41
The Impact of Better-than-Average Bias and Relative Performance Pay on Performance Outcome Satisfaction Prior research in psychology has documented the existence and causes of the Better-than- Average (BTA) bias in individuals (Alicke, 1985; Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher, Yurak and Vredenburg, 1995). The BTA bias is an individual’s perceived ability to contribute to an organization relative to that of a peer. There is also an extensive literature on the topic of job satisfaction (Lawler 1971, 1990; Crosby 1976; Society for Human Resource Management [SHRM] 2012). Job satisfaction is a multi-faceted concept encompassing an individual’s satisfaction with various aspects of the job such as the job or the task itself, compensation, the job environment, safety, variety of work, supervision, training, knowledge and capabilities of the individual and management’s evaluation and recognition of the performance outcomes. Our study is an attempt to connect these two streams of research as posited in the following research questions: (1) Do entry-level accountants display a better-than-average (BTA) bias? (2) Does the BTA bias affect their perceived performance outcome satisfaction (POS)? (3) How is the impact of BTA bias on POS moderated by relative performance pay? Performance outcome satisfaction is a construct comprising of the following two components of job satisfaction: the satisfaction of an employee with his or her relative performance pay and his or her relative performance evaluation (SHRM 2008, 2012; Vidal and Nossol, 2011). The research questions we explore in our study were motivated by a number of factors. Recruitment and retention of qualified accountants is a growing challenge facing

Running head: PERFORMANCE OUTCOME SATISFACTION

  • Upload
    usask

  • View
    0

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

The Impact of Better-than-Average Bias and Relative Performance Pay on

Performance Outcome Satisfaction

Prior research in psychology has documented the existence and causes of the Better-than-

Average (BTA) bias in individuals (Alicke, 1985; Alicke, Klotz, Breitenbecher, Yurak

and Vredenburg, 1995). The BTA bias is an individual’s perceived ability to contribute to

an organization relative to that of a peer. There is also an extensive literature on the topic

of job satisfaction (Lawler 1971, 1990; Crosby 1976; Society for Human Resource

Management [SHRM] 2012). Job satisfaction is a multi-faceted concept encompassing an

individual’s satisfaction with various aspects of the job such as the job or the task itself,

compensation, the job environment, safety, variety of work, supervision, training,

knowledge and capabilities of the individual and management’s evaluation and

recognition of the performance outcomes. Our study is an attempt to connect these two

streams of research as posited in the following research questions:

(1) Do entry-level accountants display a better-than-average (BTA) bias?

(2) Does the BTA bias affect their perceived performance outcome satisfaction (POS)?

(3) How is the impact of BTA bias on POS moderated by relative performance pay?

Performance outcome satisfaction is a construct comprising of the following two

components of job satisfaction: the satisfaction of an employee with his or her relative

performance pay and his or her relative performance evaluation (SHRM 2008, 2012;

Vidal and Nossol, 2011).

The research questions we explore in our study were motivated by a number of

factors. Recruitment and retention of qualified accountants is a growing challenge facing

2

accounting firms and other organizations wanting to hire accountants (AICPA, 2003;

Lornic, 2006). The current market for those entering the profession has been termed as a

“buyer’s market” (Robinson, 2006: 47). In this type of environment organizations that are

successful in enabling their employees to achieve job satisfaction are more likely to

overcome the challenges of recruitment and retention.

An improved understanding of the factors which can affect the job satisfaction of

its employees can also help an organization avoid the negative consequences that can

result when employees are dissatisfied. Examples of negative consequences include

absenteeism, poor quality work, and an increase in employee turnover. The cost to the

accounting organization to address these types of challenges and restore customer

confidence can be significant.

There are several important factors that underscore our belief in the contextual

appropriateness and relevance of the accounting industry for our research. The existence

of the BTA bias in accountants and the impact of the BTA bias on the POS of

accountants are both open questions that need to be verified by empirical research and the

answers to which cannot be directly inferred from the results of prior research. The

practice of accounting and the nature of the accounting profession possess characteristics

that contribute to viable null hypotheses.

Like any other organization, an accounting organization has the need and desire to

understand the factors that can affect the performance outcome satisfaction of its

employees. Accounting organizations are also unique in that they sell professional

services—knowledge and expertise—with a cadre of employees who are a high

3

achieving, highly educated group with a clear sense of self identity. Accounting firms

represent knowledge-based organizations (Sheehan, Vaidyanathan and Kalagnanam,

2005), which rely exclusively upon the capabilities, knowledge and expertise of their

employees. Generally speaking like most other professionals accountants are likely to

have a heightened sense of their potential to contribute and a greater sense of self-worth,

and therefore have higher expectations of the performance pay that they should receive.

Consequently, the population of professional accountants is one in which we expect its

members to have strong perceptions that they are better-than-average when comparing

themselves to individuals from the general population. But will accountants exhibit a

BTA bias when the comparison other is a fellow accountant who has also undergone a

similar educational and professional training?

Professional accountants are trained to believe in the role and importance of

exercising professional judgment. Education and training as auditors and controllers will

have highlighted the imperfect nature of the judgement process endemic in performance

evaluation and in the application of accounting principles in decision making. The

consequence of this training and exposure to exercising and being subjected to

professional judgement is that the existence and the strength of the BTA bias is a-priori

an open question.

Second, the impact of the BTA bias on performance outcome satisfaction, a

priori, is also an open question worthy of empirical study. Performance evaluation in

knowledge-based organizations is largely subjective because the capabilities of the

employees and their application of those capabilities in performing their job are less

4

easily measurable. This implies that performance evaluations and the performance pay

may be less predictable resulting in a higher frequency of divergences between

expectations and actual outcomes. By contrast, in industrial organizations the knowledge

and expertise of frontline employees and their application of their capabilities in

performing their jobs is more easily measurable because the association between an

individual’s contribution and the output is better defined (Sheehan et al., 2005). This

implies that performance evaluations and performance pay may be more predictable in

this organizational environment and consequently there is limited scope for BTA bias to

impact POS and overall job satisfaction.

In addition to the foregoing, there has been a call for more research in the area of

performance pay in the accounting profession (Ross and Bomeli, 1971, Strawser,

Ivancevich and Lyons, 1969) and the economics of job satisfaction (Clark and Oswald

1996). Research outside of accounting has recently focused on the link between

performance pay and job satisfaction (Green and Heywood, 2008) but this research did

not consider the impact of BTA bias.

From a research motivational perspective, the possibility that the performance

evaluation process (Fletcher, 2001; Gabris and Ihrke, 2001; Sudin, 2011) for accounting

firms may produce undesirable results due to the presence of BTA bias among employees

is worthy of investigation. The potential findings of our study should be of interest to

management because when forewarned management may be able to take corrective

actions that are sensitive to the existence of the BTA bias among employees. For

example, if confirmed, the negative influence of BTA bias on POS is evidence that the

5

BTA bias should be on management’s radar. Similarly, a finding of positive influence of

relative performance pay on POS would be evidence that pay matters to accountants: the

higher the relative performance pay received, the higher the reported POS. Moreover, a

positive interaction effect if it exists would be evidence that relative performance pay can

be a strategic tool that management can use to positively influence at least two

components of job satisfaction or to at least counteract the potential negative influence of

the BTA bias on job satisfaction.

To summarize, our study seeks to contribute to the literature on the use of

performance pay by focusing on the antecedents of performance outcome satisfaction

(POS), an important component of job satisfaction. Our study is unique in that our

measure of an individual’s perceived contribution focuses on the capabilities side through

the use of the BTA bias rather than the output side such as an individual’s productivity.

We used a hybrid research method comprised of a survey followed by an

experiment involving two case studies and concluding with a questionnaire. We used the

survey to collect BTA scores and establish the existence of BTA bias among the study’s

participants. The purpose of the experiment was to provide us with performance outcome

satisfaction (POS) data on the participants. In the experiment we asked each participant to

read two case studies which placed our study’s participants in a realistic scenario

occurring in an accounting firm.

Our results provide strong evidence and support for the first two questions, and

moderate support for the third. The results indicate that (1) entry-level accountants

6

exhibit a BTA bias, (2) BTA bias negatively influences POS and (3) relative performance

pay moderates the negative impact of BTA bias on POS.

The paper is organized as follows. In the next section we develop our hypotheses,

following which we describe our research methodology and then present our results.

Finally, we conclude the paper with a summary of our findings and their potential

implications. We also discuss some of the study’s limitations and provide suggestions for

future research.

THEORY AND HYPOTHESES DEVELOPMENT

Pay for performance is considered a critical component of an organization’s management

control system (Kaplan and Norton, 2001; Merchant and Van der Stede, 2003; Otley,

2003) and is widely used in organizations. For example, Long (2002) reported that 94%

of Canadian organizations use some type of performance pay, with individual

performance pay being the most common (88%). Organizations use pay for performance

to increase goal congruence, employee motivation and ultimately job satisfaction

(Lawler, 1971; Long, 2002, 2010; Otley, 2003; SHRM, 2012).

Recent surveys by the Canadian Institute of Chartered Accountants (CICA) of its

members indicate that compensation, including performance pay, ranks among the top

five factors that respondents consider important (CICA, 2006, 2007). In fact, it can

matter to such an extent that, according to Parker and Kohlmeyer (2005) and the SHRM

survey (2012), negative perceptions regarding pay can lead to lower job satisfaction,

decreased organizational commitment and increased turnover in organizations. However,

7

recent research also suggests that the role of pay for performance as it pertains to job

satisfaction has not been resolved (Green and Heywood 2008).

Job satisfaction is a multi-dimensional construct which pertains to satisfaction

along several dimensions such as (1) performance evaluation, (2) performance pay, (3)

promotion, (4) supervisors, (5) co-workers, (6) work environment and (7) the job itself

(Smith, Kendall and Hulin, 1969). Lawler’s (1971, 1990) research suggests that job

dissatisfaction most likely occurs when an individual receives less pay than what he/she

expects to receive. Moreover, respondents to a job satisfaction survey conducted by the

Society for Human Resource Management (SHRM) ranked Compensation/Pay among

the top three factors contributing to job satisfaction in seven out of ten years between

2002 and 2012 and among the top five in all ten years. For example, in 2008 (2012) 92%

(98%) of the respondents rated compensation/pay as important or very important.

Additionally, 37% (39%) rated opportunities for variable pay (including bonuses,

commissions and other forms) as very important. Similarly, 82% (90%) of the

respondents to the survey in 2008 (2012) rated management’s recognition of employee

performance as important or very important to achieving job satisfaction (SHRM, 2008,

2012). These results reinforce the importance of satisfaction with performance pay and

satisfaction with performance evaluation as critical dimensions of job satisfaction, and

our research focuses on these two dimensions.

Crosby’s (1976) research suggests that pay dissatisfaction occurs under six

different conditions: (1) when there is an inconsistency between the outcome individuals

desire and the amount they actually receive, (2) when they are aware that their peer (the

8

target used in their comparison) received more than they did; (3) when they believe that

they are entitled to more, (4) when, based on past experience, they expected more than

they received, (5) future expectations for achieving better outcomes are unlikely, and (6)

they absolve themselves of any personal responsibility for the lack of better outcomes.

In addition to the above Vroom (1964), Adams (1965), and Lawler (1973)

introduce an employee’s perception of his (or her) contribution and the reward they

receive (or do not receive) as determinants of job satisfaction. This is the key tenet of

equity theory. The theory suggests that most employees will not object to performance

pay if it is consistent with what they perceive as equitable, is in line with their

expectations, and is clearly in addition to their base pay. Schein (1965) states that when

individuals join an organization, they do so based on the expectations of their inputs or

contributions (i.e., effort), and the rewards (e.g., performance pay) they will receive for

those contributions. In doing so, they implicitly develop a contribution-reward ratio. The

literature on equity theory (Adams, 1965), informs us that an individual’s satisfaction

with performance pay is largely contingent upon the comparison(s) of his or her

perceived contribution-reward (C-R) ratio with his or her perception of the C-R ratio of

similar others. Accordingly if an individual’s perception of his or her C-R ratio is higher

than his or her perception of the C-R ratio of a peer, then the individual’s satisfaction

with the performance pay received will be lower than it would otherwise be. An

individual’s feeling regarding the link between his or her perceived relative ability to

contribute and reward will likely be stronger when there is a higher level of subjectivity

in the performance evaluation process. This is because subjectivity potentially reduces

9

the predictability of performance evaluations and rewards, thereby increasing the

potential for dissatisfaction following a performance evaluation.

In summary, individuals use at least two filters in determining whether the

contribution-reward ratio is fair. The first filter is an internal or intangible calculation of

the contributions made and rewards received. The second filter is an assessment of the

same contribution-reward ratio, but this time in comparison with select peers. This

contribution-reward ratio impacts an individual’s job satisfaction level. Recent research

also suggests that fairness of performance evaluation procedures has at least an indirect

effect on job satisfaction (Lau, Wong and Eggleton, 2008).

Equity theory, however, is silent on the factors that can affect an individual’s

perception of his or her C-R ratio relative to that of a peer. Expectancy theory (Vroom

1964), on the other hand, includes three components: expectancy, instrumentality and

valence. Expectancy includes self-efficacy, goal difficulty and control. Self-efficacy,

which is the individual’s belief about their ability to successfully perform a particular

behaviour, is particularly relevant to our paper; our variable ‘perceived relative ability to

contribute’ (i.e., BTA bias) is very similar in nature. Instrumentality is the belief that a

person will receive a reward if the expectation is met; the receipt of performance pay

aligns with this component of the theory. Valence is the value an individual places on

rewards; our POS variable attempts to capture valence.

Drawing upon equity and expectancy theories we develop our model (see Figure

1), wherein we establish a relationship between an individual’s perceived relative ability

to contribute and satisfaction with performance pay and satisfaction with performance

10

evaluation (i.e., POS). As shown in the lower half of Figure 1, we operationalize

perceived relative ability to contribute as the BTA bias.

The BTA bias was first reported by Alicke (1985) and subsequently by Dunning,

Meyerowitz and Holzberg (1989), Alicke et al. (1995) and Suls (2002). The existence of

BTA bias as first reported by Alicke was based on using 362 traits adopted from a list of

555 personality traits words used by Anderson (1968). Findings from the studies cited

above report the existence of the BTA bias, as well the fact that the degree of bias

diminishes as the comparison group becomes more defined. However, none of these

studies examined the potential impact of the BTA bias on job satisfaction (or its

individual components).

Individuals with a BTA bias are likely to make high assessments of their own

abilities relative to peers. Consequently such an individual will believe that they can

contribute more to the job than the peer and meet the expectations of the job, and also

receive a relatively more favourable performance evaluation and a reward; this aligns

with the component of instrumentality within expectancy theory.

How satisfied an individual is with their performance evaluation and performance

pay depends on the value they place on rewards; this aligns with the valence component

of expectancy theory. Our dependent variable, POS, indirectly measures the valence

component. Individuals with a higher degree of BTA bias who place a high value on

rewards are more likely to be dissatisfied with their performance outcomes, relative to

peers, because they have higher expectations. In other words, when BTA bias is high the

performance evaluation and performance pay received will likely be inconsistent with the

11

expectations that have been formed about them and consequently the POS will be low.

This leads to our first hypothesis:

H1. Performance outcome satisfaction (POS) is negatively related to BTA bias.

The second factor that can influence POS is the amount of performance pay (the

reward) received relative to that of a peer (Long, 2010; SHRM, 2012). High relative

performance pay can potentially narrow the gap between an individual’s expected and

actual performance outcome. This is because the individual perceives a higher relative

performance pay as an acceptable outcome given their perceptions about their own ability

to contribute and what they believe they should receive as a reward. Consequently, a high

relative performance pay will contribute to reducing the degree of inconsistency between

expected and actual performance outcome which, in turn, will result in a greater

likelihood of the individual reporting increased satisfaction with their performance

outcome. This leads to our second hypothesis.

H2. Performance outcome satisfaction (POS) is positively related to relative

performance pay.

It is possible for the relative performance pay received to mitigate the negative

impact of BTA bias; whether it does or not will likely depend upon the amount of the

performance pay (relative to what peers) received. While a substantially higher relative

reward may result in a significant increase in the level of POS sufficient to overcome the

negative effect of BTA bias on POS, a relative reward which is only slightly higher may

not have the same effect. In other words, the amount of the relative performance pay

received has the potential to temper the negative effect of BTA bias on POS. We

12

conjecture the existence of a positive interaction effect between BTA bias and relative

performance pay on POS; this leads to our third hypothesis.

H3. Relative performance pay positively interacts with BTA bias in influencing

POS.

RESEARCH METHODOLOGY

In this section we present the research methodology, including the study’s design, its

participants, and the variables used. Two groups of 80 and 84 (total 164) entry-level

accountants in public practice, who were also students in a graduate accounting program,

participated in the study. We adopted a hybrid design comprised of a survey followed by

an experiment involving two case studies and concluding with a post-experimental

questionnaire.1

Survey

We used the survey to collect BTA scores and establish the existence of BTA

bias. Participants assessed themselves, relative to two target peer groups, on the 24 traits

identified by Libby and Thorne (2004) as essential for auditors to possess (see Table 1).

The two peer groups were as follows: (1) a particular individual representing an

immediate work associate like a fellow classmate and (2) a general comparison target—

the average professional auditor—representing a broad comparison group. The use of two

1 Prior to designing and developing the instruments, we collected information from four senior managers,

two partners, and twelve entry-level accountants employed with four different accounting firms to enhance

the construct and face validity of the instrument planned for use in the study. The information we collected

concerned the frequency of performance pay usage for entry-level accountants, the person/position in the

firm responsible for determining the amount of performance pay that entry-level accountants receive, and a

description or list of the primary daily duties of entry-level accountants including the percentage of time

spent on audit related duties and tasks performed by them. We also collected information about the types

of metrics commonly used to evaluate entry-level performance when determining their performance pay

and the range of performance pay amounts used for entry-level accountants working in accounting firms.

13

peer groups was to confirm findings in prior research that the BTA bias diminishes as the

peer group becomes more focused (Alicke 1985; Suls 2002).

We use these specific traits because they are considered as important for auditors

to possess, i.e., they are relevant to the entry-level accountant’s job which is primarily

audit-related.2 Following from our discussion of expectancy theory in the previous

section, we believe that possessing these traits, rather than other general traits, will

potentially increase an individual’s self-efficacy with respect to the audit function. We

used a 9-point Likert scale anchored as follows: 0 = much less than the target group or

individual, 4 = about the same as the target group or individual, and 8 = much more than

the target group or individual.

As a form of control the order of the self-assessment peer groups was varied

between the surveys completed by the participants. The rationale for this manipulation in

the ordering sequence was to ensure construct validity of the BTA scores and the POS

measures we would be obtaining. By providing a well-defined and familiar peer, such as

a fellow classmate seated next to them, we believed that the BTA scores and the POS

measures would be more strongly anchored than if they were based on comparisons with

a general target group. At this point the participants were unaware that the primary BTA

scores used in our research study would be those that required them to compare

themselves specifically to their adjacently seated classmates.

2 According to Libby and Thorne (2004, 460) these traits are “… required, at minimum, to ensure auditors’

formulation of professional judgment in accordance with Generally Accepted Accounting Principles

(GAAP).” Moreover, results from our research conducted prior to developing the research design as well

as those from the post-experimental questionnaire confirmed that approximately 70% of the entry-level

accountant’s duties are audit related.

14

Furthermore we believed that this was more representative of the process that

would occur in accounting firms when performance evaluation outcomes were awarded.

It was likely that an employee would be more inclined to choose the peer to be a familiar

work associate similar to themselves rather than someone they did not know. It should be

noted that the participants in our study knew each other very well; this put them in a

better position to compare themselves with their similar other relative to the condition

used in Alicke (1985) where participants came to know their comparison other only

subsequent to a three-minute conversation with the comparison target with whom they

had not previously met. Consequently our experimental condition allows us to have a

greater degree of confidence in the BTA scores we elicited from the participants.

Moreover the fact that BTA bias exists despite each participant being strongly familiar

with the peer is very telling of one’s perceived self-efficacy relative to a peer.

Experiment

The purpose of the experiment was to provide us with performance outcome

satisfaction (POS) data. Each participant read two case studies which placed them in a

realistic scenario occurring in an accounting firm. Each scenario described the

participant’s performance as well as the performance of their immediate work associate

and the results of the subsequent performance evaluation including the amount of

performance pay awarded to the participant (the respondent) and his or her work

associate. The two cases differed only with respect to information about the amount of

the performance pay awarded to the participant (the respondent) and his or her immediate

work associate. In Case 1 (2), we stated that as an outcome of the performance

15

evaluation the participant received performance pay of $1,500 ($500) whereas the

participant’s immediate work associate (i.e., the individual they were asked to compare

themselves to) received $500 ($1,500).3 Thus in Case 1 (2) , the participant would be

able to infer that he or she received $1,000 more (less) than their immediate work

associate.

After reading each case study the participant responded to 31 questions using a 9-

point scale. The questions were distributed as follows: 16 pay and work related

questions, two manipulation check questions and 13 general information and background

type questions. The purpose of the questionnaire was to elicit the participants’ ratings of

their satisfaction with their performance pay, and the satisfaction with their performance

evaluation, which when combined provided us with a measure of their performance

outcome satisfaction (POS). To reduce the possibility of creating a demand effect we

randomly distributed the questions addressing the variables of interest throughout the

questionnaire section of the case study.

Post-Experimental Questionnaire

Finally, as a validation check, participants completed a questionnaire through

which we obtained their assessment of the importance of the 24 traits (Libby and Thorne

2004) that we use to measure BTA bias. Using a 10 point scale (anchored at both ends

by 0 = Not at All Important and 9 = Very Important), participants rated how important it

was for auditors to possess each of the 24 traits. In addition participants answered a few

general questions addressing demographic information.

3 These amounts were based on our research conducted prior to developing the experiment (see foot-note

#1).

16

Variables Used in the Study

The BTA bias is defined as the difference between their actual total BTA score and the

total BTA scale mean. In our scale, the maximum possible score for each trait is 8, so the

maximum total score for the 24 traits is 192 (24 × 8). The scale mean for each trait is 4,

so the total BTA scale mean is 96 (24 × 4). However, we use the total BTA scores as our

measure of the BTA bias to test the study’s hypotheses.

Relative amount of performance pay, the other independent variable, was

constructed as a dichotomous variable and was set at either an amount of $500 or $1,500

consistent with the information provided in the cases (coded as 0 and 1 for the purpose of

the analysis).

The dependant variable, POS, is constructed as the sum of an individual’s

satisfaction with his or her relative performance pay and the individual’s satisfaction with

his or her relative performance evaluation. The logic behind this construction of POS is

that in situations where pay for performance exists the overall outcome of the

performance evaluation process (i.e., performance outcome) consists of two separate, but

related, components: (1) the relative performance evaluation rating, i.e., how one’s

performance is rated relative to others, and (2) the consequence of the evaluation which is

a reward or a punishment (higher or lower relative performance pay).

Performance outcome, logically, cannot occur without both events having taken

place. Therefore, when attempting to ascertain an individual’s satisfaction with their

performance evaluation it would be inappropriate to elicit a response regarding

satisfaction with just the performance pay without allowing the respondent to associate

17

the amount with a performance evaluation that logically would have preceded the reward.

Similarly, it would be inappropriate to elicit a response regarding satisfaction with just

the performance evaluation rating without the respondent being able to associate that

evaluation with the relative amount of their performance pay. Consequently, we believe,

and have adopted the view of Green and Heywood (2008) that satisfaction with the

performance outcome cannot be established without considering both the satisfaction

with the evaluation rating and satisfaction with the consequence of the evaluation (i.e.,

the performance pay). The following comments provide assurance to our decision to

combine the effects of performance evaluation and the consequence of the evaluation into

a single variable (Vidal and Nossol, 2011, 1721):

In addition to the usual difficulties in obtaining data from organizations,

there is the additional difficulty that, when relative performance is

communicated in firms, it typically has explicit or implicit monetary

consequences. For instance, a worker being informed that he has

performed better than his colleagues may reasonably conclude that he has

a chance of receiving a promotion to a better paid position, which could

translate into higher effort. This makes it difficult to disentangle the direct

effect of relative performance feedback from the effect of the monetary

reward to which such feedback is often linked.

For this reason we provided participants both the information regarding the outcome of

their performance evaluation and the performance pay ($500 or $1,500) at the same time

(i.e., together).4

For the reasons cited above we do not deconstruct our POS construct and

analyze the impact of our independent variables upon each of its individual POS

4 We asked participants to provide separate satisfaction scores rather than just one because we were

concerned that the term “performance outcome” may have been confusing to the participants; to avoid any

such confusion and to ensure that the participants understood the questions asked of them we decided to

have them rate the two satisfaction scores separately with the plan of combining them into a single score

for the purposes of our analysis.

18

components separately (i.e., performance pay satisfaction and performance evaluation

satisfaction).

RESULTS

First we present general descriptive data regarding the participants, followed by the

results of our preliminary tests to examine: (1) how our participants rated the 24 traits

considered as important for auditors to possess and (2) the existence of the BTA bias.

Next we present the results for each of the three hypotheses in the order in which they

were posed. We use the data collected from all 164 participants in conducting our

preliminary tests and from 162 participants in testing our hypotheses (two participants

failed the manipulation check).

Prior to combining the data collected from the 80 and 84 participants (adding up

to 164), we did a number of tests to ensure that the data between the two groups were

compatible. We tested for differences in the BTA scores by gender, year of study (first

versus second year student), and alphabetical versus reverse alphabetical order of listing

the traits. We found no statistically significant differences between the two groups with

respect to gender, year of study, order of pay manipulation in the two cases (i.e., whether

the amount of performance pay in the first or second case was less or more than their

immediate work associate) and ordering of traits when they compared themselves to their

immediate work associate.5

5 We found a moderately statistically significant difference in the BTA scores between the two groups,

when comparing themselves to the average professional auditor, with respect to the ordering of the traits.

This difference could potentially impact our findings with respect to the second preliminary test; however,

it has no effect with respect to the results pertaining to the three hypotheses because the BTA scores we use

19

The average age of the 164 participants was 25.29 years; 97 (59%) were female,

and 67 (41%) were male. All but two were employed as entry-level accountants with an

average length of work experience of 13.1 months as accountants. One hundred and fifty

six (95.1%) participants indicated that they were employed in public practice. Moreover,

162 (98.8%) participants reported that they plan to pursue a professional accounting

designation. One hundred and twenty six participants (77.8%) reported that performance

pay was used in their accounting firms at the entry accountant level, and 145 (88.4%)

reported that performance pay was used at some level in the accounting firm where they

were employed. One hundred and twenty eight participants (78%) responded to the

question “generally speaking were you aware of the amount of performance pay received by

others working at the same level as you in your firm.” Of these 128 respondents, 91 (71.1%)

indicated that they were aware of the amount of performance pay received by others in

their accounting firm6. The descriptive statistics highlight both the predominant use of

performance pay and a fairly high level of awareness among participants of the amount of

performance pay received by their work associates. We believe that the combination of

both the high level of performance pay usage and general awareness of the amount of

performance pay received by others enhances the validity of our study.

Preliminary Tests

to test the hypotheses are those that participants report when comparing themselves to their immediate

work associate. 6 This finding is important as it demonstrates that even though the amount of performance pay awarded to

employees may not be publicly announced they are able to obtain information about the performance pay

received by others in their firm.

20

We conducted two preliminary tests to examine: (1) how our participants rated the 24

traits considered as important for auditors to possess and (2) the existence of a BTA bias

among our participants (entry-level accountants).

Importance of the 24 Traits

The 164 entry-level accountants rated each of the 24 traits on a 10 point scale (0 =

Not at All Important, 9 = Very Important). The results, as shown in Table 1, indicate that

entry-level accountants rated 21 of the 24 (91.7%) traits as important (i.e., they assigned a

score that was statistically significantly higher than the mid-point score of the scale (as

per Alicke et al. (1995)); the difference between mean scores of the 164 participants and

the mid-point of the scale was positive and statistically significant for 21 of the 24 traits.

The difference for the trait “benevolent” was positive, but not statistically significant (t =

0.576, p = 0.566). The trait “sensitive” had a negative difference but it was not

statistically significant (t = -0.574, p = 0.567). Finally the difference for the trait “warm,”

although statistically significantly different from zero, was negative thereby suggesting

that participants did not rate it as important (t = -2.579, p = 0.011). Collectively since 21

out of 24 traits were rated as important (i.e., statistically significant) we interpret this as

providing overall support for the findings of Libby and Thorne (2004), and ultimately

providing support for their usage in our study.

We also conducted reliability tests to ensure that we could add the individual

BTA scores for the original group of 24 traits as well as for the subset of 21 traits. Our

reliability tests resulted in Cronbach’s Alpha scores of (0.896 and 0.914) and (0.883 and

0.905) respectively, for comparisons with the average professional auditor and with their

21

immediate work associate. In their totality these results support the observation that the

24 (21) trait ratings, for each respective comparison group, consistently reflect the

construct (i.e., BTA bias) that it is measuring. Essentially we interpret the results to

indicate that the 24 (21) traits included in the instrument provide a reliable measure that

can be used to test for presence of the BTA bias. Nonetheless, as mentioned above, we

aggregate the BTA scores for the subset of 21 traits that were considered important by the

participants, and use these scores in all the subsequent tests.

Finally a t-test was performed on the entire set of the 21 traits which were rated as

important by the sample subjects, to examine their overall (combined) importance. The

results show that collectively the 21 traits were rated as statistically significant or

important (t = 8.536, p = 0.000), which again corroborates the findings of Libby and

Thorne (2004), and also provides additional support for using the 21 traits in our study to

investigate the presence of a BTA bias for entry-level accountants.

Existence of BTA Bias

Our next preliminary test investigates the existence of BTA bias among entry-

level accountants, and whether the degree of BTA bias diminishes as the group becomes

more focused. The method used in our study to address this question, similar in principle

to the one used by Alicke et al. (1995), involved computing, for each of the 164

participants, the BTA bias measure when participants compared themselves to the

average professional auditor and when they compared themselves to their immediate

work associate. Within each comparison group the BTA bias was measured as the

22

difference between the total BTA score for 21 traits and the total scale mean adding up to

84 (21 × 4).

The results presented in Table 2 provide evidence of a BTA bias regardless of the

comparison group (p = 0.000 for both comparison groups). We also find a statistically

significant difference between the entry-level accountant’s comparisons to the average

professional auditor and to their immediate work associate (t = 20.36, p = 0.000). This

finding suggests that the BTA bias diminishes as the peer group becomes more focused

(see Table 2). This finding was originally reported by Alicke (1985) and Alicke et al.

(1995) and our result corroborates that finding for the accounting profession.

Hypothesis Tests

H1 and H2 investigate whether the BTA bias and performance pay, respectively,

influence POS; H3 investigates the interaction effect. We used regression analysis to test

the three hypotheses. We used the following model for H1 and H2

iidummyiibiasdummyi eGenWkExpBTAPerfPayoPOS )()()()()(4321

where,

POS = Performance outcome satisfaction

PerfPaydummy = 0 (relatively lower pay) or 1 (relatively higher pay)

BTAbias = BTA bias

WkExp = Length of work experience in months

GENdummy = A gender dummy, 0 = male 1 = female

e = error term

23

The regression results, shown in Table 3 indicate that the model is statistically

significant (F = 293.883, p = 0.000), and that the adjusted R-squared is 78.4%. We also

find that both the performance pay (t = 34.044, p = 0.000) and the BTA bias (t = -3.057, p

= 0.000) are statistically significant, while gender (t = 0.114, p = 0.909) and length of

work experience (t = -0.492, p = 0.623) are not statistically significant. The statistically

significant negative coefficient of the BTA bias is evidence that H1 is supported. We

interpret this result to mean that as an individual’s degree of BTA bias increases, their

POS will decrease. The more an individual believes that their ability to contribute,

relative to peers, is higher (i.e., that they are better-than-average), the greater their

expectations about their performance outcome (i.e., evaluation ratings and performance

pay). At high levels of BTA, holding everything else constant, the satisfaction with the

performance outcome is likely to be lower. The positive coefficient value for

performance pay suggests that as performance pay increases, POS will also increase,

which supports H2.

To test H3, we expanded the previous model to include an interaction variable

between performance pay and BTA bias; the model is shown below.

The regression results in Table 4 indicate that the model is statistically significant

(F = 237.631, p = 0.000), and the adjusted R-squared is 78.6%. We find a significant

main effect for both performance pay (t = 2.842, p = 0.005) and the BTA bias (t = -4.113,

p = 0.000)). We also find a moderately statistically significant interaction effect (t =

i i bias i dummy

i dummy i i bias dummy i

e BTA PerfPay

Gen WkExp BTA PerfPay o POS

) * (

) ( ) ( ) ( ) ( ) (

5

4 3 2 1

24

1.866, p = 0.063). Gender (t = 0.114, p = 0.909) and length of work experience (t = -

0.494, p = 0.662) are not statistically significant. Our results for the interaction term

provide modest support for H3. The result suggests that the negative impact of BTA bias

on POS is somewhat tempered by the level of performance pay. A more detailed analysis

suggests that the difference between the satisfaction levels reported by the high BTA

group and the low BTA group is lower under the ‘high relative performance pay’

condition as compared to the same difference under the ‘low relative performance pay’

condition. In other words the difference in POS levels for the high BTA and the low BTA

groups, although present, is not as pronounced under the ‘high relative performance pay’

condition compared to the ‘low relative performance pay’ condition. This finding leads

us to believe that performance pay can be viewed as a moderating variable which

influences the relationship between BTA bias and POS.

ADDITIONAL TESTS

As noted previously, with regard to H3, the results show that there is modest statistical

support for this hypothesis (p = 0.063). To better understand this finding we tested H1

again, by regressing POS on BTA bias separately for each of the two performance pay

conditions (i.e., where the participant’s performance pay was less (more) than their

immediate work associate). Table 5 presents the results for these two regressions. Our

model for these two tests is as follows:

iidummyiibiasi eGenWkExpBTAoPOS )()()()(32

1

25

Table 5 indicates that the BTA bias significantly influences POS (t = -5.663, p =

0.000) in the condition where the relative performance pay is less than that of the

immediate work associate. We also find a significant effect for gender (t = -3.178, p =

0.002). The overall model is statistically significant (F = 13.870, p = 0.000), with an

adjusted R-squared of 19.3%. However, the test results shown in Table 5, for the

condition where the relative performance pay is more than that of the immediate work

associate, reveal that the BTA bias does not have a statistically significant effect on POS

(t = -1.150, p = 0.252). Similar to the previous test, we found a significant effect for

gender (t = 2.155, p = 0.033). We found this model to be moderately significant (F =

2.245, p = 0.085), and the adjusted R-squared was 2.3%.

A comparison of the above two results suggests an explanation for the moderate

significance of the interaction variable. Providing a relatively higher performance pay

somewhat mitigates the impact of BTA on POS. It would appear that when the relative

performance pay is larger it has the ability to confirm for individuals their perception that

they are better than average, and this BTA bias has a reduced influence on POS.

Collectively our results strongly support H1; the important point here is that when an

individual perceives their relative ability to contribute (proxied by their BTA bias) as

being higher, but is rewarded less than their immediate work associate (proxied by the

relative performance pay), they are less satisfied with their performance outcome (i.e.,

satisfaction with their performance evaluation and with their performance pay).

A question that may arise regarding our POS construct is whether an individual is

likely to rate their performance evaluation satisfaction as high (low) and their

26

performance pay satisfaction as low (high). This question could arise because participants

provided separate responses to each aspect of the overall performance evaluation. To

examine for the possibility of such an occurrence, we computed the Pearson correlation

between the two satisfaction ratings. Should individuals rate one of their satisfaction

scores high (low), and the other as low (high) the Pearson correlations should not be

significant. The Pearson correlations, between satisfaction with performance evaluation

and satisfaction with performance pay under the two conditions of performance pay,

show that in the condition where performance pay is lower (higher) than that of the

immediate work associate, the correlation coefficient was 0.458 (0.570). Both

coefficients are statistically significant at the 1% level (p = 0.01). This finding reinforces

the use of a single variable, POS, by combining the two separate but related components.

In addition to the correlation analysis, we tabulated the number of participants

who provided similar satisfaction ratings for each component of performance evaluation.

The results, in tables 6 and 7, show that the subjects are clustered in two groups: those

that are satisfied with both components and those that are dissatisfied with both

components. In the condition where pay was less than that of their immediate work

associate, 94% of the subjects were generally dissatisfied with both their performance

evaluation and the amount of their performance pay. Similarly, in the condition where

pay was greater than that of their immediate work associate, 70% of the subjects were by

and large satisfied with both their performance evaluation and the amount of their

performance pay. The combined results further support our use of the POS variable.

27

DISCUSSION

Our study focused on the link between an individual’s perceived ability to

contribute relative to their peers (operationalized as the better-than-average-or BTA-bias)

and the individual’s satisfaction with the outcomes of the performance evaluation process

(i.e., with the evaluation as well as the associated reward); we operationalized this

construct as performance outcome satisfaction (POS). We also examined the impact of

performance pay upon that link. The use of BTA bias as a proxy for an individual’s

relative ability to contribute is an important methodological contribution of this study.

We are not aware of any other study that has examined the relationship between BTA

bias and POS or used BTA scores as a proxy for BTA bias.

We found that in general our participants, i.e., entry-level accountants, rated

themselves as better than the average audit professional and their immediate work

associate. We also found that both the BTA bias and relative performance pay

individually influenced POS; finally we found a moderately significant interaction effect.

In their entirety, the results indicate that the greater an entry-level accountant believes

they are better-than-average the more likely their performance outcome satisfaction will

fall.

The implications of each hypothesis for accounting firms and management

accountants are as follows. The importance of H1 is that it draws attention to the role of

an intangible factor—BTA bias—in determining POS. Empirical support for H1 is a

signal for an accounting firm, hoping for high levels of POS among the employees from

its performance evaluations and pay for performance programs, to recognize that POS is

28

being influenced by its employees’ BTA Bias. If the performance evaluation and

compensation processes ignore BTA Bias then it is highly likely that the performance

outcomes resulting from the organization’s performance evaluation process will be

inconsistent with the expectations of the employees and low levels of POS (and

potentially job satisfaction) will result. In other words H1 may provide an explanation for

an accounting firm that is already experiencing low levels of POS and job satisfaction

among its employees.

But what comfort is there to be had in the knowledge that POS of employees is

being influenced in part by factors external to management control systems? What is

management to do in such situations? This is a challenging question for accounting firms.

Since the perceptions that are formed by employees are about their own abilities relative

to that of peers one strategy for managers and management control systems designers,

wishing for employees to experience high levels of POS and job satisfaction, will be to

attempt to influence the formation of such perceptions and expectations. This can be

possible if management creates opportunities for employees to better understand the

capabilities of their peers. Whether or not management can provide such opportunities is

an open question. Our H2 and H3 may offer a potential (although different) solution.

Finding support for H2 implies that when accountants receive a larger relative

performance pay, other things held equal, there will be an increase in their level of POS.

Consequently one would infer that for accountants (the employees), relative performance

pay matters since for these individuals a large relative reward is effectively

acknowledging their contributions and confirming their high expectations.

29

The key hypothesis for our paper is H3. The finding of a moderately significant

positive interaction effect is evidence that relative performance pay could mitigate the

negative influence of BTA bias on POS and amplifies the positive influence of relative

performance pay on POS. Such a finding would be relevant to management since it

reveals relative performance pay as one tangible factor that can be used to counter the

negative influence of BTA bias. As well it suggests that the positive impact which high

relative performance pay will have on POS will be even further enhanced when

performance pay is provided to employees who have high self-efficacy. Given our

assertion that accounting firms are examples of organizations where such employees will

be prevalent, support for H3 is a clear indication that relative performance pay matters to

accountants and that accounting firms should care about the amount of performance pay

they dispense to their employees. In other words relative performance pay can become a

strategic tool by which management can seek to establish higher levels of POS and job

satisfaction among the employees, especially when those employees have high levels of

self-efficacy exhibited through high levels of BTA bias.

If self-assessments and the use of performance pay in accounting firms influence

POS for entry-level accountants, as suggested by our research, the implication is that

firms should strive to avoid incongruences between the expectations that employees form

based on their sense that they are better-than-average and the performance evaluation

being provided to them. If the outcome of the performance evaluation is congruent with

the expectations formed by employees it can result in improved POS. Since expectations

are unobservable it poses a major challenge for firms as to the steps that can be taken to

30

close the gap between employee expectations and the performance outcome. Our finding

that performance pay mitigates the BTA bias on POS suggests that performance pay can

become a management tool to diminish the lack of congruency that might otherwise exist.

The preceding suggests an interesting direction for future research which is to

study the scope for directly confronting the BTA phenomenon during the performance

evaluation process or even through periodic communication. Given that communication

between employees and senior management has been ranked as one of the most important

factors affecting job satisfaction (SHRM, 2012), it is tempting to conclude from our work

that if a firm was to communicate to its employees during the performance evaluation

process a rationale that directly focused on the employee’s sense of BTA, it would allow

the employees to revise their sense of BTA and thereby rationalize the evaluation. This

would consequently lead to an improvement in POS from what it would have been

otherwise. Our research does not test this conjecture and thus such a test can form the

basis of a future study.

A related issue is the potential benefit to firms to pre-empt the BTA formation

process in its employees by directly managing the formation of the BTA bias in

employees. Through on-going feedback and mentoring and training it may be possible for

firms to reduce the BTA bias in its employees, thereby managing individual expectations.

Following from expectancy theory a change in one’s self-efficacy should lead to a change

in expectations. Perhaps one way of reducing this bias is to create an environment where

high levels of interaction exist among peers. Alicke (1985) and Alicke et al. (1995) also

state that the BTA scores will fall (i.e., become smaller) as the peer group becomes more

31

well-defined – going from a large peer group to a specific work associate (our results

corroborate these findings). This change can also be interpreted to mean that the BTA

bias declines as the comparison group becomes more focused. Miceli and Lane (1991)

suggest that a critical component in equity theory is the selection of the peer group; they

state that any individual comparing themselves to a peer must perceive the target as being

similar. Therefore, organizations that encourage mutual collaboration and team work can

expect individual employees to alter their perceptions, relative to peers, due to the fact

that individuals have a better understanding of their peers’ abilities, relative to

themselves. Consequently an important future direction for research in this area

suggested by our study is to investigate how firms might manage the BTA formation in

its employees and to test if the BTA bias can be reduced and the BTA effect on POS can

be overcome.

Our study is not without limitations. Our first limitation is the use of

questionnaires/surveys and cases to collect information. Case studies may not adequately

reflect the impact of actual, real world performance pay differences. Simply stated, if

individuals know that the performance pay differences and performance appraisal results

are not real, but rather they are only fictitious, questions designed to investigate

satisfaction levels may not reflect the participants’ actual degree of (dis)satisfaction.

A second limitation of our study relates to the problems associated with self-

evaluations. The self-evaluation questionnaires/surveys used in our research are subject

to participants providing answers which they may perceive as either socially desirable, or

32

what they believe the researcher is trying to find. Either of these conditions could bias

the results.

A third limitation of our study stems from the small sample size. Although

conducting an experiment in a controlled setting (i.e., classroom) provides certain types

and levels of control, it also limits us from generalizing the results of the study to

populations other than the one being represented by our participants. A fourth and final

limitation in our study relates to the ability to design reasonable survey questions and

cases that adequately probe performance outcome satisfaction levels, while balancing

between using instruments that are too complex and not overly transparent. Although a

number of steps were taken to develop and implement a questionnaire/survey and cases

that addresses both challenges, it should be noted that they are not without their inherent

risks.

In summary, the results of our study are important to the accounting profession as

they fill a gap, both in the accounting literature and at the operations or practical level for

any accounting firms using or contemplating the use of performance pay. Our study

provides accounting firms, their partners, managers and employees with valuable

information regarding the influence of performance pay and self-assessments upon POS.

33

REFERENCES

Adams, J. S. 1965. Inequity in social exchange. In Advances in Experimental Social

Psychology, ed. L. Berkovitz, Vol. 2. New York: Academic Press.

Alicke, M. D. 1985. Global self-evaluation as determined by the desirability and

controllability of trait adjectives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

49(6): 1621-1630.

Alicke, M. D., M. L. Klotz, D. L. Breitenbecher, T. J. Yurak, T. J., and D. S.

Vredenburg. 1995. Personal contact, individuation, and the better-than-average

effect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 68(5): 804-825.

American Institute of Certified Public Accountants (AICPA). 2003. Top CPA firm

concern: recruitment and retention. The Practicing CPA (online publication),

December.

Anderson, N.H. 1968. Likableness ratings of 555 personality-trait words. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology 9(3): 272-279.

Canadian Institute of Chartered Accountants (CICA). 2006. Findings: The Price of

Happiness. CA Magazine (September): 7.

Canadian Institute of Chartered Accountants (CICA). 2007. What CAs want. CA

Magazine (August): 9.

Clark, A.E. and A.J. Oswald. 1996. Satisfaction and comparison income. Journal of

Public Economics 61: 359-381.

Crosby, F. 1976. A model of egoistical relative deprivation. Psychological Review 83:

95-113.

Dunning, D., J.A. Meyerowitz and A. D. Holzberg. 1989. Ambiguity and self-evaluation:

The role of idiosyncratic trait definitions in self-serving assessments of ability.

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 57(6): 1082-1090.

Fletcher, C. 2001. Performance appraisal and management: The developing research

agenda. Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology 74: 473-487.

34

Gabris, G.T. and D. M. Ihrke. 2001. Does performance appraisal contribute to heightened

levels of employee burnout. Public Personal Management 30(2): 157-172.

Green, C. and J. S. Heywood. 2008. Does performance pay increase job satisfaction.

Economica 75:710-728.

Greenberg, J. 1990. Organizational justice: Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Journal of

Management 16(2): 399-432.

Kaplan, R.S. and D. P. Norton. 2001. The strategy focused organization: How scorecard

companies thrive in the new business environment. Boston, MA: Harvard

Business School Press.

Lau, C.M., K.M. Wong and I.R.C. Eggleton. 2008. Fairness of performance evaluation

procedures and job satisfaction: the role of outcome-based and non-outcome-

based effects. Accounting and Business Research 38(2): 121-135.

Lawler, E.E. 1971. Pay and organizational effectiveness: A psychological view. New

York: McGraw Hill.

Lawler, E.E. 1973. Motivation in work organizations. Monteray, CA: Brooks/Cole.

Lawler, E.E. 1990. Strategic pay: Aligning organizational strategies and pay systems.

San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass.

Libby, T., and L. Thorne. 2004. Auditors’ virtue: A qualitative analysis and

categorization. Business Ethics Quarterly 14(3): 479-98.

Long, R. J. 2002. Performance pay in Canada. In Michelle Brown and John S. Heywood,

(Eds.). Paying for performance: an international comparison. Armonk, NY: M.E.

Sharpe.

Long, R. J. 2010. Strategic compensation in Canada. Fourth edition. Scarborough, ON:

Nelson Thomson Learning.

Lornic, J. 2006. The war for talent. CA Magazine (August): 24-29.

35

Merchant, K.A., and W. A. Van der Stede. 2003. Disciplinary constraints on the

advancement of knowledge: The case of organizational incentive systems.

Accounting, Organizations and Society 28(2): 251-286.

Miceli, M.P. and M. C. Lane. 1991. Antecedents of pay satisfaction: A review and

extension. Research in Human Resources Management 9: 235-309.

Otley, D. (2003). Management control and performance management: whence and

whither. The British Accounting Review (35): 309-326.

Parker, R.J. and J. H. Kohlmeyer. 2005. Organizational justice and turnover in

accounting firms. Accounting, Organisations and Society (30): 357-369.

Robinson, G. 2006. Why fair pay? CA Magazine (December): 47-48.

Ross, T.L. and E. C. Bomeli. 1971. A comment on accountants’ job satisfaction. Journal

of Accounting Research 9(2): 383-388.

Schein, E. 1965. Organizational psychology. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

Sheehan, N.T., G. Vaidyanathan and S. Kalagnanam. 2005. Value creation logics and the

choice of management control systems. Qualitative Research in Accounting and

Management 2(1): 1-28.

Smith, P.C., L. Kendall and C. Hulin. 1969. The measurement of satisfaction in work and

retirement. Chicago, IL: Rand McNally.

Society for Human Resource Management. 2008. 2008 employee job satisfaction:

Meeting the needs of a multi-faceted workforce. SHRM, Alexandria, VA.

Society for Human Resource Management. 2012. 2012 employee job satisfaction and

engagement: How employees are dealing with uncertainty. SHRM, Alexandria,

VA.

36

Strawser, R.H., J. M. Ivancevich and H. L. Lyons. 1969. A note on the job satisfaction of

accountants in large and small CPA firms. Journal of Accounting Research 7:

339-345.

Sudin, S. 2011. Fairness of and satisfaction with performance appraisal process. Journal

of Global Management 2(1): 66-83

Suls, J., K. Lemos, and S.H. Lockett. 2002. Self-esteem, construal, and comparisons with

self, friends and peers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 82(2): 252-

261.

Vidal, J.B., and M. Nossol. 2011. Tournaments without prizes: Evidence from Personnel

records. Management Science 57(10): 1721-1736.

Vroom, V.V. 1964. Work and motivation. New York: Wiley.

37

Figure 1 Equity Theory, Performance Outcome Satisfaction and Job Satisfaction Model

(combining Adams (1965) and Smith et al. (1969))

Individuals's perception of their ability to contribute relative

to a peerJo

PerformanceOutcome

Satisfaction

Job Satisfactoin

PerformancePay

OPERATIONAlIZED AS

Invidual Better-Than-Average (BTA) Bias

As either $500 or $1,500

Satifaction with Performance Evaluation +

Satisfaction with Performance Pay

Job Satisfaction

38

TABLE 1

Libby and Thorne’s 24 Traits & Individual Trait Rating t-tests

This table provides a list of the traits used to measure the existence of BTA bias. The t-values pertain to

the ratings provided by participants with respect to the importance of each trait for their job as entry-

level accountants.

Trait: t-value

Alert 17.040***

Altruistic 4.644***

Benevolent 0.576

Careful 30.006***

Cheerful 2.975***

Concerned with public interest 32.345***

Cooperative 13.561***

Courageous 10.481***

Diligent 27.885***

Enlightened 5.128***

Even-handed 10.804***

Farsighted 11.737***

Healthy scepticism 37.503***

Independent 28.483***

Integrity 52.195***

Objective 37.950***

Polite 5.787***

Principled 24.087***

Resourceful 16.280***

Sensitive -0.574

Tactful 8.580***

Thoughtful 5.270***

Truthful 25.059***

Warm -2.579**

N = 164

df = 163

* significant at p ≤0.10, ** significant at p ≤0.05, ***significant at p ≤0.01

39

TABLE 2

t-test of Mean Trait Ratings for Different Comparison Groups (using ratings for 21

traits)

TABLE 3

Regression of POS on Performance Pay, BTA Bias, Work experience and Gender

(under both conditions of relative performance pay)

Total BTAScore In Comparison to: Mean Score

Difference

from the Mean

Score of 84 t value p value*

Average Professional Auditor 106.043 22.043 17.530 0.000

Immediate Work Associate 88.884 4.884 4.993 0.000

N = 164

df = 163

* Reported p-values are for 2-tailed tests

Unstandardized

(Standardized)

Beta's t-value p-value*

Performance Pay

9.049

(0.773) 34.044 0.000

BTA Bias

-0.043

(-0.103) -3.057 0.000

Length of Work Experience (months)

-0.013

(-0.013) -0.492 0.623

Gender

0.031

(0.003) 0.114 0.909

N = 324 293.883 0.000

df1 = 4 df2 = 319

Adj R-Sq =

0.784

Dependent variable = POS

Gender = Dummy Variable (0 = male & 1 = female)

Performance Pay: Dummy Variable (0 = $500 (lower relativepay); 1 = $1,500 (higher relative pay))

* Reported p-values are for 2-tailed tests

40

TABLE 4

Regression of POS on Performance Pay, BTA Bias, Work experience, Gender and

an Interaction Term (under both conditions of relative performance pay)

TABLE 5

Regression of POS on BTA Bias, Work experience and Gender (when relative

performance pay is lower and greater than their immediate work associate)

Unstandardized

(Standardized)

Beta's t-value p-value*

Performance Pay

5.484

(0.534) 2.842 0.005

BTA Bias in Comparison to their Immediate Work

Associate

-0.063

(0-.151) -4.113 0.000

Length of Work Experience (months)

-0.013

(-0.013) -0.494 0.622

Gender

0.031

(0.003) 0.114 0.909

Interaction Between Performance Pay & BTA Bias

0.040

(0.343) 1.866 0.063

N = 324 F = 237.631 0.000

df1 = 5 df2 = 318

Adj R-Sq =

0.786

Dependent variable = POS

Gender = Dummy Variable (0 = male & 1 = female)

Performance Pay: Dummy Variable (0 = $500 (lower relativepay); 1 = $1,500 (higher relative pay))

* Reported p-values are for 2-tailed tests

Unstandardized

(Standardized)

Beta's t-value p-value*

Unstandardized

(Standardized)

Beta's t-value p-value*

BTA Bias

-0.065

(-0.405) -5.663 0.000

-0.021

(-0.091) -1.150 0.252

Length of Work Experience (months)

0.005

(0.012) 0.165 0.87

-0.031

(-0.055) -0.699 0.486

Gender

-0.906

(-0.225) -3.178 0.002

0.968

(0.168) 2.155 0.033

N = 162 F = 13.870 0.000 N = 162 F = 2.245 0.085

df1 = 3 df2 = 158 df1 = 3 df2 = 158

Adj R-Sq =

0.193

Adj R-Sq =

0.023

Dependent Variable = POS

Gender: Dummy Variable (0 = male & 1 = female)

* Reported p-values are for 2-tailed tests

Performance Pay is less than their immediate

work associate

Performance Pay is more than their

immediate work associate

41

TABLE 6

Crosstab of Performance Pay Satisfaction and Performance Evaluation Satisfaction

(when relative performance pay is lower)

TABLE 7

Crosstab of Performance Pay Satisfaction and Performance Evaluation Satisfaction

(when relative performance pay is higher)

0 = Very

Dissatisfied 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8 = Very

Satisfied Total

0 = Very

Dissatisfied 33 14 1 0 1 0 0 0 0 49

1 12 18 9 4 0 0 0 0 0 43

2 17 6 25 0 0 0 0 0 0 48

3 2 2 4 4 1 0 0 0 0 13

4 4 0 0 3 1 0 0 0 0 8

5 0 0 0 3 0 0 0 0 0 3

6 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

7 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

8 = Very

Satisfied 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 68 40 39 14 3 0 0 0 0 164

Performance Evaluation Satisfaction

Performance pay

satisfaction

0 = Very

Dissatisfied 1 2 3 4 5 6 7

8 = Very

Satisfied Total

0 = Very

Dissatisfied 0 0 0 0 1 0 0 0 0 1

1 0 0 0 0 2 0 0 0 0 2

2 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 1 0 3

3 0 0 0 1 3 5 0 0 0 9

4 0 0 1 0 7 4 1 0 0 13

5 0 0 1 0 9 24 3 1 0 38

6 0 0 0 0 4 14 11 5 5 39

7 0 1 0 0 4 2 3 11 2 23

8 = Very

Satisfied 0 0 0 0 2 2 10 4 18 36

Total 0 1 3 1 32 51 29 22 25 164

Performance Evaluation Satisfaction

Performance pay

satisfaction