24
 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning LINDY J. WOODROW  Faculty of Educati on and Social Work University of Sydney NSW 2006, Australia  Email: l.woodrow@e dfac.usy d.edu.a u This study applies theorizing from educational psychology and language learning to hypoth- esize a model of language learning that takes into account affect, motivation, and language learning strategies. The study employed a questionnaire to assess variables of motivation, self- efcac y , anx iet y , and langua ge lea rni ng stra teg ies. The samp le con sist ed of 275 adva nce d lea rn- ers studying English for academic purposes prior to entering Australian universities. The data  were analyzed using both variable- and person-centered approaches. The variable-centered approaches included correlational analysis and structural equation modeling, whereas the person-centered approaches utilized cluster analysis and prole analysis using multidimen- sional scaling (PAMS). The ndings supported the hypothesized model of adaptive learning and highlighted the relevance of research in educational psychology for informing language learning research. THIS STUDY USED CONS TRUCTS FROM both language learning and educational psychol- ogy to propose a model of adaptive language learning. Adaptive learning comprises constructs that ar e related to succe ssf ul language per- formance. This model reects the relation- ship betwe en motivation, self-e fcac y, anxiet y , and le arning strategi es to a measure of oral language performance. Research in educat ion suggests that adaptive learning reects a task- ori ent ed rat her than a self-orie nte d goa l ori- entation (Ames, 1992), acc ura te self-efc acy app raisals (Bandura, 1997), low test anxiety (Zei dne r , 1998), and the use of app ropriat e learn- ing str ate gie s (Pi ntr ich & DeGroot, 1990). In language learning research, an integrative goal orientation rather than an instrumental orien- tation is related to adaptive learning (Gardner, 1985). Recently , research has a lso highlighted the importance of intrinsic rather than extrinsic mo- tivation (Noels, 2001). Language learning anxi- ety has been found to have a detrimental effect on language learning (Horwitz, Horwitz, & Cope, The Modern Language Journal , 90, iii, (2006) 0026-7902/06/297–319 $1.50/0 C 2006 The Modern Language Journal 1986), particularly where foreign language speak- ing is concerned (Lucas, 1994). In addition, the frequent use of certain language learning strate- gies has been consistently found to be related to performan ce (Oxfo rd, 1996). This study sought to provide empirical evidence for a hypothesized model of adap tive lea rni ng compri sing aff ect , mo- tivation, and language learning strategies in rela- tion to oral performance in English. There has beenmuch deb ate overtheconstruct  of motivation in both language learning and its relationship to language performance. There is some consensus regarding the orientation of mo- tivation in that a focus on the learning task is likely to be related to high achievement. In lan- guage learning, an integrative orientation, which refers to a focus on the target culture and lan- guage, may be considered more adaptive, and thus more likely to result in successful language learning than an instrumental goal orientation.  An instrumental goal orientation, which refers to some form of reward, for example, nancial- or career-related reasons for learning a language, may be considered less adaptive than an integra- tive orientation (Gardner, 2001; Gardner & Mac- Intyre, 1993; Tremblay & Gardner, 1995). In ed- ucational research, there is a long tradition that distinguishes goal orientations as being task goals

A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

  • Upload
    marcos

  • View
    221

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    1/23

    A Model of Adaptive Language

    LearningLINDY J. WOODROWFaculty of Education and Social WorkUniversity of SydneyNSW 2006, AustraliaEmail: [email protected]

    This study applies theorizing from educational psychology and language learning to hypoth-esize a model of language learning that takes into account affect, motivation, and language

    learning strategies. The study employed a questionnaire to assess variables of motivation, self-efficacy, anxiety, and language learning strategies. The sample consisted of 275 advanced learn-ers studying English for academic purposes prior to entering Australian universities. The datawere analyzed using both variable- and person-centered approaches. The variable-centeredapproaches included correlational analysis and structural equation modeling, whereas theperson-centered approaches utilized cluster analysis and profile analysis using multidimen-sional scaling (PAMS). The findings supported the hypothesized model of adaptive learningand highlighted the relevance of research in educational psychology for informing languagelearning research.

    THIS STUDY USED CONSTRUCTS FROM

    both language learning and educational psychol-ogy to propose a model of adaptive languagelearning. Adaptive learning comprises constructsthat are related to successful language per-formance. This model reflects the relation-ship between motivation, self-efficacy, anxiety,and learning strategies to a measure of orallanguage performance. Research in educationsuggests that adaptive learning reflects a task-oriented rather than a self-oriented goal ori-entation (Ames, 1992), accurate self-efficacyappraisals (Bandura, 1997), low test anxiety(Zeidner, 1998), and the use of appropriate learn-ing strategies (Pintrich & DeGroot, 1990). Inlanguage learning research, an integrative goalorientation rather than an instrumental orien-tation is related to adaptive learning (Gardner,1985). Recently, research has also highlighted theimportance of intrinsic rather than extrinsic mo-tivation (Noels, 2001). Language learning anxi-ety has been found to have a detrimental effecton language learning (Horwitz, Horwitz, & Cope,

    The Modern Language Journal, 90, iii, (2006)0026-7902/06/297319 $1.50/0C2006 The Modern Language Journal

    1986), particularly where foreign language speak-

    ing is concerned (Lucas, 1994). In addition, thefrequent use of certain language learning strate-gies has been consistently found to be related toperformance (Oxford, 1996). This study soughtto provide empirical evidence for a hypothesizedmodel of adaptive learning comprisingaffect, mo-tivation, and language learning strategies in rela-tion to oral performance in English.

    There hasbeen much debateover the constructof motivation in both language learning and itsrelationship to language performance. There issome consensus regarding the orientation of mo-tivation in that a focus on the learning task islikely to be related to high achievement. In lan-guage learning, an integrative orientation, whichrefers to a focus on the target culture and lan-guage, may be considered more adaptive, andthus more likely to result in successful languagelearning than an instrumental goal orientation.An instrumental goal orientation, which refersto some form of reward, for example, financial-or career-related reasons for learning a language,may be considered less adaptive than an integra-

    tive orientation (Gardner, 2001; Gardner & Mac-Intyre, 1993; Tremblay & Gardner, 1995). In ed-ucational research, there is a long tradition thatdistinguishes goal orientations as being task goals

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    2/23

    298 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    (also known as mastery or learning) or perfor-mance goals (also known as ego or ability goals).A learner who displays a task goal orientation ismotivated by academic tasks and achievement forits own sake, whereas a learner who displays a per-formance goal orientation is motivated by achiev-

    ing success in comparison to others (Ames &Archer, 1988). A task goal orientation has repeat-edly been found to be related to high academicachievement. In addition, there is evidence thatpersonal goal orientations may be influenced byteachers, schools, and home environments (An-derman & Maehr, 1994). This dichotomous viewof motivation has recently been challenged, anda more complex model of motivational orienta-tion has emerged. A dimension of approach andavoidance was introduced into the conceptualiz-ing of performance motivation (Urdan, 1997).A performance approach orientation refers toa need to outperform others, whereas a perfor-mance avoid orientation refers to the desire toavoid displaying low ability. There is consider-able empirical support for this conceptualization(Elliot, 1999; Elliot & Church, 1997; Middleton& Midgely, 1997; Midgley et al., 1998). A perfor-mance approach goal orientation may be usefulin some circumstances, particularly in preparingfor exams (Elliot, McGregor, & Gable, 1999). Aperformance avoid goal orientation is viewed as

    less desirable andmay be consideredmaladaptive.Emerging research indicates that in some circum-stances, for example, sports, a task goal orienta-tion may also be relevant because the participantis motivated by the fear of failure arising from notbeing able to complete the given task (Conroy &Elliot, 2004).

    The application of goal theory has received lit-tle attention in language learning research de-spite its significance in education studies. In the1990s, researchers called for a diversification of

    the construct of motivation in language learn-ing research. It was felt that perhaps the di-chotomousdistinction between integrative andin-strumental motivation was insufficient (Crookes& Schmidt, 1991; Dornyei, 1990, 1994; Oxford,1994; Oxford & Shearin, 1994). As a result,many studies have emerged that have adoptedconstructs of motivation from different disci-plines, notably education and workplace research(Dornyei, 2000; Noels, 2001). It is important toassess the relevance of alternative conceptualiza-

    tions of motivation in language learning. In edu-cation studies, goal orientations have been foundto be related to other constructs of adaptive learn-ing, such as self-efficacy (Smith, Duda, Allen, &Hall, 2002), low anxiety (Middleton & Midgely,

    1997),andlearning strategies (Elliot & McGregor,1999).

    In educational research and recent researchinto language learning, motivation models haveincluded a construct of affect (MacIntyre, Mac-Master, & Baker, 2001). Affect in motivational

    studies is conceptualized as including anxiety orsome form of self-construct or both. There is em-pirical evidence to suggest that affect is a signif-icant predictor of both academic and linguisticsuccess (Horwitz, 2001). In this study, positiveaffect is conceptualized as comprising high self-efficacy and low anxiety. There is much confusionbetween self-constructs such as self-esteem, self-concept, and self-efficacy. Self-esteem is a rathergeneral term concerned with a persons senseof self-worth, irrespective of domain (e.g., learn-ing a language). Self-efficacy is distinct from self-concept both methodologically and theoretically.Self-concept tends to be more general and maybe measured by questions relating to the like-lihood of success in broad areas, such as, Howgood are you at writing? Self-efficacy is a moreclearly delineated construct. Questions to mea-sure self-efficacy tend to be specific and matchedby criterial tasks, such as, How confident are youthat you can write a sentence describing yourself ?Self-efficacy questionnaires often use can-do-typescales. This type of question would be followed by

    a task, and the judgments and task performancewould be examined. Self-efficacy has been cred-ited with more predictive and explanatory powerthanself-concept (Bandura, 1997; Bong, 2002; Pa-jares & Johnson, 1996; Skaalvik, 1997).

    In language learning, studies into self-efficacyare rather rare despite empirical evidence show-ing the relevance of self-efficacy in predictingachievement in academic learning. Bong (2001)found that self-efficacy beliefs are related acrosssubjects, including language learning. Tremblay

    andGardner (1995), in an expansion of the Gard-nerian model of motivation, included self-efficacyin their model. However, the scale referred to fu-ture achievement rather than present ability. Assuch, this model could include hope and inten-tion. Such judgments would have a weak predic-tive power. Indeed, a direct path from self-efficacyto achievement was not included in their model.However, there has been a great deal of researchinto the construct of self-confidence in languagelearning. This construct differs from self-efficacy

    in two ways. First, it includes a component ofanxiety, and second, the items used to measurethe construct are very general (Clement, Dornyei,& Noels, 1994; Clement, Gardner, & Smythe,1980; Gardner, Tremblay, & Masgoret, 1997).

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    3/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 299

    Early studies into language learning anxietyconcluded that measures of test anxiety, whichis the construct most commonly used in educa-tional research, were not appropriate for use inlanguage learning (Scovel, 1978; Young, 1991).Since those early studies, there has been a great

    deal of research that confirms the negative re-lationship between anxiety and language perfor-mance. Some researchers claim anxiety is one ofthe strongest predictors of foreign language per-formance (MacIntyre, 1999).

    Learning strategies represent an important ele-mentin successful learning.According to Pintrichand colleagues, research into self-regulation hasidentified three general categories of strategies:cognitive, metacognitive, and resource manage-ment (Pintrich & Garcia, 1991). Cognitive strate-gies refer to strategies, such as rehearsal, that areused to learn presented material. Metacognitivestrategies concern the process of learning and in-clude monitoring and planning. Resource man-agement refers to how students manage time andincludes help-seeking strategies. Generally, stud-ies have found that metacognitive strategies aremore predictive of high academic performancethan cognitive strategies and resource manage-ment (Garcia & Pintrich, 1996). A great deal ofresearch has been conducted into language learn-ing strategies. The classification of these strate-

    gies tends to be complex (Oxford & Crookall,1989), and replication of categories of strategiesis problematic (Woodrow, 2005). The issue ofsocial strategies seems to be the major distin-guishing feature of language learning strategieswhen they are compared to academic learningstrategies.Schmidt and Watanabe(2001)adoptedPintrichs classification in a large-scale study andfound the instrumentation and classification to bereliable andvalid. Despite problems with the mea-surement and classification of learning strategies,

    there is a consensus that strategy use is relatedto language performance (Oxford, 1996; Park,1997).

    THE CURRENT STUDY

    The participants in this study were from neigh-boring Asian countries. The study intended inpart to examine the effect of the proximity ofthese countries. There are similarities in the cul-tural heritage of China, Japan, and Korea because

    these cultures have a commonhistory informedbyConfucianism. There are similarities among theeducational environments of Confucian heritagecultures. Research has indicated that participantsfrom these cultures may have motivational pro-files different from those of participants residing

    in a Western setting. For example, there is evi-dence that Asian studentstend to attribute successand failure to personal effort, whereas Westernstudents tend to attribute success and failure totheir ability (Biggs, 1996; Grant & Dweck, 2001).

    This study sought to provide evidence to sup-

    port a hypothesized model of adaptive learningcomprising motivation, affect, and learning strate-gies applicable to language learning. In addition,the study aimed to provide evidence for the rel-evance of the concepts of goal orientation andself-efficacy to language learners.

    METHOD

    Participants

    The participants in this study were students tak-ing English for academic purposes (EAP) coursesat intensive language centers in Australia prior touniversity entry. Most of these students came fromneighboring Asian countries. A total of 40.8%(n = 149) of the participants were from Confu-cian heritage cultures (Korean-, Japanese-, andChinese-speaking countries). Most of the studentsplanned to take postgraduate courses (54.2%,n = 149), with the greatest number of them in-tending to study commerce and business (39.3%,n = 108). In all, there were 275 students (male,

    n = 139; female, n = 136) who participated bycompleting a questionnaire on adaptive learning.

    Instrumentation

    The questionnaire used in the study consistedof subscales to measure motivation, self-efficacy,anxiety, and language learning strategies. It com-prised 75 items (Woodrow, 2003) and was devel-oped from a larger questionnaire validated in apilot study (Woodrow, 2001).

    There are some issues concerning the useof self-report questionnaires. In order to over-come these concerns, 47 participants took partin semistructured interviews, which presented theconstructs of adaptive learning in depth and gavethe participants the opportunity to comment onissues raised by the questionnaire. The question-naire was piloted and revised according to theresults, taking into account the participants com-ments. A major concern of self-report instru-mentation, particularly when measuring subjec-

    tive and attitudinal variables, is that respondentsselect responsesaccording to how they wish to por-tray themselves (Oller, 1982). This effect may beminimized, to a certain extent, by response andrespondent confidentiality and careful wordingof items. The instrumentation chosen to measure

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    4/23

    300 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    items for this study came, as far as possible, fromvalid and reliable extant scales. The instrumenta-tion to measure affect, constructed for the study,was simplified after the pilot study so that it wouldcontain little content that could be construedas being socially desirable or undesirable (Holt-

    graves, 2004). In addition, the researcher was un-known to the respondents. According to the in-terviews, the participants were very interested inthe notion of motivation and particularly in theconcept of affect. Of 47 participants, 45 reportedexperiencing second language speaking anxietyto some extent.

    Motivation goal orientations were measuredusing the Patterns of Adaptive Learning Sur-vey (PALS) personal goal orientation subscales(Midgely et al., 1997). The items were adaptedto apply to adult English language learners andused a 5-point Likert scale. In addition, languagelearning motivation items were taken from the at-titude/motivation test battery (AMTB; Gardner,1985). Four items relating to motivational inten-sity were also included.

    A subscale to measure second language speak-ing anxiety was developed by the researcherbecause the existing instrumentation was notwholly relevant for participants studying in asecond language environment as opposed to aforeign language environment. The subscale re-

    flected speaking anxiety experienced both in classand outside of class. Of the 22 original items usedfor this subscale in the pilot study, 11 items wereretained, on theoretical and empirical grounds,in the final version of the questionnaire. Theseitems referred to communication situations andreflected the setting, interlocutor, level of formal-ity, and whether the participant was initiating orresponding to communication. To promote en-gagement with the questionnaire task and thusavoid the issue of satisficing (Krosnick, 1999),

    items were simplified greatly after the pilot study.Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-pointLikert-type scale the extent to which they felt anx-ious in particular situations.

    There was no available measure of languagelearning self-efficacy, so the subscale was con-structed according to Banduras recommenda-tions for scale construction (Bandura, 1995). A10-point can-do scale was used. The items also re-flected the in- and out-of-class distinction.

    Language learning strategies were measured

    using Schmidt and Watanabes (2001) strategiessubscale. After trial in the pilot study, 20 of theoriginal items for this subscale were retained. Thepilot study failed to replicate theclassificationpro-posed by Schmidt andWatanabe (Woodrow, 2001,2003, 2005), so the strategies were reclassified as

    cognitive, metacognitive, and social, based on theresults of an exploratory factor analysis.

    The measure of performance used in the studywas based on the International English LanguageTesting Service (IELTS) oral assessment. Thistest comprised a three-stage scripted interview

    that was assessed according to profile descrip-tors of language performance by a certified ex-aminer. The assessment included interview-typequestions, discussion tasks, and a short oral pre-sentation.

    RESULTS

    Reliability and Validity of the Instrumentation

    Confirmatory factoranalysis (CFA) was selectedas the most appropriate statistical method to as-sess the reliability and validity of the instrumenta-tion because judgments were made a priori re-garding the latent variables of the study. FourCFAs were estimated using Linear Structural Re-lations (LISREL) software for motivation goal ori-entations, second language speaking self-efficacy,second language speaking anxiety, and languagelearning strategies. The models were estimatedbased on correlation matrices to accommodate5-point answers because these variables cannotbe classed as truly continuous (see Appendix A).

    Language learning motivation orientation itemsrelating to instrumental goal orientations fromGardners (1985) AMTB were removed from theanalysis because these items were highly kurtotic,and kurtosis can cause problems in CFA analysis.This result was probably due to the ultimate goalof the students in the sample. The participantswere studying EAP with the aim of entering Aus-tralian universities. This goal may be regarded ashighly instrumental; thus a highly kurtotic resultcould be expected.

    CFA is classed as structural equation mod-elling (SEM). In evaluating model fit for the CFAmodels, multiple indexes were used. In additionto reporting the chi statistic ( 2), normed 2

    ( 2/dfchi-square divided by degrees of free-dom) was reported because it is not very sensitiveto sample size. Fit indexes were used as recom-mended by Holmes-Smith (2000). They maintainthat the goodness-of-fit index (GFI) and the ad-justed goodness-of-fit index (AGFI) should be .90or greater to indicate a reasonable fit. The root

    mean square residual (RMR) and the root meansquare error approximation (RMSEA) should bein the region of .05 to indicatea goodfit, althoughlarger values are acceptable when the other in-dexes indicate a good fit. The normed fit index(NFI) and the non-normed fit index (NNFI) and

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    5/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 301

    the comparative fit index (CFI) should have in-dexes of .90 or over.

    The model estimated for motivation comprisedthree factors representing task, performance ap-proach goal, and performance avoid goal ori-entations. The model indicated a moderate to

    good fit to the data. The factor loadings werevery similar to those reported by Midgely and col-leagues (1998). The factor structure is illustratedin Figure 1, and the fit indexes are in Appendix B.Reliability of the subscale was calculated based onthe CFA model, which is viewed as superior toother coefficients because it takes account of er-ror. The coefficients for the subscales were taskgoal = .70; performance approach = .86; and per-formance avoid = .77.

    A two-factor model was estimated for secondlanguage speaking self-efficacy, and a good fit tothe data was found. The figure for self-efficacy is

    FIGURE 1Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Motivation Goal Orientations

    Learn Even ifMake

    Mistakes

    Interested

    Want toGet Better

    Learn WhenReally Think

    Like LearningNew Things

    Do BetterThan Others

    Others ThinkI'm Good

    Feel SuccessfulDoing BetterThan Others

    Show Teacher

    Only One WhoCould Answer

    Task

    PerformanceAvoid

    PerformanceApproach

    0.64

    0.56

    0.77

    0.49

    0.66

    0.53

    0.50

    0.44

    0.29

    0.48

    0.60 0.48 0.75 0.60 0.55

    0.60

    0.66

    0.48

    0.49

    0.58

    0.630.72 0.50 0.63 0.67

    0.72

    0.84

    0.75

    0.71

    0.68

    Look Like

    Can't Speak

    Not Embarrass

    Self Not Look Stupid

    Avoid Look

    Unable at Tasks

    Others Not

    Think I'm Poor

    0.17

    0.310.59

    presented in Figure 2 and the fit indexes are inAppendix B. The reliability coefficients for theself-efficacy subscales were high with .92 for bothin-class and out-of-class self-efficacy and a com-bined coefficient of .96.

    A two-factor model was also estimated for sec-

    ond language speaking anxiety that related to in-andout-of class communication. Two error covari-ances were included in the model that seemedto relate to native speaker interaction outside ofclass. However, this effect was not sufficient towarrant a third factor. A further error covariancewas included in the model that seemed to relateto public performance of speaking English. Themodel is presented in Figure 3, and the fit in-dexes are presented in Appendix B. The modelprovided a moderate fit to the data. The model-calculated reliability coefficients were .89 for in-class anxiety and .87 for out-of-class anxiety. The

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    6/23

    302 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    FIGURE 2Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Second Language Speaking Self-Efficacy

    Answer TeacherQuestion

    GroupSpokesperson

    ParticipateRole Play

    ParticipateFormal

    Discussion

    Ask Lecturer

    Converse MoreThan 1 NS

    Converse KnownNS

    ConverseUnknown NS

    Out-of-ClassSelf-Efficacy

    In-classSelf-Efficacy

    0.24

    0.24

    0.24

    0.26

    0.25

    0.14

    0.16

    0.51

    0.87

    0.87

    0.87

    0.86

    0.70

    0.92

    0.93

    0.87

    0.70

    overall reliability was .94, indicating a reliable in-strument.A three-factor model reflecting metacognitive,

    cognitive, and social strategies was estimated forlanguage learning strategies. A moderate fit tothe data was achieved with the removal of certainitems on empirical and theoretical grounds. How-ever, the error variances were rather high for themodel. The model is presented in Figure 4, andthe fit indexes in Appendix B. Reliability for cog-nitive strategies was .81; for social strategies, it was.78; and for metacognitive strategies, it was .79.

    The reliability of the combined subscales was .90.Two variable-based methods were used to assess

    the feasibility of the hypothesized model of adap-tive learning, correlation analysis, and structuralequation modeling.

    Correlates of Oral Performance

    Simple bivariate correlation analysis using aPearson product-moment technique was used toassess the initial linear relationship between the

    variables. These correlations were calculated us-ing SPSS computer software. Several of the pro-posed variables indicated significant correlationswith oral performance, as measured by the IELTS-type oral assessment. The effect sizes were calcu-lated using r squared (r2) to indicate the amount

    of variance of the dependent variable accountedfor by the independent variables. The correlationmatrix for all summated subscale means in Ap-pendix A shows that there were significant corre-lations between oral performance and a task goalorientation (r = .244, p =

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    7/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 303

    FIGURE 3Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Second Language Speaking Anxiety

    Ask QuestionTeacher

    SpeakInformally

    Teacher

    GroupDiscussion

    Role Play

    OralPresentation

    FormalDiscussion

    Converse MoreThan 1 NS

    StartConversation NS

    AnswerLecturer

    Ask Lecturerfor Advice

    AnswerQuestion

    Unknown NS

    Out-of-ClassAnxiety

    In-ClassAnxiety

    0.27

    0.51

    0.46

    0.44

    0.45

    0.45

    0.46

    0.28

    0.13

    0.61

    0.59

    0.85

    0.7

    0.73

    0.75

    0.74

    0.74

    0.64

    0.62

    0.93

    0.85

    0.73

    -0.13

    0.45

    -0.11

    0.64

    assumption. Four main variables were included inthe model: motivation, affect, intensity, and learn-ing strategies.

    In the model, anxiety and self-efficacy wereclassed as affective variables. These variables werenegatively correlated. A variable of positive affect

    was proposed comprising high self-efficacy andlow anxiety. To focus on positive affect, items foranxiety were reverse-coded so that a high scoreindicated low anxiety and a low score indicatedhigh anxiety.

    The motivation variable in the model was madeup of integrative motivation and task goal motiva-tion, thus representing positive goal orientations.The oral performance variable was included inthe model as an observed variable rather than asa latent variable because there were no indicators

    for it as was the case with the other latent variables.A variable of intensity was included in themodel based on items from the observed vari-ables. Two major factors emerged from these vari-ables relating to effort and persistence.

    The structural model indicated a good fit to thedata, as is indicated in Figure 5 and Appendix B.Understandably, a high error variance was associ-ated with the oral performance variable becausethis model accounts for only a part of the variancein relation to motivation, affect, and strategies.

    Language level and aptitude were not the majorissue in this study. Only a restricted variable of lan-guage performance was used in this study, that is,oral performance as measured by an IELTS-typeof assessment.

    In the model, intensity and strategies are not di-rectly related to performance. However, there isa relationship between strategies, intensity, affect,and motivation, indicating that strategies and in-tensity are influenced by affect and motivation.

    Cluster Analysis

    Two methods of case-based analyses were usedto provide support for the proposed model ofadaptive learning, thus examining the model

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    8/23

    304 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    FIGURE 4Confirmatory Factor Analysis for Language Learning Strategies

    Cognitive

    OrganizeWhat Learned

    Social

    Metacognitive

    Go ThroughFeedback

    RereadMaterials

    Reviewfor Tests

    MemorizeWords

    DetermineUnfamiliarConcepts

    Keep up

    Work WithOthers

    Discuss CourseWith Others

    RelateWords

    Guess FromContext

    EvaluateProgress

    Decide What

    to Learn

    Ask Teacherto Explain

    Practice WithNSS

    0.43

    0.51

    0.58

    0.74

    0.68

    0.73

    0.66

    0.310.41

    0.51

    0.57

    0.52

    0.59

    0.77

    0.76

    0.63

    0.53

    0.43

    0.65

    0.67

    0.83

    0.57

    0.59

    0.66

    0.55

    0.61

    0.49

    0.59

    0.76

    0.68

    0.64

    0.70

    0.75

    from an individual rather than a variable perspec-tive: cluster analysis andprofileanalysis using mul-tidimensional scaling (PAMS).

    A hierarchical cluster analysis was used to deter-mine whether the participants could be groupedaccording to scores on the subscales of adaptivelearning in a way that was similar to the proposedmodel. Wards minimum variance method wasused because it is considered to be one of themost effective in terms of recovery performance

    (Milligan & Cooper, 1987). Cases in Cluster 1 hadhigh scores on IELTS, task goal, self-efficacy, andall strategies (particularly metacognitive), and lowscores on performance goals (particularly perfor-mance avoid) and anxiety. Cases in Cluster 2 had

    lower scores on IELTS, task goal, and all strate-gies, and higher scores on performance goals andanxiety. The clusters and variable means are pre-sented in Table 1. These results are consistentwith the correlations and structural model andindicate support for the hypothesized model ofadaptive learning, with Cluster 1 reflecting adap-tive learning and Cluster 2 reflecting less adaptivelearning.

    Profile Analysis and Multidimensional Scaling(PAMS)

    Profile analysis was performed to explore ingreater depth the evidence for the model of

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    9/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 305

    FIGURE 5Structural Model of Adaptive Learning

    Cognitive

    Metacognitive

    Social

    Strategies

    Motivation

    Affect

    Intensity

    IntegrativeOrientation

    AnxietyReverseCoded

    TaskOrientation

    Self-Efficacy

    Effort Persistance

    IELTS

    0.71 0.83

    0.74

    0.94 0.52

    0.160.53 0.74

    0.43

    0.45

    0.72

    0.49 0.32

    0.54

    0.10

    0.85

    0.730.11

    0.55

    0.32

    0.36

    0.76

    0.31

    0.68

    0.95

    Key to variablesCog Cognitive strategiesMeta Metacognitive strategiesSoc Social strategiesInteg Integrative goal orientationTask Task goal orientationRevanx Anxiety reverse codedEfficacy Self-efficacyEffort EffortPersist Persistence

    IELTS Oral performance

    adaptive learning and to provide a second case-based method of analysis. PAMS is a relativelynew technique proposed by Davison (1994) that,like cluster analysis, shifts the focus of the analysis

    from the variables in question to the cases or sub-jects. However, cluster analysis does not accountfor potentially important differences between in-dividuals within these groups. Using PAMS, a

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    10/23

    306 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    TABLE 1Clusters, Means, and Standard Deviations for All Variables

    Adaptive Learning Less Adaptive Learning

    Variable M N SD M N SD

    IELTS 6.28 95 0.09 5.78 148 0.08

    Task Goal 4.07 103 0.05 3.70 154 0.68Performance Approach Goal 2.49 103 0.10 2.81 154 0.08Performance Avoid Goal 2.30 103 0.08 2.83 154 0.07Integrative 4.33 103 0.06 4.07 154 0.05Intensity 4.00 103 0.07 3.37 154 0.79Anxiety In Class 1.82 103 0.45 2.64 154 0.05Anxiety Out of Class 1.86 103 0.06 2.62 154 0.80Self-Efficacy In Class 8.00 103 1.29 5.82 154 1.59Self-Efficacy Out of Class 8.05 103 1.15 5.87 154 1.74Cognitive Strategies 3.86 103 0.51 3.35 154 0.53Social Strategies 3.34 103 0.89 2.84 154 0.06Metacognitive Strategies 4.01 103 0.45 3.34 154 0.05

    Note. IELTS = International English Language Testing Service (IELTS) oral assessment.

    more precise analysis of individuals patterns ofadaptive learning was produced by separating in-dividuals scores into profile patterns and levels.Also, PAMS characterizes profiles in terms of con-tinuous profile match parameters (Davison, 1994;Davison, Kim, & Ding, 2001). This method pro-poses an alternative approach to analyses of databased on a generalization of the Q factor model.

    PAMS reparameterizes the linear latent model as-sociated with factor analysis to produce estimatesbased on score profile.

    Initially, idealized profiles were generated us-ing multidimensional scaling (MDS) in SPSS 10.Results of the initial MDS clearly indicated a two-dimensional solution for each variable of adaptivelearning. Results of the profile analysis for goalorientations showed a distinction between taskand performance goals and between approachand avoid orientations as would be expected.Figure 6 shows that Dimension 1 was defined byhigh scoreson performanceorientations,both ap-proachand avoid, and by a low score on a task goalorientation. Because these are profiles, the mirrorimage of these results would refer to a person whohad a high task goal orientation and low perfor-mance orientations. Dimension 2 was defined byhigh scores in a performance avoid goal orienta-tion and low scores in performance approach andtask goal orientations. The mirror image of thisprofile indicatedthat a personwithhigh approachgoals would have a low avoid goal. These profiles

    are displayed in Figure 6, and the dimensions ac-cording to the items are displayed in Table 2.

    In the analysis of the self-efficacy scores,Dimen-sion 1 was defined by high scores on self-efficacy

    and low scores on anxiety. Thus, a profile of posi-tive affect indicated high self-efficacy and low anx-iety, whereas a profile of negative affect indicatedhigh anxiety andlowself-efficacy. Dimension2 wasless distinct but generally supported the notion ofan in- and out-of-class distinction, particularly interms of self-efficacy, with low scores on in-class ef-ficacy and high scores on out-of-class self-efficacy.

    These profiles are displayed in Figure 7, andthe dimensions and their items are presented inTable 3.

    The profile analysis for language learningstrategies was less distinct than for the othervariables. There was an indication of a cognitive/metacognitive dimension and a social/individualdimension. Clear dimensions were not antic-ipated because learning strategies were notrelated directly to performance and because ofthe apparent lack of stability of the instrumenta-tion indicated by replication problems. The lackof replication is probably due to the lack of clearconceptualization of language learning strategies.

    Dimension 1 was defined by higher scores onmetacognitive strategies and lower scores on cog-nitive strategies. Interacting with native speakersrepresented the height of metacognitive strate-gies, whereas surface strategies, such as reviewingfor tests and memorizing vocabulary, representedthe lowest cognitive strategies. Dimension 2 wasdefined by high scores on social strategies and lowscoreson theother individual strategies. This find-

    ing is particularly interesting in light of the cul-tural grouping of the sample. Most of the samplecame from Confucian heritage culture countriessuch as China, Japan, and Korea. Confucian

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    11/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 307

    FIGURE 6Graphical Representation of Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) Solution for Goal Orientation Profiles

    Motivation Profiles

    -1

    -0.8

    -0.6

    -0.4

    -0.2

    0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    1

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19

    Dimension 1 Task/Performance

    Dimension 2 Approach/Avoid

    TABLE 2

    Estimated Two-Dimensional Scale Values of 19 Goal Orientation Variables for Profile Analysis

    Number and Variable Type Item Dimension 1 Dimension 2

    1 Integrative At ease with native speakers 0.85 0.042 Task Learn from mistakes 0.64 0.103 Task Interested in English work 0.59 0.044 Integrative Understand culture 0.52 0.215 Integrative Participate in cultural groups 0.73 0.046 Task Really think about learning tasks 0.27 0.047 Task Like to learn new things 0.55 0.018 Task Want to get better at English 0.84 0.049 Integrative Meet varied people 0.83 0.06

    10 Performance Approach Doing better than others 0.46 0.4011 Performance Approach Other students think Im good 0.82 0.0212 Performance Approach Feel successful if do better than others 0.25 0.4113 Performance Approach Show teacher better than others 0.87 0.1414 Performance Approach Only one to answer teachers question 0.65 0.4815 Performance Avoid Others wont think poor at English 0.56 0.3116 Performance Avoid Avoid looking like cant do tasks 0.47 0.2817 Performance Avoid Avoid looking stupid 0.87 0.5018 Performance Avoid Do work to avoid embarrassing self 0.30 0.0719 Performance Avoid Avoid looking like cant speak English 0.57 0.12

    heritage cultures are said to be collectivist andwork for the good of the group, whereas Westerncultures are said to be individualistic and workfor the good of the self. Figure 8 illustrates the

    profiles, and Table 4 presents the dimensions andcorresponding items.

    Correlations between the MDS profiles and thelevel factors are presented in Appendix A. The

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    12/23

    308 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    FIGURE 7Graphical Representation of Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) Solution for Affect Profiles

    Affect Profiles

    -0.8

    -0.6

    -0.4

    -0.2

    0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    1

    1.2

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19

    Dimension 1 Anxiety/Self-Efficacy

    Dimension 2 In/Out Class

    TABLE 3

    Estimated Two-Dimensional Scale Values of 19 Affect Variables for Profile AnalysisNumber Item Dimension 1 Dimension 2

    1 Anxiety In Class Question from teacher 0.62 0.052 Anxiety In Class Informal conversation with teacher 0.67 0.023 Anxiety In Class Group discussion 0.71 0.044 Anxiety In Class Role play 0.41 0.055 Anxiety In Class Oral presentation 0.35 0.036 Anxiety In Class Formal discussion 0.45 0.057 Anxiety Out of Class Talk with more than one native speaker 0.55 0.078 Anxiety Out of Class Start conversation with native speaker 0.66 0.059 Anxiety Out of Class Academic lecturer asks question 0.46 0.05

    10 Anxiety Out of Class Ask academic subject lecturer a question 0.55 0.0111 Anxiety Out of Class Native speaker asks 0.59 0.0712 Efficacy In Class Question from teacher 0.93 0.2813 Efficacy In Class Act as spokesperson 0.74 0.3714 Efficacy In Class Role play 0.67 0.3715 Efficacy In Class Formal discussion 0.61 0.2216 Efficacy Out of Class Ask questions of academic subject lecturer 0.89 0.0717 Efficacy Out of Class Converse with more than one native speaker 0.69 0.2918 Efficacy Out of Class Start a conversation with known native speaker 0.91 0.3919 Efficacy Out of Class Start a conversation with unknown native speaker 0.57 0.51

    motivation MDS 1 profile associated with highperformance goals and low task goals was neg-atively correlated with the affect MDS 1 profile(anxiety/self-efficacy), the strategies MDS 1 pro-

    file (cognitive/metacognitive), motivational in-tensity, and oral performance. This finding sup-ported the proposed model of adaptive learning.The MDS 1 affect profile (anxiety/self-efficacy)

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    13/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 309

    FIGURE 8Graphical Representation of Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) Solution for Language Learning Strategies

    Strategies Profiles

    -1

    -0.8

    -0.6

    -0.4

    -0.2

    0

    0.2

    0.4

    0.6

    0.8

    1

    1.2

    1.4

    1.6

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

    Dimension 1 Cognitive/Metacognitive

    Dimension 2 Social/Individual

    TABLE 4Estimated Two-Dimensional Scale Values of 15 Strategies Variables for Profile Analysis

    Number and Variable Type Item Dimension 1 Dimension 2

    1 Cognitive Organize 0.02 0.202 Cognitive Go through assignment 0.36 0.053 Cognitive Reread course materials 0.13 0.494 Cognitive Review for tests 0.81 0.165 Cognitive Memorize words 0.29 0.476 Cognitive Determine concepts for test 0.36 0.107 Cognitive Keeping up 0.25 0.018 Social Assignments with others 0.65 0.739 Social Discuss with others 0.57 0.48

    10 Metacognitive Relate words to each other 0.08 0.15

    11 Metacognitive Guess from context 0.33 0.6212 Metacognitive Evaluate progress 0.48 0.2813 Metacognitive Decide what you need to learn 0.11 0.0114 Metacognitive Ask teacher to explain 0.21 0.3815 Metacognitive Practise with native speakers 1.34 0.66

    was positively correlated with strategies MDS 1(cognitive/metacognitive), oral performance,and motivational intensityagain lending sup-port for the model. There was a negative corre-lation with the strategies MDS 2 profile (social/

    individual) and oral performance, whereas thestrategies MDS 1 profile (cognitive/metacogni-tive) indicated a low correlation with oral perfor-mance. (See Appendix A.)

    DISCUSSION

    The results of this study indicate support forthe hypothesized model of adaptive languagelearning with a distinct division between students

    who comply with the model of adaptive learningand those who do not. According to the model,successful learners show a task goal orientationand positive affect, and are more likely to use

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    14/23

    310 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    metacognitive language learning strategies thanunsuccessful learners. By contrast, less successfullearners are more likely to show a performanceavoid orientation and display negative affect.

    However, this model provides only some of theaspects concerning language proficiency and ac-

    quisition.Themodel concerns somevariables thatare related to spoken language performance, andthese variables may be manipulated to a certaindegree at classroom and institution level.

    The oral performance variable in this modelrepresents a rather narrow strand of languageproficiency. Oral performance as measured by anIELTS-typespeaking testwas selected for two mainreasons. First, this type of assessment is a valid andreliable measure of oral performance. The assess-ment was conducted by a certified IELTS exam-iner according to usual examining practices. Thismeasure of oral performance is the most com-monly used measure for university entrance forinternational students in Australia and thus wasrelevant to thereal-world needs of theparticipantsin the sample. The second reason for the choiceofvariable was the relationship between the affectivevariables and speaking as outlined in previous re-search, which emphasizes the saliency of speakingskills (Phillips, 1992; Price, 1991). Often, studentsmay feel comfortable with other skills but experi-ence a high degree of anxiety when speaking.

    Future research is needed to replicate themodel and to considerother variables that may in-fluence language performance. The most obviousof thesevariables would be level of English. Partic-ipants in this study were all of a similar linguisticlevel and were due to take the IELTS. It is assumedthat the majority of these participants would besuccessful; therefore, they could be classed as ableto follow a tertiary course of study in English.It would be interesting to investigate the rolethat language level plays in a model of adaptive

    learning.The participants in this study were mostly inter-

    national students from Confucian heritage cul-tures. There are similarities in the educationalsystems and attitudes toward education in thesecountries. Typically, education is highly prized.High value is placed on effort, which is viewedas being directly linked to high academic achieve-ment (Grant & Dweck, 2001). The majority ofinternational students attending Australian uni-versities are from Confucian heritage culture

    countries; thus, it is important to consider mo-tivational profiles characteristic of these students.The results of this study indicate that the in-

    strumentation to measure constructs of adaptivelearning was both reliable and valid and that

    the constructs used were appropriate to languagelearners.

    The study provided empirical evidence for thereliability and validity of the PALS instrumen-tation used in a second language setting. Themeans, standard deviations, and alpha reliabil-

    ity scores for this study were similar to those ofthe original validation study by Midgely and col-leagues (1997). These similarities are shown inTable 5. The confirmatory factor structure andfactor loadings were also similar to those of thePALS by Midgely et al. (1997).

    A task goal orientation was found to be themost predictive of oral ability. This result is consis-tent with findings in educational research. How-ever, the study indicated that neither an integra-tive nor an instrumental goal orientation was re-lated to oral performance. This finding suggeststhat this conceptualization of motivational con-struct is not applicable to the sample. Gardnersmodel is directed at bilingual learners who haveas their goal native-speaker-like linguistic compe-tence. The participants in this study were short-term sojourners who planned to study at a tertiarylevel in English and then return to their own coun-tries. It is understandable that an integrative goalorientation may not be relevant to these partici-pants. There was a strong correlation between in-tegrativeandtask goal orientations indicating that

    both of these conceptualizations share a common-ality of intrinsic motivation. It is surprising thatmotivational intensity was not related to oral per-formance. This finding may possibly be explainedby the fact that effort variables tend to focus onformal learning, whereas oral competence maydepend upon a broader learning experience suchas in- and out-of-class communication.

    The two subscales used to measure affect in-dicated that in a second language learning envi-ronment a focus on out-of-class communication is

    also relevant to adaptive learning. The strategiessubscale, though reasonably reliable and valid,

    TABLE 5Comparison of Present Study and Patterns ofAdaptive Learning Survey (PALS)

    PresentStudy PALS

    Goal M SD M SD

    Task 3.86 0.66 .68 3.98 0.89 .78Performance 2.65 1.01 .86 2.68 1.08 .86

    ApproachPerformance 2.61 0.86 .77 2.41 0.91 .75

    Avoid

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    15/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 311

    did not produce the same results as did Schmidtand Watanabes (2001) study. It is probably un-reasonable to expect one instrument to measurestrategies variables reliably across samples due tothe variation in student variables. Perhaps it istime to take stock and consider more situated ap-

    proaches to the measurement and classificationof language learning strategies (Woodrow, 2005).Support for the model has some implications

    for the classroom: the need to promote a task goalorientation, high self-efficacy, low anxiety, and theuse of language learning strategies, particularlymetacognitive learning strategies. Support for themodel of adaptive learning is significant becauseof the possibility of manipulating these constructsat the classroom level.

    The application of goal orientation theory tolanguage learning has interesting implications.The current study indicated that the theory is rel-evant to language learners and that a tripartiteconceptualization of goals is useful. In educationstudies, there is evidence that personal goal orien-tations may be manipulated at the classroom andschool levels (Ames, 1992; Anderman & Maehr,1994). Thus, it is important to promote the valueof task goals and to minimize focus on norma-tive and competitive language learning tasks bothwithin the classroom environment and within theschool itself.

    The construct of performance avoid goal ori-entation is particularly interesting because this isthefirst time this construct has been used with lan-guage learners. The construct indicates that a fearof failure is indeed a motivating force for languagelearners. An interesting result emerged from thestudy concerning the relationship between a per-formance avoid goal and a task goal. Usually theseconstructs are orthogonal or negatively related.Most previous studies have used Western partici-pants, whereas this study used mainly Asian par-

    ticipants. This study found a positive relationship(r = .25, p < .01). This result means that par-ticipants can be motivated by the task while theyare, at the same time, afraid of failure, which sug-gests that ethnicity influences motivational pat-terns. There is need for further research in thisarea.

    Other areas of the model raised interesting av-enues for further research. The relationship be-tween intensity and strategies seemed to functionas moderating variables and were not directly re-

    lated to the performance variable.The advantage of a model of adaptive learn-ing lies in the extent to which these constructscan be manipulated at the classroom and schoollevels (Anderman & Maehr, 1994). A task goal ori-

    entation may be promoted over a performancegoal by focusing students on the task or the pro-cess of learning in the classroom rather than ongetting a better score than another student. So,to promote a task goal, the focus should be oncriterion assessmentassessment in terms of task

    completionrather than on norm-referencing,which assesses students in comparison to others.This change in focus can be promoted at bothschoolandclassroom levels. Both schoolandclass-room discourse should focus on thetask, learning,and understanding rather than on examinationscores.

    The performance avoid goal orientation is con-sidered a maladaptive construct and is related toanxiety (Middleton& Midgely, 1997).Thus, teach-ers should not try to motivate students with threatsof failure, which are counterproductive.

    The application of the self-efficacy construct iswidespread in educational studies yet it is not socommon in language learning studies. Recent re-search indicates some evidence that self-efficacy isrelevant to language learning (Bong, 2001; Yuenget al., 2000). This study provided further sup-port for the relevance of this construct. In fact,self-efficacy showed the strongest loading ontothe latent variable of positive affect proposed inthe structural model, and, in the correlationalanalysis, self-efficacy was the strongest predictor

    of oral performance. Bandura (1997) referredto three sources of self-efficacy beliefs: mastery,vicariousness, and persuasion. In the classroom,self-efficacybeliefs maybepromoted through scaf-folding whereby learner support from the teachermoves from high to low, thus increasing the likeli-hood of success, and through decreasing negativeself-efficacy judgments, which could lead to fail-ure anda tendency togive up.Vicariousness refersto a learner adopting positive beliefs based on ob-serving the success of another student, If she can

    do it, so can I. Vicarious self-efficacy beliefs canbe promoted through the use of pair and groupwork. Perhaps the use of mentoring may be usefulfor enhancing self-judgments by pairing more ex-perienced and less experienced students. Beliefsbased on persuasion again may be facilitated bygroup and pair work and by teacher input.

    According to Pintrich (2003), self-efficacy be-liefs may be enhanced by providing accurate andpositive feedback to students concerning theircompetence and by giving them advice on de-

    veloping this competence. Further research is re-quired to assess how and what type of feedbackmay influence self-efficacy beliefs.

    Anxiety and self-confidence were combined toform a variable of positive affect in the structural

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    16/23

    312 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    model. However, the correlational analysis, al-though high (around r = .6, p=

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    17/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 313

    Davison, M. L. (1994). Multidimensional scaling modelsor personality responding. In S. Strack & M. Lorr(Eds.), Differentiating normal and abnormal person-ality(pp. 196216). New York: Springer.

    Davison, M. L., Kim, S., & Ding, S. (2001, April). Pro-file analysis via multidimensional scaling (PAMS):

    Exploring the prominent profile patterns in data. Pa-

    per presented at the annual meeting of Amer-ican Educational Research Association, Seattle,

    Washington.Dornyei, Z. (1990). Conceptualizing motivation in for-

    eign language learning. Language Learning, 40,4578.

    Dornyei, Z. (1994). Understanding L2 motivation: Onwith the challenge. Modern Language Journal, 78,519538.

    Dornyei, Z. (2000). Motivation in action: Towards a pro-cess oriented conceptualization of student moti-

    vation. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70,

    519538.Elliot, A. J. (1999). Approach and avoidance motiva-

    tion and achievement goals. Educational Psycholo-gist, 34(3), 169189.

    Elliot, A. J., & Church, M. (1997). A hierarchical modelof approach and avoidance achievement motiva-tion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72,218232.

    Elliot, A. J., & McGregor, H. (1999). Test anxiety andthehierarchicalmodel of approach andavoidanceachievement motivation. Journal of Personality andSocial Psychology, 76, 628644.

    Elliot, A. J., McGregor, H., & Gable, S. (1999).Achievement goals, study strategies, and examperformance: A mediational analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 549563.

    Garcia, T., & Pintrich, P. (1996). The effects of au-tonomy on motivation and performance in thecollege classroom. Contemporary Educational Psy-chology, 21, 477486.

    Gardner, R. C. (1985). Social psychology and languagelearning. London: Edward Arnold.

    Gardner, R. C. (2001). Integrative motivation andsecond language acquisition. In Z. Dornyei &R. Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second lan-

    guage learning (pp. 119). Honolulu: Universityof Hawaii.

    Gardner, R. C., & MacIntyre, P. D. (1993). A studentscontributions to second language learning, PartII: Affective variables. Language Teaching, 26, 111.

    Gardner, R. C., Tremblay, P., & Masgoret, A. (1997). To-wards a full model of second language learning:An empirical investigation. Modern Language Jour-nal, 81, 344362.

    Grant,H., & Dweck,C. S. (2001). Cross cultural responseto failure: Considering outcome attributions with

    different goals. In F. Salili, C. Chiu, & Y. Y. Heng(Eds.), Student motivation: The culture and contextof learning (pp. 203219). New York: Plenum.

    Holmes-Smith, P. (2000). Introduction to structural equa-tion modeling using LISREL. Melbourne, Australia:

    School Research, Evaluation and MeasurementServices.

    Holtgraves,T. (2004). Social desirability andself-reports:Testing models of socially desirable responding.Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(2),161172.

    Horwitz, E. K. (2001). Language anxiety and achieve-

    ment. Annual Review of Applied Linguistics, 21,112126.

    Horwitz, E. K., Horwitz, M. E., & Cope, J. (1986). For-eign language classroomanxiety. Modern LanguageJournal, 70, 125132.

    Krosnick, J. A. (1999). Survey research. Annual Reviewof Psychology, 50, 537567.

    Lucas, J. (1994). Communication apprehension in theESL classroom: Getting our students to talk. For-eign Language Annals, 593598.

    MacIntyre, P. D. (1999). Language anxiety: A reviewof research for language teachers. In D. J. Young

    (Ed.), Affect in foreign language and second languagelearning: A practical guide to creating a low anxiety

    classroom atmosphere(pp. 2445). Boston: McGraw-Hill.

    MacIntyre, P. D., MacMaster, K., & Baker, S. C. (2001).The convergence of multiple models of motiva-tion for second language learning: Gardner, Pin-trich, Kuhl and McCroskey. In Z. Dornyei & R.Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second language ac-quisition (pp. 461492). Honolulu: University ofHawaii.

    Middleton, M., & Midgely, C. (1997). Avoiding thedemonstration of lack of ability: An under-explored aspect of goal theory. Journal of Educa-tional Psychology, 89, 710718.

    Midgley, C., Kaplan, A., Middleton, M., Maehr,M., Urdan, T., Anderman, L., et al. (1998).The development and validation of scales as-sessing students achievement goal orientations.Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 113131.

    Midgley, C., Maehr, M., Hicks, L., Roeser, R., Urdan, T.,& Anderman, E. (1997). Patterns of adaptive learn-ing survey. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan.

    Milligan, G. W., & Cooper, M. C. (1987). Clustering

    methods. Applied Psychological Measurement, 11,329354.

    Naveh-Benjamin, M. (1991). A comparison of trainingprograms intended for different types of test anx-ious students: Further support for an information-processing model. Journal of Educational Psychol-ogy, 83, 134139.

    Noels, K. (2001). New orientations in language learn-ing motivation: Towards a model of intrinsic, ex-trinsic orientations and motivation. In Z. Dornyei& R. Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second lan-guage learning (pp. 4369). Honolulu: University

    of Hawaii.Oller, J. W. (1982). Gardner on affect: A reply to Gard-ner. Language Learning, 32, 183189.

    Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language learning strategies: Whatevery teacher should know. Boston: Heinle.

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    18/23

    314 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    Oxford, R. L. (1994). Where are we regarding languagelearning motivation?Modern Language Journal, 78,512514.

    Oxford, R. L. (Ed.). (1996). Language learning strategiesaround the world. Honolulu: University of Hawaii.

    Oxford, R. L., & Crookall, D. (1989). Research on lan-guage learning strategies: Methods, findings and

    instructional issues. Modern Language Journal, 73,404419.

    Oxford, R. L., & Shearin, J. (1994). Language learningmotivation: Expanding the theoretical construct.Modern Language Journal, 78, 1228.

    Pajares, F., & Johnson, M. (1996). Self-efficacy beliefsand the writing performance of entering highschool students. Psychology in Schools, 33, 163175.

    Park, G. P. (1997). Language learning strategies andEnglish proficiency in Korean university students.Foreign Language Annals, 30, 211221.

    Phillips, E. (1992). The effects of language anxiety

    on students oral test performance and attitudes.Modern Language Journal, 76, 1426.

    Pintrich, P. (2003). A motivational science perspectiveon the role of student motivation in learning andteaching contexts. Journal of Educational Psychol-ogy, 95, 667686.

    Pintrich, P., & DeGroot, E. (1990). Motivational andself-regulated learning components of classroomacademic performance. Journal of Educational Psy-chology, 82, 3340.

    Pintrich, P., & Garcia, T. (1991). Student goal orienta-tion and self-regulation in the college classroom.Advances in Motivation and Achievement, 7, 371402.

    Price, M. L. (1991). Thesubjective experience of foreignlanguage anxiety: Interviews with highly anxiousstudents.In E. K. Horwitz & D. J. Young (Eds.),For-eign language anxiety: From theory to research to class-

    room practices(pp.101108). Englewood Cliffs,NJ:Prentice Hall.

    Schmidt, R., & Watanabe, Y. (2001). Motivation, strategyuse and pedagogical preferences. In Z. Dornyei &R. Schmidt (Eds.), Motivation and second language

    learning (pp. 313352). Honolulu: University ofHawaii.

    Scovel, T. (1978). The effect of affect on foreign lan-guage learning: A review of the anxiety research.Language Learning, 28, 129141.

    Skaalvik, E. (1997). Self-enhancing and self-defeatingego orientations: Relations with task and avoid-

    ance orientation, achievement, self-perceptionsand anxiety. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89,7181.

    Smith, M., Duda, J., Allen, J., & Hall,H. (2002). Contem-porary measures of approach and avoidance goalorientations: Similarities and differences. BritishJournal of Educational Psychology, 72, 155190.

    Tobias, S. (1985). Test anxiety: Interference, defectiveskills and cognitive capacity. Educational Psycholo-gist, 3, 135142.

    Tremblay, P., & Gardner, R. (1995). Expanding the mo-tivation construct in language learning. Modern

    Language Journal, 79, 505520.Urdan, T. (1997). Achievement goal theory: Past results

    and future directions. Advances in Motivation andAchievement, 10, 99141.

    Woodrow, L. J. (2001). Towards a model of adaptive learn-ing: A pilot study. (ERIC Document ReproductionService No. ED456645)

    Woodrow, L. J. (2003). A model of adaptive learning of in-ternational English for academic purposes (EAP) stu-

    dents at Australian universities. Unpublished doc-toral thesis, University of Sydney.

    Woodrow, L. J. (2005). The challenge of measuringlanguage learning strategies. Foreign Language An-nals, 38, 90100.

    Young, D. J. (1991). Creating a low-anxiety classroomenvironment:What does anxietyresearch suggest?Modern Language Journal, 75, 426438.

    Yueng, A. S., Chui, H. S., Lau, I. C., McInerney, D. M.,Russell-Bowie, D., & Suliman, R. (2000). Where isthe hierarchy of academic self-concept? Journal ofEducational Psychology, 92, 556567.

    Zeidner, M. (1998). Test anxiety: The state of the art. NewYork: Plenum.

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    19/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 315

    APPENDIX ACorrelation Matrices

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Motivation Goal Orientations

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

    1 1.002 .31 1.003 .07 .26 1.004 .45 .11 .09 1.005 .05 .51 .32 .08 1.006 .22 .19 .15 .27 .21 1.007 .30 .14 .02 .42 .02 .36 1.008 .03 .31 .10 .08 .39 .14 .01 1.009 .01 .25 .50 .09 .38 .11 .14 .30 1.00

    10 .33 .17 .08 .29 .15 .27 .22 .13 .08 1.0011 .14 .21 .61 .06 .34 .17 .10 .14 .51 .26 1.0012 .12 .38 .27 .06 .42 .19 .15 .34 .31 .27 .31 1.0013 .13 .39 .27 .07 .48 .15 .11 .34 .41 .18 .31 .45 1.00

    14 .04 .29 .55 .04 .31 .12 .11 .17 .62 .08 .59 .31 .36 1.0015 .02 .26 .41 .07 .25 .09 .01 .18 .41 .08 .57 .30 .30 .67 1.00

    Key to Variables1 Task 1 Like tasks that learn from even if make mistakes2 Avoid 1 Others wont think Im poor at English3 Approach 1 Doing better than other students4 Task 2 Interested in English work5 Avoid 2 Avoid looking like I cant do English tasks6 Task 3 I like English learning tasks best when I really have to think7 Task 4 Like to learn new things8 Avoid 3 Avoid looking stupid9 Approach 2 Other students in my English class think Im good at English

    10 Task 5 Want to get better at English11 Approach 3 Do better than most of the other students12 Avoid 4 Dont embarrass myself13 Avoid 5 Dont look as though I cant speak English in my class14 Approach 4 Show my English teacher that Im better at English15 Approach 5 Only one who could answer the teachers questions in my class

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Self-Efficacy

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8

    1 1.002 .77 1.003 .74 .79 1.004 .75 .73 .76 1.005 .62 .50 .53 .59 1.006 .60 .52 .50 .58 .63 1.007 .59 .51 .52 .56 .63 .85 1.008 .52 .50 .49 .55 .59 .79 .81 1.00

    Key to Variables1 In-Class Self-Efficacy Answer question from teacher2 In-Class Self-Efficacy Act as group spokesperson3 In-Class Self-Efficacy Take part in role play

    4 In-Class Self-Efficacy Participate in formal discussion5 Out-of-Class Self-Efficacy Ask questions to lecturer6 Out-of-Class Self-Efficacy Take part conversation more than one NS7 Out-of-Class Self-Efficacy Start a conversation with known NS8 Out-of-Class Self-Efficacy Start a conversation with unknown NS

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    20/23

    316 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Second Language Speaking Anxiety

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    1 1.002 .66 1.003 .67 .53 1.00

    4 .56 .47 .53 1.005 .50 .43 .48 .70 1.006 .61 .49 .56 .59 .58 1.007 .37 .54 .36 .30 .32 .39 1.008 .35 .52 .42 .29 .28 .37 .85 1.009 .55 .49 .40 .44 .42 .44 .54 .52 1.00

    10 .45 .51 .38 .38 .33 .39 .54 .52 .81 1.0011 .37 .43 .32 .35 .28 .33 .60 .60 .57 .58 1.00

    Key to Variables1 In-Class Anxiety Answer question teacher2 In-Class Anxiety Speak informally to my teacher3 In-Class Anxiety Take part in group discussions

    4 In-Class Anxiety Take part in a role play5 In-Class Anxiety Give an oral presentation6 In-Class Anxiety Contribute to formal discussions7 Out-of-Class Anxiety Take part conversation more than one native speaker (NS)8 Out-of-Class Anxiety Start conversation with native speaker9 Out-of-Class Anxiety Answer question from lecturer

    10 Out-of-Class Anxiety Ask lecturer for advice11 Out-of-Class Anxiety Answer question unknown NS

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Language Learning Strategies

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15

    1 1.002 .41 1.003 .33 .35 1.004 .39 .38 .30 1.005 .33 .42 .24 .39 1.006 .38 .38 .27 .32 .29 1.007 .58 .45 .42 .37 .35 .47 1.008 .37 .15 .25 .22 .34 .26 .30 1.009 .29 .15 .14 .25 .15 .20 .15 .42 1.00

    10 .31 .35 .23 .30 .19 .31 .37 .40 .44 1.0011 .43 .30 .30 .31 .23 .38 .41 .35 .36 .57 1.0012 .27 .26 .15 .27 .27 .20 .22 .28 .38 .44 .40 1.0013 .18 .20 .14 .20 .19 .07 .14 .32 .40 .38 .19 .43 1.00

    14 .24 .32 .23 .30 .14 .28 .39 .27 .21 .31 .32 .30 .13 1.0015 .35 .32 .26 .22 .22 .31 .38 .35 .20 .32 .33 .34 .15 .64 1.00

    Key to Variables1 Cognitive Strategy Go through assignments after feedback2 Cognitive Strategy Reread course materials3 Cognitive Strategy Review ahead for tests4 Cognitive Strategy Memorize words5 Cognitive Strategy Determine unfamiliar concepts for tests6 Cognitive Strategy Concerned with keeping up7 Cognitive Strategy Organize what learned8 Metacognitive Strategy Relate words to each other9 Metacognitive Strategy Guess words from context

    10 Metacognitive Strategy Evaluate progress11 Metacognitive Strategy Decide what need to learn12 Metacognitive Strategy Ask teacher to explain13 Metacognitive Strategy Practice with native speakers14 Social Strategy Work with others on assignments15 Social Strategy Discuss course with classmates

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    21/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 317

    Structural Equation Model

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10

    1 1.002 .59 1.003 .30 .30 1.00

    4 .37 .37 .53 1.005 .15 .20 .42 .46 1.006 .15 .05 .07 .26 .14 1.007 .22 .09 .18 .35 .18 .65 1.008 .43 .37 .59 .48 .29 .18 .25 1.009 .23 .24 .38 .21 .19 .01 .03 .49 1.00

    10 .24 .10 .10 .19 .04 .26 .33 .14 .03 1.00

    Key to Variables1 Task goal2 Integrative orientation3 Cognitive strategies4 Metacognitive strategies

    5 Social strategies6 Anxiety reversed7 Self-efficacy8 Effort9 Persistence

    10 International English Language Testing Service oral assessment

    All Summated Subscale Means

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13

    1 1.00

    2 .14 1.003 .23 .48 1.004 .59 .17 .28 1.005 .38 .10 .16 .35 1.006 .16 .02 .20 .06 .12 1.007 .11 .13 .22 .03 .05 .58 1.008 .18 .00 .17 .04 .17 .59 .39 1.009 .22 .08 .17 .12 .12 .47 .64 .67 1.00

    10 .30 .08 .06 .30 .56 .11 .01 .18 .15 1.0011 .37 .05 .02 .37 .39 .31 .17 .34 .29 .53 1.0012 .15 .06 .11 .21 .27 .17 .09 .16 .17 .42 .46 1.0013 .24 .01 .23 .10 .07 .23 .24 .28 .33 .10 .19 .04 1.00

    Significant at .01 level.Significant at .05 level.Key to Variables1 Task goal orientation2 Performance approach goal orientation3 Performance avoid goal orientation4 Integrative goal orientation5 Motivational intensity6 In-class anxiety7 Out-of-class anxiety8 In-class self-efficacy9 Out-of-class self-efficacy

    10 Cognitive language learning strategies11 Metacognitive language learning strategies12 Social language learning strategies13 IELTS oral performance indicator

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    22/23

    318 The Modern Language Journal 90 (2006)

    Correlations Between Multidimensional Scaling (MDS) Profiles

    1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11

    1 1.002 .28 1.003 .34 .20 1.00

    4 .24

    .09 .02 1.005 .11 .11 .00 .10 1.006 .15 .04 .04 .72 .03 1.007 .41 .05 .02 .22 .12 .11 1.008 .01 .09 .03 .21 .05 .16 .29 1.009 .20 .04 .24 .31 .01 .25 .12 .06 1.00

    10 .17 .02 .30 .16 .05 .13 .06 .14 .53 1.00

    11 .26 .07 .01 .32 .08 .25 .12 .24 .16 .09 1.00

    Significant at the .05 level. Significant at the .01 level.Key to MDS Profiles1 Task versus performance goal profile2 Approach versus avoid goal profile3 Motivation profile level4 Anxiety versus self-efficacy profile5 In-class versus out-of-class affect profile6 Affect profile level7 Cognitive versus metacognitive strategies profile8 Social versus individual strategies profile9 Strategies level profile

    10 Motivational intensity11 Oral performance

    APPENDIX BFit Indexes

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Motivation Goal Orientations

    2 p 2/df RMR RMSEA GFI AGFI CFI NFI NNFI

    Motivation Goals 194.96 .000 2.22 .06 .07 .91 .87 .90 .84 .88

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Second Language Speaking Self-Efficacy

    2 p 2/df RMR RMSEA GFI AGFI CFI NFI NNFI

    Self-Efficacy 61.17 .00 3.22 .05 .09 .95 .90 .97 .96 .96

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Second Language Speaking Self-Efficacy

    2 p 2/df RMR RMSEA GFI AGFI CFI NFI NNFI

    Anxiety 172.62 .00 4.32 .07 .11 .90 .83 .93 .84 .90

    Confirmatory Factor Analysis of Model of Language Learning Strategies

    2 p 2/df RMR RMSEA GFI AGFI CFI NFI NNFI

    Strategies 180.50 .00 2.07 .05 .06 .92 .88 .92 .86 .90

  • 7/29/2019 A Model of Adaptive Language Learning (MLJ 2006)

    23/23

    Lindy J. Woodrow 319

    Structural Model of Adaptive Learning

    2 p 2/df RMR RMSEA GFI AGFI CFI NFI NNFI

    Model 72.56 .00 2.41 .05 .07 .95 .91 .95 .92 .92

    Note. 2 = chi square statistic; 2/df= normed chi square divided by degrees of freedom; RMR= root meansquare residual; RMSEA = root mean square error approximation; GFI = goodness-of-fit index; AGFI =adjusted goodness-of-fit index; CFI = comparative fit index; NFI = normed fit index; NNFI = non-normed fitindex.

    AAUSC Series Editorship Assumed by Carl Blyth

    Carl Blyth, University of Texas, Austin, assumed the role of Series Editor of the annual volume ofthe American Association of University Supervisors, Coordinators, and Directors of Foreign LanguagePrograms on January 1, 2006. Following Sally Magnan, University of Wisconsin, who began the series in1990 and edited it through the 2006 volume, Blyth will now accept proposals for volumes beginning in2008. The Series has been published since its inception by Thomson/Heinle: www.heinle.com.

    AAUSC volumes address the needs and interests of scholars who direct multisection courses in foreignlanguages. Their scope is broad and not limited to one or two years of instruction, in recognition ofthe impact that lowerlevel language courses have on the entire undergraduate curriculum and thusthe role that the Language Program Director plays in that curriculum. They also address concerns ofgraduate student professional development with regard to teaching, and programmatic issues affecting

    language learning in the postsecondary environment.

    Recent volumes include:2005 Internet-Mediated Intercultural Foreign Language Education, Julie A. Belz and Steven L. Thorne,

    editors2004 Language Program Articulation: Developing a Theoretical Foundation, Catherine M. Barrette and Kate

    Paesani, editors2003 Advanced Foreign Language Learning: A Challenge to College Programs, Heidi Byrnes and Hiram H.

    Maxim, editors

    The upcoming volumes are:2006 Insights From Study Abroad for Language Programs, Sharon Wilkinson, editor

    2007 From Thought to Action: Exploring Beliefs and Outcomes in the Foreign LanguagePrograms, H. Jay Siskin, editor

    Send proposals for future volumes to Carl Blyth: [email protected]