Download ppt - Contingency Diagramming 2

Transcript
Page 1: Contingency Diagramming 2

1

Causality Test

Stimulus Test

Related Outcomes Test

60” Test

Response-Unit Test

Table of Contents.Click me to begin Part 2. Or you can review by

clicking a test.

Page 2: Contingency Diagramming 2

2

Sixty-Second

TestDoes the outcome follow the response by more than sixty

seconds?

Page 3: Contingency Diagramming 2

3

Rudolph the rat is in the Skinner Box. The trainer delivers a drop of water one day after Rudolph presses the lever.

Before

Rudolph has no drop

of water

Behavior

Rudolph presses the

lever

After

Rudolph has a drop of water one day later

How about this contingency? Will the water reinforce the lever

press? Not a chance.

Page 4: Contingency Diagramming 2

4

The trainer changes the strategy;

now Rudolph receives the water one hour after his lever press.Before

Rudolph has no drop

of water

Behavior

Rudolph presses the

lever

After

Rudolph has a drop of water one hour later

Is an hour delay better? Will the water reinforce the

lever press? Still not a chance.

Page 5: Contingency Diagramming 2

5

A two-month old baby smiles and her father gives her attention,

one day later.Before

Baby receives

no attention

Behavior

Baby smiles

After

Baby receives attention one day

later How about this contingency? Will the attention reinforce the

smile? You guessed it, still not a

chance.

Page 6: Contingency Diagramming 2

6

For an outcome to reinforce or punish a response,

It must pass the 60 second test; it must follow the behavior by no more than 60 seconds

Page 7: Contingency Diagramming 2

7

This contingency is not correct.

Before

Bob does not have an

A in psychology

Behavior

Bob studies right up to test-time

After

Bob has an A in

psychology

Page 8: Contingency Diagramming 2

8

Before

Bob does not have an

A in psychology

Behavior

Bob studies right up to test-time

After

Bob has an A in

psychology

Even the quickest of teachers will not have Bobby’s test gradedwithin 60 seconds of the end of studying.

So the delay from the studying until he sees his A is greater than 60 seconds.

Page 9: Contingency Diagramming 2

9

Even though, when you do well on a test after studying

you seem to study more in the future,

this is not a direct-acting reinforcement contingency.

Don’t give up on behavior analysis yet

because, later on, you will learn about analogs

to basic reinforcement contingencies.

But now, you have to get the basics down

for basic contingencies.

Page 10: Contingency Diagramming 2

10

Click the button next to the contingency that passes the 60 second test.

Before

Bob has no tokens

Behavior

Bob reads EPB

Monday night

After

Bob gets a token

Tuesday

Before

Bob has no tokens

Behavior

Bob answers

questions in class

After

Bob immediately gets a token

A

BClick me if you

want to see the Pink Sheet

Page 11: Contingency Diagramming 2

11

9. 60” TestDoes the outcome follow theresponse by more than 60 seconds?If so, find an outcome that followssooner.

Click me to go back to the Question

Page 12: Contingency Diagramming 2

12

No, the token is delivered a day after the reading behavior.Before

Bob has no tokens

Behavior

Bob reads EPB

Monday night

After

Bob gets a token

Tuesday

Before

Bob has no tokens

Behavior

Bob answers

questions in class

After

Bob immediately gets a token

A

B

Page 13: Contingency Diagramming 2

13

Correct.Before

Bob has no tokens

Behavior

Bob reads EPB

Monday night

After

Bob gets a token

Tuesday

Before

Bob has no tokens

Behavior

Bob answers

questions in class

After

Bob immediately gets a token

A

B

Page 14: Contingency Diagramming 2

14

Click the button next to the contingency that passes the 60 second test.

Before

Dale will have no ticket

Behavior

Dale drives over the

speed limit

After

Dale will have a ticket

in a few minutes

Before

Dale does not see the speedometer pass 55

Behavior

Dale pushes the pedal

After

Dale does see the

speedometer pass 55

A

B

Page 15: Contingency Diagramming 2

15

No, it takes more than 60” from the response to the outcome of a ticket

Before

Dale will have no ticket

Behavior

Dale drives over the

speed limit

After

Dale will have a ticket

in a few minutes.

Before

Dale does not see the speedometer pass 55

Behavior

Dale pushes the pedal

After

Dale does see the

speedometer pass 55

A

B

Page 16: Contingency Diagramming 2

16

Before

Dale will have no ticket

Behavior

Dale drives over the

speed limit

After

Dale will have a ticket

in a few minutes.

Before

Dale does not see the speedometer pass 55

Behavior

Dale pushes the pedal

After

Dale does see the

speedometer pass 55

A

B

Correct, even though he may immediately know he will have a ticket, the outcome of actually receiving the ticket follows the response by more than 60”

Page 17: Contingency Diagramming 2

17

Did you see the use of future tense? Don’t be caught making the mistake in thinking that it passes the test because

he immediately knows he will have a ticket (he even knows it will cost

money later too). Ask yourself when the observable outcome (ticket or money) is delivered, not when he “knows” when it will be delivered.

Page 18: Contingency Diagramming 2

19

Related Outcomes Test

Is the before condition related to the after

condition?

Page 19: Contingency Diagramming 2

20

This seems like a vague concept, but a couple examples will help you understand.

Before

Teacher announces nap time

Behavior

Bob calls the teacher

a jerk

After

Bob loses some tokens

This contingency is not correct.The before conditionis not related to the after condition.

Page 20: Contingency Diagramming 2

21

While this incorrect example seems like a likely order of events, it is not a correct contingency.

Before

Teacher announces nap time

Behavior

Bob calls the teacher

a jerk

After

Bob loses some tokens

A common error students make when writing contingencies

is to simply specify three events that occur over a period of time.

Page 21: Contingency Diagramming 2

22

While this incorrect example seems like a likely order of events, it is not a correct contingency.

Before

Teacher announces nap time

Behavior

Bob calls the teacher

a jerk

After

Bob loses some tokens

Just because you’ve specified three conditions that occur in order,

it does not necessarily represent a contingency.

Page 22: Contingency Diagramming 2

23

BeforeTeacher

announces nap time

Behavior

Bob calls the teacher

a jerk

After

Bob loses some tokens

This contingency is correct.The before conditionis related to the after condition.

Bob has all of his tokens

Page 23: Contingency Diagramming 2

24

BeforeTeacher

announces nap time

Behavior

Bob calls the teacher

a jerkBob has all of his tokens

This is a sequence of conditions, but the before helps reflect the change caused by the behavior.

After

Bob loses some tokens

Page 24: Contingency Diagramming 2

25

Does this pass the related-outcomes test?A. YesB. No

Before

Mom makes liver for dinner

Behavior

Bob gives his dinner to the dog

After

Mom will not pay Bob’s allowance

Page 25: Contingency Diagramming 2

26

Does this pass the related-outcomes test?A. YesB. No

Before

Mom makes liver for dinner

Behavior

Bob gives his dinner to the dog

After

Mom will not pay Bob’s allowanceNo, the before

condition of mom making dinner is not related to the after of

Bob losing his allowance

Page 26: Contingency Diagramming 2

27

Does this pass the related-outcomes test?A. YesB. No

Before

Mom makes liver for dinner

Behavior

Bob gives his dinner to the dog

After

Mom will not pay Bob’s allowance

Right! The before condition is not related to

the after condition.

Page 27: Contingency Diagramming 2

28

Before

Mom makes liver for dinner

Behavior

Bob gives his dinner to the dog

After

Mom will not pay Bob’s allowance

This contingency is not correct.The before conditionis not related to the after

condition.

Page 28: Contingency Diagramming 2

29

BeforeMom makes

liver for dinner

Behavior

Bob gives his dinner to the dog

After

Mom will not pay Bob’s allowanceMom will pay

Bob’s allowance

This contingency is correct.The before conditionis related to the after condition.

Page 29: Contingency Diagramming 2

30

BeforeMom makes

liver fordinner

Behavior

Bob gives his dinner to the dog

After

Mom will not pay Bob’s allowance

Mom will pay Bob’s

allowance

Now it is clear Bobby would have received his allowance, if he hadn’t fed his dinner to the dog.

Page 30: Contingency Diagramming 2

31

When we talk about the outcomes of a behavioral contingency being related,

we mean that the before condition is somehow changed by the behavior,

resulting in the after condition.

The behavior causes the after condition (the consequences)

and the before condition can typically, but loosely,

be considered the opposite of that after condition.

Page 31: Contingency Diagramming 2

32

The before condition is the way things would have remained

had the particular behavior not occurred. It is not simply anything that occurred

right before the behavior; it must somehow be related to the after

condition.

Page 32: Contingency Diagramming 2

33

Here’s another illustration.

Bobby Brat wants some of Nice Norman’s candy,

so he say’s “give me your lunch.”

Page 33: Contingency Diagramming 2

34

When Norman refuses, Bobby immediately picks up Norman’s lunch box

and throws it.

Page 34: Contingency Diagramming 2

35

Norman cries, Norman cries, and Bobby picks and Bobby picks up all the up all the candy.candy.

Page 35: Contingency Diagramming 2

36

Does this pass the related-outcomes test? Are the before and after condition opposites?

A. YesB. No

Before

Bobby wants

Norman’s candy

Behavior

Bobby says, “give me

your lunch.”

After

Bobby has candy

Page 36: Contingency Diagramming 2

37

Does this pass the related-outcomes test? Are the before and after condition opposites?

A. YesB. No

Before

Bobby wants

Norman’s candy

Behavior

Bobby says, “give me

your lunch.”

After

Bobby eats Norman’s

candyWanting candy is not the opposite of having candy. Not wanting could be the opposite of wanting (but we make little use

of the term wants as you’ll see later in the course).

Page 37: Contingency Diagramming 2

38

Does this pass the related-outcomes test? Are the before and after condition opposites?

A. YesB. No

Before

Bobby wants

Norman’s candy

Behavior

Bobby says, “give me

your lunch.”

After

Bobby has candy

Right, wanting candy is not the opposite of having candy. Not wanting could be the opposite

of wanting (but we make little use of the term wants as you’ll see

later in the course).

Page 38: Contingency Diagramming 2

39

Before

?

A. Bobby demands candyB. Bobby has no candyC. Norman refuses to give candy

Behavior

Bobby throws the lunchbox

After

Bobby has candy

Click me if you want to see the Pink

Sheet

Page 39: Contingency Diagramming 2

40

2. Related Outcomes TestIs the before condition related tothe after condition? (Often therelationship is one of opposites.) If not, change one or both of theconditions.

Click me to go back to the Question

Click me to go back to the Question

Page 40: Contingency Diagramming 2

41

Before

?

Behavior

Bobby throws the lunchbox

After

Bobby has candy

A. Bobby demands candyB. Bobby has no candyC. Norman refuses to give candy

No, remember that the before

and after conditions are

usually opposites.

Page 41: Contingency Diagramming 2

42

Before

?

A. Bobby demands candyB. Bobby has no candyC. Norman refuses to give candy

Behavior

Bobby throws the lunchbox

After

Bobby has candy

No, remember that the before

and after conditions are

usually opposites.

Page 42: Contingency Diagramming 2

43

Before

Bobby has no candy

Behavior

Bobby throws the lunchbox

After

Bobby has candy

A. Bobby demands candyB. Bobby has no candyC. Norman refuses to give candy

Yes, has no candy reflects the change to the after condition. And, Bobby

demands candy is not the opposite of Bobby has

candy.

Page 43: Contingency Diagramming 2

44

Response-Unit Test

Are there any interruptions of

greater than 60 seconds during theresponse?

Page 44: Contingency Diagramming 2

45

Jaci has an entire research paper to write

and she finishes the entire paper in three days

of hard work.

Page 45: Contingency Diagramming 2

46

What do you think of this contingency?

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

Page 46: Contingency Diagramming 2

47

Do you think Jaci (or anyone else) could write for 3 days without any interruptions greater than 60 seconds?

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

A. YesB. No

Page 47: Contingency Diagramming 2

48

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

A. YesB. No

Do you think Jaci (or anyone else) could write for 3 days without any interruptions greater than 60 seconds?

No, even though we may say someone writes for three days

does that really mean they continually wrote without any interruptions?—Of course not.

Page 48: Contingency Diagramming 2

49

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

A. YesB. No

Do you think Jaci (or anyone else) could write for 3 days without any interruptions greater than 60 seconds?

Correct!, even though we may say someone writes for three

days we surely wouldn’t mean it is one continuous reinforceable

response-unit.

Page 49: Contingency Diagramming 2

50

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

While writing an entire research paper, even the best of students will have to take a

break from the glaring computer screen for at least a few minutes.

Page 50: Contingency Diagramming 2

51

And if there’s a break in writing of more than 60 seconds,

the outcome cannot reinforce that entire sequence of behavior.

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

Page 51: Contingency Diagramming 2

52

However, one day she gets a burst of energy

and begins writing for 20 minutes

without taking any breaks at all.

Page 52: Contingency Diagramming 2

53

So it might be possible to write on a research paper for 20 minutes

without any breaks.

Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire research

paper

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

Page 53: Contingency Diagramming 2

54

And the sense of accomplishment following the writing can reinforce that writing

because there were no breaks more than 60 seconds.Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire paper

on the research

paper for 20 minutes

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

Page 54: Contingency Diagramming 2

55

Even though the behavior lasts longer than 60 seconds,

it is still a reinforceable response-unit because there are no breaks greater than 60

seconds during the response.Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire paper

on the research

paper for 20 minutes

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

Page 55: Contingency Diagramming 2

56

Writing on a research paper for 20 minutes is a reinforceable response-unit

because she did not take a break lasting more than 60 seconds.Before

Jaci has no sense of

accomplishment

BehaviorJaci writes an entire paper

on the research

paper for 20 minutes

After

Jaci has a sense of

accomplishment

Page 56: Contingency Diagramming 2

57

Besides making the mistake of looking at large non-behaviors such as writing a whole paper,

some also try to analyze the repetition of behavior over time—where there are breaks.

Before

No down payment for

car

Behavior

Puts $100 in the bank each week for the

whole summer

After

Down payment for car

A reinforceable response-unit is not a series of behaviors over time—even if it leads to a reinforcing outcome

Page 57: Contingency Diagramming 2

58

Besides making the mistake of looking at large non-behaviors such as writing a whole paper,

some also try to analyze the repetition of behavior over time—were there are breaks.

Before

No down payment for

car

Behavior

Puts $100 in the bank

each week for the whole

summer

After

Down payment for car

A reinforceable response-unit is not a series of behaviors over time—even if it leads to a reinforcing outcome

Today

Page 58: Contingency Diagramming 2

59

Here is the correct contingency:

Before

$100 not in bank

Behavior

Puts $100 in the bank

today each week for the

whole summer

After

$100 in bank

Page 59: Contingency Diagramming 2

60

This test is designed to help ensure the behavior you are analyzing

is actually one reinforceable response-unit.

Page 60: Contingency Diagramming 2

61

When we talk about the reinforceable response-unit,

we are only concerned with the behavior box;

keep this in mind.

Behavior

Bob studies

Page 61: Contingency Diagramming 2

62

Often students confuse this test with the 60 second test.

When checking the behavior listed in the behavior box,

ask yourself “Are there any

interruptions of greater than 60 seconds during the response?”

Page 62: Contingency Diagramming 2

63

If there are long breaks,

you don’t have a true reinforceable response-unit,

you actually have something else,

an analog to a reinforceable response-unit

(you’ll get to that starting in chapter 22).

Page 63: Contingency Diagramming 2

64

Nice Norman is playing quietly by himself.

He takes a truck from the toy box and pushes it around the floor for a few minutes,

then he returns it to the box.

Ten minutes later, Norman is back at the toy box searching for another toy.

Page 64: Contingency Diagramming 2

65

Three times, he returns to the toy box;

once to get an airplane, another time to get a Power Ranger,

and finally he finds an engineer hat.

Norman plays with each of them with 5 to 10 minute breaks in between,

when he wonders around the room, looking for fun.

Page 65: Contingency Diagramming 2

66

The teacher sees Norman playing quietly with the hat,

and he goes up to Norman

and gives him some candy.

Page 66: Contingency Diagramming 2

67

Do you think the candy will reinforce all of Norman’s playing?

A. Yes B. No

Page 67: Contingency Diagramming 2

68

Do you think the candy will reinforce all of Norman’s playing?

A. Yes B. No

It probably wouldn’t because of the numerous long breaks he

took.

Page 68: Contingency Diagramming 2

69

Do you think the candy will reinforce all of Norman’s playing?

A. Yes B. No

Correct! It probably wouldn’t

because of the numerous long breaks he took.

Page 69: Contingency Diagramming 2

70

Just to make sure you are getting this concept down,

here’s another scenario to test your behavior-analytic repertoire.

Page 70: Contingency Diagramming 2

71

Jaci is craving a healthy pizza

so she makes one from scratch.

Page 71: Contingency Diagramming 2

72

She prepares the veggies and mixes the dough…

Then stops to check her email…

Page 72: Contingency Diagramming 2

73

And 15 minutes later she puts on the veggies and low-fat cheese.

Finally, with the pizza made,

she puts it in the preheated oven.

Page 73: Contingency Diagramming 2

74

BeforeJaci doesn’t

have prepared, uncooked

pizza

Is this behavior a reinforceable response-unit?

A. Correct reinforceable response-unitB. Fails reinforceable response-unit test

Behavior

Jaci makes pizza from

scratch

AfterJaci has

prepared, uncooked

pizza

Click me if you want to see the Pink

Sheet

Page 74: Contingency Diagramming 2

75

6. Response-Unit TestAre there any interruptions ofgreater than 60 seconds during theresponse? If there are then thebehavior is not a response unit.Rewrite the behavior.

Click me to go back to the Question

Page 75: Contingency Diagramming 2

76

BeforeJaci doesn’t

have prepared, uncooked

pizza

Behavior

Jaci makes pizza from

scratch

AfterJaci has

prepared, uncooked

pizza

A. Correct reinforceable response-unitB. Fails reinforceable response-unit test

No, there will be a break while she checks e-

mail.

Is this behavior a reinforceable response-unit?

Page 76: Contingency Diagramming 2

77

BeforeJaci doesn’t

have prepared, uncooked

pizza

Behavior

Jaci makes pizza from

scratch

AfterJaci has

prepared, uncooked

pizza

A. Correct reinforceable response-unitB. Fails reinforceable response-unit test

Right on. It fails the response-unit test

because she takes a break to check her e-

mail.

Is this behavior a reinforceable response-unit?

Page 77: Contingency Diagramming 2

78

Before

Jaci doesn’t see the pizza

cooking

How about this behavior?

A. Correct reinforceable response-unitB. Fails reinforceable response-unit test

Behavior

Jaci puts pizza in the

oven

After

Jaci sees the pizza

cooking

Click me if you want to see the Pink

Sheet

Page 78: Contingency Diagramming 2

79

6. Response-Unit TestAre there any interruptions ofgreater than 60 seconds during theresponse? If there are then thebehavior is not a response unit.Rewrite the behavior.

Click me to go back to the Question

Click me to go back to the Question

Page 79: Contingency Diagramming 2

80

Before

Jaci doesn’t see the pizza

cooking

How about this behavior?

Behavior

Jaci puts pizza in the

oven

After

Jaci sees the pizza

cooking

A. Correct reinforceable response-unitB. Fails reinforceable response-unit test

No, Jaci puts the pizza in the

oven without a break.

Page 80: Contingency Diagramming 2

81

Before

Jaci doesn’t see the pizza

cooking

How about this behavior?

Behavior

Jaci puts pizza in the

oven

After

Jaci sees the pizza

cooking

A. Correct reinforceable response-unitB. Fails reinforceable response-unit test

You’re right. Putting the pizza in the oven is a single response without breaks.

Page 81: Contingency Diagramming 2

82

Let’s try and clarify the difference between the 60 second test and the reinforceable response unit

test.

Page 82: Contingency Diagramming 2

83

Jaci is a high-paid administrator in a residential facility

for the developmentally disabled population.

It’s Monday and she goes to work for a full day,

which includes a lunch break.

Page 83: Contingency Diagramming 2

84

She stays after work to conduct a one hour training workshop

for which she will receive extra money

on her paycheck, as long as she hands in proof of completion (this takes no time at all—no breaks).

Friday comes and she picks up her pay- check.

Page 84: Contingency Diagramming 2

85

She then treats herself to an evening of shopping for new clothes

before going home to sip some warm Earl Gray tea.

Page 85: Contingency Diagramming 2

86

Before

Jaci will have

regular pay check on

Friday

Behavior

Jaci hands in proof of

completing workshop

After

Jaci will have extra money on Friday’s

check

This contingency is a common student error: It fails the 60 second test, because the outcome follows the response by more than 60 seconds.

Page 86: Contingency Diagramming 2

88

Before

Jaci does not see her time card

with 8 hours

Behavior

Jaci works all day Monday

AfterImmediately

after, Jaci sees her time card

with 8 hoursClick me if you want

to see the Pink Sheet

This contingency passes the 60 second test, since the outcome does not follow the response by more than 60 seconds.

However, it fails the reinforceable response unit test. There are breaks of greater than 60 seconds during the response.

Page 87: Contingency Diagramming 2

90

Before

Jaci has no tea

Behavior

Jaci makes tea

After

Jaci has tea

Usually, just by looking at the description of the behavior in the behavior box, you can’t be sure if it passes the reinforceable response-unit test.

Page 88: Contingency Diagramming 2

91

Before

Jaci has no tea

Behavior

Jaci makes tea

After

Jaci has tea

Many words used to describe behavior, by themselves, cannot show if there were interruptions of greater than 60 seconds. You need to look hard at the example or behavior itself to see if there are interruptions of greater than 60 seconds.

Page 89: Contingency Diagramming 2

92

In the next example choose the scenario that describes the behavior

that passes the reinforceable response-unit test.

Page 90: Contingency Diagramming 2

93

Before

Jaci has no tea

Behavior

Jaci makes tea

After

Jaci has tea

Scenario #1: Jaci takes the hot water, pours it into a cup, and adds a tea bag.

Scenario #2: Jaci puts water on the stove to boil. She comes back later and takes the hot water, pours it into a cup, and adds a tea bag. Left-click the scenario that passes the

reinforceable response-unit test.

Page 91: Contingency Diagramming 2

94

Before

Jaci has no tea

Behavior

Jaci makes tea

After

Jaci has tea

Scenario #1: Jaci takes the hot water, pours it into a cup, and adds a tea bag.

Scenario #2: Jaci puts water on the stove to boil. She comes back later and takes the hot water, pours it into a cup, and adds a tea bag. There’s a 60 second break between putting the water on

the stove and combining the hot water with the tea bag.

Page 92: Contingency Diagramming 2

95

Before

Jaci has no tea

Behavior

Jaci makes tea

After

Jaci has tea

Scenario #1: Jaci takes the hot water, pours it into a cup, and adds a tea bag.

Scenario #2: Jaci puts water on the stove to boil. She comes back later and takes the hot water, pours it into a cup, and adds a tea bag. Yes, there’s no break in scenario #1, but

scenario #2 involves a break.

Page 93: Contingency Diagramming 2

96

Causality TestIs the outcome caused by

the response?

Page 94: Contingency Diagramming 2

97

Before

Mom is not home

Behavior

Bob plays the golf game

After

Mom is home

Jimmy loves to play golf on his computer. He plays it so much, his Mom often comes home to see him playing in the middle of a game.

Page 95: Contingency Diagramming 2

98

Before

Mom is not home

Behavior

Bob plays the golf game

After

Mom is home

Jimmy loves to play golf on his computer. He plays it so much, his Mom often comes home to see him playing in the middle of a game.

A. YesB. No

Does Bob’s playing golf cause or produce the outcome, Mom is home?

Page 96: Contingency Diagramming 2

99

Before

Mom is not home

Behavior

Bob plays the golf game

After

Mom is home

Jimmy loves to play golf on his computer. He plays it so much, his Mom often comes home to see him playing in the middle of a game.

A. YesB. No

Does Bob’s playing golf cause or produce the outcome, Mom is home?

Mom comes home because her workday is over or any number of reasons. But Jimmy’s playing golf on the computer does not cause Mom to come home in a normal

situation.

Page 97: Contingency Diagramming 2

100

Before

Mom is not home

Behavior

Bob plays the golf game

After

Mom is home

Jimmy loves to play golf on his computer. He plays it so much, his Mom often comes home to see him playing in the middle of a game.

A. YesB. No

Does Bob’s playing golf cause or produce the outcome, Mom is home?

Yes, of course Jimmy’s playing golf on the computer does not cause Mom to come home in a normal situation. She just

comes home because work is over.

Page 98: Contingency Diagramming 2

101

Before

Mom is not home

Behavior

Bob plays the golf game

After

Mom is home

The causality test only tests whether the behavior caused, produced or prevented the outcome in some way.The causality test does not test whether the before

caused the behavior. So just look between the Behavior Box and After Box to check causality.

Page 99: Contingency Diagramming 2

102

Before

No sight of cool ball-in-

flight

Behavior

Bob uses the

controller to swing the

club

After

Cool sight of ball-in-flight

Here’s another example.

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails related-outcomes testC. Fails causality test

Page 100: Contingency Diagramming 2

103

Before

No sight of cool ball-in-

flight

Behavior

Bob uses the

controller to swing the

club

After

Cool sight of ball-in-flight

Here’s another example.

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails related-outcomes testC. Fails causality test

Sorry, the sight of the ball-in-flight is the opposite of no sight of the ball in flight, so the before

and after are related.

Page 101: Contingency Diagramming 2

104

Before

No sight of cool ball-in-

flight

Behavior

Bob uses the

controller to swing the

club

After

Cool sight of ball-in-flight

Here’s another example.

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails related-outcomes testC. Fails causality test

Sorry, Bob using the control to swing results in the

immediate sight of the ball-in-flight.

Page 102: Contingency Diagramming 2

105

Before

No sight of cool ball-in-

flight

Behavior

Bob uses the

controller to swing the

club

After

Cool sight of ball-in-flight

Here’s another example.

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails related-outcomes testC. Fails causality test

Yes, this is correct. Notice one difference between the these two

tests—the causality test is concerned with the Behavior and

After condition…

Page 103: Contingency Diagramming 2

106

The causality test may take a little practice.

We need to have the behavior related to the outcome,

either by causing it or preventing it.

Page 104: Contingency Diagramming 2

107

Before

Bob is losing at

the roulette wheel

Behavior

Bob changes his

bet

After

Paul wins at the slot

machines

Now, we can all see that Bob’s betting did not cause Paul to win at the slot machines.

Sometimes, however, the distinction is not so obvious, and the result is superstitious behavior –

Behavior that is accidentally reinforced by coincidental outcomes.

Page 105: Contingency Diagramming 2

108

Before

Bob is losing at

the roulette wheel

Behavior

Bob crosses his fingers

After

Bob wins the next spin

In this instance, it is likely that Bob will cross his fingers more often in the future

because winning has immediately followed it in the past,

but we still don’t have a behavioral contingency

because Bob’s winning is not contingent on (it’s not caused by) his finger crossing.

Page 106: Contingency Diagramming 2

109

Before

Bob has a low video

game score

Behavior

Bob aims carefully

After

Bob has a high video game score

There you go – causality. Bob’s carefully aiming causes him to have a high video game score.

Page 107: Contingency Diagramming 2

110

Paul’s softball teammates get annoyed when he’s up to bat

because he has an elaborate series of movements he goes through every time.

Paul says this ritual brings him good luck,

but it really doesn’t.

Page 108: Contingency Diagramming 2

111

Before

Paul has no home run

BehaviorPaul looks at the ball

as he swings hard in its path

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails action test

Page 109: Contingency Diagramming 2

112

Before

Paul has no home run

BehaviorPaul looks at the ball

as he swings hard in its path

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails action test

No, looking at the ball and swinging hard are

the important behaviors that cause

the home run.

Page 110: Contingency Diagramming 2

113

Before

Paul has no home run

BehaviorPaul looks at the ball

as he swings hard in its path

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails action test

Paul receives the sight of a home

run.

Page 111: Contingency Diagramming 2

114

Before

Paul has no home run

BehaviorPaul looks at the ball

as he swings hard in its path

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails action test

Looking and swinging are

active.

Page 112: Contingency Diagramming 2

115

Before

Paul has no home run

BehaviorPaul looks at the ball

as he swings hard in its path

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails action test

Yes, looking at the ball and swinging hard are

the important behaviors that cause

the home run.

Page 113: Contingency Diagramming 2

116

Before

Paul has no home run

Behavior

Paul performs his ritual

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails related-outcomes test

Click me if you want to see the Pink

Sheet

Page 114: Contingency Diagramming 2

118

Before

Paul has no home run

Behavior

Paul performs his ritual

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails related-outcomes test

Strike. No, you should check the Pink Sheet.

Click me to see the Pink Sheet

Page 115: Contingency Diagramming 2

119

Before

Paul has no home run

Behavior

Paul performs his ritual

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails related-outcomes test

Strike. No, Paul receives the home

run.

Page 116: Contingency Diagramming 2

120

Before

Paul has no home run

Behavior

Paul performs his ritual

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails related-outcomes test

Strike. Although getting a home run is unrelated to his ritual, the related outcomes

test refers to the relation between the before and after

conditions.

Page 117: Contingency Diagramming 2

121

Before

Paul has no home run

Behavior

Paul performs his ritual

After

Paul gets a home run

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails causality testC. Fails receiver testD. Fails related-outcomes test

Correct. It passes the receiver test because

he receives the sight of his hit go over the

fence. And it passes the related-outcomes

test because no homers is related to

homers.

Page 118: Contingency Diagramming 2

122

Warning:Don’t be a casualty of

mispronouncing causality.

It’s easy to misread causality as casualty, but then the “casualty”

criterion doesn’t make a heck of a lot of sense.

Page 119: Contingency Diagramming 2

123

Causality Test

Stimulus Test

Related Outcomes Test

60” Test

Response-Unit Test

Table of Contents.

Page 120: Contingency Diagramming 2

124

Stimulus TestIs the before and after a

stimulus, event or condition?

Page 121: Contingency Diagramming 2

125

This contingency is not correct.

Before

Bob is having

trouble with his

assignment

Behavior

Bob asks for help

After

Bob does his assignment

The before and after conditions cannot be behavior of the person we are analyzing.

Page 122: Contingency Diagramming 2

126

This contingency is correct.

Before

Bob is having

trouble with his

assignment

Behavior

Bob asks for help

AfterBob is not

having trouble with

his assignment

The after condition is now a stimulus, event or condition for the person we are analyzing, not a behavior of that person.

Page 123: Contingency Diagramming 2

127

However, the before and after conditions can be behavior of someone else;

which makes it a stimulus, event or condition to the behaver.Before

Teacher doesn’t help

Bob

Behavior

Bob asks for help

After

Teacher helps Bob

Page 124: Contingency Diagramming 2

128

Here’s the same contingency written a little differently:

BeforeTeacher

doesn’t help Bob

Behavior

Bob asks for help

AfterTeacher helps

Bob

Remember, receives is not behavior, so it’s fine in the before and after boxes.

Bob receives no help from the teacher

Bob receiveshelp from

the teacher

Page 125: Contingency Diagramming 2

129A. YesB. No

Do the before or after conditions circled above have a behavior of the behaver?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy (Hint: Jimmy’s the behaver).

Page 126: Contingency Diagramming 2

130A. YesB. No

Do the before or after conditions above have a behavior of the behaver?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

Sorry, says is behavior of the person we are

analyzing, right?

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 127: Contingency Diagramming 2

131A. YesB. No

Do the before or after conditions above have a behavior of the behaver? (Jimmy)

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

Correct, says is Jimmy’s

behavior.

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 128: Contingency Diagramming 2

132A. YesB. No

So then does this pass the stimulus test?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Click me to see the stimulus test

Page 129: Contingency Diagramming 2

133

7. Stimulus TestIs the before & after a stimulusevent or condition, and not a behavior of the behaver? If it isn’t then change the stimulus. (Hint: If the outcome is another behavior of the behaver, you probably don’t have a correct outcome. Exceptions: self-reinforcement, seeing, hearing, orthe opportunity for activity.)

Click me to go back to the

question

Page 130: Contingency Diagramming 2

134A. YesB. No

So then does this pass the stimulus test?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

againBecause the before and after condition

have Jimmy’s behavior, it fails

the stimulus test.

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 131: Contingency Diagramming 2

135A. YesB. No

So then does this pass the stimulus test?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

againYes, because the before and after

condition have Jimmy ’s behavior, it fails the stimulus test.

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 132: Contingency Diagramming 2

136

A. Action testB. Specific behavior testC. Dead-man testD. All of the above

What other tests does this contingency fail?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 133: Contingency Diagramming 2

137

A. Action testB. Specific behavior testC. Dead-man testD. All of the above

What other tests does this contingency fail?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

againYes, there’s no action in “gets,” but what about the other tests?

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 134: Contingency Diagramming 2

138

A. Action testB. Specific behavior testC. Dead-man testD. All of the above

What other tests does this contingency fail?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

Yes, “gets” is not a concrete behavior,

but what about the other tests?

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 135: Contingency Diagramming 2

139

A. Action testB. Specific behavior testC. Dead-man testD. All of the above

What other tests does this contingency fail?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

Yes, a dead man can “get,” but

what about the other tests?

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 136: Contingency Diagramming 2

140

A. Action testB. Specific behavior testC. Dead-man testD. All of the above

What other tests does this contingency fail?

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

Yes, “gets” fails each of these tests because it’s really the same as

“receives,” which involves nothing on Jimmy’s part—it describes no muscular, glandular or electrical

activity.

When Jimmy says Daddy correctly, Dad gives Jimmy a big smile. We want to analyze Jimmy’s saying Daddy.

Page 137: Contingency Diagramming 2

141

This example is kind of complex, let’s start with the before condition.

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Behavior

Jimmy gets a smile

from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

Jimmy’s is the behaver, so his behavior should be written in the behavior box, not the before condition, nor the after.

Page 138: Contingency Diagramming 2

142

This example is kind of complex, let’s start with the before condition.

Before

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

BehaviorJimmy gets a

smile from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

againJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

So let’s move the behavior hereJimmy’s is the behaver, so his behavior should be written in the behavior box, not the before condition.

Page 139: Contingency Diagramming 2

143

BeforeJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

BehaviorJimmy gets a

smile from Dad

AfterJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

again

The after condition also has Jimmy’s behavior, which fails the stimulus test.

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Page 140: Contingency Diagramming 2

144

BeforeJimmy says “Daddy” correctly

BehaviorJimmy gets a

smile from Dad

AfterJimmy says

“Daddy” correctly again

To find out what to write in the after box, you need to ask What happened immediately after Jimmy said “Daddy”?

Jimmy says “Daddy” correctly

Jimmy has a smile

from Dad

Jimmy has no smile from Dad

And immediately before, the opposite was the no-smile condition.

Page 141: Contingency Diagramming 2

145

Remember the problem about Tommy and his mom at the donut shop?

He didn’t stop screeching until he had a donut in his hands.

Page 142: Contingency Diagramming 2

146

.

Analyze the next contingency with respect to Tommy’s behavior.

Page 143: Contingency Diagramming 2

147

Before

Tommy is pointing to the donut

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails dead-man testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

Behavior

Tommy screeches

After

Tommy isn’t pointing to the donut

Click me if you want to see the Pink

Sheet

Even if the before or after has a passive verb, it may still be active. If the before or after condition passes the action test then the diagram fails the stimulus test. Analyze this contingency, if it fails a test which one is it?

Page 144: Contingency Diagramming 2

149

Before

Tommy is pointing to the donut

Behavior

Tommy screeches

After

Tommy isn’t pointing to the donut

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails dead-man testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

No, you should check the Pink

Sheet.

Click me if you want to see the Pink

Sheet

Page 145: Contingency Diagramming 2

150

Before

Tommy is pointing to the donut

Behavior

Tommy screeches

After

Tommy isn’t pointing to the donut

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails dead-man testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

No. Can a dead-man screech?

Page 146: Contingency Diagramming 2

151

Before

Tommy is pointing to the donut

Behavior

Tommy screeches

After

Tommy isn’t pointing to the donut

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails dead-man testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

No, Tommy’s pointing is the opposite of not

pointing.

Page 147: Contingency Diagramming 2

152

Before

Tommy is pointing to the donut

Behavior

Tommy screeches

After

Tommy isn’t point to the

donut

A. Correct contingencyB. Fails dead-man testC. Fails causality testD. Fails stimulus test

Correct. Tommy’s behavior should not be in the Before or After conditions.

Page 148: Contingency Diagramming 2

153

Before

Clothes on the floor

Behavior

Jen yells

After

Sue apologizes

A. Correct afterB. Fails causality testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test Click me if you want

to see the Pink Sheet

Remember the problem with Jennifer’s roommate throwing clothes on the floor?

Now we’ll analyze Jen’s behavior, so her behavior shouldn’t be in the before or after conditions, though Sue ’s behavior can be in the before and after conditions.

Page 149: Contingency Diagramming 2

155

A. Correct afterB. Fails causality testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

Before

Clothes on the floor

Behavior

Jen yells

After

Sue apologizes

No, take a closer look at the before

condition…

Page 150: Contingency Diagramming 2

156

A. Correct afterB. Fails causality testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

Before

Clothes on the floor

Behavior

Jen yells

After

Sue apologizes

No, Jen’s yelling causes Sue to

apologize.

Page 151: Contingency Diagramming 2

157

A. Correct afterB. Fails causality testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

Before

Clothes on the floor

Behavior

Jen yells

After

Sue apologizes

Yes. Clothes on the floor is not related to Sue

apologizes. It passes the causality test because

Sue apologizing is contingent (caused by) Jen yelling. And since

Jen is the behaver, we’re cool with the

stimulus test.

Page 152: Contingency Diagramming 2

158

A. Correct afterB. Fails causality testC. Fails related-outcomes testD. Fails stimulus test

Before

Clothes on the floor

Behavior

Jen yells

After

Sue apologizes

Even though the condition is a behavior, it’s fine as long as it’s not a behavior of the

person we are analyzing.

Page 153: Contingency Diagramming 2

159

You’re finished. So now you can relax

If you want to review any of the tests you can

continue to the next slide and choose one. Or

you can hit “Esc” to finish.

Page 154: Contingency Diagramming 2

160

Causality Test

Stimulus Test

Related Outcomes Test

60” Test

Response-Unit Test

You’re finished. If you’d like to review any of the tests, just click on the test name below.