99655Avanzini

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

o9youihonuihoiuhnoiuhu

Citation preview

  • INTERPLAY OF THE TWO CULTURES: NEUROAESTHETICS

    Neuroscience and music

    Giuliano Avanzini

    Received: 3 November 2011 / Accepted: 27 April 2012 / Published online: 21 July 2012! Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei 2012

    Abstract Music is part of all known human cultures,which suggests that musical competences are encoded in

    the human brain. Attempts to define the cerebral organi-

    sation of music perception and production date back to thebeginning of the modern era of the neurosciences, but the

    subject has been adequately developed on only over the last

    two decades. The combined approaches of neuropsycho-logical, neurophysiological and neuroimaging methods

    have demonstrated that the musical information reaching

    the primary auditory cortex undergoes a series of processesinvolving both hemispheres, but with predominance of the

    right. Particularly interesting is the demonstration of

    overlapping brain mechanisms that create and maintainlearned sound categories in both the linguistic and musical

    domains i.e. Brocas area, whose functional properties

    hierarchically organise perceptually discrete elements(words or musical tones) into structured sequences based

    on syntactic principles. Only a few studies have addressed

    the challenging task of investigating the neurobiologicalbasis of music by aesthetic categories. One possible

    approach is to take advantage of neurophysiologicalmethods capable of detecting brain responses to novel

    sound combinations as a signature of creativity. Most

    sound data relate to the brain processing of music-relatedemotions. The results of studies carried out over the last

    10 years demonstrate that intensely pleasurable responses

    to music correlate with activity in the brain regionsinvolved in the emotional colour of biologically important

    activities such as nutrition and reproduction. Whether such

    structures are of general interest for the perception of theartistic quality of music is an exciting question that needs

    to addressed by comparing the results obtained in other

    domains of neuroaesthetics.

    Keywords Music ! Neuromusic ! Neuroaesthetics ! Musicperception ! Music and languages ! Music and emotions

    1 Introduction

    It is probably impossible to find a single human beingbelonging to any culture who has no experience of music.

    However, although everyone would say that he knows what

    music is, any attempt to explain all of its relevant aspectswould be consistently inadequate. Even the popular dis-

    tinction between noise and music is inappropriate, because

    the noise produced by percussion instruments forms part ofthe acoustic material currently used in music. Conse-

    quently, when searching for a definition of music, we areleft with the tautological statement that music is a set of

    musically organised sounds. The definition of music by

    aesthetic categories is even more elusive: what gives a setof musically organised sounds the quality of an artistic

    product? Nobody could disagree with Edward Hanslicks

    description of the concordance and opposition of sounds,their running away and catching each other, their growing

    up and dying, this is what our spirit perceives as beautiful

    (Hanslick 1854), but whether any given musical composi-tion is perceived as artistic by different listeners depends

    on such a multifactorial process that the result is often

    unpredictable.

    This contribution is the written, peer-reviewed version of a paperpresented at the Golgi Symposium on Perspectives inNeuroaesthetics, held at the Accademia Nazionale dei Lincei in Romeon June 13, 2011.

    G. Avanzini (&)Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta,Via Celoria 11, 20133 Milan, Italye-mail: [email protected]

    123

    Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304

    DOI 10.1007/s12210-012-0180-6

  • These considerations suggest that any attempt to discuss

    the role of the neurosciences in fostering our understandingof musical abilities in a neuro-aesthetic perspective is so

    difficult that I am challenged to try.

    2 Music and the brain

    The relationship between music and brain functions or

    dysfunctions has been a subject of interest since timeimmemorial. In ancient civilisations, chants and the sounds

    of instruments were used to induce a positive effect on an

    individuals cognitive and psychological functioning, andto cure melancholy and other psychopathological altera-

    tions. However, it was only when the fine structure of the

    brain and its correlations with sensory and motor functionscame to be known that it was possible to investigate the

    cerebral organisation of musical competences.

    The first attempts to localise musical functions can befound in papers published in 1865 by Bouillaud and 1888

    by Knoblauch, who coined the word amusia and ana-

    lysed the disturbed musical functions of patients with braindiseases. However, although interest never ceased, there

    were only relatively few further publications until 1977,

    when the now classic book Music and the Brain, editedby Macdonald Critchley and R. A. Henson (Critchley and

    Henson 1977), came out. This book substantially alerted

    the scientific community to the neurological aspects ofmusic by providing a complete account of the literature on

    the subject: no more than thirty papers, including that of

    Alajouanine (1948) on Ravels degenerative encephalopa-thy and the paper by Luria and Tsvetkova (1965) on

    Shebalins stroke.

    Subsequently, various groups gradually emerged thatgave a new impetus to investigating the relationships

    between the neurosciences and music. Although I risk

    forgetting some important name, I would mention Robert JZatorre and Isabel Peretz in Montreal, Carol L Krumhansl

    in New York, Diana Deutsch in San Diego (la Jolla),

    Sandra Trehub in Toronto, Eckart Altenmueller inHannover, John Sloboda in Keele, Mireille Besson in

    Marseille, Christo Pantev in Munster, Mari Tervaniemi

    in Helsinki, Timothy D. Griffiths in Newcastle-upon-Tyne,Joseph P. Rauschecker in Washington, Gottifried Schlaug

    in Boston. These and the many other groups that have

    entered the field over the last 10 years were present at theconference The Biological Foundations of Music (New

    York 2000) sponsored by the New York Academy of

    Sciences, and at the series of Neurosciences and Musicconferences sponsored by the Mariani foundation of

    Milano (Venice 2003, Leipzig 2005, Montreal 2008, and

    Edinburgh 2010). These meetings originated a series ofvolumes published in the Annals of the New York

    Academy of Sciences, which reflect the development of

    Neuromusic in the last 10 years (Zatorre and Peretz 2001;Avanzini et al. 2003, 2005; Dalla Bella et al. 2009).

    A Medline search shows that about 2,000 papers have

    been published in qualified scientific journals since 1964,three-quarters of them in the last 12 years. This bears

    witness of the growing interest in this field of the neuro-

    sciences and, although this is not surprising (because thepresence of music in all human cultures indicates that

    musical competences are encoded in our brain), what israther surprising is the fact that it developed so much later

    than the interest in language, which has been one of the

    main objects of brain research ever since Brocas pio-neering observations (Broca 1861).

    The investigations have taken advantage of the wide

    range of modern techniques and have led to impressiveadvances in our understanding of the musical brain. The

    results were made possible by the development of methods

    capable of analysing the specific musical quality of theacoustic stimuli used in the experiments.

    3 Methods

    3.1 Neuropsychology

    The classical approach to the study of cognitive functions

    is based on tests specially designed to assess the func-tion(s) of interest. To investigate musical abilities, Isabelle

    Peretz has developed a battery of musical tests since 1987

    (the Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusias, MBEA;Peretz et al. 2003) based on a modular model of the pro-

    cessing components involved in music perception (Fig. 1).

    She has subsequently adjusted and validated the MBEAusing different groups of individuals and neurological

    patients, and has thus provided the scientific community

    with a standardised instrument for assessing cognitivemusical functions (Peretz et al. 2003; Peretz and Coltheart

    2003). The MBEA includes a series of tests designed to

    assess the ability to recognise melodic organisation (con-tour, interval scale), temporal organisation (rhythm and

    metre), and musical memory. The tests are based on a pool

    of 30 musical phrases written with sufficient complexity toguarantee its processing as a meaningful structure rather

    than a simple sequence of tones. The principle is to

    introduce manipulations that alter the original sequences,and the task is to recognise that the manipulated stimulus is

    different from the original. In the case of the melodic

    organisation tests, the pitch is modified so that it is out ofscale (scale-violated melody), the contour is changed

    without affecting the key (contour-violated melody), and

    the interval is changed while preserving the original con-tour and scale (interval-violated melody). In the case of

    296 Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304

    123

  • temporal organisation, the manipulation affects the dura-

    tion of two adjacent tones, which leads to a change inrhythmic grouping while retaining the same metre (rhythm-

    violated stimulus), or the metre itself (metre-violated

    stimulus). The final test concerns the ability to recognise apreviously presented melody in a group of new melodies

    (memory recognition).

    The MBEA principle of using acoustic material in whichmusically relevant parameters have been violated has been

    widely adopted in investigations of the brain processing of

    music, in which cerebral responses are revealed by meansof electrophysiological recordings or functional imaging

    techniques.

    3.2 Neurophysiology

    While searching for perception-related electrical activitiesof the brain, Naatanen et al. (1978) discovered a negative-

    going peak of event-related potentials (ERPs) that was

    elicited by a deviance within a sequence of otherwiseregular stimuli (Fig. 2). Currently known as mismatch

    negativity (MMN), this can occur in any sensory system

    but has been most frequently studied in terms of auditionand vision. Auditory MMN is a fronto-central negative

    potential with sources in the primary and non-primaryauditory cortex, and a typical latency of 150250 ms.

    Further investigations by Mari Tervaniemi of the same

    Helsinki group (Saarinen et al. 1992) demonstrated that

    MMN provides a suitable index of the detection of deviant

    sounds in a regular sequence (Tervaniemi 2001). MMN canalso be recorded by means of magneto-encephalography

    (MEG), which has some advantages over electroencepha-

    lography in terms of source analysis. The main MMNgenerator is located within and near the primary auditory

    cortex, with a small contribution coming from the oper-

    cular part of the inferior frontal gyrus.Another deviance-related component of ERPs is early

    right anterior negativity (ERAN), which was first described

    by Koelsch et al. (2000) and named by analogy with earlyleft anterior negativity (ELAN), an ERP effect that is

    elicited by syntactic irregularities during language per-

    ception (Friederici 2002). ERAN is usually maximalbetween 150 and 250 ms, has an anterior scalp distribution

    and often right-hemispheric prevalence, and its main gen-

    erators seem to be located in the inferior fronto-lateralcortex (Brodmanns area 44), with additional contributions

    coming from the ventrolateral premotor cortex and the

    anterior superior temporal gyrus (Fig. 3). Both ERAN andMMN are elicited by acoustic events that do not match a

    preceding sequence, but they differ in terms of topography;

    furthermore, the model of regularities relevant to MMN isextracted online from the acoustic environment, whereas

    the structural model of ERAN processing is based onrepresentations of syntactic regularities that already exist in

    a long-term memory format. In other words, the repre-

    sentations of regularities building the structural model of

    Acoustic input

    Acoustic analysis

    Pitch organization TemporalorganizationAcoustic-toPhonologicalconversion

    Rhythmanalysis

    Meteranalysis

    Contouranalysis

    Intervalanalysis

    Tonalencoding

    Emotionexpression

    analysis

    MusicalLexicon

    PhonologicalLexicon

    Vocal planformation

    Associativememories

    Singing Tapping Speaking

    Acoustic input

    Acoustic analysis

    Pitch organization TemporalorganizationAcoustic-toPhonologicalconversion

    Rhythmanalysis

    Meteranalysis

    Contouranalysis

    Intervalanalysis

    Tonalencoding

    Emotionexpression

    analysis

    MusicalLexicon

    PhonologicalLexicon

    Vocal planformation

    Associativememories

    Singing Tapping Speaking

    Fig. 1 Modular model of musicand language processingaccording to Peretz andColtheart (2003). The followingprocessing components: tonalencoding, interval analysis,contour analysis, musicallexicon, vocal plan formationand singing are specific tomusic. Component whosespecificity to music arecurrently unknown are: rhythmanalysis, meter analysis, andemotion expression analysis. Aneurological anomaly mayeither damage a processingcomponent or interfere with theflow of information betweentwo components (redrawn fromPeretz and Coltheart 2003)

    Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304 297

    123

  • the acoustic environment are more sensory in the case of

    MMN, and more cognitive in the case of ERAN (Koelsch

    2009).We have used a different neurophysiological approach

    in our laboratory to investigate brain responses to musical

    material by analysing the ERPs elicited by individual notesbelonging to structured or unstructured sequences (Minati

    et al. 2008, 2010). We consistently observed differences in

    the latency, duration and topographical distribution of theN2 components elicited by structured or unstructured

    stimuli (Fig. 4), but their significance in relation to the

    musical quality of the stimuli is still under investigation.

    3.3 Neuroimaging

    Because of their unique ability to describe neural activities

    in real time at a scale of milliseconds, neurophysiological

    techniques are first-line methods in neuroscience investi-gations, but they are much less satisfactory in terms of

    spatial resolution, which is one of the main strengths of

    imaging techniques. Combined neurophysiological andimaging investigations (such as can be obtained by making

    neurophysiological recordings in the context magnetic

    resonance imaging, MR) have been profitably used inneuromusic research as in many other fields of the neuro-

    sciences. Moreover, functional magnetic resonance imag-

    ing (fMRI) has effectively advanced our understanding ofthe musical brain whenever a high-time resolution is not

    required. Figure 5 shows a good example of the power of

    fMRI to investigate the cortical representation of the sound

    Fig. 2 Mismatch negativity response to a 2.5 % pitch change.a Event-related potentials (ERP) to a frequently presented standardtone (thin line) and to an infrequently presented deviant tone (thickline). The x axis represents the time after the sound onset inmilliseconds and the y axis the strength of activity in microvolts.b The waveform is calculated by subtracting the ERP to standard tonefrom the ERP to deviant tone. On the schematic brain the recordingelectrodes (Fz, Rm) are indicated (from Tervaniemi 2001)

    Fig. 3 Early right anterior negativity (ERAN) elicited by irregularchord sequences. a chords built on the tones of the C major scale.Within the tonal hierarchy the chord on the first scale tone (tonicchord) is the most stable chord, followed by the dominant and thesubdominant, whereas the submediant and the supertonic representless stable chords. The major chord on the second tone of a majorscale can be interpreted as the dominant to the dominant (squarebrackets). In majorminor tonal music, chord functions are arrangedwithin harmonic sequences according to certain regularities, forexample the dominant-tonic progression shown in b (left) is a typicalmarker for the end of a harmonic sequence. On the contrary theharmonic progression shown in b (right) ending on dominant todominant cord is highly irregular, c the final chord of the irregularsequence shown in b (right) elicits an ERAN better detectable in thedifference wave, which represents regular subtracted from irregularchords. d With MEG, the magnetic equivalent of the ERAN waslocalized in the inferior frontolateral cortex. The dipoles indicate thegrand-average of single-subject dipole solutions (modified fromKoelsch 2009)

    298 Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304

    123

  • parameters relevant to music; the imaging subtraction

    procedure is particularly useful when comparing differ-ences in the functional status of the brain with respect to a

    specific system.

    The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow bymeans of positron emission tomography (PET) is also

    useful for mapping the cerebral areas involved in musicprocessing, not least because the images can be superim-

    posed on MR images using fusion techniques. Figure 6

    shows some examples of the results that can be obtained

    using this method.

    4 Anatomo-functional organisation of the musicalcompetences of the brain

    Like any other type of acoustic stimulation, a musical

    stimulus activates the peripheral and central systems that

    Fig. 4 Event-related potential traces (solid line, average; dotted lines,average7SD) from the mesial frontal (Fz) and central (Cz) electrodes,for notes in melodies and unstructured note sequences. The two traces

    clearly differ in the N2 component that is delayed and increased induration when evoked by structured stimulus. Left column musicians,Right column non-musicians (from Minati et al. 2008)

    Fig. 5 fMRI activation elicited by different auditory stimuli. Theposition and orientation of the sections is depicted in the bottompanels. Note that unlike the standard use the left hemisphere is on theleft side and the right one to the right. Note also that the axial sectionis tilted relative to the horizontal plane to show the surface of theplanum temporale on which (structural MRI, bottom) the Heschlgyrus is represented as a white area. The differential activation,

    obtained by contrasting noise to silence, fixed pitch to noise, tonicmelodies to fixed pitch and random melodies to fixed pitch aredepicted in different grey gradation (from darker to lighter). Theactivation area elicited by increasingly complex acoustic stimulimoves increasingly anteriorly to the primary auditory area andbecomes increasingly lateralised to the right hemisphere (fromPatterson et al. 2002)

    Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304 299

    123

  • convey the information to the primary auditory cortex(Brodmanns area 41, the supratemporalis granulosa) inthe Heschl gyrus on the superior face of the temporal lobe

    (also called the planum temporale). Area 41 is a granularcortex and, like all sensory cortices (also called konio-

    cortices), is characterised by a predominance of non-

    pyramidal neurons that distorts the eulaminated pattern ofnon-specialised isocortices (such as area 42 that surrounds

    area 41). A topographically ordered frequency representa-

    tion is clearly recognizable in the primary auditory cortexwith high frequencies being represented mediocaudally and

    low frequencies rostrolaterally. Subjects with right lesionsinvolving the primary auditory cortex have deficits in pitch

    perception. A detailed analysis of the topographical dis-

    tribution of the responses to pitch and melody on the pla-num temporale has been carried out by Patterson et al.(2002). The experiments showed that the representation of

    noise, notes with a fixed pitch, tonic melodies and randommelodies only partially overlap, because the topographical

    representation of the activations elicited by increasingly

    complex acoustic stimuli moves increasingly anteriorly tothe primary auditory area and becomes increasingly lat-

    eralised to the right hemisphere (Fig. 5).

    It has been shown that other cortical areas within oroutside the temporal lobe are activated when listening to

    music or discriminating specific music components (e.g.

    musical tempi and metre, Fig. 6). The order of activation(and therefore the significance of the activations) is

    sometimes difficult to define because of the limited time

    resolution of imaging techniques. The analysis of theauditory cortex organization supports the role of the

    superior temporal gyrus for complex sound processing

    (Rauschecker 1998). Comparative studies in non-human

    primates and humans led Rauschecker and Scott (2009)

    and DeWitt and Rauschecker (2012) to identify the supe-rior temporal gyrus and its projections to ventrolateral

    prefrontal cortex as the auditory anteroventral stream for

    language and music processing. Liegeois-Chauvel et al.(1998) studied the specific contribution of different tem-

    poral areas in patients who had undergone temporal cor-

    tectomies for the treatment of drug-refractory epilepsies,and confirmed the importance of the superior temporal

    gyrus in melody processing. They also demonstrated adissociation between rhythm and metre, and the critical

    involvement of the anterior part of the superior temporal

    gyrus in metrical processing.Additional data on the cortical areas involved in music

    perception will be provided below, in the sections dis-

    cussing the relationships between music and language, andbetween music and emotions.

    Further information on the organization of the musical

    brain has been obtained from the analysis of brainmechanisms of musical performance. With this approach

    Schlaugh (2001), Pantev et al. (2001) and Pasqual-Leone

    (2001) provided the evidence of plastic adaptive changesin different brain areas of musicians playing musical

    instruments. In particular Schlaugh (2001) and Schlaugh

    et al. (2009) found that bimanual motor skill acquisitionthat is necessary to play a musical instrument is associated

    to an increased size of the anterior section of corpus

    callosum.

    5 Music and language

    Music and language are human activities that are common

    to all human cultures and are both based on sounds that arehierarchically organised within a syntactic framework.

    Their mutual relationship is demonstrated by a number of

    facts. First of all, the ability of the brain to recognisetransposed pitch intervals, which makes it possible to

    organise sounds on the basis of the same harmonic and

    melodic patterns regardless of their height, is also a basicprerequisite for linguistic communication. Every vowel is

    characterised by the first two formant frequencies deter-

    mined by vocal tract resonance, which have a definiteinterval ratio between them and in relation to the funda-

    mental frequency (Ross et al. 2007). The broad interindi-

    vidual variability of the fundamental frequency (betweenmales and females) does not impair our ability to recognise

    a vowel uttered by different speakers because of our ability

    to recognise transposed intervals (Patel 2008).Furthermore, a number of studies have demonstrated

    that a musical education facilitates the ability to reproduce

    the non-familiar sounds of a foreign language and, moregenerally, the acquisition of a second language.

    Fig. 6 Activations in left medial frontal cortex (Broadman area 9) fornon-musicians (a) and musicians (b) during the discrimination ofmusical tempi. The regional blood flow, as an index of neural activitywas measured by PET and shown as group mean PET images for eachtask (contrasted with rest) overlaid on MRIs (from Parsons 2001)

    300 Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304

    123

    Chris Gao

    Chris Gao

  • One decisive demonstration of the overlapping brain

    mechanisms that create and maintain learned sound cate-gories in both the linguistic and musical domains came

    from electrophysiological studies demonstrating that the

    ERPs evoked by linguistic and musical incongruities wererecorded from the same cortical region: i.e. Brocas area

    (Fig. 3) (Maess et al. 2001; Koelsch et al. 2002; Koelsch

    2006; Patel 2003). It is interesting to note that the relevantstimulus in both cases was a syntactic musical or linguistic

    irregularity, thus leading to the conclusion that Brocasarea is endowed with functional properties related to

    organising perceptually discrete elements (words or musi-

    cal tones) into hierarchically structured sequences on thebasis of syntactic principles. These findings that support the

    common nature of the mechanisms by which the brain

    processes language and music need to be evaluated withreference to the existing differences between the musical

    and linguistic domains, which formed the basis of

    Aniruddh Patels book Music, Language and the Brain(2008). He exhaustively reviewed evidence taken from the

    cognitive and neurosciences and discussed it with reference

    to the shared syntactic integration resource hypothesis,which postulates that the two domains have distinct and

    domain-specific syntactic representations, but share neural

    resources for activating and integrating them during syn-tactic processing (Patel 2003) and can account for obser-

    vations of a double dissociation between aphasia and

    amusia (Luria and Tsvetkova 1965; Peretz et al. 1994]. Theimpressive bulk of the information supports the authors

    statement that music and language are complex constel-

    lations of subprocesses, some of which are shared andothers not..Exploring this network of relationscanimprove our understanding of how the mind assembles

    complete communicative abilities from elementary cogni-tive processes.

    6 Neuroscience and music as an aesthetic experience

    At the beginning of this chapter, I quoted EdwardHanslicks statement concerning musical beauty, which

    indicates the essence of the aesthetic musical experience as

    being captured by the dynamics of the sounds that relatetheir tempo to our own experience of time. As a lover of

    music, I completely agree with Hanslicks view but, as a

    neurophysiologist, I find it desperately difficult to sub-stantiate this feeling using neurophysiological tools.

    Nevertheless, I will try to discuss some data that may be

    relevant to our perception of music from an aesthetic per-spective, although I am aware that they will be only indi-

    rectly related to the core of the music. In doing this, the

    main risk is to give Western tonal music (the subject ofmost studies of the aesthetic aspects of music) a universal

    value. This is obviously not true because the tonal system

    is by no means the universal paradigm of music even inWestern cultures, and has attained its dominant position

    only over the last three centuries or so. What we can learn

    from these studies may therefore not apply to other musicalsystems, including those used in Western music before the

    introduction of the so-called baroque style or in the

    twentieth century.One aspect that qualifies a human activity as artistic is

    creativity. The difference between a common figurativeworker and an artist was happily expressed by Pablo

    Picasso, who once said that there are painters who trans-

    form the sun to a yellow spot, but there are others who,with the help of their art and their intelligence, transform a

    yellow spot into the sun. Artistic creativity is what sets the

    boundary between an ordinary and an artistic musicalproduct, and can be defined as an ability to establish novel

    melodic, harmonic, rhythmic and timbre relationships

    between the notes.There is no doubt that humans can perceive the signa-

    ture of a creative act in a musical composition but the

    question is: can such a perception be objectively describedusing a neurobiological approach? Most experiments

    involving the brain and music have been carried out using

    rather trivial musical material that is devoid of any creativevalue, and therefore does not seem to be adequate to pro-

    vide an answer to the question. However, the responses to

    melodic and harmonic irregularities (MMN and ERAN)may provide an interesting means of investigation. In the

    literature, the alterations that evoke these responses are

    negatively seen as violations, but Stefan Koelsch (seeKoelsch 2009) has shown that ERAN can be elicited by

    Neapolitan chords even in a regular dominant to tonic

    harmonic progression. The Neapolitan chord characterisingthe Neapolitan school has been widely used by non-

    Neapolitan composers, and is not perceived as a viola-

    tion of musical rules, although it may be unexpected to alistener of a dominant to tonic progression.

    It can therefore be realistically hypothesised that the

    perception of a musical passage that sounds unexpected tothe listener because of its originality could be revealed by

    recording brain responses, which would provide research-

    ers with an objective means of studying the effect ofmusical creativity.

    6.1 Music and emotions

    The idea that music expresses emotions is a commonplace

    that is often assumed to explain the essence of the art ofmusic. However, it is important to say that intensity of

    emotional feeling is not always a measure of the artistic

    quality of the music that elicits it. Leaving aside theemotional responses aroused by a piece of music simply

    Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304 301

    123

    Chris Gao

    Chris Gao

    Chris Gao

  • because of its associations with a certain memory (as is

    typical in the case of tunes related to a privileged love

    experience), there are other instances in which music-associated emotion is not related to the musics artistic

    quality, such as the sound of an organ in a church, military

    marches, anthems, etc., to which the emotional response oflisteners may be very intense even if the artistic quality of

    the music is desperately poor. I will consider here only the

    emotions that seem to be primarily related to the intrinsicfeatures of music.

    Rigorous studies of the effects of musical tunes chosen

    to induce pleasant or unpleasant subjective responses havebeen carried out by Blood et al. (1999), Koelsch (2005),

    Koelsch et al. (2006) using PET, fMRI and EEG record-

    ings). Pleasant emotions were induced by joyful andmainly consonant musical tunes, whereas musical pieces

    with various degrees of dissonance were used to induce

    unpleasant emotions. The main changes induced byunpleasant music were observed within an extensive neu-

    ronal network of limbic and paralimbic structures consist-ing of the amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampal

    gyrus, and temporal poles. During the presentation of

    pleasant music, activations were observed in the ventral

    striatum and the anterior superior insula. Additional acti-vations in response to consonant (in comparison with dis-

    sonant) music were observed in the larynx representation

    within the Rolandic operculum (Broadmanns area 43) andthe inferior frontal gyrus (Broadmanns areas 44i, 45/46).

    Using a slightly different approach, Blood and Zatorre

    (2001) tested the effects of musical stimuli capable ofeliciting intensely pleasurable responses in a group of

    subjects who described them as shivers-down-the-spine or

    chills.Such chills were found to be highly reproducible in the

    same individual by a specific type of music, thus providing

    the investigators with the possibility of studying emotion-related changes in regional cerebral blood flow by means of

    a PET scan and MRI. The results demonstrated the acti-

    vation of a circuit that included the ventral striatum, thedorsomedial midbrain, the amygdala and hippocampus that

    correlated with the intensity of the chills (Fig. 7). Thesestructures form part of a reward/motivation circuit that is

    Fig. 7 Brain activations associated with music-related chills. Regres-sion analyses were used to correlate regional blood flow (measured byPET) with ratings of chills intensity (010). Neuroanatomical regionswith positive (left column) and negative (right column) correlations,are shown as t-statistic images superimposed on correspondingaverage MRI scans (x, y coordinates refer to location in stereotaxicspace. a (sagittal section, x = 4 mm) shows positive rCBF correla-tions in left dorsomedial midbrain (Mb), right thalamus (Th), AC,

    SMA, and bilateral cerebellum (Cb). b (coronal section, y = 13 mm)shows left ventral striatum (VStr) and bilateral insula (In; also AC).c (coronal section, y = 32 mm) shows right orbitofrontal cortex (Of).d (sagittal section, x = 4 mm) shows negative rCBF correlations inVMPF and visual cortex (VC). e (sagittal section, x = 21 mm) showsright amygdala (Am). f (sagittal section, x = -19 mm) shows lefthippocampus/amygdala (H/Am) (from Blood and Zatorre 2001)

    302 Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304

    123

    Chris Gao

    Chris Gao

  • known to be activated during pleasurable emotions, such as

    those related to nutrition, sexual activities or drugs ofabuse.

    The idea that music-related pleasurable emotion is asso-

    ciated with the activation of the same structures as thoseinvolved in the emotional colour of such biologically

    important activities as nutrition and reproduction is certainly

    interesting. These findings may open up the way towards afurther understanding of the brain processing associated with

    the arousal of aesthetic emotion that invades us when lis-tening to or playing a piece of music we love.

    Acknowledgments The generous support of the Mariani Founda-tion to the organization of the series of meetings The Neurosciencesand Music is gratefully acknowledged.

    References

    Alajouanine T (1948) Aphasia and artistic realization. Brain71:229241

    Avanzini G, Faienza C, Minciacchi D, Lopez L, Majno M (eds)(2003) The neurosciences and music. Ann N Y Acad Sci, vol999, p 548

    Avanzini G, Lopez L, Koelsch S, Maino M (eds) (2005) Theneurosciences and music IIfrom perception to performance.Ann N Y Acad Sci, vol 1060, p 490

    Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ (2001) Intensely pleasurable responses to musiccorrelate with activity in brain regions implicated with rewardand emotion. PNAS USA 98:1181811823

    Blood AJ, Zatorre RJ, Bermudez P, Evans AC (1999) Emotionalresponses to pleasant and unpleasant music correlate withactivity in paralimbic brain regions. Nat Neurosci 2:382387

    Bouillaud JB (1865) Sur la faculte du langage articule. Arch Gen MedI:575

    Broca P (1861) Remarque sur la sie`ge de la faculte du languagearticule, suives dune observation daphemie (perte de la parole).Bull Soc Anat Paris 36:330337

    Critchley M, Henson RA (eds) (1977) Music and the brain.Heinemann, London

    Dalla Bella S, Kraus N, Overy K, Pantev C, Snyder JS, TervaniemiM, Tillmann B, Schlaug G (eds) (2009) The Neurosciences andMusic IIIdisorders and plasticity. Ann N Y Acad Sci, vol1169, p 569

    DeWitt I, Rauschecker JP (2012) Phoneme and word recognition inthe auditory ventral stream. PNAS USA 109(8):505514

    Friederici AD (2002) Towards a neural basis of auditory sentenceprocessing. Trends Cognitive Sci 6:7884

    Hanslick E (1854) Vom Musikalisch-Schonen LeipzigKnoblauch A (1888) Ueber Storungen der musikalischer

    Leistungsfahigkeit infolge von Gehirnlasionen. Dtsch Arch KlinMed 43:331

    Koelsch S (2005) Investigating emotion with music: neuroscientificapproaches. In: Avanzini G, Lopez L, Koelsch S, Maino M (eds)The neurosciences and music IIfrom perception to perfor-mance. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol 1060,pp 412418

    Koelsch S (2006) Significance of Brocas area and ventral premotorcortex for music-syntactic processing. Cortex 42:518520

    Koelsch S (2009) Music-syntactic processing and auditory memory:similarities and differences between ERAN and MMN. Psycho-physiology 46:179190

    Koelsch S, Gunter TC, Friederici AD, Schroger E (2000) Brainindices of music processing: Non-musicians are musical.J Cognitive Neurosci 12:520541

    Koelsch S, Gunter TC, vonCramon DY, Zysset S, Lohmann G,Friederici AD (2002) Bach speaks: a cortical language-networkserves the processing of music. Neuroimage 17:956966

    Koelsch S, Fritz T, vonCramon DY, Muller K, Friederici AD (2006)Investigating emotion with music: an fMRI study. Hum BrainMapp 27:239250

    Liegeois-Chauvel C, Peretz I, Baba M, Laguitton V, Chauvel P(1998) Contribution of different cortical areas in temporal lobesto music processing. Brain 121:18531867

    Luria A, Tsvetkova L (1965) Futer J Aphasia in a composer. J NeurolSci 2:288292

    Maess B, Koelsch S, Gunter TC, Friederici AD (2001) Musicalsyntax is processed in the area of Broca: an MEG study. NatNeurosci 4:540545

    Minati L, Rosazza C, DIncerti L, Pietrocini E, Valentini L, Scaioli V,Loveday C, Bruzzone MG (2008) FMRI/ERP of musical syntax:comparison of melodies and unstructured note sequences.NeuroReport 19:13811385

    Minati L, Salvatoni L, Rosazza C, Pietrocini E, Visani E, Panzica F,Scaioli V, Avanzini G, Franceschetti S (2010) Event-relatedpotential (ERP) markers of melodic processing: the N2 compo-nent is modulated by structural complexity, not by melodicmeaningfulness. Brain Res Bull 30:2328

    Naatanen R, Gaillard AWK, Mantysalo S (1978) Early selective-attention effect on evoked potential reinterpreted. Acta Psychol42:313329

    PantevC, EngelienA,CandiaV,Elbert T (2001)Representational cortexin musicians. Plastic alterations in response to musical practice.In: Zatorre RJ, Peretz I (eds) The neuroscience andmusic.Annals ofthe New York Academy of Sciences, vol 930, pp 300314

    Parsons LW (2001) Exploring the functional neuroanatomy of musicperformance, perception and comprehension. Ann N Y Acad Sci930:211230

    Pasqual-Leone A (2001) The brain that play music and is changed by it.In: Zatorre RJ, Peretz I (eds) The neuroscience andmusic.Annals ofthe New York Academy of Sciences, vol 930, pp 315329

    Patel AD (2003) Language, music, syntax and the brain. Nat Neurosci6:674681

    Patel AD (2008) Music, language and brain. Oxford University Press,New York

    Patterson RD, Uppenkamp S, Johnsrude IS, Griffiths TD (2002) Theprocessing of temporal pitch and melody information in auditorycortex. Neuron 36:767776

    Peretz I, Coltheart M (2003) Modularity of music processing. NatNeurosci 6:688691

    Peretz I, Kolinsky R, Tramo M, Labreque R, Hublet C, Demeurisse G,Belleville S (1994) Functional dissociations following bilaterallesions of auditory cortex. Brain 117:12831302

    Peretz I, Champod AS, Hyde K (2003) Varieties of musical disorders.The Montreal Battery of Evaluation of Amusia. In: Avanzini G,Faienza C, Minciacchi, Lopez L, Majno M (eds) The neurosci-ences and music. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences,vol 999, pp 5875

    Rauschecker JP (1998) Cortical processing of complex sounds. CurrOpin Neurobiol 8:516521

    Rauschecker JP, Scott SK (2009) Maps and streams in the auditorycortex: non human primates illuminate human speech processingNat. Neurosciences 12:718724

    Ross D, Choi J, Purves D (2007) Musical intervals in speech. PNAS104(23):98529857

    Saarinen J, Paavilainen P, Schoger E, Tervaniemi M, Naatanen R(1992) Representation of abstract attributes of auditory stimuli inthe human brain. NeuroReport 3(12):11491151

    Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304 303

    123

  • Schlaugh G (2001) The brain of musicians. A model for functionaland structural adaptation. In: Zatorre RJ, Peretz I (eds) Annals ofthe New York Academy of Sciences, vol 930, pp 281299

    Schlaugh G, Forgeard M, Zhu Lin, Norton A, Norton A, Winner E(2009) Training-induced neuroplasticity in young children. In:Dalla Bella S, Kraus N, Overy K, Pantev C, Snyder JS,Tervaniemi M, Tillmann B, Schlaug G (eds) The neurosciencesand music III: disorders and plasticity. Annals of the New YorkAcademy of Sciences, vol 1169, pp 205208

    Tervaniemi M (2001) Musical sound processing in the human brain.Evidence from electric and magnetic recordings. In: Zatorre RJ,Peretz I (eds) The biological foundations of music. Annals of theNew York Academy of Sciences, vol 930, pp 259272

    Zatorre RJ, Peretz I (eds) (2001) The biological foundations of music.Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, vol 930, p 462

    304 Rend. Fis. Acc. Lincei (2012) 23:295304

    123

    Neuroscience and musicAbstractIntroductionMusic and the brainMethodsNeuropsychologyNeurophysiologyNeuroimaging

    Anatomo-functional organisation of the musical competences of the brainMusic and languageNeuroscience and music as an aesthetic experienceMusic and emotions

    AcknowledgmentsReferences