28
International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology 1–28 © The Author(s) 2015 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0306624X15590809 ijo.sagepub.com Article Tests of Theories of Crime in Female Prisoners: Social Bond and Control, Risk Taking, and Dynamic Systems Theories Marc A. Lindberg 1 , April Fugett 1 , Ashtin Adkins 1 , and Kelsey Cook 1 Abstract Several general theories of crime were tested with path models on 293 female prisoners in a U.S. State prison. The theories tested included Social Bond and Control, Thrill/Risk Seeking, and a new attachment-based Developmental Dynamic Systems model. A large battery of different instruments ranging from measures of risk taking, to a crime addiction scale, to Childhood Adverse Events, to attachments and clinical issues were used. The older general theories of crime did not hold up well under the rigor of path modeling. The new dynamic systems model was supported that incorporated adverse childhood events leading to (a) peer crime, (b) crime addiction, and (c) a measure derived from the Attachment and Clinical Issues Questionnaire (ACIQ) that takes individual differences in attachments and clinical issues into account. The results were discussed in terms of new approaches to Research Defined Criteria of Diagnosis (RDoC) and new approaches to intervention. Keywords female prisoners, attachment and crime, risk seeking theories of crime, social bond theories, ACIQ, RDoC diagnoses, individual versus group analyses, dynamic systems theories of crime Although there is an extensive literature proposing and testing several general theories of crime, these have been primarily developed and tested with male prisoners and delin- quents. These have not, however, been as rigorously studied and tested on women 1 Marshall University, Huntington, WV, USA Corresponding Author: Marc A. Lindberg, Department of Psychology, Marshall University, 1 Marshall Drive, Huntington, WV 25755, USA. Email: [email protected] 590809IJO XX X 10.1177/0306624X15590809International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative CriminologyLindberg et al. research-article 2015 by guest on June 20, 2015 ijo.sagepub.com Downloaded from

Tests of Theories of Crime in Female Prisoners

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

International Journal ofOffender Therapy and

Comparative Criminology 1 –28

© The Author(s) 2015Reprints and permissions:

sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav DOI: 10.1177/0306624X15590809

ijo.sagepub.com

Article

Tests of Theories of Crime in Female Prisoners: Social Bond and Control, Risk Taking, and Dynamic Systems Theories

Marc A. Lindberg1, April Fugett1, Ashtin Adkins1, and Kelsey Cook1

AbstractSeveral general theories of crime were tested with path models on 293 female prisoners in a U.S. State prison. The theories tested included Social Bond and Control, Thrill/Risk Seeking, and a new attachment-based Developmental Dynamic Systems model. A large battery of different instruments ranging from measures of risk taking, to a crime addiction scale, to Childhood Adverse Events, to attachments and clinical issues were used. The older general theories of crime did not hold up well under the rigor of path modeling. The new dynamic systems model was supported that incorporated adverse childhood events leading to (a) peer crime, (b) crime addiction, and (c) a measure derived from the Attachment and Clinical Issues Questionnaire (ACIQ) that takes individual differences in attachments and clinical issues into account. The results were discussed in terms of new approaches to Research Defined Criteria of Diagnosis (RDoC) and new approaches to intervention.

Keywordsfemale prisoners, attachment and crime, risk seeking theories of crime, social bond theories, ACIQ, RDoC diagnoses, individual versus group analyses, dynamic systems theories of crime

Although there is an extensive literature proposing and testing several general theories of crime, these have been primarily developed and tested with male prisoners and delin-quents. These have not, however, been as rigorously studied and tested on women

1Marshall University, Huntington, WV, USA

Corresponding Author:Marc A. Lindberg, Department of Psychology, Marshall University, 1 Marshall Drive, Huntington, WV 25755, USA. Email: [email protected]

590809 IJOXXX10.1177/0306624X15590809International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative CriminologyLindberg et al.research-article2015

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

2 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

prisoners and this neglect seems to have been most severe for the classic Social Bond and Control theory of crime and Thrill/Risk Seeking theories. Thus, it was the purpose of the present investigation to explore the applicability of these criminological theories to females and to test a new one coming from developmental dynamic systems approaches. We will now turn to brief discussions of the theories and why these tests are important in dealing with the complexities involved in reducing rates of recidivism.

Social Bond and Control Theories

A criminological theory that focuses on the importance of relationships in relation to criminal behavior is the social bond theory. Hirschi’s (1969) original theory suggested that delinquent behaviors begin when social bonds are poor or absent, which in turn lead to weaker levels of control. Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) went on to amend this theory, suggesting that there are three major components to criminality: self-control, parental management, and opportunities for deviance. The basis for self-control was theorized to develop within the family through proper supervision and monitoring. They suggested that in the absence of proper parental management, low self-control can develop, leaving one with an increased likelihood of engaging in criminal acts (Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990). Therefore, according to Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) theory, it is predicted that those who experienced poor parental management and chaotic family environments as a child will have low self-control. This, along with the presence of criminal opportunities to commit crime, is theorized to produce indi-viduals who are more likely to engage in criminal activities. This model therefore predicts that adverse childhood events like parental neglect and abuse, and growing up in families with few rules and regulations should lead to one’s development of crimi-nal thoughts and behaviors. Because the Adverse Childhood Events Questionnaire (ACE; Felitti et al., 1998) has scales specific to parental abuse and neglect, it was selected as a measure to test this theory in this investigation. Furthermore, because the Attachment and Clinical Issues Questionnaire (ACIQ; Lindberg & Thomas, 2011) contains the family rigidity versus chaos scale measuring the extent to which families have and follow rules, it was also selected for use in testing this model.

It should be noted that the low self-control theory is not always clear on whether there is a separation between a need and motivation for control or if it is just an expres-sion of whether or not it is just a behavioral tendency. Because it has been reasoned that the behavioral measures are circular tautologies (Akers, 1991; Geis, 2008; Grieger, Hosser, Alexander, & Schmidt, 2012), where lack of control is measured in terms of criminal and delinquent behaviors, and because these two different meanings of con-trol have been used by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990), it was reasoned that the more appropriate measure should tap desire for control. This measure of motivation for control and rules has been precisely measured by the Control scale of the ACIQ that looks at one’s desire for control which according to Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) is interpreted as coming from family rules, a lack of parental abuse and neglect, as well as a lack of exposure to peers who do not control themselves as measured by their delinquent behavior.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 3

Thrill/Risk Seeking Theories

Another set of theories regarding the key causes of crime comes from the sensation seeking and risk taking models of desistance. This approach suggests that individuals prone to desistance are born with a tendency to like the risks and thrills they can get from engaging in dangerous behaviors (Zuckerman, 1994). In support of this, it has been found that risk taking behaviors, such as driving in a reckless manner, engaging in violent and power sports, taking drugs, engaging in promiscuous sex, and gam-bling are all correlated with one another (Endresen & Olweus, 2005; Zuckerman, 2007). Zuckerman (2007) has reasoned that because crime and delinquency are also correlated with the above mentioned risky behaviors, there must be a common under-lying factor. This common factor was labeled “sensation seeking.” Sensation seeking is theorized to be a trait defined as the seeking of varied, novel, complex, and intense situations and experiences. Zuckerman (2007) has theorized that sensation seeking is associated with a general deviance factor that includes drug use, sexual risks, and disobedience of the law. He also suggested that the use of alcohol and drugs increases criminal behavior because they have a disinhibiting effect on this risky behavior, and because drugs and gambling require money to sustain them. Thus, they act in feed-back loops to increase criminal tendencies that were primed by the sensation seeking tendencies.

In expanding Zuckerman’s (2007) theory, Desrichard and Denarie (2005) asked what could be responsible for this incentive to seek out more stimulation. They found that risk taking is driven by negative affectivity such as high levels of anxiety and shame leading one to be more likely to seek out intense stimulation to achieve positive affect. If this model is correct, then it is predicted here that Shame and Anxiety would lead to crime because they are theorized to be indicators of negative feelings of self and things in general. Thus, the Anxiety and Shame scales on the ACIQ should predict Sensation Seeking. Sensation Seeking, in turn, would then predict reports of criminal behavior as well as scores on screening measures for alcohol and drug dependence. Furthermore, alcohol and drug scores would also be theorized to predict an increase in reports of criminal behaviors through disinhibition, and also serve as a feedback loop to crime increasing its occurrence because of the increased need for money to purchase the alcohol and drugs.

The Attachment and Developmental Systems Theory of Crime

There are clear theoretical and empirical findings that point to an attachment hypoth-esis of criminal behavior (Belsky, 1999a, 1999b; Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Fearon, Bakermans-Kranenburg, IJzendoorn, Lapsley, & Roisman, 2010; Fearon & Belsky, 2011; Hoeve et al., 2012; Lindberg, Fugett, & Lounder, 2014; Wampler & Downs, 2010). However, the Girl’s Study Group by Zahn and colleagues (2010) concluded that “Findings on attachment, although commonly more associated with girls than boys, are inconsistent across studies, in part because the concept is difficult to

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

4 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

measure” (p. 1). This problem of measurement was addressed by the ACIQ (Fugett, Thomas, & Lindberg, 2014; Lindberg, Fugett, & Thomas, 2012; Lindberg & Thomas, 2011). The ACIQ battery of 29 scales measures attachments to a variety of different significant others, assumes dynamic systems theories of development, and taps clini-cal issues that can be related to these attachment patterns.

Why would insecure attachments be one of the most important factors in the development of criminal behavior? First, it has been demonstrated that attachment processes play an important role in the development of parental compliance and authority, and conscience development (Thompson, 2009; van Ijzendoorn, 1997). Second, the hallmark of insecure attachment is that insecure patterns do not lead one to go to secure others for comfort when dealing with negative affect (Cassidy & Shaver, 2008) which has been theorized to be fundamental to the development of criminal behavior (Hirschi, 1969). Furthermore, if one has insecure attachments and a variety of clinical issues such as jealousy, abusiveness, anger, and so on, then they will have difficulties fitting into and being accepted by prosocial peer groups. Furthermore, as Coid (1992) and Deklyen and Greenberg (2008) have pointed out, insecure attachments negatively influence the child’s “theory of mind,” which ren-ders infants and adults poor at inferring their own or others’ emotions and mental states (Fonagy et al., 1997). This failure to understand not only their own but also others’ emotions and mental states has been shown to be related to the inability to empathize with others (Adshead, 2002; Hoffman, 2000). Attachment relationships to partners are also seen as essential ingredients in how one deals with negative affect and unpredictability (Lindberg et al., 2011; Lindberg et al., 2012; Simpson, Collins, & Salvatori, 2011). These insecure relationships developing in adolescence are theo-rized to add to the problems associated with emotional regulation and criminal behav-ior (Sampson & Laub, 1993, 2005). Thus, in line with Sampson and Laub (1993, 2005), it is held that one must focus on a variety of social networks in creating devel-opmental pathways (equifinality) to different (multifinality) trajectories toward ver-sus away from criminal and psychopathologic behaviors across the life span (Rowe, Osgood, & Nicewander, 1990; Schreck, Stewart, & Osgood, 2008; Thronberry & Krohn, 2005). For those not familiar with developmental systems theories, “equifi-nality” is the principle that in open biological systems, similar results may be achieved with different initial conditions and “multifinality” states that they can result in many different manifestations (Cicchetti & Aber, 1998; Granic & Hollenstein, 2003; Thelen & Smith, 1998). Thus, criminals can reach these end states through many different means (different kinds of insecure attachment patterns and clinical issues), and these can be manifested in many ways such as assault, robbery, and so on. In line with the above, the place where all low self-control theories depart from this one is in mea-surement and then the theoretical and pragmatic implications of stipulating that it is just not a “strength of bond” variable, but rather, “types of bonds” that are most influ-ential and that several different kinds of insecure bonds can eventuate in criminal behavior. This points to the importance of individual differences and it is also pre-dicted that data from the individual profiles will demonstrate this and represents the most significant point of departure of the present theory.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 5

Although the above attachment theory predicts who will be most vulnerable to crime and psychopathology, one also needs to account for why some turn to criminal behavior and why others do not. It is held here that criminal behavior is a way of achieving feelings of power, excitement, and control, and that when faced with nega-tive affect, can serve to hijack the system of insecurely attached individuals who are genetically prone (Olsson et al., 2005) to this stimulation and who do not get these feelings when turning to others (Belsky, 1999a, 1999b; Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Fearon et al., 2010; Fearon & Belsky, 2011). This part of the present dynamic systems theory is closely tied with the physiological literature on addictive substances and behaviors. According to the dopamine hypothesis (Lindberg, Dementieva, & Cavender, 2011; Volkow, Fowler, & Wang, 2003; Volkow, Ruben, Baler, & Goldstein, 2011), those with deficiencies of dopamine (the abused and insecurely attached) are more likely to turn to addictive substances and behaviors that take over the central nervous system and flood receptor cites normally stimulated by food and normal social stimulation (Zuckerman, ; 1994; 2007). Furthermore, these types of sensation seeking behaviors are theorized to overshadow turning to others to modulate feelings of security (Lindberg et al., 2011; Volkow et al., 2003; Volkow et al., 2011). If these individuals would turn to others, they would also have a variety of attachment-related clinical issues, such as anxiety, jealousy, anger, rumination, abusiveness, shame, control, denial of feelings, perfectionism, and/or mistrust that would increase rather than decrease the stress and coping with negative affect (Lindberg et al., 2012). The stron-ger the clinical issues and insecure attachments, the more likely one would turn to crime. Furthermore, if they did turn to others, their insecure attachment behaviors and internal working models would not be effective in affect regulation through these means creating an overshadowing phenomenon where the thrills, excitement, and power felt from criminal activity would produce stronger positive affect versus the negative affect of damaging relationships as suggested by Hirschi (1969). Choosing crime over alcohol, drugs, and/or other addictions would be partially determined by the level of peer crime and the individual’s attraction and addiction to criminal sensa-tion seeking, as measured by a crime addiction scale.

Although attachment and clinical issues are considered to be fundamental to the development of criminal behavior (Lindberg et al., 2013; van Ijzendoorn, 1997) dynamic systems and life course history approaches (Belsky & Pluess, 2009; Bjorklund, Ellis, & Rosenberg, 2007) have suggested that this will only be one of the significant paths one must travel on the way to a criminal lifestyle. Ellis and Bjorklund (2012) Moffit (2005, 2006), and Lindberg et al. (2014) have emphasized the role of stressful environments occurring early in development as well as the adaptive functions of crime, such as the inherent power, thrills, and excitement from committing the crime.

To summarize, these attachment, physiological, and life course theories when taken together suggest that early stressful environments are the first part of the trajectory toward crime. The troubling effects of these early environments are then predicted to be exacerbated by the following: (a) higher levels of insecure attachments and clinical issues; (b) epigenetic increases in the expression of a genotype that predisposes one to

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

6 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

find criminal behavior fun, exciting, and power inducing as measured by a crime addiction scale; and (c) exposure to peer criminal behavior when growing up. These three in turn would be significant predictors of reported criminal behavior.

The purpose of the present investigation was to test the above three models of crime in more rigorous path analyses. The models by Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) and Zuckerman (1994, 2007) are hypothesized to not hold up well under the rigors of path modeling. Although these social control and risk theories capture important ingredients in the development of criminal behavior, neither captures the important interactions, individual differences, and other salient features. Thus, it is hypothe-sized that the current attachment and dynamic systems theory offered here will pro-vide a significant improvement in predicting criminal behavior and be the best model in terms of fit and amount of variance accounted for. However, it is also hypothesized that most of the scales of the ACIQ will correlate significantly with total crimes reported, but that these correlations will be low because of the individual differences in developmental trajectories toward crime (equifinality). Finally, when examining the individual profiles of criminals all scoring high on the same crime such as assault, the participants will not all fit these patterns. Rather, it is hypothesized that they will show large individual differences in attachment patterns and clinical issues, and it will be seen that the present approach offers a way to move from group to individual analyses in interventions.

Method

Participants

Of the 348 females who were recruited on a volunteer basis from the Lakin Women’s Correctional Center in Point Pleasant, West Virginia, 20 individuals became confused with the numbering of the questions, 24 did not finish for unknown reasons, and 11 did not consent to participate. Thus, 293 questionnaires were utilized for the subsequent analyses. All inmates who were not in detention for recent infractions were brought to the room for possible testing. At the request of the institution, if they did not want to participate, they were told to just sit quietly and relax. Two proctors roamed the room to personally and quietly answer questions. Participants were told to not put their names on anything so that anonymity could be safeguarded and so that no one could know who said what. The major risk in this investigation was a breach of anonymity. To protect anonymity and comply with institution and institutional review board (IRB) requirements, no individual criminal records were reviewed. The research was approved by the Office of Research Integrity.

The following demographic information was gathered from the participating female inmates. In all, 272 (93.79%) participants identified as female, with 14 (4.83%) iden-tifying as male and 4 (1.37%) identifying as other. Notably, some of the females were apparently undergoing hormone therapy and thought of themselves as male. All of the data were included. Age was specified in terms of a Likert-type scale range where 178 (60.96%) identified as being between the ages of 22 and 35; 86 (29.45%) identified as

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 7

being between the ages of 36 and 49; 26 (8.90%) identified as being between the ages of 50 and 65; and none of the participants identified as being 66 or above. In addition, 101 (36.59%) participants identified their father’s highest level of education as being high school, 95 (34.42%) noted their father obtained a 3-11th grade education, 45(16.30%) endorsed some college, 23 (8.33%) endorsed their father being a college graduate, and 12 (4.35%) identified their father as attending graduate school. Ninety-six (33.33), 90 (31.25%), 61 (21.18%), 34 (11.81), and 7 (2.43) identified their moth-ers level of education as being a high school graduate, 3rd to 11th grade, some college, college graduate, and graduate school, respectively.

The majority of inmates were Caucasian (266, 90.78%), with 14 (4.78) identifying as African American, 5 (1.71%) identifying as Native American, 5 (1.71%) identifying as other, and 3 (1.02%) identifying as Hispanic. Seven (2.4%) attended graduate school, 19 (6.51%) were college graduates, 103 had some college level education (35.27%), 90 (30.82%) were high school graduates, and 73 (25.00%) did not complete high school.

Dependent variable measure. The key dependent variables used in this investigation were participant reports of numbers of arrests for different kinds of crimes committed (see Lindberg et al., 2013, for validity checks of this measure against more objective measures). Although a major problem with this was that subjective rather than the more objective measures were used, it also had several advantages. The perceived advantages were that such anonymous reports could increase honesty and reliability of reporting and reduce the fear that one’s scores could be linked to other measures pro-vided. Furthermore, to comply with IRB protocol, case records were not used. See Table 1 for each type of reported crime.

Instruments

The following measures were administered, totaling 319 questions.

ACIQ. The ACIQ (Lindberg & Thomas, 2011; Lindberg et al., 2012) is a test battery containing 29 scales measuring attachment and related clinical issues. It includes scales measuring avoidant, anxious resistant, codependent/preoccupied, and secure attachments to mother, father, and partner. The instrument’s use of continuous scales allows for the avoidance of problems associated with typologies (Osgood, 2005) and conceptualizes attachment as a multidimensional phenomenon (Lindberg & Thomas, 2011). Furthermore, it has scales tapping relations with peers, religious organizations, two family scales, and two sex scales. It also contains the clinical scales of shame, mistrust, jealousy, withdrawal, control, denial of feelings, anxiety, anger, perfection-ism, abusiveness, and rumination. It also has excellent fake good and fake bad scales, and it is fairly immune from social desirability as measured by the Marlow Crowne scale (Fugett et al., 2014). Finally, it contains method malingering scales, wherein one can determine whether an individual is taking the test via a random response set by just filling in answers without reading or understanding the questions (Fugett et al., 2014).

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

8 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

In initial studies, the scales of the ACIQ have been shown to have good coeffi-cient alphas of .79 (Lindberg & Thomas, 2011), and the average coefficient alpha for this sample was .81. Factor analytic studies on the scales have shown that they load on attachment figures and clinical issues rather than attachment styles as have been assumed by traditional attachment researchers (Lindberg & Thomas, 2011). It has also been found to predict to whom one turns in times of stress and also show substantial correlations with mother and father warmth (Lindberg et al., 2012), showing concurrent and discriminant validity as compared with the Experiences in Close Relationships Questionnaire. Furthermore, the attachment scales have been shown to be good predictors of the effects of divorce, with the attachment scales carrying the weight over divorce in predicting problems in adolescence in regres-sion analyses (Lindberg, McMillion, & Thomas, 1999). It has also successfully predicted scores on the Beck Depression Inventory (Taylor & Lindberg, 2006), alcoholics versus controls and adolescents predicted to be alcoholics (Lindberg, Fugett & Carter, in press), as well as eating disorders versus controls (Lindberg, Thomas, & Smith, 2004). It has also successfully predicted numbers of crimes com-mitted among youthful male offenders at a maximum-security prison (Lindberg et al., 2014). Finally, it offers individual profiles that highlight important individual differences necessary to treatment.

The “deviation statistic” incorporating individual differences. One thing that must be considered is how different insecure attachments and clinical issues can be entered into a path if there are so many individual differences leading to crime. A significant problem associated with dynamic systems theories and the construct of equifinality is that if there are many different trajectories to crime, and so many types of insecurity that can do this, then how can one provide a summary statistic that captures degrees of deviation from the norm? For example, one can conceptualize that either high scores on preoccupied mother (depicting enmeshment) or very low scores on this scale (the participant did not care about the feelings of their mother at all) could both be pre-dictive of crime. Absolute difference scores were therefore calculated to attempt to measure degrees of deviation from the norms of Lindberg and Thomas (2011). It was predicted that the stronger the deviations, the more likely that the participant would engage in crime. This “spread from the mean” score would then make it possible to use

Table 1. Frequencies of the Different Crimes Reported.

Frequencies 0 arrests 1 arrest 2-4 arrests 5-8 arrests 9+ arrests

Sex crimes 275 5 0 2 1bManslaughter 242 47 0 2 1Drug crimes 153 71 55 7 0Fraud 170 71 41 8 3Burglary 214 63 14 1 1Assault 186 61 34 6 6

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 9

this as a general measure of degree of dysfunctional attachments and clinical issues. This statistic was entered into the path.

CAGE and D-CAGE Questionnaires. The CAGE is named for its four questions that per-tain to Cutting back on drinking, feeling Annoyed that others ask you to cut back on drinking, feeling Guilty about drinking, and needing a drink first thing in the morning as an Eye opener. The CAGE is a brief screening measure used to detect alcoholism, and it has been found to reliably detect alcoholism among both general and clinical populations (Aertgeerts, Buntinx, Fevery, & Ansoms, 2000; Ewing, 1984; Fiellin, Reid, & O’Connor, 2000; Mayfield, McLeod, & Hall, 1974; Reynaud, Schwan, Loi-seaux-Meunier, Albuisson, & Deteix, 2001). The CAGE has demonstrated high test–retest reliability (.80-.95), and adequate correlations (.48-.70) with other screening instruments (Dhalla & Kopec, 2007). The questionnaire has been found to be a valid tool for detecting alcohol abuse and dependence in medical and surgical inpatients, ambulatory medical patients, and psychiatric inpatients (average sensitivity 0.71, specificity 0.90; Dhalla & Kopec, 2007). Furthermore, research has revealed that there are no tests that are as short as the CAGE, while still being as accurate in detecting substance use, and that longer tests, such as the Michigan Alcohol Screening Test (MAST) are no more accurate in predicting substance use (Brown, Leonard, Saunders, & Papasouliotis, 2001). Accuracy rates for the CAGE are variable and range from 60% to 95% and specificity rates for the CAGE range from 40% to 90% (Brown et al., 2001). In this sample, the coefficient alpha was .84.

To perform a similar measure for drug use the D-CAGE was used where alcohol was substituted for drugs. The D-CAGE adaptation for detecting drug use has been demonstrated to have reliability, accuracy, and specificity rates that are similar to the original version of the CAGE (Brown et al., 2001). The following statements were rated according to a Likert-type scale: I feel like I should cut down on my drug use; People annoy me by criticizing my drug use; I feel bad or guilty about my drug use; I have used drugs to improve my mood or steady my nerves. In this sample, the coeffi-cient alpha for the D-CAGE was .85.

ACE. The ACE contains several questions pertaining to aversive childhood experi-ences, such as recurrent physical abuse, emotional abuse, and sexual abuse. In addi-tion, it contains questions about whether the household in which a person grew up contained an alcohol or drug abuser, someone who engaged in criminal behavior, and/or someone with a mental illness, as well as questions pertaining to abuse and neglect. Research has found that ACE scores are positively correlated with social, emotional, and cognitive impairments, as well as risky health behaviors (Anda et al., 2006; Felitti et al., 1998; Hillis, Anda, Felitti, & Marchbanks, 2010; Weiss & Wag-ner, 1998). The ACE has also been found to be related to criminal behavior as would be predicted by many of the aforementioned theories of crime (Friestad, 2012; Fri-estad, Ase-Bente, & Kjelsber, 2012; Sharp, Peck, & Hartsfield, 2012). For the pur-pose of this study, the questions on the ACE were adapted to a Likert-type scale format to increase the psychometric sensitivity of the different subscales of the ACE,

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

10 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

where 1 = never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often, 4 = very often. The coefficient alpha for the ACE in this sample was .93.

Brief Sensation Seeking Screening–4 (BSSS-4). The BSSS-4 is a short assessment tool pertaining to an individual’s level of sensation seeking. The BSSS-4 is strongly cor-related with longer sensation seeking measures, such as the BSSS and the SS2, and has predicted risk taking behaviors among those of different ages, particularly with regard to risk taking behaviors pertaining to drug and alcohol (Stephenson, Hoyle, Palmgreen, & Slater, 2003). The average coefficient alpha for this sample was .87

Crime Addiction Scale. Related to this theoretical construct is the Crime Addic-tive Scale developed here. Its items are designed to more specifically target the above notions of sensation seeking but contextualizing the intensity of the sen-sation or thrill one gets out of engaging in their most frequent type of criminal behavior. The four stems following the question of “When you were just beginning your most typical kind of crime,” were (a) how much fun did you think you were going to have, (b) how exciting was it, (c) how much power did you feel, and (d) how much did you think you were going to gain? The purpose of these questions was to test a more specific type of addictions hypothesis suggesting that the degree of positive stimulation from criminal behavior is the addictive element predicting degrees of desistance. This is conceptualized as the phenotypic expression of the genotypes underlying increased trajectories toward crime. The average coefficient alpha for this sample was .92.

Procedures

Participants were tested in groups of approximately 25 to 35 over the course of 2 days. Each group was read each item on the questionnaire by a test proctor, and the first 10 items were read twice. The participants were asked not to work ahead, and at least one proctor observed. The additional proctor(s) also was available to answer any questions that were raised. The participants answered each question on a scantron answer sheet. The scantrons were numbered, and no identifying information was obtained. The test-ing process took 1 hr and 15 min. The scantron bubble answer sheets were scored electronically.

Results

The first set of analyses involved the use of Path analyses to test the general theories of crime outlined in the introduction. Path analysis is a theory-driven variant of struc-tural equation modeling (SEM). It uses a confirmatory hypothesis testing approach to the multivariate analysis of a structural theory based on certain circumstances (Byrne, 1998). In this study, the large number of scales and the medium sample size allowed the use of path analyses to calculate the path estimates on each of the mediational models (Byrne, 1998).

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 11

Social Bond and Control Theories

Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) theory was analyzed by a PROC CALIS correla-tional procedure using SAS to conduct a mediational path analysis to determine the causal effect among the above variables, measured in terms of the ACE, the Family Rigidity versus Chaos ACIQ scale, the ACIQ Control Scale, with the total Crimes reported variable serving as the dependent variable to test Gottfredson and Hirschi’s model of crime. Overall, the model did not yield a good fit across measures: χ2(2) = 11.92, p < .01, good of fit index (GFI) = .98, root mean square error of approxima-tion (RMSEA) estimate = .14. Approximately 12.87% of the variance in Control was explained by the exogenous variables (ACE, Family Rigidity vs. Chaos) in the model. In addition, approximately 8.29% of the variance in Crime was explained by the predictor variables (ACE, Family Rigidity vs. Chaos, Control) A path diagram with standardized estimates can be viewed in Figure 1. While all three paths were significant (as can be seen in Figure 1), the overall model fell short in terms of mul-tiple fit indices.

Thrill/Risk Seeking Theories

As with the previous model, Zuckerman’s (2007) model was tested by a PROC CALIS correlational procedure using SAS was used to conduct a mediational path analysis to test the hypothesized links among the variables Shame, Anxiety, Alcohol Use (CAGE), Drug Use (D-CAGE), Risk, and Crime. Overall, the model did not yield a good fit across all measures: χ2(5) = 27.72, p < .01, GFI = .97, RMSEA estimate = .13. Because the overall model did not have a good fit, additional analyses were not completed. A path diagram with standardized estimates and significance marked can be viewed in Figure 2.

Figure 1. Standardized results for PATH list mediational model of Gottfredson and Hirschi (1990) for all crime.*p < .05 level.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

12 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

The Attachment and Dynamic Systems Theory of Crime

The SAS PROC CALIS correlational procedure was also used to conduct a media-tional path analysis to determine the causal effects among the ACE, Fun Crime, Peer Crime, the ACIQ, and Crime. Several different measures of fit are reported for each model (see McDonald and Ho, 2002, for a discussion on indices and a general discus-sion of Path and SEM). Overall, the model had a good fit: χ2(2) = .34, p = .84, GFI = .999, RMSEA estimate = .00. Approximately 38.73% of the variance in Peer Crime was explained by the exogenous (ACE, Fun Crime, ACIQ) variables in the model. The exogenous variable (ACE) also accounted for 11% of the variance in Fun Crime and 12.88% in the ACIQ. In addition, approximately 18.48% of the variance in Crime was explained by the predictor variables (Fun Crime, Peer Crime, ACIQ, ACE). A path diagram with standardized estimates and significance marked can be viewed in Figure 3. All paths were significant with the exception of the path from the ACIQ to Peer Crime.

To test the importance of individual differences as predicted by dynamic systems theories’ emphases on equifinality, the ACIQ profiles of those who scored high on crime of assault are presented in Figures 4 to 7. Only those reporting being arrested 5 to 8 times or arrested 9+ times were examined to see whether they all exhibited the same characteristics on the ACIQ. When reading the figures, it is important to note that each profile for each prisoner is based on the normalized data of Lindberg and Thomas (2011). Based on this large control population, the mean is 100, and the standard devi-ation is 15. The number next to the standard score it is the percentile rank. The graph to the right then shows the score on the scale with 95% confidence intervals around it such that individual graphic representations are easily viewed by the clinician or researcher. To examine the rest of the profile data, email the senior author to see how

Figure 2. Standardized results for path list mediational model of Zuckerman (2007) for all crime.*p < .05 level.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 13

diverse and divergent data were and that the profiles presented here are typical of the kinds of individual differences found in this population.

A second and more direct way of exploring equifinality is by examining individ-ual ACIQ profiles of those who scored high on a particular crime like assault. If “unifinality” is the rule where profiling, personality theories, and older theories of criminology apply, then one would predict strong similarities in terms of the same kinds of attachment patterns and clinical issues evident in individuals’ high on one type of crime. A dynamic systems theory emphasizing equifinality, however, would predict that different kinds of insecurity and clinical issues would be evident in the profiles of those reporting high levels of the same crime. It is to these individual profiles that we now turn.

The first inmate in Figure 4 scored high (operationally defined here as a standard deviation above the mean) on the Avoidant and Ambivalent Mother, Father, and Partner scales. This fits well with classic attachment theory and research has shown that Avoidant individuals to be more likely to engage in assaults and be bullies (Berlin, Cassidy, & Appleyard, 2008). This individual also scored over a standard deviation higher on the scales of Sexual Arousal, Family Suppression of Feelings, Withdrawal, Denial of Feelings, Control, Shame, Anxiety, Anger, and Abusiveness. Thus, this pro-file is almost a classic representation of the literature testing groups at the nomothetic level. However, no other profiles perfectly matching this one were found, and as will be seen, this has important implications for intervention.

Consider next the profile in Figure 5. In contrast to the first profile, this individual was not Avoidant to either Mother or Father, but rather, would be scored as Mixed or disorganized to these figures where one is scored as mixed if they score a standard deviation or higher on two incompatible scales. For this participant, the two incompat-ible scales a standard deviation above the mean were Codependent and Ambivalent. Mixed attachments have also been implicated in cases of mental illness, anger, assault,

Figure 3. Standardized results for path list mediational model offered here.*p < .05 level.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

14

Examinee 4144

SCALE SCORE PCTL PROFILE

========== ===== ==== ======================================================================================================

50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

MOM-SECURE 63 * 2 ^ (-^--X------^-----) ^

^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 146 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ (---------^-----X---)

-CODEP. 87 *19 ^ ^ ^

(-^------X--^---------|-) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 154 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ ^ (------^---------X

DAD SECURE 71 * 3 ^ ^ (----^X--------^--) ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 119 88* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (---^--------X^------) ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 82 *12 ^

^ ^ (-----^-X-------^------) | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 125 93* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (--^---------^----X----^-) ^ ^

PRTNR-SECURE 102 54

^ ^ ^ ^ (---------|-X-------^--) ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 136 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (--^---------^-----X---^) ^

-CODEP. 109 72 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (---|--------X^--------)^ ^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 136 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (-^---------^-----X---^)

^SXTOT 113 77* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (---------^--X------^-) ^ ^ ^

SARTOT 136 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (-------^---------^-----X--)^ ^

JEALTOT 107 68 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (----|------X--^------) ^ ^ ^ ^

FRVCTOT 99 49 ^ ^ ^ ^ (----^--------X|---------^---) ^

^ ^ ^

FSUPTOT 131 96* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (---^---------^---------^X--) ^ ^

RELGTOT 98 42 ^ ^ ^ ^ (---^-------X-|---------^)

^ ^ ^ ^

PEERTOT 99 45 ^ ^ ^ ^ (-^--------X|--------)^ ^ ^ ^ ^

WIENTOT 122 92* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-------^---------^-X------)^ ^ ^

DENTOT 125 93* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (----^---------^----X----^) ^ ^

TRTOT 108 73 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^(--------|-------X-^--------)^ ^ ^ ^

OBCOTOT 115 83* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ |(--------^----X----^--) ^ ^ ^

CTRLTOT 117 85* ^ ^ ^ ^

^ (-|---------^------X--^----) ^ ^ ^

PERFTOT 90 25 ^ ^ ^ (^---------X---------|-----) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

SHATOT 123 92* ^ ^ ^

^ ^ | (----^---------^--X------) ^ ^

ANXTOT 127 94* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-^---------^------X--^--) ^ ^

ANGTOT 151 99* ^ ^

^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ (---^---------^---------X

ABRTOT 135 97* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^(--------^---------^----X--) ^ ^

FAKING 52 * 1 ^ X ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

RANDOM 92 32 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ X | ^ ^ ^ ^

^50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

Number of omissions: 0

Fig

ure

4. P

rofil

e of

a fe

mal

e cr

imin

al s

cori

ng h

igh

on t

he c

rim

e of

abu

se—

Exam

inee

414

4.N

ote.

Num

ber

of o

mis

sion

s =

0. P

aren

thes

es a

roun

d ea

ch “

x” a

re 9

5% c

onfid

ence

inte

rval

s.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

15

EXAMINEE: 6185

SCALE SCORE PCTL PROFILE

========== ===== ==== ======================================================================================================

50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

MOM-SECURE 102 53 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (------|-X-------^)

^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 105 71 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-----|----X----^---) ^ ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 119 90* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (------^--------X^------) ^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 126 93* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (---------^-----X---^-) ^ ^

DAD SECURE 106 61 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (--|-----X---^---) ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 111 77* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (|---------^X--------) ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 121 92* ^ ^ ^ ^

^ | (---^---------^X-------)^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 121 90* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (------^---------^X------) ^ ^ ^

PRTNR-SECURE 84 *16 ^ ^ ^ (---^---X-----^--------)| ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 124 93* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-^---------^---X-----^) ^ ^

-CODEP. 124 94* ^ ^

^ ^ ^ | (-^---------^---X-----^) ^ ^

-AMBIV. 133 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (---^---------^--X-----)^ ^

SXTOT 113 77* ^

^ ^ ^ ^ (---------^--X------^-) ^ ^ ^

SARTOT 112 81* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (----|---------^-X-------^-) ^ ^ ^

JEALTOT 133 98*

^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (---^---------^--X-----)^ ^

FRVCTOT 88 *20 ^ ^ ^ (-^-------X-^---------|------) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

FSUPTOT 111 79* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (------|---------^X--------^) ^ ^ ^

RELGTOT 98 42 ^ ^ ^ ^ (---^-------X-|---------^) ^ ^ ^ ^

PEERTOT

67 * 2 ^ ^ (----X--^---------^--) ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

WIENTOT 143 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (---^---------^---------^--X ^

DENTOT 140 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (--^---------^---------X--) ^

TRTOT 114 82* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (----|---------^---X-----^--) ^ ^ ^

OBCOTOT 126 95* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^(--------^-----X---^--) ^ ^

CTRLTOT 113 79* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (---|---------^--X------^--) ^ ^ ^

PERFTOT 127 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (---^---------^------X--^--) ^ ^

SHATOT 143 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^(--------^---------^--X--) ^

ANXTOT 127 94* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-^---------^------X--^--) ^ ^

ANGTOT 110 79* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (----|---------X---------) ^ ^ ^

ABRTOT 93 36 ^ ^ ^ ^ (-------^--X------|--------)^ ^ ^ ^ ^

FAKING 71 * 3 ^ ^ ^X ^ ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

RANDOM 108 73 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | X ^ ^ ^ ^

^50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

Number of omissions: 0

Fig

ure

5. P

rofil

e of

a fe

mal

e cr

imin

al s

cori

ng h

igh

on t

he c

rim

e of

abu

se—

Exam

inee

618

5.N

ote.

Num

ber

of o

mis

sion

s =

0. P

aren

thes

es a

roun

d ea

ch “

x” a

re 9

5% c

onfid

ence

inte

rval

s.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

16

EXAMINEE: 61176

SCALE SCORE PCTL PROFILE

========== ===== ==== ======================================================================================================

50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

MOM-SECURE 86 *20 ^ ^ ^ (^-----X---^------) | ^ ^ ^ ^

^-AVOID. 102 66 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-------|-X-------^-) ^ ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 113 82* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-|---------^--X------^-) ^

^ ^

-AMBIV. 113 80* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (|---------^--X------^-) ^ ^ ^

DAD SECURE 89 *24 ^ ^ ^ ^(-------X^-------) | ^ ^

^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 104 67 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (------|---X-----^---) ^ ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 106 66 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-----|-----X---^-----) ^ ^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 97 49 ^ ^ ^ ^ (----^------X--|---------) ^ ^ ^ ^

PRTNR-SECURE 84 *16 ^ ^ ^ (---^---X-----^--------)|

^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 101 56 ^ ^ ^ ^ (^---------|X--------^-) ^ ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 124 94* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-^---------^---X-----^) ^ ^

-AMBIV. 128 96* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (-------^-------X-^---) ^

^SXTOT 113 77* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (---------^--X------^-) ^ ^ ^

SARTOT 85 *16 ^ ^ ^ (---^----X----^---------|--) ^ ^ ^

^ ^

JEALTOT 110 73 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (--|---------X--------)^ ^ ^ ^

FRVCTOT 110 76* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-------|---------X---------^)

^ ^ ^

FSUPTOT 117 85* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (--|---------^------X--^----) ^ ^ ^

RELGTOT 115 81* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (|---------^----X----^---) ^ ^ ^

PEERTOT 65 * 1 ^ (----X----^---------^) ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

WIENTOT 147 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ |

^ (^---------^---------^-----)X ^

DENTOT 120 89* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-------^---------X-------) ^ ^ ^

TRTOT 132 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

| (--^---------^---------^-X--) ^ ^

OBCOTOT 95 42 ^ ^ ^ ^ (-----^----X----|------) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

CTRLTOT 142 99* ^ ^ ^ ^

^ | ^ (---^---------^---------^-X) ^

PERFTOT 114 80* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (---|---------^---X-----^--) ^ ^ ^

SHATOT 92 35 ^ ^ ^

^ (-------^-X-------|------) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

ANXTOT 104 63 ^ ^ ^ ^ (---------|---X-----^----) ^ ^ ^ ^

ANGTOT 122 91* ^ ^

^ ^ ^ | (-----^---------^-X-------) ^ ^

ABRTOT 121 89* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ |(--------^---------^X------) ^ ^ ^

FAKING 81 *10 ^

^ ^ ^X ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

RANDOM 115 84* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ X ^ ^ ^

^50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

Number of omissions: 0

Fig

ure

6. P

rofil

e of

a fe

mal

e cr

imin

al s

cori

ng h

igh

on t

he c

rim

e of

abu

se—

Exam

inee

611

76.

Not

e. N

umbe

r of

om

issi

ons

= 0

. Par

enth

eses

aro

und

each

“x”

are

95%

con

fiden

ce in

terv

als.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

17

EXAMINEE: 61264

SCALE SCORE PCTL PROFILE

========== ===== ==== ======================================================================================================

50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

MOM-SECURE 112 71 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-------^-X-------) ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 121 90* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-^---------^X------) ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 110 75 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (----|---------X---------) ^ ^ ^

-AMBIV. 113 80* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (|---------^--X------^-) ^ ^ ^

DAD SECURE 120 91* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

| (^---------X------) ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 98 54 ^ ^ ^ ^ (-^-------X-|-------) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-CODEP. 127 96* ^ ^ ^ ^

^ | ^(--------^------X--^---) ^ ^

-AMBIV. 109 75 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-----|--------X^--------)^ ^ ^ ^

PRTNR-SECURE 97 44 ^ ^ ^

^ (---^------X--|--------)^ ^ ^ ^ ^

-AVOID. 142 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ (-------^---------^-X--) ^

-CODEP. 131 98* ^ ^

^ ^ ^ | ^ (------^---------^X----) ^ ^

-AMBIV. 149 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ (^---------^--------X)

SXTOT 116 86* ^

^ ^ ^ ^ | (-------^-----X---^---) ^ ^ ^

SARTOT 125 94* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-----^---------^----X----^) ^ ^

JEALTOT 131 97*

^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (-----^---------^X-----) ^ ^

FRVCTOT 116 86* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (----|---------^-----X---^---) ^ ^ ^

FSUPTOT 84 *14 ^ ^ ^ (----^---X-----^---------|---) ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

RELGTOT 106 63 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (------|-----X---^------) ^ ^ ^ ^

PEERTOT

123 96* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (^---------^--X------) ^ ^

WIENTOT 126 95* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | (-----^---------^-----X---^) ^ ^

DENTOT 115 83* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-|---------^----X----^---) ^ ^ ^

TRTOT 117 88* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (--|---------^------X--^----) ^ ^ ^

OBCOTOT 135 98* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (-^---------^----X----) ^

CTRLTOT 152 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ ^ (------^---------^---------X

PERFTOT 110 74 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-----|---------X---------^) ^ ^ ^

SHATOT 106 70 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (-------|-----X---^------) ^ ^ ^ ^

ANXTOT 107 74 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ (------|------X--^-------) ^ ^ ^ ^

ANGTOT 138 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (---^---------^-------X-^) ^

ABRTOT 138 99* ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ | ^ (------^---------^-------X-) ^

FAKING 76 * 6 ^ ^ ^ X ^ ^ | ^ ^ ^ ^ ^

RANDOM 96 45 ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ X | ^ ^ ^ ^

^50 60 70 80 90 100 110 120 130 140 150

Number of omissions: 2

Fig

ure

7. P

rofil

e of

a fe

mal

e cr

imin

al s

cori

ng h

igh

on t

he c

rim

e of

abu

se—

Exam

inee

612

64.

Not

e. N

umbe

r of

om

issi

ons

= 2

. Par

enth

eses

aro

und

each

“x”

are

95%

con

fiden

ce in

terv

als.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

18 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

and criminality (Lindberg et al., 2014; Lyons-Ruth & Jacobvitz, 2008). However, this is a different kind of attachment pattern with different kinds of attachment thoughts, feelings, and behaviors than those of one who is just Avoidant. It also needs to be pointed out that this individual in Figure 5 did not score a standard deviation above the mean of the group on Anger or Abusiveness. However, this individual did score very high on measures of Jealousy, Denial of Feelings, Rumination, Anxiety, and Shame, and very low on Peer Support.

The profile in Figure 6 did not display the same high scores of insecurity to Mother or Father, but did show a mixed pattern to Partner fitting quite nicely with the sugges-tions of Sampson, Laub, and Wimer’s (2006) developmental theory suggesting that partner relations are important in carving one into and out of lives of crime. This indi-vidual scored high on Anger, Abusiveness, Family Suppression of feelings, Denial of Feelings, Withdrawal and Mistrust and very low on Peer Support. This might be more in line with the social cognitive information processing theories of Dodge and Coie (1987) where interventions are geared toward helping such individuals develop more accurate theories of mind. However, this is still different than the other profiles pre-sented thus far.

The profile in Figure 7 shows an individual who scored as Avoidant to Mother, Mixed toward Father with the interesting Secure/Codependent-Preoccupied Attachment patterns, and Mixed Avoidant, Codependent, and Ambivalent Attachments toward Partner revealing three very different attachment patterns to significant others. She also scored high on Sexual Intimacy, Sexual Arousal, Jealousy, Family Rigidity versus Chaos, Withdrawal, Denial of Feelings, Peer Support, Mistrust, Rumination, Control, Anger, and Abusiveness. Figure 7, therefore, represents a female who has many issues.

Discussion

Social Bond and Control Theories

While Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) overall model did not yield a good fit, it should be noted that “control” operated in an opposite fashion to what was predicted by Gottfredson and Hirschi. Why did we empirically find the opposite result to what was expected by theory and some past data? The answer is to be found in the different mea-sures of “control” used in this discipline. According to Grieger et al. (2012), in the criminology literature, self-control has been conceptualized in a multitude of ways (e.g., Gottfredson & Hirschi, 1990; Moffitt et al., 2011). Grieger et al. further reasoned that the measurement of self-control has been typically done by using either attitudes or behaviors analogous to the criminal behavior it is supposed to be predicting. Thus, if one wants to predict stealing, one should not use a measure of stealing as a sign of lack of impulse control, as is typically done in this literature. This would be circular, or tau-tologous, reasoning (Akers, 1991; Geis, 2008). Furthermore, the imprudent behaviors used in assessments of self-control correlate with one another only weakly (Tittle, Ward, & Grasmick, 2003a, 2003b), resulting in poor coefficient alphas of these vari-ables. It should be emphasized that the ACIQ measure of control was the participant’s

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 19

need for control. In summary, when “control” was measured as something where degrees of effortful regulation of the self by the self are used (Duckworth, 2011), then opposite results to what was predicted by Gottfredson and Hirschi were found. This goes beyond mere academic debate and goes to the heart of treatment as well. What is most important with the tests of this model was that those who say that control is lack-ing in criminals are either using the term in a circular fashion or they are wrong and dangerously so. In terms of intervention, it is dangerous to misdiagnose, and it is even more dangerous to misdiagnose in the opposite direction.

Thrill/Risk Seeking Theories

According to Zuckerman’s (2007) model of sensation seeking, high scores on mea-sures of Shame and Anxiety should lead to Sensation Seeking. Sensation seeking would then lead to Alcohol and Drug use as well as Criminal Behavior. Furthermore, Alcohol and Drug use would increase the Criminal Behaviors by having a disinhibit-ing effect on an individual. When tested with the more rigorous path modeling proce-dures, these hypothesized paths did not result in a significant model.

The Attachment and Dynamic Systems Theory of Crime

As predicted, the model that was significant was the attachment and dynamic systems theory offered here. It was found that the ACE contributed to the crime addiction scale, peer crime, and increased individual differences in attachment and clinical issues. These, in turn, together all led to increases in reports of criminal behavior. Thus, whereas the older models did not work when tested by the more rigorous path analy-ses, the present model did. (Note: It should be pointed out that tests of Agnew’s, 2005, strain theory and Sampson and Loeb’s, 2006, age graded theory were also performed with similar tepid results but space prohibits a full presentation. Write to the authors for these analyses.) In addition to providing the significant path model, the present developmental dynamic systems theory also suggests that individual differences will be the rule rather than the exception, and it is to these notions that we will now turn.

Analyses of Individual Differences and Equifinality

A feature of the developmental dynamic systems theory of Lindberg et al. (2014) and the path and theory presented here is the emphasis on equifinality. Equifinality sug-gests that there can be several different developmental trajectories that lead to similar developmental outcomes just as several different pathogens can lead to the same kinds of cancer. This suggests that different kinds of insecurity and clinical issues can lead to the same developmental outcome, in this case, crime.

A first prediction from this theory is that many different kinds of attachment inse-curity and clinical issues rather than just a few would correlate with arrest reports. From this, one would predict that there would be low but significant correlations between all the scales of the ACIQ and crime. Examining these correlations revealed

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

20 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

that in looking across all crimes and scales of the ACIQ, 28 of the 29 ACIQ scales had significant correlations with the crimes. (A copy of these can be obtained by emailing the authors.)

In summary, in looking over these and the rest of the individual profiles, and in line with Lindberg et al.’s (2014) emphasis on equifinality, the profiles clearly demon-strated that there were several different developmental trajectories in attachments and clinical issues that were associated with the crime of assault. No individual matched any other, and no individual perfectly fit any kind of group mean. This approach to theory suggests that different kinds of insecurity, not just one particular type of inse-curity, lead to the same developmental outcome, in this case, the crimes of assault. Thus, different patterns of insecurity and clinical issues lead to the same crime just like several different kinds of pathogens can lead to the same kind of cancer. Furthermore, in viewing the figures it is easy to see that none of the traditional models of criminol-ogy fit any of these participants very well. Furthermore, it is not hard to imagine how each of the above patterns of attachment and clinical issues eventuated in one who engaged in several criminal acts of assault. What was also striking was that not one of these individuals who gave valid responses was securely attached, which is similar to the findings of Lindberg et al. who tested adolescent males in a maximum-security prison.

This is similar to the call for dealing with individual differences as suggested by Andrews, Bonta, and Hoge (1990) and Andrews et al. (2011) who have proposed a Risk-Need-Responsivity (RNR) model of recidivism. While agreeing with this and other calls for attention to individual differences (van der Stouwe, Asscher, Stams, Dekovic, & van der Laan, 2014; Hoogsteder et al., 2014), the present approach goes one step further by not using demographic and other nomothetic variables. Rather, the present model and results suggest that attachment and clinical issues for each indi-vidual in different kinds of eco systems are the more proper focus of study. This also clarifies how one can perform more effective differential diagnoses for treatment and intervention. This is also more in line with the National Institute of Health movement toward Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) in diagnoses (Insel, 2013) rather than Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) taxonomies. This move away from the DSM is now required by the National Institutes of Health where mecha-nisms of change are emphasized (Insel, 2013), and the current data suggest how the ACIQ can provide such a required measure with its data on individual differences and mechanisms of action.

The above discussions also harken back to the old but still unresolved issues brought up by Cronbach’s (1957, 1975) notions of the two disciplines of psychology. According to Cronbach (1957), the correlational tradition was said to study variance among organisms while the experimental tradition was said to study variance among treatments. Cronbach (1957) went on to reason that if the traditions are kept indepen-dent, they can give only wrong answers or no answers at all. It is important to recon-sider the above in terms of how one can begin to provide ways of moving from this nomothetic level to the ideographic level deduced from the path analyses and pre-sented in the profiles and then go back again in nomothetic designs allowing for

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 21

testing of mechanisms of change harkened for years ago in our history. In the sections that follow on interventions, it will be seen that by dealing with the individual differ-ences found with the psychometrically derived individual differences, one can then test these using the nomothetic approach to see whether such attention to such indi-vidual differences improves rates of recidivism. Thus, we suggest taking the RNR model from Andrews et al. (2011) to its logical conclusion where true responsivity to individual differences will be measured and used instead of nomothetically derived variables.

Limitations and Caveats

One potential problem inherent in this and similar designs could be that these inmates did not take this seriously and did not fill out the questionnaires reliably or validly. This possibility was examined by looking at their coefficient alphas for each of the ACIQ and ACE scales. The results from this group were comparable with those of typical college students taking these instruments in classroom settings. Based on the similar high coefficient alphas between the two groups, it can be assumed that compli-ance was not an issue.

It could also be possible that the operationalized dependent variable measure of crime was not a robust or valid measure. For example, one could be concerned that self-reports on the number of total crimes for which one was arrested, might be an issue if the prisoners did not feel comfortable with full, and truthful, disclosures. While there are always concerns about self-reports, there is some evidence that this issue was not an overwhelming problem for this population. If one were mistrustful in reporting crimes, then it would be expected that the ACIQ scale of mistrust would correlate negatively with the crimes committed statistic. The opposite was found; as mistrust increased, reports of crimes also increased. Finally, this dependent variable has shown robust effects with other criminal populations and has passed several such validity tests (Lindberg et al., 2014.).

Finally, it might be reasoned that the independent variables used to measure the key constructs in social control and sensation seeking were not robust measures of the variables in question. However, an inspection of the general correlations provides evi-dence that seems to refute this hypothesis. (Interested readers can email the senior author for these data.) Basically, they show that the measures introduced here correlate at much higher levels than the more traditionally used demographic measures typically used in the criminology literature. Thus, all variables that have been considered in the past with the grand old theories worked; they just did not work as well as the measures used here, and even some of these like Control did not work in the precise predicted fashions as tested using the present more rigorous analyses.

Another limitation of this study was that which is inherent in any correlational investigation. That is, lives of crime might produce the attachment and clinical issues and reports of adverse childhood experiences instead of being produced by them. Path analyses, although more sophisticated, do not get around this traditional critique of these designs. Finally, the constructs selected to test the theories might not fully tap

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

22 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

what these theorists in criminology had in mind. These critiques must be kept in mind when considering the data and the rest of this discussion.

How does the present approach move from the above ideographic diagnoses to interventions that can be tested at the nomological level? It is to these most important issues in criminology, clinical psychology, and psychiatry that we now turn.

Implications, Interventions, and Future Directions

If individual differences are the rule rather than the exception as the path analyses and profiles suggest, then how can one design more effective early release and parole intervention programs that fit all these diverse perspectives and findings in an inte-grated empirically based program? Here we briefly outline an approach that might be able to combine these two traditions in a concrete example. Because it is axiomatic in most areas of medicine that effective treatments need effective differential diagnoses, one should begin with administration of the ACIQ to determine the most significant attachment and clinical issues for each individual. Therapy modules dealing with each of the 29+ issues and developmental processes involved for each issue (anger, mis-trust, shame, etc.) would then be put on secured web sites where the participant would have to perform daily exercises for each diagnosed issue over extended periods of time. However, the present systems approach and path analyses suggest that such ther-apy alone would be minimally effective if only performed in a clinic setting. Participants would also have to have three peer recovery, 12 step, and/or family mem-bers, support people with whom they discuss the modules during each week thereby helping them form new attachments and peer supports. Thus, these interventions also borrow from criminological theories and the present model that stress “carving the criminal and addict” out of their dysfunctional peer group environments (Sampson & Laub, 2005). The first modules worked on for all would be the crime addiction and drug/alcohol addiction modules. Such approaches to intervention should “put the com-mon factors that work best in therapy on steroids” (McLellan, McKay, Forman, Cacciola, & Kemp, 2010; Mee-Lee, McLellan, & Miller, 2010; Miller et al., 2010; Petersilia, 2004) because it would (a) focus on alliance focused processes to not only the clinician but also to three support individuals in the participant’s ecosystem in a coordinated scheme, (b) provide participant extensive feedback and monitoring through the computerized modules, (c) provide feedback and effective location moni-toring to the therapist with this at least twice per week decreasing safety concerns, (d) provide support people feedback about twice per week, (e) provide individualized plans depending on their most outstanding issues (f) use a coordinated and simplified “stages of change” for each issue worked in each module, and (g) provide heavy dos-ages at low cost. Thus, the present suggested intervention approach combines the com-mon factors known to produce change at a much smaller cost for higher dosage while increasing the safety concerns of the community.

The above approach focusing on individual differences can then be tested against more traditional approaches in nomothetic designs where mechanisms of change in terms of attachments, alliances, changes in secure base behaviors, changes in response

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 23

patterns to the most profound clinical issues for the individual, and so on can be tested. This should also address RDoC’s concerns of measuring mechanisms of change while giving impetus to go back to old unresolved issues in our history as well.

Summary

In summary, Gottfredson and Hirschi’s (1990) social bond and control theory and Zuckerman’s (2007) risk taking theory of criminology were tested using path model approaches. Although there were some marginally good fits, none were very robust. There was, however, a good fit for the attachment and dynamic systems path model showing how adverse childhood events lead to problems in attachment and clinical issues, increases in the crime addiction measure, and increases in peer crime exposure. This model was significant in predicting reports of criminal behavior. However, there were significant correlations between 28 of the 29 scales of the ACIQ and reported arrests suggesting several different causes of crime. An analysis of individual differ-ences in the profile data revealed that several different kinds of insecurity and clinical issues play different roles for different prisoners arrested for even the same kinds of crime. An intervention was suggested that utilized the differential diagnostics of the ACIQ in forging a new approach to intervention that might save money and increase safety for the community.

Acknowledgments

We thank Keith Beard for his work on editing and consulting on this manuscript.

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported in part by a grant to Marc A. Lindberg and April Fugett from the Justice Assistance Grant Program Division, Office of Research and Strategic Planning, Division of Justice and Community Services, State of West Virginia.

References

Adshead, G. (2002). Three degrees of security: Attachment and forensic institutions. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 12, 31-45.

Aertgeerts, B., Buntinx, F., Bande-Knops, J., Vandermeulen, C., Roelants, M., Ansoms, S., & Fevery, J. (2000). The value of CAGE, CUGE, and AUDIT in screening for alcohol abuse and dependence among college freshmen. Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 24, 53-57.

Agnew, R. (2005). Why do criminals offend? A general theory of crime and delinquency. Los Angeles, CA: Roxbury.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

24 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

Akers, R. L. (1991). Self-control as a general theory of crime. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 7, 201-212.

Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., Walker, J., Whitfield, C. L., Bremner, J. D., Perry, B. D., & Giles, W. H. (2006). The enduring effects of abuse and related adverse experiences in childhood: A convergence of evidence from neurobiology and epidemiology. European Archives of Psychiatry & Clinical Neurosciences, 256, 174-186.

Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., & Hoge, R. D. (1990). Classification for rehabilitation: Rediscovering psychology. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 17, 19-52. doi:10.1177/0093854890017001004

Andrews, D. A., Bonta, J., Wormith, S. J., Guzzo, L., Brews, A., Rettinger, J., & Rowe, R. (2011). Sources of variability in estimates of predictive validity: A specification with level of service risk and need. Criminal Justice and Behavior, 38, 413-432.

Belsky, J. (1999a). Interactional and contextual determinants of attachment security. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory and research (pp. 249-264). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Belsky, J. (1999b). Modern evolutionary theory and patterns of attachment. In J. Cassidy & P. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment theory and research (pp. 141-161). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environ-mental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135, 885-908.

Berlin, L. J., Cassidy, J., & Appleyard, K. (2008). The influence of early attachments on other relationships. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 333-347). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Bjorklund, D. F., Ellis, B. J., & Rosenberg, J. S. (2007). Evolved probabilistic cognitive mecha-nisms: An evolutionary approach to gene X environment X development interactions. In R. V. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 1-39). New York, NY: Elsevier.

Brown, R. L., Leonard, T., Saunders, L. A., & Papasouliotis, O. (2001). A two-item conjoint screen for alcohol and other drug problems. The Journal of the American Board of Family Practice, 14(2), 95-106.

Byrne, B. M. (1998). Structural equation modeling with Lisrel, Prelis, and Simplis: Basic con-cepts, applications, and programming. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. (2008). Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical appli-cations (2nd. ed.). New York: The Guilford Press.

Cicchetti, D., & Aber, J. L. (Eds.). (1998). Special issue: Contextualism and developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 10, 137-426.

Coid, J. (1992). DSM III diagnosis in criminal psychopaths. Criminal Behavior and Mental Health, 2, 78-95.

Crawford, J. R., & Henry, J. D. (2004). The positive and negative affect schedule (PANAS): Construct validity, measurement properties and normative data in a large non-clinical sam-ple. The British Psychological Society, 43, 245-265.

Cronbach, L. J. (1957). The two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 12, 671-684.

Cronbach, L. J. (1975). Beyond the two disciplines of scientific psychology. American Psychologist, 30, 116-127.

Deklyen, M., & Greenberg, M. T. (2008). Attachment and psychopathology in childhood. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver, (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 637-665). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 25

Desrichard, O., & Denarie, V. (2005). Sensation seeking and negative affectivity as predictors of risky behaviors: A distinction between occasional versus frequent risk-taking. Addictive Behaviors, 30, 1449-1453.

Dhalla, S., & Kopec, J. A. (2007). The CAGE questionnaire for alcohol misuse: A review of reliability and validity studies. Clinical and Investigative Medicine, 30, 33-41.

Dodge, K. A., & Coie, J. D. (1987). Social-information-processing factors in reactive and proac-tive aggression in children’s peer groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 1146-1158.

Dube, S. R., Felitti, V. J., Dong, M., Chapman, D. P., Giles, W. H., & Anda, R. F. (2003). Childhood abuse, neglect and household dysfunction and the risk of illicit drug use: The adverse childhood experience study. Pediatrics, 111, 564-572.

Duckworth, A. L. (2011). The significance of self-control. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108, 2639-2640.

Ellis, B. J., & Bjorklund, D. F. (2012). Beyond mental health: An evolutionary analysis of development under risky and supportive environmental conditions: An introduction to the special section. Developmental Psychology, 48, 591-597.

Endresen, I. M., & Olweus, D. (2005). Participation in power sports and antisocial involvement in preadolescent and adolescent boys. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46, 468-478.

Ewing, J. A. (1984). Detecting alcoholism: The CAGE Questionnaire. Journal of the American Medical Association, 252, 1905-1907.

Fearon, R. P., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., IJzendoorn, M. H., Lapsley, A.-M., & Roisman, G. I. (2010). The significance of insecure attachment and disorganization in the development of children’s externalizing behavior: A meta-analytic study. Child Development, 81, 435-456.

Fearon, R. P., & Belsky, J. (2011). Infant-mother attachment and the growth of externalizing problems across the primary-school years. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52, 782-791.

Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., . . . Marks, J. S. (1998). The relationship of adult health status to childhood abuse and house-hold dysfunction. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 245-258.

Fiellin, D. A., Reid, M. C., & O’Connor, P. G. (2000). Screening for alcohol problems in pri-mary care: A systematic review. Achieves of Internal Medicine, 160, 1977-1989.

Fonagy, P., Target, M., Steele, M., Leigh, T., Levinson, A., & Kennedy, R. (1997). Morality, disruptive behavior, borderline personality disorder, crime, and their relationship to secu-rity of attachment. In L. Atkinson & K. J. Zucker (Eds.), Attachment and psychopathology (pp. 223-274). London, England: Guilford Press.

Friestad, C. (2012). Making sense, making good, or making meaning? Cognitive distortions as targets of change in offender treatment. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 56, 465-482.

Friestad, C., Åse-Bente, R., & Kjelsberg, E. (2012). Adverse childhood experiences among women prisoners: Relationships to suicide attempts and drug abuse. International Journal of Social Psychiatry, 60 (1), 40-46.

Fugett, A., Thomas, S., & Lindberg, M. A. (2014). The many faces of malingering and par-ticipant response strategies: New methodologies in the Attachment and Clinical Issues Questionnaire (ACIQ). Journal of General Psychology, 141, 80-97.

Geis, G. (2008). Self-control: A hypercritical assessment. In E. Goode (Ed.), Out of control: Assessing the general theory of crime (pp. 203-216). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

26 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

Gottfredson, M. R., & Hirschi, T. (1990). A general theory of crime. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.

Granic, I., & Hollenstein, T. (2003). Dynamic systems methods for models of developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 641-669.

Grieger, L., Hosser, D., Alexander, F., & Schmidt, A. F. (2012). Predictive validity of self-reported self-control for different forms of recidivism. Journal of Criminal Psychology, 2, 80-95.

Hillis, S. D., Anda, R. F., Felitti, V. J., & Marchbanks, P. A. (2010). Adverse childhood experi-ences and sexual risk behaviors in women: A retrospective cohort study. Family Planning Perspectives, 33, 206-211.

Hirschi, T. (1969). Causes of delinquency. Berkeley: University of California Press.Hoeve, M., Stams, J. M., van der Put, C. E., Dubas, J. S., van der Laan, P. H., & Gerris, J. R.

M. (2012). A meta-analysis of attachment to parents and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40, 771-785.

Hoffman, M. L. (2000). Empathy and moral development. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Hoogsteder, L. M., Kuijpers, N., Stams, G. J. J. M., Van Horn, J. E., Hendricks, J., & Wissink, I. B. (2014). Study on the effectiveness of Responsive Aggression Regulation Therapy (Re-ART). International Journal of Forensic Mental Health, 13, 25-35.

Insel, T. (2013, April 29). Transforming diagnoses. NIMH Director’s Blog. Retrieved from NIMH.nih.gov/about/director. Also see [email protected]

Lindberg, M. A., Dementieva, Y., & Cavender, J. (2011). Why has the BMI gone up so drasti-cally in the last 35 years? Journal of Addictive Medicine, 5, 272-278.

Lindberg, M. A., Fugett, A., & Carter, J. E. (in press). Tests of the Attachment and Clinical Issues Questionnaire (ACIQ) as it applies to alcoholism. Journal of Addictive Medicine.

Lindberg, M. A., Fugett, A., & Lounder, L. (2014). The attachment and clinical issues ques-tionnaire: A new methodology for science and practice in criminology and forensics. International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology, 58, 1166-1185.

Lindberg, M. A., Fugett, A., & Thomas, S. W. (2012). Comparing measures of attachment: “To whom one turns in times of stress,” parental warmth, and partner satisfaction. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 172, 329-352.

Lindberg, M. A., & Thomas, S. (2011). The Attachment and Clinical Issues Questionnaire (ACIQ): Scale development. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 173, 41-62.

Lindberg, M. A., McMillion, L., & Thomas, S. (1999, April). Attachment dynamics of children of divorce as a function of age and gender. Paper presented at the Biannual Meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, NM.

Lindberg, M. A., Thomas, M., & Smith, L. (2004, May). Empirical support for an attachment hypothesis of eating disorders. Paper presented at the American Psychiatric Association, New York, NY.

Lyons-Ruth, K., & Jacobvitz, D. (2008). Attachment disorganization: Genetic factors, parent-ing contexts, and developmental transformation from infancy to adulthood. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 666-697). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Mayfield, D., McLeod, G., & Hall, P. (1974). The CAGE Questionnaire: Validation of a new alcoholism screening instrument. American Journal of Psychiatry, 131, 1121-1123.

McDonald, R. P., & Ho, M. H. R. (2002). Principles and practice in reporting structural equation modeling. Psychological Methods, 7, 64-82.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

Lindberg et al. 27

McLellan, A. T., McKay, J. R., Forman, R., Cacciola, J., & Kemp, J. (2010). Reconsidering the evaluation of addiction treatment: From retrospective follow-up to concurrent recovery monitoring. Addiction, 100, 447-458.

Mee-Lee, D., McLellan, T., & Miller, S. D. (2010). What works in substance abuse and depen-dence treatment. In B. L. Duncan, S. D. Miller, B. E. Wampold, & M. A. Hubble (Eds.), The heart and soul of change: What works in therapy (2nd ed., pp. 393-417). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Miller, S. D., Hubble, M. A., Duncan, B. L., & Wampold, B. E. (2010). Delivering what works. In B. L. Duncan, S. D. Miller, B. E. Wampold & M. A. Hubble (Eds.), The heart and soul of change: What works in therapy (2nd ed., pp. 421-429). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

Moffitt, T. E. (2005). The new look of behavioral genetics in developmental psychopathology: Gene environment interplay in antisocial behavior. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 533-554.

Moffitt, T. E. (2006). Life-course-persistent versus adolescence-limited antisocial behavior. In D. Cicchetti & Cohen (Eds.), Developmental psychopathology: vol. 3. Risk, disorder, and adaptation (2nd ed., pp. 570-598). Hobokken, NJ: John Wiley.

Moffitt, T. E., Arsenault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox, R. J., Harrington, H., & Caspi, A. (2011). A gradient of childhood self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety. In Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America (Vol. 10, pp. 2693-2698).

Olsson, C. A., Byrnes, G. B., Lotfi-Miri, M., Collins, V., Williamson, R., Patton, C., & Anney, R. J. (2005). Association between 5-HTTLPR genotypes and persisting patterns of anxiety and alcohol use: Results from a 10-year longitudinal study of adolescent mental health. Molecular Psychiatry, 10, 868-876.

Osgood, D. W. (2005). Making sense of crime and the life course. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science, 602, 196-211.

Petersilia, J. (2004). What works in prisoner reentry? Reviewing and questioning the evidence. Federal Probation, 68(2), 4-8.

Reynaud, M., Schwan, R., Loiseaux-Meunier, M. N., Albuisson, E., & Deteix, P. (2001). Patients admitted to emergency services for drunkenness: Moderate alcohol users or harm-ful drinkers? American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 96-99.

Rowe, D. C., Osgood, W., & Nicewander, A. W. (1990). A latent trait approach to unifying criminal careers. Criminology, 28, 237-270.

Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (1993). Crime in the making: Pathways and turning points through life. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Sampson, R. J., & Laub, J. H. (2005). A general age-graded theory of crime: Lessons learned and the future of life-course criminology. In D. Farrington (Ed.), Advances in Criminological Theory: Vol. 13. Testing integrated developmental/life course theories of offending (pp. 161-185). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers.

Sampson, R. J., Laub, J. H., & Wimer, C. (2006). Does marriage reduce crime? A counterfactual approach to within-individual causal effects. Criminology, 44, 465-508.

Schreck, C. J., Stewart, E. A., & Osgood, D. W. (2008). A reappraisal of the overlap of violent offenders and victims. Criminology, 46, 871-906.

Sharp, S. F., Peck, B. M., & Hartsfield, J. (2012). Childhood adversity and substance use of women prisoners: A general strain theory approach. Journal of Criminal Justice, 40, 202-211.

Simpson, J. A., Collins, W. A., & Salvatori, J. E. (2011). The impact of early interpersonal experience on adult romantic relationship functioning: Recent findings from the Minnesota

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from

28 International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology

Longitudinal Study of Risk and Adaptation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20, 355-359.

Stephenson, M. T., Hoyle, R. H., Palmgreen, B. P., & Slater, M. D. (2003). Brief measures of sensation seeking for screening and large-scale surveys. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 72, 279-286.

Taylor, J., & Lindberg, M. A. (2006, March). Analyzing depression from a developmental sys-tems perspective. Paper presented at the Conference on Human Development, Louisville, KY.

Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (1998). Dynamic systems theories. In W. Damon (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 1. Theoretical models of human development (5th ed., pp. 563-633). New York, NY: Wiley.

Thompson, R. A. (2009). Early attachment and later development: Familiar questions, new answers. In J. Cassidy & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), Handbook of attachment: Theory, research, and clinical applications (2nd ed., pp. 348-365). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Thornberry, T. P., & Krohn, M. D. (2005). Applying interactional theory to the explanation of continuity and change in antisocial behavior. In D. P. Farrington (Ed.), Integrated develop-mental and life course theories of offending: Advances in criminological theory( vol. 14, pp. 183-209). New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction.

Tittle, C. R., Ward, D. A., & Grasmick, H. G. (2003a). Gender, age, and crime/deviance: A chal-lenge to self-control theory. Journal of Research in Crime and Delinquency, 40, 426-453.

Tittle, C. R., Ward, D. A., & Grasmick, H. G. (2003b). Self-control and crime/deviance: Cognitive vs. behavioral measures. Journal of Quantitative Criminology, 19, 333-366.

van der Stouwe, T., Asscher, J. J., Stams, G. J. J. M., & Dekovic M., & van der Laan, P. H. (2014). The effectiveness of Multisystemic Therapy (MST): A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 34, 468-481. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2014.06.00610:1016/j.cpr.2014.06.006

van Ijzendoorn, M. H. (1997). Attachment, emergent morality, and aggression: Toward a developmental socioemotional model of antisocial behaviour. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 21, 703-727.

Volkow, N. D., Fowler, J. S., & Wang, G. J. (2003). The addicted human brain: Insights from imaging studies. Journal of Clinical Investigation, 111, 1444-1451.

Volkow, N. D., Ruben, D., Baler, R. D., & Goldstein, R. Z. (2011). Addiction: Pulling at the neural threads of social behaviors. Neuron, 69, 599-602.

Wampler, R., & Downs, A. (2010). Parent and peer attachment in minority males at high risk for delinquency. Journal of Clinical Social Work, 38, 107-119.

Weiss, J. S., & Wagner, S. H. (1998). What explains the negative consequences of adverse childhood experiences on adult health? Insights from cognitive and neuroscience research [Editorial]. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14, 356-360.

Zahn, M. A., Agnew, R., Fishbein, D., Miller, S., Winn, D.-M., Dakoff, G., . . . Chesney-Lind, M. (2010, April). Understanding and responding to girls’ delinquency, Girls’ Study Group. US Department of Justice. Retrieved from www.ojp.usdoj.gov/ojjdp

Zuckerman, M. (1994). Behavioral expressions and biosocial bases of sensation seeking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

Zuckerman, M. (2007). Sensation seeking and risky behavior. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

by guest on June 20, 2015ijo.sagepub.comDownloaded from