    and of avoidance-evoking functions is a well-known phenomenon now, this is the first

    time that both types of responses were collected concurrently in the same experiment.

    Besides, this preparation is relevant because of the interesting results it has yielded in

    this regard, namely the lack of synchronicity between respondent elicitation and

    avoidance responding. As mentioned previously, many participants learned to produce

    avoidance responses to stimuli that did not elicit an increase in autonomic arousal (A

    and B stimuli), and showed derived transfer of these avoidance responses to other

    stimuli in an equivalence relation with the former (D and/or F), again in the absence of

    any increase in elicited autonomic arousal to these stimuli. The main difference with

    prior studies on the transfer of respondent elicitation (e.g. Dougher et al., 1994;

    Rodrguez-Valverde et al., 2009) was that in the present study participants could

    produce avoidance responses to prevent shock. Accordingly, this seems the most likely

    and parsimonious explanation for the observed lack of elicited SCRs. In other words, it

    might be the case that, for a significant number of participants, the eliciting functions of

    the stimuli (during both conditioning and the transfer tests) were altered by the

    availability of the opportunity to avoid. This is consistent with findings in the clinical

    literature showing that the availability of opportunities to engage in avoidance (in the

    form of safety behaviors) for patients with anxiety disorders may have a fear reduction

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    36/60

    35

    effect (e.g., Rachman, Radomsky, &Shafran, 2008). We agree with authors like De

    Houwer, Vandorpe, and Beckers (2005), and Hermans et al. (2006), that the analysis of

    verbal processes occurring during the experimental tasks (e.g., rules about the

    occurrence of shock or about how to avoid it) is key for understanding conditioning in

    humans. In our opinion, this sort of analysis may shed light on the reasons why

    avoidance behavior had a fear reduction effect in the presence of shock-related cues

    (cues that signaled impending shock). Propositional models like Lovibonds expectancy

    theory of avoidance (Lovibond, 2006) seem a particularly appropriate option (from a

    cognitive point of view) to explain these findings. Basically, during avoidance

    conditioning participants would formulate the rule that shock will not be presented once

    avoidance responding (that has been effective in the past) is produced. Accordingly,

    presence of a shock-signaling stimulus would not elicit significant fear if the participant

    were certain that they would have the opportunity to avoid (as was the case in these

    experiments with the green context).

    The effect of respondent extinction on avoidance behavior

    As previously mentioned, our main goal was to examine the impact of

    respondent extinction on both directly conditioned and derived avoidance, as an

    exploratory analogue of exposure therapy. Accordingly, for the analysis of the impact of

    respondent extinction, we only considered participants who (1) had shown differential

    SCR conditioning and avoidance conditioning to A1/B1 (in Phase 2); and (2) had

    produced derived avoidance responses to D1 and/or F1 (in Phase 3: transfer test).

    In Experiment 1, respondent extinction in the same context in which aversive

    conditioning took place did not prevent avoidance for 66.7% participants (6 out of 9). It

    is important to notice that most of them (4/6) showed a recovery of previously

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    37/60

    36

    extinguished elicited SCRs in the context of avoidance. This fear-renewal effect has

    been reported in the experimental and clinical literature on the context-sensitivity of

    extinction (Craske & Mystkowski, 2006; Hermans et al., 2006; Neumann &

    Longbottom, 2008; Vansteenwegen et al., 2006). The use of extinction in multiple

    contexts has been presented as a way to reduce fear renewal and hence avoidance

    (Hermans et al., 2006). In Experiment 2, we found that instructing participants that the

    avoidance context was similar to the extinction context, served to significantly reduce

    (almost eliminate) the fear-renewal effect (only one out of 10 participants showed

    elicited SCRs to Class 1 stimuli when back to the avoidance context). Contrarily to

    what is assumed, however, the reduction of elicited fear responses did not lead to less

    avoidance: nine out of 10 participants continued producing avoidance responses to

    Class 1 stimuli when back to the avoidance context. Apparently, context-similarity

    training served to potentiate the lack of synchronicity between fear and avoidance

    responses already observed in Experiment 1. Perhaps the participants expectations of

    being shocked againas most of them indicated at the end of the study by informal post-

    task reports played an important role (i.e., rules like: white and green are similar, but

    pressing Q with the green circle works to prevent shock for sure, so Ill keep doing it

    just in case). Again, we believe that this points to the potential relevance of further

    research on the role of verbal processes (i.e., of rules such as expectations, beliefs, or

    verbal formulation of past experiences) on avoidance conditioning and generalization.

    In addition, the present findings have shown that trying to control avoidance

    behavior just by extinguishing previously conditioned fear responses is a difficult

    endeavor. Exposure can be conceived of as a strategy intended not only to the extinction

    of conditioned fear responses, but also to disconfirming (i.e., changing the content of)

    rules about avoidance in the feared situation or event (e.g., Now this is safe; nothing

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    38/60

    37

    bad really happens if I stand the situation) (e.g., Salkovskis, Hackmann, Wells, Gelder,

    & Clark, 2007). In regard to this, we believe that it is worth considering the evidence

    showing the futility (at least in some situations and for some individuals) of attempts to

    changing the content of avoidance rules, and the potential utility of addressing the

    function of those rules (as an intervention target) as the key for the maintenance of

    pathological avoidance (Hayes, Wilson, et al., 1996). Accordingly, interventions aimed

    at altering the believability of avoidance rules may well be an alternative to traditional

    exposure-like techniques in targeting resistant forms of avoidance responding (see

    Luciano et al., 2012, for an experimental analogue preparation).

    We would like to point out some caveats in regard to the features of this study.

    First, the participants expectations were not measured throughout the procedure, so no

    information was obtained about what rules the participants were deriving and following

    when they continued avoiding in the absence of noticeable fear responses. Although

    concurrent measurement of expectancies has pros and cons (e.g., producing expectancy-

    ratings on a trial by trial basis might interfere with physiological measures of

    responding, or might have an effect on the very acquisition of conditioning), future

    research might provide creative ways to target the symbolic nature of these experiences,

    as indicated by De Houwer et al. (2005). Second, our preparation did not incorporate

    specific conflicting contingencies for avoidance and non-avoidance other than the

    presence or absence of shock. This was intended to make sure that avoidance was a

    direct consequence of the aversive elicitation functions of Class 1 stimuli, and not

    related to the loss of any additional short/long term appetitive consequences. However,

    future studies might test whether the occurrence of additional contingencies for non-

    avoidance enhances the effect of respondent extinction on avoidance responding. Third,

    the conditional discrimination training procedure employed here, presented both the

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    39/60

    38

    sample and comparison stimuli simultaneously on the screen. It could be argued that

    this might have resulted in unintended training of bidirectional relations, and thus that

    equivalence occurred on the basis of directly trained symmetry. There is evidence,

    however, that nonhumans and preverbal humans fail symmetry tests after conditional

    discrimination training in the same format as that employed in the present series.

    Additionally, no study has shown that the simultaneous presentation of sample and

    comparison stimuli is a critical variable in determining whether or not derived relations

    emerge during test trials. Finally, future studies might introduce a between-subjects

    control condition in which participants were not presented with respondent extinction,

    in order to gain further information about the role of such manipulation on subsequent

    avoidance responding.

    Conclusions

    While exposure therapy is the treatment of choice for anxiety disorders (e.g.,

    Deacon & Abramowitz, 2004), the processes that might be responsible of its

    effectiveness are still in need of further analysis for a complete understanding.

    Researchers have claimed for the need of analogue studies on how anxiety disorders are

    developed and also on the mechanisms of the exposure techniques used to treat those

    disorders (Abramowitz, in press; Craske & Mystkowski, 2006; Hermans et al., 2006).

    Here we have presented translational research in which a commonly reported clinical

    phenomenon, namely the use of exposure techniques and their effects on avoidance

    behavior, was translated into basic processes to be tested in the laboratory context.

    Specifically, with the present series of experiments we aimed at creating an analogue

    that functionally matched, as closely as possible, a clinical situation in which the

    therapist made in vivo exposure to the feared event in the same context where fear was

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    40/60

    39

    conditioned (white circle) but not in the context where avoidance behavior usually

    occurs (green circle). In such hypothetical clinical situation, treatment would be over

    once the patient successfully underwent successful extinction of conditioned fear

    responses to the feared object and related events, and she could stay calm in the

    conditioning/extinction context. However, what our results show is that as soon as the

    patient is back to the context where it is possible to engage in avoidance responding

    (green context), she continues to produce avoidance responses to the feared event and to

    other stimuli in an equivalence relation with the former (Experiment 1). Possibly, one

    common reaction by the clinician in such situation would be to work with the patient

    and teach her that the exposure context and the context in which avoidance occurs do

    not have to be different, that is, that just as she can stay calm in the exposure context,

    she could stay calm in the avoidance one (i.e. the instruction the white and green circle

    are similar in Experiment 2). But again, according to our results, although this seems to

    alleviate anxiety (reduction of fear responses), the patient would continue to show

    avoidance responding to the feared object when she had the chance to (i.e. the findings

    in Experiment 2). Given that in this experimental series we have found a very little

    effect of respondent extinction on avoidance, and in a attempt to further understand

    what exposure techniques are made of, and under which conditions they work,

    subsequent studies from our lab have incorporated a personal values context a

    meaning and the practice of defusion from thoughts and sensations about impending

    shock, in order to facilitate non-avoidance, with highly promising results (Luciano,

    Valdivia, et al., 2012). We hope that this line of research will improve our

    understanding of the process of change underlying the effectiveness of exposure

    techniques for the treatment for anxiety disorders.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    41/60

    40

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    42/60

    41

    References

    Abramowitz, J. S. (in press). The practice of exposure therapy: Relevance of cognitive-

    behavioral and extinction theory.Behavior Therapy. doi:

    10.1016/j.beth.2013.03.003

    Augustson, E. M., & Dougher, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking

    functions through stimulus equivalence classes.Journal of Behavior Therapy and

    Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 181197.

    Barlow, D. H. (2002).Anxiety and its disorders: The nature and treatment of anxiety

    and panic. New York: Guilford Press.

    Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral science (2nd

    ed.).

    Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Craske, M. G., & Mystkowski, J. L. (2006). Exposure therapy and extinction: clinical

    studies. In M. G. Craske, D. Hermans, & D. Vansteenwegen (Eds.),Fear and

    learning: From basic processes to clinical implications (pp. 217-233).

    Washington DC: American Psychological Association.

    Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (1990). The electrodermal system. In J.

    T. Cacioppo, & L. G. Tassinary (Eds.),Principles of psychophysiology (pp. 295-

    324). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    De Houwer, J. (2009). The propositional approach to associative learning as an

    alternative for association formation models.Learning & Behavior, 37, 1-20.

    De Houwer, J., Vandorpe, S., & Beckers, T. (2005). On the role of controlled cognitive

    processes in human associative learning. In A. J. Wills (Ed.),New directions in

    human associative learning(pp. 41-63). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum

    Associates.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    43/60

    42

    Deacon, B. J., & Abramowitz, J. S. (2004).Cognitive and behavioral treatments of

    anxiety disorders: a review of meta-analytic findings.Journal of Clinical

    Psychology, 60, 429-441.

    Dougher, M. J., Augustson, E., Markham, M. R., Greenway, D. E., &Wulfert, E.

    (1994). The transfer of respondent eliciting and extinction functions through

    stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior,

    62, 331351.

    Dougher, M. J., Hamilton, D. A., Fink, B. C., & Harrington, J. (2007). Transformation

    of the discriminative and eliciting functions of generalized relational stimuli.

    Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88, 179-197.

    Dymond, S., & Rehfeldt, R. A. (2000). Understanding complex behavior: The

    transformation of stimulus functions. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 239-254.

    Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2009).A contemporary behavior analysis of anxiety and

    avoidance.The Behavior Analyst, 32, 7-27.

    Dymond, S., Roche, B., Forsyth, J. P., Whelan, R., & Rhoden, J. (2007).

    Transformation of avoidance response functions in accordance with same and

    opposite relational frames.Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 88,

    249-262.

    Forsyth, J. P., Eifert, G. H., & Barrios, V. (2006). Fear conditioning in an emotion

    regulation context: A fresh perspective on the origins of anxiety disorders. In M.

    G. Craske, D. Hermans, & D. Vansteenwegen (Eds.),Fear and learning: From

    basic processes to clinical applications (pp. 133-153). Washington, DC:

    American Psychological Association.

    Hayes, S. C. (1976). The role of approach contingencies in phobic behavior.Behavior

    Therapy, 7, 28-36.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    44/60

    43

    Hayes, S. C., Wilson, K. G., Gifford, E. V., Follette, V. M., & Strosahl, K. D. (1996).

    Experiential avoidance and behavioral disorders: a functional dimensional

    approach to diagnosis and treatment.Journal of Consulting and Clinical

    Psychology, 64, 1152-1168.

    Hermans, D., Craske, M. G., Mineka, S., & Lovibond, P. F. (2006). Extinction in

    Human Fear Conditioning.Biological Psychiatry, 60, 361-368.

    Lovibond, P. F. (2006). Fear and avoidance: An integrated expectancy model. In M. G.

    Craske, D. Hermans, & D. Vansteenwegen (Eds.),Fear and learning: Basic

    science to clinical application (pp. 117-132). Washington, DC: American

    Psychological Association.

    Lovibond, P. F., Davis, N. R., & OFlaherty, A. S. (2000). Protection from extinction in

    human fear conditioning.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38, 967-983.

    Lovibond, P. F., Mitchell, C. J., Minard, E., Brady, A., &Menzies, R. G. (2009). Safety

    behaviours preserve threat beliefs: Protection from extinction of human fear

    conditioning by an avoidance response.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47,

    716-720.

    Luciano, C., Valdivia, S., Ruiz, F. J., Rodriguez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Dougher, M.

    J., et al. (2012). A laboratory demonstration of the benefits of using a defusion-

    based intervention to reduce avoidance behavior.Paper submitted for

    publication.

    Marks, I. M. (1981). Cure and care of neuroses. Wiley: New York

    Mowrer, O. H. (1960).Learning theory and behavior. New York: Wiley.

    Neumann, D. L., & Longbottom, P. L. (2008). The renewal of extinguished conditioned

    fear with fear-relevant and fear-irrelevant stimuli by a context change after

    extinction.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 188-206.

  • 7/28/2019 ACT. Extinction of Aversive Eliciting Functions as an. Luciano_et_al_in_press

    45/60

    44

    Powers, M. B., Smits, J. A. J., & Telch, M. J. (2004). Disentangling the effecs of safety-

    behavior utilization and safety-behavior availability during exposure-based

    treatments: A placebo-controlled trial.Journal of Consulting Clinical Psychology,

    72, 448-454.

    Rachman, S. (1977). The conditioning theory of fear-acquisition: a critical examination.

    Behaviour Research and Therapy, 15, 375-387.

    Rachman, S. (1991).Neo-conditioning and the classical theory of fear

    acquisition.Clinical Psychology Review, 11, 155-175.

    Rachman, S., Radomsky, A. S., & Shafran, R. (2008). Safety behavior: A

    reconsideration.Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 163-173.

    Roche, B., Kanter, J. W., Brown, K. R., Dymond, S., Fogarty, C. C. (2008). A

    comparison of direct versus derived extinction of avoidance. The

    Psychological Record, 58, 443-464.

    Rodrguez-Valverde, M., Luciano, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Transfer of

    aversive respondent elicitation in accordance with equivalence relations.Journal

    of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 92, 85-112.

    Salkovskis, P. M., Hackmann, A., Wells, A., Gelder, M. G., & Clark, D. M. (2007).