144
The Effect of Fluency on Reading Comprehension A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the College of Education University of Houston In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Education by Beth Egmon May, 2008

Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

The Effect of Fluency on Reading Comprehension

A Dissertation Presented to the Faculty of the College of Education

University of Houston

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Education

by

Beth Egmon

May, 2008

Page 2: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

UMI Number: 3309545

Copyright 2008 by

Egmon, Beth

All rights reserved.

INFORMATION TO USERS

The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy

submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and

photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper

alignment can adversely affect reproduction.

In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript

and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized

copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion.

®

UMI UMI Microform 3309545

Copyright 2008 by ProQuest LLC.

All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against

unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code.

ProQuest LLC 789 E. Eisenhower Parkway

PO Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346

Page 3: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Copyright

By

Beth Egmon

May, 2008

Page 4: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

The Effect of Fluency on Reading Comprehension

A Dissertation for the Degree

Doctor of Education

by

Beth Egmon

Approved by Dissertation Committee:

t (f&£^ Dr. Richard F. Abrahamson, Chairperson

\£±1 erine Horn/Committee Member

Dr. Lane R. Gauthier, Committee Member

It UCr^, r>

Dr. Peter J. Gingiss, Committee Member

Dr. Lee Mountain, Committee Member

Dr. R Colle

May 2008

Page 5: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Acknowledgements

Harold R. McAlindon once said, "Do not follow where the path may lead. Go

instead where there is no path and leave a trail." I would like to thank all of those who

helped me leave a trail where there was no path - family, friends, coworkers, and

especially all those who served on my committee - without the support of all these, this

endeavor would not have been possible. Thank you.

iii

Page 6: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

The Effect of Fluency on Reading Comprehension

An Abstract Presented to the Faculty of the College of Education

University of Houston

In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree

Doctor of Education

by

Beth Egmon

May, 2008

Page 7: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Egmon, B. "The Effect of Fluency on Reading Comprehension." Unpublished Doctor of Education Dissertation, University of Houston, May, 2008.

Abstract

The purpose of the study was to describe the relationship between the reading

fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students. The current literature on

fluency indicated that there is a positive relationship between reading fluency and reading

comprehension (Stecker, Roser, & Martinez, 1998). A formal study examining the

relationship between fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students was

needed at this time as the most recent studies combining fluency with comprehension

have focused on third and fifth graders (Rasinski, 1990) as well as second graders (Stahl

& Heubach, 2005). The results of the both studies indicated that fluency is a reasonable

predictor of comprehension. However, this relationship must be established in first grade

students.

The study addressed the following research question: What is the relationship

between the reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students? This

question was answered using a multivariate correlational research design examining first

grade students in a rural school district in the southwestern United States. The

instrumentation used was the Texas Primary Reading Inventory as it measured both

comprehension and fluency.

The study was able to establish a strong positive relationship between fluency and

comprehension in first grade students. Furthermore 17% of the variance in reading

comprehension was explained by fluency while 35.6% of the variance was explained by

Page 8: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

the other controlling variables. The regression coefficents indicated that at-risk and

fluency are the greatest contributing factors to the changes in reading comprehension.

These findings indicated that indeed fluency instruction must be an integral part

of the first grade curriculum in order to enhance comprehension. Future research should

be conducted to determine which methods of fluency instruction make the most impact in

first graders. Meanwhile, first grade practitioners should be engaging in activities such as

buddy reading, choral reading, and repeated reading activities. It is hoped that these

findings will lead to greater awareness amongst practitioners about the importance of

fluency instruction in first grade as well as greater awareness amongst the research

community to provide evidence-based instructional methods specific for first grade

classrooms.

VI

Page 9: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Chapter Page

I. INTRODUCTION 1

Need for the Study 2 Statement of the Problem 6 Purpose of the Study 6 Research Question 6 Hypothesis 7 Definition of Terms 7 Summary 8

II. REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE 11

Reading in the Twentieth Century 11 Theoretical Framework 15 History of Comprehension 20 Reading Policies 24 Current Research in Comprehension 26 Current Research in Fluency 40 Intersection of Fluency and Comprehension . 53 Research on First Grade Students 58

III. METHODOLOGY 62

Research Design 62 Participants 62 Instrumentation 63 Data Collection Procedures 67 Data Analysis Procedures 67 Limitations of the Study 72

IV. RESULTS 75

Descriptive Statistics 75 The Relationship Between Fluency and

Comprehension 81

Page 10: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

V. DISCUSSION 86

Significant Findings of the Study 86 Implications for Current Research 87 Implications for Future Research 87 Implications for Practice 88

REFERENCES 96

APPENDIX A TPRI STUDENT RECORD SHEET 119

APPENDIX B TPRI CLASS SUMMARY SHEET 131

Vll l

Page 11: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1 Chall's Stages of Development 18

2 First Grade Student Population 2006-2007 64

3 Variables 68

4 First Grade Student Sample 76

5 Descriptive Statistics 78

6 Frequencies for Beginning of Year 79

7 Frequencies for End of Year 80

8 Correlation Matrix for Difference in Total Reading Comprehension, Age, Educational Status, Economic Status, and Difference In Fluency 81

9 Ethnicity by At-Risk Cross Tabulation 82

10 Multiple Regression Analysis of the Relationship Between Reading Comprehension, Gender, Ethnicity, Age, Educational Status, Migrant, At-Risk, LEP, Economic Status, and Fluency 84

11 First Grade Oral Reading Fluency Norms 89

12 Oral Reading Fluency Scale 91

IX

Page 12: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

1 The gradual release of responsibility model of instruction 37

x

Page 13: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Introduction. Chapter One

Introduction

In 1967, Bond and Dykstra published their landmark First Grade Studies where

they determined that the key factor to a first grader's reading success was his/her teacher.

They did not find one specific method for the classroom teacher to utilize. Since then,

many studies have been done in an effort to find the key to reading success. However, as

the International Reading Association acknowledged in its 1999 position statement, no

one method of reading instruction is guaranteed to work with every child (Rasinski &

Padak, 2000).

When examining the best methods of reading instruction, a savvy instructor first

examines the purpose of reading. The purpose of reading is comprehension (Bender &

Larkin, 2003) and like any other skill, it must be taught and must be practiced. Inasmuch

as the purpose for reading is comprehension, instructors seek to find the most effective

methods for improving comprehension. The National Reading Panel (2000) concluded

that fluency was closely associated with comprehension. In fact, Pikulski and Chard

(2005) defined fluency and pointed out its link to comprehension when they stated

reading fluency refers to efficient, effective word-recognition skills that

permit a reader to construct the meaning of text. Fluency is manifested in

accurate, rapid, expressive oral reading and is applied during, and makes

possible, silent reading comprehension. (Pikulski & Chard, 2005, p. 510)

Thus, to improve comprehension, one must increase fluency. Rasinski and Padak

(2000) pointed out that "reading fluency is a significant obstacle to proficient reading for

elementary students and many older readers experiencing difficulty in learning to read"

Page 14: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

2

(Rasinski & Padak, 2000, p. 104). In 1983, Richard Allington published "Fluency: The

Neglected Reading Goal" in The Reading Teacher where he contended that reading

fluency as a skill was not being taught. Thirteen years later in 1996, Rasinski and Zutell

looked at current reading programs and discovered that Allington's warnings about

fluency being overlooked had not been heeded. Fluency was being ignored as part of the

reading instructional process.

Need for the Study

Under the No Child Left Behind Act of 2001(2002), students must be reading on

grade-level by third grade. Thus it is incumbent upon the first and second grade teachers

to have the students reading on grade level at those respective primary grades as well. It

is evident, however, that not all students are reading on grade level at this time; in fact,

not even a majority are reading on grade level. The Nation's Report Card for Reading is

based on the National Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) which is given to

grades four and eight. The NAEP measures reading comprehension in three contexts of

reading: reading for literary experience, reading for information, and reading to perform

a task (Lee, Grigg & Donahue, 2007). The NAEP achievement levels are basic,

proficient, and advanced. The Nation's Report Card for Reading in 2007 indicated that

only 41 percent of fourth graders and 34 percent of eighth graders were reading at

proficient or advanced levels. 67 percent of fourth graders and 74 percent of eighth

graders were reading at or just above basic level. "Basic denotes partial mastery of

prerequisite knowledge and skills that are fundamental for proficient work at a given

grade" (Lee, Grigg & Donahue, 2007, p. 6). (Note: Percentages do not equal 100

percent because of rounding.) Furthermore, Juel (1988) found that "a child would remain

Page 15: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

3

a poor reader at the end of fourth grade if the child was a poor reader at the end of first

grade" (Juel, 1988, p. 437).

The problem becomes further complicated when examining the National

Endowment for the Arts' (NEA) Research Report entitled To Read or Not To Read: A

Question of National Consequence (2007). In this report, NEA pointed out three

alarming conclusions: (1) "Americans are spending less time reading. (2) Reading

comprehension skills are eroding. (3) These declines have serious civic, social, cultural,

and economic implications" (NEA, 2007, p. 7). The NEA report discussed the

implications of these trends and pointed out that "employers now rank reading and

writing as top deficiencies in new hires" (NEA, 2007, p. 16).

With that many students reading below grade level and less and less time being

devoted to reading, the question becomes what can be done instructionally to help the

nation's students become better readers. The National Reading Panel (2000) cited five

components of reading that need to be in place in order for reading to occur: phonemic

awareness, phonics, fluency, comprehension, and vocabulary. Of these five components,

"fluency has been shown to have a 'reciprocal relationship' with comprehension, with

each fostering the other" (Stecker, Roser, & Martinez, 1998, p. 306).

This reciprocal relationship between fluency and comprehension has brought

these reading components to the forefront of the literacy community. Beginning in 1997,

Jack Cassidy, former president of the International Reading Association, has led the

annual publication of a survey in Reading Today entitled "What's Hot, What's Not." His

team continues to survey twenty-five notable literacy leaders throughout the world to

determine the hot topics in the field of literacy. When the survey was first released in

Page 16: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

4

1997, fluency was not even considered a topic and comprehension was considered to be

"not hot" (Cassidy & Wenrich, 1997, p. 34). By 2000, the survey indicated that

comprehension was "not hot" but "should be hot" (Cassidy & Cassidy, 2000, p. 28). It

wasn't until 2003 that fluency arrived on the scene. In 2003, survey respondents

indicated that fluency was a "very hot" topic and comprehension was a "hot" topic as

well (Cassidy & Cassidy, 2003, p. 18). From 2004-2006, both fluency and

comprehension were "very hot" topics (Cassidy & Cassidy, 2004, p. 3; Cassidy &

Cassidy, 2005, p. 8; Cassidy & Cassidy, 2006, p. 8). The 2007 survey indicated that

fluency was still a "very hot" topic and comprehension was a "hot" topic. Furthermore,

respondents for the 2007 survey indicated that fluency "should be very hot" and

comprehension "should be extremely hot" (Cassidy & Cassidy, 2007, p. 10).

Respondents for the 2008 survey indicated that fluency was again a "very hot" topic and

comprehension was not only "hot" but "should be extremely hot" (Cassidy & Cassidy,

2008, p. 10).

Comprehension continues to be an extremely hot issue due to the evolving needs

of literacy. Coiro and Dobler (2007) discussed this new literacies perspective.

According to this new literacies perspective, reading comprehension

becomes an important issue to study (Coiro, 2003 a) because new

comprehension skills, strategies, and dispositions may be required to

generate questions, and to locate, evaluate, synthesize, and communicate

information on the Internet (Leu et al., 2004). Similarly, this perspective

posits that traditional reading skills are necessary, but not sufficient, to read

and learn from information on the Internet. (Coiro & Dobler, 2007, pp. 217-

Page 17: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

5

218)

The necessary traditional reading skills referenced above include those reading

skills that allow students to comprehend, generate questions, locate, evaluate, synthesize,

and communicate information. In order to comprehend, the current literature on fluency

indicated that there is a positive relationship between reading fluency and reading

comprehension. In a 1990 study, Rasinski used a correlational research design to

examine that relationship between fluency and comprehension in seventy-seven third

grade students and sixty-five fifth grade students in a large Midwestern city. His findings

indicated that fluency is a reasonable predictor of comprehension in third and fifth

graders (Rasinski, 1990). A study by Stahl and Heubach (2005) indicated that fluency-

oriented reading instruction leads to gains in comprehension in second grade students.

Using a pretest-posttest design, researchers discovered that students who received

fluency-oriented reading instruction made "significantly more than 1 year's reading

growth in one school year" (Stahl & Heubach, 2005, p. 190).

At this time, the research indicates there is a relationship between the reading

fluency and reading comprehension of students and the Rasinski (1990) study along with

the Stahl and Heubach (2005) study are indicative of the nature of that relationship in

second, third and fifth grade students. A formal study indicating the nature of the

relationship between fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students is needed

at this time as the most recent studies combining fluency with comprehension have

focused on second, third and fifth graders. However, at this critical juncture it is

imperative that the nature of the relationship between the reading fluency and reading

comprehension of first grade students be determined. This information is crucial for

Page 18: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

6

practitioners who are developing instructional plans that will most significantly impact

reading comprehension of first graders.

Statement of the Problem

The National Reading Panel (2000) cited numerous studies throughout its report

indicating that there is a positive relationship between reading fluency and reading

comprehension, especially in upper elementary, secondary and adult readers. However,

no studies were cited in their report specific to first grade. Furthermore, in order to

determine how much instructional time should be devoted to reading fluency instruction,

one needs to know how strong the relationship is between reading fluency and reading

comprehension. The relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension of

first grade students is unclear. This study determined that relationship and will thus

allow first grade practitioners to make essential instructional decisions that will impact

student reading.

Purpose of the Study

The purpose of the study was to describe the relationship between reading fluency

and reading comprehension of first grade students.

Research Question

As stated earlier, the purpose of the study was to describe the relationship

between reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students. Given this

purpose, the study addressed the following research question: What is the relationship

between reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students?

Page 19: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

7

Hypotheses

Null Hypothesis. The research question posed in the previous section of this

paper is the basis for the following null hypothesis: There is no statistically significant

relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students.

Directional Research Hypothesis. In April 2000, the Report of the National

Reading Panel: Teaching Children to Read was released in which the panel concluded

that fluency was closely associated with comprehension and teachers needed to be aware

of this so that they could teach for fluency to improve comprehension (National Institute

of Child Health & Human Development - Report of the National Reading Panel:

Teaching Children to Read website, Fluency subsection). The National Reading Panel

cited a study by Pinnell, Pikulski, Wixson, Campbell, Gough, and Beatty (1995), which

indicated that 44% of the fourth and fifth grade students sampled were dysfluent readers.

Furthermore, this dysfiuency resulted in students having difficulty with comprehending

the text that they were reading (Fluency subsection). Grace Oakley concurred with the

findings of the panel but reported that the nature of the relationship between fluency and

comprehension remained unclear (Oakley, 2003, Fluency section). Inasmuch as the

aforementioned literature in this proposal suggests that there is a positive relationship

between reading fluency and reading comprehension, this study will test the following

directional research hypothesis: There is a statistically significant positive relationship

between reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students.

Definitions of Terms

Automaticity. The word "automaticity" is defined as "fluent processing of

information that requires little effort or attention, as sight-word recognition" (Harris &

Page 20: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

8

Hodges, 1995, p. 16). This occurs when a reader can read so well he or she does not have

to think about the individual words. The reader can then think about other things, such as

comprehension of text.

Decoding. The word "decoding" is defined as "to determine what sounds

particular letters make to decipher words" (Bender & Larkin, 2003, p. 212). Decoding is

very prevalent in beginning readers who are trying to decide what a word is by sounding

it out.

Prosody. The word "prosody" is defined as "the pitch, loudness, tempo, and

rhythm patterns of spoken language" (Harris & Hodges, 1995, p. 196). Prosody is more

commonly referred to as reading with expression.

Reading Fluency. The phrase "reading fluency" is defined as involving

"accurate reading of connected text at a conversational rate with appropriate prosody or

expression" (Hudson, Lane & Pullen, 2005, p. 702). Thus, when considering a reader's

fluency, one looks at accuracy, rate (speed), and prosody (expression).

Reading Comprehension. The phrase "reading comprehension" is defined as

"the process of simultaneously extracting and constructing meaning" (Snow & Sweet,

2003, p. 1). Reading comprehension is actually thinking about the text and making

meaning out of it.

Summary

The Nation's Report Card for Reading in 2007 indicated that the reading

comprehension of a majority of 4l and 8th graders was below grade level. Educators

must look to research to see how best to improve the comprehension of the nation's

Page 21: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

9

readers. Research indicated that there is a positive relationship between reading fluency

and reading comprehension in students as young as second grade.

According to the world's literacy leaders, fluency and comprehension not only

continue to be very hot topics in the field of literacy, they should be very hot topics.

With the increasing demands in the field of informational literacy, reading

comprehension is more important than ever.

As mentioned earlier, this study described the relationship between reading

fluency and reading comprehension in first grade students. From this, educators will be

able to determine whether or not fluency has a significant impact upon reading

comprehension in first grade students. Furthermore, as Shanahan (2002) points out, the

value in the research is also the ensuing discussion. Once the research is complete, there

will be more empirical data for first grade teachers to formulate their thoughts and

language arts curriculum decisions. If the empirical evidence holds, and there is indeed a

significant relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension the

implications for first grade language arts curriculum are exciting. With proper alterations

in curriculum and appropriate focus on increasing reading fluency, reading

comprehension will increase as well. First grade students will be on their way to reading

on grade level and being successful life-long learners.

This chapter has outlined the need for the study, the statement of the problem, the

purpose of the study, as well as stated the research question, the null hypothesis, the

directional research hypothesis, and provided definitions of key terms. The next chapter,

Chapter Two, will review the literature in terms of historical background of reading

education, theoretical framework of comprehension and fluency, historical background of

Page 22: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

10

comprehension and fluency, reading policies, current research developments in reading

comprehension and fluency, the intersection of fluency and comprehension, as well as the

need for the study with first grade students.

Page 23: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Review of the Literature. Chapter Two

Introduction

The purpose of the study was to describe the relationship between the reading

fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students. Given this purpose, the study

addressed the following research question: What is the relationship between the reading

fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students? This study tested the

following directional research hypothesis: There is a statistically significant positive

relationship between the reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade

students. Chapter One introduced the research question and directional research

hypotheses as well as outlined the need for the study. Chapter Two will review the

literature in terms of historical background of reading education, theoretical framework

of comprehension and fluency, historical background of comprehension and fluency,

reading policies, current research developments in reading comprehension and fluency,

the intersection of fluency and comprehension, as well as the need for the study with first

grade students.

Reading in the twentieth century

The so-called reading wars are not new. In fact, debate about reading instruction

occurred throughout the twentieth century in the United States. During the 1920s, the

controversial topics included the value of silent reading over oral reading. Another

controversial topic was whole word instruction as opposed to phonics instruction.

Educators also warned about the dangers of teaching the alphabet before teaching words.

A strong testing movement and development of standardized reading assessments to

determine reading readiness for formal instruction marked the period from 1920 to 1940.

Page 24: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

12

During the 1930s and 1940s, whole word instruction (also known as "look-say") was

prevalent in many American classrooms. "By repeatedly encountering the same words in

text, children were expected to learn to recognize entire words without attending to

phonics or decoding strategies" (Quick, 1998, p. 255). During the 1930s, reading

instruction was considered its own discipline, little or no effort was made "to relate

instruction in reading to other curriculum fields or indeed to provide guidance in the

reading activities carried on in them" (Brueckner, 1939, p. 284).

Prior to 1960, the primary method of reading instruction in the United States was

the basal reader approach.

In response to a national survey of instructional practices in reading

conducted by Ralph Steiger (1958), 69% of the respondents reported using

one basal series, 20% reported using two basals, and 11% reported using

three or more basals. That, as you can see, adds up to 100% of the respondents

reporting that they used basals. (Graves & Dykstra, 1997, p. 342)

However, in 1955, Flesch published Why Johnny Can't Read and attacked the

basal approach to reading instruction. He challenged whole word instruction indicating

that sight reading methods were not helping children read. Rather, he advocated a return

to a phonics approach (Graves & Dykstra, 1997).

In the 1960s, Chall began analyzing reading programs and teacher performance as

well as surveying the research on reading. The result was Learning to read: The great

debate (1967). Chall examined two schools of thought in reading instruction: whole

word instruction and phonics instruction. Chall's research indicated that for long-term

results, phonics instruction is clearly better than whole word instruction. Phonics

Page 25: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

13

instruction resulted in better word attack skills that were beneficial to students as they

faced unknown words in later years. While whole word instruction was beneficial in the

early years, students lacked the word attack skills necessary to transition to independent

reading.

As mentioned in Chapter One, Bond and Dykstra (1967) published their landmark

First Grade Studies where they determined that the key factor to a first grader's reading

success was his/her teacher. They were unable to isolate any specific teaching

methodologies that were preferable in aiding reading instruction. Bond and Dykstra

concluded that children learn by a variety of methods of instruction and a combination of

approaches developed by a child's teacher is best.

The 1970s brought the whole language philosophy of teaching reading. Whole

language advocates believed that "critical skills like phonemic awareness, phonics,

vocabulary, reading fluency, and comprehension strategies can be learned through

exposure to reading and writing activities, not through systematic instruction"

(Blaunstein & Lyon, 2006, p. 5). The role of the whole language teacher was to provide

discovery learning opportunities for the students.

Basically, children are expected to learn phonics and other basic reading

skills on their own with only minimal guidance from the teacher. Many

advocates of whole language actually believe that too much phonics

instruction is harmful to children, that it will turn them into "word callers"

and will destroy their love for reading. The role of the whole language

teacher is to help students "discover" how our writing system works

without providing systematic instruction. Their goal for reading instruction

Page 26: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

14

is to instill a love of reading, not the ability to read, seemingly without the

realization that the latter is the pathway to the former. (Blaunstein & Lyon,

2006, p. 5)

Whole-language became the primary focus of reading instruction by the 1990s.

California and Texas were the two largest states adopting basal readers. Because of the

move towards whole language, these two states insisted that textbook publishers begin

publishing books with literature components (Pressley, Allington, Wharton-McDonald,

Block & Morrow, 2001, p. 18). However, during the 1990s, the reading wars erupted

pitting whole language against basic skills instruction. What emerged was a call for

balancing skills instruction and whole language components.

In addition, there was increasing appreciation that the most respected and

respectable of the scholars documenting the need for skills instruction

argued that such instruction should be accompanied by immersion in

literature and composing, with Marilyn Adams (1990) and Jeanne Chall

(1967/1983) both making such a case. A recent National Research Council

panel, which argued in its final report, Preventing Reading Difficulties in

Young Children (Snow, Burns, & Griffin, 1998), that the scientific

literature favored skills instruction in beginning reading, also made the case

that such instruction should occur in the context of extensive reading of real

literature and writing. (Pressley et al., 2001, p. 25)

In summation, reading instruction in the twentieth century has been marked by

controversy. The 1920s saw the debate over silent reading versus oral reading. During

the 1920s to 1940s the debates raged over whole word instruction ("look-say") versus

Page 27: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

15

phonics instruction. Prior to the 1960s basal readers became popular. During the 1970s

whole language became popular, despite the cries for a phonics approach; but by the

1990s balanced literacy emerged, balancing skills instruction and whole language

components.

Theoretical Framework

Fluency

"The word fluency comes from the Latin fluens, meaning to flow. Hence, oral

reading fluency is generally described as flowing, smooth, and effortless" (Moskal &

Blachowicz, 2006, pp. 2-3). There are two major theories used to describe fluency's role

in reading: the theory of automatic information processing (or automaticity) and the

theory of prosody. Automaticity occurs when a reader is able to decode to a high enough

level of automatic information processing that he or she can focus on creating meaning

from text (LaBerge & Samuels, 1974). Nicholson and Tan (1999) provided the example

of walking.

Anyone who has watched 2-year-olds walking knows that they have

definitely not yet overlearned this skill. They still put mental effort into

walking. They look where they are walking, learn to adjust their pace, and

so on. But walking, for the adult, requires no conscious attention - except

as Samuels (1976) pointed out, when the ground is icy and attention must

be used to avoid falling. In addition, while walking, the mind can be

thinking about something else other than the process of walking. This is

another aspect of automaticity. The mastery of one skill to the point of

Page 28: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

16

effortlessness enables you to do something else at the same time. (Nicholsan &

Tan, 1999, pp. 151-152)

Automaticity is emphasized because it is the key to comprehension. "Humans have only

so much cognitive capacity to devote to a particular task. In cognitive psychology, this is

known as the assumption of limited processing capacity, or limited cognitive resources"

(Nathan & Stanovich, 1991, p. 176).

"When children learn to recognize many words automatically and to read grade-

level text at a reasonable rate, their oral reading still many not sound 'natural,' because

they do not yet read with expression - or prosody" (Osborn, Lehr, & Hiebert, 2003, p. 5).

Prosody occurs when a reader can read with appropriate expression and phrasing

(Rasinski & Hoffman, 2003). There are six basic components of prosodic reading:

"pausal intrusions, length of phrases, appropriateness of phrases, final phrase

lengthening, terminal intonation countours, and stress" (Kuhn & Stahl, 2002, p. 5).

Another important model is Keith Stanovich's Interactive-Compensatory model

where Stanovich (1986) describes the number of interactions a reader has with text.

The effect of reading volume on vocabulary growth, combined with the large skill

differences in reading volume, could mean that a "rich-get-richer" or cumulative

advantage phenomenon is almost inextricably embedded within the

developmental course of reading progress. The very children who are reading

well and who have good vocabularies will read more, learn more word meanings,

and hence read even better. Children with inadequate vocabularies - who read

slowly and without enjoyment - read less, and as a result have slower

development of vocabulary knowledge, which inhibits further growth in reading

Page 29: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

17

available. Walberg (Walberg et al., 1984; Walberg & Tsai, 1983), following

Merton (1968), has dubbed those educational sequences where early achievement

spawns faster rates of subsequent achievement "Matthew effects," after the

Gospel according to Matthew: "For unto every one that hath shall be given, and

he shall have abundance: but from him that hath not shall be taken away even

that which he hath (XXV:29)." (Stanovich, 1986, p. 381)

The Matthew effect becomes more pronounced when one takes into account the

difference in reading volume between avid and reluctant readers. Students who read on

average ninety minutes per day, read 4.7 million words annually as opposed to students

that read on average two minutes per day. Their annual number of words read amounts

to 51,000 (Anderson, Wilson, & Fielding, 1988, p. 292).

Vaughn and Linan-Thompson (2004) contended that fluency is a key element of

reading for primary students (p. 50). According to Minskoff (2005), fluency refers to

"both a stage of learning to read and a distinct set of reading skills" (p. 122). Chall

(1978) best described this stage of learning. Chall based her work on the work of Piaget

as she proposed the stages of reading development described in Table 1. While

confirmation and fluency is stage two of Chall's model, Fountas and Pinnell (2006)

pointed out that fluency is more than just a stage or a label. Rather, it is a "characteristic

of effective reading at every level" (p. 74) and in fact changes depending upon the

context.

To review, in terms of fluency, the theory of automaticity by LaBerge and

Samuels (1974) and the theory of prosody as described by Rasinski and Hoffman (2003)

are key. In order for one to become fluent, one must spend time with text. This is

Page 30: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

addressed by Stanovich's (1986) Interactive-Compensatory model. Fluency is a

developmental process as described by Chall's stages of reading development.

Table 1

Chall's Stages Of Development

Stage Title Age Description

Prereading Birth to Age 6

Initial Reading, or Grades 1, Ages 6-7

Decoding

Confirmation and End of Grade 1 to

Fluency end of Grade 3,

Ages 7-8

Learning the New Grades 4-8,

Ages 9-13

Multiple High School,

Viewpoints Ages 14-18

Construction and College,

Reconstruction Ages 18 and above

Phonemic awareness, print

concepts, letter knowledge,

vocabulary and syntax

Sound - symbol

correspondence

Decodes fluently, moves to

more complex text, confirms

what is already known

Students are reading to learn

rather than learning to read

Synthesizing

Weigh and add information

from text to world view

Note. Adapted from Chall (1978).

Page 31: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

19

Comprehension

One major theoretical framework applied to reading comprehension is schema

theory. Reading comprehension depends on acquired knowledge and how it fits into

one's schema (Duffy, Roehler & Mason, 1984).

Reading is seen as an active process of constructing meaning by connecting old

knowledge with new information encountered in text. Readers build meaning by

engaging in a series of recursive interactions. In each interaction readers generate

a model that provides the best possible fit with the data perceived to be in the text

. . . . Gradually, iteration by iteration, readers construct their own meaning.

(Pearson, Roehler, Dole, & Duffy, 1992, p. 149)

A study led by Stahl (1989) addressed the issue of prior knowledge when the reader

encounters difficult vocabulary in unfamiliar text. "According to schema theory, the

reader's background knowledge serves as scaffolding to aid in encoding information

from the text. Thus, a person with more background knowledge is able to comprehend

better than a person with less knowledge" (Stahl, Jacobson, Davis, & Davis, 1989, pp.

284-285).

Louise Rosenblatt (1978) described her transactional theory of literacy work

wherein the reader actually has a transaction with the text. The foundation of her theory

is that a "text, once it leaves its author's hands, is simply paper and ink until a reader

evokes from it a literary work . . . " (p. ix). Rosenblatt further contended that reading

processes, such as comprehension, occur during this transaction.

A person becomes a reader by virtue of his activity in relationship to a

text, which he organizes as a set of verbal symbols. A physical text, a set

Page 32: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

20

of marks on a page, becomes the text of a poem or of a scientific formula

by virtue of its relationship with a reader who can thus interpret it and

reach through it to the world of the work. (Rosenblatt, 1978, pp. 18-19)

To conclude, two major theoretical frameworks are important when discussing

reading comprehension. One is schema theory and how prior knowledge fits into one's

schema. The other theory is Rosenblatt's transactional theory of literacy work.

History of Comprehension

Prior to 1826, an early view of the reading process followed what is known as the

memoriter model. In this model, decoding was the focus of beginning reading

instruction. Meaningful text was not introduced to the reader until decoding was

mastered. The only text considered to be comprehensible text for the student was

decodable text. The memoriter model also involved the acquisition of a sight vocabulary.

This facilitated comprehension. Comprehension was also aided by the rote memorization

of text. Comprehension mastery by the student was indicated when the student could

communicate the written text effectively through oral reading (Robinson, Faraone,

Hittleman, & Unruh, 1990).

The interlocking and step-by-step models of reading comprehension marked the

mid-nineteenth century. The interlocking model described three stages of reading:

mechanical reading (sight words, decoding, oral reading exercises), intellectual reading

(reading fast for meaning), and rhetorical reading (expressive reading). The interlocking

piece that made the entire process work was comprehension.

Preparation for the meaning of the text not only assisted in one's mastery of the

mechanics of reading, but also enabled the reader to comprehend the text through

Page 33: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

21

independent silent reading, culminating eventually in expressive reading - the

final goal of the reading act. (Robinson, et al., 1990, p. 23)

The step-by-step model was similar to the interlocking model. It included the same three

stages of reading (mechanical, intellectual, and rhetorical). However, under the step-by-

step model, the components of reading and learning occurred one step at a time. First one

learned mechanical reading, then one could engage in intellectual reading. Finally, one

could read rhetorically (Robinson, et al., 1990).

The pendulum swung towards the end of the nineteenth century, which was

marked by the thought-getting model of the reading process.

In this model (1) reading presupposes life experiences; (2) reading is primarily a

receptive process (i.e., silent reading) with thought-getting (i.e., thinking) as its

goal - a process that involves perceiving text instantaneously (i.e., written or

printed words arranged in sentences), associating text with spoken words, and

relating these ideas into thought; and (3) reading becomes an expressive act (i.e.,

oral reading) through thought-giving, when the thought acquired is shared with

others. (Robinson, et al., 1990, p. 48)

By 1917, Gray indicated that the way to improve reading comprehension was

through silent reading. Gray suggested that prior knowledge improves comprehension

along with knowledge of the elements upon which meaning depends, such as syntax.

Gray also contended that fast readers are usually better at comprehension than slow

readers (Gray, 1917).

The debate in the 1970s and early 1980s centered around whether or not reading

comprehension was a "top down" or "bottom up" process. Those that contended that

Page 34: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

22

reading comprehension was a "top down" process believed that the reader relied on his or

her background knowledge and ability to make decisions about the meaning of text.

Those that contended that reading comprehension was a "bottom up" process believed

that the reader relied on visual data for comprehension, focusing on letters, words and

sentences (Samuels, & Kamil, 1984).

In the late 1970s, Rumelhart introduced the interactive model of reading, blurring

the distinction between decoding and comprehension, indicating that each interacts with

the other.

In Rumelhart's model, the reader processes factors like letter features and

sounds (referred to as bottom-up factors) at the same time as factors like his

or her knowledge of the topic of the text and the situation in which it is read

(referred to as top-down factors). This interactive model, therefore,

suggests that reading involves simultaneous parallel processing of both

bottom-up and top-down factors. The model proposes that the weight given

to particular factors, whether top-down or bottom-up, will depend on

characteristics of the reader (such as decoding ability), the text (such as

familiarity of topics), and the context or environment in which the reading

takes place. Thus, the importance of top-down and bottom-up factors will

differ from reader to reader, text to text, and situation to situation. (Maria,

1990, p. 5)

Another interactive model was introduced in 1979 by Kintsch. The Kintsch

model assumed that comprehension was a complex process and that these processes

could operate either in serial or parallel. It specified three types of operations.

Page 35: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

23

First, the meaning elements of a text are organized into a coherent whole.

During this stage of processing, some elements are subjected to multiple

processing which, in turn, leads to better differential retention among the

text elements. Second, another set of operations compresses the full

meaning of the text into its gist. The third component generates new texts

from the memorial consequences of the comprehension processes. The

ultimate goal of this model is to be able to specify how a text is processed

sentence by sentence and to specify the outputs of the various stages of

comprehension. (Samuels & Kamil, 1984, p. 216)

By 1980, Stanovich introduced his interactive-compensatory model of reading.

The compensatory assumption states that a deficit in any knowledge source

results in a heavier reliance on other knowledge sources, regardless of their

level in the processing hierarchy. Thus, according to the interactive-

compensatory model, the poor reader who has deficient word analysis skills

might possibly show a greater reliance on contextual factors. (Stanovich, 1980, p.

64)

Through this process, a reader can compensate for their deficiencies by applying "top

down" processing and "bottom up" processing.

That is, if a person were reading a particularly difficult text that challenged his

ability to decode, but he had a great deal of background knowledge about the

topic, he could compensate for his decoding deficiencies by relying on top-down

processes to successfully comprehend the text. (Almasi, 2003, p. 74)

Page 36: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

24

To sum up, prior to 1826, it was believed that the only comprehensible text was

decodable text. This was known as the memoriter model. By the mid-nineteenth

century, the interlocking and step-by-step models took hold. By the end of the nineteenth

century the thought-getting model of the reading process was becoming popular and

involved reading as a receptive process. By 1917, Gray suggested prior knowledge

improved comprehension. But the debate by the 1970s and 1980s centered around "top

down" and "bottom up" models. A combination of the two found its way into three

interactive models by Rumelhart, Kintsch and Stanovich.

Reading Policies

In 1967, Bond and Dykstra published their landmark First Grade Studies where

they determined that the key factor to a first grader's reading success was his/her teacher.

They were unable to isolate any specific teaching methodologies that were preferable in

aiding reading instruction. From that time forward, there were a plethora of government

acts and actions that impacted reading instruction - ranging from the National

Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) in 1971 to the federal reports of the early

1980s knows as A Nation at Risk and Becoming a Nation of Readers (Allington &

McGill-Granzen, 2000). But historically, fluency has been overlooked. Fluency did not

receive enough attention for several reasons, including

the prevalence of strategies designed for individual instruction (Kuhn, 2003), an

assumption that increased amounts of decoding instruction would automatically

lead to improved fluency, (Allington, 1983; Fleisher, Jenkins, & Pany,

1979/1980), and reliance on round-robin reading as one of the primary approaches

for oral reading instruction. (Ash, Kuhn, & Walpole, 2003, p. 338)

Page 37: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

25

This was true until the Report of the National Reading Panel: Teaching Children to

Read was issued in April 2000 and found that fluency and comprehension were uniquely

tied together and were key factors in determining reading success (National Institute of

Child Health & Human Development (NICHD), 2000, p. 3-3). Further research by Kuhn

and Stahl (2003) pushed fluency to the forefront.

In examining the research on fluency, the National Reading Panel examined both

guided oral reading procedures and repeated readings to ascertain their effectiveness on

fluency development. "These two procedures have been widely recommended as

appropriate and valuable avenues for increasing fluency and overall reading

achievement" (NICHD, 2000, p. 3-28). The Panel further found that "comprehension is

critically important to the development of children's reading skills and therefore to the

ability to obtain an education" (NICHD, 2000, p. 4-1). In examining the research on

comprehension, the National Reading Panel focused on 205 studies in sixteen categories

of instruction. They concluded that only seven strategies appear to have a scientific basis

upon which one can reasonably conclude that with proper instruction the comprehension

of "normal" readers will improve. These strategies include: comprehension monitoring,

cooperative learning, use of graphic organizers, question generation, question answering

and summarization, and multiple strategies (NICHD, 2000). While the Report of the

National Reading Panel has come under much criticism, the sections on fluency and

comprehension have remained relatively unscathed (Garan, 2002). Hiebert and Fisher

(2005) did point out, however, that 75% of the texts used in the fluency studies utilized

controlled vocabulary, as opposed to current basals that have "substantially more rare

Page 38: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

26

words, and approximately 70% of these words appear a single time" (Hiebert & Fisher,

2005, p. 443).

As a result of the work of the National Reading Panel, Congress enacted the No

Child Left Behind Act of 2001 (NCLB), which emphasized reading instruction. Under

NCLB, Congress mandated "access of children to effective, scientifically based

instructional strategies and challenging academic content" (115 STAT. 1441). NCLB

also mandated that schools must meet adequate yearly progress, and the law delineated

the conditions under which each school could meet adequate yearly progress. Another

key initiative of NCLB was the Reading First grants which were developed to

provide assistance to State educational agencies and local educational agencies in

establishing reading programs for students in kindergarten through grade 3 that

are based on scientifically based reading research, to ensure that every student can

read at grade level or above not later than the end of grade 3. (115 STAT. 1535)

In summation, prior to 2000, fluency was largely overlooked in the reading

policies of the United States. However, the Report of the National Reading Panel:

Teaching Children to Read found that fluency and comprehension were uniquely tied

together. Because of the work of the National Reading Panel, Congress enacted NCLB

mandating scientifically based instruction. Fluency and consequently comprehension

were coming to the forefront of American education.

Current Research in Comprehension

In 2000, the RAND Reading Study Group (RRSG) was formed to make

recommendations about how the Department of Education might make better decisions

about the research it funds. The RRSG focused on reading comprehension and suggested

Page 39: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

27

that future research should focus on instruction, teacher preparation and professional

development, and assessment. Furthermore, the RRSG recommended a "targeted

research agenda that is sustainable, sizable, and cumulative" (Sweet & Snow, 2002, p.

46). The RRSG defined reading comprehension as

the process of simultaneously extracting and constructing meaning through

interaction and involvement with written language. In brief, understanding

comprehension requires these elements: the reader, the text, and the

activity, or purpose for reading. These elements define a phenomenon -

reading comprehension - that occurs within a larger sociocultural context

that shapes and is shaped by the reader. The broader context infuses each

of the three elements. (Snow & Sweet, 2003, p. 10)

There are three levels in the hierarchy of comprehension: literal comprehension,

interpretation, and critical evaluation. Literal comprehension involves comprehension of

the specific details of the text and comprises the lowest level in the hierarchy. The

middle level is interpretation and involves thinking about what is implied in the text.

Skills in this level include inferencing, generalizing, drawing conclusions, predicting, and

perceiving relationships (Schwartz, 1988). The highest level in the hierarchy of

comprehension is critical evaluation and occurs when "the reader evaluates written

material by measuring it against some evidence or standard and then making a judgment

about its veracity, accuracy, and quality" (Schwartz, 1988, p. 184).

The purpose of reading is comprehension (Bender & Larkin, 2003) and like any

other skill, it must be taught and must be practiced. In determining how to teach

comprehension skills, it is useful to examine the comprehension skills of good readers.

Page 40: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

28

Block and Israel (2005) reported on six research-based findings that describe the thinking

processes used by expert readers when comprehending. First, good readers not only set a

purpose for reading but they also apply what has been read to their own lives. Second,

good readers use their own comprehension process when faced with textual confusion.

These processes include predicting, drawing conclusions, summarizing, and inferring.

Third, good readers make inferences, draw conclusions, create visual images, and engage

in metacognition. Fourth, good readers rely on prior knowledge to help make meaning

when encountering unfamiliar text. Fifth, good readers use text features, story structure

and graphic organizers to help make meaning. Finally, good readers generate and answer

questions to help with meaning-making (Block & Israel, 2005). All of these mean that

good readers monitor their comprehension when reading (Pearson, Roehler, Dole &

Duffy, 1992).

Applegate, Quinn, and Applegate (2006) described the profiles of eight cognitive

approaches students utilize when discussing what they have read. Literalists believe that

the answers to all questions will be found in the text. Fuzzy Thinkers answer all

questions but their reasoning is often vague and ambiguous. Left Fielders have

unpredictable responses and those responses will often seem incoherent or illogical. Quiz

Contestants tend to rely on their background knowledge to answer questions but will not

utilize any information from the text that was just read. Politicians will answer questions

by providing the answer they think the teacher wants to hear, even if it has little to do

with the text. Dodgers actually avoid answering the question and will change the

question to something that they prefer answering. Authors will add more details to the

story than was actually in the text, as they prefer their own version of the story.

Page 41: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

29

Minimalists provide short responses and are unwilling to elaborate and explain their

thinking (Applegate, et al., 2006).

By understanding the profiles of the eight approaches readers utilize when

comprehending text, teachers can begin to make sound instructional decisions about

methodologies to use in teaching comprehension strategies. Barton and Sawyer (2003)

point out that "given the great diversity of written materials available to readers, the act

of comprehension is by nature so sophisticated that no single instructional method can be

sufficient for all readers with all texts in all learning situations" (p. 334). Barton and

Sawyer described six instructional touchstones that could be used to aid in a student's

comprehension development. These include providing repeated exposure to a variety of

texts, helping the student make reader / text connections, providing opportunities for

focused student responses through writing, talking, and drawing, directly teaching the

various comprehension strategies, providing visual structures, and assisting students in

developing their metacognition when reading (Barton and Sawyer, 2003).

There are a plethora of strategies that can be used to aid in the development of

comprehension. One such strategy is the teaching of story grammar, which provides a

cognitive structure for helping student identify the important parts of a story. In a study

by Williams (2005), the researcher found that when primary students were given explicit,

direct instruction about text structures, gains were made in comprehension. A second

strategy involves the use of student think-alouds that helps readers monitor their thinking

and thus improve their comprehension. Other strategies involve question answering,

summarization, and improvisational drama (Bender & Larkin, 2003). Teachers can also

ask strategic questions about context clues thus helping students begin to self-monitor

Page 42: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

30

when encountering unfamiliar text. Teachers should also model using textual and visual

clues to make predictions about the text. Activating prior knowledge, also known as

frontloading, can help students identify a schema in which to put the new knowledge

gleaned from the text. Students should also be taught to skim texts strategically (Bishop,

Reyes & Pflaum, 2006). Minskoff (2005) pointed out that some basic guidelines should

be followed when providing reading comprehension instruction. Explicit instruction

should be provided at pre-reading, actual reading and post-reading phases and a

multisensory approach should be used. Reading comprehension should be fun and

motivating and as such, different formats should be used for teacher questions and

student responses. When providing strategy instruction, teachers should instruct students

to become actively engaged with the reading material (Minskoff, 2005).

In the primary grades, the goal of comprehension instruction is to "build readers'

thinking processes so that they can read a text with understanding, construct memory and

metacognitive representations of what they understand, and put their new understandings

to use when communicating with others" (Block, Rodgers & Johnson, 2004, p. 4). One

of the ways this can be done is through the Talking Drawings strategy. This strategy

allows primary children to draw pictures of their current content knowledge about a

particular topic prior to reading. Then, after reading a given text and discussing with a

partner, the children are allowed to revise their drawing. By examining the pre-and post-

reading artwork, the teacher can see what advances were made in student knowledge

(Paquette, Fello, & Jalongo, 2007). Another strategy helpful for primary students is a

manipulation strategy. "Having young readers manipulate objects to correspond to the

Page 43: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

31

characters and actions in a text greatly enhances comprehension as measured by both

recall and inference tests" (Glenberg, Brown & Levin, 2007, p. 389).

Comprehension instruction is not unique to primary students however; it is an

integral part of the curriculum throughout a student's education, especially as reading

demands increase in the upper grades (Duffy, 2003). "Research has shown that many

children who read at the third grade level in grade 3 will not automatically become

proficient comprehenders in later grades. Therefore, teachers must teach comprehension

explicitly, beginning in the primary grades and continuing through high school" (Bishop,

Reyes & Pflaum, p. 66). Dymock (2007) and Pressley (2002) agreed that

comprehension must be explicitly taught. Furthermore, "comprehension abilities are not

skills that, once mastered, never need to be relearned. Rather, comprehension is an

ability that, with high-quality instruction, constantly deepens and broadens over time,

enabling students to appreciate more sophisticated and subtle meanings" (Block & Israel,

2005).

Comprehension strategies must be modeled and taught. They should be

integrated throughout the curriculum and used in the content area classes as well (Duffy,

2003; Lapp, Fisher & Grant, 2008; Ness, 2007). Students should engage in self-directed

learning as it has been found to improve their motivation to read and thus their reading

comprehension. Cooperative learning can also improve reading comprehension,

especially when it is text-based and utilizes open-ended questions. Students should be

exposed to wide variety of diverse texts and should engage in meaningful writing

activities to solidify their reading comprehension (Biancarosa, 2005, pp. 17-19).

"Comprehension lessons should include modeling, think-alouds, scaffolding, guided

Page 44: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

32

practice, and independent silent-reading opportunities to use comprehension processes

independently" (Block & Israel, p. 96). Think alouds occur through read-alouds during

which time the teacher models his or her thinking orally to illustrate comprehension

processing while reading (Davey, 1983).

There are a number of strategies that appear to improve comprehension. These

include

monitoring students' understanding and making adjustments as needed;

activating and applying relevant prior knowledge (for example, by making

predictions); generating questions; thinking aloud; attending to and

uncovering text structure; drawing inferences; constructing visual

representations; and summarizing. With each strategy, explicit teaching

should include information about what the strategy is, when it is used, how

it is used, and why it is worth using. (Duke, 2004, p. 41-42)

Furthermore, struggling readers will need additional support, especially in the

area of expository text. Many struggling readers actually "prefer information texts and

view the act of reading as one of 'work' to learn information rather than one of recreation

to 'enjoy' a story" (Reutzel, Camperell, & Smith, 2002, p. 337). In this technological

age, more support will also be needed to help students comprehend and evaluate Internet

content (Coiro, 2003). Context clues become very important at this point. They are

useful for "broadly comprehending text as well as for specifically learning new words.

Instruction in context problem solving pays great dividends" (Greenwood & Flanigan,

2007, p. 249).

Page 45: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

33

In addition to context clues, the types of activities that can support struggling

readers with their comprehension include: (1) elaborative interrogation, where students

ask "why" questions; (2) text organization instruction, where students learn to use

graphic organizers; and (3) social and self-regulatory process instruction, where students

develop self-efficacy in order to impact achievement and learn to self-regulate their

comprehension processes (Reutzel, Camperell, & Smith, 2002). Communicative reading

strategies have also been shown to help struggling readers. With the use of

communicative reading strategies, the instructor provides "contextually supported

feedback to help children reconstruct the author's message" (Crowe, 2005, p. 34).

Liang and Dole (2006) described five research-based comprehension instructional

frameworks that focused on either understanding the text or utilizing comprehension

strategies. These frameworks included the Scaffolded Reading Experience (SRE),

Questioning the Author (QtA), Collaborative Strategic Reading (CSR), Peer-Assisted

Learning Strategies (PALS) and Concept-Oriented Reading Instruction (CORI). SRE

involves the use of prereading and postreading activities that will scaffold the individual

reader and promote comprehension. QtA involves the use of teacher and student

generated questions about the author's ideas. In CSR, students collaboratively move

through four processes - previewing, monitoring, summarizing, and wrapping up. PALS

pairs a high and low reader together and they each take turns reading, developing main

ideas, and predicting. CORI is used with content area subjects and allows students to ask

questions, gather information, comprehend and integrate, and then communicate their

learning (Liang & Dole, 2006).

Page 46: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

34

Another framework or technique in developing a reader's comprehension is story

retelling. In a story retelling, the reader can either verbally or in writing tell what he or

she remembers about a given story.

Retelling requires the child to construct a personal text, making inferences based

on original text as well as on prior knowledge. Research demonstrates that

instruction and practice in retelling usually result in the development of

comprehension, a sense of story structure, and oral complexity in a child's use of

language. (Morrow, 1989, p. 54)

In a 1994 study, researchers found that middle school students were particularly

successful with story retelling when they used advance organizers as a prereading activity

and combined the organizers with prereading discussions (Rinehart, Barksdale-Ladd,

Paterson, 1994, p. 244).

Research has also shown that i^ciprocal teaching will aid in the development of

comprehension. Through the use of four strategies - summarizing, questioning,

clarifying, and predicting - the teacher and students engage in discussion. The teacher

models the use of the strategies and then the students use the strategies in a small group

format (Stahl, 2004). Studies by Van Keer (2004) and Van Keer and Verhaeghe (2005)

indicated that explicit modeling, followed by a whole group discussion and then cross-

age peer tutoring will also improve reading comprehension.

One of the factors that impacts a student's reading comprehension is their

vocabulary development. "One of the longest, most clearly articulated lines of research

in literacy education describes the strong connection between readers' vocabulary

knowledge and their reading comprehension" (Blachowicz & Fisher, 2004, p. 66). As

Page 47: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

35

Boulware-Gooden, Carreker, Thornhill and Joshi (2007) pointed out, "students need

vocabulary knowledge and metacognitive skills so they can monitor their understanding

and reflect on what has been read" (Boulware-Gooden, et al., 2007, p. 71). Children who

come from homes that do not have print-rich environments and do not engage in shared

reading experiences with adults tend to have poorly developed oral language. "Limited

vocabulary knowledge is a major contributor to the oral language deficits that diminish

young children's comprehension" (Reutzel, Camperell, & Smith, 2002, p. 323). Juel and

Deffes (2004) report, "research suggests that the vocabulary of entering 1st graders

predicts not only their word reading ability at the end of 1st grade but also their 11th grade

reading comprehension" (Juel & Deffes, 2004, p. 31).

It is important to note that while the focus in improving reading comprehension

has been through the use of reading strategies, writing can also help as well. Reading and

writing are integrated processes. After all, "opportunities to respond in writing allow

students to think again about their reading, this time on paper" (Harvey & Goudvis, 2000,

p. 30). In a 2007 study, researchers reported on four writing strategies that teachers can

use to improve reading comprehension. These include About/Point (a summarizing

strategy), Cubing, (a strategy for asking questions from multiple perspectives), Four

Square Graphic Organizer (a strategy for organizing thoughts) and Read, Respond,

Revisit, Discuss (an interactive journaling strategy) (Wallace, Pearman, Hail, & Hurst,

2007).

Yet another significant factor in comprehension development is that of

motivation. Not only must students have the skill to read, they must have the desire to

read as well. "As individuals read more, they read better and learn more about the world.

Page 48: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

36

The result is better comprehension . . . " (Gambrell, Block & Pressley, 2002, pp. 7-8).

Duffy (2003) concurred with the aforementioned and stated that "oral language and

vocabulary are the basis of comprehension ... [and] comprehension is enhanced when

students are engaged and motivated" (Duffy, 2003, pp. 5-6).

Tied in with motivation is engagement. A study by Lutz, Guthrie and Davis

(2006) found that reading comprehension increased when students were engaged in

learning and there was a "high complexity of literacy tasks in which students are

engaged" (Lutz, et al., 2006, p. 13).

Pardo (2004) agreed that a strong vocabulary, motivation and engagement are key

to developing readers with good comprehension. She also pointed out the students must

have good decoding skills and must be fluent readers. If students are no longer working

on decoding issues, and "as word reading becomes automatic, students become fluent and

can focus on comprehension" (Pardo, 2004, p. 273).

One way to support students' comprehension is through instructional scaffolding

(Duffy, p. 55). Clark and Graves (2004) reported that scaffolding is useful in several

ways in that it can "aid students by helping them to better complete a task, to complete a

task with less stress or in less time, or to learn more fully than they would have

otherwise" (Clark & Graves, 2004, p. 571). Scaffolding is perhaps best understood by

examining the gradual release of responsibility model (Pearson & Gallagher, 1983)

shown in Figure 1. In this gradual release of responsibility model, students gradually

progress from

situations in which the teacher takes the majority of the responsibility for

successfully completing a reading task, to situations in which students assume

Page 49: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

37

increasing responsibility for reading tasks, and finally to situations in which

students take all or nearly all the responsibility for reading tasks. At any point in

time, teachers should scaffold students enough so that they do not give up on the

task or fail at it but not scaffold them so much that they do not have the

opportunity to actively work on the problem themselves (Clark & Graves, 2004,

p. 571).

Figure 1. The gradual release of responsibility model of instruction.

The gradual release of responsibility model of instruction

Proportion of responsibility for task completion

All teacher Joint responsibility All student

Retrieved October 13, 2006 from http.7/home.earthlink.net/~ihholly/gradualrelease.htm

Effective comprehension instruction utilizes this gradual release of responsibility

and utilizes strategies to teach students to have a plan of action. It does not involve

merely reading a passage and answering a few questions. These strategies include the

ability to "self-monitor, summarize, use graphic organizers, ask questions, use semantic

organizers, identify story structures, relate reading material to prior knowledge, and use

mental imagery" (Minskoff, 2005, p. 138). These strategies must be taught. As Carnine

et al. (2006) pointed out, "good instruction is the most powerful means of promoting

Page 50: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

38

proficient comprehension and preventing comprehension problems" (p. 209).

Furthermore, this instruction must begin in the primary grades (Vaughn & Linan-

Thompson, 2004).

However, a study done in 1978 by Dolores Durkin indicated that comprehension

was not being taught - it was being tested but not being explicitly taught. As Vaughn and

Linan-Thompson (2004) reported,

Durkin found that in a study of over 4000 minutes of 4l grade reading

instruction, only 20 minutes of comprehension instruction was recorded.

These findings shocked researchers and teachers at the time. More recent

studies reveal that explicit comprehension instruction is still not being

provided as often as it should be. (Vaughn & Linan-Thompson, 2004, p. 100)

Durkin (1978) described classroom after classroom that was focused on volumes of ditto

sheets. This proved problematic for comprehension as the sheer magnitude of ditto

sheets

often meant that several days intervened between the time a story was read by

children and the time their teachers queried them about it. With the delay, it was

impossible to ascertain whether the questions were assessing the ability to

comprehend or the ability to recall what had been comprehended. (Durkin, 1978,

p. 524)

Durkin (1983) indicated that part of the problem was that teacher's manuals provided a

great deal of comprehension assessment with little accompanying comprehension

instruction. "Why comprehension is constantly tested but rarely taught has no obvious

Page 51: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

39

explanation. Perhaps as more is learned about the comprehension process, more

suggestions for teaching it will get into manuals" (Durkin, 1978, pp. 280-281).

Pressley (2002) pointed out that education is still focusing less on comprehension

instruction and more on word recognition skills (p. 388). In a 1998 study, researchers

discovered that not much progress had been made in comprehension instruction since the

Durkin study in 1978.

... despite a great deal of research in the past two decades on how to

promote children's comprehension of what they read, we observed only

rare instances of explicit comprehension instruction. Indeed, the situation

still seems to be much as Durkin described, with a great deal of testing of

comprehension but very little teaching of it. (Pressley, Wharton-

McDonald, Mistretta-Hampston, & Echevarria, 1998, pp. 186-187)

A study by Wendler, Samuels and Moore (1989) found similar results. Further, they

found that much of the comprehension instructional time was actually comprehension

assessment with the teacher asking the students questions. Twenty-two years later,

however, some of Durkin's concerns were addressed. Dole (2000) reported about a paper

presented to the American Educational Research Association wherein Rosenshine (1997)

discussed the extent to which cognitive strategy instruction has been implemented

in recent basal reading programs . . . . Developers of basal programs have paid

attention to the research on comprehension strategies and now include these

strategies as part of their instructional program. (Dole, 2000, p. 62)

Page 52: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

40

While comprehension and comprehension strategy instruction are vital, Nelson-

Herber and Johnston (1989) pointed out that teachers should not lose sight of the ultimate

goal of reading - enjoyment of literature.

All children should have access to whatever skills they may need to help

them understand and enjoy stories. Our mistake in teaching is that we

sometimes focus so hard on the skills and strategies that we (and the

children) lose sight of the goal. Perhaps we need to remind ourselves that

one way children learn to read better is by reading more. There needs to be

a time for teaching and a time for reading for pleasure. (Herber & Johnston, 1989,

p. 269)

As seen in the aforementioned studies, the purpose of reading is comprehension

and it is a skill that must be taught and practiced. There are a variety of strategies to be

taught including teaching students cognitive structures for identifying story grammar and

using think-alouds to model the comprehension strategies. Comprehension must be

explicitly taught, especially in the primary grades. Scaffolding, such as through the

gradual release of responsibility model, can be useful. Comprehension instruction is

beginning to occur more in classrooms and it is vital that this trend continue.

Current Research in Fluency

Fluency is a key to reading instruction. One text described it as the "bridge

between word recognition and comprehension" (Carnine, Silbert, Kame'enui, Tarver, &

Jungjohann, 2006, p. 141). In fact, the reader must indeed use comprehension in order to

support fluency (Fountas & Pinnell, 2006; Daly, Chafouleas & Skinner, 2005). But, as

Topping (2006) pointed out "fluency is of little value in itself- its value lies in what it

Page 53: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

41

enables" (Topping, 2006, p. 106). Fluency is important because fluent readers are more

likely to comprehend and thus are more likely to choose to read. Fluent reading also

requires less effort than decoding (Daly, Chafouleas, & Skinner, 2005). "Fluency enables

students to focus on constructing meaning from text" (Walley, 1993, p. 526). However, it

is important to note that a reader can be fluent without comprehending. Cole (2004)

described a group of English language learners who could read English fluently with

absolutely no comprehension. They had mastered decoding but not comprehension.

Research has shown that

most children develop into fluent readers by third grade. Approximately 75

percent of students who are poor readers in third grade continue to be lower

achieving readers in ninth grade and, in essence, do not recover their reading

abilities even into adulthood. (Corcoran & Davis, 2005, p. 105)

Cole (2004) described the attributes of a fluent reader. First, they have a large sight

vocabulary. Second, a fluent reader effectively uses decoding strategies. A fluent reader

also reads audibly and in phrases or chunks. When reading a rehearsed text, a fluent

reader can read at a smooth, steady pace. Fluency is impacted by variables such as type

of text being read, purpose for reading, and prior knowledge about the topic of the text

(Johns, 2005). It is noteworthy that students will have different needs in regards to the

amount of practice time they will require in improving both their fluency and accuracy

(Carnine et al., 2006). Reading fluency is impacted by the different demands text

features place on readers. For example, familiarity with a genre type will facilitate

fluency, as will prior knowledge about text structures, content, themes and ideas,

language and literary features, vocabulary and words. The complexity of sentences will

Page 54: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

42

also impact a reader's fluency (Fountas & Pinnell, 2006). Worthy and Broaddus (2002)

compared reading fluency to being a musician.

Just as musicians learn common chords and melodic sequences, fluent

readers must have a vocabulary of high-frequency words, graphophonic

skills, and strategies for accurately decoding new words. Frequent

opportunities to practice identifying words through meaningful reading and

writing experiences help the reader to achieve automatic word identification

or automaticity, just as practicing scales and favorite pieces helps the

musician to develop technical expertise. (Worthy & Broaddus, 2002, p. 335)

O'Connor, Bell, Harly, Larkin, Sackor, and Zigmond (2002) conducted a fluency

intervention study on upper elementary students, specifically third through fifth grade

students. O'Connor et al. found that those who were the farthest behind in terms of

fluency made the greatest gains if they were continuously given books to read that were

on their reading level (Rasinski, 2003). Stahl and Heubach (2005) conducted a similar

study on second grade students with similar results. Further research reported that

explicit fluency instruction should begin no later than second grade (Moskal

&Blachowicz, 2006) with some contending that fluency measures should actually begin

during the middle of first grade (Chard, Pikulski, & McDonagh, 2006, p. 56).

Block and Israel (2005) pointed out that students actually needed to practice with

materials both below their level, on their level, and occasionally above their level. They

used the following analogy.

Fluency development is analogous to learning to play tennis. A tennis

player is tested to determine her level of play and then placed in a

Page 55: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

43

category with players at this same level. On occasion she'll play down a level to

get in a quick game without the need to exert a lot of effort, or play up a level in

order to challenge herself and determine where she needs practice. To be the best

one can be and to develop skill level most efficiently, one needs to play (or read

in this case) within the range of skill level where frequent successes will be

obtained. Repeated reading - repeated tennis, read a lot at the right level - play a

lot at the right level. (Block & Israel, 2005, pp. 82-83)

As in tennis, the amount of practice needed in order to become a fluent reader varies with

each individual. It becomes the role of the instructor to determine who needs the most

practice and develop a practice schedule for those needing a more intense workout

(Carnine, et al., 2006).

Allington (1999) stated "research has shown that kids with a low degree of

fluency are less likely to understand what they read" (Allington, 1999, p. 12). Archer,

Gleason, and Vachon (2003) provide the following example to illustrate this point.

A recent incident with a young friend provided the perfect analogy. He

had just obtained his driver's permit at age 15-1/2 and begged to drive to

the store. As drivers, our cognitive resources must respond to two aspects

of driving: the mechanics of driving (brakes, gas, windshield wipers, etc.)

and road hazards. As a new driver, Matt was deeply engrossed in the

mechanics of driving. As he searched for the windshield wipers, he pulled

into the wrong lane, and we were faced with a semi-truck. After one of us

grabbed the wheel and pulled us back to safety, Mart's gift to us became

evident: the perfect fluency analogy. Attend to the mechanics and face a

Page 56: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

44

semi. Attend to decoding and miss the gist. (Archer, et al., 2003, p. 96)

Bender and Larkin (2003) described the areas of the brain that are involved in fluent

reading versus decoding.

If the brain of a student reading a particular passage tends to be more active in

Broca's area ... than in the visual cortex or Wenicke's area ..., that student is

concentrating on the meaning .... The student would more than likely

demonstrate very fluent reading skills on that particular passage. (Bender &

Larkin, 2003, p. 124)

Fluent reading does not just mean fast reading. Allington (2001) described other features

that must be considered as well.

Clay and Imlach (1971) conducted the classic study on the development of

reading fluency. They examined the reading behaviors of 100 beginning

readers and noted that those early readers making the greatest progress not

only read faster and more accurately but also with better phrasing and

intonation. While the lowest-progress readers read aloud in one and two

word segments, the highest-progress readers read in five to seven word

phrases. Of course, reading in phrases produced faster reading as well.

The high-progress readers also spontaneously self-corrected four and five

times as many of their word pronunciation errors as did the lower-progress

readers. Thus, these two characteristics, phrase reading with appropriate

intonation and spontaneous self-correction of many misread words, were

clearly associated with those children making better progress in learning to

read. (Allington, 2001, p. 71)

Page 57: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

45

The importance of fluency cannot be overstated. In a study of struggling older

readers by Archer et al. (2003), fluency was determined to be a foundation skill. Students

who did not become fluent readers in primary grades, grew further and further behind as

they advanced in years. Archer et al. (2003) recommended that struggling older readers

receive reading practice in the areas of guided reading, choral reading, partner reading,

and repeated reading activities to enhance fluency development. Garriot and Jones

(2005) stated "building fluency is a major issue with struggling middle grade readers,

who may have done well in elementary school but find themselves stymied by more

demanding middle school texts" (Garriot & Jones, 2005, p.67). Blau (1999)

recommended that students in second through fifth grades receive fluency instruction

through the following strategies: modeling of fluent reading, repeated readings in class,

use of phrased reading in class, use of tutors in class, and use of reader's theater in class.

Bullion-Mears, McCauley and McWhorter (2007) recommended some of these

performance techniques, such as reader's theater and poetry, not only for fluency practice

but also to build comprehension. They recommend taking nonfiction text and turning it

into poems and reader's theater. This allows the students to work on both fluency and

comprehension, while navigating the more difficult nonfiction text.

Allinder, Dunse, Brunken, and Obermiller-Krolikowski (2001) studied fluency in

at-risk readers and students with learning disabilities. The treatment group was taught

with specific oral reading fluency strategies. Teacher-student conferences were held with

the control group during which time students were instructed to do their best while

reading. Results indicated that "all students improved on a standardized norm-referenced

test of comprehension, but students who used a specific oral reading strategy made

Page 58: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

46

significantly greater progress in reading, as measured by curriculum-based measurement

maze procedures" (Allinder et al., 2001, p. 48). Conderman and Strobel (2006)

recommended that students with disabilities utilize a guided repeated oral reading

technique to promote oral reading fluency. This technique allows for an initial reading,

or cold read, of a passage on Monday, and then multiple opportunities to practice the

same passage throughout the week. Finally, on Friday, the student reads the passage for

the final time, or the hot read, and data is again collected. The student's progress is

noted. This data provides the framework for future interventions.

Begeny and Martens (2006) also looked at low-performing readers by establishing

reading fluency interventions with a group of third graders. Like the Allinder et al. study,

Begeny and Martens found that students made greater progress in reading when measured

by maze comprehension passages. Begeny and Martens also found that oral reading

rates improved as well with the introduction of reading fluency interventions These

interventions included word-list training, listening passage preview, and repeated reading

instruction. The National Reading Panel (2000) also found that

the analysis of guided oral reading procedures led to the conclusion that such

procedures had a consistent, and positive impact on word recognition, fluency,

and comprehension as measured by a variety of test instruments and at a range of

grade levels (NICHD, 2000, p. 3-3).

Repeated readings is a method developed by Samuels (1979). "The method

consists of rereading a short, meaningful passage several times until a satisfactory level

of fluency is reached. Then the procedure is repeated with a new passage" (Samuels,

1979, p. 403). Repeated readings combined with word boxes, a phonics technique, have

Page 59: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

47

also proved useful in increasing the fluency rates of high school students that had severe

reading delays. Devault and Joseph (2004) studied three high school students who were

severely delayed readers. Their research indicated that all three students increased their

fluency rates when presented with the instructional techniques of repeated reading

coupled with word boxes. Samuels, Schermer and Reinking (1992) warned

although it (repeated reading) is a useful technique for nonautomatic

decoders, it is not recommended for students who are already reading

fluently. The method is satisfying because it works, and students who

have had histories of reading failure can experience the feeling of being

able to read with expression and understanding. (Samuels, et al., 1992, p. 138)

Therrien and Kubina (2006) describe repeated reading as an efficient technique for

helping students to gain reading fluency.

Repeated reading directly targets oral reading fluency and can easily be

integrated in an existing reading program. Previous research has shown

that repeated reading is effective with a variety of students, including

students with disabilities. Using essential instructional components and

selecting appropriate materials maximizes the effectiveness of repeated

reading. (Therrien & Kubina, 2006, p. 159)

Yurick, Robinson, Cartledge, Lo, and Evans (2006) furthered this notion with their study

on repeated readings. Yurick et al. conducted three experiments focusing on the effects

of peer-mediated repeated readings on reading fluency and comprehension. Their

findings indicated that oral reading rate, reading accuracy and comprehension improved

more during peer-mediated repeated reading than during silent sustained reading.

Page 60: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

48

Samuels (2002) reported

in the two decades since the method (repeated readings) was first

introduced, more than 100 studies have been published that have tested the

repeated reading method. A consistent finding from these studies is that

repeated reading practice produces statistically significant improvement in

reading speed, word recognition, and oral reading expression on the

practice passages. (Samuels, 2002, p. 179)

Readers' theater is a form of repeated readings done in a "meaningful and

purposeful context" (Strecker, 1999, p. 329). The scripts are adapted from a piece of

prose or poetry so they are suitable for oral reading (Hertzberg, 2000, p. 22). Corcoran

and Davis (2005) conducted a study assessing the effects of readers' theater on second

and third grade special education students' fluency. The results from this study indicated

both reading attitudes and confidence levels of these struggling readers improved as they

repeatedly practiced these readers' theater scripts in their groups. Furthermore, their

fluency rates improved as well: "the number of words read correctly per minute

increased overall as a class by an increase of 17 additional words read correctly in spring

versus winter" (Corcoran & Davis, 2005, p. 110). Griffith and Rasinski (2004) reported

students in Griffith's classroom made 2.3 years reading growth in terms of

comprehension and increased their reading rate by 47.4 words per minute as a result of

the use of readers' theater in the classroom throughout the year. Keehn (2003)

conducted a readers' theater study wherein one treatment group received readers' theater

intervention and the other treatment group received readers' theater intervention plus

explicit instruction. Both groups made "statistically significant growth in oral reading

Page 61: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

49

fluency during the nine-week Reader's Theater intervention ... but there was no

significant difference in growth made by the two treatment groups..." (p. 49).

Yet another intervention that appears to improve fluency is paired reading

(Mastropieri, Leinert, and Scruggs, 1999). In paired reading, a strong reader is paired

with a struggling reader. In a case study by Ferrara (2005), a struggling reader was

instructed with paired reading. The struggling reader's fluency was then examined pre-

and post- paired reading. Her pre-intervention reading rate was 84 words per minute and

post-intervention reading rate rose to 140.6 words per minute. Nes (2003) found similar

results in a paired reading study. Her results indicated that "reading fluency improved

substantially for all participants, while accuracy and comprehension remained stable and

high throughout the study" (Nes, 2003, p. 179).

Dowrick, Kim-Rupnow, and Power (2006) describe a process known as video

feedforward which also improves reading fluency. The principle behind video

feedforward is to video the child reading fluently, show the child the video, thus

encouraging the child to read more fluently.

The images of fluent passage reading were achieved mostly by capturing

the child's echo reading, editing out the tutor's modeling, and interspersing

glimpses of the tutor's face as cutaways. The accurate recognition of sight

words was achieved by taking advantage of the improvements that occurred

by the sixth or seventh time through the flashcards. On the feedforward

principle, it was important to select and repeat the rare successes of

individually difficult words rather than make the easier choice of selecting

better known words .... (Dowrick, et al., 2006, p. 198)

Page 62: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

50

Lionetti and Cole (2004) found that listening while reading also increased reading

fluency. Researchers tried adjusting the rate at which students were listening while

reading, one rate resembling the reader's natural oral reading rate, and one rate

approximately 20% above the reader's current oral reading rate. "Results indicated that

both rates increased words correct per minute and a high level of accuracy was

maintained ... neither intervention had any effect on comprehension" (Lionetti & Cole,

2004, p. 114).

Explicit timing also seems to have had an impact upon students' fluency scores.

In a study by Cates and Rhymer (2006), students who knew they were being explicitly

timed when they were reading improved their reading rate over times when they did not

think they were being timed. Moskal (2006) discovered that timing of students during

repeated readings did in fact improve their time. In this study, the researcher also found

that students could self-manage their repeated readings; the teacher did not have to

conduct all the timings. The students were self-directed and still made the fluency gains.

Evidence-based literacy practices have impacted fluency scores as well. In a

study by Greenwood, Tapia, Abbott, and Walton (2003), teachers formed cohorts and

engaged in evidence-based literacy staff development. These evidence-based literacy

practices included: shared book experience, phonemic awareness, repeated reading,

initial reading blending, early intervention reading, partner reading, word family books,

dolch words, writer's workshop, reading class-wide peer tutoring, spelling class-wide

peer tutoring, partner reading questions, and reciprocal teaching. As a result of the

implementation of these evidence-based literacy practices, overall fluency scores

improved 58.3 words per minute. Welsch (2006) listed many of these evidence-based

Page 63: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

51

literacy practices as well. He also added repeated reading with teacher/peer/audiotape/

CD modeling; choral reading, use of praise; use of appropriate-level text, use of

predictable or patterned text, use of word/phrase/letter-naming drills, use of computer

programs, and use of parent/school reading programs.

Blevins (2000) described the types of direct instruction and feedback that students

need in regards to fluency instruction. Students should be explicitly taught the sound-

spelling correspondences and should practice new or difficult words. Teachers should

explain the return-sweep eye movement and should also teach appropriate phrasing and

intonation. Motivation is also key and can be done with incentives, charting and rewards.

Hasbrouck and Tindal (2006) offered a cautionary note about reading fluency and

the recent push to increase fluency scores.

... there appears to be a tendency among some educators to believe that raising a

student's fluency score is "the" main goal of reading instruction. As important as

fluency is, and as valuable as the information obtained from fluency-based

assessments can be for instructional decision making, we caution teachers and

administrators to keep fluency and fluency-based assessment scores in

perspective. Helping our students become fluent readers is absolutely critical for

proficient and motivated reading. Nonetheless, fluency is only one of the

essential skills involved in reading. (Hasbrouck & Tindal, 2006, p. 642).

Johns (2005) echoed this point when discussing the factors that impact fluency. A

student's fluency is affected by the type of text being read - either narrative or

informational. It can also be affected by the purpose for reading, as well as prior

knowledge. Simply put, "fluency norms give no attention to these important

Page 64: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

52

considerations, so it is up to teachers to be mindful of these variables when assessing oral

reading rate and using oral reading fluency norms" (Johns, 2005, p. 5). Torgeson and

Hudson (2006) indicated that reading fluency is influenced by the following: proportion

of "sight" words in a passage; variations processing speed of "sight" words, decoding

fluency of unknown words, use of context clues to facilitate word identification, speed

with which word meanings are determined, speed with which overall meaning is

determined, and differences in the value the reader places on speed versus accuracy in

reading. Rhodes and Dudley-Marling (1996) further stated

no one is always a fluent reader; each of us can think of situations in which

our reading was or could be less fluent. Thus we cannot set as a goal

helping students to become fluent readers in all situations. Our goal instead

must be to help them become fluent readers in an increasingly wider range

of reading situations (Rhodes & Dudley-Marling, 1996, p. 154).

Fluency research was brought to the forefront with an examination of the research

by Kuhn and Stahl (2003). Their pivotal work examined the significant pieces of

research on fluency. They found that

(a) fluency instruction is generally effective, although it is unclear whether this is

because of specific instructional features or because it involves children in

reading increased amounts of text; (b) assisted approaches seem to be more

effective than unassisted approaches; (c) repetitive approaches do not seem to

hold a clear advantage over nonrepetitive approaches; and (d) effective fluency

instruction moves beyond automatic word recognition to include rhythm and

Page 65: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

53

expression, or what linguists refer to as the prosodic features of language. (Kuhn

& Stahl, 2003, p. 3)

The aforementioned studies indicated fluency is a key component of reading

instruction. Most children are fluent by third grade. Those who are not yet fluent need to

be given books on their reading level. Those that read dysfluently are likely to not

comprehend what they read. Fluent reading is more than reading fast; it also is reading

with phrasing and intonation. Techniques for improving fluency include repeated

readings, readers' theater, paired reading, video feedforward, listening while reading,

explicit timing, shared book experience, phonemic awareness, partner reading, and

reciprocal teaching. Fluency is impacted by the type of text being read, purpose for

reading and the reader's prior knowledge.

Intersection of Fluency and Comprehension

"Fluency has been shown to have a 'reciprocal relationship' with comprehension,

with each fostering the other," (Stecker, Roser, & Martinez, 1998, p. 306). Pressley,

Gaskins and Fingeret (2006) stated "fluency and comprehension are not so much linear

processes but are interdependent in a 'blurry' sort of way" (Pressley, et al., 2006, p. 62).

They further contended that "comprehension strategies should be taught to all readers

from the beginning of reading instruction, even if they have not yet become fluent"

(Pressley, et al., 2006, p. 62). The link between fluency and comprehension was best

described by Allington (1999).

Research has shown that kids with a low degree of fluency are less likely to

understand what they read. The skills of summarizing, analyzing, and

synthesizing material - essential for high-level thinking - seem to require

Page 66: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

54

fluent reading. When kids read fluently, paying attention to phrasing and

intonation, it's obvious that they understand what they're reading. But

when kids read word by word, syllable by syllable, or even phrase by

phrase in that familiar monotone, it's a signal that their attention is not

directed at making sense out of the text. Instead, they're spending their

cognitive energy on decoding. (Allington, 1999, p. 12)

In two studies by Berninger, Abbott, Vermeulen, and Fulton (2006), researchers

examined at-risk second grade readers. Both studies found a relationship between

fluency and comprehension. In Study 1, "both accuracy and rate of word-level and text-

level oral reading were correlated significantly with and contributed unique variance to

reading comprehension in at-risk second-grade readers" (Berninger, et al., 2006, p. 348).

Study 2 "also demonstrated that instruction that integrated phonological decoding, real-

word reading, text reading, and reading comprehension improved reading fluency in at-

risk second-grade readers" (Berninger, et al., 2006, p. 348).

A study by Bryant, Vaughn, Linan-Thompson, Ugel, Hamff & Hougen (2000)

examined the reading outcomes for middle school students with and without reading

disabilities. Researchers examined average achievers, low achievers, and students with

reading disabilities.

The fluency results were particularly encouraging for all three groups of

students. Data showed that with intensive practice, students with reading

disabilities in particular gained from the program, which consisted of two to

three 30-minute fluency training sessions per week. These data suggest that

students with reading disabilities can benefit from a fluency-building

Page 67: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

55

strategy and that many struggling readers would profit from repeated

reading fluency instruction incorporated into their curricula. However,

most middle-school curricula do not include fluency building as a target

skill. ... students with reading disabilities need to learn effective decoding

strategies (e.g., word identification) and develop fluency (e.g., partner

reading) satisfactorily before comprehension can take place more readily.

(Bryant, etal., 2000, p. 251)

A study by Fuchs, Fuchs, Hosp, and Jenkins (2001) examined seventy middle

school and junior high students.

... students' oral reading fluency was most strongly associated with

capacity to read passages and answer questions about those passages on a

widely used, commercial achievement test of reading comprehension ....A

correlation between oral reading fluency and performance on the Reading

Comprehension subtest of the Stanford Achievement Tests of .91 is

nonetheless impressive because oral reading fluency, on its fact, does not

require students to understand what they read. The high correlation for oral

reading fluency, however, does corroborate theoretically driven hypotheses

about the potential of oral reading fluency as an indicator of overall reading

competence. (Fuchs, et al., 2001, p. 245)

Hitchcock, Prater, and Dowrick (2004) examined the effects of tutoring and video

self-modeling on four first-grade students with reading difficulties. Reading fluency,

"measured in number of correct words per minute, double for three students and

quadrupled for the fourth by the end of eight weeks. Reading comprehension measured

Page 68: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

56

in number of correct responses, reached pre-established criteria" (Hitchcock, et al., 2004,

p. 89). The tutoring and the video self-modeling significantly impacted both reading

fluency and comprehension.

Text presentation also impacts reading fluency and comprehension in struggling

readers. In a study by Lagrou, Burns, Mizerek and Mosack (2006), researchers presented

groups of students with reading material from a book and reading material that was on a

separate typed page. While there was no difference in mean fluency and comprehension

scores for the two highest reading skills groups,

the difference between the Typed and Book conditions was significant for both

fluency and comprehension among children in the lowest reading group. Students

in the lowest reading group read more fluently and with better comprehension

from the Book condition than from the Typed condition (Lagrou, et al., 2006, p.

100).

The fluency and comprehension of struggling readers in kindergarten through

third grade was examined in a study by O'Connor, Harty, and Pulmer (2005).

Researchers found that Tier 3 interventions made the most difference in fluency and

comprehension. Tier 3 interventions included daily small group or individual instruction.

Comprehension scores increased 15% and fluency scores increased 100%. Vaughn,

Linan-Thompson, Kouzekanani, Bryant, Dickson, and Blozis (2003) also examined the

impact of small group and individual instruction on fluency and comprehension. The

results of their study indicated that optimal grouping formats included 1:1 (one teacher

with one student) and 1:3 (one teacher with three students) instruction. 1:10 (one teacher

Page 69: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

57

with ten students) instruction did not have as strong a benefit as the smaller group

instruction.

Reutzel and Hollingsworth (1993) examined the effects of fluency training on the

reading comprehension of second graders. Oral recitation lessons improved oral reading

fluency; round robin reading did not. Furthermore, researchers learned that fluency

training had a statistically significant impact on comprehension. "These results provide

tentative evidence for a causal effect of fluency training upon students' reading

comprehension as well as an argument against the use of a RR (round robin) reading

approach for developing either fluency or comprehension" (Reutzel & Hollingsworth,

1993, p. 325).

Stage and Jacobsen (2001) examined the predictive nature of fluency scores on

comprehension. They examined one hundred seventy-three fourth graders in the state of

Washington. Their research determined that "oral reading fluency probes administered in

September, January, and May reliably predicted May WASL reading performance"

(Stage & Jacobsen, 2001, p. 407) The WASL is the Washington Assessment of Student

Learning and is a performance-based test measuring reading comprehension.

In the previous section, research indicated that repeated readings were useful in

increasing reading fluency rates. Repeated readings can also improve comprehension

scores. A study by Therrien (2004) indicated that "repeated reading improves the

reading fluency and comprehension of both nondisabled (ND) students and students with

LD (learning disabled). All students obtained a moderate mean increase in fluency ...

and a somewhat smaller mean increase in comprehension" (Therrien, 2004, p. 257).

Page 70: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

58

Vaughn, Chard, Bryant, Coleman, Tyler, Linan-Thompson, and Kouzekanani

(2000) conducted a 12-week study examining fluency and comprehension interventions

for third grade students. Their research indicated that fluency and comprehension

interventions created "statistically significant effects for rate of reading and correct words

read per minute (but not accuracy or comprehension) for both partner reading and

collaborative strategic ..." (Vaughn, et al., 2000, p. 325).

Manset-Williamson and Nelson (2005) examined the effect of explicit instruction

and guided reading on fluency and comprehension in upper elementary and middle

school students with reading disabilities. Their findings indicate that both approaches

had relatively the same impact on fluency scores. However, explicit instruction had a

greater impact on comprehension than did guided reading (Manset-Williamson & Nelson,

2005, p. 69).

To reiterate, research has shown that fluency and comprehension are related

aspects of reading. Video self-modeling impacts fluency and comprehension as does text

presentation. Research has also shown that small group or individualized instruction

seems to make the most difference in impacting the fluency and comprehension of

struggling readers. Repeated reading not only improves fluency but comprehension

scores as well. According to the research, explicit instruction and guided reading impact

fluency and explicit instruction impacts comprehension more than guided reading does.

Research on First Grade Students

Early intervention is critical. A study by Coyne, Kame'enui, Simmons and Harn

(2004) indicated "strong responders to kindergarten intervention can experience an

Page 71: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

59

inoculation effect through the middle of first grade with research-validated classroom

reading instruction" (p. 90). Furthermore,

first grade represents a pivotal time in reading development, as students

learn to read words by sight and through decoding strategies. Ultimately,

children must be able to accurately and quickly identify words in text - that

is, they must develop oral reading fluency. The development of oral

reading fluency ... is a gradually developing, complex skill. (Speece &

Ritchey, 2005, p. 394)

Speece and Ritchey (2005) examined the development of oral reading fluency in first

grade students. Researchers found that

letter-sound fluency was a uniquely significant predictor of first-grade

reading fluency .... Growth in first-grade oral reading fluency accounted

for the most unique variance in second-grade growth and end-of-year

performance. The results suggest that word reading fluency should be

regarded as developing concomitantly with early word recognition rather

than as a later-developing skill. (Speece & Ritchey, 2005, p. 387)

Research has shown that explicit instruction improves reading comprehension in

older students (Manset-Williamson & Nelson, 2005). However, explicit instruction is

also useful for first grade students as well. Eilers and Pinkley (2006) examined the effect

of explicit instruction

in the metacognitive strategies of making text connections, predicting, and

sequencing .... Results showed a significant difference in students'

awareness of comprehension strategies and comprehension of text as

Page 72: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

60

measured by the Index of Reading Awareness and the Beaver

Developmental Reading Assessment before and after the intervention.

These findings suggest that students in primary grades may benefit from

explicit instruction in reading comprehension strategies at the same time

they are learning to decode words. (Eilers & Pinkley, 2006, p. 13)

A study by Garner and Bochna (2004) also found that explicit instruction is useful for

first grade students. In their study, Garner and Bochna provided intervention group

students with instruction in narrative text structure. "Intervention group students

demonstrated superior comprehension in relation to all story elements and more

frequently displayed metalinguistic awareness of text structure by labeling and giving

examples of story structure concepts" (Garner & Bochna, 2004, p. 69). Pearson and

Duke (2002) agreed that explicit instruction of comprehension strategies improved

comprehension scores of primary students. They also contend

comprehension improves when teachers design and implement activities

that support the understanding of the texts that students will read in their

classes. Comprehension and decoding can exist side by side as

instructional goals and valued student outcomes in an exemplary and

comprehensive literacy program for primary grade children. (Pearson & Duke,

2002, p. 247)

Fuchs and Fuchs (2005) found that repeated reading was another strategy useful

in helping improve reading fluency and comprehension in first grade students. In

monitoring early reading development in first grade students, however, a study by Fuchs,

Fuchs, and Compton (2004) found that word identification fluency was more predictive

Page 73: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

61

of early reading development than nonsense word fluency. "Because predictive validity

with respect to end-of-year text-reading fluency and comprehension is stronger for word

identification fluency than for nonsense word fluency, word identification fluency

provides a stronger basis for formulating screening decisions in October of first grade"

(Fuchs, Fuchs, & Compton, 2004, p. 19).

The aforementioned research indicated that early intervention is critical. By the

time students are in first grade, explicit instruction needs to occur in order to teach

fluency and comprehension strategies. Letter-sound fluency in first grade students has

been found to be a significant predictor of first grade reading fluency. Repeated reading

is a useful strategy in improving fluency and comprehension in first grade students.

Conclusion

Chapter Two reviewed the literature in terms of historical background of reading

instruction, theoretical framework of comprehension and fluency, historical background

of comprehension, reading policies, current research developments in reading

comprehension and fluency, the intersection of fluency and comprehension, as well as the

research on first grade students. The research on first grade students indicates that early

intervention is critical, which is the foundation of this study. Chapter Three will describe

the methodology of the study, including the research design, the participants, the

instrumentation, the data collection procedures, the data analysis procedures and the

limitations of the study.

Page 74: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Methodology. Chapter Three

Introduction

The purpose of the study was to understand the relationship of reading fluency

and reading comprehension. Chapter Three will describe the methodology of the study,

including the research design, the participants, the instrumentation, the data collection

procedures, the data analysis procedures and the limitations of the study.

Research Design

Given that the purpose of the study was to understand the relationship between

the reading fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students, the research

design for this study was a multivariate correlational research design. Gall, Gall & Borg

(2003) stated "correlational research designs are used for two major purposes: (1) to

explore causal relationships between variables and (2) to predict scores on one variable

from research participants' scores on other variables" (Gall, et al., 2003, p. 325). This

research design was chosen because it will serve the second of these purposes and will

explore the predictive relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension.

The researcher also used gender, ethnicity, educational status, economic status, at-risk,

migrant, and limited English proficient (LEP) as controlling variables in relationship to

reading comprehension.

Participants

The participants for this study were drawn from the population of first grade

students in a rural school district in the southwestern United States that services a total of

approximately 3500 students. Table 2 indicates the breakdown of the student population.

The total first grade population in this rural district was 262 students. Of that population,

Page 75: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

63

fifty-three percent were male and forty-seven percent were female. In terms of ethnicity,

fifteen percent were African-American, fifty-four percent were Hispanic, and thirty-one

percent were white, non-Hispanic. Eighty-five percent of this population were in regular

education classes, with ten percent in special education and five percent of these first

graders identified as gifted and talented. Forty-eight percent of the population were at-

risk. Furthermore, sixty-two percent of the population were on free lunch and ten percent

of the population was on reduced lunch. This qualified the school as a Title I school.

Instrumentation

The instrument used to measure both reading fluency and reading comprehension

was the Texas Primary Reading Inventory (TPRI). According to the TPRI Teacher's

Guide 2006-2008, the TPRI is an "early reading instrument designed to identify the

reading and comprehension development of students in kindergarten and grades 1-2" (p.

1). The first grade TPRI measured the following reading concepts: phonemic awareness,

graphophonemic knowledge, reading accuracy, reading fluency, reading comprehension,

and reading vocabulary. For the purposes of this study, the researcher examined reading

accuracy, reading fluency, and reading comprehension. Copies of the protocol sheets for

the individual student records and the class summary sheets are available Appendix A

and B, respectively. The TPRI consisted of eleven tasks which the student must

perform. The first nine tasks were: blending word parts; blending phonemes;

Page 76: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

64

Table 2

First Grade Student Population 2006-2007

Gender Number Percent

Male

Female

140

122

53

47

Ethnicity Number Percent

African American

Hispanic

White

40

142

80

15

54

31

Educational Status Number Percent

Special Education

Gifted and Talented

Regular Education

At-Risk

Limited English Proficient

27

13

222

125

35

10 Migrant

10

5

85

48

13

4

Economically Disadvantaged Number Percent

None

Free Lunch

Reduced Lunch

74

163

25

28

62

10

Page 77: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

65

deleting initial sounds; deleting final sounds; and graphophonemic knowledge of initial

consonants, final consonants, middle vowels, initial blends and final blends. The fluency

and comprehension section of the TPRI began with Task 10 wherein the student was

presented with a word list to determine appropriate story placement. In Task 11, the

student read the story. During this time, the teacher took a running record indicating the

errors the student made. The teacher then determined the accuracy level - either

independent, instructional or frustration level, depending on the number of errors the

student makes. The teacher also recorded the time, factored in the errors, and recorded

the words correct per minute. This yielded a fluency score. The teacher then assessed

the child's comprehension with eight questions - three explicit questions, three implicit

questions, and two vocabulary questions. The three explicit questions were factual

questions based upon the story. The three implicit questions could be answered through

inferencing. The vocabulary questions required the student to use context clues to

determine the meaning of a given vocabulary word. After the student answered these

eight questions, the teacher indicated the number of questions the student answered

correctly. This yielded a comprehension score.

The TPRI was validated in 1998. In a validation study by Foorman, Fletcher,

Francis, Carlson, Chen, Mouzaki, et al., (1998),

the first TPRI reading comprehension story correlated moderately to highly

with all of the validation measures, including the reading comprehension

measures, with the exception of the serial naming tasks. ... The second

TPRI comprehension story did not correlate as highly with the validation

Page 78: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

66

measures as the first TPRI comprehension story. While the correlations

were not as high, they were significant, particularly with other reading

comprehension measures. The pattern of correlations for the second TPRI

comprehension story were similar to the pattern of correlations between the

first comprehension story and the validation tasks. This similar pattern of

correlations provides some evidence of construct validity for the two

comprehension stories. (Foorman, et al., 1998, p. 79)

Reliability, however, was not determined in the 1998 study. Rather,

recommendations were made for revisions in the TPRI. The most significant change

recommended for the comprehension section of the test, was for the TPRI to begin using

less familiar stories. For example, the 1998 version of the TPRI used a story from Danny

and the Dinosaur. The authors of the validity study pointed out "comprehension

passages that were less familiar to students demonstrated the strongest reliability and

validity data" (Foorman, et al., 1998, p. 87). Thus, changes were made in the TPRI and

another test of reliability was conducted the following year in 1999. In this reliability

study, Cronbach's alpha was computed with the alpha coefficient ranges from 0 to 1.0

reported as the index of internal consistency. The researchers examined each of the

different tasks in kindergarten, first and second grade. These tasks included phonemic

awareness, graphophonemic knowledge, and reading comprehension tasks. Upon

examining the first grade comprehension portion of the reliability study, alpha ranged

from 0.42 to 0.69. The researchers conducting the reliability study established ranges to

determine the practical significance of the reliability coefficients. These ranges were as

Page 79: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

67

follows: "Poor (0-.39), Adequate (.40 - .59), Good (.60-.79) and Excellent (.80-1.0)

(Center for Academic and Reading Skills and Texas Institute for Measurement,

Evaluation, and Statistics, 1999, p. 5). According to the researchers, the reliability

coefficients for the reading comprehension portion of the first grade test were in the

Adequate and Good ranges. The rest of the test in 1999 was found to be reliable as well.

Thus by 1999, the TPRI was determined to be both reliable and valid.

Data Collection Procedures

In order to collect the data the ensuing procedures were followed. First, human

subjects approval was obtained from a rural school district in the southwestern United

States. Upon agreement from the school district, the researcher then sought human

subjects approval from the University of Houston. Once human subjects approval was

obtained from both entities, the researcher will obtain a cover letter indicating approval

for the study from the rural school district. The researcher then contacted the language

arts supervisor for the school district to obtain the copies of the TPRI data teachers turned

in as archival data for the 2006-2007 school year.

Data Analysis Procedures

All data was analyzed utilizing the software, Statistical Package for the Social

Sciences (SPSS). The analytic approach that was utilized in this study was regression

analysis as the researcher was trying to predict the outcome of given variables. Both the

predictor variable, reading fluency, and the outcome variable, reading comprehension,

were continuous variables. Gender, ethnicity, educational status, economic status, at-risk,

migrant, and LEP are all control variables. These variables are described in further

Page 80: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Table 3

Variables

Name of the Variable

Gender

DOB

Description of Variable

A participant's gender

A participant's age in

months

Numerical Value

1 = Male

2 = Female

78-104

Ethnicity A participant's ethnicity 1 = African American

2 = Hispanic

3 = White, non Hispanic

1 = Special Education

2 = Gifted and Talented

3 = Regular Education

Economic Status A participant's economic 1 = Not economically identified

status 2 = Free Lunch

3 = Reduced Lunch

At-Risk

Migrant

A participant's at-risk

status

A participant's migrant

status

0=No

l=Yes

0 = No

1 =Yes

LEP A participant's Limited 0 = No

English Proficiency status 1 = Yes

Educational Status A participant's

educational status

Page 81: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

69

Table 3 (continued)

Variables

Accuracy Based on percentage of 1 = Listening

words read correctly 2 = Instructional (90 - 94%)

3 = Independent (95 - 100%)

Reading Fluency Number of words read 0-200

correct per minute

Reading Comprehension Number of questions 0-8

answered correctly,

maximum of 8 correct

detail in Table 3. The Accuracy Variable is coded as Listening, Instructional or

Independent. Those students in the Independent category read with 95% or better

accuracy. Students in the Instructional category read with 90-94% accuracy and students

in the Listening category reached frustration level (89% or less) in terms of accuracy and

the test administrator dropped down a level and read a story orally to the students to test

for listening comprehension.

Field (2005) described regression analysis as being able to fit "a predictive model

to our data and use that model to predict values of the dependent variable

(DV) from one or more independent variables (IVs). Simple regression seeks to predict

an outcome variable from a single predictor variable whereas multiple regression seeks to

predict an outcome from several predictors" (Field, 2005, p. 144). The hypotheses being

tested is listed below. Y; is the outcome variable, reading comprehension and Xj is the /th

Page 82: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

participant's score on the predictor variable, reading fluency, bi is the gradient of the

straight line fitted to the data and bo is the intercept of the line; these are the regression

coeffecients. s t is a residual term representing the difference between the score

predicted by the line for participant i and the score that participant / actually obtained

(Field, 2005).

Y\ = (bo + biXi) + s,

To determine the line of best of fit, the method of least squares was used. The

residuals indicated the points that lie above and below the line. Two goodness of fit

indices were used to assess how well this line fits the actual data. The first was to

calculate the value of R , known as the coefficient of determination, which represents

"the amount of variance in the outcome explained by the model (SSM) relative to how

much variation there was to explain in the first place (SSj)" (Field, 2005, p. 148). SSM

the model sum of squares using the differences between the mean value of Y and the

regression line. SSj is the total sum of squares using the differences between the

observed data and the mean value of Y. The model is as follows:

SSr

A second use of the sums of squares was through the use of the F-test. According to

Field (2005),

This test is based upon the ratio of the improvement due to the model

(SSM) and the difference between the model and the observed data (SSR).

In fact, rather than using the sums of squares themselves, we take the mean

sums of squares (referred to as the mean squares or MS). To work out the

mean sums of squares it is necessary to divide by the degrees of freedom....

Page 83: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

71

For SSM the degrees of freedom are simply the number of variables in

the model, and for SSR they are the number of observations minus the

number of parameters being estimated . . . . The result is the mean squares

for the model (MSM) and the residual mean squares (MSR). (p. 150)

The model is as follows:

If the model is good, the F-ratio will be larger than 1 as the top half of the above equation

should be larger than the bottom half. F was also tested for statistical significance.

In utilizing regression analysis, Osbourne and Waters (2002) pointed out that four

assumptions are made that researchers must test. First, regression analysis assumes that

variables are normally distributed. "Outliers can be identified either through visual

inspection of histograms or frequency distributions, or by converting data to z-scores"

(Osbourne & Waters, 2002, p. 2). Second, regression analysis assumes a linear

relationship between the independent and dependent variable. This was tested through

the use of scatterplots of residuals to examine linear and curvilinear relationships. A

third assumption is that the variables are measured without error and thus are reliable.

This was tested through the calculation of Cronbach's alpha. The fourth and final

assumption is the assumption of homoscedasticity. "Homoscedasticity means that the

variance of errors is the same across all levels of the IV (independent variable). When

the variance of errors differs at different values of the IV, heteroscedasticity is indicated"

(Osbourne & Waters, 2002, p. 6). In order to check this assumption, the researcher made

a visual examination of "the standardized residuals (the errors) by the regression

standardized predicted value" (Osbourne and Waters, 20020, p. 6).

Page 84: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

72

Limitations of the Study

A first limitation is the study was limited to first grade students. Future studies

can focus on other grade levels.

As with all correlation research, a second limitation of this study is that the

correlations that were obtained in this study through multiple regression do not establish

a definitive cause-and-effect relationship between reading fluency and reading

comprehension. The correlations however, described the strength and direction of the

relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension.

Thirdly, Gall, Gall and Borg (2003) also pointed out that "another problem with

using correlational statistics to identify variables that may be causally related to complex

behavior patterns or abilities is that success in many of the complex activities that interest

us probably can be achieved in different ways" (p. 329). For example, Student A may

have difficulty reading because the males in his family do not place a high importance on

reading. Student B, on the other hand, also male, may excel in reading because males in

his family are expected to be successful.

A fourth limitation of the study is the complex nature of reading itself. For

example, it is possible for a student to decode effortlessly yet be unable to comprehend.

Even though one has mastered one of the component parts of reading, does not indicate

mastery of reading itself. Causal relationship studies break "down complex abilities and

behavior patterns into simpler components" (Gall, Gall, & Borg, 2003, p. 329).

A fifth limitation is related to the comprehension measure itself, specifically prior

knowledge.

Page 85: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

73

. . . each student's prior knowledge is likely to influence his or her performance

on comprehension questions. Because there is no way to measure what portion of

students' success in answering comprehension questions is based on their prior

knowledge, it is impossible to measure the amount of comprehension that was

caused by their reading ability alone (Daly, Chafouleas, & Skinner, 2005, p. 137).

Summary

The study described the strength and nature of the relationship between gender,

ethnicity, educational status, reading fluency and reading comprehension. From this,

educators will be able to determine whether or not reading fluency can predict reading

comprehension. Furthermore, as Shanahan (2002) pointed out, the value in the research

is also the ensuing discussion. This study provides more empirical data for first grade

teachers to formulate their thoughts and language arts curriculum decisions. If the

empirical evidence holds, and there is indeed a significant relationship between reading

fluency and reading comprehension the implications for first grade language arts

curriculum are exciting. With proper alterations in curriculum and appropriate focus on

increasing reading fluency, reading comprehension will increase as well. First grade

students will be on their way to reading on grade level and being successful life-long

learners.

Chapter One of the research proposal introduced the research question and

directional research hypotheses, as well as outlined the need for the study. Chapter Two

reviewed the literature concerning reading fluency and reading comprehension in terms

of historical background, theoretical framework and current research developments.

Chapter Three described the methodology to be used for the study. In Chapter Three, the

Page 86: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

74

research design, the participants, the instrumentation, the data collection procedures, the

data analysis procedures and the limitations of the study are described in detail. Protocol

sheets of the TPRI for the individual students and class summary sheets can be found in

Appendix A and B respectively. In Chapter Four, the results of the study will be

presented.

Page 87: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Results. Chapter Four

Introduction

The purpose of the study was to understand the relationship of reading fluency

and reading comprehension. Chapter Four will explain the results of the study, beginning

with descriptive statistics about the sample. Then the multiple regression analysis results

will be reported in regards to the relationship between fluency and reading

comprehension as well as the controlling variables.

Descriptive Statistics

The sample being studied consisted of 234 students from a southern rural school

district in the southwestern United States. Looking at Table 4, one can see that the males

comprised 51.7% of the total sample with females making up the other 48.3%. There

was a large Hispanic representation (54.3%) with a smaller White, non-Hispanic sample

(30.8%) and even smaller African-American proportions of the sample (15.0%). Of

significance was that 46.6% of the sample was at-risk and 38.9% were on free / reduced

lunch. The campus was a Title I campus. Table 5 indicates that the mean age at the time

of end of year testing was 88.37 months (7.4 years). The standard deviations for the

demographic data were low, indicating that the data were clustered closely around the

mean.

Page 88: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

76

Table 4

First Grade Student Sample 2006-2007

Gender Number Percent

Male

Female

121

113

51.7

48.3

Ethnicity Number Percent

African American

Hispanic

White

35

127

72

15.0

54.3

30.8

Educational Status Number Percent

Special Education

Gifted and Talented

Regular Education

At-Risk

Limited English Proficient

Migrant

17

13

204

109

30

8

7.3

5.6

87.2

46.6

12.8

3.4

Economic Status Number Percent

Not economically identified 143

Free Lunch 24

Reduced Lunch 67

61.1

10.3

28.6

n = 234

Page 89: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

77

Looking at the Beginning of Year (BOY) results in Table 6, the mean fluency rate

was 37.90 words correct per minute with a standard deviation of 16.38. Also of

significance was the Total Reading Comprehension mean of 4.66 (with a possible high of

8.00). The BOY Accuracy results reveal a mean of 2.03 indicating that the average

student was Instructional in terms of accuracy at the beginning of the year. The

frequencies in Table 6, however, show 32.9% of the student sample in the Listening

category, 31.2% of the sample in the Instructional category, and 35.9% in the

Independent category. Those students in the Independent category read with 95% or

better accuracy. Students in the Instructional category read with 90-94% accuracy and

students in the Listening category reached frustration level (89% or less) in terms of

accuracy and the test administrator dropped down a level and read a story orally to the

students to test for listening comprehension.

Looking at the End of Year (EOY) results in Table 7, the mean fluency rate was

61.32 words correct per minute with a standard deviation of 23.64. Also of significance

was the Total Reading Comprehension mean of 6.68 (with a possible high of 8.00). The

EOY Accuracy results revealed a mean of 2.76 indicating that the average student was

Instructional - Independent in terms of accuracy at the end of the year. The frequencies

in Table 7, however, show a more complete picture with 0.9% of the student population

in the Listening category, 21.8% of the population in the Instructional category, and

77.4% in the Independent category.

Page 90: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Table 5

Descriptive Statistics

Variable

Gender

Ethnicity

DOB

Educational Status

Migrant

At-Risk

LEP

Economic Status

BOY Accuracy

BOY Fluency

BOY Total Rdg Comp

EOY Accuracy

EOY Fluency

EOY Total Rdg Comp

DiffAcc

Diff Fluency

Diff Total Rdg Comp

Mean

1.48

2.16

88.37

2.80

.03

.47

.13

1.68

2.03

37.90

4.66

2.76

61.32

6.68

.74

35.11

2.02

Standard Deviation

.50

.66

5.17

.55

.18

.50

.34

.89

.83

16.14

2.04

.45

23.64

1.27

.80

20.81

1.93

n = 234

Page 91: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Table 6

Frequencies for Beginning of

Year

BOY Accuracy Number Percent

Listening

Instructional

Independent

77

73

84

32.9

31.2

35.9

BOY Total Reading Comp

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

Number

~9

11

14

28

44

39

42

31

16

Percent

Ts

4.7

6.0

12.0

18.8

16.7

17.9

13.2

6.8

n = 234

Page 92: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Table 7

Frequencies for End of Year

EOY Accuracy Number Percent

Listening

Instructional

Independent

2

51

181

.9

21.8

77.4

EOY Total Reading Comp Number Percent

3

4

5

6

7

8

1

5

11

20

46

81

70

.4

2.1

4.7

8.5

19.7

34.6

29.9

n = 234

The mean difference in accuracy was .74 with a standard deviation of .81. The

mean difference in fluency was 23.42 with a standard deviation of 7.51. The mean

difference in total reading comprehension was 2.02 with a standard deviation of 1.93.

Students, on average answered two more questions correctly on the comprehension

portion at the end of the year than they did at the beginning of the year.

Page 93: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

81

The Relationship Between Fluency and Comprehension

Table 8 presents the correlation matrix for difference in total Reading

Comprehension, age, educational status, economic status, and difference in fluency. The

data in Table 8 reflects the differences between the end of year fluency and reading

comprehension scores and the beginning of year fluency and reading comprehension

scores. The correlations indicated that there is a positive relationship between fluency

and comprehension (R = .41). The results of the Pearson's chi-square test indicated that

there was a significant association between at-risk and ethnicity x (2) = 19.53, p<.001.

The crosstabulation table can be seen in Table 9.

Table 8. Correlation Matrix for Difference in Total Reading Comprehension, Age,

Educational Status, Economic Status, and Difference in Fluency

(!) (2) (3) (4) (5)

Difference in Reading 1.00 -.17** -.12** -.14** 4 1 *

Comprehension

Age -.17** 1.00 -.12** -.09** -.14

Educational Status -.12** -.12** 1.00 .05 .01

Economic Status -.14** -.09 .05 1.00 -.01

Difference in Fluency .41* -.14** .01 -.01 1.00

Note. N = 234. Difference in Total Reading Comprehension is the dependent variable.

Correlations marked with a * are significant at p<.001 and correlations marked with a **

are significant at p<.05.

Page 94: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

82

Table 9. Ethnicity by At-Risk Cross-Tabulation

Ethnicity

Total

African-American

Hispanic

White, Non-Hispanic

No

16

55

54

125

At-Risk

Yes

19

72

18

109

Total

35

127

72

234

The Model Summary from SPSS provided the R, R2, adjusted R2, and standard

error for the overall regression model. The R2 indicated that 17.1% of the variance in

reading comprehension can be explained fluency. When looking at the controlling

variables, R indicated that 35.6% of the variance in reading comprehension can be

explained by a combination of fluency, age, gender, ethnicity, educational status, migrant

at-risk, LEP, and economic status. By calculating the difference in the Adjusted R2 and

R , the researcher determined the variance if the study were generalized from the sample

to the population as a whole. The difference in Adjusted R2 and R2 was .004 when

examining the dependent and independent variable. The difference was .026 when

adding the controlling variables. Thus, if this study were generalized to the population as

a whole, one could expect a .4% variance when looking at the dependent and independent

variable and 2.6% variance when adding the controlling variables.

Table 10 shows the multiple regression analysis for the relationship between the

difference in total reading comprehension, gender, ethnicity, age, educational status,

migrant, at-risk, LEP, economic status, and difference in fluency. The amount of

Page 95: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

83

variance explained by the regression equation is statistically significant, as shown by F =

13.77, (p<.05).

Table 10 presents the standardized and unstandardized regression coeffecients and

their accompanying t values and levels of significance. The t values that are statistically

significant at p<.05 are ethnicity, age, migrant, at-risk, LEP, and difference in fluency.

All other values are p>.05. The beta weights provided an indication of the relative

contribution of the variable reading comprehension when the other variables are

controlled. The greatest influence was at-risk, followed by difference in fluency. The

dependent variable, difference in reading comprehension, was negatively impacted by

ethnicity, and age. The comparison group, coded 0, for migrant is a person who is not a

migrant. The comparison group, coded 0, for at-risk is a person who is not at-risk. The

regression coefficients for both migrant and at-risk are positive indicating that the

dependent variable, difference in reading comprehension, is positively impacted by being

either a migrant or being at-risk. The comparison group, coded 0, for LEP is a person

who is not LEP. The regression coefficient for LEP is negative indicating that the

dependent variable, difference in reading comprehension, is negatively impacted by being

LEP.

Page 96: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

84

Table 10. Multiple Regression Analysis of the Relationship Between Reading

Comprehension, Gender, Ethnicity, Age, Educational Status, Migrant, At-Risk, LEP,

Economic Status, and Fluency

B Beta t p

(Constant)

Gender

Ethnicity

Age

Ed. Status

Migrant

At-Risk

LEP

Eco. Status

Difference in Fluency

Note. R2 = .36, F (9,224) = 13.77, p. = .001

Dependent Variable: Difference in Reading Comprehension

8.43

-.09

-.50

-.07

-.27

1.20

1.34

-.84

.02

.03

-.02

-.17

-.18

-.08

.11

.35

-.15

.01

.28

4.08

-.41

-2.86

-3.20

-1.38

2.05

5.19

-2.39

.16

4.76

.00

.69

.01

.01

.17

.05

.00

.02

.87

.00

Summary

Chapter One of the research proposal introduced the research question and

directional research hypotheses, as well as outlined the need for the study. Chapter Two

reviewed the literature concerning reading fluency and reading comprehension in terms

of historical background, theoretical framework and current research developments.

Page 97: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

85

Chapter Three described the methodology to be used for the study including the research

design, the participants, the instrumentation, the data collection procedures, the data

analysis procedures and the limitations of the study. In Chapter Four, the results of the

study, beginning with descriptive statistics about the sample were presented. Then the

multiple regression analysis results were reported in regards to the relationship between

fluency and reading comprehension as well as the controlling variables. Chapter Five

will discuss these results in further detail.

Page 98: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Discussion. Chapter Five

Introduction

The purpose of the study was to describe the relationship of reading fluency and

reading comprehension. Chapter Five will explain the significant findings of the study,

implications of the study for current research, implications for future research, and

implications for current practices.

Significant Findings of the Study

The purpose of the study was to describe the relationship between reading fluency

and reading comprehension of first grade students. Given this purpose, the study

addressed the following research question: What is the relationship between reading

fluency and reading comprehension of first grade students? At R = .41, there was a

strong positive relationship between reading fluency and reading comprehension. Those

students who have higher reading fluency scores will have higher reading

comprehension. Conversely, those students with low reading fluency scores will have

low reading comprehension. Fluency and comprehension in first grade students are

related.

Furthermore, as indicated by R , 17% of the variance in reading comprehension

can be explained by reading fluency. Reading is a complex activity and, as such, is

impacted by many things. Based on the R of the controlling variables, 35.6% of the

variance in reading comprehension is explained by other factors, such as age, gender,

ethnicity, educational status, migrant, at-risk, LEP, and economic status. The regression

coefficients indicated that at-risk and fluency are the greatest contributing factors. While

Page 99: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

87

the at-risk findings were not consistent with current literature, it is possible that the

school was conducting interventions with the at-risk population, thus raising their fluency

scores. This is worth further investigation.

Implications for Current Research

The results of this study are consistent with the findings of Berninger, et al.

(2006), Stage and Jacobsen (2001), Vaughn, et al. (2000), Rasinski (1990) and Stahl and

Heubach (2005) who all found a relationship between fluency and comprehension.

Berninger, et al. (2006) found a relationship between fluency and comprehension in

second grade students as did Stahl and Heubach (2005). Stage and Jacobsen (2001)

looked at fourth graders. Vaughn, et al. (2000) examined third grade students while

Rasinski (1990) studied third and fifth grade students. All of these researchers were able

to establish a positive relationship between fluency and comprehension at their respective

grade levels. The current study established this relationship in first grade students.

Implications for Future Research

This research study, like many of the ones that have gone before it, has achieved

its purpose and answered its research question. However, in answering the research

question, more questions then come into focus and there exist implications for future

research.

The relationship between fluency and comprehension has been established in first

grade students. Based on the concept that the purpose of reading is comprehension

(Bender & Larkin, 2003), this study has determined that comprehension can be improved

by an increase in fluency. However, this is one study with a sample from one rural

Page 100: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

88

community in the southwestern United States. Other similar studies should be done with

larger samples in other parts of the country to create more generalizability in the study.

In addition to replicating the current study on a larger scale, the next step in the

research is to determine the best ways to improve fluency in first grade students.

Suggestions include choral reading, repeated readings, poetry, and reader's theater.

Studies have been done that examine the benefits of such repeated reading activities,

however, they have not been done with first graders. For example, Martinez, Roser and

Strecker (1999) examined the effect of reader's theater in second grade students.

Dowhower (1987) described the effects of repeated reading on second-grade readers'

fluency and comprehension. O'Connor, White and Swanson (2007) looked at repeated

and continuous reading with second and fourth grade students. Repeated reading has been

used successfully in studies from second (Dowhower, 1987) through eighth grade

(Mercer, Campbell, Miller, Mercer & Lane, 2000). Furthermore, repeated reading has

been studied on students who read between first (Weinstein & Cooke, 1992) and fifth

grade levels (Homan, Klesius, & Hite, 1993). It is now time for studies to be conducted

on actual first graders.

Implications for Practice

According to the TPRI manual, "once students begin to decode individual words

automatically and rapidly, they are on their way to becoming fluent readers" (TPRI,

2006, p. 42). To be considered a fluent reader in first grade, a student should be reading

approximately 60 words per minute by year's end. Students reading less than 40 words

per minute will need help to attain automaticity through regular practice opportunities

(Good, Simmons, Kame'enui, Kaminski, & Wallin, 2002). The most recent national oral

Page 101: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

89

reading fluency norms were published by Hasbrouck and Tindal (2006). Table 11

indicates the norms that were developed for first grade based on data from schools in 23

states.

Table 11

First Grade Oral Reading Fluency Norms

Percentile Winter Spring

Words Correct Per Minute Words Correct Per Minute

"90 81 HI

75 47 82

50 23 53

25 12 28

10 6 15

Standard Deviation 32 39

Number 16,950 19,434

Note. Adapted from Hasbrouck & Tindal (2006).

In order to become a fluent reader, and consequently comprehend well, a student

needs multiple practice opportunities throughout the day. This can be achieved through a

variety of ways. Those following a balanced literacy approach to teaching reading may

find the following suggestions useful. The first fifteen minutes of the reading block can

be spent on whole group activities, such as the morning message or some other form of

interactive writing. During this fifteen minutes, the teacher can also address new words

for the word wall, as well as review word wall words, and high frequency words. The

Page 102: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

90

next fifteen minutes of the reading block can be spent on a mini-lesson taught with either

a read-aloud or shared reading. Using the read-aloud or shared reading, the teacher

accomplishes several things. First and foremost, the teacher is able to model fluent

reading for the students. Second, the teacher is able to use the Think Aloud strategy to

model self-questioning and show students his or her thinking processes while reading,

thus improving comprehension (Davey, 1983). Third, the teacher can teach the strategy

lesson of the day using authentic literature. Fourth, during shared reading, on days when

the big book or poem is being repeated, the students can read chorally / antiphonally to

work on prosody (expression).

The next phase of the reading block should be spent in guided reading and

stations. Guided reading group time should include: a running record on one student, a

fluency rate assessment on one student (to assess fluency growth), familiar reading (to

increase fluency), traditional word work, and work with a new leveled text. It is during

the work with the new book that comprehension strategies should be taught. In order to

assess fluency, teachers can use commercial fluency assessments or teachers can also

listen to children read any passage and rate their reading performance using a rubric. The

rubric in Table 12 is one such rubric and was adapted by Rasinski and Padak (2008) from

a federal research project that was funded to determine the fluency of U.S. fourth graders.

While the teacher is with the guided reading group, the rest of the class can be at

literacy work stations. Stations should include a computer station, a listening station, a

library station, a word work station, a poetry station, and a "fluency" station. The

computer station can feature fluency software which is designed to increase fluency while

Page 103: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Table 12

Oral Reading Fluency Scale

5

Outstanding

4

Satisfactory

3

Unsatisfactory

2

Unsatisfactory

1

Unsatisfactory

Note. From Rasinski & Padak (2008), p. 35.

Appropriate phrasing. Regressions or

repetitions, if any, do not detract from

presentation. Expressive. Appropriate

rate. Few hesitations or stops.

Mostly appropriate phrasing. Expressive

interpretation inconsistent. Rate generally

appropriate. Occasional hesitations or

stops.

Reads in short inappropriate phrases. Little

expressive interpretation. Rate

inappropriately slow. Extended hesitations

and stops.

Word-by-word reading. Very little or no

expression or interpretation. Excessively

slow (or fast) rate.

Excessive word recognition errors

significantly disrupt fluency and meaning

Page 104: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

92

maintaining comprehension. Also useful are the electronic book programs. In a study by

Grimshaw, Dungworth, McKnight and Morris (2007), researchers found that the

provision of narration in the electronic books led to "significantly higher comprehension

scores than when narration was absent" (p. 583). After following along with the

electronic book, students can then create electronic story maps and other organizers to

demonstrate their comprehension of the book.

The listening station is where students can follow along with books on tape or

CD. According to Zutell and Rasinski (1991), "all readers need models of fluent reading

in their literacy experiences" (p. 216). The listening station is one place where readers

can get that modeling while doing independent practice (Rasinski, 2008).

At the library station, students can engage in paired reading, also known as buddy

reading. Here students read familiar books with the support of a buddy who is more

fluent in order to increase the individual's fluency (Diller, 2007). This works by having

the more fluent reader follow along silently when the student is ready to read alone. He

doesn't chime in again until the other reader makes a mistake (Rasinski, 2008). Students

can also read books independently and write responses to books, again demonstrating

their comprehension of the books read.

At the word work station, students can do a variety of activities. Students

struggling with decoding in the early part of first grade can continue with letter sorts and

blending sounds to make words. Students who are ready for more advanced word work

can do activities such as sorting words, making words and illustrating words (Diller,

2003). They can also "play games with high-frequency words that encourage automatic

recognitions, such as Concentration, Memory, or Hangman ... [to] help students read

Page 105: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

93

more automatically as they quickly recognize more words embedded in text" (Diller,

2007, p. 84).

In order to work on prosody (expression), students can participate at the poetry

station. At the poetry station, students will read poetry, build poems, change poems,

listen to poems, tape record poems to take home, write poems, and find special words or

kinds of words in poems (Diller, 2003). When reading the poems and taping the poems,

students will work on reading in phrases expressively as well as developing rhythm and

rhyme. This will help their phrasing and intonation to improve (prosody) thereby

improving their fluency.

At the "fluency" station, the teacher has several options which can be changed

throughout the year. During the first part of the first grade, this can be a Big Book

Station wherein the students engage in repeated reading of familiar text. According to

Blum and Koskinen (1991),

repeated reading offers considerable benefits as a strategy for enhancing fluency

and comprehension while fostering expertise. This approach seems to contribute

to an increase in content and strategy knowledge as well as motivation. In

addition, repeated reading procedures allow students to work at a level of

difficulty where they can be successful" (p. 199).

This "fluency" station can also become a Reader's Theater station. In reader's theater,

students read from scripts they are holding. "Without movement, costumes, props or

scenery, the performers have only one attribute to make their performance meaningful

and satisfying: their voices. And in order to use their voices well, performers must

practice the text beforehand" (Rasinski, 2003, p. 105). It is this repeated practice (also

Page 106: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

94

known as repeated reading) that builds fluency. The performance of the Reader's Theater

scripts also lends an authentic purpose for the reading. Researchers have found that

students can gain an average of 1.1 years growth in reading during a ten-week period

when reader's theater is implemented in the classroom (Martinez, Roser, & Strecker,

1999). To keep this station fresh throughout the year, it can also become a recording

station where students record their reading, listen to their reading, and self-evaluate using

a fluency rubric.

After guided reading and literacy work stations, teachers should spend five to ten

minutes in share time, where students share what they worked on and learned in work

stations. Teachers should also include 20 minutes of independent reading at an

appropriate point in their day.

Summary

Chapter One of the research proposal introduced the research question and

directional research hypotheses, as well as outlined the need for the study. Chapter Two

reviewed the literature concerning reading fluency and reading comprehension in terms

of historical background, theoretical framework and current research developments.

Chapter Three described the methodology to be used for the study including the research

design, the participants, the instrumentation, the data collection procedures, the data

analysis procedures and the limitations of the study. In Chapter Four, the results of the

study, beginning with descriptive statistics about the sample were presented. Then the

multiple regression analysis results were reported in regards to the relationship between

fluency and reading comprehension as well as the controlling variables. Chapter Five

explained the significant findings of the study, implications of the study for current

Page 107: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

95

research, implications for future research, and implications for current practices. These

implications are especially exciting when one considers the impact practitioners can have

on their first graders if they introduce fluency instruction to complement their reading

instruction. Many of the fluency activities are simple and easy to assimilate into existing

curriculum. Furthermore, these activities will improve comprehension, thereby creating

better readers. Better readers means successful citizens (NEA, 2007). Furthermore, "The

more that you read, the more things you will know. The more that you learn, the more

places you'll go" (Seuss, 1978). As for the children, as Dr. Seuss (1990) would say, "and

will you succeed? Yes! You will, indeed! (98 and 3/4 percent guaranteed.) KID, YOU'LL

MOVE MOUNTAINS!"

Page 108: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

References

Allinder, R.M., Dunse, L., Brunken, CD., and Obermiller-Krolikowski, H.J. (2001).

Improving fluency in at-risk readers and students with learning disabilities.

Remedial and Special Education, 22(1), 48-54.

Allington, R.L. (1999). Fluency: A vital key to comprehension. Instructor, 113(5), 12-

13.

Allington, R.L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected reading goal. The Reading Teacher, 36,

556-561.

Allington, R.L. (2001). What really matters for struggling readers: Designing research-

based programs. New York: Addison-Wesley Educational Publishers, Inc.

Allington, R.L., and McGill-Franzen, A. (2000). Looking back, looking forward: A

conversation about teaching reading in the 21st century. Reading Research

Quarterly, 35(1), 136-153.

Almasi, J.F. (2003). Teaching strategic processes in reading. New York: The Guilford

Press.

Anderson, R.C., Wilson, P.T. & Fielding, L.G. (1988). Growth in reading and how

children spend their time outside of school. Reading Research Quarterly, 23(3),

285-303.

Applegate, M.D., Quinn, K.B. & Applegate, A.J. (2006). Profiles in comprehension.

The Reading Teacher, 60(1), 48-57.

Archer, A.L., Gleason, M.M. & Vachon, V.L. (2003). Decoding and fluency:

Foundation for struggling older readers. Learning Disabilities Quarterly,

26(2), 89-101.

Page 109: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

97

Barton, J. & Sawyer, D.M. (2003). Our students are ready for this: Comprehension

instruction in the elementary school. The Reading Teacher, 57(4), 334-347.

Begeny, J.C. (2006). Assisting low-performing readers with a group-based reading

fluency intervention. School Psychology Review, 35(1), 91-107.

Bender, W.N. & Larkin, M.J. (2003). Reading strategies for elementary students with

learning disabilities. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press, Inc.

Berninger, V.W., Abbott, R.D., Vermeulen, K., & Fulton, CM. (2006). Paths to reading

comprehension in at-risk second-grade readers. Journal of Learning Disabilities,

39(4), 334-351.

Biancarosa, G. (2005). After third grade. Educational Leadership, 63(2), 16-22.

Bishop, P.A., Reyes, C. & Pflaum, S.W. (2006). Read smarter, not harder: Global

reading comprehension strategies. The Reading Teacher, 60(1), 66-69.

Blachowicz, C.L.Z. & Fisher, P. (2004). Vocabulary lessons. Educational Leadership,

61(6), pp. 66-69.

Blau, L. (1999). 5 surefire strategies for developing reading fluency. Instructor, 110(7),

28-30.

Blaunstein, P. & Lyon, R. (2006). Why kids can't read: Challenging the status quo in

education. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield Education.

Blevins, W. Building fluency: Lessons and strategies for reading success. New York:

Scholastic.

Block, C.C. & Israel, S.E. (2005). Reading First and beyond: The complete guide for

teachers and literacy coaches. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press.

Block, C.C, Rodgers, L.L., & Johnson, R.B. (2004). Comprehension process

Page 110: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

98

instruction: Creating reading success in grades K-3. New York: The

Guilford Press.

Blum, I.H., & Koskinen, P.S. (1991). Repeated reading: A strategy for enhancing

fluency and fostering expertise. Theory Into Practice, 30(3), 195-200.

Bond, G.L. & Dykstra, R. (1967). The cooperative research program in first-grade

reading instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 2, 5-142.

Boulware-Gooden, R., Carreker, S., Thornhill, A., & Joshi, R.M. (2007). Instruction of

metacognitive strategies enhances reading comprehension and vocabulary

achievement of third-grade students. The Reading Teacher, 61(1), 70-77.

Brueckner, L.J. (1939). The changing elementary school. New York: Inor Publishing

Company.

Bryant, D.P., Vaughn, S., Linan-Thompson, S., Ugel, N., Hamff, Allison, & Hougen, M.

(2000). Reading outcomes for students with and without reading disabilities in

general education middle-school content area classes. Learning Disability

Quarterly, 23(4), 238+.

Buck, J., & Torgesen, J. (2002) The relationship between performance on a measure of

oral reading fluency and performance on the Florida Comprehensive Assessment

Test. Retrieved June 4, 2007, from

http://www.fcrr.org/TechnicalReports/TechnicalReportl.pdf

Bullion-Mears, A., McCauley, J.K., & McWhorter, J.Y. (2007). Erupting with great

force: Performing text to enhance reading comprehension. Science Scope, 31(1),

42-47.

Page 111: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Carnine, D.W., Silbert, J., Kame'nui, E.J., Tarver, S.G. & Jungjohann, K. (2006).

Teaching struggling and at-risk readers. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey:

Pearson Education, Inc.

Cassidy, J. & Wenrich, Judith K. (1997). What's hot, what's not for 1997. Reading

Today, 14(4), 34.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2000). What's hot, what's not for 2000. Reading Today,

17(3), 1,28.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2003). What's hot, what's not for 2003. Reading Today,

20(3), 1,18.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2004). What's hot, what's not for 2004. Reading Today,

21(5), 3-4.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2005). What's hot, what's not for 2005. Reading Today,

22(3), 1,8-9.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2006). What's hot, what's not for 2006. Reading Today,

23(3), 1,8-9.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2007). What's hot, what's not for 2007. Reading Today,

24(4), 1,10-11.

Cassidy, J. & Cassidy, D. (2008). What's hot for 2008. Reading Today, 25(4), 1,10-11.

Cates, G.L. & Rhymer, K.N. (2006). Effects of explicit timing on elementary students'

oral reading rates of word phrases. Reading Improvement, 43(3), 148-156.

Page 112: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

100

Center for Academic and Reading Skills & Texas Institute for Measurement, Evaluation,

and Statistics. (1998-1999). Technical Report: Texas Primary Reading

Inventory. Houston: University of Texas-Houston Health Science Center &

University of Houston.

Chall, J.S. (1967). Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.

Chall, J.S. (1978). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill Book

Company.

Chard, D.J., Pikulski, J J., & McDonagh, S.H. (2006). Fluency: The link between

decoding and comprehension for struggling readers. In Rasinski, T., Blachowicz,

C. & Lems, K. (2006). Fluency instruction: Research-based best practices (pp.

39-61). New York: The Guilford Press.

Clark, K.F. & Graves, M.F. (2004). Scaffolding students' comprehension of text. The

Reading Teacher, 58(6), 570-579.

Cole, A.D. (2004). When reading begins: The teacher's role in decoding,

comprehension, and fluency. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Coiro, J. (2003). Rethinking comprehension strategies to better prepare students for

critically evaluating content on the internet. The New England Reading

Association Journal, 39(2), pp. 29-34.

Coiro, J. & Dobler, E. (2007). Exploring the online reading comprehension strategies

used by sixth-grade skilled readers to search for and locate information on the

Internet. Reading Research Quarterly, 42(2), 214-257.

Conderman, G. & Strobel, D. (2006). Problem solving with guided repeated oral reading

instruction. Intervention in School and Clinic, 42(1), 34-39.

Page 113: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

101

Corcoran, C.A. & Davis, A.D. (2005). A study of the effects of readers' theater on

second and third grade special education students' fluency growth. Reading

Improvement, 42(2), 105-111.

Coyne, M.D., Kame'enui, E.J., Simmons, C. & Harn, B.A. (2004). Beginning reading

intervention as inoculation or insulin: First-grade reading performance of strong

responders to kindergarten intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37(2),

90-104.

Crowe, L.K. (2005). Comparison of two oral reading feedback strategies in improving

reading comprehension of school-age children with low reading ability. Remedial

and Special Education, 26(1), 32-42.

Daly III, E.J., Chafouleas, S. & Skinner, C.H. (2005). Interventions for reading

problems: Designing and evaluating effective strategies. New York: The

Guilford Press.

Davey, B. (1983). Think aloud: Modeling the cognitive processes of reading

comprehension. Journal of Reading, 27, 44-47.

Devault, R. & Joseph, L.M. (2004). Repeated readings combined with word boxes

phonics technique increases fluency levels of high school students with severe

reading delays. Preventing School Failure, 49(1), 22-27.

Diller, D. (2003). Literacy work stations: Making centers work. Portland, Maine:

Stenhouse Publishers.

Diller, D. (2007). Making the most of small groups: Differentiation for all. Markham,

Ontario: Pembroke Publishers Limited.

Page 114: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

102

Dole, J.A. (2000). Explicit and implicit instruction in comprehension. In Taylor, B.M.,

Graves, M.F., & Van Den Broek, P. (2000). Reading for meaning: Fostering

comprehension in the middle grades (pp. 52-69). New York: Teachers College

Press.

Dowhower, S.L. (1987). Effects of repeated reading on second-grade transitional

readers' fluency and comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 22(4), 389-

406.

Dowrick, P.W., Kim-Rupnow, W.S., & Power, T.J. (2006). Video feedforward for

reading. The Journal of Special Education, 39(4), 194-207.

Duffy, G. (ed). (2003). Improving comprehension: 10 research-based principles.

Washington, D.C.: National Education Association.

Duffy, G.G., Roehler, I.R. & Mason, J. (1984). Comprehension instruction:

Perspectives and suggestions. New York: Longman.

Duke, N.K. (2004). The case for informational text. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 40-

44.

Durkin, D. (1978-1979). What classroom observations reveal about reading

comprehension instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14(4), pp. 481-533.

Durkin, D. (1983). Teaching them to read. Boston: Allyn and Bacon, Inc.

Dymock, S. (2007). Comprehension strategy instruction: Teaching narrative text

structure awareness. The Reading Teacher, 61(2), 161-167.

Eilers, L.H. & Pinkley, C. (2006). Metacognitive strategies help students to comprehend

all text. Reading Improvement, 43(1), 13-29.

Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications.

Page 115: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

103

Flesch, R. (1955). Why Johnny can't read - and what you can do about it. New York:

Harper & Row.

Foorman, B.R., Fletcher, J.M., Francis, D.J., Carlson, CD., Chen, D. Mouziaki, A., et al.

(1998). Technical Report: Texas Primary Reading Inventory [Tech. Rep.].

Austin: Texas Education Agency.

Fountas, I.C. & Pinnell, G.S. (2006). Teaching for comprehending and fluency: Thinking,

talking, and writing about reading, K-8. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Fuchs, D. & Fuchs, L.S. (2005). Peer-assisted learning strategies: Promoting word

recognition, fluency, and reading comprehension in young children. The Journal

of Special Education, 39(1), 34-44.

Fuchs, L.S., Fuchs, D. & Compton, D.L. (2004). Monitoring early reading development

in first grade: Word identification fluency versus nonsense word fluency.

Exceptional Children, 71(1), 7-21.

Fuchs, L.S., Fuchs, D., Hosp, M.K. & Jenkins, J. (2001). Oral reading fluency as an

indicator of reading competence: A theoretical, empirical, and historical analysis.

Scientific Studies of Reading, 5(3), 239-256.

Gall, M.D., Gall, J.P. & Borg, W.R. (2003). Education research: An introduction (7th

ed). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Gambrell, L.B., Block, C.C., & Pressley, M. (2002). Improving comprehension

instruction: An urgent priority. In Block, C.C., Gambrell, L.B. & Pressley, M.

(eds.) (2002). Improving comprehension instruction: Rethinking research,

theory and classroom practice (pp. 3-16). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Page 116: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

104

Garan, E.M. (2002). Resisting reading mandates: How to triumph with the truth.

Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Garner, J.K., & Bochna, C.R. (2004). Transfer of a listening comprehension strategy to

independent reading in first-grade students. Early Childhood Education Journal,

32(2), 69-74.

Garriott, M. & Jones, L. (2005). Closing the fluency gap in the middle grades.

Principal, 85(1), 67-68.

Glenberg, A.M., Brown, M. & Levin, J.R. (2007). Enhancing comprehension in small

reading groups using a manipulation strategy. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 32, 389-399.

Good, R.H., Simmons, D.S., Kame'enui, E.J., Kaminski, R.A., & Wallin, J. (2002).

Summary of decision rules for intensive, strategic, and benchmark instructional

recommendations in kindergarten through third grade (Tech. Rep. No. 11).

Eugene, OR: University of Oregon.

Graves, M.F., & Dykstra, R. (1997). Contextualizing the first-grade studies: What is the

best way to teach children to read? Reading Research Quarterly, 32(4), 342-344.

Gray, W.S. (1917). The relation of silent reading to economy in education. In Whipple,

G.M. (ed). (1917). Second report of the committee on minimal essentials in

elementary school subjects: Sixteenth yearbook of the national society for the

study of education, part I. Bloomington, IL: Public School Publishing, 17-32.

Greenwood, C.R., Tapia, Y., Abbott, M., & Walton, C. (2003). A building-based case

study of evidence-based literacy practices: Implementation, reading behavior,

Page 117: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

105

and growth in reading fluency, K-4. The Journal of Special Education, 37(2), 95-

110.

Greenwood, S.C., & Flanigan, K. (2007). Overlapping vocabulary and comprehension:

Context clues complement semantic gradients. The Reading Teacher, 61(3), 249-

254.

Griffith, L.W. & Rasinski, T.V. (2004). A focus on fluency: How one teacher

incorporated fluency with her reading curriculum. The Reading Teacher, 58(2),

126-137.

Grimshaw, S., Dungworth, N., McKnight, C. & Morris, A. (2007). Electronic books:

Children's reading and comprehension. British Journal of Educational

Technology, 38(4), 583-599.

Harris, T.L. & Hodges, R.E. (Eds.). (1995). The literacy dictionary: The vocabulary of

reading and writing. Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Harvey, S. & Goudvis, A. (2000). Strategies that work: Teaching comprehension to

enhance understanding. Markham, Ontario: Pembroke Publishers Limited.

Hasbrouck, J. & Tindal, G.A. (2006). Oral reading fluency norms: A valuable

assessment tool for reading teachers. The Reading Teacher, 59(7), 636-644.

Hertzberg, M. (2000). 'So we can learn something as well as doing something fun':

Learning about reading through readers' theatre. Australian Journal of Language

and Literacy, 23(1), 21+.

Hiebert, E.H. & Fisher, C.W. (2005). A review of the National Reading Panel's studies

on fluency: The role of text. The Elementary School Journal, 105(5), 443-460.

Page 118: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

106

Hitchcock, C.H., Prater, M.A. & Dowrick, P.W. (2004). Reading comprehension and

fluency: Examining the effects of tutoring and video self-modeling on first-grade

students with reading difficulties. Learning Disability Quarterly, 27(2), 89-103.

Holly, J. (n.d.). Gradual release of responsibility. Retrieved April 29, 2006.

http://home.earthlink.net/~jhholly/gradualrelease.htm

Homan, S.P., Klesius, J.P., & Hite, C. (1993). Effects of repeated readings and

nonrepetitive strategies on students' fluency and comprehension. Journal of

Educational Research, 87(2), 94-99.

Hudson, R.F., Lane, H.B., & Pullen, P.C. (2005). Reading fluency assessment and

instruction: What, why, and how? The Reading Teacher, 58(8), 702-714.

Johns, J.L. (2005). Fluency norms for students in grades one through eight. Illinois

Reading Council Journal. 33 (4), 3-8.

Juel, C. (1988). Learning to read and write: A longitudinal study of 54 children from

first through fourth grades. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80(4), 437-447.

Juel, C. & Deffes, R. (2004). Making words stick. Educational Leadership, 61(6), 30-

34.

Keehn, S. (2003). The effect of instruction and practice through readers theatre on young

readers' oral reading fluency. Reading Research and Instruction, 42(4), 40-61.

Kuhn, M.R. (2004/2005). Helping students become accurate, expressive readers:

Fluency instruction for small groups. The Reading Teacher, 58(4), 338-344.

Kuhn, M.R. & Stahl, S.A. (2003). Fluency: A review of developmental and remedial

practices. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1), 3-21.

Page 119: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

107

LaBerge, D. & Samuels, S.J. (1974). Toward a theory of automatic information

processing in reading. Cognitive Psychology, 6, 293-323.

Lagrou, R.J., Burns, M.K., Mizerek, E.A., & Mosack, J. (2006). Effect of text

presentation on reading fluency and comprehension: An exploratory analysis.

Journal of Instructional Psychology, 33(2), 100-109.

Lapp, D., Fisher, D., & Grant, M. (2008). "You can read this text - I'll show you how":

Interactive comprehension instruction. Journal of adolescent & Adult Literacy,

51(5), pp. 372-383.

Lee, J., Grigg, W., and Donahue, P. (2007). The nation's report card: Reading 2007

(NCES 2007-496). Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics,

Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.

Lionetti, T.M., & Cole, C.L. (2004). A comparison of the effects of two rates of listening

while reading on oral reading fluency and reading comprehension. Education and

Treatment of Children, 27(2), 114-129.

Lutz, S.L., Guthrie, J.T. & Davis, M.C. (2006). Scaffolding for engagement in

elementary school reading instruction. The Journal of Educational Research,

100(1), 3-20.

Manset-Williamson, G., & Nelson, J.M. (2005). Balanced, strategic reading instruction

for upper-elementary and middle school students with reading disabilities: A

comparative study of two approaches. Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 28(1), 59-

74.

Martinez, M., Roser, N.L. & Strecker, S. (1999). 'I never thought I could be a

star': A readers theatre ticket to fluency. The Reading Teacher, 52(4), 326-334.

Page 120: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

108

Mastropiere, M.A., Leinart, A., & Scruggs, T.E. (1999). Strategies to increase reading

fluency. Intervention in School and Clinic, 34(5), 278-83+.

McGuinness, D. (2004). Early reading instruction: What science really tells us about

how to teach reading. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

McKenna, M.C. & Stahl, S.A. (2003). Assessment for reading instruction. New York:

The Guilford Press.

Maria, K. (1990). Reading comprehension instruction: Issues and strategies. Parkton,

MD: York Press.

Mercer, CD., Campbell, K.U., Miller, M.D., Mercer, K.D. & Lane, H.B. (2000). Effects

of a reading fluency intervention for middle schoolers with specific learning

disabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 15(4), 179-189.

Minskoff, E. (2005). Teaching reading to struggling learners. Baltimore: Paul H.

Brookes Publishing Company.

Moskal, M.K. & Blachowicz, C. (2006). Partnering for fluency. New York: The

Guilford Press.

Morrow, L.M. (1989). Using story retelling to develop comprehension. In Muth, K.D.

(ed). (1989). Children's comprehension of text: Research into practice (pp. 3 7 -

58). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Morrow, L.M., Gambrell, L.B. & Pressley, M. (Eds.). (2003). Best practices in literacy

instruction. New York: The Guilford Press.

Moskal, M.K. (2006). Students self-managed repeated reading: Successful fluency

development for disfluent readers. Illinois Reading CouncilJournal, 34(1), pp. 3-

11.

Page 121: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

109

National Institute of Child Health & Human Development. (2000). Report of the

national reading panel: Teaching children to read: An evidence-based

assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for

reading instruction: Reports of the subgroups. Washington, D.C.: NICHD.

Nelson-Herber, J. & Johnston, C.S. (1989). Questions and concerns about teaching

narrative and expository text. In Muth, K.D. (ed). (1989). Children's

comprehension of text: Research into practice (pp. 263-280). Newark, DE:

International Reading Association.

Nathan, R.G. & Stanovich, K.E. (1991). The causes and consequences of differences in

reading fluency. Theory Into Practice, XXX(3), 176-184.

National Endowment for the Arts (2007). To read or not to read: A question of national

consequence (Research Report #47). Washington, D.C.: Office of Research &

Analysis, National Endowment for the Arts.

National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to read: An evidence-based

assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for

reading instruction. Washington, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health and

Human Development.

Nes, S.L. (2003). Using paired reading to enhance the fluency skills of less-skilled

readers. Reading Improvement, 40(4), 179-192.

Ness, M. (2007). Reading comprehension strategies in secondary content-area

classrooms. Phi Delta Kappan, 89(3), 229-231.

Nicholson, T. & Tan, A. (1999). Proficient word identification for comprehension. In

Thompson, G.B. & Nicolson, T. (eds.) (1999). Learning to read: Beyond

Page 122: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

110

phonics andwhole language (pp. 150-173). New York: Teachers College

Press.

No child left behind act of 2001. (2002). Retrieved March 3, 2004.

http://vvww.ed.gov/policv/elsec/leg/esea02/107-110.pdf

Oakley, G. (2003). Improving oral reading fluency (and comprehension) through the

creation of talking books. Reading Online, 6(7). Retrieved April 7, 2004.

http://www.readingonline.org/articles/art index.asp?HREF=oaklev/index.html

O'Connor, R.E., Harty, K.R., & Fulmer, D. (2005). Tiers of Intervention in kindergarten

through third grade. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(6), 532-538.

O'Connor, R.E., White, A., & Swanson, H.L. (2007). Repeated reading versus

continuous reading: Influences on reading fluency and comprehension. Council

for Exceptional Children, 74(1), pp. 31-46.

Osborn, J., Lehr, F., & Hiebert, E.H. (2003). A Focus on Fluency. Honolulu, HI: Pacific

Resources for Education and Learning.

Osborne, Jason & Elaine Waters (2002). Four assumptions of multiple regression that

researchers should always test. Practical Assessment, Research & Evaluation,

8(2). Retrieved September 3, 2007 from

http://PAREonline.net/getvn.asp?v=8&n=2.

Paquette, K.R., Fello, S.E. & Jalongo, M.R. (2007). The talking drawings strategy:

Using primary children's illustrations and oral language to improve

comprehension of expository text. Early Childhood Education Journal, 35(1),

65-73.

Page 123: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

I l l

Pardo, L. (2004). What every teacher needs to know about comprehension. TheReading

Teacher, 58(3), pp. 272-280.

Pearson, P.D., Roehler, L.R., Dole, J.A., & Duffy, G.G. (1992). Developing expertise in

reading comprehension. In Samuels, S.J. & Farstrup, A.E. (1992). What research

has to say about reading instruction (pp. 145-199). Newark,DE: International

Reading Association.

Pearson, P.D., & Duke, N.K. (2002). Comprehension instruction in the primary grades.

In Block, C.C., & Pressley, M. (eds). (2002). Comprehension instruction:

Research-basedbest practices (pp. 247-258). New York: Guilford Publications.

Pearson, P.D., & Gallagher, M.C. (1983). The instruction of reading comprehension.

Contemporary Educational Psychology, 8, 317-344.

Pikulski, J.J., & Chard, D.J. (2005). Fluency: Bridge between decoding and reading

comprehension. The Reading Teacher, 58(6), 510-519.

Pressley, M. (2002). Comprehension strategies instruction: A turn-of-the-century status

report. In Block, C.C. & Pressley, M. (Eds.), Comprehension instruction:

Research-based best practices (pp. 11-27). New York: Guilford.

Pressley, M. (2002). Conclusion: Improving comprehension instruction: A path for the

future. In Block, C.C, Gambrell, L.B., & Pressley, M. (2002). Improving

comprehension instruction: Rethinking research, theory, and classroom practice

(pp. 385-399). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Pressley, M., Allington, R.L., Wharton-McDonald, R., Block, C.C. & Morrow, L.M.

(2001). Learning to read: Lessons from exemplary first-grade classrooms. New

York: The Guilford Press.

Page 124: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

112

Pressley, M., Gaskins, I.W. & Fingeret, L. (2006). Instruction and development of

reading fluency in struggling readers. In Samuels, S.J. & Farstrup, A.E. (eds).

What research has to say about fluency instruction (pp. 47-69). Newark, DE:

International Reading Association.

Pressley, M. & Hilden, K. (2006). Teaching reading comprehension. In McKeough, A.,

Phillips, L.M., Timmons, V., & Lupart, J.L. (Eds.). (2006). Understanding

Literacy Development: A Global View (pp. 49 - 64). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum Associates.

Pressley, M, Wharton-McDonald, R., Mistretta-Hampston, J., & Echevarria, M. (1998).

Literacy instruction in 10 fourth- and fifth-grade classrooms in upstate New York.

Scientific Studies of Reading, 2(2), 159-194.

Quick, B.N. (1998). Beginning reading and developmentally appropriate practice (DAP):

Past, present, and future. Peabody Journal of Education, 73(3/4), 253-272.

Rasinski, T. & Padak, N. (2000). Effective reading strategies: Teaching children who

find reading difficult. Upper Saddle River, New Jersey: Prentice Hall Co.

Rasinski, T.V., & Hoffman, J.V. (2003). Oral reading in the school literacy curriculum.

Reading Research Quarterly, 38(4), 510-522.

Rasinski, T.V., Padak, N.D., Church, B.W., Fawcett, G., Hendershot, J., Henry, J.M.,

Moss, B.G., Peck, J.K., Pryor, E., & Roskos, K.A. (Eds.). (2000). Teaching word

recognition, spelling and vocabulary: Strategies from the reading teacher.

Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Page 125: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

113

Rasinski, T.V. (2003). The fluent reader: Oral reading strategies for building word

recognition, fluency, and comprehension. New York: Scholastic Professional

Books.

Rasinski, T.V. (1990). Investigating measures of reading fluency. Educational Research

Quarterly, 14(3), 37-44.

Rasinski, T.V. & Padak, N. (2008). Evidence-Based Instruction in Reading: A

Professional Development Guide to Fluency. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

Reutzel, D.R., Camperell, K., & Smith, J.A. (2002). Hitting the wall: Helping struggling

readers comprehend. In Block, C.C., Gambrell, L.B. & Pressley, M. (eds.)

(2002). Improving comprehension instruction: Rethinking research, theory and

classroom practice (pp. 321-353). San Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Reutzel, D.R., & Hollingsworth, P.M. (1993). Effects of fluency training on second

graders' reading comprehension. Journal of Educational Research, 86(6), 325-

331.

Rhodes, L.K. & Dudley-Marling, C. (1996). Readers and writers with a difference: A

holistic approach to teaching struggling readers and writers. Portsmouth, NH:

Heinemann.

Rinehart, S.D., Barksdale-Ladd, M.A., & Paterson, J.J. (1994). Increasing story recall

through prereading instruction: Use of advance organizers combined with

teacher-guided discussion. In Sturtevant, E.G. & Linek, W.M. (eds.). (1994).

Pathways for literacy: Learners teach and teachers learn (pp. 237-247).

Commerce, TX: The College Reading Association.

Page 126: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

114

Robinson, H.A., Faraone, V., Hittleman, D.R., & Unruh, E. (1990). Reading

comprehension instruction 1783 - 1987: A review of trends and research.

Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Rosenblatt, L.M. (1978). The reader, the text, the poem. Carbondale and Edwardsville,

IL: Southern Illinois University Press.

Samuels, S.J. (1979). The method of repeated readings. The Reading Teacher, 32, 403-

408.

Samuels, S.J. (2002). Reading fluency: Its development and assessment. In Farstrup,

A.E., & Samuels, S.J. (eds.). (2002). What research has to say about reading

instruction, (pp. 166-183). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Samuels, S.J. & Kamil, M.L. (1984) Models of the reading process. In Pearson, P.D.,

Barr, R., Kamil, M.L., & Mosenthal, P.B. (Eds.). (1984). Handbook of reading

research (pp. 185-224). New York: Longman.

Samuels, S.J., Schermer, N. & Reinking, D. (1992). Reading fluency: Techniques for

making decoding automatic. In Samuels, S.J. & Farstrup, A.E. (1992). What

research has to say about reading instruction (pp. 124-144). Newark,DE:

International Reading Association.

Schwartz, J.I. (1988). Encouraging early literacy: An integrated approach to reading

and writing in N-3. Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

Seuss, Dr. (1978). I Can Read With My Eyes Shut. New York: Random House Books

for Young Readers.

Seuss, Dr. (1990). Oh, The Places You '11 Go. New York: Random House Books for

Young Readers.

Page 127: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

115

Shanahan, T. (2002). What reading research says: The promises and limitations of

applying research to reading education. In Farstrup, A.E. & Samuels, S.J. (Eds.).

(2002). What research has to say about reading instruction (pp. 8-24).Newark,

DE: International Reading Association.

Snow, C.E. & Sweet, A.P. (2003). Reading for comprehension. In Sweet, A.P. & Snow,

C.E. (Eds.). (2003). Rethinking reading comprehension, (pp. 1-11). New York:

The Guilford Press.

Speece, D.L. & Ritchey, K.D. (2005). A longitudinal study of the development of oral

reading fluency in young children at risk for reading failure. Journal of Learning

Disabilities, 38(5), 387-399.

Stage, S.A. & Jacobsen, M.D. (2001). Predicting student success on a state-mandated

performance-based assessment using oral reading fluency. School Psychology

Review, 30(3), 407-419.

Stahl, K.A.D. (2004). Proof, practice, and promise: Comprehension strategy instruction

in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 57(7), pp. 598-609.

Stahl, S.A., & Heubach, K. (2005). Fluency-oriented reading instruction. Journal of

Literacy Research, 37(1), 25-60. In Dougherty Stahl, K.A., & McKenna, M.C.

(Eds.). (2006). Reading research at work: Foundations of effective practice (pp.

177-213). New York: The Guilford Press.

Stahl, S.A., Jacobson, M.G., Davis, C.E. & Davis, R.L. Prior knowledge and difficult

vocabulary in the comprehension of unfamiliar text. Reading Research

Quarterly, 24, 27-43. In Dougherty Stahl, K.A., & McKenna, M.C. (Eds.).

Page 128: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

116

(2006). Reading research at work: Foundations of effective practice (pp. 284-

302). New York: The Guilford Press.

Stanovich, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in reading: Some consequences of individual

differences in the acquisition of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 21(4), pp.

360-407.

Stanovich, K.E. (1980). Toward an interactive-compensatory model of individual

differences in the development of reading fluency. Reading Research Quarterly,

16(1), pp. 32-71.

Stecker, S.K., Roser, N.L. & Martinez, M.G. (1998). Understanding oral reading

fluency. In Shanahan, T., & Rodriguez-Brown, F.V. (Eds.), 47' yearbook of the

National Reading Conference (pp. 295-310). Chicago: National Reading

Conference.

Sweet, A.P. & Snow, C.S. (2002). Reconceptualizing reading comprehension. In Block,

C.C., Gambrell, L.B. & Pressley, M. (eds.) (2002). Improving comprehension

instruction: Rethinking research, theory and classroom practice (pp. 3-16). San

Francisco: Jossey-Bass.

Texas primary reading inventory (2006). Austin, Texas: The University of Texas

System and Texas Education Agency.

Therrien, W.J. (2004). Fluency and comprehension gains as a result of repeated reading:

A meta-analysis. Remedial and Special Education, 25(4), 252-261.

Therrien, W.J. and Kubina, R.M. (2006). Developing reading fluency with repeated

reading. Intervention in School and Clinic, 41(3), 156-160.

Topping, K.J. (2006). Building reading fluency: Cognitive, behavioral, and

Page 129: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

117

socioemotional factors and the role of peer-mediated learning. In Samuels, S.J. &

Farstrup, A.E. (eds). What research has to say about fluency instruction (pp. 106-

129). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

Torgesen, J.K. & Hudson, R.F. (2006). Reading fluency: Critical issues for struggling

readers. In Samuels, S.J. & Farstrup, A.E. (eds). What research has to say about

fluency instruction (pp. 130-158). Newark, DE: International Reading

Association.

Vaughn, S., Chard, D.J., Bryant, D.P., Coleman, M., Tyler, B.J., Linan-Thompson, S., &

Kouzekanani, K. (2000). Fluency and comprehension interventions for third-

grade students. Remedial and Special Education, 21(6), 325+.

Vaughn, S., & Linan-Thompson, S. (2004). Research-based methods of reading

instruction: Grades K-3. Alexandria, VA: Association for Supervision and

Curriculum Development.

Vaughn, S., Linan-Thompson, S., Kouzekanani, K., Bryant, D.P., Dickson, S. & Blozis,

S.A. (2003). Reading instruction grouping for students with reading difficulties.

Remedial and Special Education, 24(5), 301+.

Van Keer, H. (2004). Fostering reading comprehension in fifth grade by explicit

instruction in reading strategies and peer tutoring. British Journal of Educational

Psychology, 74, pp.37-70.

Van Keer, H. & Verhaeghe, J.P. (2005). Effects of explicit reading strategies instruction

and peer tutoring on second and fifth graders' reading comprehension and self-

efficacy perceptions. The Journal of Experimental Education, 73(4), 291-329.

Wallace, R., Pearman, C , Hail, C, & Hurst, B. (2007). Writing for comprehension.

Page 130: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

118

Reading Horizons, 48(1), 41-56.

Walley, C. (1993). An invitation to reading fluency. The Reading Teacher, 46 (6), pp.

526-527.

Weinstein, G. & Cooke, N.L. (1992). The effects of two repeated reading interventions

on generalization of fluency. Learning Disability Quarterly, 15(1), pp. 21-28.

Welsch, R.G. (2006). 20 ways to ... increase oral reading fluency. Intervention in

School and Clinic, 41 (3), 180-183.

Wendler, D., Samuels, S.J. & Moore, V.K. (1989). Comprehension instruction of

award-winning teachers, teachers with master's degrees, and other teachers.

Reading Research Quarterly, 24(4), pp. 382-401.

Williams, J.P. (2005). Instruction in reading comprehension for primary-grade students:

A focus on text structure. The Journal of Special Education, 39(1), pp. 6-18.

Worthy, J. and Broaddus, K. (2002). Fluency beyond the primary grades: From group

performance to silent, independent reading. The Reading Teacher, 55(4), 334-

343.

Zutell, J. & Rasinski, T.V. (1991). Training teachers to attend to their students' oral

reading fluency. Theory Into Practice, 30(3), 211-217.

Page 131: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Appendix A

TPRI Student Record Sheet

Page 132: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

120

T ! P I R I 1 • Student Record Sheet Grade 1

Student:

Teacher:

Date of Administration;

BOY . ./ /

MOY / /

EOY /. . . /

• Start of Beginning-of-Year (BOY) Screening Section •

Screening 1 Graphophonemic Knowledge

-

1

2,

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8,

9.

10.

Utter Soupd

L 1

0 o

N n

1 i

R r

E e

H h

W w

U u

Y y

AnswH*

L 1 / I / |

; O o Ibl \

! N n /n / '

! 1 i IV

' R r hi

j E e lei

| H h Ibl

| W w Ivjl

| U u /u /

i Y y /y/ /T or e/

TOTAL CORRECT: flOpossibte;

' ^ ^ n

Branching Rules

8-10 correct—Developed: Go to Screening 2, page 47.

0-7 cornet—Still Developing: Go to Screening 3, page 48.

Screening 2 I Word Reading Screening 3 Phonemic Awareness

ami ftie#jl»'»

1. become

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

school j

queen !

great <

summer |

honey I

push I

asked i

TOTAL CORRECT: (8 possible}

Branching Rules

3-8 correct—Developed; Go to Task 10, page 61-

0-2 correct—Still Developing: Go to Screening 3, page 48.

Remember, pronounce letter sounds, not letter names, j and say the word silently to yourself first. j

1. /ml /a/ /ss/ |

2. Ibl loot Iml !

3. I\NI lal Isl Ipl |

4. Is/ IV Ixl /eel IXJ \

5. /ph/ lol Inel \

6. Ibl HI /u/ M ft/ !

mass

boom

wasp

street

phone !

blunt

TOTAL CORRECT: (6 possible)

Branching Rules

5-6 correct—Developed: Go to Task 10, page 61.

0-4 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 1, page 52.

• End o f BOY Sc reen ing Sec t ion • •wry o* fr>.i? Sysrem and Texa1. Edwt<Jtt©n Ayemy

Page 133: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

121

Start of End-of-Year (EOY) Screening Section

Screening 4 Word Reading

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

1.

8.

around

swing !

test

been

lion

young ]

matter j

people j

TOTAL CORRECT: (8 possible)

Branching Rules

5-8 correct—Developed: Go to Task 10, page 61.

0-4 correct—Still Developing: Go to Screening 5, page 50.

Screening 5 Phonemic Awareness

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

Remember, pronounce letter sounds, not letter names, and say the word silently to yourself first.

/s/ l\l rV M lerl

Iml let / l l / low/

HI l\l lew/ :

/a/ lb/ /ou/ In/ Idl

/sh/ /ou/ / I / 161 /er/ ;

M lot Ihl lot Idl lyl \

silver

mellow |

f lew

abound j

shoulder

nobody I

TOTAL CORRECT: 16 possible)

Branching Rules

5-6 correct—Developed: Go to Task 10, page 61.

0-4 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 1, page 52.

End of EOY Screening Section •

Page 134: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

122

Inventory Section (BOY, MOY and EOY)

For MOY and EOY: If a student scored SD on a task when you administered the TPRl earlier in the year, re-administer every item within the task during the next administration.

Phonemic Awareness

ttwwfttt? Weird Nits "Amm ' " MOV S«r»«»1>

cov

Remember, pronounce letter sounds, not letter names. \ \ and say the word silently to yourself first. 1

1. M /ery/

2. /sh/ /are/

3. / t h / / ing/

4. /br/ /anch/

5. Id Irl /awl/

very

share

th ing

! branch

crawl

TOTAL CORRECT; {5 possible)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 2, page 53.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 5, page 56.

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered

Wmdtafll

Phonemic Awareness

jWufif- f^jjHPpf

Remember pronounce letter 50 and say the word silently

1. / I / /a/ /mb/

2. /ch/ /'i/ In!

3. Ibl /a/ Inl 161

4. Id l\l lol /ck7

5. Is/ 1X1 Irl lol /ng/

./nds.. nor /etter names, to yourself first. \

lamb ; j |

u- [ '• i chin j ;

band j j

clock |

strong i ! 1

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 3, page 54.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 5, page 56,

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered.

Phonemic Awareness Branching Rules

Dating ImtialfourKis Answer

Remember pronounce letter sounds and sa\ the word silentlv to yc

1. hair

2. name

3. sold

4. f l ight

5, crest

w i thou t the

w i thou t the

w i thou t the

w i thou t the

w i thou t the

/h /

Inl

N

m Id

I 1 not letter names, \ \ urself first ] 1

! . ! ! i air ; i !

! aim ; |

i old ' \

l ight ; ;

rest

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 4, page 55.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 5, page 56.

• Check box if Task is Developed This Task no longer needs to be administered.

md T y x ^ Educa!'

Page 135: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

123

Phonemic Awareness Branching Rules

Dtletfng RnsJ Sounds Answw BOY

CM) MOV . • * f . ~

Scowfofl Stout KM)

Remember, pronounce letter sounds, and say the word silently to yo

1. rode w i thou t the

2. for t w i thou t the

3. bloom wi thou t the

4. grain w i thou t the

5. stayed w i thou t the

161

IV

An/

M

/ d /

not letter names, j urself first. 1

row j !

i for ;

blue ; i j

g^y ! j !

stay j 1 j

TOTAL CORRECT: IS possible)

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 5, page 56,

0-3 correct—Stili Developing: Go to Task 5, page 56.

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered,

Graphophonemic Knowledge

Initial Consonant Substitution Answw $ei . '»•»." • '-stiff Swwto.fl Swm%n *snhl>

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

-°g

—°g

og

—09

....og

! f

fa : I ! h I I i

i ! I I : |

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 6, page 57.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 10, page 61.

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered

Graphophonemic Knowledge

Pinal Consonant Substitution

1. sa__

2. s a _

3. sa....

4. s a _

5. fa ._

Answer

t

! d

! g

i p

t

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

BOY Stt» (8,1)

MOV Score to, t)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 7, page 58.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 10, page 61.

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered.

O JOOo Trio Univi 1 To »,is FdiKjtson Ago re

Page 136: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

124

Graphophonemic Knowledge

1

2.

3.

4.

5.

b 9

b_g

b _ g

b_g

P._t

a

e

i

u

i o

'

i ' ;

I ; !

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 8, page 59.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 10, page 61.

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered.

Graphophonemic Knowledge

•Cry MOV "tfY initial Bttmttr« Substitution Answer s ^ . n *«£$,„ j ^ i )

1.

2.

3.

4,

5.

__am

am

.__op

—Op

_ °P

cl

I gr i < i dr j j j

' f l

\ st

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 9. page 60

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 10, page 61.

• Check box if Task is Developed This Task no longer needs to be administered.

Blends In ftfMri Position

Graphophonemic Knowledge

Answer swett,!) sewiflM} Sc*re<o.t}

1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

be

g a _

ca

du._

f a _

st !

, SP i

j st | i ;

! sk I \ ! st • ; I

TOTAL CORRECT: (5 possible)

Branching Rules

4-5 correct—Developed: Go to Task 10, page 61.

0-3 correct—Still Developing: Go to Task 10, page 61.

• Check box if Task is Developed. This Task no longer needs to be administered.

O 2006 The University of Texas System oivd Tends Education AyetKy

Page 137: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

125

• Word List fo r Story P lacement ! Use at each administration time point to determine which story the student should read.

Word List for Story Placement Criteria for Story Placement

; "*«KP«r Star. (0,ir Seo* %,i)

1, bump

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

family

push

front

hurry

turned

lesson

early

earn

voice

sugar

bodies

hours

business

island

TOTAL CORRECT: (IS possible)

;

!

;

i

wte «t Road Cort

0-3

4-6

7

8

9-15

iMfty

Story 1, page 62.

Story 2, page 64.

Story 3, page 66.

Story 4, page 68.

Story 5, page 70.

Placing Students Into the Same Story If a student reads a story more than once, use a different color pen/pencil to mark errors and cal­culate the score. Another option is to photocopy a clean page and staple it to the booklet.

Moving Students Back a Story If students are unable to read the story in which they are placed, have them read the previous story. If students are unable to read Story 1, read Story 3 or 4 from kindergarten EOY to obtain a Listening Comprehension score.

These results do not need to be recorded on the Student Inventory Summary She? Scores ate used only for appropriate placement in the story.

Page 138: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

126

Task 11 Story 1 - The Sun

If a student reads the same story more than once, please use a different color ink to mark errors.

"Look at the big, yel low sun," said Sis. ~'-'.'''M

"I like to play in the sun, I like to run in the sun," "No," said Jan, "I do not like to play in the sun. I do not like to

run in the sun. The sun is too hot ." "But, Jan," said Sis, "we have fun in the sun. We go to the pond in the sun.

We get soaked in the sun." "I like to go to the pond, I like to get wet. I like to play in the sun, t oo , " said Jan.

Accuracy Level

Pni

Inst

ind

m m m m n H r Of WTQfA I I I tftft 9 I K

• O Y M O T M Y

10 or more errors *

6-9 errors • :

0-5 errors ; I

Fluency Rate

aov

MOV

EOYj

MOV

EOV

94

94

94

The reading rate goal is 60 S-WCPM by the end of grade

Administer at! of the Reading Comprehension questions below. For directions, go to the TPRI Teacher's Guide, page 63.

Task 11

Story 1 - The Sun

Reading Comprehension

Tell me two things that Sis likes to do in the sun. Answer; Play in tho sun; run in the sun; have fun in the sun; go to the pond and get wet in the sun.

2 What do the children do at the pond? Answer: Get wet.

3. In this story, who does not like the sun? Answer: Jan.

A. Where do the children in the story like to play? Answer. Outside in the sun; in the pond.

5. Why does Jan change her mind about liking the sun? Answer: She likes to go to the pond and get wet.

6. Why doesn't Jan like the sun? Answer: It is too hot.

7. What does soaked mean in the sentence, "We get soaked in the sun"? soaked Answer: To get completely wet. Score 0: A little wet; damp.

8. What does pond mean in the sentence, "We go to the pond in the sun"? pond Answer: A small lake Score 0: A fountain; pool; ocean.

K

BOY Total Exp

BOY Total Imp

BOY Total Vocab

MOY Total Exp

MOY Total Imp

MOY Total Vocab

.u

EOY Total Exp

EOY Total Imp

EOY Total Vocab

TOTAL CORRECT: (8 possible)

BOY MOY EOY

End of Assessment

Please record results on Student Inventory Summary Sheet.

C ?CX)& 'he UnivprstTy rtf Tcx-v; Syi tpm and "ex.Vi Education A g e . x y

Page 139: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

127

Story 2 - At the Pond

If a student reads the same story more than once, please use a different color ink to mark errors.

Tom and Sid like the pond - *,TZ, *,."» ' „ ' " " ' ' ~ *,'.

Dad likes the pond, too. Tom fed the yellow duck Sid fed a brown fish. Dad said, "Look at the blue bird."

"I see a green frog," said Torn. "Can we jump in the pond?" asked Tom and Sid. "No," said dad, "Mom said we

cannot get wet." "But, dad, it is hot," said Tom and Sid. "It is hot," said dad. In went dad. In went Tom and Sid.

Accuracy Level

of trrerafit t h * ntr& MVMOVIOV mM9 Fluency Rate

*«*<£83S) Fro

but

Ind

9 or more errors

0-4 errors

MOV

BOY

WOT

to*

76 1X60 =

76 I 1x60 =

76 1x60 =

The reading rate goal is 60 S-WCPM by the end of grade '

Administer all of the Reading Comprehension questions below. For directions, go to the TPRI Teacher's Guide, page 65.

Reading Comprehension

Stotyi-Atthttood ;-*^?*%F |

1. What does Tom see? A green frog,

2. What did Tom and Sid ask dad? Answer: If they could jump into the pond

3. When the boys first asked to jump into the pond, dad said no. Why did he say no? Answer: Mom said the boys could not get wet.

4. Where does the story take place? Answer: At a pond.

5. What changed dad's mind about letting the boys jump in the pond? Answer: He agreed it was hot.

6. How do you think mom will feel when she sees that dad, Tom and Sid are wet? Answer: Upset; mad.

7, What does fed mean in the sentence, "Tom fed the yellow duck"? fed Answer: Gave food-Score 0: To eat.

8. What does pond mean in the sentence, "Can we jump in the pond"? pond Answer: A small lake. Score 0: A fountain, pool; ocean.

BOV Total Exp

BOY Total Imp

BOY Total Vixab

cov CM)

MOY Total Exp

MOY Total Imp

MOY Total Vocab

EOY Total Exp

EOY Total Imp

EOY Total Vocab

hlffi-End of Assessment

Please record results on Student Inventory Summary Sheet.

£J 2006 The University of Texab System and Texas Education Ago-icy

TOTAL CORRECT: (8 possible)

BOY MOY

Page 140: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

128

Story 3 - Carla Makes Tacos

If a student reads the same story more than once, please use a different color ink to mark errors.

Carla's family had tacos for lunch. • -, . , " -,- i , _ ,

Carta helped. She put the salt on the tacos. She put the hot pepper on the tacos. Her family was happy to have

tacos. They sat down for lunch, "Please pass the tacos," said papa. "Oh, n o l " said papa. "They are too ho t ! "

"Oh, no! Pass the milk, f as t i " said litt le brother. Morn looked at Carla, "You put too much hot pepper on the

tacos!" Carla said, "The hot pepper is red. The salt is whi te . There was a mix-up." Carla fel t very sad. Papa said,

"Oh, wel l , that's OK. Everyone get in the van, Taco Bell, here we come!"

Accuracy Level

•ov

Fluency Rate

MM

liwt

tad

1 ! 1 11 or more errors i ; j

5-10 errors | I i

0-4 errors

MOV

toy

* * - jJ5£3o x60 =

X60 :

ix60=]

The reading rate goa! is 60 S-WCPM by the end of grade 1.

Administer all of the Reading Comprehension questions below. For directions, go to the TPRI Teacher's Guide, page 67.

Reading Comprehension

Story $ - Carta Malm Ham ••anf W6Y (0,1)

When does Carla's family eat tacos? Answer: At iunch.

2. Why were the tacos too hot? Answer: Carla put too much hot pepper on the tacos.

5. Where did the family end up going that day? Answer: Taco Bell,

4. Who tasted the tacos first? Answer: Papa.

5. Why did little brother ask for the milk? Answer: His mouth (tongue) was burning.

6. Why did Carla feel sad? Answer: She put too much pepper on the tacos.

7, What does pass mean in the sentence, "Pass the milk, fast"? pass Answer: To hand from one person to another. Score 0; To throw; toss.

8. What does mix-up mean in the sentence, "There was a mix-up"? mix-up Answer: Mistake. Score 0: To stir.

BOY Total Exp

BOY Total Imp

BOY Total Vocab

MOY Total Exp

MOY Total Imp

MOY Total Vocab

EOY Total Exp

EOY Total Imp

EOY Total Vocab

End of Assessment

Please record results on Student Inventory Summary Sheet,

« ^006 The Unwetvity of Tea-a* f.yir^srt . ;nd T A * 3 > buu<3-,ior A t j e w y

TOTAL CORRECT: (8 possible)

SOY MOV EOY

Page 141: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Story 4 - Will and the Skunks

If a student reads the same story more than once, please use a different color ink to mark errors.

Will likes to go on the bus to school because he gets to see his friends.:--: • ^..'^•".-• -"*-.;: < He always sits with his very best friend. Each morning, he runs to the bus stop so that he will not miss the bus. One very odd day, Will ran to the bus stop, as he did every day. When he got there, he stopped. "What is this?" At the bus stop sat a skunk. The skunk asked, "Is this the bus stop for school?" Will stopped and rubbed his eyes, "Is this a talking skunk?" he asked. "How can a skunk talk at a bus stop?" When the bus came, the doors opened and the skunk hopped on the bus. Will got on, too. "Oh, no! This cannot be," Will said, as he rubbed his eyes again. The bus was full of skunks.

Accuracy Level Fluency Rate

Pro

fvwt

M

P «k»Y»r

15 or more errors . ,

i ! ;

8-14 errors { 1 ; 0 7 errors

TOY

MOV

WY

i e .

», The reading rate goal is 60 S-WCPM by the end of grade 1.

Administer all of the Reading Comprehension questions below. For directions, go to the TPRI Teacher's Guide, page 69.

Reading Comprehension

Stay 4 - wm*ntfth9 #wWfcr wot «0¥ SOY

i . Why does Will like to ride the school bus? Answer: So he can see his friends,

2 What did the skunk ask Will? Answer: If this was the bus stop for school.

3. What did Wit! see when he got on the bus? Answer: it was filled with skunks.

Why was it an odd day? Answer: It was odd to see a skunk at the bus stop

Why did Will rub his eyes? Answer; He could not believe what he was seeing were working correctly.

to make sure they

6. Who normally rides the bus to school? Answer: People; kids, children.

7. What does 00*0' mean in the sentence, "One very odd day, Will ran to the bus stop, as he did every day"? odd Answer; Strange; unusual; not normal. Score 0; Special; good, an odd number.

What does miss mean in the sentence, "Each morning, he runs to the bus stop so that he wil l not miss the bus"? miss Answer: Not catch; not ride Score 0: To feel sad.

End of Assessment

Please record results on Student inventory Summary Sheet.

TOTAL CORRECT: (B possible)

BOY Total Exp

BOY Total Imp

BOY Total Vocab

nr BOY

MOY Total Exp

MOY Total Imp

MOY Total Vocab

EOY Total Exp

EOY Total Imp

EOY Total Vocab

MOY •

•& J0Q£ The Uni

Page 142: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

130

Story 5 - Feeling Lazy

If a student reads the same story mare than once, please use a different color ink to mark errors.

It was a very hot, sunny summer day, , "

Jon and Ming had been watching TV a!! morning. Mom told them to turn off the TV. "Go play outside," she

said. "Growing boys need to run and play in the sunshine." Jon and Ming felt lazy. They just wanted to sit

around all day, "You cannot sit around all day. Out you go! " said mom. The boys picked up their basketball

and went outside. They began dribbling the ball and shooting baskets. The boys across the street saw them

playing basketball. "Can we play, too?" they asked. "Yes," said Jon and Ming. Mom went outside with glasses

of punch and a big grin. "Playing outside with friends is the best way to spend a summer day," said mom. The

boys agreed with mom. It was more fun to play than to be lazy all day.

Accuracy Level Fluency Rate

144 -

144 -

144 • j

=

= =

A.

T

|*

x60 =

x60 = |

x60 = j

The reading rate goal is 60 S WCPM by the end of grade 1.

Administer ail of the Reading Comprehension questions below. For directions, go to the TPRI Teacher's Guide, page 71.

Reading Comprehension

1. What are the names of the two boys in the story? Answer: Jon and Ming.

2. What had Jon and Ming been doing all morning? Answer: Watching "IV

3. What did mom tell the boys to do? Answer: To turn off the TV and go outside and play.

4. Why did mom want the boys outside? Answer: !t is not good for growing boyi to sit around and watch TV all day; it is more enjoyable to be outside playing than inside watching TV.

Why did mom have a big grin on her face when she brought the boys punch? Answer: She was pleased the boys were playing outside.

If mom did not ask the boys to go play outside, how would they have spent the rest of the day? Answer, Watching TV.

7. What does lazy mean rn the sentence, "ion and Ming felt lazy"? lazy Answer; inactive; not wanting to do much Score 0: Bored; sleepy.

S. What does grin mean in the sentence, "Mom went outside with glasses of punch and a big grin"? grin Answer: A smile. Score 0: To laugh.

^J

81

BOY Total Exp

BOY Total Imp

BOY Total Votab

MOY Total Exp

MOY Total Imp

MOY Total Vocab

EOY Total Exp

EOY Total tmp

EOY Total

End of Assessment

Please record results on Student Inventory Summary Sheet.

TOTAL CORRECT: (8 possible)

Page 143: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

Appendix B

Class Summary Sheet

Page 144: Effect of fluency on reading comprehension

132

*.>n?»fiy uoti^J'H'j ^ ^ i t">p 'n*Mi ^PK^I K' AitMSftjun "i)l 900? ©

# 5 -^ < ****

I I I

kuotfMnC) Xj«)nq»OA A

§§5«858SW5^ : . ^^» .

iuoii««nO )p||dxa J

lpii^r« l;?t I » A B I Xssjnwv -^ e l

H^M^f fefl >pu*iaiBu.j £§

Bfefe^:^ | H f|>MOA »!PPIW 0 £

BfeW*#.l*I; | | M c*u«uosu<o |ei;iu| & J

HSfSS"?r- *̂ " E M tpunos |»|l!U| B in

B^w^-" M M sued pjou 11 in

^g»^«|(|»l«$%y '

Tea

cher

:

Dat

e:

Cam

pus:

E9

!J