13
This article was downloaded by: [Tulane University] On: 29 August 2013, At: 10:42 Publisher: Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK International Journal of Social Research Methodology Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tsrm20 Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work Michael Rowe Published online: 26 Feb 2007. To cite this article: Michael Rowe (2007) Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work, International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 10:1, 37-48, DOI: 10.1080/13645570600652792 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13645570600652792 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the “Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis, our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as to the accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinions and views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors, and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Content should not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sources of information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims, proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to or arising out of the use of the Content. This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing, systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms- and-conditions

Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

  • Upload
    michael

  • View
    215

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

This article was downloaded by: [Tulane University]On: 29 August 2013, At: 10:42Publisher: RoutledgeInforma Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registeredoffice: Mortimer House, 37-41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

International Journal of SocialResearch MethodologyPublication details, including instructions for authors andsubscription information:http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/tsrm20

Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and theEthnography of Police WorkMichael RowePublished online: 26 Feb 2007.

To cite this article: Michael Rowe (2007) Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnographyof Police Work, International Journal of Social Research Methodology, 10:1, 37-48, DOI:10.1080/13645570600652792

To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13645570600652792

PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

Taylor & Francis makes every effort to ensure the accuracy of all the information (the“Content”) contained in the publications on our platform. However, Taylor & Francis,our agents, and our licensors make no representations or warranties whatsoever as tothe accuracy, completeness, or suitability for any purpose of the Content. Any opinionsand views expressed in this publication are the opinions and views of the authors,and are not the views of or endorsed by Taylor & Francis. The accuracy of the Contentshould not be relied upon and should be independently verified with primary sourcesof information. Taylor and Francis shall not be liable for any losses, actions, claims,proceedings, demands, costs, expenses, damages, and other liabilities whatsoeveror howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with, in relation to orarising out of the use of the Content.

This article may be used for research, teaching, and private study purposes. Anysubstantial or systematic reproduction, redistribution, reselling, loan, sub-licensing,systematic supply, or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. Terms &Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/page/terms-and-conditions

Page 2: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

Int. J. Social Research Methodology

Vol. 10, No. 1, February 2007, pp. 37–48

ISSN 1364–5579 (print)/ISSN 1464–5300 (online) © 2007 Taylor & Francis

DOI: 10.1080/13645570600652792

Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police WorkMichael Rowe

Taylor and Francis LtdTSRM_A_165253.sgmReceived 11 March 2005; Accepted 18 July 2005

10.1080/13645570600652792International Journal of Social Research Methodology1364-5579 (print)/1464-5300 (online)Original Article2007Taylor & Francis101000000February [email protected]

Ethnographies of policing have referred to the difficulties that researchers face as they

encounter corruption, malpractice and police deviance. This paper reflects on the author’s

ethnographical work with a British police force and suggests that while only relatively

minor instances of misbehaviour were witnessed, these nonetheless raised challenging

ethical issues. In addition to exploring the practicalities of negotiating access to the field and

the difficulties of securing informed consent, the paper highlights the importance of

anticipating the ethical dimensions of the mundane realities encountered during fieldwork.

Introduction

This article examines the difficulties of operationalizing principles of ethical research

in the context of a study of British policing. While other ethnographies of policing have

faced ethical dilemmas relating to serious incidents of malpractice or police deviance,

this researcher’s experience was that only apparently minor dilemmas were encoun-

tered. The article argues, however, that these low-level issues might be more typical of

the research experience and that, nonetheless, they raise important ethical concerns.

The molehills of ethical dilemmas proved as difficult to negotiate as the anticipated

mountains would have done had they materialized. For 18 weeks, in 2004, I accompa-

nied uniformed police officers, usually constables, as they went about their routine

activities in three areas within one British police service. Approximately 660 hours were

spent ‘in the field’. The research explored the factors that shape officers’ decision

making and their exercise of discretion. While designing the project, and discussing it

with academic colleagues and gatekeepers within the police service, I read many of the

Dr Michael Rowe is director of the Institute of Criminology, Victoria University, Wellington. He has published

widely on topics relating to diversity and policing, including, in 2004, a book Policing, Race and Racism that explores

the impact of the Lawrence Inquiry on the British police service. Correspondence to: Michael Rowe, Institute of

Criminology, School of Social and Cultural Studies, Victoria University, PO Box 600, Wellington, New Zealand.

Email: [email protected]

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 3: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

38 M. Rowe

methodological texts on ethnography and accounts by previous researchers who had

used similar methods with the police. This preparation shaped my expectations of the

difficulties facing a police ethnographer. Moreover, while ethnography was new to me,

I had considerable previous experience of police research and felt familiar with many

aspects of police subculture. Many previous studies, as outlined further below, saw

ethnographers confronted with instances of police corruption or use of excessive

violence and it was partly from this vicarious experience that my expectations were

shaped. Despite these potential problems an observational approach was preferable to

other qualitative methodologies such as interviews or documentary analysis, a point

made by Punch (1979, p. 4) in his classic study of policing in Amsterdam. Similarly,

Reiner advocated this approach on the grounds that:

the wish to penetrate [the] low-visibility [of police work] is why participant observation

has been the main technique adopted by researchers wishing to analyse the practices and

cultures of policing. All other methods rely on some sort of account offered by the police

themselves … the veracity of which is often precisely the question being studied. (2000,

p. 219)

This article focuses on three broad themes—access and reliability, informed consent,

and ethical dilemmas relating to malpractice. In conclusion it is argued that doing

research in a dynamic environment such as policing means that principles laid down

in the texts or professional codes of practice are very difficult to operationalize. The

research was in the tradition described by van Maanen (1995, p. 9) as ‘critical ethnog-

raphy’, which locates subjects in a broader social, political, cultural and historical

context than they themselves may be aware of. Following van Maanen’s (1995)

insistence that contemporary emphasis on textual analysis in ethnography (see e.g.

Willis, 2000) can be reconciled with a more traditional factuality, this article provides

insight into one attempt to conduct ethnographical research.

Access and Reliability

While methods textbooks give advice on accessing research subjects, this is often

narrowly understood as a formal pre-requisite. In practice, access is not just established

at the outset but, rather, a process of continuing negotiation and explanation. While a

contractual form of access was necessary, this represented only a preliminary require-

ment, an agreement that was essential in order to proceed with the work but was insuf-

ficient on a day-to-day basis in terms of securing the participation of subjects. Indeed,

demonstrating that formal access was agreed at a high level might have been counter-

productive when trying to convince junior staff that I was trustworthy, since they were

sometimes concerned that I had been ‘planted’ by senior managers to report on them.

For ethical and practical reasons I decided against attempting a covert programme

of research. Such an approach clearly breaches the principle that the subjects of

research should offer their ‘informed consent’ to being included. As will be discussed

further below, it is far from clear that this principle was met fully in any meaningful

sense, but a covert approach would have allowed no prospect of doing so. Those who

have conducted covert research have tended to use an ‘ends justify the means’

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 4: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

International Journal of Social Research Methodology 39

argument, such that exposing malfeasance of some kind could not have been achieved

in any other way. Fountain (1993) described her study of drug dealers on this basis, and

noted that she often felt that she was betraying subjects who had treated her as a friend,

unaware that she was conducting research. However, she arrived at a fundamentally

pragmatic position:

After the euphoria over all the information had died down, however, I felt that we had

abused the hospitality of this friendly man [a cannabis dealer], who had so clearly enjoyed

our company. We agreed that we would not have spent so long with him, listening to his

long rambling tales, it they had not been so useful for research purposes. We did it again,

though. (Fountain, 1993, p. 166)

Covert fieldwork might be preferred because of the greater reliability of findings.

Fielding noted that covert research, while demanding and ethically problematic, offers

the advantage that the researcher is ‘… much less likely to disturb the setting, avoiding

the risk of studying an artefact of [their] presence rather than normal behaviour’ (1993,

p. 159). Additionally, some justify covert research if access cannot be obtained by other

means. Six months or so before I began my study the British Broadcasting Corporation

(BBC) documentary The Secret Policeman had revealed, through the use of hidden

recording equipment, the racist views of some police trainees (BBC, 2003). In an effort

to demonstrate that I was not seeking a similar exposé of police behaviour an overt

approach seemed most appropriate, and on this basis the permission of the Chief

Constable was sought. Concern that the police service might be a relatively closed

organization following The Secret Policeman affair proved unfounded and formal

agreement was straightforward.

Furthermore, an overt approach was preferable in this case, since the pressure of

sustaining a covert project would have been considerable and the risk of losing the data

too great in the event of being ‘uncovered’. One sergeant reinforced my confidence that

I had followed the best course when he noted—in no uncertain terms—that I would

have been physically thrown out of the police station had officers suspected that I was

an undercover researcher.

While formal agreement was vital to the project, it was not enough to ensure the

meaningful cooperation of officers. The police constables (PCs) that were shadowed

were picked on a structured but random basis, whereby a series of shifts were identi-

fied but particular staff were only identified during the initial briefing. Sometimes the

officer(s) were selected simply because they volunteered, or because their colleagues

were attending court or training, or doing administrative duties away from the ‘front-

line’ work relevant to the study. Since more than five weeks were spent in each police

station and only six shifts worked out of each of them, the pool was relatively small

and so there were times when officers were selected simply because it was their turn to

have me tag along. Usually the shift sergeant allocated me to officers, although there

were occasions were none was on duty and so I negotiated directly with the PCs

involved. These few instances did bring into sharp relief the fact that access was a

continuing matter of negotiation, even once formal agreements had been signed. Not

only did I have the challenge of negotiating my way past the police-only rear entrance

to the station at various times of the day and night, I then had to sit in a briefing room

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 5: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

40 M. Rowe

under the curious gaze of officers who were clearly wondering who the quiet stranger

in the corner was. More than once I was mistaken for a fellow officer, albeit in plain

clothes, since it was relatively routine for unfamiliar colleagues to be wandering

around the police station. Usually the sergeants working on each shift knew I would

be attending and what my remit was, but this was not always the case and I had to

explain myself to them as well as to the PCs in the briefing room. My standpoint as a

researcher was clearly as an ‘outside outsider’ in Reiner’s typology (Reiner, 2000),

which meant that I lacked the occupational and cultural capital that might make

acceptance easier to secure. However, I was otherwise relatively well placed in this

respect. Like many of the officers, I was a white male in my mid 30s and I benefited

from the experience of previous research that had grounded me in many aspects of

police work.

Gaining access in terms of being given permission on a daily basis to accompany

officers was one thing: not getting thrown out the police station by suspicious officers

was clearly of fundmental importance, but was not enough in itself. Access was of wider

significance in terms of gaining the trust of officers such that they would share their

perspectives with me candidly. Whyte noted the importance of personal relations in his

classic study of ‘an Italian slum’:

I soon found out that people were developing their own explanation about me: I was

writing a book about Cornerville. This might seem entirely too vague an explanation, and

yet it sufficed. I found that my acceptance in the district depended on the personal rela-

tionships I developed far more than upon any explanations I might give. Whether it was a

good thing to write a book about Cornerville depended entirely on people’s opinions of

me personally. If I was all right, then my project was all right; if I was no good, then no

amount of explanation could convince them that the book was a good idea. (1969/1943,

p. 300)

While there were times when officers seemed to be guarded and reluctant to engage in

discussion, these were relatively rare. I soon noticed that the way in which I completed

my field notes seemed to influence the extent to which officers talked freely: making

notes in the rear of a patrol car, for example, inhibited discussion as officers were

reminded that I was making a record of what they said. Once a sergeant who had been

discussing an incident with PCs spotted that I was making notes across the other side

of the room—not, as it happened, about their conversation—and said to me ‘I hope

you’re not going to cost me my job’ and was noticeably more reticent in my presence

thereafter. Several officers expressed their desire to read my notes but, although I

consented to this in order to cultivate their trust, none actually did so. On one occasion

I was assigned to two female PCs, one of whom I had not previously met. The officer I

had met took her colleague to one side, into the ladies toilets, for a ‘quick word’ before

we went on patrol: although I do not know what was said between them, neither spoke

to me for several hours afterwards. Equally there were times when it seemed officers

were presenting the ‘official line’ on their work, rather than sharing their own perspec-

tives. Emerson, Fretz, and Shaw noted that writing is a ‘strange, marginalizing activity,

marking the writer as an observer, rather than as a full ordinary participant’ (1995,

p. 37). Moreover, they noted, writing sets the researcher apart conceptually and

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 6: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

International Journal of Social Research Methodology 41

mentally from their subjects, which is paradoxical in terms of the realist and empa-

thetic tradition of ethnography.

By spending upwards of five weeks in each station, I hoped officers would come to

trust me. For this reason too, I assured them that my notes would be both anonymous

and confidential. By working late and night shifts and ‘staying the course’ when shifts

over-ran or when officers were tied up in tedious routine activity, I hoped to demon-

strate commitment to experiencing the reality of police work, even when that meant

sitting in a car all night with little to do. After a few weeks in one police station a

sergeant I had spoken to on a few occasions told me that officers had dubbed me

‘University Mike’, and assured me that being given a nickname—something assigned

to many of the officers—was a sure sign of acceptance.

Becoming ‘one of the team’ might help to develop trust, but it also presents potential

problems. Occasionally, officers, perhaps conscious of the repercussions they might

experience, urged me—quite unnecessarily, I assured them—that I should not ‘get

involved’ should trouble occur, as they were trained, and insured, for such eventuali-

ties. Marks’s (2004) account of being instructed by members of the Durban public

order policing unit in the use of semi-automatic machine guns—‘just in case’ it proved

necessary for her to defend herself—was not an experience shared by this ethnogra-

pher. There were occasions, though, when, in order to increase officers’ view of my

reliability and trustworthiness, I occasionally found myself taking details of those

involved in minor road traffic accidents, advising victims of crime, or helping to shift

fallen branches from the carriageway. Only on rare occasions did such interventions

give me serious cause for concern. One such time was when two PCs and I responded

to reports of a ‘break-in in-progress’ at around 04.00 am one summer morning. As we

ran towards the address I was asked by one officer to cover the rear of the property in

case the offender fled in that direction. It was only as I walked out of sight that I began

to wonder what I would do if a frightened burglar suddenly jumped out in front of me:

while my adrenalin levels were high, I was not convinced that I would be much use

should there be a physical confrontation. Fortunately, or otherwise, there was no sign

of the intruder. As we ran off in the direction he may have taken, the PC, who had

established some distance on me, turned, threw me a bunch of keys and shouted ‘bring

the car round’. Without too much option I picked up the keys and ran back to the car.

It was only as I drove cautiously around the corner and pulled alongside an officer that

I did not know who eyed me—clearly a civilian— with some suspicion that it occurred

to me that driving the patrol car might not have been entirely within my remit!

On another occasion, I was left with a comparatively junior PC and a suspect in the

station ‘chute’ where prisoners are held before being formally ‘booked in’ to the

custody suite. As we chatted I noticed that the prisoner surreptitiously put something

in his mouth and swallow it. The PC did not seem to notice and I was unsure whether

to mention it to him. I was concerned that if the prisoner had swallowed drugs this

might have serious repercussions for both him and the officer. Methodologically,

however, I did not want to interfere in the situation and influence the way in which

events unfolded. Eventually I decided that the potential impact on both the prisoner

and the officer was primary, and when the opportunity arose I discreetly mentioned to

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 7: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

42 M. Rowe

the officer what I had seen. He thanked me and discussed this with his more

experienced colleague, and the two of them decided to ‘keep this between ourselves’.

While such incidents suggest some success in building trust and negotiating mean-

ingful access, ultimately there is no way of establishing the extent to which officers genu-

inely shared their perspectives on police work and the public at large. Waddington

noted, following his study of policing, that ‘… it is a least conceivable that they sought

to protect themselves against any damage that I might do … one can only speculate about

how things were done in my absence …’ (1999, p. 212). Frequently I was surprised by

the candid comments that officers made about their work, senior managers and the

nature of policing. The extent to which critical opinions were offered suggested that

officers were speaking honestly and directly, but it is inevitable that the research subjects

‘framed’ their views as they sought to tell me about their work or were keen to show me

incidents or events that would be, they thought, particularly salient. On a number of

occasions officers informed the control room, as we began a shift, that I was in company

with them, and sometimes they asked that the controller be aware of this when assigning

us to jobs. This clearly raised the prospect that the control room might not have sent

me to situations thought to be dangerous or problematic in some way, although we could

hear all of the radio traffic for the police area and it did not seem to me that there were

such incidents happening while we were being kept away. At other times officers asked

me if there was anything in particular that I wanted to see or experience during the shift:

we could have done some traffic stops, for example, if I had wanted to. When asked, I

told the officer not to do anything in particular with me in mind, and explained that I

wanted to see the usual flow of police work. What I cannot account for, however, is the

number of times when officers were selective on my behalf without discussing it with

me, and it must be assumed that this happened to some extent.

The Ethics of Informed Consent

Since the principle of informed consent is a fundamental canon of research methods, I

tried to ensure that participants were fully aware of the nature of the study and asked

explicitly for their consent. Unlike some other professional associations, such as the

American Sociological Association, the British Sociological Association (BSA) code of

practice does not offer absolute rules but establishes guiding principles. Informed

consent is clearly central to the BSA code, as paragraphs 16 and 17 demonstrate:

As far as possible participation in sociological research should be based on the freely given

informed consent of those studied. This implies a responsibility on the sociologist to

explain in appropriate detail, and in terms meaningful to participants, what the research is

about, who is undertaking and financing it, why it is being undertaken, and how it is to be

disseminated and used.

Research participants should be made aware of their right to refuse participation whenever

and for whatever reason they wish. (2002)

Fetterman maintained that ‘ethnographers must be candid about their task, explaining

what they plan to study and how they plan to study it’ (1989, p. 134); this was the

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 8: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

International Journal of Social Research Methodology 43

approach I adopted. However, practical problems made this difficult to realize. Often

it was not feasible to seek the consent of all those encountered. Members of the public,

whether victims, witnesses or enquirers, sometimes were told of the research and

assured that I was not interested in their personal details. Often when officers intro-

duced me they described my role in generic terms—’he’s a researcher from the Univer-

sity’—that actually revealed little about what I was doing, or they mis-described me as

‘an occupational psychologist’, ‘criminal profiler’, or—in jest—‘the chief constable’.

Fetterman’s purist approach might have required me to clarify my position, but to do

so would have damaged my relation with officers since it might have got in the way of

their main business. While people were asked for their permission before I entered

their home, for example, it was rare that they were in a position to withhold their

consent, or at least could not do so without inconvenience or embarrassment. Equally,

those who had been victims of crime, or witnesses, may have been vulnerable or

distressed, and it is highly likely that they did not properly understand the research. In

such circumstances, the extent to which consent was given in a meaningful way was

limited. Very occasionally people taken into custody were asked if they had any

objection to my presence. Again the extent to which consent could be offered in

aproperly ‘informed’ manner seems likely to have been restricted by the subordinate

position of the arrestee.

The ethical dilemmas arising from such encounters were mitigated by a number of

factors. First, anonymity was ensured by the data protection agreement signed with the

police service, which stated that no personal details would be divulged, and that my

research notes would be kept in a secure environment. Furthermore, only minimal

details of individuals were recorded; certainly, no full names or precise locations were

included. Despite the fact that the research was conducted on an ethical basis in the

strict narrow sense, it was unavoidable that, by encountering members of the public in

situations of distress, there was a danger that details of their private lives were being

extrapolated and that they were often unable to properly consent to this. Watching for

several hours as officers and paramedics dealt with a man who had attempted suicide

was a deeply uncomfortable experience in terms of research ethics: clearly he had not

consented to my presence. With relevance to this study, Hey reflected on her study of

schoolgirl friendships that ‘despite seeking to establish non-exploitative field relations,

I was never able to evade the facts … not only was I generally more powerful than most

of the girls but my agenda was in part to appropriate parts of their lives for my own use’

(2002, p. 75). Fundamentally, though, these individuals were not my research subjects

in the strict sense, as my focus was on the perspectives and behaviour of police officers.

Relations with officers were negotiated on an ongoing basis and their informed

consent was sought, although this too was subject to certain qualification related to the

institutional context in which the research was conducted. While I explained the

rationale of the work and offered officers the opportunity to decline to participate, in

practice it was unlikely that any would do so, and in fact none did. Since shift sergeants

were responsible, with informal negotiation involving myself and the PCs, for allocat-

ing me to particular officers as each shift began work, the extent to which they were in

a position to opt out of the study was limited by the implicit power relations at play. As

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 9: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

44 M. Rowe

the sergeants were in a position of authority over the PCs, it must be assumed that their

assent to my presence was informed, in part at least, by the recognition that it was their

supervisor who was ‘requesting’ that they accommodate me during the shift. Norris

encountered a similar problem and noted that ‘when sergeants assigned me to specific

officers it was difficult to know whether having me along constituted an order’ (1993,

p. 129). Although officers tended to be friendly toward me, it might be assumed that

those who were less enthusiastic may have simply kept their reservations hidden even

as they accepted that I would be observing their work.

The fluid nature of policing meant that officers were encountered who did not know

what my role was. Although I became familiar to most officers working in the stations

included in the study, there were occasions when I observed other officers who had no

idea of my role. Sometimes I was introduced; more usually, circumstances meant that

this was not possible. While it might be, as with members of the public, that these

officers were not the direct subjects of the research, it is clear that their perspectives on

police work inform the study’s findings. It must also be noted that officers may not

have fully understood the scope of the project and remit of issues that were of interest.

This difficulty was compounded since there was no direct opportunity, in most cases,

to explain the research in detail. While a document outlining the project had been

circulated, in practice few officers had seen it.

‘Guilty Knowledge and Dirty Hands’

My prior reading of ethnographic accounts of police work left me with the impression

that it was likely that I would encounter malpractice or unethical behaviour of some

kind. Such an experience might mean that I would need to choose between reporting

officers, and so breaking the promise of anonymity that I had made, or ‘turning a blind

eye’, and so conniving in behaviour that was itself unethical, illegal, or both. Relating

to his own ethical dilemmas, discussed further below, Norris points out that the ‘soci-

ological literature on policing includes well-documented cases of police deviance … if

I had not contemplated having to face such an issue I would, indeed, have been naïve’

(1993, p. 140). In discussing his study of policing in Amsterdam, Punch made a similar

point, noting that: ‘the literature on the police alerts one to widespread and deeply

engrained malpractices such as corruption, mistreatment of suspects, racial prejudice

and the denial of rights to suspects’ (1979, p. 12). Fetterman defines the twin problems

in the following terms:

Guilty knowledge is confidential knowledge of illegal or illicit activities. Dirty hands refers

to situations in which the ethnographer cannot emerge innocent of wrongdoing. (1989,

p. 135)

Having re-visited much of the ethnographic literature on police work (Foster, 1989;

Holdaway, 1983; Marks, 2004; Norris, 1993; Punch, 1979; Westmarland, 2001), one of

the striking features of my own research is that I did not experience any incidents of

serious malpractice. Norris (1993, pp. 133–135) witnessed a situation in which officers

used excessive force on a prisoner and then concocted charges against him. Punch

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 10: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

International Journal of Social Research Methodology 45

(1979) uncovered, somewhat inadvertently, rumours of corruption. Marks (2004,

pp. 876–881) described her experience with the public order unit in Durban on night

patrol during which she was instructed how to use an Uzi machine gun, asked to search

a female suspect and warned by officers that some of their activities were likely to shock

her. She described how she ‘felt morally compromised by knowing that many of the

responses of the platoon were brutal and completely disregarding of the human rights

framework that was supposed to guide police behaviour’ (Marks, 2004, p. 879).

Accounts of racist, sexist and other forms of offensive language litter the police studies

literature (Graef, 1990; Reiner, 1978), but this too was relatively rare given that it seems

to have been an endemic feature of police subculture as described in other studies.

Against the expectations raised by this literature, the ethical dilemmas I faced were

relatively minor. There are a number of reasons why this may have been the case. First,

it may have been a matter of happenstance. Although I spent more than 600 hours

completing the study, it is possible that I did not witness incidents of serious wrong-

doing, just as I did not deal with the serious road accidents that officers confidently

predicted I would be faced with. I did not accompany officers to the scene of a murder

or a street robbery, and yet such events, especially the latter, do feature in police work.

It is possible that other researchers have sought out more dramatic aspects of police

work. Furthermore, it might be that I did witness such events, but that I did not judge

that officers had behaved inappropriately. Certainly there were circumstances in which

officers used violence, but, even having carefully reviewed my field notes, I judged this

to have been proportionate. On one occasion officers used some force to restrain a

drunk prisoner, who had been highly aggressive prior to arrest. Upon arrival at the

custody suite the man calmed down, but as his handcuffs were removed inside the cell

he tried again to physically assault officers. In response, several staff rushed in and

jumped on him until his hands and feet could once again be secured. As this was done

the custody sergeant looked at me with some concern and asked who I was and what I

was doing. He seemed acutely aware that I had witnessed events, but I did not think that

the force used had been excessive: as soon as he stopped struggling officers desisted and

once he was re-secured they left him alone in the cell. Perhaps another researcher

would have reached a different conclusion.

Third, it is possible that officers successfully hid their malpractice from me and

managed to dupe me in relation to their real attitudes and activities. This would be

consistent with Punch’s experience, where it was only towards the end of his research

that he began to realize that their deviance had been successfully concealed (Punch,

1979). A recurring theme among police officers during my study was to remind one

another that I had concealed surveillance equipment and that I was from the ‘integrity

squad’ sent from headquarters to check on them. While superficially said in jest, this

does indicate that officers, for all their candour, did not entirely forget that an outsider

was present. Reiner noted that while these problems are generic to participant observa-

tion research, they are especially acute in the context of policing, since ‘… there is the

particular skill officers are likely to have in tactics for covering up what they do not

want known. Police researchers are investigating subjects whose job it is to investigate

the deviance of others’ (2000, p. 219).

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 11: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

46 M. Rowe

While I did not have experiences relating to ‘guilty knowledge and dirty hands’ to

rival those of other researchers in terms of drama or seriousness, there were times when

I witnessed incidents or heard things that caused concern. The most prominent of these

related to an incident in which an officer lied to a victim of a minor crime. Having

listened to her account of the recent theft of a mobile phone by a youth known to her—

and to the police—the officer and I left, telling her that we would go and speak to the

offender and find out what he had to say. Even at this stage I was surprised by the direct

way in which the officer challenged the woman’s version of events by telling her that

‘you shouldn’t have lent it to him if you knew he was a thief’. Once we had left, the

officer told me that in his opinion the woman may not have had her phone stolen at all;

she had some form of learning difficulties and could have loaned her phone to the

youth who had failed to return it as agreed. Even if this were not the case, and he had

stolen it, he would tell us that he had borrowed the phone since he would be astute

enough to lie his way out of trouble. Given the woman’s vulnerability she would not be

a credible witness, and the youth’s account was likely to be accepted. In any event this

was, to repeat a phrase much used by officers, a ‘weary job’: one that related to a minor

incident and offered little or no prospect of any criminal proceedings. The constable

told me that his next step was to tell

a little white lie, I’m going to tell her that I spoke to [the youth’s] mother and she promises

to make him bring the phone back as soon as he gets home. There’s no point making a

crime report, or a statement. We would have got the same result after two hours’ paper-

work, so why bother? And I don’t like getting my pen out.

After a brief drive around the neighbourhood we returned to the woman’s house and

the officer gave her this version of events.

I was surprised by this episode: both that the officer had lied to the woman and that

he had been so open to me about it. I did not disagree with his interpretation of events,

or that there was little chance of recovering her property. However, my experience

during the study was that officers were quite candid with victims in these circum-

stances. That the woman had learning difficulties seemed to compound the problem as

far as I was concerned, and I identified that three options were available to me: to say

nothing, to discuss the matter with the officer or, most gravely, to report it to a more

senior officer. The first of these protected my position as a researcher since it meant

that I was not intervening. However, this meant that I was doing nothing to address the

officer’s inappropriate behaviour—which clearly fell short of the proclaimed standards

of the police service that stipulate that officers would ‘provide support and reassurance

in a fair and professional manner’.

Alternatively, I could have reported the episode to a senior officer and left the service

to take the appropriate action. In terms of Israel’s review of the grounds for guarantee-

ing subject confidentiality (Israel, 2004), this would have been against the duty of fidel-

ity made while securing access. Israel also noted that consequentialist arguments are

often used to justify maintaining confidentiality, and negative repercussions were

envisaged in this context as it would have been difficult to regain the trust of other

officers. Furthermore, it would potentially spoil the field for future researchers, which

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 12: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

International Journal of Social Research Methodology 47

runs contrary to the BSA code of ethics. In this case I decided not to refer the matter to

any other party, primarily because I judged that the gravity of the officer’s action

weighed less heavily than the possible consequences that may follow from a reporting

of it. Ultimately this response was based upon a calculation of outcome rather than the

content of the action—and so reflects broader debates about the status of ethics in a

postmodern era. Bauman (1993) has noted that ethical certainty is difficult to secure in

postmodern times, marked by an absence of social norms. While this might be liberat-

ing in some respects, it tends to replace a coherent ethical framework with ‘… the strag-

gly shoots of never ending, never resolved moral anxiety’ (Bauman, 1993, p. 80).

Promises of anonymity are conditional, however. If an officer had committed a crimi-

nal offence of a serious nature then I might have decided that details had to be passed

on to authorities inside or outside of the police service. Even in such circumstances

though, as Norris (1993, p. 140) notes, such decisions will always remain a matter of

judgement—what constitutes a ‘serious’ offence?—and it might be that the public

interest is best served not by ‘blowing the whistle’ in response to the first instance of

deviance but by keeping quiet and trying to establish ‘structural regularities’ that might

provide a more rigorous basis from which reform can be instigated.

Conclusion: Situational Ethics and Reflexivity

Reviewing my field notes and the experiences of other researchers who have conducted

ethnographic studies of policework, similarities are apparent in terms of negotiating

access and gathering reliable data. The formal negotiation process regarding access

proved relatively straightforward. Informally, though, access was an ongoing matter of

negotiation for the duration of the study. As others have also noted, I can never be sure

of the extent to which I won the trust of the officers that I observed and it is likely that

some officers framed their attitudes and behaviour. To that extent my experiences were

broadly consistent with those of other ethnographers.

Where my experience diverged was in the absence of dramatic ethical dilemmas

faced during my 18 weeks on patrol. I found molehills where I had expected moun-

tains. One reason for this may have been the focus on studying the normal routine of

police work. The issues faced by researchers such as Punch (1979) or Norris (1993)

have been alluded to in the previous discussion of the ethics of ‘guilty knowledge and

dirty hands’. Reiner (2000) recalled the dilemma he faced a few days before the 1987

general election having been given information by a senior officer that, should he have

revealed it to the media, might have had a significant impact on the political agenda.

Before beginning my research I was aware that I might witness events or uncover infor-

mation that would raise ethical conundrums. Other researchers might bear in mind

that, just as policing can be dull, repetitive and mundane, so too the experiences of the

police researcher can be less than sensational. However, there were ethical issues to be

faced even in the ordinary. Ultimately I think I arrived at the correct decision in the

circumstances, but it is hard to draw general conclusions of prescriptive value. It is the

nature of ethnographic research that the principles contained in methodological

textbooks or professional codes of conduct will be stretched and perhaps distorted as

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013

Page 13: Tripping Over Molehills: Ethics and the Ethnography of Police Work

48 M. Rowe

they are applied in dynamic situations. Since policing is unpredictable, the ethical

dilemmas police researchers might face cannot be easily anticipated. Given this,

Norris’s conclusion that ethics are inevitably situational (Norris, 1993) was borne out

in this study. If an absolute code of ethics is not feasible, researchers must be prepared

to be reflexive in terms of ethical dilemmas and the methodological difficulties experi-

enced in securing informed consent and meaningful access to research subjects.

References

Bauman, Z. (1993). Postmodern ethics. Oxford: Blackwell.

British Broadcasting Corporation (2003, 21 October). The secret policeman. Documentary, BBC1.

British Sociological Association (2002). Statement of ethical practice for the British Sociological

Association, http://britsoc.co.uk (accessed 24 February 2005).

Emerson, R. M, Fretz, R. I., & Shaw, L. L. (1995). Writing ethnographic fieldnotes. Chicago, IL:

University of Chicago Press.

Fetterman, D. M. (1989). Ethnography: Step-by-step. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Fielding, N. (1993). Ethnography. In N. Gilbert (Ed.), Researching social life (pp. 154–171). London:

Sage.

Foster, J. (1989). Two stations: An ethnographic study of policing in the inner city. In D. Downes

(Ed.), Crime and the city: Essays in memory of John Barron Mays (pp. 128–153). London:

Macmillan.

Fountain, J. (1993). Dealing with data. In D. Hobbs & T. May (Eds.), Interpreting the field: Accounts

of ethnography (pp. 145–173). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Graef, R. (1990). Talking blues: The police in their own words. London: Fontana.

Hey, V. (2002). ‘Not as nice as she was supposed to be’: Schoolgirls’ friendships. In S. Taylor (Ed.),

Ethnographic research: A reader (pp. 67–90). London: Sage.

Holdaway, S. (1983). Inside the British Police. Oxford: Blackwell.

Israel, M. (2004). Strictly confidential?: Integrity and the disclosure of criminological and socio-legal

research. British Journal of Criminology, 44(5), 715–740.

Jupp, V. (1989). Methods of criminological research. London: Routledge.

Marks, M. (2004). Researching police transformation: The ethnographic imperative. British Journal

of Criminology, 44, 866–888.

Norris, C. (1993). Some ethical considerations on field-work with the police. In D. Hobbs & T. May

(Eds.), Interpreting the field: Accounts of ethnography (pp. 122–143). Oxford: Clarendon Press.

Punch, M. (1979). Policing the inner city. London: Macmillan.

Reiner, R. (1978). The blue-coated worker: A sociological study of police unionism. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press.

Reiner, R. (2000). Police research. In R. D. King & E. Wincup (Eds.), Doing research on crime and

justice (pp. 205–235). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

van Maanen, J. (1995). An end to innocence: The ethnography of ethnography. In J. van Maanen

(Ed.), Representations in ethnography (pp. 1–35). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Waddington, P. A. J. (1999). Policing citizens. London: UCL Press.

Westmarland, L. (2001). Gender and policing. Cullompton: Willan Publishing.

Whyte, W. F. (1969/1943). Street corner society—The social structure of an Italian slum. Chicago, IL:

University of Chicago Press.

Willis, P. (2000). The ethnographic imagination. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Dow

nloa

ded

by [

Tul

ane

Uni

vers

ity]

at 1

0:42

29

Aug

ust 2

013