45
This is the author’s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for pub- lication in the following source: Campbell, Marilyn A., Slee, Phillip T., Spears, Barbara, Butler, Des,& Kift, Sally (2013) Do cyberbullies suffer too? Cyberbullies’ perceptions of the harm they cause to others and to their own mental health. School Psychology International, 34(6), pp. 613-629. This file was downloaded from: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/59983/ c Copyright 2013 SAGE Publications Ltd Notice: Changes introduced as a result of publishing processes such as copy-editing and formatting may not be reflected in this document. For a definitive version of this work, please refer to the published source: https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034313479698

Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

This is the author’s version of a work that was submitted/accepted for pub-lication in the following source:

Campbell, Marilyn A., Slee, Phillip T., Spears, Barbara, Butler, Des, & Kift,Sally(2013)Do cyberbullies suffer too? Cyberbullies’ perceptions of the harm theycause to others and to their own mental health.School Psychology International, 34(6), pp. 613-629.

This file was downloaded from: https://eprints.qut.edu.au/59983/

c© Copyright 2013 SAGE Publications Ltd

Notice: Changes introduced as a result of publishing processes such ascopy-editing and formatting may not be reflected in this document. For adefinitive version of this work, please refer to the published source:

https://doi.org/10.1177/0143034313479698

Page 2: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

1  

Do cyberbullies suffer too? Cyberbullies’ perceptions of the harm they cause to others

and to their own mental health.

Marilyn A Campbell1 Phillip T Slee2, Barbara Spears3, Des Butler1 and Sally Kift1

1Queensland University of Technology

2Flinders University

3University of South Australia

Corresponding author:

Marilyn Campbell, Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technology, Kelvin Grove Q 4059, Australia. Email: [email protected]

Page 3: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

2  

Abstract

While it is recognised that there are serious correlates for students who are victims of cy-

berbullying including depression, anxiety, lower self-esteem and social difficulties, there

has been little research attention paid to the mental health of students who cyberbully. It is

known that students who traditionally bully report they feel indifferent to their victims,

showing a lack of empathy and that they themselves are at increased risk for psychosocial

adjustment. However, there is scant research on the mental health associations of students

who cyberbully or their awareness of their impact on others. The current study sought to

ascertain from Australian students who reported cyberbullying others in years 6 to 12 (10-

19 years of age), their perceptions of their mental health and the harm they caused to and

the impact their actions had, on their victims. Most students who cyberbullied did not think

that their bullying was harsh or had an impact on their victims. They reported more social

difficulties and higher scores on stress, depression and anxiety scales than those students

who were not involved in any bullying. The implications of these findings for the mental

health of the cyberbullies and for psychologists in schools who assist them, are discussed. 

Keywords

Cyberbullies, mental health, SDQ, DASS-21, anxiety, depression, school psychology 

Page 4: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

3  

Introduction

Bullying is a persistent and long standing problem in most countries in the world at

all educational levels (Kowalski, Morgan, & Limber, 2012; Rios-Ellis, Bellamy, & Shoji,

2000; Topcu & Erdur-Baker, 2012; Smith et al., 2008; von Marees & Peterman, 2010).

While conceptions of bullying may subtly differ between countries depending on language

and cultural practices, a generally accepted definition of traditional bullying is that it is de-

liberate, unprovoked, aggressive behaviour, which is actioned repeatedly in an attempt to

hurt the victim, and involves an imbalance of power (psychological or physical) between

the victim and the perpetrator (Cheng, Chen, Ho, & Cheng, 2011). The recent, emergent

form of cyberbullying however, has added some complexity to the debate over definition,

including the issue of the anonymity of those involved and the 24/7 nature of the act. This

newer form of bullying has become truly a global phenomena transcending country

boundaries and researched in all countries where adolescents have access to technology

(Cassidy, Jackson, & Brown, 2009; Li, 2006, Monks, Robinson, & Worlidge, 2012; Pop-

ovic-Citic, Djuric, & Cvetkovic, 2011; Yilmaz, 2011). Cyberbullying interactions are de-

fined as usually repeated, harmful interactions which are deliberately offensive, humiliat-

ing, threatening and power-assertive, and are enacted using electronic equipment, such as

cell (mobile) phones or the Internet, by one or more individuals towards another. Cyberbul-

Page 5: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

4  

lying can take the form of hurtful instant or email messages, images, videos, calls, exclud-

ing or preventing someone to be part of a group or an online community (Cross et al., 2011;

Spears, Kofoed, Bartolo, Palermiti, & Costabile, 2011) which can be enacted in overt or

covert ways (Spears, Slee, Owens, & Johnson, 2008; 2009).

Most research to date has been understandably for a new phenomenon, on the

prevalence of such behaviour and often on the prevalence of victimisation (Sakellariou,

Carroll, & Houghton, 2012). However, it is important to understand this behaviour from the

perspective of those students who are the perpetrators in order to determine effective pre-

vention and intervention strategies. Understanding the association between students’ perpe-

tration of cyberbullying and their mental health and wellbeing is equally important. Thus

this paper examines cyberbullies’ perceptions of the harm they cause to others and to their

own mental health.

Perceptions of students who bully

While there are numerous studies on the perceptions of the impact of traditional

bullying on students who have been victimised (e.g. Hughes, Middleton, & Marshall,

2009), there are less studies about the perceptions of those students who perpetrate the bul-

lying. For traditional bullies it has been shown that they report more positive attitudes to-

Page 6: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

5  

wards supporting aggressive retaliation (Bradshaw, O’Brennan, & Sawyer, 2008), feel

‘good and happy or ‘mad and angry’ when they bully other children (Boulton & Un-

derwwood, 1992) or feel indifference or satisfaction when bullying others (Borg, 1998).

Fewer bullies than victims report that “it is very unpleasant” when they see a child being

bullied (Houndoumadi & Pateraki, 2001). Students who bully report feeling very little

when observing others being bullied and feel that the victims deserved it (Rowe, Theriot,

Sowers, & Dulmus, 2008). Several studies have shown a weak to moderate relationship

between empathy and bullying others (Correia & Dablert, 2008; Jolliffe & Farrington,

2006).

With the rise of cyberbullying there is some speculation that students who cyber-

bully, may feel more powerful benefiting from the potential of greater anonymity and the

wider audience who can witness their actions (Steffgen, Konig, Pfetsch, & Melzer, 2011). It

has also been hypothesised that the lack of immediate feedback from the victim could lead

to even harsher cyberbullying (Conn, 2004). As many cyberbullies also bully in other tradi-

tional ways (Cross et al., 2009), the lack of empathy evident in traditional bullying could be

magnified by the use of technology to bully others. This has lead to the proposition that

cyberbullies may experience even less empathy for their victims than traditional bullies or

conversely cyberbullying may attract students who exhibit low trait empathy. Preliminary

Page 7: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

6  

studies seem to bear this out with a negative relationship being found between empathy and

cyberbullies, even more than traditional bullies (Ang & Goh, 2010; Schultze-Krumbholtz &

Scheithauer, 2009).

Correlates of bullying perpetration

Association of empathy with bullying perpetration. Empathy has been defined as ‘the abil-

ity to understand and share another’s emotional state or context’ (Cohen & Strayer, 1996, p.

988). As Caravita et al. (2008) have noted it is a complex construct comprising cognitive

and affective aspects. The cognitive component refers to skills of recognising others’ emo-

tions and taking others’ perspectives. The affective component involves the ability to share

others’ feelings. The display of empathy becomes more complex with age. Evidence sug-

gests that empathy is linked to both bullying behaviour and to defending behaviour (Gini,

2006). Research conducted by Gini (2011) has shown that bullies are morally competent to

judge actions but “show significant deficiencies with respect to moral sentiments and car-

ing, and higher levels of moral disengagement” (p. 607).

Psychological adjustment. Researchers have also found poorer psychosocial relationships

for children who engage in bullying (Sourander et al., 2010). For example, Kaltiala-Heino,

Rimpela, Rantanen, and Rimpela (2000) carried out a study involving over 26,000 Finnish

Page 8: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

7  

adolescents and found that involvement in the perpetration of bullying was associated with

a range of mental health problems (such as anxiety, depression and psychosomatic symp-

toms). Psychosomatic problems have also been found to be associated with bullying perpe-

tration in a meta-analytic study (Gini & Pozzoli, 2009) and students who traditionally bully

have been found to be at increased risk for difficulties at school and behaviour maladjust-

ment (Andreou, Vlachov, & Didaskalour, 2005; Carlson & Cornell, 2008; Hampel, Manal,

& Hayer, 2009; Murray-Harvey & Slee, 2010; Nansel, Haynie, & Simonsmorton, 2003;

Wei & Chen, 2011). Bullies are at increased risk for substance abuse (Sourander et al.,

2007) with Niemela and colleagues (2011) demonstrating that bullying others at age eight

predicted illicit drug use at age 18 among males.

Cyberbullies’ mental health

With the advent of cyberbullying it has been hypothesised that students who were

victims of cyberbullying could have more detrimental outcomes than traditional victims

possibly due to the unique aspects associated with cyberbullying: the 24/7 nature; the ano-

nymity aspects and the broader audience available; not to mention the power that the writ-

ten and visual electronic media can have (Campbell, Cross, Spears, & Slee, 2010; Cross et

al., 2009; Spears et al., 2008; 2009). The emerging evidence shows that cyber victims do

indeed exhibit more symptoms of depression than traditional victims (Perren, Dooley,

Page 9: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

8  

Shaw, & Cross, 2010; Raskauskas, 2010). The corollary of this has led to some speculation

that there could also be more detrimental outcomes for those students who cyberbully, than

those who traditionally bully. However, there is scant research on this question with only a

few studies examining the mental health of cyberbullies. In an early study Ybarra and

Mitchell (2004) reported that 39% of students who harassed others online dropped out of

school, 37% showed delinquent behaviour, 32% frequent substance abuse and 16% were

severely depressed. Gradinger, Strohmeier and Spiel (2009) investigated adjustment for

reactive aggression and instrumental aggression in a sample of 761 Grade 9 students in

Austria. Those students who self-reported bullying others in both traditional and cyber

ways scored highest on these externalising problems of both reactive and instrumental ag-

gression compared to non-involved students, cyberbullies only and traditional bullies only.

Although depressive and somatic symptoms were measured, they were only reported for

students who had been victimised, not those who bullied others. Another study measured

levels of depression of students who cyberbullied in Years 6-10 in the United States of

America. It was found although depression scores were higher than for non-involved stu-

dents, the cyberbullies’ levels of depression were lower than those engaged in traditional

bullying (Wang, Nansel, & Iannotti, 2011).

The current study sought to add to the extant literature by ascertaining from Austra-

Page 10: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

9  

lian students who reported cyberbullying others in Year 6 to 12, their perceptions of the

impact and harm they thought their actions had on the students they had cyberbullied. In

addition, the associated mental health of these students was examined.

Method

Participants

The present study uses data from a large-scale school-based survey of students’ bul-

lying experiences. Three thousand, one hundred and twelve students from Grade 6 to 12

(1572 girls 50.5 % and 1535 boys 49.4% ) from 29 different schools, both government and

non-government in three Australian states participated. The age range was from 9 to 19-

years-old (M = 13.96, SD =1.87). Most students (87.5%, n=2724) were able to access the

Internet from their home and 83.1% (n=2586) owned their own mobile (cell) phone. This

paper focused only on those students who reported cyberbullying others or who were not

involved in any form of bullying, in any role.

Procedure

Ethical clearance was obtained both from the universities involved and the various

educational systems as well as the participating schools. Participation was voluntary and

Page 11: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

10  

only students who wished to participate and had written parental consent took part. Ap-

proximately 30% of eligible students undertook the survey due to the active parental con-

sent required. No data was available from students who did not return the parental consent

form and therefore the demographics of non-responders were not available. The surveys

were administered to students in their classrooms during class time by a research assistant

and standardized instructions were read out loud to participants prior to survey administra-

tion. There were between 15 and 25 students per testing session and each session took be-

tween 30 – 45 minutes. The anonymity of the survey responses was emphasized verbally

and in writing to the students. The survey was conducted between August and September

(Term 3) when students had spent the previous 6-7 months of the school year together.

Measures

An anonymous, self-report paper-based survey was conducted, consisting of four sec-

tions. The first section asked for demographic information of gender, age and year of

school, internet access at home and ownership of a mobile (cell) phone. The second section

obtained information about cyberbullying experiences. The following definition of cyber-

bullying was provided (following recommendations of Solberg & Olweus (2003) to im-

prove the validity of responses).

Cyberbullying is when one person or a group of people repeatedly try to

Page 12: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

11  

hurt or embarrass another person, using their computer or mobile phone, to use

power over them. With cyberbullying, the person bullying usually has some advan-

tage over the person targeted, and it is done on purpose to hurt them, not like an

accident or when friends tease each other.

A filter question of “Have you cyberbullied someone this year?” (since January this

year –beginning of the school year in Australia) was used to establish cyberbullying perpe-

tration. If the students answered no they were directed to skip this section. The next ques-

tion determined the frequency of the bullying perpetration on a five point scale of “every

day, most days, one or two times a week, once a week and less than once a week”. These

frequencies mirrored those used by Slee (1993). The following question asked how harsh or

cruel they considered their cyberbullying to be: with a response on a 5 point Likert scale

from ‘’not at all harsh’ to ‘really harsh’. The ensuing question asked what impact they

would say their cyberbullying had on the other person’s life: with a 5 point Likert scale

from ‘no impact (their life was not affected)’ to ‘a huge impact (it totally changed their

life)’.

The third section asked about traditional or face-to-face bullying experiences mirroring

the cyberbullying questions. The fourth section of the survey used the Strength and Diffi-

culties Questionnaire (Goodman, 1997) to ascertain conduct problems, hyperactivity, peer

Page 13: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

12  

relationship problems and pro-social behaviour and the DASS-21(Lovibond & Lovibond,

1995) to ascertain depression, anxiety and stress symptoms.

The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) is a self-report (11-17 years version)

behavioural screening device that measures both positive and negative attributes (Good-

man, 1997). It consists of 25 items and includes five subscales: emotional symptoms, con-

duct problems, hyperactivity/inattention, peer relationship problems and pro-social behav-

iour. Each of the subscales contains 5 items. All subscales except for the pro-social behav-

iour are summed to obtain a total ‘difficulties’ score. For each item, participants indicate,

on a three point scale, how things had been for them over the last/previous six months. The

reliabilities for the SDQ for the current study using Cronbach’s alpha were: 0 .75 for the

emotional symptoms scale; 0.61 for the conduct problems scale; 0.66 for the hyperactiv-

ity/inattention scale 0 .57 for the peer relationship problems scale; 0.75 for the pro-social

behaviour scale and 0.82 for the total scale.

The Depression, Anxiety, Stress Scale (DASS-21) is the short form of Lovibond and Lovi-

bond’s (1995) 42-item self-report measure of depression, anxiety and stress. It consists of

21 items and includes the three 7-item subscales of depression, anxiety and stress. Partici-

pants rate the extent to which they experienced each state over the past week on a 4-point

Page 14: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

13  

Likert rating scale. Scores can be calculated for all subscales by adding each of the items

together and a total score obtained by summing all 21 items. The scores for all items are

doubled to ensure consistency with the original 42 item version of the scale. The alpha

coefficients obtained for the current study were 0.90 for the Depression scale; 0.85 for the

Anxiety scale; 0.87 for the Stress scale, and 0.95 for the Total scale.

The survey ended with a list of counselling services available in the area for any students

who became distressed or wanted more information on bullying.

Data Analysis

Means on the SDQ and DASS-21 and subscales were calculated separately for each

of the categories of bully or not involved. Relationships between demographic variables

and bully and non involved students were examined using chi-square and correlation as

appropriate given the level of measurement. T-tests were used to explore the differences in

means among the two groups on each of the variables. As the SDQ and DASS-21 are corre-

lated, in this sample (r = .64, p<.01) a MANOVA was employed to estimate the differences

in SDQ and DASS-21 among the two groups controlling the correlation between the de-

pendent variables.

Page 15: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

14  

Results

In the sample of 3,112 students, 278 (8.9%) students reported cyberbullying others

by a filter question of yes or no during that school year (January through August

/September). This includes students who reported cyberbullying others as their only role,

cyberbullying and being a victim of cyberbullying, bullying others traditionally and by cy-

ber means and bullying others by traditional and cyber means as well as being a victim

themselves of both types of bullying. Slightly more males than females reported cyberbul-

lying others (153 males (55%) and 125 (45%) females). There was no significant difference

between the ages of those who cyberbullied and those who were not engaged in any way in

bullying (χ2 (1, N=2047) =0.74, p =.39. The Year 9 students (13-14 years old) comprised

the largest percentage (28.5%) who reported cyberbullying others but there was no relation-

ship between age of those who cyberbullied and those not involved (r(2045)=.008, p= 0.39

(see Table 1).

Insert Table 1 about here

Perceptions of the effects of cyberbullying

Compared to 66.4% of cyber victims (Campbell et al., 2012) who thought that the

bullying they had received was harsh to very harsh, only 43.6% of cyberbullies thought that

Page 16: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

15  

their bullying behaviour was harsh to very harsh. In addition, while 34.6% of cyber victims

(Campbell et al., 2012) believed that the bullying had a great impact on their lives, only

26% of the cyberbullies thought their actions had such an impact on their victim’s life. For

those students who cyberbullied others there was no correlation between age and their rat-

ing of how harsh or cruel they considered the cyberbullying to be (r = .122, n = 226, p =

.07). There were also no gender differences found. For those who cyberbullied others, there

were no gender or age differences in their ratings of the impact that cyberbullying had on

the person’s life whom they had cyberbullied. Bullies perceived harshness of their actions

was correlated with those students reporting more difficulties on the SDQ (r =.154, p<.05)

and more problems on the DASS-21 (r=.129, p < .01). Those who felt their cyberbullying

had more impact on their victims were correlated with more difficulties on the SDQ

(r=.211, p< .01) and higher scores on the DASS-21 (r = .196, p <.01).

Mental health

There were significant differences in all of the SDQ subscales between those stu-

dents who cyberbullied and those not involved in bullying. Those who cyberbullied others,

scored higher on the conduct problems scale (M = 3.48) than those who had not reported

cyberbullying others (M = 1.84) (t = -10.972 p = <. 001). They also scored higher on the

hyperactivity scale subscale (M = 4.94) (M = 3.70) (t = -7.645, p < .001); the peer relation-

Page 17: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

16  

ship problems subscale (M = 2.58) (M = 1.72) (t = -6.080, p< .001); and the emotional

problem subscale (M = 3.37) (M = 2.48), (t = -4.7758, p < .001). However, those who re-

ported that they did not engage in cyberbullying scored higher on the pro-social behav-

iour(M = 7.11) subscale than those who reported that they had cyberbullied others (M =

6.08) (t = 5.533, p < .001).

Insert Table 2 about here

There were significant differences in the total DASS-21 score for those who reported that

they had cyberbullied others compared with involved students F (1, 1867) = 39.95, p <.001)

. Those who cyberbullied others, scored higher on the DASS-21 stress subscale (M =

11.76) than those who had not reported cyberbullying others (M = 6.90), (t = -6.521, p =

.000). They also scored higher (M = 7.33) (M = 4.75) on the DASS-21 anxiety scale (t = -

4.027 p = .000) and higher on the DASS-21 depression scale (M = 9.82) (M = 5.91) (t = -

5.054, p = .000).

For the purposes of this study a comparison by MANOVA was made of those students who

reported cyberbullying others and those who were not involved in any forms of bullying in

any roles. The MANOVA of bullies and not involved students showed significant differ-

ences in social difficulties and mental health (F (2, 1866) =57.65, p <.001, Hotelling’s

Page 18: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

17  

Trace T=.062, partial Є2=.058). An examination of the bivariate results showed there was a

significant difference on the SDQ total difficulties scores ( F (1, 1867) = 115.007, p <.001).

Discussion

In the current study 8.9% of students reported cyberbullying others. This finding is

consistent with the lower prevalence of cyberbullying compared to traditional bullying

(Cross et al., 2009; Sourander et al., 2010). While the prevalence of cyberbullying varies

considerably in studies largely due to a lack of a standardised definition, measurement is-

sues and the different ages of the samples (Cross et al., 2011), the finding in this study, of

students who reported cyberbullying others, was similar to the 7% found in another Austra-

lian sample of Year 8 students in 2004 (Campbell, 2005) and to the 6.4% found in Turkish

middle schools (Yilmaz, 2011).

The findings in the present study indicated that slightly more males than females

reported engaging in cyberbullying. This is similar to other studies where males reported

cyberbullying others more frequently than females (Li, 2006; Popovic-Citic, 2011; Ybarra

& Mitchell, 2004) but contrary to other studies which have found no gender differences for

cyberbullies (Hinduja & Patchin, 2008; Williams & Guerra, 2007). In relation to age differ-

ences in the present study the year nine students (approx. age equivalent to 14 years) re-

ported the highest incidence of cyberbullying others. This is consistent with Cross and col-

Page 19: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

18  

leagues study (2009) in Australia, and with reports from America and Canada that older

students tend to cyberbully more than traditionally bully in Canadian schools (Beran, Ri-

naldi, Bickham & Rich, 2012; Mishna, Khoury-Kassabri, Gadalla, & Daciuk, 2012).

In the present study the cyberbullies’ scores on the ‘harshness’ and ‘impact’ of their

behaviour were lower than the cyber victims’ scores, reported in research by Campbell et

al. (2012), suggesting that cyberbullies are either not aware of the effect of their bullying or

are deliberately ignoring its effects (Gini et al., 2011). Of concern is that 57% did not think

their bullying behaviour was harsh, and that 74% did not think that it had an impact on their

target’s life. The lack of empathic awareness for the harshness and impact of their behav-

iour on others is supportive of findings from previous studies, which found a negative rela-

tionship between empathy and cyberbullies (Ang & Goh, 2010; Schultze-Krumbholtz &

Scheithauer, 2009). König, Gollwitzer and Steffgen (2010) found that, compared to non-

cyberbullies, cyberbullies show less empathy for others being victimized. The findings

from this study are in accordance with studies showing a negative relationship between

empathy and aggression (e.g. Batanova & Loukas, 2011).

Although there has been little empirical research on the way in which adolescents

rationalise or justify their actions for cyberbullying (Hymel, Schonert-Reichl, Bonanno,

Vaillancourt, & Rocke Henderson, 2010), it has been shown that students who cyberbully

Page 20: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

19  

reported their main motive was to make themselves feel good (Wilton & Campbell, 2010).

One avenue for explaining the findings from the present study utilises the notion of moral

disengagement. Bullying could be considered an immoral or amoral act: where one in-

volves not conforming to patterns of accepted conduct (immoral), and the other an absence,

indifference or disregard for moral beliefs (amoral). Moral disengagement has been ex-

plained as a social cognitive process by which one is able to justify committing immoral

acts (Bandura, Barbaranelli, Caprara, & Pastorelli, 1996). In disengaging from moral stan-

dards students can protect themselves from the negative affective reactions associated with

engaging in bullying. This can be achieved in a number of ways: e.g. restructuring the

harmful behaviour as socially acceptable e.g. the victim ‘deserved it’; diffusing responsibil-

ity for the action; minimizing the harmful consequences of the bullying. Gini (2011) found

that bullies are morally competent to judge actions but “show significant deficiencies with

respect to moral sentiments and caring” (p. 607). Of course, other possibilities exist for

explaining the present findings including the relative anonymity provided by the internet.

Suler (2005) also reported that when online, people can behave in ways they might not

normally behave, contributing to the lack of connection and consequences they may have

with those they interact with online.

The findings from the current research add to the literature in terms of correlates of

Page 21: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

20  

students’ perceptions of their mental health and wellbeing and the effects and impact of

their cyberbullying behaviour. As noted in this paper there is a growing body of research

addressing the issue of bullying and moral development and the ability to empathise and

understand the feelings of others. Cyberbullying adds another level of complexity to the

research in as much as relationships conducted in cyberspace provide a greater level of

anonymity, and perceived lack of consequences to the bullying behaviour. It has been noted

that in cyberspace there are also reduced social and contextual cues that may have a disin-

hibiting effect on the perpetrator’s behaviour (Ang & Goh, 2010; Suler, 2005). However,

further research is needed to disentangle factors contributing to on-line bullying behaviour

in relation to the matter of moral behaviour to better inform school-based intervention pro-

grams.

Limitations

In considering the findings from the present study a number of limitations to the

study need to be kept in mind. This study was cross sectional in nature and conclusions are

limited to temporal associations rather than causal inferences. The use of self report to as-

sess prevalence is potentially problematic in terms of social desirability and the possibility

of increasing shared method variance thereby potentially strengthening the association be-

tween findings. However, it is worth noting that Juvonen, Nishina, and Graham (2001)

Page 22: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

21  

have argued that studies which focus on adolescents’ own subjective experience can be

reliably measured through self report. Furthermore, an additional possibility was that as the

questionnaire sections were not counterbalanced, the students who reported cyberbullying

others might have highlighted their personal difficulties as justification for their behaviour.

Finally, no sound claims can be made for the representativeness of the sample given that

the sample was drawn from schools in three states in Australia. Overall, the findings re-

garding the perceptions of cyberbullies of the effect of their behaviour and the associations

with their mental health and well being should be viewed with caution but do warrant repli-

cation.

Implications Although most students who cyberbully do so outside of school grounds and outside

of school hours (Cross et al., 2009; Smith et al., 2008), nevertheless the ramifications of

their behaviour usually appear at school. This has implications for psychologists working in

schools to assist in providing a safe environment for students to learn. All bullying, includ-

ing cyberbullying, is embedded in a socio-ecological perspective and is not just a dyadic

relationship between a student who bullies and the student they target (Pepler, Jiang, &

Connolly, 2008). System level approaches, such as policies, codes of conduct and school

climate are therefore all important for prevention and intervention (Yoon, Bauman, Choi, &

Page 23: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

22  

Hutchinson, 2011; Richard, Schneider, & Mallet, 2011). School psychologists have an im-

portant role to play in assisting the school leadership team to improve the school climate

and to make teachers aware of and concerned about cyberbullying (Cassidy, Brown, &

Jackson, 2012). Interventions can also be made at the class level by school psychologists

with direct lessons or using the quality circle approach (Paul, Smith, & Blumberg, 2012).

Finally, school psychologists can assist at the individual level with students who are victim-

ised but also with students who cyberbully.

Although the incidence of cyberbullying seems not to be as great as traditional bul-

lying (Campbell et al., 2012; Cross et al., 2009) there has been some emerging evidence

that cyberbullying has more associations with mental health difficulties for students who

are cyberbullied (Campbell et al., 2012) and this study has shown that cyberbullies them-

selves also have social difficulties as well as more mental health concerns than students not

involved in bullying. Clearly, cyberbullies do suffer: in terms of their own mental health

and in their social experiences with others. They showed higher scores in all SDQ subscales

than those not involved in cyberbullying, and higher levels of stress, depression and anxiety

than those non-involved students. Their low level of awareness of the harshness and impact

their behaviour has on others’ lives infers a lack of empathic awareness and perhaps also

indicates limited moral engagement. The students who cyberbully as well as the students

Page 24: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

23  

they bully, need the services of psychologists working in schools. The finding that students

who cyberbully seem to lack empathy, is similar to students who bully traditionally. Al-

though there are many calls for programs for teaching these students empathy – that is un-

derstanding and sharing the emotions of others - this is a difficult task. These students often

do not see they have harmed another student and do not want to change as they often have

an inflated sense of their own importance and are getting things they want (O’Moore &

Kirkham, 2001). One avenue that holds some promise is motivational interviewing as a

strategy to bring these students to a point of wanting to change their behaviour and then

helping them to do so (Resnicow, McMaster, & Rollnick, 2012).

Acknowledgement: This study was funded by the Australian Research Council Grant LP0882087. The authors wish to acknowledge the contribution of Robyn Garland as project manager.

Page 25: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

24  

References

Andreou, E., Vlachou, A., & Diaskalou, E. (2005). The roles of self-efficacy, peer interac-

tions and attitudes in bully-victim incidents: Implications for intervention policy-

practices. School Psychology International, 26, 545-562. doi:

10.117/0143034305060789

Ang, R., & Goh, D. (2010). Cyberbullying among adolescents: The role of affective and

cognitive empathy, and gender. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 41,

387-30.

doi:10.1007/s10578-010-0176-3

Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of

moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and

Social Psychology, 71, 364-374. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.71.2.364

Batanova, M.D., & Loukas, A. (2011). Social anxiety and aggression in early adolescents:

Examining the moderating roles of empathic concern and perspective taking. Jour-

nal of Youth and Adolescence, 40, 1534-1543. doi: 10.1007/s10964-011-9634-x

Beran, T., Rinaldi, C., Bickham, D., & Rich, M. (2012). Evidence for the need to support

adolescents dealing with harassment and cyber-harassment: Prevalence, progres-

Page 26: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

25  

sion, and impact. School Psychology International, 33, 562-576. doi:

10.1177/0143034312446976

Borg, M.G. (1998). The emotional reactions of school bullies and their victims. Educational

Psychology, 18, 433-444. doi:10.1080/0144341980180405

Boulton, M.J., & Underwood, K. (1992). Bully/victim problems among middle school chil-

dren.

British Journal of Educational Psychology, 62, 73-87. doi:10.1111/j.2044-

8279.1992.tb01000.x

Bradshaw, C., O’Brennan, L., & Sawyer, A. (2008). Examining variation in attitudes to-

ward aggressive retaliation and perceptions of safety among bullies, victims, and

bully-victims. Professional School Counselling, 12, 10-21.

doi:10.5330/PSC.n.2010-12.10

Campbell, M. A. (2005). Cyber bullying: An old problem in a new guise? Australian Jour-

nal of Guidance and Counselling, 15, 68-76. doi:10.1375/ajgc.15.1.68

Campbell, M., Cross, D., Spears, B., & Slee, P. (2010). Cyberbullying - Legal implications

for schools. Occasional Paper #118. Melbourne: Centre for Strategic Education.

Page 27: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

26  

Campbell, M., Spears, B., & Slee, P. T., Butler, D., & Kift, S. (2012). Victims’ perceptions

of bullying: Traditional and cyber and the psychosocial correlates of their victimisa-

tion. Emotional and Behavioural Difficulties, 17, 389-401.

Carlson, I., & Cornell, D. (2008). Differences between persistent and desistant middle

school bullies. School Psychology International, 29, 442-451. doi:

10.1177/0143034308096433

Cassidy, Q., Brown, K., & Jackson, M. (2012). ‘Under the radar’: Educators and

cyberbullying in schools. School Psychology International, 33, 520-532. doi:

10.1177/0143034312445245

Cassidy, W., Jackson, M., & Brown, K. (2009). Sticks and stones can break my bones, but

how can pixels hurt me?: Students’ experiences with cyber-bullying. School Psy-

chology International, 30, 383-402. doi: 10.1177/0143034309106948

Cheng, Y.-Y., Chen, L.-M., Ho, H.-C., & Cheng, L. (2011). Definition of school bullying in

Taiwan: A comparison of multiple perspectives. School Psychology International,

32, 227-243. doi:10.1177/0143034311404130

Conn, K. 2004. Bullying and harassment: A legal guide for educators. Alexandria: ASCD.

Page 28: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

27  

Correia, T., & Dabler, C. (2008). School bullying. Belief in a personal just world of bullies,

victims, and defenders. European Psychologist, 13, 248-246. doi: 10.1027/1016-

9040.13.4.248

Cross, D., Monks, H., Hall, M., Shaw, T., Pintabona, Y., Erceg, E., ... Lester, L. (2011).

Three-year results of the Friendly Schools whole-of-school intervention on chil-

dren's bullying behaviour. British Educational Research Journal, 37, 105-129. doi:

10.1080/01411920903420024

Cross, D., Shaw, T., Hearn, L., Epstein, M., Monks, H., Lester, L., & Thomas L. (2009).

Australian covert bullying prevalence study (ACBPS). Retrieved from http://www.

deewr.gov.au/Schooling/NationalSafeSchools/Pages/research.aspx.

Gini, G. (2006). Social cognition and moral cognition in bullying: What’s wrong? Aggres-

sive Behavior, 32, 528-539. doi: 10.1002/ab.20153

Gini, G., & Pozzoli, T. (2009). Social support, peer victimisation, and somatic complaints:

A mediational analysis. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 45, 358-363.

doi:10.1111/j.1440-1754.2009.01501.x

Gini, G., Pozzoli, T., & Hauser, M. (2011). Bullies have enhanced moral competence to

judge relative to victims, but lack moral compassion. Personality and Individual

Page 29: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

28  

Differences, 50, 603-608. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.12.002

Goodman, R. (1997). The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: A research note. Jour-

nal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 38, 581-586. doi:10.1111/j.1469-

7610.1997.tb01545.x

Gradinger, G. P., Strohmeier, D., & Spiel, C. (2009). Traditional bullying versus cyberbul-

lying. Journal of Psychology, 217, 205-213. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.4.205

Hampel, P., Manhal, S., & Hayer, T. (2009). Direct and relational bullying among children

and adolescents: Coping and psychological adjustment. School Psychology Interna-

tional, 30, 474-490. doi 10.1177/0413034309107066

Hinduja, S., & Patchin, J. (2008). Cyberbullying: An exploratory analysis of factors related

to offending and victimization. Deviant Behavior, 29, 129-156. doi:

10.1080/01639620701457816

Hughes, P., Middleton, K., & Marshall, D. (2009). Students’ perceptions of bullying in

Oklahoma public schools. Journal of School Violence, 8, 216-232. doi:

10.1080/15388220902910656

Huondoumadi, A., & Pateraki, L. (2001). Bullying and bullies in Greek elementary schools:

Pupils attitudes and teachers’/parents’ awareness. Education Review, 53, 19-26. doi:

10.1080/0013191012003361 9

Page 30: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

29  

Hymel, S., Schonert-Reichl, K.A., Bonanno, R.A., Vaillancourt, T., & Rocke Henderson,

N. (2010). Bullying and morality: Understanding how good kids can behave badly.

In S. R. Jimerson., S.M. Swearer, & D.L. Espelage (Eds.), Handbook of bullying in

schools: An international perspective (pp. 101-118). New York: Routledge.

Jolliffe, D., & Farrington, D. (2006). Examining the role between low empathy and bully-

ing. Aggressive Behaviour, 32, 540-510. doi 10.1002/ab.20154

Juvonen, J., Nishina, A., & Graham, S. (2001). Self reviews versus peer perceptions of vic-

tim status among early adolescents. In J. Juvonen & S. Graham (Eds.), Peer har-

assment in school: The plight of the vulnerable and victimized (pp. 105-124). New

York: Guilford Press.

Kaltiala-Heino, R., Rimpela, M., Rantanen, P., & Rimpela, A. (2000). Bullying at school -

an indicator of adolescents at risk for mental disorders. Journal of Adolescence, 23,

661-674. doi:10.1006/jado.2000.0351

Konig, A., Gollwitzer, M., & Steffgen, G. (2010). Cyberbullying as an act of revenge?

Australian Journal of Guidance and Counselling, 20, 210-224.

Doi:10.1375/ajgc.20.2.210

Kowalski, R., Morgan, C., & Limber, S. (2012). Traditional bullying as a potential warning

sign of cyberbullying. School Psychology International, 33, 505-519. doi:

Page 31: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

30  

10.1177/0143034312445244

Li, Q. (2006). Cyberbullying in schools: A research of gender differences. School Psychol-

ogy International, 27, 157-170. doi: 10.1177/0143034306064547

Li, Q. (2007). New bottle but old wine: A research of cyberbullying in schools. Computers

in Human Behavior, 23, 1777-1791. doi:10.1016/j.chb.2005.10.005

Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the depression anxiety stress

scales. Sydney: Psychology Foundation.

Mishna, F., Khoury-Kassabri, M., Gadalla, T., & Daciuk, J. (2012). Risk factors for in-

volvement in cyber bullying: Victims, bullies and bully-victims. Children and Youth

Services Review, 34, 63-70. doi: 1.0.1016/j.childyouth.2011.08.032

Monks, C.P., Robinson, S., & Worlidge, P. (2012). The emergence of cyberbullying: A

survey of primary school pupils’ perceptions and experiences. School Psychology

International, 33, 477-491. doi: 10.1177/0143034312445242

Murray-Harvey, R., & Slee, P.T. (2010). School and home relationships and their impact on

school bullying. School Psychology International, 31, 271-295. doi:

10.1177/0143034310366206

Nansel, T., Haynie, D., & Simonsmorton, G. (2003). The association of bullying and vic-

timization with middle school adjustment. Journal of Applied School Psychology,

Page 32: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

31  

19, 45-61. doi: 10.1300/J008v19n02_04

Niemelä, S, Brunstein-Klomek, A., Sillanmäki, L., Helenius, H., Piha, J, Kumpulainen, K.,

... Sourander, A. (2010). Childhood bullying behaviors at age eight and substance

use at age 18 among males. A nationwide prospective study. Addictive Behaviors,

10, 1211-1219. doi: 10.1007/s00127-010-0292-1

O’Moore, M., & Kirkham, C. (2001). Self-esteem and its relationship to bullying behav-

iour. Aggressive Behavior, 27, 269-283. doi:10.1002/ab.1010

Paul, S., Smith, P.K., & Blumberg, H.H. (2012). Revisiting cyberbullying in schools using

the quality circle approach. School Psychology International, 33, 492-504. doi:

10.1177/0143034312445243

Pepler, D.C., Jiang, W., & Connolly, J. (2008). The development of bullying. International

Journal of Adolescent Medicine and Health, 20, 113-119. 

Doi:10.1515/IJAMH.2008.20.2.113

Perren, S., Dooley, J., Shaw, T., & Cross, D. (2010). Bullying in school and cyberspace:

Associations with depressive symptoms in Swiss and Australian adolescents. Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry and Mental Health, 4, 28. doi:10.1186/1753-2000-4-28

Popovic-Citic, B., Djuric, S., & Cvetkovic, V. (2011). The prevalence of cyberbullying

among adolescents: A case study of middle schools in Serbia. School Psychology

Page 33: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

32  

International, 32, 412-424. doi: 10.1177/0143034311401700

Raskauskas, J. (2010). Text-bullying: Associations with traditional bullying and depression

among New Zealand adolescents. Journal of School Violence, 9, 74-97. doi:

10.1080/1538220903185 605

Resnicow, K., McMaster, F., & Rollnick, S. (2012). Action reflections: A client-centered

technique to bridge the WHY-HOW transition in motivational interviewing. Behav-

ioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 40, 474-480. doi:

10.1017/S1352465812000124

Richard, J.F., Schneider, B.H., & Mallet, P. (2011). Revisiting the whole-school approach

to bullying: Really looking at the whole school. School Psychology International,

33, 263-284. doi: 10.1177/0143034311415906

Rios-Ellis, B., Bellamy, L., & Shoji, J. (2000). An examination of specific types of Ijime

within Japanese schools. School Psychology International, 21, 227-241. doi:

10.1177/0143034300213001

Rowe, W.S., Theriot, M.T., Sowers, K.M., & Dulmus, C.N. (2004). Perceptions of bullying

and non-bullying children. Journal of Evidence-Based Social Work, 1, 159-174.

doi:10.1300/ j394v01n02 11

Sakellariou, T., Carroll, A., & Houghton, S. (2012). Rates of cyber victimization and

Page 34: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

33  

bullying among male australian primary and high school students. School

Psychology International, 33, 533-549. doi: 10.1177/01433034311430374

Schultze-Krumbholz, A., & Scheithauer, H. (2009). Social-behavioral correlates of cyber-

bullying in a German student sample. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psy-

chology, 217, 224-226. doi:10.1027/0044-3409.217.4.224

Slee, P. T. (1993). Bullying: A preliminary investigation of the nature and effects on social

cognition. Early Childhood Development and Care, 87, 47-57.

doi:10.1080/0300443930870105

Smith, P. K., Mahdavi, J., Carvalho, M., Fisher, S., Russell, S., & Tippett, N. (2008). Cy-

berbullying: Its nature and impact in secondary school pupils. Journal of Child Psy-

chology and Psychiatry, 49, 376-385. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01846.x

Solberg, M. E., & Olweus, D. (2003). Prevalence estimation of school bullying with the

Olweus Bully/Victim Questionnaire. Aggressive Behavior, 29, 239-268.

doi:10.1002/ab.10047

Sourander, A., Jensen, P., Ronning, J., Niemala, S., Helenius, H….Almqvist, F. (2007).

What is the early adulthood outcome of boys who bully or who are bullied in

childhood? The Finnish “From a Boy to a Man” study. Pediatrics, 120, 397-404.

doi:10.1542/peds.2006-2704

Page 35: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

34  

Sourander, A., Klomek, A. B., Ikonen, M., Lindroos, J., Luntamo, T., Koskelainen, M.,

...Helenius, H. (2010). Psychosocial risk factors associated with cyberbullying

among adolescents. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67, 720-728.

doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.79  

Spears, B.A, Kofoed, J., Bartolo, M.G., Palermiti, A., & Costabile, A. (2011). Positive uses

of social networking sites: Youth voice perspectives. In A. Costabile & B. Spears

(Eds.), The impact of technology on relationships in educational settings (pp. 7-21).

Routledge: London.

Spears, B. A., Slee, P. T., Owens, L., & Johnson, B, (2008). Behind the Scenes: Insights

into the Human Dimension of Covert Bullying. Report prepared for DEEWR:

Canberra

Spears, B.A., Slee, P. T., Owens, L., & Johnson, B. (2009). Behind the scenes and screens:

Insights into the human dimension of covert and cyberbullying, Zeitschrift für Psy-

chologie / Journal of Psychology, 217, 189-196. doi: 10.1027/0044-3409.217.4.189

Steffgen, G., Konig, A., Pfetsch, J., & Melzer, A. (2011). Are cyberbullies less empathic?

Adolescents’ cyberbullying behaviour and empathic responsiveness. Cyberpsychol-

ogy, Behavior and Social Networking, 14, 643-648. doi:10.1089/cyber.2010.0445

Page 36: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

35  

Suler, J. (2005). The online disinhibition effect. International Journal of Applied Psycho-

analytic Studies, 2, 184–188. doi: 10.1002/aps.42

Topcu, C., & Erdur-Baker, O. (2012). Affective and cognitive empathy as mediators of

gender differences in cyber and traditional bullying. School Psychology Interna-

tional, 33, 550-561. doi: 10.1177/0143034312446882

Von Marees, N., & Petermann, F. (2012). Cyberbullying: An increasing challenge for

schools. School Psychology International, 33, 467-476. doi:

10.1177/0143034309352416

Wang, J., Nansel, T., & Iannotti, R. (2011). Cyber and traditional bullying: Differential

association with depression. Journal of Adolescent Health, 48, 415-417. doi:

10.1016/j.jadohealth.2010.07.012

Wei, H., & Chen, J. (2011). The moderating effect of Machiavellianism on the relationships

between bullying, peer acceptance, and school adjustment in adolescents. School

Psychology International, 33, 345-363. doi: 10.1177/01430343111420640

Williams, K. R., & Guerra, N. G. (2007). Prevalence and predictors of Internet bullying.

Journal of Adolescent Health, 41, S14-S21. doi:10.1016/j.jadohealth.2007.08.018

Page 37: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

36  

Wilton, C., & Campbell, M.A. (2011). An exploration of the reasons why adolescents en-

gage in traditional and cyberbullying. Journal of Educational Sciences and Psy-

chology, 63, 101-109.

Ybarra, M., & Mitchell, K. (2004). Online aggressor/targets, aggressors, and targets: A

comparison of associated youth characteristics. Journal of Child Psychology and

Psychiatry, 45, 1308-1316. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00328.x

Yilmaz, H. (2011). Cyberbullying in Turkish middle schools: An exploratory study. School

Psychology International, 32, 645-654. doi: 10.1177/0143034311410262

Yoon, J., Bauman, S., Choi, T., & Hutchinson, A. (2011). How South Korean teachers han-

dle an incident of school bullying. School Psychology International, 32, 312-329.

doi: 10.1177/0143034311402311

Page 38: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

37  

 

Table 1.

Number and gender of cyber and traditional bullies and victims

Age Gender

Mean (SD) Male Female Total number

Cyberbullying

Bullies 14.90(1.8) 25 (1.6%) 17 (1.1%) 42 (1.3%)

*Victims 13.96 (1.8) 53 (3.5%) 85 (5.4%) 139 (4.5%)

*Bully-victims 14.26 (1.8) 32 (2.1%) 16 (1%) 48 (1.5%)

Traditional bully-ing

Bullies 13.81 (1.7) 61 (4%) 34 (2.2%) 95 (3.1%)

*Victims 13.38 (1.8) 230 (15%) 269 (17.1%) 500 (16.1%)

*Bully-victims

13.38 (1.6) 74 (4.8%) 73 (4.6%) 147 (4.7%)

Cyber and tradi-tional bullying

Bullies 14.84 (1.3) 12 (0.08%) 7 (0.04%) 19 (0.6%)

*Victims 13.27 (1.9) 39 (2.5%) 101 (6.4%) 140 (4.5%)

*Bully-victims

13.96 (1.6) 70 (4.6%) 99 (6.3%) 169 (5.4%)

Page 39: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

38  

**Uninvolved stu-dents

14.19 (1.9) 939 (61.2%) 871 (55.4%) 1813(58.3%).

* The results for these students are discussed in another paper

**These were students who were not involved in any form of bullying in any role

       

         

         

Page 40: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

39  

Page 41: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

40  

Table 2.

Mean scores for bullies, victims, and bully-victims

Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) DASS 21

Emotional symptoms

score

M (SD)

Conduct problem

score

M (SD)

Hyperac-tivity score

M (SD)

Peer prob-lem score

M (SD)

Total Dif-ficulties

score

M (SD)

Pro-social behaviour

score

M (SD)

Depres-sion

M (SD)

Anxiety

M (SD)

Stress

M (SD)

DASS total

M (SD)

Cyberbullying

*Bullies 3.7 (2.80) 3.1 (1.81) 4.9 (2.00) 1.97 (1.52) 13.2 (5.69) 6.7 (2.34) 8.41 (10.17)

4.72 (6.18) 9.13 (10.00)

22.26 (23.87)

Victims 3.5 (2.74) 2.3 (2.20) 4.1 (2.29) 2.5 (2.10) 12.5 (7.06) 6.9 (2.38) 11.16 (11.91)

8.23

(9.83)

11.36 (9.94)

30.84 (29.71)

Page 42: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

41  

Bully-victims 2.5 (2.36) 3.0 (2.22) 4.7 (2.25) 3.2 (2.13) 13.5 (6.12) 5.7 (2.94) 8.05 (8.23) 7.22 (9.58) 9.56 (8.63) 24.83 (24.81)

Traditional bullying

*Bullies 2.3 (2.48) 2.9 (2.5) 4.6 (2.26) 1.8 (1.81) 11.7 (6.19) 6.4 (2.23) 7.22 (9.55) 5.56 (6.80) 9.54 (9.04) 22.44 (22.65)

Victims 3.1 (2.55) 2.0 (1.61) 3.7 (2.31) 2.2 (1.80) 11.0 (5.94) 7.6 (1.93) 7.72 (9.17) 6.00 (7.10) 9.29 (8.60) 23.02 (22.53)

Bully-victims 3.1 (2.56) 2.8 (2.02) 4.6 (2.35) 2.1 (1.74) 12.7 (5.88) 6.7 (2.21) 8.13 (9.68) 6.57 (7.29) 10.61 (9.62)

25.31 (24.45)

Cyber and traditional bullying

*Bullies 2.3 (2.38) 4.4 (2.09) 5.9 (2.39) 2.2 (1.73) 14.7 (5.90) 5.8 (1.44) 9.33 (10.13)

5.89

(6.88)

11.67 (9.21)

26.89 (23.11)

Victims 4.7 (2.82) 2.7 (1.89) 4.5 (2.22) 3.3 (2.15) 15.3 (6.48) 7.5 (2.05) 14.62 (12.00)

11.73 (10.00)

15.35 (10.92)

41.70 (29.83)

Page 43: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

42  

Bully-victims 4.0 (2.67) 3.6 (2.20) 5.1 (2.26) 2.9 (2.12) 15.6 (6.52) 6.4 (2.59) 12.09 (12.12)

9.41 (10.39)

14.13 (11.15)

35.63 (31.55)

*Uninvolved students 2.5 (2.24) 1.8 (1.68) 3.7 (2.20) 1.7 (1.62) 9.7 (5.68) 7.1 (2.09) 5.92

(8.66)

4.75

(7.18)

6.90

(8.29)

17.57 (22.28)

*Only the results of these students are discussed in this paper.

Page 44: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

43  

Author Biographies Marilyn Campbell is a professor at the Queensland University of Technology. She is a registered teacher and a registered psychologist. Previous to this Marilyn supervised school counsellors and has worked in infants, primary and secondary schools as a teacher, teacher-librarian and school counsellor. Her main clinical and research interests are the prevention and intervention of anxiety disorders in young people and the effects of bullying, especially cyberbullying in schools. Address: Faculty of Education, Queensland University of Technol-ogy, Brisbane Q 4059, Australia. Email: [email protected] Barbara Spears is Co-director of the Citizenship and Wellbeing Research Group in the Centre for Research in Education at the University of South Australia. She is the co-editor of The Impact of Technology on Relationships In Educational Settings and lead author of the In-sights Into the Human Dimension of Covert Bullying report. She is a member of the National Technology and Wellbeing Roundtable, the National Centre Against Bullying and is a leading researcher with the Young and Well Cooperative Research Centre. Address: School of Education, University of South Australia, Magill SA 5072 Email: [email protected]

Phillip Slee is a professor in Human Development in the School of Education at Flinders University. He is a trained teacher and registered psychologist. He has published extensively in the field of child development, bullying, school violence, stress, and mental health. He has a particular interest in the practical and policy implications of his research. Details of some of his work is available on the web site http://www.caper.com.au. Address: School of Education, Flinders University, GPO Box 2100, Adelaide SA 5001 Email: [email protected]

Page 45: Accepted Version (PDF 233kB)

44  

Des Butler is a Professor of Law at the Faculty of Law, Queensland University of Technology where he served as Assistant Dean, Re-search (1997-2002). He was awarded his doctorate in 1996 for a study of legal liability for psychiatric injury caused by negligence and is the author or co-author of 16 books on topics including psychiatric injury caused by negligence, contract law and media law. He has been a chief investigator on Australian Research Council grants studying teachers’ duties to report suspected child abuse and cyberbullying in schools. Address: Faculty of Law, Queensland University of Technology 2 George Street Brisbane 4000 Q Email: [email protected]

Sally Kift is Deputy Vice-Chancellor (Academic) at James Cook University (JCU) and President of the Australian Learning and Teaching Fellows (ALTF). Prior to commencing at JCU in May 2012, Sally was a Professor of Law at Queensland University of Technology, where she has served as Law Faculty Assistant Dean, Teaching & Learning (2001-2006) and QUT’s foundational Director, First Year Experience (2006-2007). Sally is a national Teaching Award recipient, an Australian Learning and Teaching Council (ALTC) Senior Fellow and an ALTC Discipline Scholar: Law. She has published widely in legal education and criminal law and is a chief investigator on Australian Re-search Council grants investigating cyberbullying in schools. Address: James Cook University TOWNSVILLE Q 4811 Email: [email protected]