Transcript
Page 1: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGBESIN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL*University of Murcia, Spain

ABSTRACT

This paper begins with a revisión of the learning strategy literature andhighlights the overlapping which currently characterizes the field as regards is-sues considered criterial for their definition. The role of consciousness in learn-ing strategy use is discussed and the terms process, strategy and technique aredefined in the light of a classroom-based empirical study carried out with chil-dren aged between eight and ten. From verbal report data gathered individuallyand in groups, a classification scheme is presented of children's metacognitive,socio-affective and cognitive strategies and techniques.

RESUMEN

En este artículo se hace una revisión de la literatura sobre las estrategiasde aprendizaje, destacando el alto grado de superposición en cuanto a las ca-racterísticas que las definen. Analizamos el papel de la consciencia en el usode estrategias y ofrecemos una definición de proceso, estrategia y técnica, ba-sada en un estudio empírico llevado a cabo con niños de ocho a diez añosen el marco formal de aula. Se incluye una clasificación detallada de estrategiasy técnicas metacognitivas, socioafectivas y cognitivas, identificadas en los niñosmediante el uso de entrevistas individuales y en grupo.

RESUME

Cet article est une revisión de tout ce qui a été écrit jusqu'á présent ápropos des strategies d'apprentissage; il nous fait remarquer á quel point lescaractéristiques qui définissent ees strategies peuvent se superposer. Il analyse

• Yvette Coyle and Marisol Valcárcel currently lecture in English Studies andTeaching English to Young Learners at the University of Murcia, Faculty of Education.Yvette gained her Ph.D. in Education in 1998 from the University of Murcia where sheworks as an associate professor. Marisol holds a Ph.D. in English Philology which sheobtained in 1986 and is a professor in the same department. Their research interestsinclude teaching English at pre-school and primary level and language acquisition inchildren. They have published several books and articles in these áreas.

423CAUCE, Revista de Filología y su Didáctica, ti" 25, 2002 / págs. 423-458

Page 2: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

également le role de la conscience dans l'usage de stratégies et propose unedéfinition de processus, de stratégies et de techniques, basée sur une étudeempirique menee auprés d'enfants de 8 á 10 ans dans le cadre formel de laclasse. II offre aussi une classification détaillée de stratégies et de techniquesmétacognitives, socio-affectives et cognitives, détectées chez les enfants á tra-vers des entretiens individuéis et en groupe.

1. INTRODUCTION

Despite the profusión of rich and varied research on the role ofLearning Stratégies (LS) in second language acquisition (SLA), today,some twenty years on from the first attempts at exploring how learnersgo about the task of learning a second language, researchers are stillstruggling to agree upon a universally accepted définition for the con-cept of strategy. In 1991 Seliger complained that the indiscriminate useof the word strategy in SLA literature had brought us to a point of'semantic satiatiorí in which the term had become devoid of any realmeaning (1991, p- 36). Less pessimistically, but in the same vein, Ellis(1994) referred to the concept of 'strategy' as 'fuzzy ... and not easy totie dowrí (1994, p. 529).

The confusión surrounding the définition of 'strategy' arises from theresearch literature itself where the term has often been substituted for sy-nonyms which have blurred the inherent meaning of the word by equat-ing it with other similar concepts. Wenden (1987) has pointed out themulti-purpose use of the term to refer to all of the following: techni-ques, tactics, potentially conscious plans, consciously employed operations,problem-solving procedures, etc. (1987, p. 7). In fact a closer look at thedefinitions of Learning Stratégies offered by different researchers indicatesthe high degree of overlapping and lack of precisión in their choice ofterminology. While for Naiman et al. (1978), Stern (1983), Schmeck (1988)and Seliger (1991) stratégies are seen as general learning approaches,with the more specific learner actions receiving the ñame of techniquesor tactics, Rubin (1981) refers to general cognitive processes and specificstratégies. In her définition Wenden (1987) refers to language learning'behaviours' while O'Malley and Chamot (1990) speak about 'thoughts orbehaviours', leaving us in doubt as to whether stratégies are to be consi-dered as behavioural or mental or both. Finally Chesterfield and Chester-field's (1985) définition reflects their concern with learner interactionwhile Oxford (1990) stresses the affective side of learning.

424

Page 3: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition ofLearning Strategies, researchers have had to resort to listing what seemto be their main characteristics in an attempt to solve this conceptualproblem (Wenden, 1987; Ellis, 1994). This has clarified very little, how-ever, as the characteristics cited tend to be contradictory and vague.For example, Ellis (1994) states that 'Strategies refer to both general ap-proaches and specific actions or techniques to learn an L2' or that 'Somestrategies are behavioural while others are mental. Thus some are di-rectly observable while others are noí (1994, p. 532). Wenden (1987)lists problematicity (potential), consciousness and the directness/indirect-ness of their effect on learning as among their defining characteristics.The persistence of such ambiguity around the construct of strategy isdetrimental to the concerns of empirical research in the fields of secondand foreign language development. Researchers have recognized theneed to try to achieve some coherence across the field in terms of boththe descriptive terminology and the conceptual characteristics inherentto the construct of strategy and while attempts in this direction havebeen made (Willing, 1989; Bialystok, 1990; Oxford and Cohén, 1992)as yet no consensus has been reached.

Given the diverging opinions of individual researchers on a seriesof important, yet conflictive, issues relating to the definition of Learn-ing Strategies, it is vital that any discussion of strategies, in whatevercontext, should begin with an explicit statement of the position adoptedwith respect to these crucial conceptual and classificatory problems.The most serious points of contention in Language Learning Strategyresearch include:

1. The overlapping which occurs between terms such as process,strategy, tactic and technique.

2. The issue of consciousness as criterial to the definition of Learn-ing Strategies.

3. The distinction between Learning, Production and Communica-tion Strategies.

Each of these áreas has important implications for the nature of ourunderstanding of LS and the role they play in the language learningprocess.

Among the aims of the present study is that of proposing a frame-work for the classifieation of children's FL Learning Strategies which isboth coherent and psychologically plausible. In order to do this it isessential to provide a clear set of criteria for categorizing and distin-

425

Page 4: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

guishing between various types of strategies. To arrive at a compre-hensive characterization of LS we will begin by reviewing current theo-retical arguments on the issues highlighted above before going on tooutline our own position on these matters.

2. LEARNING STRATEGIES: PROBLEMS OF DEFINITION

One of the most glaring problems in SLA research, then, has beenthe failure to make clear distinctions between the terms 'process', 'strategy'and 'tactic' or 'technique'. These labels have been used inconsistentlyby researchers to refer both to the same and to different mechanismsof language processing. The confusión over which behaviours are tobe classified as learning strategies is indicative of the terminological andconceptual differences which characterize both theory and research inthe Learning Strategy paradigm. These differences first emerged withthe investigations of the 'Good Language Learner' in the late 1970's.This early work produced what initially appeared to be very differentinventories of Learning Strategies (Rubin, 1981; Naiman et al., 1978; Stern,1983), but which on closer inspection were seen to comprise the samebasic categories of behaviour which had been classified in differentterms. Rubin's (1981) work describes general cognitive processes andtheir underlying strategies, while Naiman et al. (1978) and Stern (1983)refer to general strategies and speciñc, observable learner techniques.However, as Skehan (1989) has shown, and as we shall discuss later,the actual behaviours described are not that unalike. The principal dif-ferences between this early research lie essentially in the terms chosento describe more general and speciñc levéis of learner behaviour.

For other researchers (O'Malley and Chamot, 1990; Oxford, 1990)strategy has been used as a general cover-term for different learnerop-erations irregardless of levéis of generality/ specificity. Their approachhas been to divide strategies into different categories or groups accord-ing to the type of behaviour they represent. Since O'Malley and Chamot(1990) are concerned primarily with strategies for learning the L2, theydescribe Metacognitive, Cognitive and Socio-affective strategies. Oxford(1990), however, aims to account for the whole repertoire of strategiesavailable for learning and using language, and so presents a multi-lev-elled classification scheme of Memory, Cognitive, Compensation, Metacog-nitive, Affective and Social strategies. Oxford's taxonomy is interesting,though at times confusing, in this respect as it includes a wide range

426

Page 5: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

of both general and specific thoughts and behaviours, all of which aretermed strategies.

More recently, however, Oxford and Cohén (1992) have argued infavour of abandoning the a priori categorization of learning strategiesinto generic groups and have opted for a classification which differen-tiates between broad bands of strategies and their related tactics. Theirdistinction, based on the difference in military usage of the words strategyand tactic, leads them to define strategy as the 'long-range art of learn-ing more easily and effectively by using major clusters of behaviours',while tactic is used to refer to the 'short-term art of using specific beha-viours or devices' (1992, p. 4). This return to the separation of languageprocessing behaviour on the general/specific dimensión would seem,superficially at least, to coincide with the same distinction posed byNaiman et al. (1978) and Stern (1983). A more detailed analysis of theirproposal, however, reveáis that what has actually happened is that theconcept of strategy has been raised to the level of what other researchers(for example Faerch and Kasper, 1980, 1983) have termed process (forexample 'forming concepts and hypotheses'; 'testing hypotheses'; 'em-bedding new material in long-term memory'), and tactics to what hasgenerally been thought of as strategy (for example 'inferencing'; askingfor clarification/verification'; 'repetitive rehearsaP, etc.).

In adopting this new emphasis on tactics to desribe what in pre-vious theory and research (including their own) had been referred toas strategy, Oxford and Cohén (1992) follow the precedent created bySeliger (1991) and Schmeck (1988). Both of these researchers have positedthe need for a theoretical distinction between higher level, general ap-proaches to learning and more specific learner activity, and both havecoincided in the choice of the terms strategy and tactic to label thesedifferences. Once again, however, we find that the actual behavioursdescribed as strategies and tactics in their accounts are the counterpartsof what to others are more accurately described as processes and strate-gies. Seliger (1991) for example talks about the strategies of 'hypothesistesting', 'simplification', and 'overgeneralization' while Schmeck (1988)cites those of 'conceptualizing', 'personalizing' and 'memorization', allof which have traditionally been thought of as more general cognitiveoperations.

This inconsistency in the use of the metalanguage involved in LearnerStrategy research is symptomatic of the vagueness of the constructsthemselves (Ellis, 1986). Researchers have called for theoretical accountsof Learning Strategies to attempt to disentangle the concepts inherent

427

Page 6: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

to this área of SLA research (Bialystok, 1983; Oxford and Cohén, 1992)but there is still much to be resolved. A priority in any study of LearningStrategies, then, must be to provide explicit definitions of the way inwhich processes, strategies and tactics or techniques are conceptualizedand understood in the research work. The position we advócate in thepresent study is that the description of children's strategies of ForeignLanguage Learning can be integrated within a theoretical framework forSLA based on cognitive accounts of language learning which describelanguage learning in terms of the acquisition of a complex skill, andhighlight the learner's active role in the process.

3. PROCKSS, STRATEGY AND TECHNIQUE IN CHILDREN'S FOREIGN LANGUAGE LEARNING

The cognitive paradigm for the explanation of SLA has emergedfrom information-processing models of learning and from develop-mental studies on the role of cognitive processes in learners, bothofwhich have sought to explain the mechanisms by which the mindacquires and stores new information. In cognitive theory learnersare said to process information and the thoughts involved in this ac-tivity are referred to as mental processes (O'Malley and Chamot, 1990).One of the fundamental principies of cognitive theory holds thatwhile all learners possess the same internal mechanisms for the ma-nipulation of incoming information (sensorial register, short-term orworking memory, long-term memory) variation in the storage capac-ity and functioning ability of these components exist across indivi-duáis. These differences are caused by the processes and strategiesthe learner activates, more or less effectively, while learning (Beltrán,1993).

Although cognitive psychologists do not coincide in either the nura-ber or ñames they attribute to these processes, there is tacit agreementas to the importance of their role in human learning. Processes are saidto act as mediators between instructional and informative input andthelearner's output to the extent that the quality and quantity of any learn-ing experience will be determined by the degree of active cognitiveprocessing the learner engages in and the effective employment ofappropriate strategies to enhance and develop these processes (Pérez,1993, in Beltrán). It is in this sense that learning strategies are describedas special ways of processing, storing, retrieving and using information(O'Malley and Chamot, 1990).

428

Page 7: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

In the present study, our use of the term process coincides withthe definitions prevalent in cognitive discussions of learning which referto the processes by which individuáis comprehend, store and retain anduse information. In the same way, and following Willis (1989, p. 139),we understand learning strategies to refer to the 'goal-directed thoughtsor actions used by the learner to transform language input into internallyavailable resources for the comprehension, retention and use of the for-eign language'. However, given the context of our study -the learningof a FL in a classroom setting; the age of the subjects involved- childrenof 8-10 years; and our ultimate pedagogical motivation, we believe itis essential to go beyond the scope of the definition cited above in ourdescription of learning strategies in an attempt to describe the precisenature of the ways in which our young learners convert the inputthey receive in the classroom context into knowledge of the foreignlanguage. For this reason, we have opted to use the construct learningtechnique to arrive at a more finely-grained analysis of the strategicbehaviour involved in FL learning. Learning techniques, then, as usedthroughout this study can be defined as 'specific, primarily task-relatedand (mostly) observable forms of language learning'. In a classroomsetting learning techniques are those manifestations of the learningprocess in action, as for example when the learner voluntarily repeatsa linguistic model or identifies content words for the understanding ofcontext-specific discourse.

The position we adopt in this respect coincides with that of Beltrán(1993) who has theorized on the relationships of technique, strategy andprocess as applied to general school-based learning. For Beltrán (1993)learning strategies are central to the process of learning, their true def-inition lying in their mediating role between the invisible, concealedprocesses of learning and specific task-related learner techniques. Strate-gies, in turn, are affected by the use of specific learner techniques. InBeltrán's model techniques are said to actívate strategies in that theyare actions linked to classroom activities, which when underway, revealthe presence of particular strategies in learners. They are distinguishablefrom strategies in that they are observable learner operations cioselylinked to the study activities suggested by the teacher in the classroomenvironment.

One of the assumptions underlying our work is that this rationale,as applied to the description of the instructional process in general, canbe adapted to provide an explanation for the learning which occurs inthe foreign language classroom. In our study of children's foreign lan-

429

Page 8: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCARCEL

guage learning strategies we acknowledge the connections outlined byBeltrán between process, strategy and technique. Strategies, as we havedefined them above, are the special thoughts or actions which indivi-duáis use to help them learn. They can take the form of mental oper-ations (inferencing) or behaviours (repetition) which act to facilítate theprocessing of information. However, our definition of techniques expandson Beltran's original definition to include learner operations which, al-though not directly observable, such as guessing meanings from theteacher's gestures or associating vocabulary with a physical action, canstill be considered as learning techniques on the grounds that: 1) theycan be inferred from detailed analysis of the learning context and theinstructional strategies used by the teacher in the presentation and prac-tice of the TL input and 2) the behaviours have been explicitly verbal-ized by the children themselves. Although not all children will neces-sarily emply ñor decribe all their learning techniques, it is assumed thatmany, especially the 'good langauge learners', will do so. Learning tech-niques, the smallest component of the student's processing mechanisms,are directed, then, at promoting the appearance in the learner of effec-tive strategies for understanding and using the FL. Their role, as conceivedfrom a cognitive perspective, has acquired much greater prominencethat that contemplated by Naiman et al. (1978) and by Stern (1983).

It is hoped that the preceding discussion has clarified the interpre-tation given in our research to the terms process, strategy and technique.The approach taken in the present study is a variation on current po-sitions in the field of SLA strategy research. While sharing many of thefeatures suggested by leading learning strategy researchers, we do notcoincide exactly with any one position. We agree, for example, withSchmeck's view that 'a learning strategy is a higher level cluster of learn-ing tactics that work together toprovide a unified learning outcome' andthat 'the term tactic refers to the specific activities of the learners' (1988,p. 171), but the interpretation we have made is different to thelong/short term distinction expressed by Oxford and Cohén (1992), andreflects our prime concern to connect the study of learning strategieswith its implications for teacher-training and practice.

4. THF. ROLE OF CONSCIOUSNESS IN DEFINITIONS OF LEARNING STRATEGIES

The idea which emerges from the previous discussion is that weconsider both learning strategies and techniques as goal-directed actions

430

Page 9: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

aimed at facilitating the acquisition, retrieval and use of knowledge. This,in fact, would seem to be the accepted view of Learning Strategy re-searchers in the fields of both cognitive psychology and SLA (Weinsteinand Mayer, 1986; Beltrán, 1993; Derry and Murphy, 1986; O'Malley andChamot, 1990; Faerch and Kasper, 1980, 1983; Ellis, 1986; Oxford andCohén, 1992). Yet discussions of language learning strategies have notalways coincided in the acceptance of other features cited by theoristsand researchers as criterial to their definition. The feature which hasbeen most conflictive in this respect is that of consciousness.

Two opposing views have emerged which differ as regards the im-portance they attach to consciousness as an essential feature of LS. Onegroup of researchers assert that LS are delibérate actions which areconsciously selected (i.e. with awareness) by the learner (Weinsteinand Mayer, 1986; McCombs, 1988; Beltrán, 1993; Cohén, 1990; Oxfordand Cohén, 1992). Oxford and Cohén (1992), for example, argüe for alearner action to be considered as strategic, learners must have somedegree of awareness that they are employing a particular strategy. Intheir opinión unconscious learning behaviours are more accurately de-scribed as automatic processes rather than strategies.

Exponents of the opposite view propose that either:

1. strategy use need not always involve concious awareness onthepart of the learner (Faerch and Kasper, 1980, 1983; Wendenand Rubin, 1987; Willing, 1989; O'Malley and Chamot, 1990;Ellis, 1896) or

2. that consciousness is not a feature of strategy use (Bialystok, 1983,1990).

Within this first group of researchers are Faerch and Kasper (1980,1983) and Wenden and Rubin (1987) who describe LS as being 'po-tentially conscious'. Faerch and Kasper (1983) recognize an indetermi-nacy about the use of consciousness as a defining criteria of strategicbehaviour. They suggest that consciousness is not constant among in-dividuáis and that some learners are more aware than others of theirmental functioning. In his discussion on LS, Ellis (1986), states thatsome strategies are conscious while others are not but gives no indica-tion as to which particular LS he is referring to in either case. Wendenand Rubin (1987) take a similar stand to that of O'Malley and Chamot(1990) who, on outlining their position from within a cognitive theoryof learning, recognize the variability inherent to the feature of con-sciousness.

431

Page 10: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

Willing's (1989) view is much less of a compromise. He states thatthe conscious/subconscious dichotomy is misleading and asserts that LScan function at both conscious and subconscious levéis of mental pro-cessing. Willing's claim is that there are 'no absolute theoretical criteriafor denying these activities to the subconscious' (1989, p. 141), arguingthat almost all mental functioning does, in fact, take place outside therealms of conscious awareness. In his opinión subconscious processesare just as capable as conscious ones of functioning in pursuit of alearning goal.

The idea that learning involves shifts between subconscious andconscious behaviour is closely related to the question of automatic andcontrolled processing (Schiffrin and Schneider, 1984, in O'Malley andChamot, 1990). In cognitive theory consciousness is closely related toattention, the control process which brings information into focal awa-reness (Schmidt, 1990). A distinction is made between learning taskswhich require high levéis of attention, controlled processing, and thosewhich do not, automatic processing. In this view skills which are lessfamiliar to the learner require greater control of processing, while moreautomatic processing is performed without awareness. For theories suchas Anderson's three-stage model of skill acquisition (Anderson, 1983,1985, in O'Malley and Chamot, 1990) learning is characterized by a shiftfrom controlled to automatic processing, through the proceduralizationof the skill. Researchers also point out that automatic processing asso-ciated with high levéis of skill learning may become conscious (con-trolled) if the learner comes across new or unfamiliar information inthe course of a learning task (Anderson, 1985, in O'Malley and Chamot;Faerch and Kasper, 1987; McLaughlin, 1983, in Willing, 1989).

Willing (1989), while accepting the distinction between automatic(subconscious) and controlled (conscious) processing, insists that therelationship between them should be viewed as a continuum rather thana question of either/or. Willing's theory is based on the idea of cognitiveunity which sees no need to differentiate between subconscious andconscious processing. He argües that

"It is true certain learning strategies, particularly those which appearto he virtually universal such as associating or simplifying, most oftenarise without deliberation and will thus he seen as operating, most typicallyon a relatively less conscious level. Other strategies, for example, such ascertain modes of inferencing, may at first he exercised consciously anddeliberately, and can later become more nearly second nature throughuse and familiarity. Still others will always continué to require larger

432

Page 11: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PR1MARY EL CLASSROOM

amounts of cognitive effort; therfore these wül continué indefinitely tobe controlled processes. However, tbe differences among these varioussorts of strategy can be seen as a matter of degree rather than of type"(1989, p. 142).

Bialystok has argued strongly against the inclusión of consciousnessas a defining criteria of strategic behaviour. She states that '...languagelearners use a number of systematic strategies for tvhich consciousnessis not a featuré (1985, in Willing, 1989, p. 141) and cites as an exam-ple the strategy of relating new vocabulary to prior knowledge to as-sist recall, which in her opinión 'need not be conscious for the learnef.Bialystok's views on consciousness stem from her attempts to developa theoretical framework which could account for the Learning and Com-munication Strategies (Ll and L2) of both adults and children (Bialystokand Ryan, 1985, in Ellis, 1994; Bialystok, 1990, 1991). Bialystok's mainline of reasoning is intended to describe strategies of Communication,but we believe her ideas deserve careful consideration in that they areexplicitly related to children's learning. Bialystok's assertion that the useof CS by adults learning a L2 is similar to the use of strategies by youngchildren when acquiring their Ll, necessarily rules out for her the im-portance attributed to the role of consciousness in other theories. Bialystokstates her point thus,

"Using consciousness as a criterion also has the rather restricting im-plícatíon that strategy use is available only to those speakers for whomconscious reflection is possible. The major group excluded by this featureis children, for whom it is usually claimed that conscious monitoring oftheir cognitive processing is not possible" (1990, p. 4).

The issue of consciousness in Learning Strategy theory and researchis clearly a complex one. Oxford and Cohén (1992) would have it thatall LS, to be considered strategic at all, must necessarily be conscious.Bialystok (1990), stating the case for children learning their first language,argües that consciousness is not criterial to definitions of LS. Willing(1989) claims much the same in his discussion of the múltiple shifts be-tween conscious and subconscious processes in language learning. Froma cognitive theory of skill acquisition O'Malley and Chamot (1990) statethat LS begin as conscious processes but can become automatic throughpractice. Faerch and Kasper (1983) and Wenden and Rubin (1987) pre-fer the term 'potentially conscious' to refer to LS. The theoretical positiontaken in the present study will depend, naturally, on the fact that thesubjeets of our research, are young primary school children. This raises

433

Page 12: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYI.E AND MARISOL VAI.CÁRCEL

the question of whether or not children's use of LS can be said to befully conscious, or subconscious or whether there is in fact an aware-ness of learning in the cognitive sense.

The research conducted so far in the field of SLA has centred al-most exclusively on adult learning and as such cannot provide answersto these questions. Before we can ascribe to any of the views expressedabove we must look to the literature on children's learning for deeperinsights into the learning process. In this sense, Miller and Ceci andHowe (in Schmidt, 1990) insist that young children learn essentiallywithout conscious awareness, suggesting that as they mature they losethe more open awareness of environment that characterizes early'incidental' learning (McLaughlin, 1990), and acquire, around the age ofthirteen, the ability to allocate their attention strategically as occurs inadult consciousness. Similarly, Nisbet and Shucksmith (1986), in a studyof children's learning in British primary schools, have argued that strategicbehaviour is largely intuitive until the age of fourteen, and determinedlargely by the teachers, either through procedures taught explicitly orthrough approaches implicit in their presentation of classroom tasks.They suggest further, that children's increasing capacity for consciousplanning and direction of their own learning begins to emerge by theage of 10. In the light of these observations, and given that the subjectsin the present study are aged between eight and nine years oíd, theposition we take here is that consciousness is not considered criterialfor our definition of learning strategies as used by the children in theirattempts to construct knowledge of the FL.

5. LANGLIAGK LEARNING ANO LANGUAGE USK: AN INTKGRATIVK VIEW

A distinction has often been made within second language learn-ing strategy research between Learning, Production and Communicationstrategies (Tarone, 1980; Faerch and Kasper, 1983). The term learningstrategies has been used to refer to the operations by which the learnerprocesses the target language (TL) input for language learning (i.e. im-proving linguistic knowledge). Distinct from these are production andcommunication strategies which are strategies of language use. Productionstrategies enable the learner to use previously acquired linguistic knowl-edge to accomplish communication goals (Ellis, 1986). Examples includesimplification or discourse planning. Communication strategies are em-ployed when the learner fails to achieve a communicative goal because

434

Page 13: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEG1ES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

of a gap in his or her lingistic knowledge. They have been classified aseither reduction strategies (eg. topic avoidance) or achievement strate-gies (eg. paraphrase, code-switching, etc.) (Faerch and Kasper, 1984).

Researchers have been divided in their interpretations of the effectof these strategies on the learning process. Some maintain that pro-duction and communication strategies cannot be considered as learn-ing strategies on the grounds that they do not contribute directly to thelearning process (Selinker, 1972; O'Malley and Chamot, 1989), that is,they are strategies motivated by the learner's desire to communicaterather than to learn. Others, however, argüe that this language learningvs. language use dichotomy is overly simplistic and difficult to sustain(Oxford and Cohén, 1992). They affirm that it is almost impossible todetermine a learner's intention in using a particular strategy and thateven if a strategy is employed for the purpose of language use, learningmay in fact take place anyw'ay (Tarone, 1981, 1983; Littlewood, 1979;Corder, 1983). This integrative view of learning, production and com-munication strategies is implicit in the work of several strategy researcherswho see communication and production strategies as complementaryto the learning process (Wong Fillmore, 1976; Stern, 1983; Chesterfieldand Chesterfield, 1985; Oxford, 1990).

In view of this background of conflicting theoretical perspectivesand conceptual confusión, a position is needed which might mediatesomehow between the more extreme positions of those who advócateeither the total separation of LS, CS and PS or those who propose theircomplete integration within broader strategy definitions. If, as we havealready suggested, strategies are to be understood as goal-directed ac-tions, then our position in this study must, necessarily, be closer to thetheoretical perspective held by those who describe strategies in termsof the goals they are said to pursue. Willing (1989) has described thegoals of LS as the 'comprehension, internalization, storage and settingup of accessingpotentialfor usable data' and that of CS as 'the success-

ful transmission and receiving of messages' (1989, p. 143), but goes onto rriake the very valid point that the realities referrecl to by these termscan and do, in fact, overlap. In his opinión 'It is of course possible toact in pursuit of two purposes at once (1989, p. 143).

From the perspective of our study this overlapping is exactly whatoccurs in an instructional setting such as that described in this researchwhere learning can be seen to take place through the active and pur-poseful use of the FL in meaningful situations. If this is so, then how jus-tifiable is it to sepárate strategies of learning from those of language use?

435

Page 14: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEI.

Such a distinction may be appropriate in naturalistic SLA or in instructio-nal settings which emphasize the formal study of language as a systemand where little communication takes place, but it is perhaps not the casein classrooms where, as Faerch and Kasper (1983) have suggested, learnersengage in communicative activities expressly for the purpose of learning.

This is the position held by Willing (1989) and the one to whichwe ascribe in this study. What we are suggesting is that, in the primaryclassroom, the goals pursued by strategies of language learning andstrategies of language use are essentially interrelated. Willing argües thesame point by describing how communicative classroom tasks in whichlearners are expected to use the FL for a particular purpose (decision-making, describing, exchanging information, etc.) involve the jointoperation of both cognitive and communication strategies for the actualperformance of the task itself and the linguistic resources needed fordoing so. In this sense he highlights the important links between theprocessing and linguistic demands of classroom activities in which lan-guage learning and language use are part of the same phenomenon.The classification of children's strategies we propose in our researchdoes not distinguish between Learning and Communication Strategies,but suggests that in the realization of classroom activities which focuson the use of the FL, all the strategies employed by the pupils will nec-essarily contribute to their FL learning.

6. STUDIES OF CIIILDRKN'S LANGUAGE LEARNING STRATF.GIF.S

With very few exceptions the majority of research work on lan-guage learning strategies has concentrated on the strategy use of youngadolescents and adults in ESL or L2 French situations. There are onlythree studies that we know of which have looked at children's use oflearning strategies, and all within an ESL context (Wong Fillmore, 1976,1979; Chesterfield and Chesterfield, 1985; Chamot and Beard El-Dinary,1999). Given the scarcity of research work with young learners thesestudies take on immense importance for their insights into children'ssecond language learning in a classroom context.

In the first of these studies Lily Wong Fillmore studied five Mexicanchildren aged between five and seven learning English at school in theUnited States. The children were each paired with a native speakingchild and their interactions were recorded for one hour every week fora period of nine months. These interactions enabled Wong Fillmore

436

Page 15: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

to highlight the cognitive and social strategies used by these childrenwhich she felt were responsible for improvements in their communicativecompetence. Wong Fillmore emphasizes the importance of the socialstrategies in this study, as the children's primary interest was in estab-lishing friendships with their classmates. Learning the language wastheir means of doing that. Linked to each social strategy is one or morecognitive strategies which reflect important differences with the researchwhich has focused on adult strategy use. Given the age of the childrenand the immersion situation of the bilingual classroom the cognitivestrategies identified here are related more to language comprehensionand use than to metacognitive awareness or the analysis of languageas a formal system. They suggest the importance of the immediate contextof speech for understanding the linguistic input, C-l Assume what peo-pie are saying is relevant to the situation at hand Metastrategy: Guess,and the significant role played by formulaic speech in language produc-tion, C-2 Get some expressions you understand and start talking. AsSkehan (1989) has pointed out underlying the Wong Fillmore study isthe theory that 'learning to talk involves talking to learn' (1989, p. 82)for communication is facilitated by the use of ready-made expressionswhich are then gradually broken down into their constituent parts, C-3Look for recurring parts in the formulas you know, a process by whichchildren infer rather than analyze the rules of the linguistic system.

Chesterfield and Chesterfield (1985) have also examined the use oflearning strategies by young children in a bilingual classroom situation.They studied both the range of strategies used by fourteen 5-6 year oídMexican-American childrenand the order in which these strategies emergedas the children's knowledge of the L2 improved. Eight of the fourteenchildren were observed on three different occasions during their firstyear at school and their strategy use recorded. This allowed the researchersto compare and contrast the learning strategies of the eight children atany one time and to analyse the variation in each of the children overa longer period. The results of this study indicated that there did seemto be a natural order for the emergence of learning strategies in youngchildren over a period of time. Chesterfield and Chesterfield reportedthat children, regardless of their L2 proficiency, employed strategies foliow-ing the same general pattern. The sequence of strategy use ranged fromprimarily receptive, independent strategies such as Repetition and Me-morization, to strategies such as Formulaic expressions and Verbal atten-tion getters which permitted children to initiate and sustain interactions.Those strategies which required greater linguistic competence, for example

437

Page 16: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VAI.CÁRGEL

Elaboration, Appeal for assistance, Request for clarification or some degreeof metacognitive awareness, Monitoring, were the last to emerge, andeven then were developed by only a few children.

In the light of these findings the 'natural order' theory can be seento have important implications for classroom teaching. Chesterfield andChesterfield feel that the child's progress in the TL would be enhancedby educational experiences constructed to foster the development ofthese learning strategies. They suggest that language teaching methodswhich generally emphasize the more elementary strategies of repetitionand memorization may be neglecting those strategies, which accordingto their study, promote greater interaction (and henee learning) amongchildren. What the Chesterfields seem to be implying is the need forinstruction to encourage children to engage in, in O'Malley's terms, moreactive mental processing.

More recently, Chamot and Beard El Dinary (1999) have carried outa study of children's learning strategies in foreign language classroomimmersion classrooms in the USA. The focus of their study was on iden-tifying the strategies that more and less effective learners use for readingand writing tasks in French, Japanese and Spanish FL classrooms. Usingthink aloud protocols, Chamot and Beard analysed the children's appre-ciations of how they approached different tasks and, in doing so, wereable to identify a hierarchically organized coding scheme of Metacogni-tive and Cognitive learning strategies. Qualitative analyses of the inter-view transcripts revealed important differences between more and lesseffective learners as regards the appropriateness of their use of learn-ing strategies for specific tasks. Effective learners were seen to be moreflexible in their strategy use and better at monitoring and inferencingthan less effective learners who tended to cling to single strategies suchas laborious decoding of individual words, seemingly unaware of itsuneffectiveness for the task in hand.

The Chamot and Beard study is important for the insights it pro-vides into the language learning processes of young children in immer-sion education. It is also relevant to our study on a number of accounts.Firstly, the coding scheme developed by the researchers is the first ofits kind to be applied to children's learning and it is significant thatmany of the learning strategies identified there have much in commonwith the findings of our research. Secondly, as regards the data collec-tion techniques used, we too share the satisfaction expressed by Chamotand Beard on the successful use of 'think aloud' procedures in children.This aspect of our study is discussed below.

438

Page 17: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

7 . STUDY PARTICIPANTS AND DATA COU.KCTION I'ROCEDURES

The study reported on in this paper formed part of a larger twoyear longitudinal research project on the teaching and learning ofEnglish as a foreign language in the primary classroom. Specifically, itfocuses on identifying and classifying the LS used by young childrenin their attempts to learn English. The present study was carried outwith a sample of eight children from the same class, identified by theirteacher to be effective language learners, during their first two years ofEFL instruction in a Spanish state primary school.

Data for the main study included classroom observations of over50 hours teaching, all of which were video recorded for transcriptionand analysis. The sub-study described here used data gathered fromthese observations as well as from retrospective and think-aloud interviews(see Table 1), which have been favoured by LS researchers for the richinformation they can provide of students' mental processing. It washoped that this would also be the case here, in spite of the age limi-tations of the subjects involved. The interviews we conducted were de-signed to access the children's reported thoughts while or immediatelyafter working on activities (storytelling, playing games, writing a com-position) they were familiar with in the classroom. The children wereencouraged to verbalize their thoughts aloud, using their Ll, and wereprompted by the interviewer to clarify and elabórate on their commentswhen necessary. All seven interviews were either video taped (1-3) oraudio taped (4-7), and transcribed for analysis.

8. DATA ANALYSIS PROCKDURES

The analysis of the observation and interview data proceeded asfollows. Three raters working together first viewed 30 hours of class-room interaction, corresponding to three complete Teaching Units,in order to identify and agree on observable behaviours (eg. spon-taneously initiating an exchange with the teacher) which wouldsuggest that children were using, albeit subconsciously, particularlearning strategies. Then, working independently, the same threeraters analysed all the interview protocols, marking each occurrenceof a potential learning technique and transferring the verbal data ontocoding sheets for each interview. Through discussion the raters com-pared and contrasted their data to reach an agreement on the nature

439

Page 18: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

Table 1.

N"

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Technique

StimulatedRecall

ImmediateRetrospection

ImmediateRetrospection

ImmediateRetrospection

Immediateretrospection

DelayedRetrospection

Evaluative

ParticipantStnicture

Smallgroup (3)

2 smallgroups (4)

4 pairs

Individual

4 pairs

Individual

Individual

Activity

Song

Oral description ofmodel house

Story

Students' writtencompositions

Board game

FL contents workedon in class

Focus

Initial exploration of children'sability to verbalize informationon processes of FL compre-hension, retention.

Strategies used in FL oral pro-duction; influence of Ll onFL production; FL rehearsal;Ss reaction to error; etc.

Strategies for retention and oralproduction; importance of Ssprevious knowledge.

Strategies for FL written pro-duction; strategies of analysis.

Oral FL production; socialstrategies.

General questions on children'sattitude and motivation towardsEFL learning

Evaluation of the children's EFLcompetence.

of the learning techniques and the appropriateness of the discursiveexamples identified, eliminating any techniques which were consideredambiguous or which had been marked by only one rater. The inter-views were then anaysed once again using the revised list of learn-ing techniques.

In order to créate a typology of interrelated actions, the learningtechniques identified from the empirical data were then grouped intothe broader category of Learning Strategies, again after previous discussionand consensus of opinión. These categories were theoretically drivenand included Metacognitive, Social/Affective and Cognitive learning strate-gies previously identified in the research literature on learning strategies.The definitions we have used in our classification of children's FL learningstrategies are presented below.

440

Page 19: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDKEN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

Metacognitive Strategies

Strategy

SELF MANAGEMENT

O'Malley et al. (1985); Chamot et al.(1987); O'Malley & Chamot (1990); Oxford(1990); Wenden (199D; Oxford & Cohén(1992); Chamot & Beard (1999)

PLANNING

O'Malley etal. (1985); Chamot etal. (1987,1988); O'Malley & Chamot (1990); Oxford(1990); Wenden (199D; Oxford and Cohén(1992); Chamot & Beard (1999)

SELF MONITORING

Naiman et al. (1978); Rubin (1981,1987); Chesterfield & Chesterfield (1985);O'Malley et al. (1985); Chamot et al.(1987, 1988); O'Malley & Chamot (1990);Ellis (1986); Oxford (1990); Wenden(1991); Chamot & Beard (1999)

Student-initiated strategic behaviour

The child's ability to regualte or supporthis own learning process by developingan understanding of the conditions orcircumstances that will promote better ormore successful learning

Deciding in advance to attend to a learningtask and ignore distractions and proposingstrategies for carrying out a language taskindividually or in groups.

The child's awareness of 'on-line' diffi-culties or problems in the performanceof a language task as well as their at-tempts to correct their own speech foraccuracy in pronunciation, grammar orvocabulary.

Social /Affective Strategies

Strategy

COOPERATING WITH OTHERS

Wong Fillmore (1976); O'Malley et al.(1985); Chamot etal. (1987, 1988); O'Malleyand Chamot (1990); Oxford (1990)

REGULATING CLASSROOM BEHAVIOUR

Cathcart (1985)

DEVELOPING A POSITIVE ATTITUDE TOWARDS THE FL

Oxford (1990)

SELF-CONFIDENCE

Naiman et al. (1978); Chesterfield andChesterfield (1985); Oxford (1990);

SHOWING INTEREST IN THE FL OUTSIDE THE

CLASSROOM

Naiman et al. (1978)

Student-initiated strategic behaviour

Helping other children to understand oruse the FL or to solve a problem inclass

Trying to control the behaviour of otherchildren by getting them to comply totheir ideas, beliefs, class rules, etc.

Expressing positive feelings towards thestudy of the FL

Using the FL confidently without fear oranxiety

Being aware of and interested in examplesof the FL outside the school environment

441

Page 20: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

CognitiveStrategy

USING PRIOR KNOWLEDGE

O'Malley etal. (1985); Chamot etal. (1987,1988); O'Malley & Chamot (1990); Oxford(1990); Oxford and Cohén (1992); Chamot& Beard (1999)

INFERENCING

Wong Fillmore (1976); Rubin (1981,1989); O'Malley et al. (1985); Chamot etal. (1987, 1988); O'Malley & Chamot(1990); Oxford (1990); Chamot & Beard(1999)

USING VISUAL, AUDITORY AND KINETIC IMAGERY

Rubin (1981, 1989); O'Malley etal. (1985);Chamot et al. (1987, 1988); O'Malley &Chamot (1990); Oxford (1990); Oxford &Cohén (1992)

REPETUION

O'Malley etal. (1985); Chamot etal. (1987,1988); O'Malley & Chamot (1990); Chester-field & Chesterfield (1985); Rubin (1989);Oxford & Cohén (1992)

LANGUAGE ANALYSIS

Wong Fillmore (1976); Rubin (1981, 1989);O'Malley et al. (1985); Chamot et al.(1987, 1988); O'Malley & Chamot (1990);Oxford (1990); Oxford & Cohén (1992);Chamot & Beard

BUILDING AUTONOMOUS DlSCOURSE

O'Malley et al. (1985); Oxford (1990)

OVERCOMING LlMITATIONS IN F L K.NOWLEDGE

Faerch and Kasper (1983); Corder(1983); Oxford and Cohén (1992); Tarone(1977); Oxford (1990); Oxford and Cohén(1992)

Strategies

Student-initiated strategic behaviour

Relating new information to previousknowledge to understand meaning in theFL

Using contextual or physical clues to makesense of the meanings being expressedin the FL

The retention and recall of specific FLvocabulary and expressions by means ofphysical, visual and gestural imagery

Voluntary on task repetition of FL voca-bulary and models and voluntary repeti-tion of FL input outside the class

The application of learned or self-dev-eloped rules to understand or producethe FL

Constructing meanings in the FL by com-bining known vocabulary and patterns ina new way.

Attempts to communicate in the FL inspite of having only limited linguisticknowledge

442

Page 21: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

9. RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

As far as we are aware, the typology of Learning Strategies pre-sented in this study is the first to have highlighted the types of thoughtsand behaviours used by children as young as eight and nine years oídfor learning English in a foreign language classroom setting. The analy-sis of the interview transcripts, coupled with the classroom observation,has revealed a variety of techniques and strategies being put into prac-tice by young EFL learners. The typology we present here includesMetacognitive and Social/Affective strategies on the one hand and Cog-nitive strategies on the other, with the latter classified according to thehigher level processes of Comprehension, Retention and Language Usewhich they work to enhance and develop. At a more specific level, moststrategy types are manifested through several different learning techni-ques. The following tables ¿Ilústrate the hierarchical grouping we havemade of Process, Strategy and Technique and include examples for eachtechnique extracted from the interview and classroom data. The numberafter each excerpt refers either to the interview or to the Teaching Unitfrom which the example was taken.

443

Page 22: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

Met

acog

niti

ve

Str

ateg

ies

Stra

tegi

es

SELF

MAN

AGEM

ENT

Tec

h ñi

ques

Dec

idin

g in

adv

ance

to

use

the

FL in

the

cla

ssro

om

Mak

ing

an e

ffor

t to

car

ry o

utcl

assr

oom

act

iviti

es w

ell

Dec

idin

g to

pay

atte

ntio

n to

the

teac

her

in c

lass

Dec

idin

g to

pay

atte

ntio

n to

oth

erch

ildre

n's

oral

pro

duct

ions

to

lear

n fr

om t

hem

Exa

mpl

es

Sil:

Si h

abla

mos

en

espa

ñol

es b

ajito

par

a qu

e no

nos

oig

a.E

loy:

Ser

ás t

ú la

que

hab

las

en e

spañ

ol.

Ent:

¿Tú

no h

abla

s en

esp

añol

nun

ca E

loy?

Elo

y: A

l pr

inci

pio

cuan

do

era

más

pe

queñ

o y

empe

zába

mos

a

dar

ingl

és s

í, pe

ro a

hora

ya

no.

Aho

ra y

a sa

bem

os b

ien

y lo

que

no e

nten

dem

os s

e lo

dec

imos

a D

on M

atía

s y

él n

os l

o ex

plic

a en

ingl

és.

(5)

Int:

¿Qué

hac

es p

ara

caer

le b

ien

a tu

pro

feso

r?El

: Y

o in

tent

o ha

cer

todo

s lo

s de

bere

sIn

t: ¿Y

en

clas

e qu

é ha

ces?

El:

Pues

in

ento

hac

erlo

de

ntro

de

lo

mej

or

posi

ble.

.. Po

rque

yo

quie

ro q

ue m

e ve

a qu

e so

y lis

to.

(6)

Sor:

Pues

yo

inte

nto,

int

ento

lo

posi

ble

para

pod

er e

nten

der

toda

sla

s pa

la...

to

das

las

pala

bras

en

ingl

és,

inte

nto

cada

vez

apr

ende

rm

ás.

JV:

(Ref

erri

ng to

the

tea

cher

) C

uand

o di

ce a

lgo

que

no h

emos

dad

oen

tonc

es p

rest

o m

ás a

tenc

ión.

(6)

Int:

¿Cre

es q

ue p

uede

s ap

rend

er d

e lo

s co

mpa

ñero

s?Y

ol:

Sí.

Int:

¿Por

qué

?Y

ol:

Porq

ue h

ay a

lgun

os q

ue s

on m

ás l

isto

s qu

e ot

ros

y lo

s qu

eso

n m

ás l

isto

s pu

es,

si y

o no

deci

r un

a fr

ase

pues

si

la d

icen

ello

s bi

en

pues

yo

, co

mo

lo h

e oí

do v

aria

s ve

ces,

pue

s sé

de-

cirla

. (6

)

n O > •z,

O 2 J8 n

Page 23: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

PLA

NN

ING

SELF

MO

NIT

OR

ING

Men

tally

reh

ears

ing

the

lang

uage

need

ed t

o pa

rtic

ípat

e in

an

up-

com

ing

activ

ity

Thi

nkin

g in

Ll

to p

repa

re w

rit-

ten/

oral

pro

duct

ion

Cor

rect

ing

your

ow

n m

ista

kes

inth

e co

mp

reh

ensi

on/p

rod

uc-

tion

/pro

nunc

iati

on o

f th

e FL

Int:

Cua

ndo

tené

is q

ue h

abla

r y

sabé

is q

ue D

on M

. va

a p

regu

ntar

a to

dos,

¿va

s en

saya

ndo

en l

a ca

beza

lo

que

vais

a e

cir

o es

perá

isa

que

os p

regu

nte

y en

tonc

es e

mpe

záis

a p

ensa

r?El

: N

o. e

stam

os p

ensa

ndo

a ve

r lo

que

vam

os a

dec

ir. Y

o, y

o pi

enso

lo q

ue v

oy a

dec

ir p

orqu

e a

lo m

ejor

est

á m

al,

lo p

iens

o y

si e

stoy

segu

ro d

e qu

e es

pue

s lo

dig

o.

Int:

Vam

os a

ver

Jos

é V

icen

te c

uand

o es

tás

escr

ibie

ndo,

esc

ribi

endo

en i

nglé

s ¿q

ué e

stás

pen

sand

o, e

n es

paño

l y

lueg

o lo

vas

pon

iend

oen

ing

lés

o es

tás

pens

ando

dir

ecta

men

te e

n in

glés

?JV

: Pi

enso

pri

mer

o un

poc

o en

esp

añol

.In

t: Pr

imer

o un

poc

o en

esp

añol

. ¿Y

lueg

o qu

é ha

ces

lo t

radu

ces?

JV:

Sí.

(4)

Int:

¿Sab

ríais

dec

ir e

n in

glés

cuá

les

han

sido

los

pro

blem

as?

San:

Dol

or d

e es

tóm

ago,

sto

mac

hach

e. L

a ca

beza

, m

m,

Hea

dach

e.JV

: /H

ill/

ill.

(3)

O SO > 50 z o A Tí t-1

50

Page 24: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

Soci

al /A

ffec

tive

Str

ateg

ies

Stra

tegi

es

CO

OPE

RA

TIN

GW

ITH

OT

HE

RS

REG

ULA

TIN

GC

LASS

RO

OM

BEH

AV

IOU

R

DE

VE

LO

PIN

GA

PO

SIT

IVE

AT

TIT

UD

ET

OW

AR

DS

LE

AR

NIN

GT

HE

FL

Tec

hniq

ues

Hel

ping

oth

er c

hild

ren

wit

h cl

ass-

room

act

ivit

ies

(com

preh

ensi

on;

read

ing;

ora

l pr

oduc

tion

)

Cor

rect

ing

othe

r ch

ildre

n's

mis

-ta

kes

(com

preh

ensi

on;

pron

un-

ciat

ion;

use

of

the

FL)

Insi

stin

g th

at o

ther

chi

ldre

n us

eFL

dur

ing

clas

sroo

m a

ctiv

itie

s

Exp

ress

ing

a pr

efer

ence

for

Eng

-lis

h ov

er o

ther

sub

ject

s

Exa

mpl

es

Sor:

(Tal

king

abo

ut a

com

posi

tion

they

hav

e w

ritt

en i

n cl

ass)

Com

o,va

rias

cosa

s no

las

ent

endí

a en

tonc

es,

mi

com

pañe

ro J

uan

Pedr

ota

mbi

én v

aria

s co

sas

no l

as e

ntie

nde,

cos

as q

ue y

o no

ent

iend

o y

él s

i la

s sa

be p

os y

o se

las

pre

gunt

o y

las

cosa

s qu

e él

no

sabe

yyo

me

las

preg

unta

y a

sí v

aria

s co

sas

que

no e

nten

dem

os e

ntre

los

dos

las

pode

mos

apr

ende

r. (

4)

Sor:

(Ref

erri

ng to

a s

ong

they

've

lear

ned

in c

lass

) y

lueg

o di

ce '

two

tiny

win

dow

s' q

ue s

on...

Son:

que

son

las

sill

as.

JP &

Sor

: (t

oget

ber)

no

son

las

vent

anas

. (1

)

(Ask

ing

and

answ

erin

g qu

esti

ons

abou

t pic

ture

s th

ey h

ave

drai

vn o

fth

eir

drea

m h

ousé

)So

r: B

ig,

no,

smal

l be

droo

m.

Four

bed

room

, be

droo

ms.

Big

or

smal

ldo

or.

Son:

Big

or

smal

l?JL

: Ju

an P

edro

¿C

uánt

as p

lant

as t

iene

tu

casa

?JP

: O

ne.

Sor:

Eng

lish,

no

Span

ish.

(U

D1)

Int:

¿Por

qué

te g

usta

el

ingl

és m

ás q

ue a

otr

as a

sign

atur

as?

Yol

: Po

r qu

e ha

ces

jueg

os,

te l

o pa

sas

bien

, ve

s ci

ntas

de

vide

o, e

nla

s ot

ras

tiene

s qu

e es

tar

siem

pre

estu

dian

do,

haci

endo

deb

eres

, ha

-ci

endo

eje

rcic

ios.

(6)

O O 2 2 en O > se n

Page 25: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

SELF

CO

NFI

DE

NC

E

SHO

WIN

GIN

TE

RE

STIN

TH

E F

LO

UT

SID

E T

HE

CLA

SSR

OO

M

Hav

ing

conf

iden

ce i

n yo

ur a

bilit

yto

und

erst

and

and

use

the

FL

Usi

ng t

he F

L i

n cl

ass

wit

hou

tbe

ing

frig

hten

ed o

f m

akin

g m

is-

take

s

Ant

icip

atin

g an

ans

wer

to

the

teac

her'

s qu

esti

ons

Usi

ng t

he F

L at

hom

e w

ith

fam

ilym

embe

rs o

r in

the

pla

ygro

und

wit

h fr

iend

s

Not

icin

g ex

ampl

es o

f th

e FL

on

the

tele

visi

ón; i

n ad

vert

isem

ents

;so

ngs,

etc

.

El:

Es b

uen

prof

esor

, po

r qu

e ah

ora

mis

mo

noso

tros

sa

bem

osm

ucha

s co

sas

que

yo s

e lo

he

preg

unta

do a

otr

os c

rios

de o

tra e

s-cu

ela

que

no l

o sa

ben

tant

o,

no e

stán

ap

rend

iend

o ta

nto

com

ono

sotr

os.

Sor:

Yo

pien

so q

ue y

o sé

muc

ho d

e in

glés

que

por

eje

mpl

o a

al-

guno

s le

s ga

no h

abla

ndo.

(6)

Int:

¿Tie

nes

mie

do a

equ

ivoc

arte

cua

ndo

habl

as e

n in

glés

?JL

: N

o, s

i m

e eq

uivo

co y

que

me

he e

quiv

ocad

o, l

o di

go b

ien.

El:

Cua

ndo

no e

stá

grab

ando

el

vide

o m

e da

igu

al q

ue m

e eq

uivo

-qu

e. (

6)

Int:

¿Te

gust

a le

vant

ar l

a m

ano

para

con

test

ar o

no?

JL:

Lev

anto

la

man

o y

empi

ezo

a de

cir

'me,

me,

me

plea

se'.

(6)

Sor:

Dig

o en

cas

a to

do l

o qu

e sé

, to

do l

o qu

e pu

edo

para

pod

eres

forz

arm

e pa

ra a

prob

ar l

a as

igna

tura

, y

tam

bién

par

a...

para

... e

nm

i ca

sa,

yo m

ucha

s ve

ces

esto

y so

la y

lue

go,

llega

n m

is p

adre

s y

todo

eso

, y

yo e

mpi

ezo

a ha

blar

en

ingl

és,

empi

ezo

a de

cirle

s lo

san

imal

es q

ue h

e ap

rend

idio

nue

vos,

em

piez

o a

cant

arle

s ca

ncio

nes,

empi

ezo

a co

ntar

les

muc

has

cosa

s y

eso

a m

is p

ad...

A m

is p

adre

sle

s da

muc

ho g

usto

, po

r qu

e m

is p

adre

s qu

iere

n qu

e yo

saq

ue l

aca

rrer

a de

... d

e pr

ofes

ora.

(6)

Int:

¿Fue

ra d

e cl

ase

te i

nter

esa

ver

cosa

s en

ing

lés?

Eloy

: Sí

por

que

mira

un

amig

o m

ío m

e de

jo u

na c

inta

que

tod

oer

a...

que

todo

era

en

ingl

és y

yo

la e

scuc

hé.

Son:

Tam

bién

cua

ndo

hay

pelíc

ulas

. Es

que

yo

teng

o un

can

al i

n-gl

és.

(6)

50 z z z 2? > jo

Page 26: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

00C

ogni

tive

St

rate

gies

CO

MPR

EH

EN

SIO

N

Stra

tegi

es

USI

NG

PRIO

R L

lK

NO

WL

ED

GE

INFE

RE

NC

ING

Tec

hniq

ues

Usi

ng p

rior

kn

owle

dge

of

th

eac

tivi

ty s

truc

ture

(ga

mes

, sto

ries

)

Usi

ng p

rior

kn

owle

dge

of

the

topi

c or

con

text

Gu

essi

ng

mea

nin

gs

from

th

ete

ache

r's

gest

ares

, mod

ulat

ed o

ut-

put

(int

onat

ion;

vol

ume;

pau

ses;

stre

ss)

Gue

ssin

g m

eani

ngs

from

pic

tu-

res,

obje

cts

blac

kboa

rd d

raw

ings

,vi

deo

sequ

ence

s

Exa

mpl

es

Int:

¿Vos

otro

s te

néis

jue

gos

de e

stos

en

casa

?So

r: Y

o sí

, te

ngo

el j

uego

de

la o

ca q

ue e

s pa

reci

do a

ést

e.JP

: Y

yo.

Int:

Y tú

tam

bién

. E

nton

ces

tú s

abes

por

las

reg

las

o po

r qu

e sa

bes

juga

r?So

r: P

orqu

e sa

bem

os j

ugar

. (5

)

Int:

(Ask

ing

how

the

pupi

l ha

d un

ders

tood

the

sto

ry T

he V

ery H

ungr

y C

a-te

rpil

lar)

¿E

sto

te l

o sa

bías

por

que

ya s

abía

s lo

que

pas

aba

a un

a or

uga?

El:

Ah

pero

eso

yo

ya l

o sa

bía.

Int:

¿Dón

de l

o ha

bías

es

tudi

ado?

El:

Pue

s es

qu

e ta

mbi

én

yo

cuan

do

es

el

tiem

po

de

los

gusa

nos,

pues

yo

cojo

gus

anos

, y

lueg

o ha

cen

el,

el c

apul

lo y

lue

go s

ale

lam

arip

osa

y y

pone

hu

evos

.In

t: ¿Y

cóm

o se

dic

e ca

pull

o en

in

glés

?El

: C

ocoo

n.

(3)

Int:

(Che

ckin

g th

eir

com

preh

ensi

on

of a

son

g th

ey h

ave

lear

ned

incl

ass)

¿Y

eso

qué

es

de

'pee

p in

side

'?JP

: P

orqu

e es

una

pal

abra

y

lueg

o di

ce q

ue

entr

e o

algo

así

.So

r: E

nton

ces

Don

M s

e po

ne a

sí (

circ

les

her

eyes

wit

h he

r ha

nds)

.JL

: El

agu

jero

y

ver

lo q

ue h

ay d

entr

o.

(1)

JP:

Don

M e

stuv

o po

nien

do e

n la

piz

arra

alg

unas

cos

as.

Sor:

Dib

ujan

do...

yo

me

acue

rdo

de m

ucha

s: s

aw,

plye

rs,

scre

wdr

iver

,ha

mm

er.

JP:

Y D

on M

nos

lo

poní

a el

dib

ujo

y lu

ego

noso

tros

ten

íam

os

que

deci

rlo.

(1

)

•< < n O > c 2 pe

Page 27: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

RE

TE

NT

ION

A

ND

R

ET

RIE

VA

L

Stra

tegi

es

VIS

UA

L,A

UD

ITO

RY

,K

INE

TIC

IMA

GER

Y

Tec

hniq

ues

Ass

ocia

ting

voc

abul

ary

and

lan-

guag

e m

odel

s w

ith

the

teac

her'

sge

stur

es

Rec

alli

ng v

ocab

ular

y an

d la

n-gu

age

mod

els

from

bla

ckbo

ard

draw

ings

and

pic

ture

s in

sto

ry-

book

s, s

lides

, vid

eo i

mag

es, e

tc.

Rec

alli

ng v

ocab

ular

y an

d la

n-gu

age

mod

els

in a

ssoc

iati

on w

ith

the

them

atic

con

text

in

wh

ich

they

wer

e fi

rst

intr

oduc

ed

Ass

ocia

ting

voc

abul

ary

and

lan-

guag

e m

odel

s w

ith

phys

ical

ac-

tion

Exa

mpl

es

JP:

Ade

más

Don

M,

cuan

do c

anta

ba l

a ca

nció

n, c

uand

o de

cía

'bui

ld'

hací

a as

í (p

rete

nds

to d

ig w

ith

a sp

adé)

. (2

)

Int:

¿Y c

ómo

os a

cord

áis

del

cuen

to,

tú q

ue l

o ha

s co

ntad

o si

n m

i-ra

rlo

cóm

o te

acu

erda

s de

tod

o?So

r:

Pue

s yo

m

e ac

uerd

o p

orq

ue

lo

vim

os

dos

vece

s yo

lo

vi

dos

vece

s en

el

vid

eo y

Don

M

a v

eces

nos

po

ne,

com

o a

vece

sno

no

s ac

orda

mos

bi

en

de

las

cosa

s no

s po

ne

el v

ideo

y

lo r

e-co

rdam

os

y lu

ego

yo

me

acue

rdo,

po

rque

cu

ando

yo

do

y al

gode

in

glés

se

me

apre

nde,

y

se m

e m

ete

en

la c

abez

a y

no

se

me

olvi

da.

(3)

Int:

(Ask

ing

abou

t vo

cabu

lary

) A

ver

JV

, ¿t

ú de

cu

áles

te

ac

uerd

asm

ejor

?JV

: D

e la

s qu

e sa

len

en e

l cu

ento

y a

lgun

a ot

ra.

(3)

El:

Otr

a co

sa

que

nos

ayud

ó a

reco

rdar

er

a cu

ando

D

on M

. co

gía

un n

ombr

e si

lo d

ecía

s y

lo a

cert

abas

lue

go l

o po

nías

en

el p

a-pe

l, es

o ta

mbi

én

nos

ayud

ó, a

yudó

más

. (3

)

n O i z w •o 5 C/J so O O S

Page 28: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

Cog

nitiv

e St

rate

gies

(co

nt.)

RE

TE

NT

ION

A

ND

R

ET

RIE

VA

L

Stra

tegi

es

RE

PET

ITIO

N

Tec

hniq

ues

Vol

unta

ry in

-cla

ss r

epet

itio

n of

vo

cabu

lary

, la

ngua

ge m

odel

s pr

e-se

nted

by

the

teac

her

Play

fully

rep

eati

ng t

o on

esse

lf o

rw

ith

frie

nds

outs

ide

clas

s (n

eww

ords

, num

bers

, so

ngs,

etc

.)

Re-

read

ing

clas

s ex

erci

se

book

to r

emem

ber

new

voc

abul

ary

Vol

unta

rily

rep

eatin

g th

e te

ache

r's

corr

ecti

ve f

eedb

ack

Exa

mpl

es

T:

Fm g

oing

to

buil

d a

hous

eSs

: w

ith

a sl

opin

g ro

ofT

: w

ith

a ch

imne

y ta

llSs

: w

ith

a ch

imne

y ta

llT

: sl

opin

g ro

ofSs

: sl

opin

g ro

ofT

: ok

? (T

U1)

JL:

yo m

ucha

s ve

ces

yo v

oy p

ensa

ndo

en l

os n

úmer

os,

cuen

to,

mir

a(s

e le

vant

a)

voy

haci

endo

pe

nsan

do,

one

two

thre

e fo

ur

five

, vo

yca

ntán

dolo

s.

(1)

Int:

¿Qué

est

udia

s, l

os f

ines

de

se

man

a?So

r: S

í y

a ve

ces

com

o m

e lo

lle

vo (

refe

rrin

g to

her

exe

rcis

e bo

ok)

por

las

tard

es c

uand

o es

toy

sola

pos

me

pong

o a

estu

diar

y a

sí n

oes

toy

abur

rida

.In

t: Y

cua

ndo

dice

s qu

e es

tudi

as ¿

qué

hace

s, v

es l

os d

ibuj

os o

lee

s o.

..?S:

No,

yo

al p

rinc

ipio

veo

los

dib

ujos

y

así

lo r

ecue

rdo

lo q

ue e

s y

lueg

o m

e po

ngo

a le

er y

ya

sé l

o qu

e es

.In

t: ¿Y

esc

ribe

s ta

mbi

én

o no

?S:

Cua

ndo,

no

, cu

ando

, co

mo

segu

ram

ente

m

ás

adel

ante

ha

rem

osdi

ctad

os

de

ingl

és

ento

nces

, yo

m

ucha

s ve

ces

cojo

el

láp

iz

y un

aho

ja,

o m

e po

ngo

el

libr

o y

empi

ezo

a di

ctar

do

s ho

jas

y lu

ego,

cuan

do,

desp

ués

de e

so.

me

pong

o a

leer

lo y

ya

me

lo s

é. (

3)

T:

For

exam

ple

if I

wan

t to

buy

sta

mps

to

send

let

ters

, w

here

do

I go

?JL

: po

st

offi

ceT

: I

go t

o th

e po

st

offi

ceJL

: I

go t

o th

e po

st o

ffic

e (T

U3)

8 > n

Page 29: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

USE

OF

LA

NG

UA

GE

F

OR

O

RA

L/W

RIT

TE

N

CO

MM

UN

ICA

TIO

N

Stra

tegi

es

LAN

GU

AG

EA

NA

LYSI

S

BU

ILD

ING

AU

TO

NO

MO

US

DIS

CO

UR

SE

Tec

h ñi

ques

Indu

cing

pho

nolo

gica

l rul

es fr

omth

e la

ngua

ge i

nput

App

lyin

g le

arne

d ru

les

to u

nder

-st

and/

prod

uce

the

FL

Usi

ng u

nana

lyse

d fo

rmul

as

Spon

tane

ousl

y in

itia

ting

an

ex-

chan

ge w

ith

the

teac

her

or a

n-ot

her

pupi

l in

the

FL

Inco

rpor

atin

g th

e te

ache

r's

feed

-ba

ck i

n or

al /

wri

tten

pro

duct

ion

Exa

mpl

es

Int:

Vos

otro

s de

cís

'pee

p' s

iem

pre

y es

o lo

veo

muy

rar

o.JL

: P

ero

en i

nglé

s se

esc

ribe

de

una

man

era

y lu

esgo

se

dice

de

otra

man

era.

Sor:

Si

las

cort

icas

más

o m

enos

se

dice

igu

ales

per

o la

s m

ás g

rand

esse

pro

nunc

ian

dist

into

. (1

)

Int:

¿Hab

éis

oído

a

Don

M

dec

ir

'ate

', 'a

te?

¿ Y

sab

éis

cuál

es

la

dife

renc

ia

entr

e 'e

at'

y 'a

te'?

JV:

Que

'a

te'

es q

ue c

omió

y '

eat'

es q

ue

com

e.In

t: B

ien,

muy

bie

n. ¿

Y c

ómo

lo s

abes

?JV

: P

orqu

e a

cuan

do d

ecía

'e

at'

ento

nces

m

e lo

dijo

. (3

)

Int:

Allí

pon

e (p

oint

ing

to t

he b

lack

boar

d)

Tod

ay i

s W

edne

sday

¿qu

équ

iere

de

cir?

Yol

: Y

o sa

bía

deci

r T

oday

is

Wed

nesd

ay'

pero

'is

' no

sab

ía m

uy b

ien

lo q

ue

sign

ific

a.

(4)

T. D

id y

ou

enjo

y yo

ur

wee

kend

?So

r: M

. it

's R

uben

's h

appy

bir

thda

y to

you

(si

ngin

g)

happ

y bi

rthd

ayto

you

.T

: It

's R

uben

's b

irth

day,

ok,

con

grat

ulat

ions

, ha

ppy

birt

hday

. (T

U 3

)

JV (

Tal

kíng

ab

out

a co

mpo

siti

on

he h

ad

wri

tten

ab

out

his

own

vil-

lage

in

w

hich

th

e te

ache

r ha

d co

rrec

ted

seve

ral

wor

ds

and

expr

es-

sion

s an

d w

hich

ha

s he

lped

him

to

wri

te a

sec

ond

com

posi

tion

ab

out

his

idea

l vi

llag

e) C

omo

escr

ibí

sobr

e A

lque

rías

las

pon

ía m

al,

y M

atía

sla

s pu

so b

ien

y m

e ac

orda

ba d

e có

mo

se e

scri

bían

...

Alg

unas

cos

asm

e la

s co

mía

y é

l m

e di

jo c

omo

las

tení

a qu

e po

ner.

(4

)

r. pe Z O O > Ti O O s

Page 30: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

Cog

nitiv

e S

trat

egie

s (c

ont.)

USE

O

F L

AN

GU

AG

E

FO

R

OR

AL

/WR

ITT

EN

C

OM

MU

NIC

AT

ION

Stra

tegi

es

BU

ILD

ING

AU

TO

NO

MO

US

DIS

CO

UR

SE

OV

ER

CO

MIN

GLI

MIT

ATI

ON

SIN

FL

KN

OW

LE

DG

E

Tec

hniq

ues

Rec

ombi

ning

lin

guis

tic

patt

ems

and

voca

bula

ry

Usi

ng s

eman

tic

sim

plif

lcat

ions

(con

tent

wor

ds)

Ove

rgen

eral

ized

use

of

'is'

Ask

ing

the

teac

her

or a

noth

erpu

pil

for

help

Usi

ng g

estu

res

Exa

mpl

es

Int:

Wha

t do

you

li

ke d

oing

at

the

wee

kend

?So

r: L

iste

n to

mus

ic,

stud

y E

ngli

sh a

nd m

athe

mat

ics.

Wat

chin

g te

le-

visi

ón,

to g

o to

mas

s, t

he

chur

ch,

to g

o to

th

e...

at

the

sw

imm

ing

pool

(6)

Int:

Do

you

rem

embe

r th

e st

ory

of t

he c

ater

piU

ar?

Can

you

tel

l m

ea

bit

of t

he

stor

y?El

: O

ne o

ne d

ay,

on M

onda

y on

e st

raw

berr

y, T

uesd

ay t

wo

bana

nas,

on W

edne

sday

fo

ur

tom

atoe

s, o

n...

on T

hurs

day

four

eg

gs,

on F

ri-

day

five

lo

llyp

op..

.In

t: an

d w

hat

happ

ened

?El

: O

n Sa

turd

ay?

On

Sat

urda

y le

af,

is b

ig a

nd c

ocoo

n...

on

Sund

ay?

butt

erfl

y (6

)

Son:

{D

escr

ibin

g a

mod

el h

ouse

the

y ha

ve m

ade

in g

roup

s) J

P, J

L a

ndm

e is

the

ce

men

tSo

r: T

he c

him

ney,

th

e ch

imne

y,

the

colo

ur b

lack

is

me.

..

(2)

Int:

Y c

uand

o es

táis

hab

land

o y

de

repe

nte

una

pala

bra

no

sabé

isde

cirl

a, ¿

qué

hacé

is?

El:

Pue

s de

cirl

e có

mo

se d

ice

en i

nglé

s. L

e di

ces

'How

do

you

say

this

in

Eng

lish?

' y

te l

o di

ce.

(3)

Int:

Per

o en

el

vid

eo

os

he

vist

o ju

gar

y ha

céis

as

í (g

estu

res)

¿N

oha

blái

s po

r se

ñas?

Yol

: Sí

, cu

ando

alg

unas

vec

es n

o lo

sab

emos

se

lo d

ecim

os a

Don

M p

or s

eñas

(5)

w n O 5 > D n

Page 31: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

OV

ERC

OM

ING

LIM

ITAT

IONS

INFL

KN

OW

LED

GE

Cod

e-sw

itchi

ng

Para

phra

sing

App

roxi

mat

ing

the

L2

wor

d

Fore

igni

zing

Avo

idin

g th

e to

pic

Int:

Cua

ndo

no s

abéi

s de

cir

las

cosa

s en

ing

lés

¿cóm

o la

s de

cís?

Yol

: A

lgun

as v

eces

cua

ndo

no s

é de

cir

una

cosa

pue

s la

dig

o en

med

io i

nglé

s m

edio

esp

añol

. (5

)

Int:

Are

you

the

old

est

or t

he y

oung

est

in y

our

fam

ily?

JV:

One

oíd

and

one

sm

all.

Int:

And

you

? In

the

mid

dle?

JV:

Yes

. (7

)

Int:

Wha

t ab

out

your

bed

room

? W

hat

furn

iture

can

you

fin

d?El

: T

wo

bed.

.. a

war

drob

e tw

o.In

t: A

nyth

ing

else

.El

: N

o, a

lin

k, a

lit.

Int:

Ligh

t.El

: Y

es.

(7)

Int:

Wha

t fo

od d

o yo

u no

t lik

e?El

: Le

gum

and

fish

and

piz

za.

(7)

Int:

(Ask

ing

abou

t bi

s m

othe

r) W

hat

colo

ur i

s he

r ha

ir?El

: M

e no

rem

embe

r.In

t: W

hat

is i

t in

Spa

nish

?El

: C

asta

ño.

Int:

Bro

wn.

(7)

o o ü 3 o z TI n i

Page 32: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCARCEL

The children who took part in the research had been identified bytheir teacher as 'good language learners', and so the techniques andstrategies they reported to be using can provide us with useful infor-mation about the cognitive processes involved in initial EFL learning.It would appear that effective learners are good at managing their ownlearning both inside and outside the classroom, since they tend to planfor learning activities and monitor their use of the FL. Similarly theyenjoy cooperating with peers either helping with problems, correctingmistakes or simply insisting that classroom rules are respected by all.Affectively, these children are highly motivated by their English classesand confident in their ability to use the FL, extending their enthusiasmfor learning to the world outside the classroom, where many of thempay attention to the English they hear in songs or on televisión andthey actively seek out opportunities to try out their growing knowledgeof the language with friends and family. It is significant that there is acertain amount of overlapping between the social and affective strate-gies shown by these children and the Attitudinal Contents containedin the Official Curriculum for foreign languages at primary level, in-dicating, consequently, that one way of developing this área of thecurriculum would be by explicitly promoting such strategy use in theclassroom.

Indeed, an important finding, coinciding with Nisbet & Shucksmith(1986), was that the learning techniques reported by these effectivelearners appeared to be closely linked to the classroom methodologythey experienced, that is to the type of communicative activities theyexperienced and more importantly, to the way in which they were pre-sented and carried out. Since English was the médium of instructionused in this classroom, these children relied on their background knowl-edge and inferred meanings from pictures, gestures, intonation and con-textual information which helped them both understand and rememberthe FL models and vocabulary, as did self initiated repetition in classand home. Since oral production was a high priority in this classroom,the children's speech revealed strategic interlanguage use in the formof unanalysed formulas, recombinations of known patterns and vocabu-lary, use of content words or compensatory techniques to convey mes-sages with their limited linguistic resources. Interestingly, some childrenalso showed evidence of having explicitly begun to form hypothesesabout the rules underlying their use of English. The question then isnot so much whether teachers can help children to learn or not, theresults of our research suggest that, at primary level, they already do,

454

Page 33: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

since many of the learning strategies developed by these children seemedto be modelled on the teacher's methodology and approach to learningEnglish.

CONCLUSIONS

The importance of this study lies, we believe, in the insights it canoffer for teachers of English to young learners. By explicitly identify-ing the types of techniques and strategies used by children to learn theFL, and by making them available to teachers, we can help to raisetheir awareness of the ways in which they can promote successfullearning in their classrooms. Pedagogically, then, the results of thisstudy point to a number of immediate implications. These include theneed for teachers to créate a supportive and affective learning environ-ment; to establish explicit links between the FL content and the chil-dren's previous knowledge; to make effective use of extralinguistic andgestural support to facilítate comprehension in the FL; to encourage theproductive use of English by teaching (and explaining) routine formu-las and by encouraging children to express themselves as best they can,despite their limited knowledge of the FL. Clearly, what is needed isfor teachers to incorpórate sound learning strategies into their teaching,so that the intuitive use of these strategies as shown by the more ef-fective learners could be extended to all children and automatized atan early age.

We would like to conclude this paper by indicating our awarenessthat the results of this study have been obtained from only a small sam-ple of eight learners. However, we also believe that the identificationof these children's learning strategies represents a significant step in ed-ucational research within the área of foreign languages. We also consi-der that further studies of this kind, carried out with a greater numberof children, both more and less effective learners, would most certainlyprovide further insights into the cognitive processes involved in initialFL learning in primary schools.

REFERENCES

BELTRÁN LLERA, J. (1993). Procesos, Estrategias y Técnicas de Aprendizaje,Madrid, Síntesis.

455

Page 34: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

BIALYSTOK, E. (1983). "Some factors in the selection and implementation ofcommunication strategies", in Faerch, C; Kasper, G., Strategies in Inter-language Communication, London, Longman.

—, (1990). Communication Strategies, Oxford, Basil Blackwell.—, and RYAN, E. B. (1985). "Toward a definition of metalingual skill", Merrill-

Palmer Quarterly, 31, pp. 229-251.CATHCART, R. (1986). "Situational differences and the sampling of young chil-

dren's school language", in Day, R. (ed.), Talking to Learn. Conversationin Second Language Acquisition, Rowley Mass, Newbury House.

CHAMOT, A. U.; KÜPPER, L; IMPINK-HERNANDEZ, M. V. (1988). A study oflearning strategies in foreign language instruction: first year report,Mclean, Va. Interstate Research Associates.

CHAMOT, A. U.; O'MALLEY, M. J.; KÜPPER, L; IMPINK-HERNANDEZ, M. V.(1987). A study of learning strategies inforeign language instruction: find-ings of the longitudinal study, Mclean, Va. Interstate Research Associates.

CHAMOT, A. U.; BEARD EL DINARY, P. (1999). "Children's Learning Strategiesin Language Immersion Classrooms", Modern Language Journal, 83/3,pp. 319-338.

CHESTERFIELD, R.; CHESTERFIELD, K. B. (1985). "Natural order in children's useof second language learning strategies", Applied Linguistics, 6, 1, pp. 45-59-

COHÉN, A. (1990). Language Learning: Lnsights for learners, teachers and re-searchers, New York, Newbury House.

CORDER, P. (1983). "Strategies of Communication", in Faerch, C; Kasper, G.,Strategies in Interlanguage Communication, London, Longman.

DERRY, S. J.; MURPHY, D. A. (1986). "Designing Systems that train learningAbility: From Theory to Practice", Review of Educational Research, 56, 1,pp. 1-39.

ELLIS, R. (1985). Understanding Second Language Acquisition, Oxford, OxfordUniversity Press.

—, (1994). The Study of Second Language Acquisition, Oxford, Oxford Uni-versity Press.

FAERCH, C; KASPER, G. (1980). "Processes and Strategies in foreign languagelearning and communication", The Lnterlanguage Studies Bulletin, 5, 1,pp. 47-118.

—, (1983). Strategies in Lnterlanguage Communication, London, Longman.—, (1984). "Two ways of defining communication strategies", Language Learn-

ing, 34/1, pp. 45-63.—, (1987). "From product to process - introspective methods in second lan-

guage research", in Faerch, C; Kasper, G., Lntrospection in second lan-guage research, Clevedon, UK. Multilingual Matters.

MCCOMBS, B. L. (1988). "Motivational Skills Training: Combining Metacogni-tive, Cognitive and Affective learning Strategies", in Weinstein, C. E.; Goetz,E. T.; Alexander, P. (eds.), Learning and Study Strategies. Lssues in Assess-ment, Lnstruction and Evaluation, New York, Academic Press.

456

Page 35: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

CHILOREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM

MCLAUGHLIN, B. (1990). "'Conscious' versus 'unconscious' learning", TESOLQuarterly, 24/4, pp. 617-634.

NAIMAN, N.; FROLICH, M.; STERN H. H.; TODESCO, A. (1978). The good lan-guage learner, Toronto, Ontario Institute for studies in Education.

NISBET, J.; SHUCKSMITH, J. (1986). Learning Strategies, London, Routledge,Kegan & Paul.

O'MALLEY, J. M.; CHAMOT, A. U.; STEWER-MANZANARES, G.; KÜPPER, L;RUSSO, R. (1985a). "Learning strategies used by beginning and interme-diate ESL students", Language Learning, 35, pp. 21-46.

—, (1985b). "Learning strategy applications with students of English as a sec-ond language", TESOL Quarterly, 19, pp. 285-296.

O'MALLEY, J. M.; CHAMOT, A. U. (1990). Learning Strategies in Second lan-guage Acquisitíon, Cambridge, CUP.

OXFORD, R. L. (1990). Language Learning Strategies: What every Teachershould Know, New York, Newbury House.

—, and COHÉN, A. (1992). "Language Learning Strategies: Crucial Issues ofConcept and Classification", Applied Language Learning, 3/1-2, pp. 1-35.

RUBÍN, J. (1981). "Study of cognitive processes in second language learning",Applied Linguistics, 11, pp. 117-131.

—, (1987). "Learner strategies: Theoretical Assumptions Research History andTypology", in Wenden, A.; Rubin, J., Learner Strategies in Language Learn-ing, Englewood Cliffs, N.J., Prentice Hall.

SCHMECK, R. (1988). "Individual Differences and Learning Strategies", inWeinstein, C. E.; Goetz, E. T.; Alexander, P. (eds.), Learning and StudyStrategies. Lssues in Assessment, Instruction and Evaluation, New York,Academic Press.

SCHMIDT, R. (1990). "The role of consciousness in language learning", AppliedLinguistics, 11, pp. 129-158.

SELIGER, H. (1991). "Strategy and Tactics in Second Language Acquisition", inMalave, L. M.; Duquette, G. (eds.), Language, Culture and Cognition,Clevedon, Multilingual Matters.

SELINKER, L. (1972). "Interlanguage", International Review of Applied Linguis-tics, X/3, pp. 201-231.

SKEHAN, P. (1989). Individual Differences in Second Language Learning, Lon-don, Edward Arnold.

STERN, H. H. (1983). Fundamental Concepts of Language Teaching, Oxford,Oxford University Press.

TARONE, E. (1977). "Conscious communication strategies in Interlanguage: AProgress Report", in Brown, H. D. et al. (eds.), On Tesol '77, WashingtonD.C., TESOL.

—, (1980). "Communication strategies, foreigner talk and repair in interlanguage",Language Learning, 30/2, pp. 417-431.

—, (1981). "Some thoughts on the notion of communication strategies", TESOLQuarterly, 15/3, pp. 285-295.

457

Page 36: Children's learning strategies in the primary FL classroom · CHILDREN'S LEARNING STRATEGIES IN THE PRIMARY FL CLASSROOM Unable, then, to agree upon one generally accepted definition

YVETTE COYLE AND MARISOL VALCÁRCEL

—, (1983)- "On the variability of interlanguage systems", Applied Linguistics,4/2, pp. 142-163.

WEINSTEIN, C. E.; MAYER, R. E. (1986). "The Teaching of Learning Strategies",in Wittrock, M. C. (ed.), Handbook of Research on Teaching, New York,MacMillan.

WENDEN, A. (1987). "Incorporating learner training in the classroom", in Wenden,A.; Rubin, J., Learner Strategies in Language Learning, Englewood Cliffs,N.J., Prentice Hall.

—, (1991)- Learner Strategies for Learner Autonomy, New York, Prentice Hall.—, and RUBÍN, J. (1987). Learner Strategies in Language Learning, Englewood

Cliffs, N.J., Prentice Hall.WILLING, K. (1989). Teaching How to Learn. Learning Strategies in ESL, Mac-

quarie University, Sydney, National Centre for English Language Teachingand Research.

WONG FILLMORE, L. (1976). The second time around: Cognitive and socialstrategies in second language acquisition, unpublished Ph.D. dissertation,Stanford University.

—, (1979). "Individual differences in second language acquisition", in Fillmore,C. J.; Wang, W.-S. Y.; Kempler, D. (eds.), Individual differences in lan-guage ahility and language hehaviour, New York, Academic Press.

—, (1982). "The language learner as an individual: Implications of research onindividual differences for the ESL teacher", in Clark, M.; Handscombe, J.(eds.), On TESOL '82, Washington, DC, TESOL.

458


Recommended