59
University of South Florida Scholar Commons Graduate eses and Dissertations Graduate School July 2018 Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the Essential for Living Communication Modality Assessment Daniella Orozco University of South Florida, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: hps://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd Part of the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons is esis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate eses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Scholar Commons Citation Orozco, Daniella, "Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the Essential for Living Communication Modality Assessment" (2018). Graduate eses and Dissertations. hps://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/7345

Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    5

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

University of South FloridaScholar Commons

Graduate Theses and Dissertations Graduate School

July 2018

Teaching Mands to Individuals with AutismSpectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the Essentialfor Living Communication Modality AssessmentDaniella OrozcoUniversity of South Florida, [email protected]

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd

Part of the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at Scholar Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in GraduateTheses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Scholar Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected].

Scholar Commons CitationOrozco, Daniella, "Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the Essential for LivingCommunication Modality Assessment" (2018). Graduate Theses and Dissertations.https://scholarcommons.usf.edu/etd/7345

Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the Essential

for Living Communication Modality Assessment

by

Daniella Orozco

Thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Master of Science Department of Child and Family Studies

College of Behavioral and Community Sciences University of South Florida

Major Professor: Catia Cividini-Motta, Ph.D, BCBA-D Kimberly Crosland, Ph.D, BCBA-D

Raymond Miltenberger, Ph.D, BCBA-D

Date of Approval: June 26, 2018

Keywords: communication modalities, mand training, verbal behavior, alternative and augmentative communication (AAC)

Copyright® 2018, Daniella Orozco

Dedication

A mi hermosa familia, gracias a todos por su constante apoyo incondicional. Gracias por

motivarme a ser la mejor version de mi, todos los días. A mis maravillosos padres, Maytte y

Alejandro, esto nunca lo hubiese podido lograr sin ustedes. Son mi major inspiración! Gracias

por creer en mi en todo momento y por siempre asegurarme de que si lo puedo lograr. Ningún

objetivo es imposible cuando uno está rodeado de personas tan increíbles. Esto es por ustedes, y

gracias a ustedes.

Acknowledgments

I would like to acknowledge my advisor, Dr. Catia Cividini-Motta for her ongoing help

and feedback, making this study possible. I would also like to acknowledge my thesis committee

members, Dr. Kimberly Crosland and Dr. Raymond Miltenberger for their advice and

knowledgeable input towards this study. Lastly, I would like to acknowledge Dr. Claudia

Campos Fleitas for her continuous help and support, throughout this entire process.

i

Table of Contents

List of Tables ................................................................................................................................... ii List of Figures ................................................................................................................................. iii Abstract ........................................................................................................................................... iv Introduction ..................................................................................................................................... 1 Method ............................................................................................................................................. 8 Participants, Settings, and Materials ................................................................................... 8 Response Measurement ....................................................................................................... 9 Reliability of the Observation and Treatment Integrity .................................................... 12 Experimental Design ......................................................................................................... 16 Phase 1:Pre-Asssessments ................................................................................................. 16 Edible Preference Assessments ............................................................................. 16 Communication Modality Preference Assessments .............................................. 16 Essential for Living Communication Modality Assessment ................................. 17 Phase 2: Mand Modality Evaluation ................................................................................. 19 Mand Selection ...................................................................................................... 19 Baseline ................................................................................................................. 19 Mand Training ....................................................................................................... 20 Results……………………..... ...................................................................................................... 23 Discussion ...................................................................................................................................... 33 References ..................................................................................................................................... 37 Appendices .................................................................................................................................... 42 Appendix A: Identifying a Vocal Profile .......................................................................... 43 Appendix B: Testing for Repertoire’s ............................................................................... 44 Appendix C: Task Analysis-Multiple Stimuli Without Replacement Preference Assessment ........................................................................................................................ 45 Appendix D: Task Analysis-Mand Training ..................................................................... 46 Appendix E: Task Analysis- EFL Communication Modality Assessment ....................... 47 Appendix F: Results from the EFL Website that recommends a communication Modality ......................................................................................... …………………….48 Appendix G: Chart that the EFL handbook provides to depict what each letter stands for ...................................................................................................................................... 49 Appendix H: Format of each mode of communication for each participant ..................... 50 Appendix I: USF IRB Approval Letter ............................................................................. 51

ii

List of Tables

Table 1: Average and Range IOA for each Assessment for all Participants ............................. 15 Table 2: Average and Range Treatment Integrity for each Assessment for all Participants ..... 15 Table 3: Summary of results of EFL communication modality and modality preference assessments .................................................................................................................................... 27 Table 4: Summary of the EFL Assessment of Current Repertoire (Percentage of Correct Responding) and of the caregiver interview .................................................................................. 27 Table 5: Summary of the 30-min structured observation for the EFL Assessment of Vocal Profile (Percentage of Correct Responding) and vocal profile identified for each participant ..... 28 Table 6: Summary of the parent interview for the EFL Assessment of Vocal Profile from Appendix A Section II ................................................................................................................... 28

iii

List of Figures

Figure 1: Results for Daphne’s, Daniel’s, and Walter’s Preference Assessments ...................... 20

Figure 2: Results of the first mand training for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter ............................. 30

Figure 3: Results of the second mand training for Daniel, and Walter ....................................... 31

Figure 4: Results for the Communication Modality Preference Assessment for Daphne, Daniel,

and Walter .................................................................................................................................... 32

iv

Abstract

McGreevy, Fry, and Cornwall (2014) developed an assessment within the Essential for

Living (EFL) manual for clinicians to identify which communication modality should be used

for each individual. This assessment identifies an AAC based on the learner’s skills, level of

problem behavior, similarities between AAC and vocal community, and size of the verbal

community. However, to date, no research has evaluated if this assessment identifies the

communication modality that will result in faster acquisition of mands in individuals with ASD.

Thus, the purpose of this study was to compare acquisition of mands across a modality identified

by the EFL communication modality assessment and two other commonly used modalities. A

secondary purpose was to determine if participants acquire mands using the mode of AAC

identified by EFL. Finally, a third purpose was to determine if the communication modality

identified by EFL communication modality assessment matches the modality currently used by

the individual. Findings showed that although all three participants acquired mands across the

three communication modalities, mands in the modality of communication recommended by the

EFL assessment were acquired faster only by 1 out of the 3 participants.

1

Introduction

According to the Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2014), 1 in 68 children are

diagnosed with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Moreover, 25 to 35% of individuals with an

ASD have limited speech (Lord & Jones, 2012), which is a common characteristic of the

disorder (Filipek et al., 1999). Individuals who do not have proficient vocal repertoires qualify

for an augmentative and alternative communication (AAC). The AAC used with these

individuals typically consists of picture-based communication systems, manual signs (MS) , or

speech-generating devices (SGDs; Tager-Flusberg & Kasari, 2013; Mirenda, 2003; Wodka,

Mathy, & Kalb, 2013). Although various modes of AAC are available and many individuals

become proficient at using these modes, acquisition of mands for some individuals is very slow

or limited potentially because the mode of AAC assigned or selected for these individuals was

inappropriate. In these cases, behavior analysts may change teaching procedures or the mode of

AAC in attempts to foster acquisition. To date, few methods for selecting an AAC modality are

available. Therefore, additional research on procedures for selecting a method of AAC that is

appropriate and results in acquisition of mands is warranted.

The different modes of AAC can be used to develop a variation of verbal operants, such

as mands, tacts, intraverbal, or echoics. However, in this study, we focused on mands because

they are the basis of all other verbal behavior. Skinner (1957) described the mand as a verbal

operant in which a response is reinforced by a distinctive consequence. Meaning that the

response is under the functional control of similar conditions of deprivation or unpleasant

stimulation. The mand is the verbal operant in which the reinforcer is specified. Moreover, a

2

mand involves requesting access to reinforcer, independent of whether or not it is present.

Furthermore, mands are sometimes classified as pure or impure mands (Skinner, 1957). Mands

that are under the control of the establishing operation (EO; Michael, 1982) are only pure mands.

An impure mand, on the other hand, is controlled by an EO and a vocal (e.g., “What do you

want?”) or a visual (e.g., presence of the item) prompt.

Often, individuals vocally state their wants and needs. However, individuals who have

limited vocal communication skills may express their wants and needs in a different mode of

communication (e.g., picture exchanges, MS, SGDs). Picture exchanges (PE) is a selection-based

communication (Sundberg & Sundberg, 1990) that consists of manual exchanges of pictures

cards that are laminated and are part of a picture exchange book. Pictures may be of different

sizes depending on the characteristics of the users and typically attach to a picture book with

Velcro™. Individuals exchange these pictures to mand for their wants and needs. For example,

Couper et al. (2014), used PE to teach children with ASD how to mand for toys. Their findings

showed that seven out of nine children successfully learned to request for preferred toys using

PE.

Another communication modality that has been evaluated in the literature is MS. Manual

signs are characterized as an unaided communication technique and is considered topography-

based communication. Multiple studies have contributed empirical support for the use of manual

sign manding in generating a functional communication repertoire when there is not any

appropriate verbal behavior (e.g., Gregory, DeLeon, & Richman, 2009; Elias, Goyos, Saunders,

& Saunders, 2008). A limited body of research has also suggested that MS language may

facilitate the development of vocal responding (Schlosser & Wendt, 2008). However, Schlosser

3

and Wendt (2008) suggested that these effects may be narrowed to those children who have

already developed a modest vocal imitation repertoire.

Individuals with an ASD may also be taught to communicate using SGDs. These consist

of a hardware, such as an iPad® or another transportable device, containing an application that

generates speech (e.g., Proloquo®). These devices are becoming progressively prominent and are

sometimes preferred over nonelectronic systems such as Picture Exchange Communication

Systems (PECS; Gevarter et al., 2013; van der Meer, Sigafoos, O’Reilly, & Lancioni, 2011).

Although supplementary research is necessary to determine all variables that affect this

preference, Son, Sigafoos, O’Reilly, and Lancioni (2006) suggested that this preference may be

due to some characteristics of the devices themselves (e.g., shape, color, size, or voice output).

Furthermore, SGDs can be understood by the entire verbal community of the individual. In

contrast, when individuals use other forms of communication (e.g., MS) their effective

communication is limited to the verbal community who understands MS. Moreover, because

SGDs produce a speech output dependent on a communicator-initiated response, the need for a

listener to model a vocal response, such as when using picture exchanges (PE), may be

eliminated. Additionally, SGD-based interventions may result in an increase in independent

vocalizations. For instance, Gevarter et al. (2016) found that for three out of four participants, the

addition of vocal language instructional methods to an SGD-based intervention led to an increase

in independent vocalizations.

Acquisition of an AAC may be affected by factors such as preference, prerequisite skills,

practice opportunities, and response effort (Sigafoos, O’Reilly, & Lancioni, 2006). The

preference and rate of acquisition for MS, PE, and SGDs was compared, using four children with

an ASD, in a study by Van der Meer, Sutherland, O’Reilly, Lancioni, and Sigafoos (2012). The

4

mands were acquired in at least one of the communication modalities, by all four of the

participants. However, the results demonstrated that the individuals attained mastery criteria at

different rates, depending on the type of communication modality used. Additionally, the

participants displayed a preference for the specific modality for which faster acquisition and

better maintenance was obtained. In a similar study, Van der Meer, Sutherland, O’Reilly, and

Lancioni (2012) found that all participants preferred an iPod touch® over PE and MS. Moreover,

all four participants acquired mands with the iPod touch® and the PE, while only two

participants acquired mands with the MS. Moreover, Gevarter et al. (2013), reviewed several

studies evaluating rate of skill acquisition and preference across PE, SGDs, and MS (e.g., Beck

et al., 2008; Bock et al., 2005). These studies suggested that there are benefits in appraising an

individual’s choice for using distinct types of communication devices. However, there are no

studies exploring variables that might have a repercussion on that preference.

Although there are no known variables as to why some individuals prefer one AAC over

another, there are some prerequisites that should be taken into consideration when choosing an

AAC. Tincani (2004) found that for individuals without hand-motor imitation skills, PE might be

the best AAC option for initial mand acquisition. For individuals with limited hand-motor

imitation skills, sign language might be just appropriate to teach. In another study, Gregory et al.

(2009) also found that preexistence of motor imitation and matching skills made a difference in

the acquisition of manual signs and PE. In this study, the three individuals that possessed these

two skills, quickly learned how to use both AAC modalities. In contrast, the other two of three

individuals who did not possess these skills did not acquire the response forms. The last

individual acquired exchange-based responses but not MS.

5

The results from these studies demonstrate that programming developed to teach

communication skills to individuals with an ASD should be individualized and that clinicians

should carefully consider the mode of communication prescribed. Currently, a few methods for

selecting an AAC modality are available. For instance, LaRue et al. (2016) evaluated a model to

identify which communication modality represents the best match for individuals in need of

communication intervention. This model consisted of teaching students to tact using various

AAC modalities to determine which AAC resulted in the fastest acquisition. Their results were

consistent with previous findings demonstrating that individuals have clear preferences for one

modality and that usually the preferred modality is associated with faster acquisition (Van der

Meer et al., 2012).

Bryen, Goldman, and Quinslik-Gill (1988) described an additional method for selecting

an AAC modality known as partner competence. This method emphasizes choosing an AAC that

others in the environment (e.g., teachers, parents, or caregivers) can teach and reinforce

adequately in the natural settings. However, it does not consider the individuals’ current skills.

Another method for selecting a mode of AAC includes observing the communicative needs of

the individual, environmental demands, and contextual characteristics (Jones, Jollef,

McConachie, & Wisbeach, 1990). These procedures consist of assessing the individual’s

mobility, posture, hand function, vision, ocular-motor, hearing, symbolic and verbal

comprehension, and communicative functions and modes. In addition, the procedure stresses that

an undetected problem in one of these areas can lead to the collapse of a planned intervention

program. Thus, it provides a step-by-step assessment that facilitates decision-making during the

selection of an AAC modality. This method is advantageous because it accounts for variability

across skills in different children however this method does not take into considerations other

6

factors that may impact acquisition and use of AAC such as level of problem behavior and size

of verbal community.

Finally, McGreevy, Fry, and Cornwall (2014) published an assessment handbook called

Essential for Living ® (EFL). This handbook is a communication, behavior, functional skills

curriculum, and skills tracking instrument for people with moderate-to-severe disabilities.

Essential for Living can be used to identify deficits in functional skills, develop meaningful goals

and behavior plans, and to keep record of problem behavior as well as skill acquisition.

Additionally, EFL is the first instrument of its sort that is established on concepts, principles, and

procedures from Applied Behavior Analysis and that incorporates speaking and listening skills

that derive from B.F. Skinner’s analysis of verbal behavior (Skinner, 1957). Furthermore, EFL

contains a communication modality assessment that considers the learner’s skills, level of

problem behavior, similarities between AAC and vocal community, and size of verbal

community; it also makes recommendations for primary and secondary modes of AAC. This

method provides guidelines for choosing primary, secondary, back-up, and concurrent methods

of communication. It also provides procedures to identify the efficacy of the chosen method of

communication. In this assessment, an individual is ‘matched’ with distinct methods of

communication modalities based on their current repertoire, each modality of communication’s

overlap with characteristics of vocal communication, and the size of the verbal community.

However, to date, there is no research to support if this assessment identifies a communication

modality that will result in acquisition of mands.

In summary, it is important to teach individuals with little or no functional speech how to

communicate. The EFL communication assessment provides a systematic method for identifying

at least one alternative mode of communication for individuals with disabilities. However, to

7

date, there is no research evaluating this assessment. Thus, the purpose of this study was to

compare acquisition of mands across a modality identified by the EFL communication modality

assessment and two other commonly used modalities. A secondary purpose was to determine if

participants acquired mands using the mode of AAC identified by EFL. Finally, a third purpose

was to determine if the communication modality identified by the EFL communication modality

assessment matches the modality currently used by the individual.

8

Method

Participants, Settings, and Materials

Three children participated in this study. Criteria for participation included a limited

vocal repertoire as reported by caregivers. This information was obtained through a

questionnaire that parents of potential participants completed during the screening process. The

questionnaire asked if the individual used an AAC and if so, which one and how it was selected.

A pseudonym was used for each participant. Daphne was a 4-year-old girl with an ASD. Daphne

used PE as her primary mode of communication and it was selected by her Board Certified

Behavior Analyst (BCBA). She manded for a limited number of items when prompted. Daniel

was a 3-year-old boy with an ASD. Daniel’s primary mode of communication was also PE

selected by his BCBA. He also had a limited communication repertoire consisting of mands for a

few items but these were always prompted. Walter was an 8-year-old boy diagnosed with an

ASD. Walter used an SGD at school and the SGD was selected by his parents. He also only

manded for items when prompted by his caregivers and teachers. At the time of this study, none

of the participants engaged in independent mands.

Sessions were conducted in therapy rooms inside of ABA clinics and in the participants’

homes. For Daphne and Daniel all sessions were conducted at the clinic where they received

ABA therapy. For Daphne, the therapy room was approximately 3 m X 2 m and contained two

tables, three chairs, and a cabinet. For Daniel, the therapy room was approximately 2 m X 2 m.

For Walter some of the sessions were conducted in his bedroom at his house which was

approximately 3 m X 4 m and contained a full size bed, one nightstand, a table, two chairs, a toy

9

bin, and a dresser. Other sessions were conducted in a therapy room at the clinic where he

received ABA therapy. This room was approximately 8 m X 4 m and contained one table, two

chairs, a TV, a ball pit, two beanbag chairs, a play tent, and a couch. Participants were recruited

through flyers posted around USF campus, distributed via email, and sent to ABA agencies that

provided a letter of support. Materials included edibles used during the edible and

communication modality preference assessments and mand training. In addition, we used

different pictures for the PE modality and iPads® programed with ProloquoTM for the SGD

modality. We also used a 7 cm X 10 cm picture of each modality for the modality preference

assessment. The picture for PE consisted of a PE book, the picture for the SGD consisted of an

iPad® with ProloquoTM on the screen, and the picture for MS consisted of the American Sign

Language (ASL) MS for “more”. Paper and pencil were used to record data and a video camera

was used to record some of the sessions.

Response Measurement

During the edible and communication modality preference assessments, data were

collected on the participants’ selections of foods and modes of communication, respectively.

Selection was defined as the participant touching, pointing to, or grabbing one of the items (or

picture) presented in the array within 5 s of the onset of the trial. During the edible preferrence

assessment, data were summarized using a point weighting scoring method similar to the one

described by Ciccone, Graff, and Ahearn (2005). That is, points were assigned to each item

based on the trial in which it was selected during each session of the preferred assessment (e.g.,

if there were 6 trials in one session, trial 1 would result in 6 points, trial 2 would result in 5

points). Then, the scores received by each item in each of the sessions were added together to

give a total score for each item. Items that received 75% or more of the possible points were

10

defined as highly preferred. The results of the communication modality preference assessment

were summarized as percentage of trials during which each mode was selected by adding the

total number of times a communication modality was selected, dividing by the number of trials

that it was available, and multiplying by 100.

During the EFL communication modality assessments, data were collected on the

participants’ current sensory, skill, and behavioral repertoires (e.g., matching, hearing, seeing;

see Appendix B) that matched those recommended for each alternative method of

communication. Each of these responses was operationally defined (see Appendix B). Sessions

consisted of five trials and we collected data on the percentage of trials in which the participant

correctly emitted each target response (e.g., matching, hearing, seeing). If the participant emitted

the target response in at least 80% of the trials in one session we deemed that that skill was part

of the participant’s repertoire.

During the mand modality evaluation, we collected data on the percentage of trials per

session with correct prompted and independent mands as well as errors across the different

communication modalities. Mands were defined as the emission of the target response in the

prescribed modality (e.g., PE, SGD, MS) in the presence of the preferred item and within 5 s of a

vocal prompt “what do you want?” Furthermore, mands were defined for each mode of

communication. For instance, in the PE condition, a mand was defined as the participant handing

the picture depicting the preferred item over to the therapist. For the SGD condition, a mand was

defined as the participant touching the icon corresponding to the preferred item with enough

force to generate audio in the iPad® programed with ProloquoTM. For the MS condition, a mand

was defined as the participant making the MS for the preferred item (e.g., making hands and

fingers into the correct form and placing them in the correct part of the body). An independent

11

mand was defined as any instance in which the participant emitted the correct response in the

correct modality without a prompt (e.g., emitted the correct MS). A prompted mand was defined

as the participant emitting the correct response in the correct modality with a prompt (e.g., using

hand over hand to guide the participant into handing over the correct card). Finally, an incorrect

response was defined as any instance in which the participant did not emit a correct response

(e.g., touched the incorrect icon on the SGD) or did not emit any response within 5 s of the vocal

prompt, “What do you want?” Percentage of trials with independent or prompted mands and

errors were calculated by totaling the number of trials with each type of responses, dividing by

the number of trials during each session, and multiplying by 100.

During the mand selection assessment, data were collected on independent mands as

these data were summarized as percentage of trials with independent mands. During baseline,

data were collected on the participants’ independent mands using each modality until stable

responding or a decreasing trend was observed during the last three sessions. At the beginning of

the session the participants had access to the materials needed for each of the communication

modalities to be assessed during mand training. Each baseline session consisted of 10 trials and

items for which the participant did not emit a mand correctly, in either mode of communication,

were selected for training. To control for difficulty level across communication modalities we

applied the following rules:

1. PE and SGD icons were identical (e.g., same size, pictures of actual object, colored

pictures, same placement of icons)

2. PE and SGD were placed in the same location at the beginning of each session to

control for the response effort required to retrieve the communication device and emit

a mand

12

3. PE and SGD were placed within 0.3 m of the participant

4. Because the target response in PE and SGD required the participant to touch or hand

over one icon, signs taught consisted of one discrete hand movement

Reliability of the Observation System and Treatment Integrity

Graduate students served as therapists and data collectors. Training was delivered in-

person and consisted of behavior skills training (BST; Miltenberger, 2012). Observers had to

obtain 90% or above agreement for a mock session before collecting reliability data.

Interobserver agreement (IOA) was calculated for at least 33% of sessions across all phases for

all participants (see Table 1). The observers independently scored sessions by directly observing

the session or by reviewing recorded footage of the sessions. Across all phases and assessments

IOA was calculated on a trial-by-trial basis by dividing the number of trials with agreement by

the total number of trials and multiplying the results by 100. For the edible preference

assessments IOA was calculated for 40% of sessions for each participant and the mean score was

100% for Daphne, 96% (range, 83-100%) for Daniel, and 100% for Walter. For the

communication modality preference assessment, IOA for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter was

calculated for 50% of the sessions. For Daphne, IOA was 100%. For Diego, IOA was 95%. For

Walter, IOA was 100%. During the EFL communication modality assessment IOA was

calculated for 36% of the assessment sessions. The average IOA for Daphne was 90% (range,

60-100%), and for Daniel and Walter it was 100%. For the first mand training, IOA for Daphne

was calculated for 36% of the sessions and the average IOA was 95% (range, 90-100%). For

Daniel and Walter, it was calculated for 40% of the sessions and the average IOA was 95%

(range, 90-100%) and 97% (range, 90-100%) respectively. For the second mand training, IOA

for Daniel was calculated for 50% of the sessions and the average IOA was 98% (range, 90-

13

100%). For Walter IOA was calculated for 33% of the sessions and the average IOA was 98%

(range 90-100%).

Treatment integrity was assessed for at least 30% of all sessions across all phases for all

participants (see Table 2). A task analysis describing the steps to be completed during each of the

assessments was used. During the edible and communication modality preference assessment

data were collected on whether or not the therapist had all materials necessary, delivered the

correct instructions, provided adequate amount of time for participant to select one of the items,

and provided correct consequences (see Appendix C). For Daphne, Daniel, and Walter, treatment

integrity for the edible preference assessments was collected on 40% of all sessions. For Daphne,

the average treatment integrity was 100%, for Daniel, the average treatment integrity was 96%

(range, 92-100%), and for Walter, the average treatment integrity was 94% (range, 85-100%). In

addition, for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter treatment integrity for the communication modality

preference assessments was collected on 50% of all sessions. For Daphne and Daniel, the

treatment integrity was 100%, for Walter, the average treatment integrity was 96%, During the

EFL communication modality assessment, we evaluated whether the observer had all the

necessary materials, set the occasion for all repertoires’ being evaluated, and provided adequate

amount of time for the individual to engage in the behavior being observed. Treatment integrity

was assessed for 36% of all sessions for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter. Treatment integrity was

100% for all three participants.

We also assessed treatment integrity of the implementation of the procedures during the

mand modality evaluation by using a task analysis that states whether or not the therapist

prompted the participants to emit the correct mands during each trial, provided the item

immediately after the participants engaged in the mand, allowed time for the participant to

14

consume the edible for a maximum of one min. Treatment integrity for the mand selection

assessment was calculated for 33% of the sessions and treatment integrity was 100% for Daphne,

Daniel, and Walter. Combined treatment integrity scores were calculated across baseline and

mand training. For Daphne, it was assessed for 36% of the sessions and the average treatment

integrity was 98% (range, 92-100%). For Daniel, treatment integrity was assessed for 43%, and

the average treatment integrity was 98% (range, 92-100%). For Walter, treatment integrity was

assessed for 38% of the sessions and the average treatment integrity was 99% (range, 97-100%).

15

Table 1. Mean and Range IOA for each Assessment for all Participants

Table 2. Mean and Range Treatment Integrity for each assessment for all participants

First Mand Training Baseline

First Mand Training Treatment

Second Mand Training Baseline

Second Mand Training Treatment

Vocal Profile Structured Observation

Repertoire Structured Observation

Mand Selection

Edible PA Communication modality PA

Daphne

Daniel

100% on 33% of sessions 95% (range, 90-100%) on 40% of sessions

93% (range, 90-100%) on 38% of sessions 95% (range, 90-100%) on 40% of sessions

N/A 95% (range, 90-100%) on 50% of sessions

N/A 100% on 50% of sessions 95%(range, 90-100%) on 33% of sessions

60% on 33% of sessions 100% on 33% of sessions

100% on 38% of sessions 100% on 38% of sessions

100% on 33% of sessions 100% on 33% of sessions

100% on 40% of sessions 96% (range, 83-100) on 40% of sessions

100% on 50% of sessions 95% on 50% of sessions

Walter

97% (range, 90-100%) on 38% of sessions

97 (range, 90-100%) on 43% of sessions

100% on 33% of sessions

100% on 33% of sessions

100% on 38% of sessions

100% on 33% of sessions

100% on 40% of sessions

100% on 50% of sessions

16

Experimental Design

A nonconcurrent multiple probe (Horner & Baer, 1978) across participants with an

adapted alternating treatments design was used in this study. We compared acquisition of mands

across three communication modalities (e.g., PE, MS, and SGD). Training was staggered across

participants. Training began with Daphne when three probes with each modality were completed

and data were on a stable trend. Training began with Daniel when Daphne had acquired at least

one of the mands and it began with Walter when Daniel had acquired at least one of the mands.

Mands were assigned to one of the communication modalities in a semi-random manner. All

mands selected for the mand modality evaluation were for items that received at least 75% of the

points during the preference assessments.

Phase 1: Pre-Assessments

Edible Preference Assessments. Preference assessments were conducted for all

participants to identify preferred items to use during the mand modality evaluations. Items used

during the preference assessments were identified through the Reinforcer Assessment for

Individuals with Severe Disability (RAISD; Fisher, Piazza, Bowman, & Amari, 1996) that were

completed by caregivers, a member of the clinical team, or both. We conduced multiple stimuli

without replacement preference assessments (MSWO; DeLeon & Iwata, 1996) for all

participants. Items that received 75% or more of the possible points were selected for the mand

modality evaluation.

Communication Modality Preference Assessment. Preference assessments were

conducted for all participants to assess whether they had a preference towards either of the

communication modalities used during mand training. These were completed before and after the

mand training assessment and consisted of a trial-based preference assessment method. Prior to

17

conducting choice trials of the preference assessments trials, we completed a series of five

exposure trials for each of communication modalities. In addition, we completed two forced

exposure trials immediately prior to each preference assessment session. During forced exposure

trials participants were prompted to use the communication modality by using hand-over-hand

prompting and mands resulted in access to the specific item. During the choice trials the three

communication modalities were placed in a row on the table approximately 0.3 m in front of the

participant and 0.1m apart from each other. In addition, pictures of each of the modalities were

placed in front of the participant and the participant was instructed to choose one. Upon

selection, the participant was given an opportunity to mand for the corresponding preferred item

and then access to that item was provided.

The three communication modalities were available during each trial and data were

collected on which mode the participant selected. The initial preference assessment trials were

embedded within the sessions of the mand selection. That is, at the beginning of each mand

selection trial we presented the participant with the pictures of the three modes of

communication, allowed the participant to make a selection, then completed that trial of the

mand selection session. This process was repeated until we completed 10 choice trials. For the

remainder of the mand selection session, during each subsequent trial, just one mode of

communication was presented to the participant. The final preference assessment was completed

following mand training and consisted of 10 choice trials.

Essentials for Living Communication Modality Assessment. The EFL communication

modality assessment was conducted as following: First, we identified a vocal profile for each of

the participants based on the six vocal profiles provided by the EFL. This vocal profile was

selected based on the individuals’ spoken-word repertoire. This consisted of evaluating if the

18

individual had any spoken words and if they did, evaluating if these words were spontaneous,

understandable, frequent, or a combination of these. The observer also evaluated if the

individuals’ spoken word repetition was controlled, uncontrolled, understandable, or a

combination of these. For this part of the assessment, we conducted an interview with the parents

(see Appendix A, Section I) and we conducted a 30-min structured observation (see Appendix A,

Section II). We then selected an alternative method of communication for participants whose

vocal profile was in the range of three to six. If participants fell in between two vocal profiles,

we selected the highest vocal profile (e.g., if they fell in between vocal profiles 2 and 3, we

selected vocal profile 3).

The alternative method of communication was selected by evaluating the following

sensory, skill, and behavioral repertoires: hearing, sight, walking, activity level, fine motor

coordination, motor imitation, matching, and level of problem behavior. Observers evaluated

these repertoires by conducting an interview with the parents (see Appendix B, Section I) and

conducting a 30-min observation (see Appendix B, Section II). Individuals were then matched to

an alternative method of communication by comparing their current repertoire to that

recommended by the EFL handbook for each of the communication modalities. As per the EFL,

we also considered the advantages and disadvantages of each alternative communication

modality in comparison to vocal communication (e.g., portability, effort, complexity,

communication skills) and the size of the person’s verbal community for each of the potential

AACs. We selected an AAC by first choosing the modality with the most repertoire matches. If

more than one modality had the same number of matches, we then selected the modality that was

most similar to vocal communication (e.g., same advantages). If we still had more than one

modality, we then selected the modality with the largest audience. Finally, if necessary, we

19

considered caregiver and clinician preference and AAC availability in selecting a mode of AAC

for the participant. Once we identified an alternative mode of communication based on the EFL

assessment, we selected two other commonly used modes. Examples of common practice AACs

include SGD, PE, MS, and the Picture Exchange Communication System (PECS, Charlop,

Carpenter, LeBlanc, & Kellet, 2002).)

Phase 2: Mand Modality Evaluation

Mand selection. Once preferred items were identified, we assessed whether the child

manded independently for these items using any of the chosen modalities of communication. For

this assessment all preferred items were available but out of reach of the participant. The session

lasted until each participant had 10 opportunities to mand with each of the communication

modalities. However, during the first 10 trials, the participant was allowed to choose one of the

communication modalities as previously described under the communication modality

preference assessment. A trial began when the therapist presented the preferred edible, within

eyesight but out of reach, to the participant. If the participant did not make a request within 5 s,

the therapist delivered a vocal prompt, “if you want something, please let me know”. The

participant was given 30 s to make a request and then another trial began. An opportunity to

mand was scored when the participant attempted to access one of the items, the participant used

the communication mode to attempt to request the item, and anytime the therapist delivered the

vocal prompt, “if you want something, please let me know”. Items for which no independent

mands were emitted in any mode of communication were selected for the mand training. Mands

were assigned to one of the communication modalities in a quasi-random basis.

Baseline. During baseline, all communication modalities were randomly paired with a

specific mand. Each session consisted of 10 trials. At the beginning of each trial, the therapist

20

ensured that the participant had access to the target communication modality (e.g., PE, MS, or

SGD), presented the preferred item at the end of the table in front of the participant but out of

reach, and a vocal prompt (e.g., “What do you want?”). The participant’s manding repertoire

with each of the different communication modalities was assessed in different sessions and the

sequence in which the communication modalities were presented was semi-random. This was

done by writing on a piece of paper the name of each communication modality and placing the

pieces of paper inside of a bag. The observer then picked a piece of paper from the bag and a

session of the selected modality was conducted. After one session had been completed for each

communication modality, the pieces of paper were returned to the bag and the process was

repeated. If the participant emitted the correct response, the item was provided to the participant

until the item was consumed. All sessions were terminated after 10 trials had been completed.

No programmed consequences were provided for problem behavior. However, if no correct

responding was emitted, praise was provided for appropriate session behavior every three trials

in an attempt to minimize the likelihood of problem behavior occurring due to an extended

period without access to reinforcers. Items with low levels of independent responding (no more

than 20% of trials) during baseline were selected for mand training.

Mand training. We conducted the mand training evaluation once for Daphne, and twice

for Daniel and Walter. We used most-to-least prompting to teach all mands across the three

communication modalities. The communication modalities used during this evaluation were PE,

MS, and SGDs (See Appendix H). Mand training sessions were completed separately for each

mode of communication and the sequence was presented semi-randomly as described in

baseline. During mand training sessions the preferred item was continuously available during the

session, within view but out of reach of the participant. Sessions included 10 trials and each trial

21

began either with a vocal prompt, “What do you want?”, or when the participant initiated a mand

for the preferred item, without the vocal prompt. For the PE and SGD, after 10 s had passed, if

the participant had failed to respond correctly, the observer physically prompted the participant

to use the communication modality appropriately by using hand-over-hand physical prompting,

and reinforced the behavior immediately after completing the mand. Hand-over-hand prompts

were faded into gesture prompts which consisted of the therapist touching the correct icon on the

PE or SGD. Similarly, for the MS sessions, after 10s had passed, the observer physically

prompted the participant to make the appropriate sign by using hand-over-hand prompting.

Hand-over-hand prompts were faded into gesture prompts which consisted of the therapist

pointing to the participant’s body part that the participant was required to touch to emit the

correct sign (i.e., cheek when the MS was candy). Criterion to fade prompts was at least 50%

independent mands in one session. If the participant independently manded for the preferred

item, no prompting was used. Therapists reinforced all prompted and independent mands

immediately after they were emitted. The participant was provided with a piece of an edible item

until they consumed it. After the participant consumed the item, the next trial started. Errors

resulted on removal of all session materials and then a full prompt to emit the correct response.

No reinforcers were delivered for mands emitted during the error correction procedure. Mastery

criteria was two sessions with at least 90% correct and independent mands. However, once

mastery criteria was met for one modality, additional sessions were completed to ensure that

performance of the mastered mand persisted and to determine whether, given additional training

sessions, the participant would acquire the other mands. Thus, we conducted mand training

sessions until each participant acquired mands across all 3 modes of communication.

22

Picture exchange (PE; see Appendix H). During this condition, a picture(s) of the

target mand was placed in front of the participant and was centered on the table 0.3 m away. For

Daphne, a picture of Smarties® was used for the mand training. For Daniel, a picture of

Smarties® was used for the first mand training and a picture of M&M® was used for the second

mand training. For Walter, a picture of Candy Corn® was used for the first mand training and a

picture of Smarties® was used for the second mand training.

Speech generating device (SGD; see Appendix H). In this condition, the participants

had access to their SGD which consisted of an iPad® with the Proloquo® application. At the

beginning of sessions, the application was opened and the screen showed the page containing the

icon of the target mand. The iPad® was placed centered on the table 0.3 m away from the

participant. For Daphne, an icon of gummies was used for the mand training. For Daniel, an icon

of Starburst® was used for the first mand training and an icon of Swedish Fish® was used for

the second mand training. For Walter, an icon of Sour Patch® was used for the first mand

training and an icon of Skittles® was used for the second mand training.

Manual sign (MS; see Appendix H). No additional materials were present during this

condition. For Daphne, Skittles® were used for this modality and she had to perform the

American Sign Language MS for candy. For Daniel, Candy Corn® was used for the first mand

training, Skittles® were used for the second mand training, and he had to perform a modified

version of the American Sign Language MS for candy to mand for both of these items. For

Walter, Starburst® were used for the first mand training and he had to perform the MS for candy.

Nutter Butter® was used for the second mand training and he had to perform the MS for cookie.

23

Results

Results for Daphne’s, Daniel’s, and Walter’s preference assessments are depicted in

Figure 1. Smarties®, Skittles®, and gummies were used for Daphne’s mand training because

they were the three items that received at least 75% of the points. Unfortunately, Daphne moved

away before the experiment finished. Thus, we did not conduct a second preference assessment

with her. Two preference assessments were conducted with Daniel and Walter to identify at least

six preferred items to be used during the initial mand training and the mand training replication.

For Daniel, Candy Corn®, Starburst®, and Smarties® were used for the first mand training and

Swedish Fish®, M&M®, and Skittles® were used for the second mand training because these

were the items that were obtained at least 75% of the points. For Walter, gummies, Candy

Corn®, and Starburst® were used for the first mand training and Sour Patch®, Nutter Butter®,

and Starburst® were used for the second mand training because they obtained 75% of the points.

During the EFL communication modality assessment we first identified each participant’s

vocal profile. This was done based on the data attained through the caregiver interview and the

structured observation of each of the participants (see Table 5 & 6). Parents reported that

Daphne, Daniel, and Walter could say 5-10, 5-10, and 10-15 words respectively and that these

were spontaneous. In addition, Daphne, Daniel, and Walter spoke these words 10-15, 5-10, and

10-15 times per day respectively but that unfamiliar people did not always understand them. All

three participants repeated these words in a nonfunctional way, and their word repetition was not

understandable by unfamiliar people. According to the interview, only Daniel was able to control

these non-functional spoken-word repetitions. Finally, all three participants could hear, see, were

ambulatory, active, had fine motor imitation, could match pictures to objects, and did not have

24

problem behavior. However only Walter had fine motor coordination. Results of the structured

interview are shown in Table 4. None of the participants emitted any of the target responses

during the observation. Therefore, based on their vocal repertoire they were classified as Vocal

Profile 4, which is defined by the EFL as “Uncontrolled or Controlled Spoken-Word Repetitions

are not understandable”.

Results of the assessment of sensory, skill, and behavioral repertoires are shown in Table

4. All three participants had similar characteristics and repertoires. They were all able to see and

hear, were active and ambulatory, and did not engage in problem behavior. Daphne and Walter

had fine motor imitation and matching skills but neither had fine motor coordination. Daniel did

not have fine motor coordination, fine motor imitation, or matching skills. Alternative modes of

communication were then selected using http://amscompare.com. For each participant we

indicated which skills they had in their repertoire such as seeing, hearing, imitation, ambulatory,

matching, fine motor, as well as problem behavior. The results of the EFL communication

modality assessment are shown in Appendix F. The EFL communication modality assessment

recommended MS for all three participants.

During the mand selection evaluation, none of the participants emitted independent

mands for the preferred edibles. These mands were therefore selected for mand training. Figure 2

depicts results for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter for the first mand training comparison. During

baseline, there were zero levels of independent mands across all communication modalities for

Daphne and Daniel. For Walter there were zero to 20% independent responding levels across all

communication modalities. In contrast, with the implementation of mand training, there was an

increase in the percentages of independent mands across all communication modalities for all

participants. For Daphne, MS was the mode recommended by the EFL assessment and the mode

25

in which she acquired mands in fewer sessions. Daphne met mastery criteria for all three

modalities within eight mand sessions of combined modalities. For Daniel, MS was the mode

recommended by the EFL. By contrast, SGD was the mode he acquired in fewer training

sessions. Daniel met mastery criteria in all three modalities within ten training sessions of

combined modality. Finally, for Walter, MS was also the mode recommended by the EFL. By

contrast, PEs was the mode he acquired in fewer training sessions. Walter met mastery criteria in

all three modalities within seven training sessions of combined modalities. Moreover, for both,

Daniel and Walter, MS required the most number of sessions to mastery.

Figure 3 depicts results for Daniel and Walter for the second mand training

comparison. Daphne was not included in this evaluation because she moved away before this

evaluation could be completed. For Daniel and Walter, results were similar to the first mand

training evaluation and we were able to see a faster acquisition of mands since they developed a

repertoire of generalized mands. During baseline independent mands were at zero to low levels.

Moreover, with the implementation of mand training, percentage of independent mands

increased until mastery criteria was met for all three modalities. For Daniel, SGD was again

acquired in fewer training sessions. However, unlike the results of the first mand training

comparison, MS and PE were acquired in the same number of training sessions. Mastery criteria

for all three modalities was met within four training sessions of each condition. For Walter, in

contrast to the first mand training evaluation where PE required fewer sessions, SGD was

acquired first. However, only one additional training session was required to meet mastery

criteria for PE. Similarly to the first comparison, acquisition of MS required the most training

sessions. Walter required a total of six training sessions of each condition to meet mastery

criteria for all three modalities.

26

Results from the communication modality preference assessments for all participants are

shown in Figure 4. Daphne preferred SGD both before and after training even though MS was

the mode acquired in fewer training sessions. Prior to training Daniel preferred MS. In contrast,

the AAC modality preferred after training was SGD and this was the modality acquired in fewer

training sessions. Finally, prior to training Walter did not show a preference towards any of the

AAC modalities. After training, he demonstrated a stronger preference for the modality in which

he acquired mands in fewer sessions, PE.

Table 3 includes a summary of the EFL communication modality assessment,

communication modality preference assessment, mand modality evaluation, and each

participant’s primary mode of communication prior to enrolling in this study. Daphne was using

PE prior to the study, however, the EFL communication modality assessment recommended MS.

By contrast, Daphne preferred the SGD before and after training even though she acquired

mands using MS faster. Daniel was using PE prior to the study and the EFL communication

modality assessment also recommended MS. Daniel preferred MS before and after the mand

training even though he acquired mands using SGD faster. Walter was using an SGD prior to the

study and the EFL communication modality assessment also recommended MS. Walter did not

have a preference before mand training. However, after mand training, he preferred PE which

was the modality for which he had faster acquisition of mands.

27

Table 3. Summary of the Results of the EFL Communication Modality and Modality Preference Assessments.

AAC Modality

Prior to study Per EFL Preferred

Pre-trg Mastered

First Preferred Post-trg

Daphne PE MS SGD MS SGD

Daniel PE MS MS SGD MS

Walter SGD MS N/A PE PE

Table 4. Summary of the EFL Assessment of Current Repertoire (Percentage of Correct Responding) and of the caregiver interview

28

Table 5. Summary of the 30-min structured observation for the EFL Assessment of Vocal Profile (Percentage of Correct Responding) and vocal profile identified for each participant

Table 6. Summary of the parent interview for the EFL Assessment of Vocal Profile from Appendix A Section II

1. Mands for

preferred edible (within sight)

2. Mands for preferred toy (within sight)

3. Functional vocal

response

Vocal

Profile

Daphne 0 0 0 4

Daniel 0 0 0 4

Walter 0 0 0 4

1. Does your child say any words?

2. If yes, how many?

3. Are these words spontaneous?

4. How many times a day does your child say these words?

5. Are these words understandable by people who don’t know your child?

6. Does your child repeat words?

7. Does your child emit these nonfunctional spoken-word repetitions at appropriate times?

8. When your child repeats words, do people who do not know your child understand these words?

Daphne Yes 5-10 Yes 10-15 Yes Yes Yes No

Daniel Yes 5-10 Yes 1-5 No Yes Yes No

Walter Yes 10-15 Yes 10-15 No Yes No No

29

Edible

Figure 1. Results for Daphne’s, Daniel’s, and Walter’s preference assessment. The Y-axis represents cumulative points that each edible received. The X-axis depicted the items available during the preference assessment. The horizontal line indicates items that obtained 75% of the total points.

30

Figure 2. Results of the first mand training for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter. The Y-axis represents the percentage of independent mands during baseline and mand training. The X-axis represents days in which sessions were conducted. Open triangles represent independent mands emitted using a SGD. Open circles represent independent mands emitted using PE. Closed squares represent independent mands emitted using manual signs.

31

Figure 3. Results of the second mand training for Daniel, and Walter. The Y-axis represents the percentage of independent mands during baseline and mand training. The X-axis represents days in which sessions were conducted. Open triangles represent independent mands emitted using a SGD. Open circles represent independent mands emitted using PE. Closed squares represent independent mands emitted using manual signs.

32

Figure 4. Results for the communication modality preference assessment for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter. Black bars area depicts percentage of trials in which the modality was chosen during the preference assessment completed before mand training. Grey bars depict percentage of trials in which the modality was chosen during the preference assessment completed following mand training. Symbol above bars indicate the AAC modality that was acquired in fewer sessions.

33

Discussion

This study sought to compare acquisition of mands across a modality of communication

recommended by the EFL communication modality assessment and two other commonly used

modalities. Prior to the study, Daphne and Daniel used PEs and Walter used a SGD. However,

the EFL communication modality assessment recommended MS for all three participants.

Therefore, skill acquisition of mands was compared using MS, PE, and SGD. All three

participants acquired the three modalities, however, they did so at different rates. Daphne

acquired MS first, SGD second, and PE last. Daniel acquired SGD first, PE second, and MS last.

Walter acquired PE first, SGD second, and MS last. For Daniel, results were replicated in the

second mand training. Daniel acquired the SGD faster than the other modalities in the first and

second mand training. For Walter, results from the mand training comparisons were very similar.

In addition, another aim of this study was to determine if participants acquired mands

using the mode of AAC identified by EFL communication modality assessment. This was the

case for all three participants as they acquired mands using the modality of communication

recommended by the EFL assessment, MS. Lastly, this study sought to determine if the

communication modality recommended by EFL communication modality assessment was the

same as the modality the participants were using prior to enrolling in this study. This was not the

case for any of the current participants as the EFL assessment recommended MS for all three and

prior to our study Daphne and Daniel were using PE and Walter was using a SGD, respectively.

34

In the current study, Previous research has also investigated the correspondence between

preference for communication modality and rate of acquisition. For instance, Van der Meer et al.

(2012) evaluated acquisition and preference of MS, PE, and SGD in four children with ASD.

Mands were acquired in at least one of the communication modalities by all four participants.

However, results indicated that depending on the type of communication modality used,

participants reached mastery criteria at different rates. They found that all participants had faster

skill acquisition with an iPod touch® and that they preferred the iPod touch® over PE and MS

Similarly, Beck et al. (2008) and Bock et al. (2005) also compared skill acquisition of mands and

results suggested that there are advantages in considering an individual’s choice for using

distinct types of communication devices.

This study also extended on Tincani (2004) which found that PE might be the best AAC

option for initial mand acquisition for individuals without hand-motor imitation skills. In this

study, Walter did not have fine-motor coordination and PE was the modality that he acquired the

fastest.

This study extended the current research on methods for selecting communication

modalities. Previous research tested a model that consisted of teaching students to tact using

various AAC modalities to determine which AAC resulted in the fastest acquisition (LaRue et

al., 2016). Another study used partner competence, which is a method that emphasizes choosing

an AAC that can be taught and reinforced by others (e.g., caregivers) in a natural setting (Bryen,

Goldman, & Quinslik-Gill, 1988). Thus this appears to be the first study to assess the

correspondence between acquisition rate of the mode of communication recommended by the

EFL communication modality assessment and other commonly used modes of communication.

In this study, all three participants had similar characteristics and MS was recommended for all

35

three by the EFL communication modality assessment, however, only Daphne acquired mands

faster using this modality. Moreover, all three participants acquired the three modalities but each

participant had a different modality that resulted in faster acquisition. Therefore this may not be a

feasible mode of AAC selection and additional research needs to be done since the recommended

AAC was not always acquired faster.

Despite its noteworthy contributions to the literature on procedures for selecting

communication modalities, the current study has a few limitations. First, Daphne moved before

the experiment had concluded so we were not able to conduct a second mand training

comparison with her. Second, this study only had three participants and all three participants had

similar characteristics and pre-requisite skills. Therefore, generality of the results are limited to

similar participants. Future research should extend by using more participants to determine the

generality of these findings. Third, participants acquired all mands in similar number of

sessions, thus conclusions about different rate of acquisition are limited. Perhaps future research

should teach more complex mands. In addition, IOA scores for one of the assessment sessions

was 60%. This happened during one of Daphne’s vocal profile structured observation sessions.

This session was only 5 trials, and it assessed whether she manded for preferred items. For this

session, the primary observer selected no for all 5 trials. In contrast, the second observer selected

yes for 2 of the trials potentially because she was Daphne’s ABA therapist and had more

exposure to her. However, the participant had to obtain 80% or higher in the session in order to

account as having the skill, and the primary observer scored 0% while the second observer

scored 40%. Furthermore, mand training included the question “what do you want?” so we

taught impure mands, however previous research assessing acquisition with mands with our

without the inclusion of the “what do you want?” prompt has not found a difference in rate of

36

acquisition (Bowen, Shillinsburg, Carr, 2012). Furthermore, in our attempt to control for

difficulty level across modalities only a few aspects of the AAC devices and training format

were addressed. Future research should also ensure that PE and SGD have the same number of

icons and perhaps that the same icons are presented in both AACs. Moreover, for Daniel the

same MS (candy) was used for both mand trainings because two different types of candy were

found to be highly preferred for him. This was an error in selecting a MS for the second mand

training comparison. Future research should ensure to target different MS across mand training

evaluations. Lastly, the experimental design delayed implementation of sessions. Thus future

research should focus on using a different experimental design to evaluate if it has a difference in

the acquisition of mands.

In summary, the results of this study indicate that the EFL Communication Modality

Assessment may not be a valid way of identifying an effective mode of communication since

results showed that the mode identified by the EFL Communication Modality Assessment was

not the best, based on speed of acquisition for two out of the three participants. Thus, this study

adds to the literature by evaluating a new systematic method for clinicians to select a mode of

communication. However, additional research is needed to compare how this assessment would

work differently with individuals who have different characteristics and do not share the same

pre-requisite skills as the participants in this study.

37

References

Barlow, D. H., & Hayes, S. C. (1979). Alternating treatments design: One strategy for comparing

the effects of two treatments in a single subject. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,

12, 199-210. doi:10.1901/jaba.1979.12-199

Beck, A. R., Stoner, J. B., Bock, S. J., & Parton, T. (2008). Comparison of PECS and the use of a

VOCA: A replication. Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 43, 198–

216.

Bock, S. J., Stoner, J. B., Beck, A. R., Hanley, L., & Prochnow, J. (2005). Increasing functional

communication in non-speaking preschool children: Comparison of PECS and VOCA.

Education and Training in Developmental Disabilities, 40, 264-278

Bondy, A. S., & Frost, L. A. (1994). The picture exchange communication system. Focus on

Autistic Behavior, 9, 1-19. doi:10.1177/108835769400900301

Bryen, D. N., Goldman, A. S., & Quinslik-Gill, S. (1988). Sign Language with students with

severe/profound mental retardation: How effective is it? Education and Training in

Mental Retardation, 23, 129-137.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (n.d.). Autism information center. Retrieved April 1,

2017, from http://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/Autism/

Ciccone, F. J., Graff, R. B., & Ahearn, W. H. (2005). An alternate scoring method for the

multiple stimulus without replacement preference assessment. Behavioral Interventions,

20(2), 121-127. doi:10.1002/bin.177

38

Couper, L., Meer, L. V., Schäfer, M. C., Mckenzie, E., Mclay, L., O’Reilly, M. F.,

Sutherland, D. (2014). Comparing acquisition of and preference for manual signs,

picture exchange, and speech-generating devices in nine children with autism

spectrum disorder. Developmental Neurorehabilitation, 17, 99-109.

doi:10.3109/17518423.2013.870244

DeLeon, I. G., & Iwata, B. A. (1996). Evaluating of a multiple-stimulus presentation format for

assessing reinforcer preferences. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 29, 519-532.

doi:10.1901/jaba.1996.29-519

Doss, S., & Reichle, J. (1991). Replacing excess behavior with an initial communicative

repertoire. In J. Reichle, J. York, & J. Sigafoos, (Eds.), Implementing Augmentative and

Alternative Communication (pp. 215-237). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

Elias, N. C., Goyos, C., Saunders, M., & Saunders, R. (2008). Teaching Manual Signs to Adults

With Mental Retardation Using Matching-to-Sample Procedures and Stimulus

Equivalence. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 1-13. doi:10.1007/bf03393053

Filipek, P. A., Accardo, P. J., Baranek, C. T., Cook, E. H. Jr, Dawson, G., & Gordon, B. (1999).

The screening and diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and

Developmental Disorders, 29, 439-473.

Fisher, W.W. Piazza, C.C., Bowman, L.G., & Amari, A. (1996). Integrating caregiver report

with a systematic choice assessment. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 101, 15-

25.

Fisher, W., Piazza, C. C., Bowman, L. G., Hagopian, L. P., Owens, J. C., & Slevin, I. (1992). A

comparison of two approaches for identifying reinforcers for persons with severe and

39

profound disabilities. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 491-498.

doi:10.1901/jaba.1992.25-491

Gevarter, C., O’Reilly, M. F., Kuhn, M., Mills, K., Ferguson, R., Watkins, L., & Lancioni, G. E.

(2016). Increasing the vocalizations of individuals with autism during intervention with a

speech-generating device. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 49, 17-33.

doi:10.1002/jaba.270

Gevarter, C., O’Reilly, M. F., Rojeski, L., Sammarco, N., Lang, R., Lancioni, G. E., &

Sigafoos, J. (2013) Comparing communication systems for individuals with

developmental disabilities: A review of single-case research studies. Research in

Developmental Disabilities, 34, 4415-4432. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2013.09.017

Gregory, M., DeLeon, I., & Richman, D. (2009). The influence of matching and motor-

imitation abilities on rapid acquisition of manual signs and exchange-based

communication responses. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 42, 399-404.

doi:10.1901/jaba.2009.42-399

Jones, S., Jollef, N., McConachie, H., & Wisbeach, A. (1990). A model for assessment of

children for augmentative communication systems. Child Language Teaching

and Therapy, 6, 305-321. doi:1177/026565909000600306

LaRue, R. H., Pepa, L., Delmolino, L., Sloman, K. N., Fiske, K., Hansford, A., Weiss, M. J.

(2016). A brief assessment for selecting communication modalities for individuals with

Autism Spectrum Disorders. Evidence-Based Communication Assessment and

Intervention, 10, 32-43. doi:10.1080/17489539.2016.1204767

Lord, C., & Jones, R. M. (2012). Annual Research Review: Re-thinking the classification of

40

autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 490-509.

doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02547.x

McGreevy, P., Fry, T., & Cornwall, C. (2014). Essential for Living: a communication, behavior

and functional skills assessment, curriculum and teaching manual for children and adults with

moderate-to-severe disabilities. Orlando, FL: Patrick McGreevy

Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative and motivational functions of stimuli.

Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149-155. doi:10.1901/jeab.1982.37-149

Miltenberger, R. G. (2012). Behavior modification: Principals and procedures (5th ed.). Pacific

Grove, CA: Wadsworth

Mirenda, P. (2003). Toward a functional augmentative and alternate communication for students

with autism: Manual signs, graphic symbols, and voice output communication aids. Language,

Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 34, 203-216. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2003/017)

Pace, G. M., Ivancic, M. T., Edwards, G. L., Iwata, B. A., & Page, T. A. (1985). Assessment of

preference and reinforcer value with profoundly retarded individuals. Journal of Applied

Behavior Analysis, 18, 249-25. doi:10.1901/jaba.1985.18-249

Paclawskyj, T. R., & Vollmer, T. R. (1995). Reinforcer assessment for children with

developmental disabilities and visual impairments. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,

28, 219-224. doi:10.1901/jaba.1995.28-219

Roane, H. S., Vollmer, T. R., Ringdahl, J. E., & Marcus, B. A. (1998). Evaluation of a brief

stimulus preference assessment. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 605-620.

doi:10.1901/jaba.1998.31-605

Schlosser, R., Blischak, D. M., & Koul, R. (2003). Roles of speech-output in AAC. In R.

Schlosser (Ed.), The efficacy of augmentative and alternative communication:

41

Toward evidence-based practice (pp. 471-532). San Diego: Academic Press.

Schlosser, R., & Wendt, O. (2008). Effects of augmentative and alternative

communication intervention on speech production in children with autism: A

systematic review. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 17, 212-230.

doi:10.1044/1058-0360(2008/021)

Sigafoos, J., & Iacono, T. (1993). Selecting augmentative communication devices for persons

with severe disabilities: Some factors for educational teams to consider. Australian and

New Zealand Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 18, 133-146.

doi:10.1080/07263869300034901

Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley.

Son, S. G., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M., & Lancioni, G. E. (2006). Comparing two types of

augmentative and alternative communication systems for children with autism. Pediatric

Rehabilitation, 9, 389-395. doi:10.1080/13638490500519984

Sundberg, C. T., & Sundberg, M. L. (1990). Comparing topography-based verbal behavior with

stimulus selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8(1), 31-41.

doi:10.1007/bf03392845

Tager-Flusberg, H., & Kasari, C. (2013). Minimally verbal school-aged children with autism:

The neglected end of the spectrum. Autism Research, 6, 468-478. doi: 10.1002/aur.1329

Tincani, M. (2004). Comparing the picture exchange communication system and sign language

training for children with autism. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities,

19, 152-163.

42

Touchette, P. E., & Howard, J. S. (1984). Errorless learning: reinforcement contingencies and

stimulus control transfer in delayed prompting. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis,

17, 175-188. doi:10.1901/jaba.1984.17-175

Van der Meer, L., Sigafoos, J., O’Reilly, M. F., & Lancioni, G. E. (2011). Assessing

preferences for AAC options in communication interventions for individuals with

developmental disabilities: A review of the literature. Research in Developmental

Disabilities, 32, 1422-1431. doi:10.1016/j.ridd.2011.02.003

Van der Meer, L., Sutherland, D., O’Reilly, M. F., Lancioni, G. E., & Sigafoos, J. (2012). A

further comparison of manual signing, picture exchange, and speech-generating devices

as communication modes for children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in

Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 1247-1257. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.04.005

Watson, P. J., & Workman, E. A. (1981). The non-concurrent multiple baseline across-

individuals design: An extension of the traditional multiple baseline design. Journal of

Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 12, 257-259.

Weitz, C., Dexter, M., & Moore, J. (1997). AAC and children with developmental disabilities. In

S. Glennen & D. DeCoste (Eds.), Handbook of augmentative and alternative

communication (pp. 395-431). San Diego, CA: Singular.

Windsor, J., Piche, L. M„ & Locke, P. A. (1994). Preference testing: A comparison of two

presentation methods. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 15, 439-455.

doi:10.1016/0891-4222(94)90028-0

Wodka, E. L., Mathy, P., & Kalb, L. (2013). Predictors of phrase and fluent speech in children

with autism and severe language delay. Pediatrics, 131(4). doi:10.1542/peds.2012-2221d

43

Appendices

Appendix A: Identifying a Vocal Profile

Section I – Questionnaire

1. 1. Does your child say any words? YES NO

2. 2. If yes, how many words does your child say? 1-5 words 5-10 words 10-15 words More than 15 Other _______

3. Are these words spontaneous? YES NO

4. How many times a day does your child say these words?

1-5 times 5-10 times 10-15 times More than 15 Other _______

5. Are these words understandable by people that do not know your child?

YES NO

6. Does your child have repeat words in a nonfunctional way?

YES NO

7. Does the child control these nonfunctional spoken-word repetitions?

YES NO

8. Are their spoken-word repetitions understandable by people who do not know your child?

YES NO

Section II – Structured Observation

1. When withholding a preferred item from the child, does the child ask for it if he/she wants it?

YES NO

2. When playing/using a toy/preferred item without the child, does the child spontaneously come up to you and ask for the toy/item?

YES NO

3. Did the child engage in any vocal responses (e.g., words) during the observation period?

YES NO

44

Appendix B: Testing for Repertoire’s

Section I- Repertoire Questionnaire Can your child hear? Yes No Can your child see? Yes No

Is your child ambulatory? Yes No Is your child Active? Yes No

Does your child have fine motor coordination? Yes No Does your child have fine motor imitation? Yes No Can your child match pictures to objects? Yes No

Does your child have any problem behavior? Yes No Section II- Repertoire Structured Observation

Testing for hearing

When playing a cartoon video, does the child follows the sounds in 80% of trials?

Yes No

Testing for Sight

When showing preferred item, does the child looks at it in 80% of trials?

Yes No

Testing for ambulatory

Does the child walk to get to preferred item in 80% of trials?

Yes No

Testing for active

Does the child grab or give any items? Yes No

Testing for fine motor

coordination

When asking a child to trace, string a bead, or put shapes through a shape cube, can the child do this

independently in at least 80% of trials?

Yes No

Testing for fine motor imitation

When shown a fine motor task, the child imitates within 5 s in at least 80% of trials?

Yes No

Testing for matching

When giving the child flashcards of shapes, animals and colors to match, can the child do this independently in at least 80% of trials?

Yes No

Testing for problem behavior

When withholding access to preferred item, did the child cry, whine, scream or display any other

inappropriate behavior?

Yes No

45

Appendix C: Task Analysis- Multiple Stimuli without Replacement Preference Assessment

Collect data on at least 33% of trials per sessionSteps Trial1

Y N NR

Trial2

Y N NR

Trial3

Y N NRTrial4

Y N NR1. Sit across from child 2. Place all items on table in a straight line 3. Items are placed 0.2 meter apart and 0.1 meter away

from participant

4. Tell the child to pick one 5. Does not provide additional prompts 6. Give child 5 s to make a selection 7. If child selects an item, remove the other 8. If child selects more than 1 item, remove both,

represent trial

9. If child does not select an item, remove both,

represent trial

10. When child selects an item allow child to consume edible or play with toy for 30s

11. While the child is consuming the edible or playing with the toy, move the leftmost item over to the rightmost position

12. Do not replace the chosen item in the array 13. Record data on item selected

TOTALSCORE

46

Appendix D: Task Analysis- Mand Training

Tg

Collect data on at least 33% of trials per session

Steps Trial 1

Y N NR

Trial 2

Y N NR

Trial 3

Y N NR

1. Ensure all materials are ready

2. Sit across from child

3. Place preferred item out of child’s reach but within eyesight

4. Wait for child to show interest towards the item (e.g., look at it, reach for it, point at it)

5. Provide Vocal prompt: “What do you want?

6. Provide correct prompt (Full prompt at 0s delay or full prompt at 5 s delay)

7. Reinforce correct response

8. Allow participant to consume edible or play with toy for 30 s

9. If child makes an error, implement error correction (remove items, present trial at full prompt with 0s delay)

10. Do not reinforce mands emitted during error correction

11. Do not provide any programmed consequences for problem behavior

12. Record data after reinforcing mand or conducting error correction

TOTAL SCORE

47

Appendix E: Task Analysis: EFL Communication Modality Assessment

Collect data on at least 33% of trials per session

Steps Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4

1. Ensure all materials are ready _____________________ _____________________ _____________________

2. Set environment as needed for skills being assessed:_____________________ _____________________

_____________________

3. Provide at least 30 s for individual to engage in behavior being observed

4. Provide appropriate consequence for problem behavior; see below

5. Do not provide any consequences for errors

6. Do not prompt

7. Provide5opportunitiesfortargetbehavior

8. Recorddataoneachskillbeingassessed

TOTAL SCORE

List problem behavior for each participant and appropriate consequence

48

Appendix F: Results for Daphne, Daniel, and Walter from the EFL website that

recommends a communication modality based on the individual’s current repertoire

49

Appendix G: Chart that the EFL handbook provides to depict what each letter stands for.

50

Appendix H: Format of each mode of communication for each participant

Participant Communication Modality Definition of Correct Response

Daphne

SGD: Ipad® with ProloquoTM application that contained 9 icons

The participant touching the icon corresponding to the preferred item with enough force to generate audio in the iPad® programed with ProloquoTM

PE: Laminated picture board with an “I want” sentence strip at the top, with Velcro attached to it and 4 laminated pictures with Velcro

The participant placing the picture of the preferred item on the sentence strip next to the “I want” icon. The participant was not required to hand the sentence strip to the therapist

MS: No additional materials were present, only the edible was placed in front of the participant

The participant making the MS for the preferred item (e.g., making hands and fingers into the correct form and placing them in the correct part of the body)

Daniel

SGD: Ipad® with ProloquoTM application that contained 9 icons

The participant touching the icon corresponding to the preferred item with enough force to generate audio in the iPad® programed with ProloquoTM

PE: Picture book with Velcro attached to it, and 10 laminated pictures with Velcro

The participant handing the picture depicting the preferred item over to the therapist

MS: No additional materials were present, only the edible was placed in front of the participant

The participant making the MS for the preferred item (e.g., making hands and fingers into the correct form and placing them in the correct part of the body)

Walter

SGD: Ipad® with ProloquoTM application that contained 9 icons

The participant touching the icon corresponding to the preferred item with enough force to generate audio in the iPad® programed with ProloquoTM

PE: Laminated picture board with an “I want” sentence strip at the top, with Velcro attached to it and 4 laminated pictures with Velcro

The participant placing the picture of the preferred item on the sentence strip next to the “I want” icon. The participant was not required to hand the sentence strip to the therapist

MS: No additional materials were present, only the edible was placed in front of the participant

The participant making the MS for the preferred item (e.g., making hands and fingers into the correct form and placing them in the correct part of the body)

51

Appendix I: USF IRB Approval Letter

2/7/2018 Daniella Orozco ABA-Applied Behavior Analysis Tampa, FL 33612 RE:

Expedited Approval for Initial Review

IRB#: Pro00032938 Title: Teaching Mands to Individuals with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An Evaluation of the

Essential for Living Communication Modalities Study Approval Period: 2/6/2018 to 2/6/2019

Dear Ms. Orozco: On 2/6/2018, the Institutional Review Board (IRB) reviewed and APPROVED the above application and all documents contained within, including those outlined below.

Approved Item(s): Protocol Document(s): Study Protocol

Consent/Assent Document(s)*: Parental Consent .pdf informed consent**

*Please use only the official IRB stamped informed consent/assent document(s) found under the "Attachments" tab. Please note, these consent/assent documents are valid until the consent document is amended and approved. **Verbal consent is not stamped.

It was the determination of the IRB that your study qualified for expedited review which includes activities that (1) present no more than minimal risk to human subjects, and (2) involve only procedures listed in one or more of the categories outlined below. The IRB may review research through the expedited review procedure authorized by 45CFR46.110 and 21 CFR 56.110. The research proposed in this study is categorized under the following expedited review category: