146
^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Page 2: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

AN EVALUATION OF THE CLASSIFICATION OF HYPERKINETIC

CHILDREN WITH THE VIGILANCE TASX AND

THE MATCHING EAMILIAR FIGURES TEST

JOHN C. SIMONEAUX, B.A., M.A.

A DISSE'RTATION

Ilv'

PSYCHOLOGY

Submitted to the Graduate Eaculty of Texas Tecb University in Partial Fulfillment of tbe Requirements for

tbe Degree of

DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

Approved

/ Accepted

August, 1982

Page 3: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I am deeply indebted to Professor Robert P. Anderson for his

direction of this dissertation and his guidance throughout my stay

at Texas Tech, I am also grateful to the other members of my com-

mittee, Professors Barnett, Bell, and Perez, and Assistant Professor

lacono for their helpful criticism, Finally, I thank Gayle for al-

ways being there.

n

Page 4: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

CONTENTS

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ii

ABSTRACT vi

LIST OF TABLES viii

I. INTRODUCTION 1

Hyperactivity , 5

Background and Current Status , , 5

Prevalence , . . . , . , , . . , ~l

Symptoms 9

EtioTogy , , , , . , . , 10

Prognosis . . , , , . , , , , 13

Treatment l^

Current Diagnostic Practices , , , , ^

Interviews . , , , . , . . , l^

Individual Tests 19

Simple Performance Tests , 19

Higher Order Cognitive Tests , , . , . , 21

Laboratory Measures ^

The Vigilance Task . . . . . . , , , , . , , , 29

Rating Scales ... , . , , , . . . 31

Research Hypotheses . , , , , , . 33

II, METHODS AND PROCEDURES , , 35

Subjects . . , , , , 35

Apparatus . , , . , . . . . . , . , . , 'l

iii

Page 5: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Instrumentation 44

Matching Familiar Figures Test ... 44

The Rating Scale for Hyperkinesis 45

Behavior Checklist 45

Slosson Intelligence Test 46

Administrative Procedures . , . . , 46

Statistical Design 47

III, RESULTS , , . . , ^ 51 Hypothesis 1 ,

Hypothesis 2 ^

Hypothesis 3 , . , fi?

Hypotheses 4 and-5 . , , , ,

Hypothesis 6 ^'

Hypothesis 7 ^

Hypothesis 8 , , ^

Hypothesis 9 '^

IV. DISCUSSION , ^ •70

Hypothesis 1 , , . . , . , , . , . . . . , . . . , , ' Hypothesis 2 , ,

Hypothesis 3 , .

Hypotheses 4 and 5

. . » . , • • ' .

Hypothesi

Hypothesi

Hypothesi

Hypothesi

s 6

s 7

s 8

s 9

Implications

81

82

83

84

86

86

89

90

IV

Page 6: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Directions for Future Research 92

Limitations 93

V. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 96

REFERENCES 99

APPENDICES 118

A. Diagnostic Criteria for Attention Deficit Disorder

with Hyperactivity 119

B. . Parent Cover Letter (St. Joseph's) 121

C. Parent Cover Letter (Levelland) 122

D. Informed Consent Form 123

E. Zukow Hyperkinesis Rating Form 125

F. -Behavior Checklist 127

G. Conners' Abbreviated Teacher Rating'Scale 128

H. Test Report Form 129

I. Texas Education Agency Guidelines for Identifying

Learning Disabled Children 130

J. Vigilance Task Booth 132

K. Audiotape Recorded Instructions 133

L. Dissertation Record Sheet 134

Page 7: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

ABSTRACT

The accurate diagnosis and placement of hyperactive children has

been recognized as critical in the planning and implementation of

psycho-educational interventions aimed at ameliorating the difficul-

ties these children inevitably encounter. The importance of correc-

tly identifying hyperactive children is underscored when the typical

interventions for treating the disorder are considered. The side-

effects of medication are sometimes worse than the disorder, Two

objective instruments (the Vigilance Task and the Matching Familiar

Figures Test) surfaced as potentially useful in diagnosing this dis-

order,

One-hundred children (27 hyperactives, 27 learning disabled, 46

"normals") were administered the Vigilance Task and the MFFT. Both

stepwise and direct discriminant analyses failed to demonstrate that

these tests had adequate discriminating abilities. Only 56% of the

children in the study were correctly classified when the yielded

discriminant function was applied. The most promising single mea-

sure was the correct detections score on the Vigilance Task which

presumably taps the attentional deficit component of hyperactivity,

The tests were found not to discriminate significantly better

when the subjects were made more homogenous (e,g,, sex, age), The

trend was for the scores of the larger group of hyperactives and

learning disabled children to be similar to each other and different

from the "normal" group. Scores on the two tests did correlate to

vi

Page 8: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

some degree with each other in the expected directions, indicating

that they probably measure similar, or coexisting phenomena, Ade-

quate split-half reliability data were obtained for both instru-

ments, Teachers and mothers strongly agreed that the children in

the hyperactive group displayed hyperactive behavior patterns. A

slight relationship was found between Slosson IQ scores and correct

detections on the Vigilance Task,

v n

Page 9: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

TABLES

1. Numbér of Subjects and Mean Age of Young/Old and Male/Female

Groups 36

2. Number of Subjects X Sex X Classification XXAge Group 39

3. Number of Subjects X Grade X Classification 39

4. Number of Subjects Tested by Time of Day 42

5. Classification by Day of the Week Tested 42

6. Kagan's Descriptors for MFFT Responders 44

7. Eigenvalues and Measures of Importance 52

8. Residual Discrimination and Test of Significance 53

9. Summary table for Discriminant Analysis Using Rao's V

Stepwise Procedure 55

10. Canonîcal Discriminant Function for Discriminant Analysis

Using Rao's V Stepwise Procedure 56

11. Standardized Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients

(Rao's V) 57

12. Classification Results Using Rao's V Stepwise Method 58

13. Classification Results Using Full Discriminant Analysis

Methods 59

14. Standardized Canonical Discriminant Function Coefficients ... 60

15. ResuUs of Classifications for Various Subsamples 62

16. Means and Standard Deviations for Scores on the Vigilance Task

and MFFT for Hyperactive, Learning Disabled, and Normal Groups . 63

17. One-Way ANOVA Results from Three Groups on Correct Detections . 64

18. One-Way ANOVA Results from Three Groups on False Alarms . . . . 6;

vm

Page 10: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

19. One-Way ANOVA Results from Three Groups on Latency 65

20. One-Way ANOVA Results from Three Groups on Errors 65

21. t^-Values and Probabilities for Scores on the Vigilance Task and

MFFT Scores 66

22. Expected Intercorrelation Directions of Vigilance Task and

MFFT Scores 67

23. Intercorrelations and Probabilities Between Scores on the

Vigilance Task and the MFFT 68

24. Intercorrelation Matrix for Teacher and Parent Ratings 70

25. Expected Correlation Directions for Test Measures and Teacher

and Parent Ratings 71

26. Obtained Correlation Coefficients for Test Measure and Teacher

and Parent Ratings 72

27. Descriptive Data Concerning Subject's IQ 73

28. t^-Tests Comparing IQ and Groups 75

29. Correlations Between IQ and Scores on the Vigilance Task and

the MFFT 75

IX

Page 11: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCT ON

Hyperactive children are said to constitute the largest category

of child psychological referrals to mental health and pediatric facili-

ties (Ross & Ross, 1976), Their behavior is most frequently charac-

terized by motoric restlessness, poor attention span, impulsivity, and

a generally excessive activity level, One indication of the preva-

lence of hyperactivity is the observation that approximately 40% of

school age children referred to mental health clinics exhibited hy-

perkinetic behavior patterns (Kahn & Gardner, 1975). Hyperactivity

is certainly a problem of some magnitude,

The accurate diagnosis and placement of hyperactive children

has been recognized as critical in the planning and implementation of

psycho-educational interventions aimed at ameliorating the difficul-

ties these children inevitably encounter. A need for standardized

procedures in the assessment of hyperactivity has been demonstrated

by Berler and Romanczyk (1980), In a survey of seven major journals

which publish in areas related to hyperactivity it was found that

while learning disabled children tended to be selected on the basis

1

Page 12: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

of standardized, objectlve tests, hyperactiye children were more fre-

quently identified through the'use of subjective indices. Qnly a

single study evaluated in the Berler and Romanczyk (.1980) eirticle used

a standardized instrument (the Kagan Matching Familicir Figures Test)

to assess hyperactivity. Forty-four percent of the investigations

employed a single teacher or parent rating scale to identify those

children with the disorder,

Teacher rating scales, used alone for diagnostic purposes, have

been shown to have questicnable validity. Kenny, Clemmens, Hudson,

Lentz, Cicci, and Nair (.1971) found that well over half of the children

referred by a school system to their diagnostic clinic were judged not

to be hyperactive by the evaluating staff. Anderson and his colleagues

(Langsdorf, Anderson, Wiechter, Madrigal, & Juarez, 1979: Waechter,

Anderson, Juarez, Langsdorf, & Madrigal, 1979) found that ethnicity

and social class influenced teacher ratings. Lower frequencies of

hyperactivity were noted in schools with white majorities while a

higher incidence was found in schools with black or Mexican-American

majorities, Langsdorf et al. (1979) explain these differences from

a sociological perspective. Findings such as these highlight the need

to differentiate between objective assessments of hyperactivity and

hyperactivity as "perceived" by a single teacher.

The importance of correctly identifying hyperactive children is

underscored when the typical interventions for treating the disorder

are considered, The medical treatment of choice for the management

of hyperactivity involves the use of cerebral stimulants or tran-

quilizers. While these procedures are frequently beneficial there

Page 13: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

are often deleterious slde^ffect to the drugs, The development of

accurate diq^gnostic tools will result in a reduction in the numher of

children being placed on such medications unnecessarily.

The phenomenon of misattribution is another unfortunate concomitant

of faulty diagnoses, Hyperactivity is most frequently associated with

physiological axplanations of etiology, When this factor becomes most

salient to those working with the particular child, other relevant

contributions to the child's heightened activity level (e.g,, modeling,

reinforced behavior, excessively stimulating environments, emotional

disturbances) may be overlooked, When this occurs professionals will

find themselves treating the wrong problem. Williamson, Anderson, and

Lundy (1980) support this notion by proposing an ecological model

of hyperkinesis in which the antecedents of hyperactive behavior are

multiple, being provided by the child's familial, academic, and phy-

siological environments. As a result of this proposed înodel the authors

suggest that treatment should be multimodal, involving the child,

his/her family, and the school

Several writers (Berler & Romanczyk, 1980; Safer & Allen, 1976;

Walden & Thompson, 1981) have explicitly called for caution in the

diagnosis of hyperactivity. Further, these investigators have encou-

raged the development of standardized. objective procedures and bat-

teries for diagnosing this disorder. This cause has been aided of late

by recent changes in psychiatric diagnostic practices. Due to current

trends in research findings, a shift in nomenclature has recently

taken effect. With the publication of the •Diagnostic and Statistical

Page 14: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Manual of Mental -Disorders, Third Edition (DSM III; American Psychi-

iatric Association, 1980), what was once commonly identified as the

hyperactive syndrome is now referred to as Attention Deficit Disorder

with Hyperactivity (ADD), This change is based primarily on programs

of research by Douglas (1972), Dykman, Ackerman, Clements, and Peters

(1971), and others.

The most frequently cited primary symptoms of ADD are hyperac-

tivity (overactivity, restlessness), inattention (distractibility,

forgetfulness), impulsivity (recklessness, inability to delay grati-

fication), and excitability (irritability, low frustration tolerance),

The complete DSM III diagnostic criteria can be found in Appendix A,

Several objective instruments exist which have been designed

to measure certain of the characteristics of hyperactive children.

The Vigilance Task, for example, has been utilized to obtain objective

indices of inattention and impulsivity (Anderson, Halcomb, &Doyle,

1973). Kagan's Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT) has been widely

cited (e.g., Campbell, 1973; Campbell, Douglas, & Morgenstern, 1971;

Hopkins, Perlman, Hechtman, & Weiss, 1979; Juliano, 1974) as a useful

instrument for identifying the impulsivity component of ADD,

In light of the need for objective measures of attention defi-

cit disorder, and the recognition that multiple indices are desirable

when measuring characteristics such as impulsivity and attention

(Paulsen & Johnson, 1980), the present study aims to assess the pos-

sibility of meeting this need through the use of the two instruments

mentioned above. This investigation will seek to determine if a

Page 15: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

5 5

discriminant analysis procedure, involving the Vigilance Task and the

MFFT as predictor variables can result in an accurate and objective

classification procedure for hyperactive children,

Hyperactivity

Background and Current Status

Descriptions of children who would now be diagnosed as hyperac-

tive date back 80 years or more (Ebaugh, 1923; Still, 1902). Through-

out the 1930s and 1940s various investigators proposed etiological

theories involving organic brain damage (Bradley, 1937; Kahn & Cohen,

1934; Orton, 1937; Strauss & Lehtinen, 1947), Much of the early re-

search proceeded from the assumption that organic pathology was re-

sponsible for the hyperkinetic child's atypical behavior. Although

it seemed obvious that brain trauma and neurological disease could

produce hyperactive behavior patterns, it became equally clear that

many children who had been diagnosed as hyperactive did not possess

unequivocal organic pathology. The term minimal brain dysfunction

(MBD) came into use to indicate the presence of one or more symptoms

considered to reflect the presence of subtle cerebral abnormalities,

As a result of the apparent dead-ends to which the pursuit of

the MBD concept was leading, an increasing number of investigators

began to deemphasize organic etiological considerations in favor of

other explanations, For some this represented the choice of a be-

havioral model; hyperactivity was viewed as a behavioral-psychological

phenomenon rather than as a syndrome, or as one symptom of a medical

Page 16: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

6 6 5

disorder, For others the inodel remained a medical one, and the use

of the terms hyperkinetic or hyperactive child syndrome signified

prtmarily that no single organic etiology accounted for cin hyperac-

tivlty,

A hy-product of the shift in terminology was an increase in con-

cern with the definition of hyperactivity, It began to be evident

that the essential difficulty of these children was with attention

rather than actiyity. Research by Dyktnan et al. (1971) and Douglas

(.1972) clarified this central role of inattention. The American Psy-

chiatric Association (1930) formally recognized these findings with

the aforementioned shift in nomenclature,

A fundamental difficulty with an activity level focus in hyper^

kinesis research is semantic. Cromwell, Baumeister, and Hawkins (1963)

noted that all responses made by an organism can be defined as activity.

Some investigators'have constructed empirical measures in order to

establish operational definitions of the hyperactive's characteristic

restlessness (Schulman, Stevens, & Kupst, 1977; Stevens, Kupst, Suran,

& Schulman, 1978). This research team's device consisted of a small

electronic package, worn at the waist, which measured activity by the

angular displacement of mercury switches. The device had adjustable

biofeedback potential; It has been found to be a reliable measure of

activity level in the classroom, Another technique is to monitor

movement from place-to-place by using photo-cells to measure the number

of times a child crosses c beam of light. This would result. however,

in a much different index from one obtained by measuring how much a

child moves while sitting in a chair. These and other measures.

Page 17: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

7

not been found to correlate well with one another (Cromwell et al.,

1963). Due to discouraging research findings, as well as the difficul-

ties involved in deciding what forms of behavior constitute "activity",

the trend in more recent investigations has been in the direction of

focusing on cognitive elements of the disorder,

Prevalence

Wender (1973) has estimated that there are over five million

children in the United States who are identified as hyperactive, The

disorder is estimated to occur in as many as 3% to 5% of prepubertal

children (American Psychiatric Association, 1980; Ross & Pelham, 1981).

The symptoms of hyperactivity apparently manifest themselves in some

form yery early in the child's life, Campbell, Schleifer, and Weiss

(1978) found that there were continuities in both maternal reports of

behavior during early childhood and later elementary school behavior,

A large portion of the children referred for mental health services

exhibit hyperkinetic behavior patterns (Kahn & Gardner, 1975; Patterson,

Jones, Whittier, & Wright, 1965), Hyperactive children often perform

poorly in school, are aggressive, and have poor peer relationships

(Rosenthal & Allen, 1978). Their symptoms create problems which may

lead to poor adjustment later in life. In addition, hyperactive

children's behavior is often disruptive to their parents, families,

teachers, and classmates,

Estimates of the prevalence of hyperactivity are highly variable,

ranging from two to three percent of all children (Sprague, 1977) to as

many as 50% of all boys (Werry & Quay, 1971). Because many definitions

Page 18: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

8 M 5

of hyperactivity have been offered, ranging from highly clinical and

subjective to entirely objective and operational, much of the varia-

bility in prevalence figures can be attributed to differences in

terminology and perspective,

Physicians frequently serve as the identifying agents for hyper-

active children. Most typically the variables considered relevant for

diagnosis include (a) symptom behaviors that are common to hyperkinesis, , , ,

(b) the sources of information available, and (c) responses to pro-

grams of medication, The usual scenario involves the parent reporting

the his/her child has been engaging in hyperkinetic behavior. The

physician examines the child, noting that indeed he/she is quite rest-

less. Hyperactivity is then diagnosed and the "appropriate" medication

is prescribed. Occasionally physicians will refer, when the resource

is available, to a psychologist or to the child's school for assistance

in confirming the diagnosis.

Parents and teachers are often the first link in the diagnostic

chain. When they are asked to describe children by checking lists of

descriptors, as many as half of all elementary school boys may be

identified as restless and inattentive (Lapouse & Monk, 1958; Werry

& Quay, 1971), Teachers rate children differently with regard to

ethnicity (Langsdorf et al,, 1979; Waechter et al., 1979).

When scores on rating scales are used to estimate prevalence, the

number of children considered hyperactive will be a direct function of

the cut-off point chosen. In a normal distribution of scores a cut-

off point two standard deviátions above the mean will identify two to

three percent of the sample as hyperactive (Sprague, 1977). As a

Page 19: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

result, there may exist significant differences with regard to the

number of children recognized as having problems severe enough to re-

quire treatment. The numbers involved in the latter group of children

could well be deflated due to a lack of economic resources on the

parents' part to seek and receive treatment for their child.

Many writers and clinicians are disturbed by the fact that

definitions of hyperkinesis vary more across situations and across time

than does the hyperkinetic child. Experts seem to agree on terminology

(Schrager et al,, 1966) more than they do in practice (Kenny et al.,

1971), Clearly the presence of childhood hyperactivity is accepted

in the literature concerning behavioral disorders in children. An

orderly ajid unified approach to its assessment and diagnosis must pre-

cede a more settled state of knowledge about prevalence, etiology,

treatment, and course. The accurate identification of hyperactivity

is critical for the children who are involved and essential if mean-

ingful research is to be undertaken concerning its treatment.

Symptoms

The term hyperactivity, although widely used and accepted, has

been found to not accurately describe the problems of these children,

Cognitive aspects of the disorder are beginning to be emphasized

rather than the child's activity Tevel, While the disorder is offi-

cially (DSM III) referred to as Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyper-

activity, the terms hyperactivity and hyperkinesis will be used in

the present study in order to facilitate expression, The cognitive

components of the disorder will be assumed when using these terms.

Page 20: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

10

The primary symptoms of hyperkinesis have been noted above and

the diagnostic criteria can be found in Appendix A. One or more sec-

ondary symptoms have been identified as being associated with hyper-

activity. Lists enumerating these secondary symptoms typically include

low self-esteem, academic skill deficits, and delinquent behavior,

Several authors (Morrison & Stewart, 1971; Safer & Allen, 1976; Wender,

1971), however, consider learning difficulties and aggressive behaviors

to be primary symptoms. Cantwell and Satterfield (1978) lend support

to the presence of learning difficulties in hyperkinetics. They re-

ported that a greater percentage of hyperactive children underachieved

in reading, spelling, and arithmetic when compared to non-hyperactive

children,' The hyperactives tended to be behind in more different sub-

jects and by more grade levels than were normals.

Etiology

Several factors have been identified as being responsible for

the presence of hyperactivity, The traditional explanation of some

sort of brain pathology or trauma resulted from observations of World

War I veterans with confirmed brain injury, Difficulties in attention

and emotional control were often noted. Due to similar symptoms in

those children exhibiting hyperactive behavior it was assumed that

this disorder also resulted from brain injury (Hallahan & Kaufman,

1976). Johnson (1981) reviewed this research and found little evi-

dence supporting the notion that organic factors play a significant

part in hyperactivity, Hyperactivity is certainly one possible symp-

tom of brain damage, Not all hyperactives, however, are brain damaged,

Page 21: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

11

nor are all brain damaged individuals hyperactive,

Psychogenic factors have been proposed as having etiological

significance in hyperactivity. The implication is that child rearing

practices, direct reinforcement, and/or modeling are responsible for

the presence of hyperkinesis. It has been demonstrated that many

hyperactives display their problematic behavior at an early age

(Campbell et al., 1978; Stewart & Olds, 1973), Several investigators

(Henderson, Dahlin, Partridge, & Engelsing, 1973; Zinna, 1979) have

proposed a temperament pattern model resulting from the early mother-

child interactions, It is evident from this work, however, that tem-

perament alone does not produce hyperactivity.

Social reinforcement may serve to perpetuate hyperactive behavior,

It is not uncommon for high activity Tevels to be reinforced (through

attention for example) on a regular schedule through adolescence,

Several clinicians (Morrison & Stewart, 1971; Willerman & Plomin, 1973)

have noted that the parents of hyperactive children are often hyperac-

tive themselves. The implication is that heightened activity level

is learned through modeling. Kaspar and Lowenstein (1971) showed

that less active children, after a 20-minute free play period, increased

their activity level to approach that of their more active playmates.

A highly active parent may have a profound effect. While this argu-

ment is intuitively appealing there has been little empirical re-t

search published in i t s support. ^

Other environmental factors discussed in re la t ion to the i r pos-

s ib le e t io log ica l contr ibut ion to hyperkinetic behavior include lead

Page 22: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

12

poisoning and radiation stress (Johnson, 1981). While lead poisoning

does seem to be linked to disturbances in brain metabolism (David,

Clark, & Voeller. 1972; David, Hoffman, Sverd, Clark, & Voeller, 1976;

Silbergeld & Goldberg, 1973, 1974; Wiener, 1970), no causal relation-

ship has been demonstrated between hyperactivity and increased body

lead levels, Ott (1974) postulated that exposure to flourescent

lighting and unshielded television tubes can result in radiation ex-

posure of sufficient intensity to cause hyperactivity, Hartley (1974)

conducted a study with rats which confirms this idea. Mayron, Ott,

Nations, and Mayron (1974) compared conventional and shielded flou-

rescent lighting in a 90-day experiment with school children. The

hyperacttve children's attention span and behavior improved only in

the shielded flourescently lighted class. Painter (1976) found a

32% drop in activity with the removal of flourescent lights. Research

in this area shows promise and deserves greater attention.

Ross and Pelham (1981) summarize etiological research by noting

that it is replete with ambiguity. It is pointed out that the various

hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, and that the proposed mechanisms

probably interact. Weiss and Hechtman (.1979) agree, concluding that

the syndrome can best be understood in terms of interactions between

social, psychological , and biological variables. It may be that

different "types" of hyperactivity exist, hence, different etiologies.

Ney (1974) has observed at least four subtypes of hyperkinesis, At

best, the present state of our knowledge concerning the etiology of

hyperactivity can be characterizec as uncertain.

Page 23: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

13

Prognosis

A note about the prognosis of hyperactivity is in order because

it also underscores the need for accurate diagnoses, Traditionally it

has been believed that hyperactive children cease to experience prob-

lems once puberty ensues, Several researchers have cited evidence to

the contrary, Heussy and Cohen (1976) reported that 50% to 70% of

hyperactives have failed at least one school grade and half of them

failed two grades by the time adolescence is reached, Twenty-five

to 60% drop out of school before graduating, In a fonow-up on 15

adolescents who had been diagnosed as hyperactive five years before,

Hoy, Weiss, Minde, and Cohen (1978) found that they performed worse

than controls on sustained attention visual-motor and motor tasks,

and on two of four reading tests. The hyperactives assigned them-

selves lower ratings on self-esteem and sociability items. It appears

from this data that hyperactives at adolescence continue to suffer

from attentional and stimulus processing difficulties that affect

both their academic and social functioning,

Mendelson, Johnson, and Stewart (1971) noted that two-thirds of

adolescent hyperactives have serious-discipline problems at home and

at school, with higher rates of suspensions and expulsions than non-

hyperactives. From 25% to 60% have had contact with legal authori-

ties (Weiss, Minde, Werry, Douglas, & Nemeth, 1971) and more hyperac-

tives are prone to alcohol abuse (Blouin, Bornstein, & Trites, 1978),

Morrison (1980) reported that, as adults, hyperactives tended to have

less education, lower work status, and a higher rate of violence and

Page 24: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

14 \

legal problems than psychiatric controls, Morrison (1980) sees these

difficulties as resuUing from failures of parental control rather

than being an effect of hyperactivity.

It is clear from this evidence that the problems of hyperactives

do not cease with the onset of puberty. Less is known about the fate

of hyperactive children once they become aduUs. In any case, it is

evident that hyperkinetic children are at risk for a variety of dif-

ficuUies in their later years. The development of early, accurate

diagnostic techniques and effective treatment methods for this dis-

order is crucial,

Treatment

There is little doubt that the existence of the CNS stimulant

drugs, primarily Ritalin and Dexedrine, contributed to the increase

in hyperkinetic diagnoses. A prescription often serves to relieve

the parents' worries about their possible responsibility for the devel-

opment and maintenance of their child's difficuUies, A medical syn-

drome provides an explanation that is external to both parents and

child (Whalen & Henker, 1976). Thus, more and more children were

diagnosed and treated, In addition, changing societal factors may

have contributed to genuine increases in hyperactive behavior (Block,

1977) and thus to increases in medical diagnoses and treatment.

The medical treatment of choice for the management of hyper-

activity involves the use of cerebral stimulants or tranquilizers.

Concern has been expressed that these children are medicated for the

teacher's convenience in order to resolve classroom difficuUies

Page 25: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

15

chemically (Safer & Allen, 1976), Walden and Thompson (1981) note

that many problematic behaviors that are defined by teachers, such

as restlessness, attention seeking, disruptiveness, and distractibi-

lity are so prevalent in elementary school children that inappropriate

labeling becomes common. Schuckit, Petrich, and Chiles (1978) confirm

this in their findings which indicate that an inaccurate use of hyper-

active labels was frequently found in difficult to handle children.

Frequently other diagnoses, rather than hyperactivity, would be

more appropriate for particular children. Staton and Brumback (1981)

complain that children are frequently medicated for "hyperactivity"

when, in fact, the hyperactivity is only a secondary symptom of another

disorder.-' They note, for example, that primary childhood depression

is sometimes associated with hyperactivity, Staton and Brumback (1981)

explain that "hyperactives" are quickly medicated without an adequate

differential diagnostic evaluation.

The correct diagnosis of hyperactivity becomes critical when

pharmacological interventions are being considered, The presence of

side-effects caused by drugs used to control the disorder may resuU

in more difficuUies than benefUs when inappropriately utilized.

Sequelae ranging from annoying to serious have been noted in approxi-

mately one-third of the children to whom drugs have been given (Walden

& Thompson, 1981). These side-effects include: loss of appetite,

sleeplessness, depression, withdrawal, cardiac changes, and retar-

dation of height and weight gain (Firestone, Poitras-Wright, & Douglas,

1978), Sandoval, Lambert, and Sassone (1981) estimate that 85?. of

Page 26: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

16

those children identified by physicians as hyperactive received a

prescription for medication for at least six months at some time during

their lives,

Several classifications of drugs have been used to treat hyper-

activity; the psychostimulants, the mood aUerators, and the neurolep-

tics. Each has been associated with various side-effects,

Common psycho-stimulants which are prescribed include methyl-

phenidate hydrochloride (Ritalin), pemoline (Cylert), amphetamine

sulfate (Benzedrine), and dextroamphetamine suUate (De3(edrine), Over

40 side-effects associated with the use of amphetamines have been

listed (Goodman & Gilman, 1975), If a side-effect persists the phy-

sician wi-ll usually shift to Ritalin or Cylert in an effort to elimi-

nate the problem,

The tricyclics, amitriptyline hydrochloride (Elavil), imipramine

hydrochloride (Thorazine), and haloperidol (Haldol), are used generally

in more difficuU cases of hyperactivity. The incidence and intensity

of their side-effects are markedly greater, however, and some physicians

have been reluctant to prescribe them.

Axelrod and Bailey (1979) warn of the careless use of drug

therapy. They note that many of the research results are conflicting,

that side-effects are often worse than the disorder, and that medica-

tions sometime have paradoxical effects, O'Leary (1980) agrees, citing

his research which demonstrated that psychopharmacological treatment

often resulted in short-term changes in social behavior but not in

long-term shifts in academic or social behavior.

The diagnosis of hyperactivity can be differentially affected by

Page 27: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

17

ethnic and/or social factors. Langsdorf et a1., (1979) and Waechter

et al., (1979) found increased incidences of perceived hyperactivity

in lower socioeconomic groups as well as in certain ethnic minorities.

Stevens' (1981) data supports this finding, noting that perceived

socioeconomic status and ethnic identification of assessees influenced

the assessor's attributions of hyperkinetic behavior. The importance

of these incorrect diagnoses is underscored when the deleterious side-

effects of the preferred treatments are considered.

The keen interest in pharmacological studies has indirectly lim-

ited research on assessment instruments. Sandoval (1977) explains

that the bulk of the literature deals with the evaluation of medica-

tions for use with hyperactive children, As a resuU of this focus,

extensive data on reliability and validity of research instruments

is not available. Typically instruments are used simply as part of

a standard assessment battery administered to each child and are not

selected particularly for a hyperactive study.

This discussion makes it clear that the proper diagnosis of hyper-

activity can be critical. The hurdles to overcome may be more pervasive

than has been implied above. Robin and Bosco (1976) write that the

different relevant social systems (schools, families, medicine) work

to obstruct the proper diagnosis and treatment of hyperactivity.

Conrad (1974) agrees and goes further by discussing the social impli-

cations of the medicalization of hyperactivity. Williamson et al..

(1980) have proposed a model of hyperkinesis which takes into account

family and academic environments as well as physiological environments.

In short, a physiological etiology of hyperkinesis may prove to be

Page 28: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

18

excessively simplistic. Appelbaum (1975) encouraged a muUidimensional

diagnostic procedure, including a neurological examination, psycholo-

gical testing, interviews, detailed medical and behavioral interviews,

and an assessment of the family's functioning.

The consequences of mislabeling can at best be merely incon-

venient and at worst catastrophic. The possible implications of a

fauUy diagnosis go beyond merely the side-effects of medications.

Important influences on a child's heightened activity level may go

unnoticed as a consequence of the blinders resuUing from pharmacolo-

gical interventions.

Current Diagnosti-c Practices

A major issue in the assessment of hyperactivity is the identifi-

cation and evaluation of methods of measurement. Numerous assessment

methods have been used or proposed. Poggio and Salkind (1979) appraised

nine instruments designed to ass'êss the disorder, Most were found to

have serious flaws. Few follow-up studies exist which examine norma-

tive, validity, and reliability data, This section will examine the

various diagnostic practices used, including (a) interviews, (b) indi-

vidual tests, (c) laboratory measures, and (d) rating scales.

Interviews

Interviews, during which a parent (usually the mother) is asked

about the child's development and current problems, are often utilized

in the diagnosis of hyperactivity. Both informal and semi-structured

formats for such interviews have been developed, Interview formats

can consist of specific questions, or of lists of specific topical

Page 29: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

19 }

areas to be covered, with questions of the interviewer's choice.

Golinko (.1978) suggested the use of the Behavioral Interview, a spe-

cific set of questions answered by the parents, which is designed to

differentiate hyperactive children from non-hyperactives, Rutter and

Brown (1966), after comparing several different interview strategies,

asserted that the parent interview can be a primary source of descrip-

tive diagnostic information about the child. They explained that

despite the fact that parents' perception of their children are sub-

jective and unsystemmatic, their exposure to the child's behavior

over a period of years and in many situations makes them potential

experts on these matters, There is cause, however, to be cautious

about the validity of retrospective material culled from the parent

interview (.Yarrow, Campbell, & Burton, 1970).

Individual Tests

Sandoval (1977) divides tests used to assess hyperactivity into

two groups: (a) simple performance tests, such as the Bender Visual

Motor Gestalt Test, the Frostig Developmental Test of Visual Perception,

the Human Figure Drawing Test, and the Porteus Maze Test, and (b) higher-

order cognitive tests, such as the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for

Children (WISC and WISC-^R), the Embedded Figures Test (EFT), and the

Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT), Projective tests and children's

personality inventories are used less frequently,

SimpTe performance terts - The Bender Visual Motor GestaU Test

has been utilized in hyperactivity research since receiving a good

score is dependent on plsnning skills as well as attention to detail,

Page 30: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

20

characteristics frequently absent in hyperkinetic children. The

measure, however, is apparently not successful in distinguishing

hyperactive from normal children (Palkes & Stewart, 1972), Dyckman,

Ackerman, Peters, and McGrew (1974) did demonstrate that the test,

under certain conditions, could differentiate between learning dis-

abled and normal children. The Bender-GestaU apparently does measure

impulsivity in some children, but there are perhaps other instruments

which provide better indicants.

Certain subtests of the Marianne Frostig Developmental Test of

Visual Perception have been found to be able to differentiate medi-

cated from non-medicated hyperactive children (Sandoval, 1977). The

Frostig apparently taps some elements of certain hyperactive charac-

teristics, In particular, the figure-ground test requires attention

to detail in the face of distracting stimuli. The test, as a whole,

requires the child to resist responding impulsively, an act that would

presumably be difficult for hyperactives. .

Drawing tests have been used as measures of attention to detail

with hyperactives, It is presumed that anti-hyperactivity medication

would positively affect attention, concentration, and impulsivity,

and hence improve performance on such tests. Palkes and Stewart (1972)

found hyperactives scores lower on the Figure Drawing Test than normals

after partialling out WISC IQ scores. Further, Millichap, Aymat,

Sturgis, Larsen, and Egan (1968) noted that the drug effect was

strongest for children who had initially scored low on the drawing

test, but were otherwise average in intelligence.

The Porteus Maze Test consists of a series of progressively more

Page 31: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

21

difficult mazes that yield an iq and a Qualitative, or Q score. The

former has been shown to differentiate hyperactive from normal sub-

jects (Spring, Yellin, & Greenberg, 1976) and to be drug sensitive

(Conners, Eisenberg, & Sharpe, 1964; Conners & Rothschild, 1968;

Conners, Rothschild, Eisenberg, Schwartz, & Robinson, 1969; Conners,

Taylor, Meo, Kurtz, & Fourmer, 1972; Epstein, Lasagna, Conners, &

Rodriguez, 1968). The Q score has not been found to be drug sensitive

(Epstein et al., 1968; Rapoport, Ouinn, Bradbard, Riddle, & Brooks,

1974), Palkes, Stewart, and Kahana (1968) demonstrated, however, that

training of hyperactive boys in verbal mediation to inhibit impulsive

behavior was reflected in improved IQ and Q scores on this test.

Higher order cognitive i:ests - Several instruments, all of which

presumably tap more complex cognitive processes than those mentioned

above, have been investigated with regard to their ability to identify

hyperactive children. The most commonly used tests in this category

include the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC and WISC-R),

the Embedded Figures Test (EFT), and the Matching Familiar Figures

Test (MFFT). The WISC has been used primarily as an index of improve-

ment in attention and concentration. The EFT is a measure of Field

Dependence-Independence, while the MFFT assesses Reflection-Impulsivity.

Despite its widespread use in clinical applications the WISC has

not been investigated in any depth with regard to identifying subtest

patterns peculiar to hyperactive children. More studies have been

completed which examine the Performance, Verbal, and Full Scale IQ

scores for different groups,

Page 32: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

22

Keogh, Wetter, McGinty, and Donlon (1973) analyzed WISC subtest

groupings including Verbal Comprehension (Information, Vocabulary,

and Comprehension), Analytic-Field-Approach (Object Assembly, Block

Design, and Picture Completion), and Attention-Concentration (Arith-

metic, Digit Span, and Coding). Hyperactive subjects tended to score

lower on the Attention'rConcentration score, but not on the other

scores. Palkes and Stewart (1972) found that controls scored signi-

ficantly higher than hyperactives on the Similarities, Picture Com-

pletion, and Mazes scores. IQ scores were also significantly higher

in controls,

Studies involving simply Verbal, Performance, and FuU Scale IQ

scores have yielded mixed resuUs, Medicated groups scored better

than placebo groups on Performance IQ in some investigations (Epstein

et al,, 1958; Knights & Hinton. 1969; Page, Bernstein, Janicki, &

Michelli, 1974) but not in others (Conners et al., 1969; Conners et al.,

1972; Conrad, Dworkin, Shai, & Tobiessen, 1971; Finnerty, SoUys, &

Cole, 1971). Knights and Hinton (1969) noted improvement in only the

Picture Completion, Block Design, and Coding subtests. Epstein et al.,

(1968) saw improvement in all subtests.

The WISC's ability to differentiate hyperactive from normal

children is questionable at best. Sandoval (1977) sees the instru-

ment as both inappropriate and unjustified as a measure of attention

and concentration, given the availability of other measures.

Field-dependence, as measured by the EFT (Witkin, 1959), has been

presumed to measure one aspect of hyperactivity, that being distracti-

bility. Field dependent children, in theory (Campbell et al,, 1971),

Page 33: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

23

have more difficuUy, due to their increased leyel of distractibility,

acting on a problem (locating a figure in a confused and distracting

context in the case of the EFT) than do fieldT-independent children,

While there is some evidence that hyperactives are more field depen-

dent than controls, performance on the EFT is not markedly influenced

by medication (Campbell et al., 1971; Cohen et al., 1972; Schleifer,

Weiss, Cohen, Elman, Cvjic, & Kruger, 1975; Winsberg, Bialer, Kupietz,

& Tobias, 1972). The EFT is not an unequiyocal measure of attention-

distractibility,

Kagan's Matching Familiar Figures Test (MFFT) has been one of the

most frequently used research instruments in the investigation of hyper^

activity. Its construction is based on the observations that some

individuals select and report solution hypotheses quickly and with mini-

mal concern for their probable accuracy, while others, of equal intelli-

gence, take more time to decide about the validity of their solutions.

The impulsive strategy has been proposed to be correlated with learning

disabilities, specifically hyperactivity, The basic task on the MFFT is

for the subject to choose the one of six facsimile items that is identi-

cal to a standard (an iHustration of a familiar object). All fac-

similes (save for one) differ from the standard in at least one de-

tail. The critical variables scored are response time to the subject's

first answer and the total number of errors across the 12-item test.

Impulsivity is recognized as one of the three major charácteris-

tics of children diagnosed as having and attention deficit disorder

(Douglas, 1976; American Psychiatric Association, 1980). The MFFT has

been the primary research tool used to measure this behavior.

Page 34: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

24

Hyperactive children have been said to represent the extremes

of impulsive behavior as described by Kagan (1966). They make de-

cisions too rapidly, fail to consider possible consequences, and sieze

the first response that comes to mind. The fact that hyperactive

children represent this extreme has been confirmed empirically

(Campbell et al., 1971), Keogh and Donlon (1972) recommended a con-

sideration of the presence of impulsivity as a necessary condition

for a diagnosis of hyperactivity. . Further, they suggested that

Kagan's MFFT be included in the standard assessment battery for hyper-

activity.

Regarding the reliability of the MFFT, there are four studies

in which^children six to 10 years of age were retested on the same

version of the MFFT after periods of one to eight weeks. Response

time reliabilities were .58, .68, .73, and .96, corresponding error

reliabilities were .39, .34, .43, and .80 (Adams, 1972: Duckworth,

Ragland, Sommerfeld, & Wyne, 1974; Hall & Russell, 1974; Siegelman,

1969). Epstein, Cullinan, and Lloyd (1977) found that the instrument

had a high level of stability over time. Block, Block, and Harrington

(1974) reported an internal consistency reliability coefficient for

MFFT response time of .89.

An extensive literature exists establishing the construct validity

of Kagan's test. Margolis, Leonard, Brannigan, and Heverly (1980) re-

ported data supporting the construct validity of the MFFJ as an index

of reflection-impulsivity, Rovet (1980) found the instrument to sig-

nificantly differentiate between previously ideniified (through means

other than the MFFT) impulsive and reflective groups.

Page 35: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

25

In four studies (Campbell et al., 1971; Cohen, Douglas, &

Morgenstern, 1971; Rapoport et al,, 1974; Schleifer et al,, 1975) the

MFFT was shown to differentiate hyperactive children of various ages

from control children. In addition, all four studies demonstrated

that the impulsivity score is influenced by active psychostimulants.

Juliano (1974) has also reported a greater ratio of impulsives to re-

flectives among hyperactives as compared with normal children.

MFFT scores have been found to be related to several of the di-

agnostic criteria characteristics of hyperactivity. Reflective four-

and eight-year-old children sustáin attention in play longer than im-

pulsives (Campbell, 1973). Impulsive pre-schoolers are likely to

start and stop their activities, and to chat or roam between activities;

reflectives sustain attention even while chatting (Messer, 1976).

The ability of reflectives and impulsives to sustain attention has

also been explored by testing their reaction time with a variable inter-

val between the signal to "get ready" and the presentation of the stimu-

lus to which they are to respond (Zelniker, Jeffrey, AuU, & Parsons,

1972). As predicted, no difference between impulsives and reflectives

was observed on the short preparatory intervals, On the longer inter-

vals, however, impulsives took longer to respond, suggesting their

greater reluctance, or inability, to sustain attention in a laboratory

task.

On a modification of Buss's aggression machine, seven-year-old

impulsive boys displayed more aggression than did reflectives (Thomas,

1971). This suggests that impulsives exercise less behavioral control

as well as less cognitive control. Mann (1973) has reported that

Page 36: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

26

reflective first^graders were more likely to choose delayed rewards

than were impulsives, Impulsives tended to opt for immediate rewards.

If the concept of reflection-impulsivity has generality, re-

flectives and impulsives should remain reflective and impulsive on

tests similar to the MFFT as well as in other test situations containing

response uncertainty. Yando and Kagan (.1970) used 10 different matching

familiar figures tests, each with a different number of facsimile fig-

ures (ranging from two to 12). Despite increasing levels of complexity

most children retained their relative rank on both response time and

errors.

Two other tests used to explore the generality of reflection-

impulsivity are the Design Recall Test (DRT) and the Haptic Visual

Matching Test (HVMT; Kagan, Rosman, Day, Albert, & Phillips, 1964),

In the DRT geometrical forms are used, and the subject must choose the

correct aUernative from his/her memory of the standard, which is

viewed first and then removed. The standard in the HVMT is either a

three-dimensional geometrical form or a familiar object that the sub-

ject feels but does not see, the correct aUernative again being chosen

from memory. Response times to the MFFT, DRT, and HVMT are moderately

intercorrelated, with intercorrelations ranging from .33 to .52

(Kagan, 1965; Kagan, Pearson, & Welch, 1966; Kagan et al., 1964).

Raven's Coloured Progressive Matrices involves the subject se-

lecting the correct variant from an array of six variants that will

complete a matri^. Hall and Russell (1974) found the MFFT latency

scores correlated .54 with mean latency to choice of variant on the

Raven.

Page 37: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

27

The above data appear to support the contention that the MFFT

may be a useful ins'trument in the identification of hyperactive child-

ren. Based on research evidence it surfaces as the most potentially

accurate measure for this purpose, It is for these reasons that the

MFFT is utilized in this study as one of the two instruments of in-

terest.

Laboratory Measures

Specialized performance measures have been explored in labora-

tory settings with regard to the information they can yield about

hyperactivity. A group of investigators have looked at measures of

automatic functioning that is subject to distraction; the Stroop Color

Distraction Test and the Santostefano and Paley Colour Distraction Test.

The task is for the subject to read a color-name in the presence of

a distracting factor, the color of the ink. It was found that these

tests did not differentiate hyperactive children from controls

(Campbell et al., 1971; Cohen, Weiss, & Minde, 1972) nor were they

drug sensitive.

Millichap and Fowler (1967) suggested that direct measures of

activity be included in evaluation studies of hyperactive behavior.

Attempts at such measurements have involved the use of a room fitted

with photoelectric cells (Ellis & Pryer, 1959) or uUrasonic sensors

(McFarland, Peacock, & Watson, 1966), placing a child's desk on a

suspended platform fitted with movement sensors (Foshee, 1958), and

placing a radio transmitter in a helmet worn by the child (Davis,

Sprague, & Werry, 1969; Herron & Ramsden, 1967). It should be noted.

Page 38: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

28

however, that most of these investigations involved mentally retarded

children, In hyperactivity research three devices have been commonly

used; the actometer, the activity recorder, and the stabilimetric

cushion, They are, respectively, a modified seU-winding wristwatch

(the actometer), a two-dimensional pedometer-type device attached to

the child's shirt back (the activity recorder), and a cushion embedded

with microswitches to detect movement while seated (the stabilimetric

cushion),

The actometer proved to be very reUáble when attached to a

machine (Schulman & Reisman, 1959) but unreliable when attached to

a child (Johnson, 1971), The actometer has been used in three clinical

tests of Jîiedication by Millichap (Millichap & Boldrey, 1967; Millichap,

Aymat, Sturgis, Larsen, & Egan, 1968; Millichap & Johnson, 1974) with

mixed resuUs.

The activity recorder has been validated with hyperactive child-

ren in that it has been found to distinguish between teacher-judged

hyperactive and non-hyperactive boys (Victor, Halverson, Inoff, &

Guczkowski, 1973), but it does not appear to be drug sensitive

(Rapoport, Abramson, Alexander, & Lott, 1971). The stabilimetric

cushion not only differentiated between groups (Sykes, Douglas, Weiss,

& Minde, 1971) but was also drug sensitive to the stimulant Ritalin,

aUhough not to the tranquilizer thioridazine (Sprague, Barnes, &

Werry, 1970; Sprague, Christensen, & Werry, 1974).

Because of the presence of instrument unreliability, the expense

of the equipment, the obtrusive nature of some instruments^ and the

problem of determining and/or controlling the context of measurement

Page 39: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

29

in a field study, direct measures of physical activity are less at-

tractive than other diagnostic jnstruments.

The Vigilance Task

Attentíon deftcits and impulsivity are two of the major behavioral

components in the diagnosis of ADD (American Psychiatric Association,

1980). Several investigators (Bakan, 1966; Jerison, 1967; Mackworth,

1968) have proposed that a vigilance paradigm lends itseU to answering

questions about the nature of attention.

The typical vigilance study is structurally very similar to many

detection and discrimination experiments. An observer is presented

with stimuli, some of which are to be reported, Specific parameters

of vigilance experiments include, (a) the vigil is maintained for ex-

tended periods (usually one-half hour or longer), (b) signals are pre-

sented infrequently and unpredictably, and (c) the signals are psycho-

physically strong (i.e., they are nearly always reported correctly by

a normal population with virtually no false alarms, but weak in a sub-

jective sense because they are not "attention demanding." The observer

must be continuously alert in order to report all signals accurately.

Detection failures are usually identified as failures of attention.

One concern in using a vigilance procedure with small children

is the issue of stimulus complexity. Jerison (1967) argued that sim-

plicity must be utilized in vigilance tasks in which the problem is to

decide whether or not a specific stimulus is a signal. He called for

the discontinuance of the use of complex sensory analyses such as

search, scanning, or memory from the study of detection of a signal

in a vigilance paradigm.

Page 40: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

30

Numerous vigilance studies have been conducted investigating

individual differences in the normal population with regard to vigi-

lance, Examples of such research include signal probability and stimu-

lus density (Jerison, Pickett, & Stenson, 1965), unwanted signals

(Colquhoun, 1961), signal rate differences (Jenkins, 1958), and rein-

forcement expectancy (Deese, 1955),

The hypothesis that the requirements of a vigilance task would

pose difficuUy for hyperkinetic children has been supported by several

investigators (Anderson, Halcomb, Gordon, & Ozolins, 1974; Doyle,

Anderson, & Halcomb, 1976; Kaspar, Millichap, Backus, Child, & Schulman,

1971; Sykes, Douglas, & Morgenstern, 1972). Hyperkinetics have been

shown to jdetect fewer correct signals and produce more false responses

than controls. Thus, it appears that hyperkinetic children have greater

than average difficulty in sustaining their attention to a continuous

performance task than do controls (Rosenthal & Allen, 1978; Sykes et

al., 1973).

Anderson et al., (1973), using the same vigilance task employed

in the present study, demonstrated that the instrument effectively dif-

ferentiated learning disabled children from normals on correct detec-

tions and false alarms. The study also suggested that a hyperactive,

hypoactive, normoactive distinction might be made.

In a later study, Anderson et al., (1974) utilized the vigilance

task to measure the effects of medication on attentional deficits in

hyperactive children. The resuUs indicated that hyperactive children

between the ages of six and eight scored more correct detections while

on medication than when off.

Page 41: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

31

Several other investigators at Texas Tech University have used

the Vigilance Task with learning disabled and/or hyperactive children,

Doyle (1973) utilized the instrument to investigate the effects of dis-

traction and attention deficits among children with learning disabi-

lities. He concluded that the Vigilance Task provided a systematic

means with which to investigate attention deficits in children with

learning disabilities.

Ozolins (1974) studied the effects of knowledge of resuUs on

the vigilance performance of hyperactive and hypoactiye children with •

learning disabilities, It was found possible to alter a child's

arousal level by manipulating which information about resuUs was

dispensed,

Finally, Langsdorf (1980) used the Vigilance Task as one cri-

terion variable in order to identify a hyperactive group. In this

study, three measures were obtained during the Vigilance Task; correct

detections (a measure of attentional abilities), false alarms (impulsi-

vity), and an activity count (a measure of motoric restlessness),

Rating Scales

Paper-pencil rating scales are frequently used as screening in-

struments for the identification of hyperactivity and sometimes play

an integral role in the diagnosis of individual cases. Scales to be

completed by the child's teacher are most commonly used, Given their

training and experience base teachers are considered to be more re-

liable raters than would be parents, Several studies attest to tea-

chers' ability to identify children who will behave deviantly during

classroom observations (Bolstad & Johnson, 1977; Bowers, 1978; Jones,

Page 42: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

32

Loney, Weissenhurger, & Gleischmcinn, 1975; Sprague & Gadow, 1976).

A teachers' rating scale developed by Conners [1969) is the most

frequently cited instrument of this kind. It exists in two forms:

(a) a 39-item form with five factorially derived scales (aggressive

conduct disorder, inattention, withdrawal, hyperactivity, and social

ability), and (b) a 10-item form, either administered separately or

embedded in a larger version. Norms and validation for the Conners

scale are presented by Werry, Sprague, and Cohen (1975). A significant

correlation between the Conners scale and observation of children's

behavior in the classroom was demonstrated by Cristensen (1975),

Sprague (1977) suggests the use of a cut-off score of 15 (two standard

deviation's above the mean) on the Conners abbreviated scale to identify

probable hyperkinetic children. Numerous investigators have used that

guideline in screening referrals.

Copeland and Weisbrodal (1978), using the Conners scale, found

that it successfully tapped observable dimensions of hyperactivity in

novel situations. Zentall and Barack (1979) investigated validity

and reliability issues. High correlations were noted which suggested

that there existed excellent predictability between the scale and

stability across time and rater,

The Zukow Hyperkinesis Rating Form (Zukow, Zukow, & Bentler,

1978) has been used for parental ratings of hyperactivity (Langsdorf,

1980). It consists of 28 behavioral items in a forced-choice format,

yielding three factor clusters: Excitability, Motor Coordination, and

Directed Attention. Zukow et al., (1978) note that the items included

in the Excitability cluster are most similar to those characteristics

Page 43: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

33

of hyperactivity that are most typicc^lly descrlbed,

The most common conclusion abouf hyperactivity is that diagnostic

information should be sought from various perspectives and sources,^

The hope is that the resuUs of numerous procedures from various sources

win converge on a single diagnostic conclusion and that some clear eti-

ology or treatment direction win emerge if enough diagnostic clues are

unearthed. This diagnostic advice derives from several facts; (a) the

inconsistency and variability of the hyperkinetic child's behavior, (b)

the continuing ambiguity surrounding the diagnosis of hyperactivity, (c)

the fact that hyperactivity may have several causes (Fish, 1971), and

(.d) the measures, tests, and scales available for the assessment of hy-

peractivity are generany only indirect indices of hyperkinetic behavior

patterns.

-Research Hypotheses

It is evident from the above discussion that the availabinUy of

objective and reliable means of classifying hyperactive children ac-

curately would serve to faciU'tate the ameU'oration of many problems

associated with their treatment and education. The extant n'terature

points to two instruments which show promise in differentiating hyper-

active and non-hyperactive children, i.e,, the Vigilance Task and the

Matching Familiar Figures Test. This study aims to assess their use-

fulness, when combined, in classifying children in a hyperactive, non-

hyperactiye dimension. The foHowing hypotheses encompass the specific

research questions of interest in this study:

1, Scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching FamiUar Figures

Test can yield discriminant functions which accurately and consistently

Page 44: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

34

differentiate hyperactive from non'-hyperactive children,

2, Neither the Vigilance Task nor the Matching Familie^r Fig-

ures Test alone can accurately and consistently differentiate hyper-

active from non-hyperactive children.

3, There win be no sex or age differences with regard to the

discriminant functions' abiUty to classify hyperactive, learning dis-

abled, and normal children accurately,

4. Hyperactive children win obtain quantitatively different

scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Famin'ar Figures Test

than learning disabled and "normal" children,

5, Normal children win obtain quantitatively different scores

on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Famin'ar Figures Test than hy-

peractive and learning disabled children.

6, Scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Familiar Fig-

ures Test win be significantly correlated (in the appropriate direc-

tions), This assumes that they are measuring similar, or coexisting

phenomenon in research subjects.

7. The Vigilance Task and the Matching Famin'ar Figures Test win

demonstrate adequate split-haU ren'abinty figures.

8. Teacher and parent ratings win be significantly correlated

(in the appropriate directions) with each other and with scores on the

Vigilance Task and the Matching Famin'ar Figures Test,

9. There win be no relationship between subjects' Slosson IQ

scores and performance on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Famin'ar

Figures Test.

Page 45: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

CHAPTER II

^ METHODS AND PROCEDURES

The present chapter describes the methodological procedures uti-

n'zed to investigate the research hypotheses. The fonowing topics are

discussed: (a) subject characteristics, (b) apparatus, (c) instrumen-

tation, Cd) procedures, and (e) statistical design.

Subjects

The subject population consisted of 100 Caucasion elementary

school students, ranging in age from five years-six months to 11 years

six months (X age = 8.78). Subjects were controned for race due to

possible differences in referral rates among various ethnic groups

(Langsdorf et al,, 1979; Waechter et al,, 1979). Further, no subjects

were included who were currently being treated for hyperactivity medi-

cally, thus eliminating any possible pharmacological confounds. The

sample was divided into two approximately equal-sized subsamples made

up of younger children (5-6 to 8-6, N = 51, X age = 7.35) and older

children (8-7 to 11-5, N = 49, X" age = 10.27), Fifty-two percent of

the subjects were male, 48% were females. The young group was composed

of 42% males, 58% females. The old group had 62% males and 38% females.

Table 1 summarizes the population data.

The Leyenand Independent School District in Levenand, Texas. and

St. Joseph's CathoU'c School in Slaton, Texas served as referral sources

Cover letters (see Appendices B and C) were sent to an parents at St.

35

Page 46: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

36

YOUNG GROUP

N

Mean Age

OLD GROUP

N

Mean Age

Table 1

Number of Subjects and Mean Age of

Young/Old and Male/Female Groups

MALE^

22

7.36

30

10.30

FEMALE

29

7.35

19

10.25

Joseph's (N = 94) asking them to allow their children to participate.

In Levenand administrators, principals, counselors, and teachers were

asked to submit names and addresses of children who they thought might

meet the requirements for inclusion in this study, Further, the South

Plains Educational Co-op (SPECO; the special education department of the

Levenand Independent School District) provided data (names and ad-

dresses) of those children who had been classified as learning disabled

and who were receiving some form of special education services. The

parents of all of these children were sent cover letters explaining

the project and asking for their participation (N = 87).

Once parents agreed to anow their children to serve as subjects

(N = 55. 58 ; in Slaton: N = 62, 71% in Levenand) they were asked to

Page 47: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

37

sign a consent form (see Appendix D) and complete two paper-pencil

forms aimed at assessing their child's hehavior. These forms included

the Zukow Rating Scale for Hyperkinesis (see Appendix E; Zukow et a1.,

1978) and a behavior checklist (see Appendix F) based on the diagnostic

criteria listed in DSM III for Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyper-

activity (see Appendix A). These forms, as wen as the ratings pro-

vided by the child's teachers, served to identify those in the hyper-

active group. A n children in the study, however, were rated on these

forms. The forms themselves are discussed more fully in the Instru-

mentation section of this chapter.

It should be noted that the seemingly low return rates are de-

ceiving., Many of the children were cuned from the study prior to

the return of the forms for various reasons other than the denial of

permission to participate, e.g,, medical treatment for hyperactivity,

race, age, etc. Virtuany all of those parents asked agreed to their

child's participation in the study.

Teachers at St. Joseph's school and three of the four elementary

schools in Levelland, as wen as elementary school counselors and ad-

ministrators in both systems, were asked to complete two forms on a n

of the children they teach or serve. These forms included the behavior

checklist described above, and the Conners' Teacher Rating Scale

(Conners, 1969; see Appendix G). Sandoval (1977) suggested asking

teachers to rate the entire class, rather than simply those suspected

to be hyperactive, in an effort to avoid having the raters become

particularly sensitive to that child's behavior. Every precaution

was taken to avoid having the teachers associate the derivation of

Page 48: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

38

the "hyperactive" U s t with these ratings. A period of several weeks

elapsed between the derivation of the n'st and the request for ratings.

Furthermore, the latter task was performed, for the most part, by SPECO

personnel, while the obtaining of the n*st from which parents were

notified was undertaken by the experimenter. Only ratings of children

whose parents had consented to allow participation were utin'zed.

Forms rating children for which consent was not procured were destroyed.

From this sampUng 48 potentiany hyperactive children and 36

learning disabled/non-hyperactive children were identified. Consents

and completed questionnaires were obtained from 40 (83%) in the hyper-

active group and 34 (94%) in the learning disabled group. Of these

children'27 hyperactives met the criteria (see below) for hyperactivity

and completed an procedures. Twenty-seven of the learning disabled

group also completed a n procedures, The majority of these children

(96%) were located in the Levenand Independent School District. There

were two children who met the hyperactive criteria at St. Joseph's.

Additionany, a group of 46 students from St, Joseph's completed a n

procedures and were included in the study. This sample largely con-

stituted the non-hyperactive/non-learning disabled group.

Once a n forms were completed appointments were arranged in order

to administer the Slosson Intenigence Test, the Matching Famin'ar

Figures Test, and the Vigilance Task, In all cases these instruments

were administered in school buildings. Details of procedures are dis-

cussed below. Immediately upon completion of a n testing a report

form (see Appendix H) and interviews (when requested) were provided

for the children's parents.

Page 49: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

39

Suhjects in the discriîninant ancilysis phase of the inyestigí^tion

were divided into three groups based on varying criteria, These three

groups included: (a) a hyperactive group, (b) a learning disabled group,

and (c) a "normaV,'.' non-hyperactive, non-learning disabled group.

Breakdowns for these groups can be found in Tables 2 and 3. The cri-

teria for inclusion in each group were as fonows:

Table 2

Number of Subjects by Sex X

Classification X Age Group

Hyperactive Lng. Disabled Normal

Male Female Male Femal^ Male Female

Young 5 8 7 7 10 14

Old 11 3 7 6 12 10

Table 3

Number of Subjects X

Grade X Classification

GRADE

Classificatton 1 2 3 4 5 6 SeU-<:t)ntained

Hyperactive 2 3 3 6 5 3 3 2

L, D. 2 4 5 4 3 7 2 0

Normal 4 7 13 11 2 9 0 0

Page 50: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

40

1, Hyperactive group. To be placed in the hyperactiye group

it was required that the child satisfy the diagnostic criteria as set

forth in DSM III and evaluated by the behavior checklist (see Appendix

F), This checklist was completed by three respondents (one parent

(mother) and two teacher/counselors) for each child. Inclusion in

this group required agreement by a n three raters that the child

satisfied DSM III criteria.

In addition to this standard, at least two raters must have

agreed on a hyperkinetic rating based on the Zukow Hyperkinesis Rating

Form (mother) and/or the Conners Teacher Rating Scale. It was felt

that the utin'zation of rigid, multiple criteria would resuU in the

identification of only those children who consistently exhibited hy-

perkinetic behavior patterns, Children who were rated as hyperactive

by some of the raters (but not up to the criteria) were not utin'zed

in the study (N = 1 0 ) ,

2. Learning disabled group. This group was included due to

the observation that hyperkinetic children are frequently identified

as learning disabled and vice^versa. Lambert and Sandoval (1980) noted

that 42.6% of hyperactives who achieved below grade level were eligible,

on the basis of objective criteria, for a learning disabled diagnosis.

An effective instrument for diagnosing hyperactivity should have the

abinty to differentiate these groups on the basis of the presence or

absence of hyperactivity, apart from the presence of other learning

disabiU'ties.

For the purpose of the present 5tudy this group was identified

in terms of the criteria defined by the Texas Education Agency as

Page 51: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

41

indicative of learning disabled children. A complete statement of

these criteria can be found in Appendix I.

A n subjects included in the learning disabled group were dieig-

nosed by the South Plains Educational Co^p as learning disabled, and

did not meet any of the criteria for being placed in the hyperactive

group, The aim was to identify hyperactive learning disabled children

as wen as hyperactive non-learning disabled children.

3, The "normal" group was^procured from the student body at

St, Joseph's school (aUhough two hyperactive students were identified

at this school), Identical procedures were employed with this group.

In order to be included subjects in this group could not satisfy any

of the crjteria for placement in the other two groups, No more than

one of the six ratings obtained could identify the child as hyperac-

tive, otherwise the child was not included in the study. This occurred

with only two subjects. Further, teachers were asked to screen out any

students that had the sn'ghtest possibin'ty of suffering from a learn-

ing disabin'ty of any sort.

The administration of a n procedures was scattered across both

time and day of the week (see Tables 4 and 5). This served to control

for any possible temporal biasing effect.

Apparatus

The Vigilance Task is an instrument which has frequently been

used by researchers and clinicians at Texas Tech University to assess

possible hyperkinesis. It has been described in detail by Anderson,

Halcomb, and Doyle (.1973). Basicany, the tas involves the child

attending to a series of visual events occurring over time.

Page 52: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

\2

Tahle 4

Number of Subjects Tested

By Time of Day

TIME OF DAY .NUMBER OF SUBJECTS TESTED

8:00 - 9:00 a.m, 8

9:00 - 10:00 a.m. 17

10:00 - 11:00 a.m. 16

11:00 - 12:00 a.m. 17

12:00 - 1:00 p.m. 9

1:00 - 2:00 p.m, 18

2:00 - 3:00 p.m. 12

3:00 - 4:00 p.m. 3

Table 5

Classification by Day of th,e Week Tested

DAY OF THE WEEK

Monday

Tuesday

Wednesd^y

Thursday

Friday

Hyperactive

5

7

3

6

6

^ CLASSIFICATION

Lng. Disa

3

5

5

9

5

ibled ^Normal

6

9

9

14

8

Page 53: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

43

The Vigilance Task consists of a visual display (5 cm X 10 cm)

of red and green U'ghts (see Appendix J for an inustration), Verbal

instructions weré provided by means of an audio recorder (see Appendix

K for script) in order to insure standardization, While sitting at a

console (approximately 1,2 m X 1,2 m) the subject observes a pair of

flashing U'ghts. The n'ghts flash in combinations of red-red, green-

green, red-green, or green-red once ever two seconds. The stimulus

duration is .2 seconds. When a red-green or green-^red combination ap-

pears, the subject is instructed to press a button mounted on a bicycle

handlebar grip, A response is scored a correct detection when the

button is pressed immediately foUowing the presentation of a red^green

or green-j^ed combination. Responses to pther combinations (i.e., red-

red, green-green) are scored as errors or false alarms. In the pre-

sent study two scores were computed for each child, the total number of

correct detections and the total number of errors. Responses were re-

corded electronicany by a counter. A printed record was produced as

wen.

A five-minute practice period preceded each administration of

the Vigilance Task. A task identical to the actual Vigilance Task was

presented save for a different (7,5) variable ratio schedule, The pur-

pose of this practice period was to insure that the child funy compre-

hended the task at hand. If it became evident that the child did not

understand the tape was replayed and the practice continued until it

was clear that the desired response was understood,

During the 30-minute experimental period. 900 total red^red,

green-green, or red-green combinations we' e presented. Sixty of these

Page 54: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

44

presentations were red-green Ccorrect detection) combinations. The

redr^green comhinations .were presented on a variable ratio 15 schedule,

with a total of 10 red-green combinations in eacK of the five-minute

intervals recorded by the counter. All other presentations were of

the red-red, green-green variety. The entire sequence of flashing

n'ghts was controned by a digital control system.

Instrumentatitjn

Matching FamiU'ar Figures Test

The school age children's form of the MFFT was used for this study,

The test format involved simuUaneous presentations of a figure (e.g,,

a boat, a pair of scissors. a telephone) with six facsimiles differing .

in one or,more details. On each of the test's 12 items the subject was

asked to select from the aUernatives the one that exactly matched the

standard. If an incorrect response was given the child was informed

of this fact and asked to try again. Correct responses were verbally

reinforced, Time to the first response (latency) and the number of er-

rors for each card were recorded.

Typicany subjects administered the MFFT are classifed on the

basis of a 2 X 2 matrix in which reflective andimpulsive children are

those who fall in two diagonal cens (see Table 6). Messer (1976)

Table 6

Kagan's Dcscriptors for MFFT Responders

RESPONSE TIME ACCURATE INACCURATE

Fast Fast Accurate Impulsive

Slow Reflective Slow Inaccurate

Page 55: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

45

suggested that the most appropriate procedure was to treat the data in

a continuous rather than a dichotomous manner. This anows latency

and error to be employed as continuous variables, thus avoiding the

hazards of dichotomization as wen as allowing for calculation of the

amounts of variance contributed separately by latency, error, and their

interaction, This is the manner in which the data were conected and

analyzed in the present study,

The Rating Scale for Hyperkinesis

The Rating Scale for Hyperkinesis (Zukow et al., 1978) is made

up of 28 items in a forced-choice format. It yields three factor

clusters: Excitability, Motor Coordination, and Directed Attention.

Zukow et. il. (1978) noted that the items defining the Excitabin'ty fac-

tor are similar to the characteristics of hyperactivity that are typi-

cany described. Langsdorf (1980) found, in using this scale, that

mothers of hyperactive children attributed more hyperkinetic charac-

teristics to their children than did mothers of two other non-hyperac-

tive groups.

This form (Appendix E) was completed by the mothers of an

children in the subject population. For the purposes of this study

ratings were considered hyperactive if the responses of the child's

mother to the first item (Unusually hyperactive: Home School Both No)

was rated in the "hyperactive" direction and the majority of items in

the Excitabin'ty cluster were endorsed.

Behavior Checkn'st

The behavior checklist has been derived entirely from the DSM

III diagnostic criteria for Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity

Page 56: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

46

(see Appendices A and F). It should be noted that these criteria are

expU'cit as to what symptoms should be present in order to identify

hyperactivity, These standards are the same which win apply for the

purposes of this study.

A n raters were asked to complete the behavior checklist for each

child. In order for a child to be placed in the hyperactive group all

raters must have agreed that the child satisfied these criteria.

Slosson InteUigence Test

The Slosson Intenigence Test (SIT) is a brief individual intel-

ngence test consisting of items adapted from-the Stanford-Binet.

Items are presented verbally and, with a few exceptions, require a

verbal response. The test takes approximately 20-minutes to admini-

ster. The SIT was used in this study to get a quick yet reasonably

van'd measure of each subject's level of intellectual functioning,

Administration Procedures

Once a n consent forms and ratings were returned arrangements

were made for testing the child. This testing was completed either

at St, Joseph's School or the South Plains Educational Co-op building,

In both cases the apparatus was located in a room free from distractions

and interruptions,

After a brief rapport-estabn'shing chat the child was admini-

stered the MFFT, The SIT was given immediately fonowing the MFFT.

At this point the subject was offered the opportunity to take a bath-

room break, being told that the next task must be uninterrupted. The

child was then escorted to the Vigilance Task apparatus and instructed

Page 57: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

47

to be seated, Instructions for the Vigilance Task itseU were recorded

on an audio tape in order to insure uniformity. The practice portion

of the tape was presented after which the practice was initiated.

Upon completion of the practice the remainder of the audio tape was

presented tsee Appendix K for script) and the testing session was be-

gun. It continued until the Vigilance Task was completed. The entire

procedure required approximately one to Ih hours to complete,

A n data for each subject were recorded on a standard data sheet

(see Appendix L) designed to facin'tate coding into a computer format.

A n raw data (including the data sheet, the Vigilance Task printout,

the SIT scoring sheet, teacher and parent ratings, and consent forms)

were numericany coded and filed,

Parents were provided with feedback on the resuUs of the test.

A brief report form (see Appendix H) was provided to explain the find-

ings for their child. The reports were written with an explicit state-

ment concerning the experimental nature of these instruments. An offer

was made on the report, as well as verbany whenever possible, to dis-

cuss the resuUs personally with the child's parents on request. Sev-

eral parents requested such an interview and they were held as soon

after the testing as possible. All parents of participating children

received at least written feedback within two days of testing,

Statistical Design

The primary statistical procedure in this study involved the

appn'cation of discriminant analysis computation. This design involves

two stages: (a) the assignments of each subject to the appropriate

diagnostic group, i.e., normal, learning disabled, hyperactive (this

Page 58: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

48

was accompnshed through the use of the various r^tings obtained from

teachers and parents), (b) the assessment stage at which time the pre-

dictor, or discriminating variables are administered.

The question at hand is essentiany one of classification, i,e.,

can the groups be differentiated on the basis of the scores obtained

on the set of discriminating variables? The objective of the analysis

is to determine the degree to which a child's profile of scores on a

set of tneasures corresponds to or resembles the typical profiles of each

of a given set of discrete classes. Specificany, the primary re-

search question involved determining if the two scores yielded by the

Matching Familiar Figures Test, along with the two scores yielded by

th'e Vigilance Task, provided accurate discrimination between hyperac-

tive and non-hyperactive children.

Discriminant analysis attempts to do this by forming one or more

n'near combinations of the discriminating variables. These "discrimi-

nant functions" are of the form:

D. = d.jZj + d.^Z^ + . . . + d.pZp

Where D- is the score on discriminant function i, the d's are weighting

coefficients, and the Z's are the standardized vaTues of the p discrimi-

nating variables used in the analysis. The maximum number of functions

which can be derived in this investigation is two. Ideany, the dis-

criminant scores (D's) for the cases within a particular group win

be fairly similar. At any rate, the functions are formed so as to max-

imize the separation of the groups. Once the discriminant functions .

have been deriyed, one is able to pursue the two research objectives of

this technique: analysis and classification.

Page 59: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

49

The analysis aspects of the technique proyide statistical tests

for measuring the success with which the discriminating varisibles actu-

a n y discriminate when combined into the discriminant functions, Since

the discriminant functions can be thought of as the axes of a geometric

space, they can be used to study the spatial relationships among the

groups. Finany, the weighting coefficients can be interpreted much

the same as in muUiple regression of factor analysis. They serve to

identify the variables which contribute most to differentiation along

the respective dimension.

The use of discriminant analysis as a classification technique

comes after the initial computation. If a set of variables resuU

which pravides significant discrimination for cases with known group

memberships, a set of classification functions can be derived which

win permit the classification of new cases with unknown memberships.

As a check of the adequacy of the discriminant functions the

original set of cases can be classified to see how many are correctly

classified by the variable being used, The procedure for classification

involves the use of separate linear combinations of the discriminating

variables for each group. These produce a probabin'ty of membership

in the respective group, and the case is assigned to the group with

the'highest probability.

Often there are more discriminating variables than are necessary

to achieve satisfactory discrimination. A stepwise procedure is typi-

cany used to select the most useful of the discriminating variables,

In this study both direct and stepwise analyses win be computed.

Additionany, several other computations win be made, in the

Page 60: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

50

form of t^-tests, correlations, and ANQVAs in order to investigate some

of the secondary hypotheses. These analyses are discussed, in detail,

in the ResuUs section of this study,

Page 61: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

CHAPTER III

RESULTS

The primary research hypotheses pertain to the usefulness of

scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Familiar Figures Test

in predicting group membershio, Specificany, the issue in question

is whether or not these scores can work to classify children accurately

into hyperactive and non-hyperactive groups. Discriminant analysis

was chosen as the most appropriate statistical tool for analyzing this

data. The SPSS (Statistical Package for the Social Sciences) subpro-

gram DISCRIMINANT (Klecka, 1975) was selected to perform this analysis.

The analysis yields, in both direct and stepwise fashion, discriminant

functions, which produce a probabin'ty of membership for each subject

in the respective groups; the case, then is assigned to the group with

the highest probabin'ty of membership,

The findings for each of the stated hypotheses are presented

separately, The first five related hypotheses are considered to en-

compass the major questions at hand and thus win be discussed in some

detail,

Hypothesis 1

The first hypothesis deals with the yielded discriminant func-

tion's ability to differentiate hyperactive from non-hyperactive child-

ren, based on scores on the Vigilance Task (correct detections and false

alarms) and the MFFT (latency and errors),

In this study, with three groups and four variables, the analysis

yielded two discriminant functions. The question is whether or not.

Page 62: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

' 52

either or hoth of the discriminant functions yielded can wprk to dif-

ferentiate hyperactive from non-hyperactive children, The relative

utinty of each of the functions can he assessed by the data in Taible 7,

Table 7

Eigenvalues and Measures of Importance

FUNCTION

1

2

EIGENVALUE

.24973

.00340

PERCENT OF VARIANCE

98.65

1.35

CANONICAL CORRELATION

.4470186

.0582502

The first eigenvalue of .24973 is more than 73 times larger than

the eigenvalue for the second function. This indicates that the second

function is \/ery weak in relation to the first. It can be seen that

the first function contains 98.65% of the total discriminating power

in this pair of equations.

A high canonical correlation coefficient indicates that a strong

relationship exists between the groups and the discriminant function.

Table 7 indicates that there is a moderate degree of relationship be-

tween the groups and the first function (relative to the second), and

virtually no relationship between the groups and the second function

(relative to the first). This means that the first function is more

powerful, in a relative sense, than the second. If the groups are not

wery different on the variables being analyzed, than all of the cor-

relations win he low. From the data contained in Table 7 it appears

that only function 1 is meaningful and at best it win have only mar-

ginal utility in explaining group differences.

Page 63: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

53

Wilk's lambda is a muUivairiate measure of group differences

over several variables (the discriminating variables). Values of lambda

denote d, high discrimination as they approach zero. Table 8 presents

this data,

Tabile 8

Residual Discrimination and

Tests of Significance

FUNCTIONS DERIVED

0

1 -

WILK'S LAMBDA

.7974593

.9966069

CHI SQUARED

21.614

0.325

DEGREES OF FREEDOM

8

3

SIGNIFICANCE LEVEL

.0057

.9553

Evidently the three groups of subjects scored somewhat differently

on at least some of the discriminating variables. This is evidenced by

a Wilk's Lambda of .7974593 which has significance at the .0057 level.

After the first (and most powerful) function is derived a good deal of

discrimtnating information is removed from the system, At this point

Lambda becomes ,9966069. This very high value indicates that any re-

maining information about group differences may not be worth pursuing.

This data demonstrates that the groups are somewhat different on their

scores on the Vigilance Task and the MFFT and that the first function

provides the bulk of the discriminating information. The remaining

function is either unimportant or statisticany spurious.

Given that hyperactive, learning disabled, and normal children do

score somewhat differently on these instruments the question which fol-

lows is whether or not these scores are different enough to anow

Page 64: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

54

consistently accurate classifications of the children based on the

scores alone. Since there were unequal numbers of subjects in each

group (Hyperactive N = 27, Learning Disabled N = 27, Normal N = 46,

Total N = 100) it would have been more probable that a particular sub-

ject would have been assigned to the normal group before he/she would

have been assigned to one of the other groups, In order to control

for these inequities adjustments for prior probabilities were made in

the analysis. These adjustments were particularly important with this

data because there existed considerable overlap among the groups (e,g,,

hyperactives scoring like normals, learning disabled children scoring

n'ke hyperactives, etc.)

Given the experimental questions implicit in this study a step-

wise procedure designed to yield a measure maximizing total group sepa-

ration was most appropriate. Rao's V measures the separation of group

centroids and does not concern itseU with cohesiveness within the

groups. The goal of this study was to maximize the differences between

the scores of hyperactive, learning disabled, and normal groups, hence,

Rao's V stepwise procedure was chosen. A fun analysis was also per-

formed in order to evaluate the discriminating abin'ties for a n of the

discriminating variables.

As a resuU of the application of Rao's V procedure two variables

(correct detections and latency) had significant discriminating power.

The resuUs of this analysis are summarized in Table 9.

It can be seen from the data in Table 9 that correct detections

on the Vigilance Task was the measure with the maximum discriminatinc

abilities. Latency on the MFFT does add some measure of unique discrim-

inating ability to the discriminant function, The change in Rao's .. V

Page 65: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

to

* CT>

OJ i ^

J 3 fO

1—

in > í

t—

«3 C

< : 4 J E 03 c:

• I —

E .r— s-o tn

•r— Q

S-o

H-

<D 1 —

J 2 fO

H-

>> S-rtJ E E =5

OO

O) S-:3

OJ u o s-

a_ OJ to

• 1 —

5: D -O)

+ J oo

> t/>

— o fO

or cn c

•r— to

rD

C3 »—I

oo

CD >

<: :z rr (—1 o

o o o

C\J 1—1

C3 I—I

oo

oo o <c

CsJ

ro cr» co

o o o o o o

C5

oo

C\J

ro cr>

o o o

CTí

C\J OsJ

o o o

oo <: = Q :s<i cû —1 ^ i-i cC

VD CM C3 ro ro co

r^ 0 0 CNJ 00 o 0 0

Z Q O UJ I—I D l | _ LLJ O I—

LLJ

CL. UJ

co

t/1

+-> o U T -OJ +-> s- u ^ <u o +-> o <u

Q

>>

u dJ

+-Í n3

CJ

Page 66: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

56 6

o co

O OJ O 0 0 O Vû

o ,—1

CU

s 03 h—

•4-» C fO c:

• r -

_E s-u to

• 1 —

Q

s-o

l+-

c: o

•r— + J o c: 3

U -

+ J c fO E

• i —

E • r ~

s-o to

• 1 —

Q

1 —

fO o

• 1—

c: o c «3

O

O) s-Z2

"O QJ O O s-

D -

dJ iT)

'r—

O-O)

+ i tr)

> to

— o fO oc: C7> C

•r— in r3 to

• p -to >.

1 —

03 C

•=£

Q UJ

I D : • - I < : 3 : =3 o o

co

t/^ < : - Q i ^ CQ _ j : ^

•— <:

o D í • - ' UJ h -h - O U_ Z < O

_ J o

O h -! - • < Z _ ! O LU

z: oí <SiCíL O O

o

O LU O

•Z. < 11 1 i—I

o D: D: <

LU o —1 < > UJ CD

CTi ro LD

o co

LO

C3 C\J 0 0 o 0 0

r--CM 0 0 C3 CTi

to ^ ^ CsJ to ro •^d-

r^ CTi ro tD i—1

^ o

CvJ

cn cn

co

•v j -

o LD OO CJ

•^ r>. r-H

o CJ

o l—l I— o

CVJ

Page 67: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

57

resulting from the inclusion of the latency score is not, however,

significant.

T^ble 10 indicates that wery n'ttle of the variables' discrimi-

nating abin'ties remain after correct detections and latency contribu-

tions are accounted for. Function 1 accounts for 99.27% of the variance

when these two variables are used. The addition of either or both of

the other two variables Cfalse alarms and errors) would have added

virtuany nothing to the discriminating abin'ty of the function.

The canonical correlation of function 1 is a measure of the

function's ability to discriminate among the groups, The canonical

correlation squared (in this case .1903093) can be interpreted as the

proportion of variance in the discriminant function explained by the

groups. Thus it can be seen from Table 10 that the first discriminant

function is moderately correlated with the groups but the second has

a yery low correlation,

The standardized discriminant function coefficients are of ana-

lytic importance in and of themselves (see Table 11). When the sign

Table 11

Standardized Canonical Discriminant

Function Coefficients (Rao's V)

MEASURE FUNCTION 1 FUNCTION 2

Correct Detections

Latency

1.02223

-0.40537

.15216

.95068

is ignored each coefficient represents the relative contribution of its

Page 68: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

58

associated variable to that function. Thus, in Table 11 it can be seen

thac correct detections is about 2H times as important as latency 1n

the first function. Latency makes the greatest contribution 1n the

second functlon but it must be remembered that this function has n'ttle

discriminatinq power.

Given a n of the above, how good is the resuUant discriminant

function at classifying children into the appropriate categories?

Table 12 summarizes the classifications resulting from the use of Rao's

V stepwise selection t iethod.

Table 12

Classification Results Using

Rao's V Stepwise Method

NO. OF ACTUAL GROUP CASES

Hyperactive 27

Learning Disabled 27

Normal 46

PREDICTED

HYPERACTIVE

1 3.7%

2 7.4%

0 0.0%

GROUP MEMBE^SHIP LEARNING DISABLED

8 19.6%

12 44.4%

4 8.7%

NORMAL

18 16.7%

13 48.1%

42 91.3% •

Percent of "Grouped" Cases Correctly Classified: 55.00%

Table 12 illustrates the ineffectiveness of the function at cor-

rectly classifying children on the basis of Vigilance Task and MFFT

scores. Only 1 (3.7%) true hyperactive child was correctly placed in

the hyperactive group; the majority were identified as norman A greater

proportion of learning disabled (44.4%) and normal (91.3%) were

Page 69: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

59

correctly placed. The high latter figure results, in part, froin the

fact th^t 73% of the total sample was classified normal. It ^ppears

from these data that scores, in general, tended to be more "normal"

than deviant. This discriminant analysis resuUed in only 55% of the

total sample being correctly classified. The tests seem to quite ac-

curately classify non-hyperactive children as norman, they also, how-

ever, tend to misclassify hyperactive and learning disabled children

as normal.

This (Rao^s V) stepwise procedure utin'zed only those variables %•

which maximize total group separation. When fun discriminant analysis

procedures were applied (i.e,, utilizing all of the discriminant vari-

ables) the resuUs were virtuany identical (see Table 13). The

Table 13

Classification ResuUs Using

FuTl Discriminant Analysis Method

NO. OF ACTUAL GROUP CASES

Hyperactive 27

Learning Disabled 27

Normal 46

PREDICTED

HYPERACTIVE

3 11.1%

1 3.7?:

1 2.2%

GROUP MEMBERSHIP

LEARNING DISABLED

8 19.6%

12 44.4%

4 8.7%

NORMAL

16 59.3%

14 48.1%

41 89.1%

Percent of "Grouped" Cases Correctly Classified: 56.

standardized discriminant function coefficients (see Table 14) demon-

strate that correct detections and latency are. again, the most heavily

Page 70: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Tablea4

Standardized Canonical Discriminant

Function Coefficients CFun)

60

MEASURE FUNCTION 1 FUNCTION 2

Correct Detections

False Alarms

Errors

Latehqy

.91389

.19761

.12060

.44817

-.01443

-.68821

.51208

.76755

weighted factors, with false alarms and errors carrying very n'ttle

weight, -Other stepwise procedures (WHk's Lambda, Mahalanobis squared

distance between closest groups, minimizing residual variance, maxi-

mizing the smallest F ratio between pairs of groups) an yielded re-

suUs identical to those noted in Table 12.

It must be noted that as with any inferential technique based on

sample data the percent correct prediction tends to overestimate the

power of the classification procedure. This is because the validation

is based on the same cases used to derive the classification functions.

The equation utilizes idiosyncratic sampling error to create classifi-

cation functions which are more accurate for that particular sample than

they would be for the full population. As a resuU, the figures shown

in Tables 13 and 14 are somewhat inflated-and the discriminant functions'

classification power is even less than that impn'ed by the tables.

To summarize the findings for the first hypothesis; scores on the

Vigilance Task and the Matching Faminar Figures Test do not yield

Page 71: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

61

dlscrimlnant functions which accurately and consistently differentiate

hyperactive from non-^hyperactive children, The correct detections mea-

sure on the Vlgilance Task (and latency on the MFFT, but to ^ much les-

ser degree) show promise as having some discriminating abin'ties.

Hypothesis 2

The second hypothesis is stated in null form and proposes that

neither the Vigilance Task nor the Matching Familiar Figures Test

alone can accurately and consistently differentiate hyperactive from

non-hyperactive children. This hypothesis has been supported in these

findings, but by defauU.

Since a combination of the best discriminating scores from the

two tests^ does not yield accurate classification, nor do such classifi-

cations resuU when all scores combined are used in the discriminant

function, it follows that neither test alone can discriminate accurately.

The Vigilance Task is the better of the two, specificany due to the

apparent discriminating abinty of its correct detections score,. but

it does not approach the level of accuracy needed in such instruments.

The latency score on the MFFT also has some discriminating abin'ty,

but again, not enough to resuU in consistently accurate classifications.

Hypothesis 3

Hypothesis 3 deals with sex and age differences with regard to

the discriminant functions ability to classify hyperactive, learning

disabled, and normal children accurately. Separate discriminant ana-

lyses were performed on subsamples of the subjects, namely males, fe-

males, young, and old. If the discriminant analyses had yielded marked

Page 72: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

62

improvements in classificgtipn accuracy this would have been reflected

in the percent of grouped cases accurately classified by the appn'cation

of the discriminant function. Since in the fun analysis the inclu'sion

of a n variables was slightly more discriminating than the stepwise

procedures, it is this former procedure which has been used to investi-

gate this hypothesis. Table 15 shows the percent of grouped cases cor-

rectly classified for the various subsamples.

Table 15

Results of Classification

For Various SubsampTes

PERCENT OF GROUPED CASES SUBSAMPLE CORRECTLY CLASSIFIED

Females 58.33%

Males 61.54%

Young 58.82%

01d 61.22%

It can be seen that the functions' discriminating abilities im-

proved slightly over the full sample in each of the four subsamples,

None of these differences, however, were substantial. There were no

subsamples which yielded discriminant functions that approached the

level of desirable discriminabin'ty for the purposes being investigated

here, that is, identifying hyperactive children from non-hyperactive

children,

Hypotheses 4 and 5

Hypotheses 4 and 5 propose that hyperactive chi ldren w i n obtain

Page 73: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

63

measurably different scores on the Vigilance Task and the Meitching

Famin'air Figures Test from the rest of the sample, and that the scores

of normal subjects win be different as well. Table 16 contains the

means and standard deviations of each measure for the three groups of

subjects. It can be seen that hyperactive and learning disabled child-

ren tended to obtain very similar scores on all measures, while normals

tended to differ from the other two groups, particularly on correct

detections,

Table 16

Means and Standard Deviations for Scores on the Vigilance Task

And MFFT for Hyperactive, Learning Disabled, and Normal Groups

GROUPS MEASURE - HYPERACTIVE LNG. DISABLED NORMAL TOTAL

Correct Detections

Mean

S. D.

False Alarms

Mean

S, D.

Errors

Mean

S, D.

Latency

Mean

S. D.

45.74

14.09

8.63

16.83

13.44

9.99

11,38

6.45

44,33

12.55

8.33

13.64

13,85

7.81

\

12,20

6.40

54.54 44.41

6.91 11.75

3.26 6.08

8.24 12.69

11.59 12.70

5,50 6.61

11,02 11.44

5.40 5.93

Page 74: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

64

An analysis of variance (see Table 17) shows that the groups are

significantly different with respect to their scores on correct detec-

tions, They are not, however, different with respect to the three

other scores (see Tables 18, 19, and 20).

Table 17

One-Way ANOVA ResuUs from

Three Groups on Correct Detections

SOURCE

Between Groups

Within Groups

^ SUM^OF SO.

2271.59

.11406.60

DF MEAN SO,

97

1135.80 9.7 .0001

117.59

R Eta

.3441

.4075 R^ = .1184

Eta Sq. = .1661

Table 18

One-Way ANOVA Results from

Three Groups on False Alarms

SOURCE

Between Groups

Within Groups

R = Eta =

SUM OF SO.

678,19

15259.17

-.1888 .2063

DF

2

,97

R2 = Eta Sq, =

MEAN SO.

339,01

157.31

.0356

.0426

F

2.16

P

.1214

Page 75: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

65

Table.l9

One-^Way ANOVA ResuUs from

Three Groups on Latency

SOURCE

Between Groups

Within Groups

R = Eta =

^ SUM OF SO.

23.51

3460.32

-.0354 .0821

Table 20

DF

2

97

Eta

MEAN SQ.

11.76

35.67

R^ = .0013 Sq. = .0067

F

.33

/

P

.7201

OnervWay ANOVA ResuUs from

Three Groups on Errors

SOURCE

Between Groups

Within Groups

R = Eta =

^ SUM OF SO.

107.77

4221.23

-.1301 .1578

DF'

2

97

Eta Sq. =

MEAN SQ.

53.89

43.52

.0169 ,0249

Ll_

1.24

P

.294

A group of ;t-tests comparing scores of each group with the other

(see Table 21) confirms the assertion that normals obtain significantly

different correct detection scores from both hyperactive and learning

disabled children, Thus, hypotheses 4 and 5 can only be accepted in

part, Hyperactive children do obtain quantitatively different scores

from normals (but not different from learning disabled children) on the

correct detections measure (but not on the other measures), Normal

Page 76: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Table 21

" tr^Values and Probahin'ties for Scores

On the Vigilance Task and the MFFT

66

- , .

GROUPS COMPARED MEASURE

Hyper/LD

Correct Detections

False Alarms

Errors

Latency

Hyper/Normal

Correct Detections

False Alarms

Errors

Latency

LD/Normal

Correct Detections

False Alarms

Errors

Latency

;t-VALUE

.39

.07

-.20

-.47

-=3704

1.55

1.26

.25

-3.89

1.75

1.45-

.84

33

33

33

37

DF

52

52

52

52

.47

.44

71

71

.42

,34

71

71

2-TAILED PROB.

.700

.944

.841

.642

.005

.130

.213

.802

.001

.088

.152

.406

children do obtain quantitatively different scores from both hyperactives

and learning disabled children on correct detections, but not on the

other measures. With respect to these instruments hyperactive and learn-

ing disabled children. as a group, seem to be different (in terms of cor-

rect detection scores) than normals, but they apparently are not dif-

ferent from each other.

Page 77: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

67

Hypothesis 6

If the scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Famin'ar

Figures Test reflect certain characteristics of cognitive experience

(e.g,, attention, impulsivity) one would expect that the respective

scores would be related to each other in appropriate directions. For

example, if false alarms on the Vigilance Task increase along with im-

pulsivity, that score would be expected to correlate negatively with

latency on the MFFT and correct detections on the Vigilance Task

(which both should increase with less impulsive subjects) and positively

with errors on the MFFT (which should increase with more impulsive sub-

jects), In short, as hyperactivity increases correct detections

should go down, false alarms should go up, latency should go up (be-

cause non-impulsive, non-hyperactive children will take their time to

insure a correct answer) and errors should go up. These expected inter-

correlation directions are summarized in Table 22. The actual inter-

correlations yielded by the data are presented in Table 23,

Table 22

Expected Intercorrelation Directions of

Vigilance Task and MFFT Scores

CORRECT FALSE ERRORS LATENCY DETECTIONS ALARMS

Correct Detections + - - +

False Alarms + +

Errors +

Latency +

Page 78: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

68

Table 23

Intercorrelations and Probabin'ties Between

Scores on the Vigilance Task and the MFFT

Correct Detections

False Alarms

Errors

*p < .001 **p .05

CORRECT DETECTIONS

FALSE ALARMS

-.3328*

ERRORS

-.3394*

.3695*

LATENCY

.2015**

-.1612

.3153*

A n of the correlations are in the expected directions, with four out

of the six being significant at least to the .001 leven

The above data thus supports hypothesis 6 (at least in part).

The scores on the tests are moderately correlated in the expected direc-

tions, lending support to the notion that they are measuring similar,

or at least coexisting characteristics,

Hypothesis 7

This hypothesis was included as a resuU of the n'mited availa-

bin'ty of renabinty data on these two instruments. While test-retest

figures would have been more helpful, that kind of data was not obtained

due to the unavailabin'ty of an adequate number of subjects who could be

retested.

Pearson r correlation coefficients were computed for the split-

half resuUs of each test. In the case of the Vigilance Task, scores

during odd and even five-minute intervals were conected separately.

For the MFFT the items were divided evenly into odd and even numbered

items.

Page 79: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

69

While correct detections on the Vigilance Task and latency on the

MFFT yielded reasonably high Pearson £'s (.8297 and .9262 respectively),

false aUrms (.7283) and particularly errors (.5447) fen short in terms

of spn't-haU reliabin'ties. It should be noted again that correct de-

tections and latency were found also to be the most powerful discrimi-

nators among the scores as wen. It is intuitively and statistically

reasonable that the less consistent measures would not be the most power-

ful discriminators.

Hypothesis 7, then, can only be supported in part. One score on

each test (correct detections on the Vigilance Task, latency on the MFFT)

demonstrates adequate splitr^half ren'abin'ties.

Hypothests 8

Hypothesis 8 is essentially concerned with two issues; (a) whether

or not parents and teachers report the same kinds of behaviors for the

same children, and (b) whether the reports of these behaviors are re-

lated to the scores that woúld be expected to resuU on the Vigilance

Task and the MFFT. The first issue is crucial to the entire study,

Teacher and parent reports were used to originany classify the sub-

jects into their respective groups. Concensus on the reports was re-

quired for the child to be placed in a group, That is, a n reporters

had to agree that the child exhibited the same kinds of behavior, If

these raters did not agree than the original classification scheme (at

least for the hyperactive group) would be suspect.

The intercorrelation matrix found in Table 24 demonstrates that

there was a considerable amount of agreement between all the raters on

all of the rating scales. Intercorrelation coefficients range from

Page 80: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Id

CsJ

cu

fO

to cn E

+-> fO

Dc:

c (U S-fT3

û -

•C3 c ro

s_

j r : o

cu

S-o

t f -

s-

«3

c p

+J fO

'ãj s_ s-o o s_ cu

co

<u S-o o

oo

CM

(U S-o o

(>0

• ) «

UD r o r H 0 0

• 0 0 CJ O cy>

* r^ 1—1

r o ( D

•K r o C>J 0 0 0 0

(U s-o o

oo

• ( D r o 0 0 CD

•K r>. r^ r^ UD

r^

c o cu s-o o

oo

o

•X OO co r«>. r^

• cr> r»> cri 00

• vo r^ LD

•K CM 0 0 «—) 0 0

•íc O

VD r^

0 0 LO

CU s-o o

LT)

* C\J 0 0 cri 0 0

• r>* CJ co 0 0

• » <

0 0 i n r>^ ( D

CvJ CM

s- s- s_ s-di <D (U (U S-

JZ .a JC ^ (U O O O O JZ fO fO '—^ fO fO •<-> (U (U S- (U (U o

f— h - (U I— I— ^

«4- i+_ 4-> M- M- q -o o o o o o (U (U S- S-

o o o o

<>o t>o

cn (/) S- S-(U (U C E E C O O o o

1 1 1 1

r-H C\J

QJ (U

s_ s-o o o o

(U s-

(U s-o o

O V) o

oo

s o . ^< : : 3

rxj

1

(U s-o o

1 — 1

1 — 1

1 — (

2 : LD Q

1 1

t—1

(U S-o o

(U S-o o

oo 1—(

»—í

1—t

5^ oo Q

1

CM

(U s-o o

(U s-o o

oo 1—1

1—1

1—1

2 1 oo Q

1 1

<>0

(U S-o o

OO OO t / ) (/) t/î t /)

O O rvl Q Q Q

cu S-o o

t>0

o

CVJ

CU S-o o

t/O

o

CU S-o o

co

r^

s-o o

t /)

co (U S-o o

oo

o o

Page 81: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

71

,6317 to .9028. Not only were teachers and parents agreeing that hyper-

actives exhibited hyperactive behavior patterns, they also agreed that

non-hyperactives did not exhibit hyperactive behavior patterns. As

might be expected the lower correlations were generany those comparing

mothers' responses (Z Score, D Score 3) with those of teachers (C Score

1, C Score 2, D Score 1, D Score 2). Teachers tended to agree quite

markedly, A n correlations were significant beyond the .001 leveK

The second issue raised in hypothesis 8 involved whether the reports

of teachers and parents are related to the scores that would be expected

to resuU in the Vigilance Task and the MFFT. For example, as teacher

and parent rating scores go up (indicating more symptoms of hyperactivity)

one might expect correct detections to go down (hence, a negative corre-

lation). The expected directions of correlation are summarized in Table

25. Actual correlation figures can be found in Table 26.

Table 25

Expected Correlation Directions for

Test Measures and Teacher and Parent Ratings

CORRECT FALSE DETECTIONS ALARMS ERRORS LATENCY

C Score 1 - + + .

C Score 2 - + + -

Z Score - + + -

D Score 1 - + + -

D Score 2 - + + -

D Score 3 - ^ + -

Page 82: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Table 26

Obtained Correlation Coefficients for

Test Measure and Teacher and Parent Ratings

72

C Score 1

C Score 2

Z Score

D Score 1

D Score 2

D Score 3

CORRECT DETECTIONS

-.3572*

-.2947*

-.3255*

-.2967*

-.2704*

-.2375**

FALSE ALARMS

.2232**

.1714

.2526

.2246**

.2621*

.1905

ERRORS

.1290

.1712

.1643

.2010**

.2332**

.1396**

LATENCY

-.0235

.0010

-.0930

.0630

.0563

-.0895

*P < .01'

**p< .05

The correlations were generany in the expected directions, with

scores for correct detections being significantly correlated with tea-

cher and parent ratings. Again, thisis to be expected given the rela-

tive success of the correct detections score in identifying hyperactive

children.

It can be concluded that the data strongly supports the first is-

sue of the hypothesis, teachers and mothers do agree on their ratings of

the children in this study. Teachers and mothers are evidently seeing

and reporting the same kinds of behavior, behavior which is consistent

with a diagnosis of hyperactivity. The second issue in hypothesis 8

was also confirmed, aUhough not so definitively. Teacher and parent

ratings do tend to be related to scores on the Vigilance Task and the

MFFT in the expected directions. The correct detections score, in

Page 83: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

73

particular, was significantly correlated with the ratings,

^ Hypothes^is 9

Hypothesis 9 deals with the relationship between scores on the

Vigilance Task and the MFFT and the subject's intein'gence. The Slosson

Intein'gence Test was administered to each subject along with the re-

search instruments, Descriptive data on subjects' lOs can be found in

Table 27.

Table 27

Descriptive Data Concerning Subjects' IQ

STATISTIC ALL SUBJECTS HYPERS. L.D.s NORMALS

Mean

Standard Deviation

103.940

14.955

101.111

15.126

97.296 109.500

11.296 14.941

It can be seen in Figure 1 that the IQ scores of this sample are

characterized by a reasonably normal distribution, aUhough sn'ghtly

skewed toward higher IQs, This skewness is largely due to the higher

than average IQs seen in the normals (a group which was made up of

students at a private elementary school),

Table 28 gives ;t-test data related to the differences in IQ

scores between the three groups, It can be seen that normal subjects

obtained significantly higher IQ scores than did learning disabled

children (t = ^3.67, p = .001), while the difference in IQs between

hyperactives and normals approached significance (;t = -2.31, p = ,024).

Page 84: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

74

30 -

S

U

B

J

E

C

T

S

25

20 -

15 -

10 -

5 -

70-79 80-89 90-99 100-109 110-119 120-129 130+

IQ

Figure lir Distribution ofjTQ.Scores

The relationship between IQ scores and performance on the Vigi-

lance Task and the MFFT is summarized in Table 29. Intein'gence scores

were correlated significantly with correct detections and false alarms

for the fun sample. More intenigent subjects tended to have more

correct detections and fewer false alarms than less intenigent

Page 85: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

Table 28

t -Tests Comparing IQ and Groups

75

GROUPS t-VALUE DF 2-TAILED PROB.

Hyper/LD

Hyper/Normal

Normal/LD

1.05

-2.31

-3.67

52

71

71

.299

.024

.001

Table 29

Correlation Between IQ and

Scores on the Vigilance Task and the MFFT

MEASURE

Correct Detections

False Alarms

Errors

Latency

ALL SUBJECTS

.28*

.33*

.14

.06

HYPERS.

.13

-.45**

_ 44**

-.00

L.D.s

.00

-.09

.16

-.12

NORMALS

.35*

-.28**

.00

-,02

* p ^ .01

**p< .05

subjects. This was particularly true with correct detections for the

normal group.

In response to hypothesis 9; there is some relationship between

subjects' Slosson 10 scores and performance on the Vigilance Task, par-

ticularly with normal subjects on correct detections. There are no sig

nificant relationships between subjects' IQ scores and either of the

Page 86: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

76

scores on the MFFT. No relationships were found between hyperactives,*

scores on the tests and IQ scores.

Page 87: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

CHAPTER IV

^ DISCUSSION

The primary purpose of this study was to investigate the effec-

tiveness of the Vigilance Task and the Matching Faminar Figures Test

for differentiating hyperactive children from non-hyperactive children.

Ostensibly these two instruments provide measures of attention and im-

pulsivity, two of the three major comoonents of Attention Deficit Dis-

order with hyperactivity. Previous research has shown the Vigilance

Task and the MFFT to be the most promising of the available objective

indices for tapping these characteristics in children. The objective

and accurate assessment of hyperactivity clearly becomes important when

diagnostic and treatment issues, as well as educational planning strate-

gies, are considered.

Nine hypotheses were tested in this study. The first two hypothe-

ses deaU with the two tests' abin'ty, when combined or taken alone, to

accurately and consistently differentiate hyperactive from non-hyperac-

tive (learning disabled and normal) children. The third hypothesis

investigated sex and/or age differences with regard to the discrimi-

nant functions' abin'ty to classify hyperactive, learning disabled, and

normal children. Hypotheses 4 and 5 involved whether or not hyperac-

tives and normals obtain measurably different scores on the instruments

than do the other groups, i.e., do hyperactives score differently on any

single measure than do normals and learninq disabled children, and do

normals score differently than hyperactives and learning disabled

children. Hypothesis 6 deaU with the question of whether the two

tests seem to be measuring the same and/or coexisting behavioral

77

Page 88: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

78

phenomena. SpnU-haU ren'abilities were assessed in hypothesis 7,

The eighth hypothesis assessed whether or not the initial raters of the

children Cteachers and mothers) were agreeing as to the types of be-

havior they reported, Finany, hypothesis 9 looked at any relationships

which existed between the subjects' IQ and performances on the Vigi-

lance Task and the MFFT, The findings for each of these hypotheses

are discussed below.

Hypothesis 1

The hypothesis that scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching

Familiar Figures Test can yield discriminant functions which accurately

and consistently differentiate hyperactive from non-rhyperactive children

was not supported, The most discriminating of the discriminant func-

tions yielded in this study (i.e., the function which took into account

a n four variables) accurately classified only 56% of the original

sample. It must be remembered that even this relatively low figure is

statistically inflated. The tests did most poorly when classifying

hyperactive children. Only one of the 27 hyperactive subjects was cor-

rectly placed in the hyperactive group.

Stepwise selection of variables revealed that correct detections

on the Vigilance Task was the most discriminating variable. Latency on

the MFFT improved discriminabin'ty by a small amount, The contribution

of the false alarm score on the Vigilance Task and the errors score on

the MFFT was negligible.

This data may support recent findings (Douglas, 1972; Dykman et

a n , 1971) which reveal an attentional deficit component as primary in

hyperactive children. IntuitiveTy it is reasonable to propose that

Page 89: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

79

attentional deficits are reflected most in the correct detections mea-

sure of the Vigilance Task. Correct detections presumably drop as

children become less attentive to the stimun'. It may also be true-

however, that motoricany restless children (a characteri$tic of ADD

which was not assessed in this study) receive lowered correct detec-

tion scores because they are too busy being active to attend. It is

difficuU to separate out the concomitant effects of attention deficit

and restlessness. While the latency score on the MFFT (which also had

some discriminating abin'ties) obviously reflects some degree of impul-

sivity, it may also be a measure of attention. It is conceivable that

children with attentional deficits choose items on the MFFT yery quickly

simply because they are unable to attend long enough to peruse the items

more carefuny,

The impulsivity component of ADD is another matter. The data in

this study indicate that either these instruments are unsuccessful in

tapping the presence of impulsiveness in hyperactive children, or the

impulsiveness that is measured by these instruments is not a major com-

ponent of the hyperactivity seen by these teachers and mothers. It is

assumed that hyperactive children will generate significantly more false

alarms on the Vigilance Task than will non-hyperactive children, and

that the former group win make more errors on the MFFT as a result of

their rapid responding. Neither of these phenomenon was observed. It

is \/ery possible that the Vigilance Task and the MFFT simply do not pro-

vide van'd measures of relative impulsivity in children, The impulsiv-

ity probably exists in hyperactives. Teachers and mothers certainly

report that those children identified as hyperactive demonstrate more

Page 90: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

80

Impulsive behaviors than do those identified as non-hyperactive, If

the impulsivity exists these instruments are apparently not measuring it

(unless elements of impulsivity are tapped in the correct detections

measure),

The four measures used in this study account for about 20% of the

variance in the discriminant function explained by the groups. This

is determined by squaring the canonical correlation (.45) yielded by

the most successful discriminant function. At least one of the scores

does seem to reveal (at least intuitively) some attention deficit com-

ponent as a primary factor in the differences between these groups. It

is not at a n clear, however, that this difference is confined to the

hyperactive group apart from the non-hyperactive group, In fact, normal

subjects were found to obtain significantly different scores on correct

detections than both hyperactive and learning disabled children, The

scores for hyperactives and learning disabled children were not signi-

ficantly different from each other, Hence, even correct detections

failed to discriminate hetween hyperactives and the learning disabled,

aUhough it did discriminate normal from non-normal children to some

degree. The usefulness of this \fery gross discriminating ability is

questionable.

- In short, this discriminant analysis provided no evidence that a

combination of the Vigilance Task and the MFFT scores can serve to ac-

curately classify children into hyperactive and non-hyperactive groups.

There is evidence that the correct detections measure of the Vigilance

Task does have some discriminating abin'ty in terms of the larger group

of hyperactive and learning disabled children combined. It is presumed

Page 91: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

81

that this score reflects the presence of some attentional deficit

which meiy he common to hyperactive and learning disabled children in

general.

Hypothesis 2

Hypothesis 2 was confirmed; neither the Vigilance Task nor the

MFFT alone was successful in accurately and consistently differenti-

ating hyperactive from non-hyperactive children. It should be noted,

however, that the Vigilance Task alone would almost do as wen as a

combination of the tests.

The Vigilance Task does seem to have promise in playing a part

in the development of instruments used to diagnose hyperactivity. It

does have some degree of abin'ty to discriminate normals from non-

normals. Kagan's MFFT, however, showed n'ttle evidence of possessing

any discriminating power. Kagan proposed that impulsive children

(hyperactives are, by definition, impulsive) win tend to score lower

on Tatency and higher on errors than win non-impulsive children.

This was not found to be the case in this study. No significant dif-

ferences were found between hyperactives, normals, and learning dis-

abled children on either of the MFFT scores. In defense of Kagan, it

may well he the case that the non-hyperactive subjects were somewhat

impulsive, thus masking the differences between the groups. Also,

Kagan suggests dichotomizing subjects based on the scores, with impul-

sives being identified as those children who respond more quickly and

with more errors than the norm. When this was done with the current

sample 27 "impul sives^' were identified. Of this group seven were

rated as hyperactive, five were learning disabled, and 15 were in the

Page 92: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

82

normal group. Hence, even when used gs suggested by its author the

instrument has poor discriminating abiU'ties.

Given the complexity and înuUipn'city of symptôms it is unU.kely

that any one or two instruments will successfully tap enough components

of ADD to enable accurate classification. Emphasis on attentional com-

ponents of the disorder does seem to be in order.

Hypothesis 3

Hypothesis 3 was accepted. There was no evidence of any sex or

age differences with regard to the discriminant function's abin'ty to

classify hyperactive, learning disabled, and normal children accurately.

It classified all sub-^^samples poorly. While the numbers of correctly

classified cases did go up somewhat when the samples were made more

homogenous with respect to age and sex, the accuracy of classification

was stin around 60%. Most of the cases correctly classified were in

the normal group. The rise in correct classifications could simply be

a reflection of the reduced sample size in each sub-sample.

The most accurate classification was seen in the males only group.

Since the incidence of hyperactivity in the population is generany

higher in males it is possible that this group consisted of proportion-

ally more "true" hyperactives as opposed to the "described" hyperac-

tives, hence, a rise in the function's ability to discriminate. It

could also be true, however, that males as a group typicany score in

the "hyperactive" directions on these tests more so than females.

Whatever the case, the discriminant function did not work signi-

ficantly better when appn'ed to only a particular sex or age group.

There may be some sex and/or age difference in the scores on these two

Page 93: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

83

instrumentsj these differences, however, are not reflected by signifi-

cant increases in the discrimination power of the functions.

Hypotheses 4 and 5

Hypotheses 4 and 5 could be accepted, but only 1n part. Normals

did seem to score differently than hyperactive and learning disabled

children, but only with respect to the correct detections measure.

Scores on each of the other measures were not significantly different

from any of the other groups.

It is assumed throughout this discussion that the correct detec-

tions measure primarily te ps some attentional component of behavior.

If this is the case normal children have significantly better attending

abilities .than do hyperactive and learning disabled children. It would

also follow that the attending skins of hyperactive and learning dis-

abled children are not significantly distinct from each other.

AUernatively, the correct detections measure could be tapping

some cognitive and/or perceptual phenomenon, apart from attentional

deficits, that is common to hyperactive and learning disabled children,

but not present in normal children. For example, the response time

of normals in a vigilance paradigm may be significantly better than that

of the other two groups. Correct detections would increase if a sub-

ject required more than two seconds, in this case, to respond. In

effect he/she would then be responding to the wrong stimulus. Hyperac-

tive and learning di$abled children mgy be, as a group, more restless

than normals and hence have a greater chance of "missing" correct re-

sponses. Only further investigation will rule out these and other

possibin'ties,

Page 94: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

84

In nght of all of the other findings the resolution of these

hypotheses is not surprising, If the performances of the various

groups had been more different from each other one would expect that

the power of the discriminant function would have been greater. Since

the discriminant function demonstrated limited discriminating ability,

with the correct detections measure providing most of the discrimi-

nation, it follows that the performance on the other measures would have

evidenced little variabin'ty between groups. This, indeed, was the

case. Future researchers would probably be wise to not consider false

alarms in a vigilance paradigm, and errors on the MFFT as potential

discriminating variables for classifying hyperactive children.

Hypothesis 6

Hypothesis 6 deals primarily with the issue of concurrent van'dity

of the instruments. If it is assumed that the Vigilance Task and the

MFFT are measuring similar or coexisting characteristics one would ex-

pect their scores to yield correlations in the appropriate directions.

The stronger the relationship the more n'kelihood there is that the

measures are tapping the same or coexistent phenomenon.

This hypothesis was supported in part. A n corre.lations were in

the appropriate directions and four out of the six were significant at

the .01 level (see Table 17). The degree of correlation, however, was

modest, ranging (in absolute terms) from .1612 (p = .055) to .3695

Cp = .001).

The measures do seem to be tapping some of the same phenomenon.

Subjects who score in one way on one of the measures do tend to score in

the "correct" theoretical directions on the other measures. For example.

Page 95: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

85

if a sutgect is relatively high in correct detections, he/she will

nkely be low on false alarms and errors, and high on latency,

The highest correlation was found between false alarms on the

VigHance Task and errors on the MFFT. If a subject is golng to give

a "wrong" response chances are it will be given on both tests. Cor-

rect detections and false alarms were also significantly related (in

the negative directions). This would not have been the case if most

of the subjects had been either over-responders or under-responders.

For over-responders (those who push the button excessively and indi-

scriminantly) false alarms and correct detections would have risen coh-

currently, they would have dropped concurrently for under-responders,

Subjects_in this study viere apparently somewhat selective in their re-

sponse strategies, While this data was not available in the present

study, it would be interesting to investigate whether or not there are

differences between over- and under-responders on the Vigilance Task.

Correct detections yielded a significant negative correlation

with errors on the MFFT (-.3394, p = .001). It is intuitively reason-

able that this may reflect either some attentional or intenigence fac-

tor. Attentive and/or intenigent children would n'kely do better on

both correct detection and error rates. The correlation of Slosson IQ

with correct detections was, in fact, ,2808, which is significant at

the .002 level. 'Errors on the MFFT and IQ correlated -.1412 (p ^ .081),

a figure which approaches significance,

Finany, errors and latency on the MFFT were significantly corre-

lated (-.3153, p = .001). This relationship. of course, is to be ex-

pected, if only from a theoretical perspective. Children who take more

Page 96: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

86

time to inspect the stimulus are nke ly to make fewer errors.

Hypothesis 7

Hypothesis 7 deals with spn't-haU ren'ability data for the two

instruments. The vandity of psychometric instruments is, of course,

n'mtted by their degree of reliabinUy, While test-retest and/or aUer-

nate form data would have been more instructive the means for obtaining

such data were not available. Scores were recorded for each measure,

however, in spn't-haU forms, yielding a rough measure of each instru-

ment's internal consistency.

Correlation coefficients actually give the reliability of only a

haU-test, Other things being equal, the longer a test, the more re-

liable it'Win be, The éffect that lengthening or shortening a test

will have on its correlation coefficient can be estimated by means of

the Spearman-Brown formula (Anastasi, 1976). When this is appn'ed to

split-haU correlation coefficients of the measures of interest it re-

suUs in ren'ability figures of ,91 for split-halves of the Vigilance

Task, and .84 for spn't halves of the MFFT.

Thus it can be said that the two instruments demonstrated adequate

split-haU reU'abin'ty figures, particularly in the case of the Vigi-

lance Task. This is statisticany reasonable because the Vigilance

Task encompasses considerably more discrete items than does the MFFT.

Longer forms of the MFFT would certainly prove to be more ren'able.

Hypothesis 8

Both elements of hypothesis 8, dean'ng with the teacher and parent

ratings, were confirmed. These ratings were used to initiany classify

Page 97: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

87

children into the hyperactive and normal groups, and also served, in

some cases, to exclude children from particular groups. Four of the

five ratings must have met the criteria for hyperactivity in order for

a particular subject to be included in the hyperactive group. If more

than two ratings (but less than five) reported hyperactive behavior the

child was not included in the study at a n . Learning disabled children

(as defined by Texas Education Agency standards) who were also reported

to be hyperactive (and satisfied the criteria for hyperactivity)

were placed in the hyperactive group. Normal children could have no

more than one "hyperactive" rating. Stringent criteria such as these

were used in order to insure that only hyperactive children were in-

cluded in the hyperactive group and that no hyperactive children were

included in the other groups,

In light of the above it is important that there be a large de-

gree of inter-rater ren'ability present in the ratings. Table 24 re-

ports these figures.. As might be expected teachers tended to agree more

with themselves and with other teachers than they did with mothers in

terms of their ratings. A n of the correlation coefficients, however,

were significant beyond the .001 level, ranging from a low of ,6317 to

a high of .9028.

Parents and teachers were apparently seeing and reporting the

same types of behavior (or absence of behavior) in the same children.

When one teacher rated a child as hyperactive, so did other teachers and

the child's mother (by definition in the current study), A n tended to

be in agreement when no hyperactivity was noted. While a n children

in the hyperactive group met the minimum (though stin stringent)

Page 98: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

88

criteria for being classified hyperactive, some of the variance found

in the ratings is a resuU of raters endorsing more items than were

required to meet criterion levels. Hence, if anything, the inter-

rater reU'abinty coefficients reported are sn'ghtly deceiving on the

conservative side. Teachers and parents were definitely agreeing

on the behaviors that they reported. It can thus be reasonably assumed

that these children identified in this study as being hyperactive do,

indeed, exhibit hyperactive symptoms both at home and at school.

The second element of this hypothesis involved the relationship

between the ratings and scores on the two research instruments. This

is essentiany another van'dity issue. If the tests do measure traits.

that are related to hyperactive behavior (.e.g., attentional deficits,

impulsivity) one would expect scores which measure these traits to be

related to ratings based on behavioral observations. This was found to

be the case with the correct detections measure but not with false

alarms, errors, or-latency. As correct detections increased mother and

teacher reports of hyperactive behaviors went down. Generany these

other scores were in the direction that would be expected but only

correct detection scores reached significance.

These data provide further evidence that the correct detections

measure is tapping some component of hyperactivity. This also lends

support to the data which indicates that the other measures do not

necessarily reflect hyperactive behavior patterns.

To summarize, mothers and teachers are seeing and reporting the

same types of behavior in these children, behavior which is consistent

with a hyperactive classification. These ratings are significantly

Page 99: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

89

correlated, at the ,01 leveU with correct detection scores on the Vigi-

lance Task but not with false alarms on the Vigilance Task and errors

on the MFFT,

Hypothesis 9

Hypothesis 9 states that there will be no relationship between

subjects' Slosson IQ scores and performance on the Vigilance Task and

the MFFT. This hypothesis could only be supported in part,

When a n subjects were taken into account there were significant

correlations between IQ and both measures on the Vigilance Task, IQ

and correct detections correlated .28 (p = .005).while IQ and false

alarms correlated -.33 (.p = .001). When the groups were considered

separately only IQ and correct detections correlated significantly

for normal subjects (r = .35, p = ,009). Neither of the measures on

the MFFT had a significant relationship with IQ.

Langsdorf (1980) found that intelligence did differentiate hyper-

active from non-hyperactive children. While significant differences

were not found in this study the trend was certainly in that direc-

tion (;t hyper/normal = -2.31, p = .024, df = 71). There was a signi-

ficant difference in intelligence between learning disabled and normal

children (;t normal/LD = -3,67, p = .001, df = 71). The use of a more

sophisticated measure of intelligence may contribute significantly to

the discriminating ability of the correct detections measure. The

unique contribution of IQ, however, may be small due to the relationship

between IQ and correct detection scores.

Page 100: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

90

Implications

It is apparent that the Vigilance Task and the MFFT scores do

not differentiate hyperactive from non^hyperactive children. The most

accurate discriminant function resulted in about 55% of the sample be-

ing correctly classified (and that figure itself is statistically in^

flated), More conventional diagnostic procedures (rating forms, be-

havioral observations, interviews) probably result in comparable, if

not better, hit rates. In short, a combination of these two instru-

ments does not provide the hoped for objective indicant of Attention

Deficit Disorder symptoms.

The measures do, however, (specifically correct detections) tend

to differentiate the larger group of learning disabled plus hyper-

active children from the "normal" group. This larger group may, in

fact, be identified as a generic learning disabled group composed of

children who have some specific developmental learning disorder (e,g,,

developmental reading disorder, developmental language disorder) and

children who could be diagnosed as having an Attention Deficit Disorder

with Hyperactivity, In this study members of the latter group could

also meet the criteria for inclusion in the former group were it not for

their hyperactivity. In fact, 20 subjects in the hyperactive group

had also been diagnosed learning disabled. Of the seven remaining four

were below the age of eigíit and may simply have yet to be identified as

learning disabled. While the numbers were too small to analyze in the

present study it is likely that a "hyoeractive only" group would have

been more distinct from the others.

The learning disabled and hyperactive children in this study are

Page 101: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

91

evidently similar with regard to the presence of an attentlon deficit

(assuming that correct detections taps attention deficits), The iíTipul-

sivlty dimension is less clear. It may be that "hyperactive only"

children would be more distinct with respect to impulsivity and motoric

restlessness. Children who are identified by parents and teachers as

displaying hyperactive behavior (at least in this group of subjects)

also meet the criteria for being identified as learning disabled.

This learning disability confound may be masking the differences be-

tween the groups. Future studies should make this distinction more

carefully,

The Vigilance Task, specifically its correct detections score, is

somewhat effective in differentiating the larger group of learning dis-

abled and hyperactive children from normals. This confirms the data

reported by Anderson and his colleagues (1973), Several different

sets of findings in this study suggest that correct detections does

have some validity in terms of its ability to tap some component of

Attention Deficit Disorder with Hyperactivity.

The MFFT was almost wholly unsucessful at discriminating between

hyperactive and non-hyperactive children, It added yery little to the

discriminating power of the Vigilance Task. From the data collected

in the present study one has trouble seeing any important relationships

between scores on the MFFT and the presence of hyperactive behavior

patterns.

It is probably overly optimistic to expect a small number of ob-

jective instruments to tap the multiplicity of behavioral, cognitive,

and emotional manifestations of Attention Deficit Disorder with

Page 102: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

92

Hyperactivity, Correct diagnoses should, nevertheless, be a high pri-

ority due to the implications of diagnostic errors. Clearly these two

instruments alone are not adequate. The Vigilance Task (or something

similar), however, shows promise of having some discriminating ability

and appears to have some validity with respect to measuring some of

the components of hyperactivity,

Directions for Future Research

This study has demonstrated that at least one objective measure

(correct detections on the Vigilance Task) has some significant rela-

tionship to the presence of hyperactive behavior patterns. The measure

presumably taps the attentional deficit component of ADD with hyperac-

tivity, Alternative vigilance paradigms and/or other measures aimed

at detecting differences in attending abilities may prove useful in

improving the discriminative ability noted in the Vigilance Task.

Future investigators should probably utilize some relatively sophisti-

cated measure of intellectual functioning as part of the test battery.

Some relationships were noted in the present study with regard to in-

telligence and correct detections.

The MFFT in its present form, has not proven to contribute signi-

ficantly to a discrimination of hyperactives from non^hyperactives.

It is possible that a more refined, longer version of the MFFT, or some

similar instrument, may prove useful in yielding data about the impul-

sivity dimension of ADD, False alarms on the Vigilance Task, also pre-

viously thought to be related to impulsivity, yielded disappointing

results as well.

The third dimension of ADD is hyperactivity or motoric restlessness.

Page 103: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

93

Several attempts have beeh made to operationalize measures of activity

with little success, These measures tend to be impractical in terms of

economics, degree of intrusiveness, reliability, etc, Nevertheless,

an agreed upon standard for measuring lîiotoric restlessness would be

very helpful in developing objective diagnostic procedures, A chair

fitted with mercury switches has been used with the Vigilance Task

in the past, providing an activity count measure along with correct

detections and false alarms. This particular measure, however, becomes

less reliable as activity increases; hyperactive children have a ten-

dency to get out of the chair. One possible alternative (in areas

where the equipment can be permanently installed) is to have the entire

floor wired for movement,

In conclusion, it is felt that the further refinements of mea-

suring devices used to tap the three components (inattention, impulsi-' '

vity, and hyperactivity) of ADD may lead to more reliable and objective

classifiers of hyperkinetic children, This is seen as a desirable

goal in light of the implications of faulty diagnoses, The present

study demonstrates that two such instruments currently available are

inadequate for this purpose when taken alone, or when combined by dis-

criminant analysis procedures, This study did indicate, however, that

one measure on the VigiUnce Task, correct detections, may prove useful

as part of a larger battery.

Limitations

The subject population of this study was obviously not a repre-

sentative sample of five- to eleven-year-olds. The "normal" grouo,

in particular, probably possessed characteristics which were quite

Page 104: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

94

different from what would b,e found in a randomly chosen sample, The

fact that they were students in a church^supported private school im-

plies that they would be distinct from public school children in several

ways, As a group their IQ scores were higher than the children in the

other two groups, This is not surprising since the other groups were

comprised of children who, by definition, have had difficulties in school,

It might have been helpful, though, to account for IQ differences in

the three groups, either by partialling out its effect or by including

the IQ measure as one of the discriminating variables. The latter ap-

proach would have been niost appropriate considering Langsdorf's (1980)

finding that there is a relationship between IQ and levels of hyperac-

tivity.

This very issue could be seen, however, as a plus for this study.

As it was, the "normal" group was more distinct from the hyperactive

group than would have been the case otherwise. That is, the normal

group constituted children who were certainly not hyperactive and not

learning disabled. These children are typically self-selected out of

private schools, If the tests had discriminating abilities they should

have certainly been able to discriminate between these two very dis-

tinct groups,

The hyperactive'and learning disabled groups were not totally

representative either, In an effort to avoid confounds resulting from

the increased attribution of hyperkinetic behavior to minority children,

this sample was limited to Caucasion elementary school students. It

would have been interesting to investigate racial and/or cultural dif-

ferences with respect to performance on these two instruments. Also

Page 105: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

95

excluded were students who were being medically treated for hyperac-

tivity, The inclusion of these children would have added an additonal

dimension and focus to the study,

The inclusion of some measure of activity level as a discrimi-

nating variable might have served to increase the power of the discrimi-

nant function. A stabilimetric cushion which was available was not used

due to the unreliability of its measurement, The addition, though, of

a valid measure of motoric restlessness might have contributed sub-

stantially to the classifying ability of the function.

Even though the Vigilance Task showed some promise in tapping

components of ADD it is doubtful that it could ever be used as part of

a standa^rd battery, The instrument, in its current design, is simply

too bulky, obtrusive, and cost-ineffective to be useful in most applied

settings, Its continued use as a research tool, however, is certainly

in order,

Page 106: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

CHAPTER V

SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS

This study investigated the feasibility of using scores on the

Vigilance Task and the Matching Familiar Figures Test in a discriminant

function in order to differentiate hyperactive from non-hyperactive

children, A review of the literature pointed to these two instruments

as being potentially the most useful in assessing the inattention and

impulsivity components of ADD with hyperactivity. There were no ade-

quate instruments extant to tap the hyperactivity dimension. It was

proposed that these two instruments might serve as a base for a battery

of tests aimed at providing objective and reliable diagnoses of hyper-

active children, The importance of accurate diagnoses was discussed in

light of the typical treatments (specifically pharmacological ones) and

their often deleterious side^ffects.

Nine hypotheses derived directly from the basic issue of the dis-

criminability of these tests. One hundred children, their teachers,

and their mothers participated in this study. The design involved two

stages (a) the assignment of each subject to the appropriate group.

i.e., normal , learning disabled, hyperactive, and (b) the assessment

stage at which time the predictor, or discriminating variables were

administered.

Children were assigned to their respective groups on the basis

of rating forms filled out by mothers and teachers. Rigid, multiple

criteria were utilized for inclusion in the hyperactive group. The

discriminating variables used were the correct detections and false

alarm scores on the Vigilance Task, and the latency and errors scores

96

Page 107: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

97

on the MFFT,

Data were analyzed using full and stepwise discriminant analyes.

The conclusions of this study were as follows:

1. Scores on the Vigilance Task and the Matching Familiar Figures

Test do not yield discriminant functions which accurately and

consistently differentiate hyperactive from non^hyperactive

children. Correct detections on the Vigilance Task was the

only measure demonstrating marked discriminating power.

2. Neither the Vigilance Task nor the MFFT alone can accurately

and consistently differentiate hyperactive from non-hyperactive

children,

3. There was some relationship between subjects' Slosson IQ and

• performance on the Vigilance Task (particularly correct de-

tections), There was no relationship between Slosson 10 and

MFFT performance.

4. Normal children obtained quantitatively different scores on

correct detections on the Vigilance Task than learning dis-

abled and hyperactive children. There were no significantly

different scores between the groups on the MFFT.

5. Scores' on the Vigilance Task and the MFFT were, in general,

significantly correlated (in the appropriate d.irections).

They are apparently measuring some similar and/or coexisting

phenomenon.

6. Adequate s p l i t - h a l f r e l i a b i l i t i e s were noted in both ins t ru -

ments, pa r t i cu la r l y the Vigilance Task.

7. Teacher and parent ratings were correlated highly with each

Page 108: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

98

other indicating that teachers and parents were reporting the

same behaviors in the same children, These ratings were also

somewhat correlated with scores on the Vigilance Task and the

MFFT in the expected directions,

8. There were no significant sex or age differences with regard

to the discriminant functions' ability to classify hyperac-

tive, learning disabled, and normal children accurately,

Page 109: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

REFERENCES

Adams, W, V, Strategy differences between reflective and impulsive

children. Child Development, 1972, 43 , 1076^-1080.

Adams, W. Lack of behavioral effects from Feingold diet violations,

Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1981, 52, 307-313.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of

mental disorders (3rd Ed,). Washington, D. C : American Psychia-

tric Association, 1980,

Anastasi, A, Psychological testing (4th Ed,). New York; MacMillan

Publishing Co,, Inc, 1976,

Anderson, R, P,, Halcomb, C , & Doyle, R, The measurement of atten-

tional deficits. Exceptional Child, 1973, 3^, 534-^538.

Anderson, R, P., Halcomb, C. G., Gordon Jr,, W.,'& Ozolins, D. A. Mea-

surement of attention distractibility in LD children. Academic

Therapy, 1974, ^, 251-266.

Appelbaum, A, S, Diagnostic considerations in the evaluation of hyper-

kinetic children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 1975, 3 , 24r-26,

Axelrod, S., & Bailey, S. L. Drug treatment for hyperactivity; Con-

troversies, alternatives, and guidelines, Exceptional Children,

1979, 45 , 544-550,

Bakan, P, (Ed.), Attention: An enduring problem in psychology, Prince-

ton; 'Van Norstrand, 1966,

Berler, E. S,, & Romanczyk, R, G, Assessment of the learning disabled

and hyperkinetic child: An analysis and critique, Journal of

Learninq Disabilities, 1980, l^, 10-12.

99

Page 110: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

100

Block, G, Hyperactivity; A cultural perspective, Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 1977, lO , 48-52.

Block, J,, Block, J, H., & Harrington, D, M, Some misgivings about the

Matching Familiar Figures Test as a measure of reflection-impûl-

sivity. Developmental Psychology, 1974, l^. 611.-632,

Blouin, A. G. A,, Bornstein, R. A., & Trites, R. L. Teenage alcohol

abuse among hyperactive children; A five-year follow-up study.

Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 1978, 8, 51-62,

Bolstad, 0, D., & Johnson, S, M, The relationship between teachers'

assessment of students and the students! actual behavior in the

classroom. Child Development, 1977, 48, 570-578.

Bowers, A'. J, Can we effectively assess hyperactivity in schools?

Child Care, Health, and Development, 1978, ^, 411-420.

Bradley, C The behavior of children receiving Benzedrine, American

Journal of Psychiatry, 1937, 33 , 669-675.

Brecky, J, The Feingold dietary treatment of the hyperkinetic syndrome,

Medical Journal of Australia, 1976, 2_, 580-590.

Brenner, J. A study of the efficacy of the Feingold diet on hyperkinetic

children,. Clinical Pediatrics, 1977, 16_, 652-656.

Campbell, S. B, Mother-ohild interaction in reflective, impulsive, and

hyperactive children, De'velopmental Psychology, 1973, 8, 341-349,

Campbell, S. B,, Douglas, V, I., & Morgenstern, G, Cognitive styles in

hyperactive children and the effect of methylphenidate, Journal

of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1971, 12 , 55-67,

Page 111: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

101

Campbell, S, B,, Schleifer, M,, & Weiss, G. Continuities in maternal

reports and child behaviors over time in hyperactive and compari-

son groups. Journal of AbnormaT Child Psychology, 1978, 6, 33-45,

Cantwell, D. P,, & Satterfield, J. H, The prevalence of academic under-

achievement in hyperactive children. Journal of Pediatric Psy-

chology, 1978, 3 . 168-471.

Cohen, N., Douglas, V., & Morgenstern, C The effect of methylphenidate

on attentive behavior and autonomic activity in hyperactive child-

ren. Psychopharmacologia, 1971, 22 , 282-294,

Cohen, N., Weiss, G,,'& Minde, K, Cognitive styles in adolescents pre-

viously diagnosed as hyperactive. Journal of Child PsychoTogy and

Psychiatry, 1972. 13 , 203-210.

Colquhon, W, P, The effect of unwanted signals in performance in a

vigilance task, Ergonomics, 1961, 4-, 41-51,

Conners, C, K., Eisenberg, L,, & Sharpe, L. Effects of methylphenidate

(Ritalin) on paired associate learning and Porteus Maze performance

in emotionally disturbed children. Journal of Consulting Psycho-

logy, 1964, 28 , 14-22.

Conners, C K., Goyette, C H., Southwick, D, A,, Lee, J, M,, &

Andrunolis, P, A, Food additives and hyperkinesis; A controlled

double-blind experiment, Pediatrics, 1976, 58, 154-166.

Conners, C K,, & Rothschild, G. H, Drugs and learning in children, In

J. Hellmuth (Ed,), Lparning disorders (Vol, 3), Seattle; Special

Child Publications, 1968,

Page 112: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

102

Conners, C, K,, Rothschild, G,, Eisenberg, L,, Schwartz, L, S,, &

Robinson, E, Dextroamphetamine sulfate in children with learning

disorders, Archives of General Psychiatry, 1969, 21_, 182-190.

Conners, C, K,, Taylor, E,, Meo, G., Kurtz, M. A,, & Fourmer, M, Magne-

sium pemoline and dextroamphetamine; A controlled study in child-

ren with minimal brain dysfunction. Psypharmacologia, 1972, 26 ,

321-336.

Conrad, P. The discovery of hyperkinesis: Notes on the medicalization

of deviant behavior, Social Problems, 1974, 23 , 12-21. II.

Conrad, W. G,„ Dworkin, E, S,, Shai, A,, & Tobiessen, J, E. Effects of

amphetamine therapy and prescriptive tutoring on the behavior and

achjevement of lower class hyperactive children, Journal of

Learning Disabilit̀S, 1971, 4, 45-53.

Cook, P,, & Woodhill. J. M. The Feingold diet, Medical Journal of

Australia, 1976, 2, 85-90,

Copeland, A. P,, & Weisbrodal, C, S, Behavioral correlates of the hyper-

activity factor of the Conners Teacher Questionnaire. Journal of

Abnormal Child PsycLology, 1978, C 339-343.

Cristensen, D. E. Effects of combining methylphenidate and a classroom

token system in modifying hyperactive behavior. American Journal

of Mental Deficiency, 1975, 80, 266-276.

Cromwell. R. L,, Gaumeister, A,, & Hawkins, W. F, Research in activity

level, In N, P. Fin^ (f^H.l, Handbook of mental deficiency. New

York; McGraw-Hill, 1963,

David, 0. J., Clark, J., & Voeller, K, Lead and hyperactivity. The

Lancet, 1972. 2, 900-903.

Page 113: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

103

Davis, K, V., Sprague. R, L., & Werry, J. S. Stereotyped behavior and

activity level in severe retardated; The effects of drugs. Ameri-

can Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1969, 72 , 721-727.

Deese, J, Some problems in the theory of vigilance. ^ Psychology Review,

1955, 62 , 359-368,

DeFillippis, N. A, The historical development of the diagnosis of

childhood hyperkinesis. Clinical Neuropsychology, 1979, U 15-19,

Douglas, V. I. Stop, look, and listen; The problem of sustained atten-

tion and impulse control in hyperactive and normal children. . Cana-

^ dian Journal of BehavioraT^ Science, 1972, A_, 259-282.

Douglas, V, I, Assessment of a cognitive training program for hyperac-

tive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1976, 4-,

389-410,

Doyle, R, B. The effects of distraction and attention deficits among

children with learning disabilities on a vigilance task, Unpub-

lished doctoral dissertation, Texas Tech University, 1973,

Doyle, R, B., Anderson, R. P,, & Halcomb, C G, Attention deficits and

the effects of visual distraction, Journal of Learning Disabili-

ties, 1976, 9, 59-65.

Duckworth, S., Ragland, C G,, Sommerfeld, R, E,. & Wyne, M, D, Modifi-

cation of conceptual impulsivity in retarded children. American

Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1974, 79, 59-63,

Dykman, R, A., Ackerman, P. T,, Clements, S, D., & Peters, J, E, Specific

learning disabilities: An attentional deficit syndrome. In H, R.

Myklebust (Ed.), Progress in Learning Disabilities, Vol. 2, New

York; Grune and Stratton, 1971,

Page 114: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

104

Dykman, R. A,, Ackerman, P, T,, Reters, 0, E,, & McGrew, J, PsycholO'-

gical tests, In C, K. Conners tEd.), Clinical use of stimulant

^ ^^gs in children, Amsterdam; Excerpta Medica,' 1974.

Ebaugh, F, G, Neuropsychiatric sequelae of acute epidemic encephali-

tis in children. Americar Journal qf^ Diseases qf Children, 1923,

25 , 89-97,

Ellis, N, R,, & Pryer, R. Ouantification of gross bodily activity in

children with severe neuropathology. American Journal of Mental

Deficiency, 1959, §3, 1034^^037,

Epstein, M. H,, Cullinan, D,^ Lloyd, J, Reliability of the MFFT scores

of learning disabled children, " Perceptual^and Motor Skills, 1977,

45 , 56-58,

Epstein, L. C , Lasagnci, L,, Conners. C, K,, & Rodriguez, A. Correlation

of dextroamphetamine excretion and drug response in hyperkinetic

children. Journal oF Nervous and Mental Disease, 1958, 45, 136-146.

Feingold, B. G. Why your child is hyperactive. New York; Random

House, 1975,

Finnerty, R. J,, SoUys, J, J., & Cole, J, 0. The use of D-amphetamine

with hyperactive children, Psychopharmalogia, 1971, 21 , 302-308,

Firestone, P,, Poitras-Wright, J., & Douglas, D. The effects of caf-

feine on hyperactive children, Journal of Learning Disabilities,

1978, 11, 133-134,

Fish, B, The "one child, one drug" myth of stimulants in hyperkinesis,

Archjves of General Psychiatry, 1971, 25 , 193-203,

Foshee, J, Studies in activity level; I. Simple and complex task

performance in defectives, American Journal o ^ Mental Deficiency,

1958, 62, 882-886,

Page 115: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

105

Golinko, B, E, Hyperactivity; Operationalization of traits using a

structured behavioral interview; A pilot study. Journal of

Pediatric Psychology, 1978, 3_, 35-44.

Goodman, L, S., & Gilman. A. (Eds,). The pharmacological basis of

therapeutics. New York: Macmillan, 1975,

Hall, V,, & Russell, W. Multitrait-multimethod analyses of conceptual

tempo, Journal of Educational Psychology, 1974, 65, 932-939,

Hallahan, D, P,, & Kauffman, J, M, lntroduction to learning disabili-

ti^s; - A psycho-behavit)ra1 approach. Englewood Cliffs, N, J.:

Prentice-Hall, 1976.

Harley, J. P,, Ray, R. S., Tomasi, L,, Eichman, P, L., Mathews, C G.,

ChuD, R,, Cleeland, C, S,, & Traisman, E, Hyperkinesis and food

additives; Testing the Feingold hypothesis. Pediatrics, 1978,

61, 818-322.

Hartley. E. R. Radiation that's good for you. Science Digest, 1974,

^76^, 39-45,

Henderson, A. T,, Dahlin, I,, Partridge, C R,. & Engelsing, E. L, A

hypothesis on the etiology of hyperactivity with a pilot study re

port of related non-drug therapy. Pediatrics, 1973, S2_y 625.

Herron, R,, & Ramsden, R. Continuous monitoring of overt human body

movement by radio telementary; A brief review. Perceotual and

Motor Skills, 1967, 2^, 1303-1308,

Heussy, H., & Cohen, A, Hyperkinetic behaviors and learning disabili-

ties followed over seven years. Pediatrics, 1976, 51, 4-10,

Page 116: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

106

Hopkins, J,, Perlman, T,, Hechtman, L,^ & Weiss, G. Cognitive style

in adults originally diagnosed as hyperactive, Journal of Child

Psychology ajid Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines. 1979, 20, 209-216,

Hoy, E., Weiss, G,, Minde, K., & Cohen, N, 'The hyperactive child at

adolescence; Cognitive, emotional and social functioning, Journal

of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1978, 6, 311-324,

Hunt, J, V, In 0, K. Buros (Ed,). The seventh mental measurements

yearbook, Vol, 1, Highland Park, N. J,: The Gryphon Press,

1972, 766-767.

Jenkins, H, M, The effect of signal-rate on performance in visual moni-

toring, American Journal of Psychology, 1958, 71, 647-561,

Jerison. J, J, Signal detection theory in the analysis of human vigi-

lance. Human Factors, 1967, £, 285-288.

Jerison, J, J,, Pickett, R, M,, & Stenson, H, H, The elicited obser-

ving rate and decision processes in vigilance. Human Factors,

1965, 1, 107-123.

Johnson, C, Hyperactivity and the machine; The actometer. Child De-

velopment, 1971, 42, 2105-2110,

Johnson, J, A, The etiology of hyperactivity. Exceptional Children,

1981, 47, 348-354,

Jones, N, M,, Loney, J,, Weissenburger, F. R., & Gleischmann, P. J, The

hyperkinetic child: What do teachers know? Psychology in the

Schools, 1975, 12, 388-392.

Juliano, D, B, Conceptual tempo, activity, and concept learning in

hyperactive and normal children, Journal ql Abnormal Psychology.

1974, 83, 629-634.

Page 117: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

107

Kagan, J. Individual differences in the resolution of response uncer-

tainty, Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 1965, 2_,

154-160.

Kagan, J,, Pearson, L,, & Welch, L. Conceptual impulsivlty and induc-

tive reasoning. Child Develooment, 1966, 3^, 583^594.

Kagan, J,, Rosman, B. L., Day, D., Albert, J,, & Phillips, W, Informa-

tion processing in the child; Significance of analytic and reflec-

tive attitudes. Psychological Monographs, 1964, 78 , (l,''Whole

No. 578).

Kahn, E., & Cohen, L, H. Organic driveness: A brain stem syndrome and

an experience, New England Journal of Medicine, 1934, 21^, 748-756.

Kahn, E.,.& Gardner, G. Hyperactivity; Predominant diagnosis in child

referrals. Frontirrs of Psychiatry, 1975, 5 , 3.

Kaspar, J, C., & Lowenstein, R. The effect of social interaction on

activity level. Child Development, 1971, 42 , 1294-1298,

Kaspar, J. C , Millichap, J. G,, Backus, R,, Child, D,, & Schulman, J. C.

A study of the relationship between neurological evidence of brain

damage in children and activity and distractibility. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 1971, 36 , 329-337.

Kenny, T. J., Clemmens, R, L., Hudson, B. W,, Lentz, G. A,, Cicci. R.,

& Nair, P, Charactsristics of children referred because of hyper-

activity. Journal of Pediatrics, 1971,- 79 , 618.

Keogh, B. K,, & Donlon, C Field dependence, impulsivity. and learning

disabilities. Journal of Learnina Disabilities, 1972, 5, 331-336,

Page 118: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

108

Keogh, B, K,, Wetter, J,, McGinty^ A,, & Donlon, G, Functional analysis

of WISC performance of learning disordered, hyperactive, and îîien-

tally disordered boys, Psychology in the Schools, 1973, 10, 178-

180,

Klecka, W, R, Discriminant analysis, In N. H. Nie, C, H, Hull, J. G.

Jenkins, K. Steinbrenner, D, H. Bent, Statistical Package for the

Social Sciences (2nd ed.). New York; McGraw-Hill, 1975, 434-467,

Knights, R, M., & Hinton, G. G, The effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin)

on the motor skills and behavior of children with learning prob-

lems, Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1969^148^, 643-653,

Lambert, N,, & Sandoval, J, The prevalence of learning disabilities

in,a sample of children considered hyperactive, Journal of Ab-

normal Child Psychology, 1980, 8 , 33-50.

Langsdorf, R, C, An empirical application of temperament theory of

child development to hyperactivity. Unpublished doctoral dis-

sertation, Texas Tech University, 1980,

Langsdorf, R,, Anderson, R, P,, Waechter, D., Madrigal, J, F., & Juarez,

L. J. Ethnicity, social class, and perception of hyperactivity,

Psychology in the Schools, 1979, 16, 233-238.

Lapouse, R., & Monk, M, A, An epidemiological study of behavior charac-

teristics in children. American Journal of Public H_eaUíi, 1958,

48, 1134-1144.

Levy, F., Dumbrell, S,, Hobbes, G,, Ryan, M,, Wilton, N., & Woodhill,

J, M, Hyperkinesis and diet, Tlie Medical Journal^of Australia,

1978, 1, 61-64.

Mackworth, J. F. Vigilance, arousal, and habituation. Psychology,

1968, 75, 308-322.

Page 119: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

109

Mann, L. Difference between reflective and impulsive children in tempo

and quality of decision making. Child Development, 1973, 4^, 274-

279,

Margolis, H,, Leonard, H., Brannigan, G, G., & Heverly, M, A, The

validity of Form F of the Matching Familiar Figures Test with

kindergarten children, Journal ol Experimental Child Psychology,

1980, 29 , 12-22.

Mayron, L, W,, Ott, J,, Nations, R., & Mayron, E, Light, radiation

and academic behavior. Academic Therapy, 1974, 10, 33-47.

McFarland, J, N., Peacock, L. J,, & Watson, J. A, Mental retardation

and activity level in rats and children. American Journal of

Mental Deficiency, 1966, 71, 381-386,

Mendelson, W., Johnson, N,, & Stewart, M. A, Hyperactive children as

teenagers; A follow-up study, Journal of Nervous and Mental

Diseases, 1971. 153, 273-279.

Messer, S, B. Reflection-impulsivity; A review, Psychological Bulle-

tin, 1976, 83, 1026-1052.

Millichap, J, C , Aymat, F., Sturgis, L. H,, Larsen K, W,, & Egan, R, A,

Hyperkinetic behavior. American Journal of Diseases of Children,

1968, 116, 235-244,

Millichap, J. C , & Boldrey, E. E, Studies in hyperkinetic behavior II.

Laboratory and clinical evaluations of drug treatments. Neurology,

1967, 21, 467-471,

Millichap, J, C , & Fowler, G, W, Treatment of "minimal brain dysfunc-

tion" syndrome, Pediatric Clinics qf North America, 1967, 14 ,

767-776,

Page 120: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

110

Mniichap, J. 6,, & Johnson,-F, H, Methylphenidate in hyperkinetic be-

havior; Relation of response to degree of activity and brain

damage, In C, K, Conners (Ed,), Clinical use of stimulant drugs

in children. Amsterdam; Excerpta Medica, 1974,

Morrison, J, R, Childhood hyperactivity in an adult psychiatric popu-

lation; Social factors, Jqurnal of Clinical Psychiatry, 1980,

41. 40-43.

Morrison, J., & Stewart, M. A, A family study of hyperactive child

syndrome. Bjological Psychiatry, 1971, 3_, 189-195.

Ney, P. G. Four types of hyperkinesis, Canadian Psychiatric Associa-

tion Journal, 1974, 19, '543-550.

O'Leary, J(, D, PilTs or skills for hyperactive children, Journal of

Applied Behavior Analysis, 1980, l^, 191-204.

Orton, S, Reading, writinq, and speech oroblems ^n children. New York;

Norton, 1937.

Ott, J. The eyes' dual function. II, Eye, Ear , Nose, and Throat

Monthly, 1974, 51, 377-381.

Ozolins, D. A. The effects of knowledge of resuUs on the vigilance

performance of hyperactive and hypoactive children with learning

disabilities. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Texas Tech

University, 1974.

Page, J. G,, Bernstein, J, E,, Janicki, R. S,, & Michelli, F, A, A

multi-clinic trial of pemoline in childhood hyperkinesis, In

C K, Conners (Ed.), Clinical use of stimulant drugs in children.

Amsterdam; Excerpta Medica, 1974.

Page 121: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

111

Painter, M, Flourescent lights and hyperactivity in children; An

experiment, Academic Therapv. 1976, 11, 181-184,

Palkes, H,, & Stewart, M. Intellectual ability and performance of

hyperactive children, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry,

1972, 42, 35-39.

Palkes, H,, Stewart, M., & Kahana, B, Maze performance of hyperactive

boys after training in self-directed verbal commands. Child

Development, 1968, 3^, 817-B26,

Patterson, G, R,, Jones, R,, Whittier, J,, & Wright, M, A. A behaviour

modification technique for the hyperactive child. Behaviour

Research and Theraoy, 1965,^1, 217-226,

Paulsen, K,, & Johnson, M, Impulsivity; A multidimensional concept

with developmental aspects, Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

1980, 8, 269-277.

Poggio, J. P., & Salkind, N. J, A review and appraisal of instruments

assessing hyperactivity in children, Learning Disability Quarterly,

1979, 1, 9-22.

Prjnz, R."J., Roberts, W, A,, & Hantman, E. Dietary correlates of hyper-

active behavior in children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical

Psychology, 1980, 48, 760-769,

Rapoport, J,, Abramson, A., Alexander, D., & Lott, I, Playroom obser-

vation of hyperactive children on medication, Journal of Child

Psychiatry, 1971, 10, 524-534.

Rapoport, J,, Quinn, P., Bradbard, G., Riddle, K., & Brooks, E. Imipra-

mine and methylphenidate treatments in hyperactive boys, Archives

of General Psychiatry, 1974, 30, 789-793.

Page 122: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

112

Robin, S,, & Bosco, J, J. The social context of stimulant drug treat-

ment for hyperkinetic children. School Review, 1976, 85, 141-154,

Rosenthal, R, H,, & Allen, T, W, An examination of attention, arousal,

and learning dysfunctions of hyperkinetic children, Psychologtcal

Bulletin, 1978, 85, 669-715.

Ross, A, 0,, & Pelham, W. E. Child psychopathology, In M, R,

Rosenzweig and Lyman W, Porter (Eds,), Annua! Review of Psychology.

Palo Alto, Calif,; Annual Reviews, Inc,, 1981.

Ross, D,, & Ross, S. Hyperactivity; Research, 'theory, and action.

New York; Wiley, 1976.

Rovet, J, A. A parametric measure of reflection-impulsivity, Journal •.

of pplied Develoomental Psychology, 1980, 1, 221-225.

Rutter, M, L., & Brown, G. W. The reliability and validity of measures

of family life and relationships in families containing a psychi-

atric patient, Social Psychiatry, 1966, 1, 38-53.

Safer, D,, & Allen, R. Hvperactive children; Diagnosis and management.

Baltimore; University Park Press, 1976,

Salzman, L. K, Allergy testing, psychological testing, and dietary

treatment of the hyperactive child syndrome, Medical Journal of

Australia, 1976, 1, 248-251.

Sandoval, J, The measurement of the hyperactive syndrome in children.

Review of Educational Research, 1977, 4^, 293-318.

Sandoval, J,, Lambert, N, M,, & Sassone, D. M. The comprehensiye treat-

ment of hyperactive children: A continuing problem. Journal of

Learning Disabilities, 1981, 14, 117-118.

Page 123: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

113

Schleifer, M,, Weiss, G., Cohen, N., Elman, M., Cvjic, H,. & Kruger, E.

Hyperactivity in preschoolers and the effect of methylphenidate.

American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1975, 45, 38-50.

Schrager, J, et a n The hyperkinetic syndrome: Some consensually

validated behavioral correlates, Exceotional Children, 1966,

31, 635-637.

Schuckit, M, A,, Petrich, J,^ & Chiles, J, Hyperactivity; Diagnostic

confusion^ Joumal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 1978, 166, 79-87.

Schulman, J,, & Reisî ian, J. An objective measure of hyperacttvity.

American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1959, 6£, 455-456,

Schulman, J. L,, Stevens, T, M,, & Kupst, M. J, The hiometer; A new •

dev-ice for the measurement and remediation of hyperactiyity. Child

Development, 1977, 48_, 1152^1154,

Siegelman, E. Reflective and impulsive observing 'behavior, Child

Development, 1969, f.C 1213-1222.

Silbergeld^, E. K,, & Goldberg, A, M, A lead-induced behavioral disorder,

Life Sciences, 1973, 13 , 1275-1283,

Silbergeld, E, K,, & Goldberg, A, M, A lead-induced behavioral dysfunc-

tion; An animal moJel of hyperactivity. Experimental Neurology,

1974, 41, 146-157.

Sprague, R, L, Psychopharmacotherapy in children, In M. F, McMillan

and S, Henae (Eds.), C hild psychiatry; Treatment and research,

New York: Bruner/Mazel,. 1977.

Page 124: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

114

Sprague, R. L., Barnes, K., & Werry, J. Methylphenidate and thion'da-

zine: Learning, reaction time, activity, and classroom behavior

1n disturbed children, American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 1970,

4£, 615-528,

Sprague, R, L., Christensen, D, E., & Werry, J, S, Experimental psy-

chology and stimulant drugs, In C K. Conners (Ed), Clinical use

of stimulant drugs i£ children. Amsterdam: Excerpta Medica, 1974,

Sprague, R, L,, & Gadow, K, D. The role of the teacher in drug treat-

ment. School Reviev, 1976, 81, 109-140.

Spring, C,, Yellin, A. M,, & Greenberg, L, Effects of imipramine and

methylphenidate in perceptual-motor performance of hyperactive

children, Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1976, 4^, 459-470.

Staton, R. D., & Brumback, R. A, Non-specificity of motor hyperactivity

as a diagnostic criterion. Perceotual and Motor Skills, 1981,

.52,.'323-332.

Stevens, G. Bias in the attribution of hyperkinetic behavior.as a func-

tion of ethnic identification and socioeconomic status. Psychology

in the Schools, 1981, l^, 99-106,

Stevens, T. M,, Kupst, M. J,, Suran, B. G,, & Schulman, J. L. Actiyity

level: A co nparison betv/een actometer scores and observer ratings,

Journal 'of Abnormal Child Psychology, 1978, 6_, 163-173,

Stewart, M. A,, & Olds, S. W. Raisinq a hyperactive child. New York;

Harper & Row, 1973,

Still, G, F. The coulstoniain lectures of some abnormal physical con-

ditions in children. Lancet. 1902, 1, 1108-1112, 1077-1082,

1163-1168.

Page 125: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

115

Strauss, A, A,, & Lehtinen, L, Psychopathology and education of the

brain injured child. New York: Grune & Stratton, 1947.

Sykes, D, H,, Douglas, V. I,, & Morgenstern, G, Sustained attention

in hyperactive children, Journal qf Child Psycholooy and Psy-

chiatry, 1973, 14, 213-220.

Sykes, D., Douglas. V,, Weiss, G,, & Minde, K. Attention in hyperac-

tive children and the effects of methylphenidate (Ritalin),

Journal qf Child PsychoTogy and Psychiatry, 1971, l^, 129-139.

Taylor, E. Food additives, allergy, and hyperkinesis, Journal of

Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1971; l^, 129-139.

Thomas, S. A. W, The role of cognitive style variables in mediating

the influence of iggressive television upon elementary school

children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of

California, Los Angeles, 1971,

Victor, J. B., Halverson, C C , Inoff, C , & Buckowski, H, J, Objec-

tive behavior measures of first- and second-grade boys' free play

and teachers' ratincs on a behavior problem checklist. Psychology

in the Schools, 197?, 10, 439-443.

Waechter, D., Anderson, R. P., Juarez, L. J., Langsdorf, R,, &.Madrigal,

J. F. Ethnic grouo, hyperkinesis, and modes of behavior. Psy-

chology in the Schools, 1979, 16, 435-439,

Walden, E, L., & Thompson, S, A. A review of some alternative approaches

to drug management of hyperactivity in children. Journal of

Learninq Disabilitirs, 1981, 14, 213-217,

Weiss, C , & Hechtman, L. The hyperactive child syndrome. Science,

1979, 205, 1348-1354,

Page 126: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

116

Weiss, G., Minde, K,,. Werry, J, S,, Douglas, V, I,, & Nemeth, E, Studies

on the hyperactive child, VIII, Five-year follow-up. Archives

of General Psychiatrv. 1971, 2^, 409-414.

Wender, P. H, jlnijmal-bm dysfunction in children. New York:

Wiley-Interscience, 1971,

Wender, P, H. - The hyperactive child; A handbook for oarents, New

York: Crown Publishers, 1973.

Werry, J. S,, & Quay, H, C, The prevalence of behayior symptoms in

younger elementairy school .children. American Journal of Ortho-

- psychiatry, 1971, 41, 136-143,

Werry, J. S., Sprague, R, L,, & Cohen, M, N, Conners teacher rating

scale for use in drug studies with chiTdren -- An empirical study,

Journal qf Abnormal Child Psychology, 1975,.!, 217-229,

Whalen, C, K,, & Henker, B, Psychostimulants and children; A review

and analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 1976, §1, 1113-1130.

Wiener, G, Varying psychological sequelae of lead ingestion in children,

- Public Health Reports, 1970, 85, 19-24.

Willerman, L., & Plomin, R, Activity level in children and their

parents. - Child Devrlopment, 1973, 44, 288-293,

Williamson, G, A,, Anderson, R, P,, & Lundy, N, C, The ecological

treatment of hyperkinesis, ^Psychology in the Schools, 1980,

11, 249-256,

Winsberg, B. G., Bialer, I., Kupietz, S., & Tobias, J. Effects of

imipramine and dextroamphetamine on behavn'or of neuropsychiatri-

cally impaired children, Journal of Psychiatry, 1972, 128,

1425-1431.

Page 127: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

117

Witkin, H, A. The perception of the upright. Scientific American,

1959, 200, 50-56.

Yando, R,, & Kagan, J. The effect of task complexity on reflectionr

impulsivity, Cognitive Psychology. 1970, 1, 192-200,

Yarrow, M, R,, Campbell, J. D., & Burton, R, V, Recollections of

childhood; A study of the retrospectiye method. Monographs of '

the Society for Research ir Child Development, 1975, 31, Serial 138

Zelniker, T,, Jeffrey, W. E,, Ault, R., & Parsons, J. Analysis and modi-

fication of research strategies of impulsive and reflective

children on the Matching Familiar Figures Test. Child Development,

- 1972, 41, 321-335.

Zentan,_S, S,, & Barack, R, S, Rating scales for hyperactivity:

Concurrent validity, reliability, and decisions to label for the

Conners and Davids Abbreviated Scales. Journal of Abnormal Child

- Psychology, 1979, T , 179-190,

Zinna, R, Is there a hyperkinetic syndrome? British Medical Journal,

1979, 1, 685.

Zukow, P. G., Zukow, A. H., & Bentler, P. M. Rating scales for the

identification and treatment of hyperkinesis. Journal of Consul-

tinq and CTinical Psychology, 1978, 46, 213-222.

Page 128: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

APPENDICES

A. DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER WITH HYPER-

ACTIVITY.

B, PARENT COVER LETTER (ST. JOSEPH'S).

C PARENT COVER LETTER (LEVELLAND).

D, INFORMED CONSENT FORM.

E, ZUKOW HYPERKINESIS RATING FORM.

F, BEHAVIOR CHECKLIST,

G, CONNER'S ABBREVIATED TEACHER RATING SCALE,

H, TEST REPORT FORM.

I. TEXAS'EDUCATION AGENCY GUIDELINES FOR IDENTIFYING LEARNING DISABLED

STUDENTS,

J, VIGILANCE TASK BOOTH.

K. AUDIQTAPE RECORDED INSTRUCTIQNS.

L. DISSERTATION RECORD SHEET.

118

Page 129: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

119

APPENDIX A; DIAGNOSTIC CRITERIA FOR

ATTENTION DEFICIT DISORDER WITH HYPERACTIVITY

The child dispUys, for his or her mental and chronologicsil age,

signs of developmentally inappropriate inattention, impulsivity, and hy-

peractivity, The signs must be reported by adults in the child's environ-

ment, such as parents and teachers. Because the symptoms are typically

variable, they ma y not be ohserved directly by the clinician. When the

reports of teachers and parents conflict, primary consideration would

be given to the teacher reports because of greater familiarity with age-

appropriate norms. Symptoms typically worsen in situations that require

self-appltcation, as in the classroom, Signs of the disorder may be ab-

sent when the child is in a new or a one-to-one situation.

The number of symptoms specified is for children between the ages

of eight and ten, the peak age range for referral. In younger children,

more severe forms of the symptoms and a greater number of symptoms are

usually present, The opposit'e is true of older children,

A. Inattention. At least three of the following;

(1) often fails to finish things he or she starts.

(2) often doesn't seem to listen.

(3) easily distracted,

(.4) has difficulty concentrating on school work or other tasks re-

quiring Sustained attention,

C5) has difficulty sticking to a play actiyity.

B, Impulsivity. At least three of the following;

(1) often acts before thinking,

(2) shifts excessiyely from one activity to another.

Page 130: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

120

(3) has d i f f i c u U y organizing work ( th is not being due tp cpgni-

t i v e impairment),

(4) needs a l o t of supervision.

(5) f requent ly ca l l s out in class.

(6) has d i f f i c u l t y awaiting turn in games or group s i tuat ions,

C, Hyperact iv i ty . At least two of the fo l lowing:

(1) runs about or climbLS excessively.

(2) has d i f f i c u l t y s i t t i n g s t i l l or f idgets excessiyely,

(3) has d i f f i c u l t y staying seated.

(4) moves about excessively during sleep.

(5) i s always "on the go" or acts as i f "driven by a motor".

D, Onset before the age of seven.

E, Duration of at least six tnonths,

F, Not due to Schizophrenia, Af fect ive Disorder, or Severe or Profound

Mental Retardation,

American Psychiatr ic Association, Diagnostic and s t a t i s t i c a l manual

o f mental disorders (3rd Ed,), Washington, D, C ; American Psy-

ch ia t r i c Associat ion, 1980, 43-44,

Page 131: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

121

APPENDIX B; PARENT COVER LETTER (ST, JOSEPH'S)

Parents,

The purpose of this letter is to ask your assistance with a re -search project which I am currently involved in. I would appreciate it n you would take a bit of your time to read this letter and consider allowing your child to participate,

The purpose of my project is to develop a group of tests which will accurately identify hyperactive children. I do need to test, however, a large group of non-hyperactive children as well. Your participation in the study will involve filling out a couple of short rating forms and a form of consent which wlll allow me to test your child.

The twotests that I v'ill be ginving are the following; 1. The Vigilance Task. This test ineasures attention span, impulsive-ness, and restlessness. It involves the child sitting at a console, watching lights flash on and off, and pushing a button occasionally. The test takes about 35 minutes to complete. 2, The Matching Familiar Figures Test, This test is also aimed at mea-suring impulsiveness. It only.takes about 10 minutes and involves your child simply pointing to a line drawing which is supposed to match another drawing, Further, I will aTso administer a very brief (.10 to 15 minute) test of intellectual functioning, I will be asking some of your child's tea-chers to fill out rating forms similar to the one you will complete,

I can assure you that the results of these tests will be confiden-tial and no one besides myself will have access to the records, I can also assure you that there is no possibility of these results being used to the detriment of your child,

The tests will be administered at St. Joseph's School during regu-lar school time (your child will miss about one hour of classroom time), I have attached a consent form which you should sign and send back to school if you agree to participate,

I would greatly appreciate your help with this project, If there are any questions you can contact me at home (797-1785). Thank you for your time and consideraticn of this matter.

John C, Simoneaux Doctoral Student - Texas Tech

Page 132: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

122

APPENDIX C; PARENT COVER LETTER (LEVELLAND)

Parents,

.P. J h n'^^^^f í^ l^l^ letter is to ask your assistance .with a re-U if i '^^''L''Í^F^ ^ " currently involved in. I would appreciate H L Iif^''^"^'^ ^^^î.^ ^^^ °^ yo"^ ti" e to read this letter and consi-der allowing your chnd to participate.

.r..^^t ?"'"P°^^.? ^y P'-oject is to develop a group of tests which will accurately identify hyperactive children. I do need to test, however, a large group of non-hyperactive children as well, Your participation in the study will involve filling out a couple of short rating forms and a torm of consent which will allow me to test your child,

The two tests that I will be giving are the following; 1. The Vigilance Task. This test measures attention span, impulsiveness, and restlessness. Involves the child sitting at a console, watching nghts flash on and off, and pushing a button occasionally. The test takes a'bout 35 minutes to complete, 2. Matching Familiar Firures Test. This test is also aimed at measuring impulsiveness. It only takes about 10 minutes and involves your child simply pointing to a line drawing which is supposed to match another drawing, ' Further, I will also administer a very brief (10 to 15 minutes) test of intellectual functioning. I will be asking some of your child's tea-chers to fill out rating forms similar to the one you will complete.

I can assure you that the results of these tests will be confiden-tial and no one besides myself will have access to the records. I can also assure you that there is no possibility of these results being used to the detriment of your child.

The tests will be administered at the South Plains Educational Co-op building at a time which 'vill be arranged later. I have attached a consent form which you should sign and send back to school if you agree to participate.

I would greatly appreciate your help with this project, If there are any questions you can contact me at the co-op (894-6858). Thank you for your time and consideration of this matter.

John C Simoneaux Doctoral Student - Texas Tech

Page 133: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

123

APPENDIX D: INFORMED CONSENT FORM

I hereby give my consent and agree to allow my child to participate

in the research project entitled "An Evaluation of the Classification

of Hyperkinetic Children with the Vigilance Task and the Matching Fa-

miliar Figures Test."

I understand that the person responsible for this project is John

C Simoneaux, Doctoral student, under the supervision of Dr. Robert P.

Anderson, Department of Psychology, 742-3736.

The objectives of this study have been explained; to evaluate the

usefulness of the Vigilance Task and the Matching Familiar Figures Test

in identifying hyperactive children.

I understand that the procedure involves my child being administered

the two tests mentioned above. The nature of these tests has been ex-

plained to me, I also understand that my child's teachers and myself

will be asked to complete a behavior rating form concerning my child,

This procedure has been judged to provide no discomfort or risks to

the subjects who participate,

Mr, Simoneaux has agreed to answer any inquiries I may have concerning

the procedures, I have haen informed that I may contact the Texas Tech

University Institutional Peview Board for the Protection of Human Sub-

jects by writing them in care of the Office of Research Services, Texas

Tech University, Lubbock, Texas 79409, or by calling 742-3884,

The following must app3ar on all consent forms:

"If this research project causes any physical injury to you ( our

child), treatment is not necessarily available at Texas Tech University

or any program pf insurance applicable to the institution and its

Page 134: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

124

personnel, Financial compensation for any such injury must be pro-

vided through your own insurance program, Further information about

these matters may he ohtained from D, J, Knox Jones, Jr,, Vice Presi-

dent fpr Research and Graduate Studies, Room 118, Administration

Buildtng, Texas Tech University, Lubbock, Texas 79409, telephone

742-2152.

I understand that my child m^y not derive therapeutic benefit from

participation in this study, I also understand that my child may dis-

continue this study at any time I (or my child) choose.

Signeiture of Parent Date

Signature of Project Director^_ Date

Page 135: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

125

APPENDIX E; ZUKOW HYPERKINESIS RATING FORM

Instructions? Circle the answer which best fits your child^s day-to-day behavior.

School

Sometimes

1, Usually hyperactive: Home

2, Jumps from one activity to another;

3, Short attention span;

4, Fidgets:

5, Is unpredictable, unmanageable;

6, Irritable;

7, Overly sensitive;

8, Quick tempered, explosiye: 1

9, Panics easily:

10, Tolerance for failure and frustration low:

11, Emotionally high strung;

12, Told ahead of time about an outing or appointment becomes anxious or upset;

13, Exceptionally clumsy:

14, Poor coordination:

15, Eyes and hands don't seem to function together;

16, Has trouble buttoning;

17, Has trouble drawing, writing;

18, Was slow learning to \;alk;

19, Trouble with bicycles:

20, Troúble catching balls;

21, Speech development has been slow:

22, Speech is not clear:

23, Reacts adversely to changes in routine;

Both

Y^s

Yes

No

No

No

Constant No

Yes No

Yes No

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

No

No

No

No

No

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

Yes

No

No

No

No

No

No

No

No

No

No

No

No

Page 136: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

126

24, C^nU seem to keep from touching everythinq

and everyone around him; Yes No

25, Not learning in school although seems "bright": Yes No

26, Is child lazy — not trying to do well in school: Yes No 27, Daydreams whild doing homework assignments; Yes No

28, Knows work orally at home -- gets to school and has to write it down —. fails miserably; Yes No

Page 137: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

127

APPENDIX F; BEHAVIOR CHECKLIST

Please place a check next to those items which are characteristic

of .

CChild's name)

Often f a i l s to f i n i s h things he or she s ta r t s ,

Often doesn't seem to l i s t e n ,

Easily d is t rac ted .

Has d i f f i c u l t y concentrating on schoolwork or other tasks re-requi r ing sustained a t ten t ion .

Has d i f f i c u l t y s t ick ing to a play a c t i v i t y .

Often acts before th ink ing,

Shi f ts excessively from one a c t i v i t y to another,

Has d i f f i c u l t y organizing work,

Needs a l o t of suparvision,

Frequently ca l l s out in class.

Has d i f f i c u l t y awaiting turn in games or group s i tuat ions.

Runs about or climbs excessively,

Has d i f f i c u l t y s i t t i n g s t i l l or f idgets excessively,

Has d i f f i c u l t y staying seated,

Moves about excessively during sleep,

Is always "on the go" or acts as i f "driven by a motor"

Adaoted from: American Psychiatric Association. Diaqnostic and s t a t i s t i c a l manualof mental disorders (3rd Ed.), Washington, D T T T ^ Ãmirican Psychiatric Associat ion, 1980, 43-44,

Page 138: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

128

APPENDIX 6; CONNERS' ABRREVIATED TEACHER RATING SCALE

Child's Name

TEACHER'S OBSERVATIQNS

Information obtained

Month Day _By^

Year

OBSERVATION

NOT AT ALL

0

DEGREE OF ACTIVITY

1. Restless or overactive,

2. Excitable, impulsive,

3. Disturbs other children,

4. Fails'to finish things he/she starts, short attention span,

5. Constantly fidgeting,

6. Inattentive, easily distracted,

7. Demands must be met immediately, easily frustrated,

8. Cries often and easily.

9. Mood changes quickly and drastically.

10. Temper outbursts, explosive and un-predictable behavior.

JUST A LITTLE

1

PRETTY MUCH 2

VERY MUCH 3

OTHER OBSERVATIONS OF TEACHER (Use reverse side if more space is needed).

Page 139: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

129

APPENDIXK; TEST REPORT FORM

Parents;

Thank you for allowing your child to participate in this study. Your cooperation has been invaluable and is certainly aporeciated, I would n k e to giye you a bit of feedback on the results of the tésts your chnd completed for íne.

Child's Name Date of Test

Basic Viqilance Task: Raw Score Percentile

Correct Detections (Attention)

False Alarms (,errors) (Impulsiveness)

Note; The higher the percentile score the better,

Matching Jamiliar Figures Test Raw Score

Latency

Errors

Explanation and Summary of Results;

If you have any questions about these results please feel free to get in touch with me either in Levelland (894-5858) or in Lubbock (797-1785). I will be glad to meet with you personally and discuss the testing. Thank you again for your lielp with this study.

John C. Simoneaux Project Director

Page 140: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

130

APPENDIX I; TEXAS EDUCATION AGENCY GUIDELINES

FOR IDENTIFYING LEARNING DISABLED STUDENTS

(H) Learning Disabled Student

(i) A student who is learning disabled is one who has been deter-mined by a îîiultidisciplinary team not to be achieving commen-surate with his/her age and ability levels. The lack of achievement is found when the student is provided with learning experiences appropriate for his/her age and ability levels in one or íîiore of the following area^; oral expression, listening comprehension, written expression, basic reading skill, read-ing comprehension, mathematics calculation, mathematics rea-soning, or spelling. The term does not include students whose severe discrepancy between ability a'nd achievement is primarily the result of; a visual, hearing, or orthopedic handicap; mental retardation; emotional disturbance; or environmental, cuUural, or economic disadvantage.

Reference

Public Law 94^142 Regulations, Section 121a.540-543.

(.ii) The admission, review, and dismissal committee or multi-disciplinary team may determine that a severe discrepancy .exists as long as the membership of the team includes at least;

(I) the student's regular teacher; or

if the student does not have a regular teacher, a regular classroom teacher qualified to teach a student of his/her age; or for a pre-kindergarten child, an individual certified to teach a child of his/her age; and

(II) a person certified or trained in the area of learning disabilities; and

(III) at least one person certified by the Texas Education Agency to conduct comprehensive assessments of intellec-tual and educational functioning,

At least one team member other than the child's regular teacher shall observe the child's academic performance in a.regular classroom settinr, In the case of pre-kindergarten, a team member shall observe the child in an environment appropriate for a child of that age,

(iii) A severe discrepancy between intellectual ability and academic achievement is defined as one where the student's assessed

Page 141: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

131

intellectual functioning is above the mentally retarded range, but where the studenfs assessed educationgl functioning in nlll / î í . - ^ "^°^^ ^^3" o"e standard deviation below the f^ .? / . ^ ^ ' ^ °^ ^^^e ^^^ student's assessed educa-

tionai tunctiomng in areas specified is more than one standard oeviation below the student's intellectual functioning.

When a student's educational performance is below the mean of tne distnct but consistent with the student's assessed intel-lectual functioning, the student is not eligible to be classi-fied as learning disabled.

(iv) As verification of the team's decision, a written report of the evaluation will be prepared and will include but not be limited to;

(I) the results of an assessment of intellectual functioning showing that the student is not mentally retarded;

(II) the results of an individual educational assessment de-scribing the area(.s) of educational achievement in which the student is deficient and substantiating a severe dis-crepancy between achievement and intellectual ability and the basis for determining each;

(III) a statement of the relevant behavior noted during the observation and the relationship of that behavior to the child's educational functioning;

(IV) a statement of educationally relevant medical findings, if any, either from fhe screening prior to referral or from subsequent examination if needed; and

(V) a statement that the severe discrepancy is not primarily the result of an emotional disturbance; visual, hearing, or orthopedic handicap; cultural differences; environmental causes; or economic disadvantage.

Each team member will certify in writing that he/she either con-curs or dissents with the results of the evaluation as set forth in the written report, If the report does not reflect an individual's conclusions, the dissenting member may proyide a separate statement,

Texas Education Agency, Policies and administrative procedures for the education qf hanr'icapped students. Department of Special Education; Austin, Texas, November, 1979, 25-27,

Page 142: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

132

APPENDIX J: VIGILANCE TASK BOOTH

Page 143: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

133

APPENDIX Kj AUDIOTAPE RECORDED INSTRUCTIONS

PriorUo Practice^SessTon

Okay listen carefully and I will tell you what we are going to

do, Make yourself comfortable first, Remember, if at any time you

don't feel well, or you need to leave, just raise your hand. Now,

grip the bicycle handle so that you can press the little button on

top, Okay? Here's what I want you to do, As long as you see two

green lights or two red lights, that's okay, just let them go; but,

when you see one green and one red blink at the same time press the

button, Okay? Don't prer.s the button until a red and a green blink

together side by side, Okay? Let's practice it first, Let's prac-

tice it for five minutes, You'll be finished when the two lights

come on and stay on, Okay, let's practice,

Prior to Experimental Session

Now we want you to do the same thing for thirty minutes, Okay?

Now watch the lights and press the button when the red and the green

blink at the same time, anJ do not press it when a red or a green

blink by themselves, Okay? Just like the end of the practice you'll

be finished when the two lights come on and stay on. Okay? Here we

go, Raise your hand if you need anything.

Page 144: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux

134

APPENDIX Lî DISSERTATION RECORD SHEET

Name;

ID#;

Day of Week;

Age:

School;

Sex;

Time of Test;

Total CDs;

Total FAs;

Total Act;

Total Errors;

Mean Latency;

Classification;

CDs Odd;

CDs Even;'

FAs Odd

FAs Even

Act Odd;

Act Even;

Grade;

IQ:

C Score 1;

C Score 2:

Z Score;

D Score 1

D Score 2

D Score 3

RT Odd;

RT Even;

Err Odd;

Err Even;

\ Viqilance Task Data

1

2

3

4

5

6

Total

1-3-5

2-4-6 1

CD

1

FA

'

AC^ .

- \

1

1

1 •- 1

1

•Matching Familiar Figures Test

Card (answer) Resp, Time Err.

1 (1)

2 (6)

3 (3)

4 (1)

-5 (2)

6 (6)

7 (3)

8 (5)

9 (4)

10 (5)

11 (2)

12 (4)

Total

Odd

Even 1

1 i —

1 j

1

1

i

1

1

Page 145: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux
Page 146: ^ 1982 John C, Simoneaux