355
Primary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative learning environment Dissertation submitted in fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Centre for Learning Innovation, Faculty of Education By Christina Chalmers (B.Ed) Queensland University of Technology 2009 ABSTRACT The current understanding of students’ group metacognition is limited. The research on metacognition has focused mainly on the individual student. The aim of this study was to address the void by developing a conceptual model to inform the use

Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

  • Upload
    vankhue

  • View
    215

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

Primary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative learning environment

Dissertation submitted in fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy

Centre for Learning Innovation, Faculty of Education

By Christina Chalmers (B.Ed)

Queensland University of Technology 2009

ABSTRACT

The current understanding of students’ group metacognition is limited. The

research on metacognition has focused mainly on the individual student. The aim of

this study was to address the void by developing a conceptual model to inform the use

Page 2: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

ii

of scaffolds to facilitate group metacognition during mathematical problem solving in

computer supported collaborative learning (CSCL) environments. An initial

conceptual framework based on the literature from metacognition, cooperative

learning, cooperative group metacognition, and computer supported collaborative

learning was used to inform the study. In order to achieve the study aim, a design

research methodology incorporating two cycles was used. The first cycle focused on

the within-group metacognition for sixteen groups of primary school students working

together around the computer; the second cycle included between-group

metacognition for six groups of primary school students working together on the

Knowledge Forum® CSCL environment. The study found that providing groups with

group metacognitive scaffolds resulted in groups planning, monitoring, and evaluating

the task and team aspects of their group work. The metacognitive scaffolds allowed

students to focus on how their group was completing the problem-solving task and

working together as a team. From these findings, a revised conceptual model to

inform the use of scaffolds to facilitate group metacognition during mathematical

problem solving in computer supported collaborative learning (CSCL) environments

was generated.

Page 3: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

iii

KEYWORDS

Computer supported collaborative learning (CSCL), group learning, group

metacognition, group problem solving, groups, Knowledge Forum®, online teams,

shared understanding, task, team.

Page 4: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

iv

TABLE OF CONTENTS

ABSTRACT ......................................................................................................................... i 

KEYWORDS ..................................................................................................................... iii 

TABLE OF CONTENTS ................................................................................................... iv 

LIST OF TABLES ........................................................................................................... viii 

LIST OF FIGURES .............................................................................................................x 

STATEMENT OF ORIGINAL AUTHORSHIP .............................................................. xii 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ............................................................................................. xiii 

DEDICATION ................................................................................................................. xiii 

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION .........................................................................................1 1.1 Overview of the study ..............................................................................................4 1.2 Discussion of terms ..................................................................................................5 

1.2.1 Groups and teams ........................................................................................... 5 1.2.2 Group problem solving and learning ............................................................. 6 1.2.3 Group problem solving with computers ........................................................ 7 1.2.4 Metacognition ................................................................................................ 8 1.2.5 Group metacognition ..................................................................................... 8 

1.3 Significance of study................................................................................................9 1.4 Chapter Overviews .................................................................................................10 1.5 Conclusion .............................................................................................................11 

CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW ...........................................................................13 2.1 Group problem solving and learning .....................................................................14 

2.1.1 Organisational factors .................................................................................. 16 2.1.1.1 Strategies to address organisational factors ........................................ 23 

2.1.2 Cognitive factors .......................................................................................... 33 2.1.2.1 Strategies to address cognitive factors ................................................ 36 

2.1.3 Summary ...................................................................................................... 41 2.2 Group metacognitive factors ..................................................................................43 

2.2.1 Group metacognitive strategies .................................................................... 47 2.2.2 Group metacognitive scaffolds .................................................................... 49 2.2.3 Summary ...................................................................................................... 54 

2.3 Conclusion .............................................................................................................55 

CHAPTER 3: RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHOD ....................................................57 3.1 Research design .....................................................................................................57 3.2 Data Collection and Analysis .................................................................................59 

Page 5: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

v

3.2.1 Observations ................................................................................................ 60 3.2.1.1 Participant observation........................................................................ 61 3.2.1.2 Video recordings ................................................................................. 62 3.2.1.3 Audio (MP3) recordings ..................................................................... 62 

3.2.2 Focus group interview.................................................................................. 63 3.2.3 Classroom artefacts ...................................................................................... 64 

3.2.3.1 Diaries ................................................................................................. 64 3.2.3.2 Checklists ............................................................................................ 65 3.2.3.3 Questionnaires..................................................................................... 68 3.2.3.4 Knowledge Forum notes ..................................................................... 70 3.2.3.5 Mathematical ranking models ............................................................. 70 

3.3 Procedure ...............................................................................................................71 3.3.1 Stage 1: Cycles of design experiment .......................................................... 71 

3.3.1.1 Cycle 1: Within-group metacognition ................................................ 74 3.3.1.2 Cycle 2: Within- and between-group metacognition .......................... 89 

3.3.2 Stage 2: Development of a unified conceptual model ................................. 99 3.4 Conclusion ...........................................................................................................100 

CHAPTER 4: RESULTS FROM CYCLE 1 ...................................................................103 4.1 Organisational themes ..........................................................................................104 

4.1.1 Theme 1 ..................................................................................................... 104 4.1.2 Theme 2 ..................................................................................................... 106 4.1.3 Theme 3 ..................................................................................................... 110 4.1.4 Theme 4 ..................................................................................................... 112 4.1.5 Theme 5 ..................................................................................................... 117 4.1.6 Theme 6 ..................................................................................................... 120 4.1.7 Organisational themes summary ................................................................ 122 

4.2 Cognitive themes .................................................................................................124 4.2.1 Theme 7 ..................................................................................................... 124 4.2.2 Theme 8 ..................................................................................................... 127 4.2.3 Cognitive themes summary ........................................................................ 128 

4.3 Metacognitive themes ..........................................................................................129 4.2.1 Theme 9 ..................................................................................................... 130 4.3.2 Theme 10 ................................................................................................... 134 4.3.3 Metacognitive themes summary ................................................................ 143 

4.4 Focus-group interview .........................................................................................144 4.5 Summary and conclusion .....................................................................................146 4.6 Implications for Cycle 2.......................................................................................147 

CHAPTER 5: RESULTS FROM CYCLE 2 ...................................................................151 5.1 Organisational themes ..........................................................................................152 

5.1.1 Theme 1 ..................................................................................................... 153 5.1.2 Theme 2 ..................................................................................................... 155 5.1.3 Theme 3 ..................................................................................................... 157 5.1.4 Theme 4 ..................................................................................................... 159 5.1.5 Theme 5 ..................................................................................................... 162 

Page 6: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

vi

5.1.6 Theme 6 ..................................................................................................... 164 5.1.7 Organisational themes summary ................................................................ 166 

5.2 Cognitive themes .................................................................................................167 5.2.1 Theme 7 ..................................................................................................... 168 5.2.2 Theme 8 ..................................................................................................... 171 5.2.3 Theme 9 ..................................................................................................... 172 5.2.4 Cognitive themes summary ........................................................................ 175 

5.3 Metacognitive themes ..........................................................................................175 5.3.1 Theme 10 ................................................................................................... 176 5.3.2 Theme 11 ................................................................................................... 179 5.3.3 Metacognitive themes summary ................................................................ 186 

5.4 Focus group interview ..........................................................................................187 5.5 Summary and conclusion .....................................................................................188 

CHAPTER 6: DEVELOPMENT OF UNIFIED CONEPTUAL MODEL ......................189 6.1 Overview of results ..............................................................................................189 

6.1.1 Problem-solving task ................................................................................. 190 6.1.2 Organisational factors ................................................................................ 192 

6.1.2.1 Summary ........................................................................................... 195 6.1.3 Cognitive factors ........................................................................................ 196 

6.1.3.1 Summary ........................................................................................... 198 6.1.4 Group metacognition ................................................................................. 199 

6.2 Group metacognitive model .................................................................................203 6.2.1 Problem-solving context ............................................................................ 204 6.2.2 Organisational factors ................................................................................ 206 6.2.3 Cognitive factors ........................................................................................ 208 6.2.4 Metacognitive factors ................................................................................. 210 6.2.5 Discussion .................................................................................................. 211 

6.3 Application of group metacognition model .........................................................215 6.4 Conclusion ...........................................................................................................217 

CHAPTER 7: CONCLUSION ........................................................................................219 7.1 Overview of the study ..........................................................................................219 7.2 Significance..........................................................................................................221 

7.2.1 Theoretical significance ............................................................................. 222 7.2.2 Practical significance ................................................................................. 224 

7.3 Limitations ...........................................................................................................226 7.4 Recommendations for further research ................................................................228 7.5 Conclusion ...........................................................................................................229 

REFERENCES ................................................................................................................231 

APPENDICES .................................................................................................................261 APPENDIX A: Checklist to observe group behaviour ..............................................261 APPENDIX B: Metacognitive questionnaire ............................................................262 APPENDIX C: Self regulatory checklist ...................................................................263 APPENDIX D: Interview questions ..........................................................................264 APPENDIX E: Initial individual questionnaire .........................................................265 

Page 7: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

vii

APPENDIX F: Final individual questionnaire ..........................................................266 APPENDIX G: Group cohesiveness questionnaire ...................................................267 APPENDIX H: Lesson plan.......................................................................................268 APPENDIX I: City information .................................................................................274 APPENDIX J: Newspaper article ..............................................................................282 APPENDIX K: Group roles, skills, and problem-solving strategies .........................283 APPENDIX L: Group diary checklists ......................................................................284 APPENDIX M: Final overall ranking system: Cycle 1 .............................................288 APPENDIX N: T-chart ..............................................................................................290 APPENDIX O: Knowledge Forum guide ..................................................................291 APPENDIX P: Group categories ...............................................................................293 APPENDIX Q: List of categories ..............................................................................294 APPENDIX R: CD: Australia’s best city ..................................................................295 APPENDIX S: Excel guide .......................................................................................296 APPENDIX T: Final CD: Cycle 1 .............................................................................297 APPENDIX U: Bales’ Interaction Process Analysis .................................................298 APPENDIX V: Group metacognition coding ............................................................299 APPENDIX W: Mathematical model for Team One ................................................300 APPENDIX X: Posters ..............................................................................................302 APPENDIX Y: IPA Coding for each group ..............................................................306 

Page 8: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

viii

LIST OF TABLES

Table 2.1. Group roles ...................................................................................................... 29

Table 2.2. Strategic questions ........................................................................................... 39

Table 3.1. Data collection methods .................................................................................. 60

Table 3.2. Cycle 1 data collected and method of analysis ................................................ 85

Table 3.3. Formation of online teams ............................................................................... 91

Table 3.4. Cycle 2 data collected and method of analysis ................................................ 98

Table 4.1. IPA domain frequency ................................................................................... 106

Table 4.2. IPA category frequency ................................................................................. 110

Table 4.3. Responses to question two on initial individual questionnaire ...................... 118

Table 4.4. Responses to question seven on final individual questionnaire ..................... 119

Table 4.5. Responses to question three on initial individual questionnaire ................... 121

Table 4.6. Frequency of task-skills chosen ..................................................................... 126

Table 4.7. Frequency of team-skills chosen ................................................................... 127

Table 4.8. Metacognitive questionnaire .......................................................................... 131

Table 4.9. Group metacognition coding for Group C .................................................... 137

Table 4.10. Group metacognition coding for Group E ................................................... 138

Table 4.11. Group plan to solve the problem and reach the goal .................................. 140

Table 5.1. IPA domain frequency ................................................................................... 154

Table 5.2. IPA domain frequency for both cycles ........................................................... 155

Table 5.3. IPA category frequency ................................................................................. 158

Table 5.4. Responses to question three on initial individual questionnaire ................... 163

Table 5.5. Responses to question four on initial inidividual questionnaire .................... 165

Table 5.6. Frequency of group roles chosen ................................................................... 169

Page 9: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

ix

Table 5.7. Frequency of task skills chosen ..................................................................... 170

Table 5.8. Frequency of team skills chosen .................................................................... 170

Table 5.9 Responses to group cohesiveness questionnaire ............................................ 174

Table 5.10. Metacognitive questionnaire ........................................................................ 177

Table Y1. IPA frequency count for each category for Group A ..................................... 308

Table Y2. IPA frequency count for each category for Group B ..................................... 311

Table Y3. IPA frequency count for each category for Group C ..................................... 314

Table Y4. IPA frequency count for each category for Group D .................................... 316

Table Y5. IPA frequency count for each category for Group E ..................................... 319

Table Y6. IPA frequency count for each category for Group F ..................................... 322

Table Y7. IPA frequency count for each category for Group G .................................... 324

Table Y8. IPA frequency count for each category for Group H .................................... 326

Table Y9. IPA frequency count for each category for Group I ...................................... 328

Table Y10. IPA frequency count for each category for Group J .................................... 330

Table Y11. IPA frequency count for each category for Group K ................................... 332

Table Y12. IPA frequency count for each category for Group L ................................... 334

Table Y13. IPA frequency count for each category for Group M .................................. 336

Table Y14. IPA frequency count for each category for Group N ................................... 338

Table Y15. IPA frequency count for each category for Group O .................................. 340

Table Y16. IPA frequency count for each category for Group P ................................... 342

Page 10: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

x

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 2.1. Model representing organisational factors ..................................................... 23

Figure 2.2. Model representing cognitive factors ............................................................. 36

Figure 2.3. Factors influencing efficiency of group problem solving and learning ......... 42

Figure 2.4. Group problem solving and learning factors .................................................. 43

Figure 2.5. Model representing metacognitive factors ..................................................... 45

Figure 2.6. Scaffolding group metacognition ................................................................... 52

Figure 2.7. Conceptual framework ................................................................................... 56

Figure 3.1. Phases of design cycles .................................................................................. 72

Figure 6.1. Complex problem solving context ............................................................... 204

Figure 6.2. Organisational factors .................................................................................. 208

Figure 6.3. Cognitive factors .......................................................................................... 209

Figure 6.4. Group metacognition model ......................................................................... 211

Figure 7.1. Final model .................................................................................................. 220

Figure Y1. IPA domain frequencies for Group A ........................................................... 306

Figure Y2. IPA domain frequencies for Group B ........................................................... 309

Figure Y3. IPA domain frequencies for Group C ........................................................... 312

Figure Y4. IPA domain frequencies for Group D ........................................................... 315

Figure Y5. IPA domain frequencies for Group E ........................................................... 317

Figure Y6. IPA domain frequencies for Group F ........................................................... 320

Figure Y7. IPA domain frequencies for Group G ........................................................... 323

Figure Y8. IPA domain frequencies for Group H ........................................................... 325

Figure Y9. IPA domain frequencies for Group I ............................................................ 327

Figure Y10. IPA domain frequencies for Group J .......................................................... 329

Page 11: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

xi

Figure Y11. IPA domain frequencies for Group K ......................................................... 331

Figure Y12. IPA domain frequencies for Group L ......................................................... 333

Figure Y13. IPA domain frequencies for Group M ........................................................ 335

Figure Y14. IPA domain frequencies for Group N ......................................................... 337

Figure Y15. IPA domain frequencies for Group O ......................................................... 339

Figure Y16. IPA domain frequencies for Group P ......................................................... 341

Page 12: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

xii

STATEMENT OF ORIGINAL AUTHORSHIP

The work contained in this thesis has not been previously submitted for a degree or a

diploma at any other higher education institution. To the best of my knowledge and

belief, the thesis contains no materials previously published or written by another

person, except where due reference is made.

Signature

Date

Page 13: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

xiii

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

Thanks to my supervisors, faculty, and family who have helped me reach this

point in my academic career.

I am most grateful to my supervisor, Associate Professor Rod Nason for his

continual support and guidance. Rod has been both mentor and friend. I also want to

thank my other supervisor Professor Cam McRobbie for his contribution to this study.

I wish to warmly thank my friends and colleagues in the School of

Mathematics, Science, and Technology (QUT) - a group in which I have the honour

of belonging to. An acknowledgement also needs to be made to the Australian

Postgraduate Award committee for their financial support.

Finally, I owe my warmest thanks to my home team – my husband Wayne and

my three inspiring daughters Kelsey, Ella, and Jenna. Thank you for your continuous

love, support, and patience.

DEDICATION

In the spirit of groups and teams, this study could not have been completed without

the support and encouragement of my own.

Page 14: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

1

CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION

Problem solving and thinking mathematically are crucial tools for

participating in the twenty-first century’s knowledge society. Students need to be

able to apply mathematical knowledge to solve problems (Queensland Studies

Authority, 2004). However, effective problem solving involves not only finding a

solution but also metacognitively monitoring the problem-solving activity (Goos,

Galbraith, & Renshaw, 2002). Difficulties in problem solving can occur if

students fail to metacognitively monitor and regulate their cognitive processes

(Garafalo & Lester, 1985; Schoenfeld, 1983).

Research in mathematical problem solving and metacognition has tended

to focus on the individual learner (Hoyles & Healy, 1994; Hurme & Järvelä,

2001). However, many researchers in the field, such as Stahl (2006), feel that the

focus needs to shift from the individual to the group and how the group solves

problems and represents knowledge. This research study addresses the need for a

shift from a focus on the individual to a focus on the group within the field of

mathematical problem solving and metacognition.

A shift in focus from the individual learner to the group is necessary for

two reasons. First, the use of group problem solving is increasing both in

education and work fields (Beatty & Barker, 2004; Dundis & Benson, 2003; Salas

& Fiore, 2004). Second, the proliferation of computer supported collaborative

learning (CSCL) and computer supported collaborative work (CSCW)

Page 15: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

2

environments in education and work fields is increasing due to the wide use of

computers (Ackerman, 2000; Bromme, Hesse, & Spada, 2005; Stahl, 2006).

Working in a group gives students access to a wide range of thinking

strategies, contributes to students’ understanding of the problem, and provides

alternative solutions (Cohen, 1994; Cullen, 1999; Gillies, 2000; Jonassen &

Kwon, 2001). However, while several studies have shown that groups are more

productive than individuals in complex problem solving, not all groups work

together cooperatively (Cohen, 1994; Johnson & Johnson, 2003; Sims, Salas, &

Burke, 2005).

In order to work effectively as a group, students need to think about their

group work by planning, monitoring, and evaluating their learning processes

within a group context (Goos et al., 2002; Hurme, Palonen, & Järvelä, 2006).

Metacognition is an essential element of group problem solving (Hinsz, 2004).

Activities such as planning how to approach a given learning task, monitoring

progress, and evaluating progress toward the completion of the task are

metacognitive processes that play a critical role in successful group learning and

problem solving.

Some researchers have suggested that computer settings can increase the

possibility of successful group learning and problem solving, as students are more

likely to work together when working on computer based tasks (Kreijns,

Kirschner, & Jochems, 2002; Light, 2004; Underwood & Underwood, 1999).

Page 16: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

3

Computers provide a medium for group problem solving by encouraging

discussion and sharing both within- and between-groups (Beamish & Au, 1995).

The purpose of computer supported collaborative learning (CSCL) thus is

to support students in learning together effectively. CSCL supports the

communication of ideas and information among learners, collaborative accessing

of information and documents, and peer feedback on learning activities (Hurme &

Järvelä, 2001). CSCL also supports and facilitates group processes and group

dynamics in ways that are not achievable by face-to-face communication, such as

having learners label aspects of their communication (Stahl, Koschmann, &

Suthers, 2006). However, CSCL environments have often not fulfilled

expectations as researchers and practitioners have failed to provide the support

that groups need to succeed (Kreijns et al., 2002).

Therefore, the aim of this study was to develop a conceptual model to

inform the use of scaffolds for within- and between-group metacognition, for

primary school students, during mathematical problem solving in CSCL

environments. In order to meet the aim of the research study, three specific

objectives were addressed:

1. To evaluate scaffolds to facilitate within-group metacognition during

mathematical problem solving for groups working around a computer.

2. To evaluate scaffolds to facilitate between-group metacognition while

building collective knowledge within a CSCL environment.

Page 17: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

4

3. To synthesise the findings from Objectives 1 and 2 into a unified

conceptual model to inform the use of scaffolds to facilitate within- and

between-group metacognition within CSCL environments.

1.1 Overview of the study

In order to address the aim and the three research objectives, a two stage

research study was conducted. The major focus in Stage 1 was on Research

Objectives 1 and 2 whilst the focus in Stage 2 was on Research Objective 3.

A ‘design research’ methodology incorporating a descriptive case study

was utilised in Stage 1. There were two cycles of design research experiments in

Stage 1 of the study. Each of the two cycles consisted of three successive phases

of (1) planning, (2) conducting, and (3) analysing and refinement. In Cycle 1, an

initial conceptual framework based on an analysis and synthesis of the research

literature was utilised to inform the planning of scaffolds for within-group

metacognition. The implementation of these scaffolds occurred in two primary

classrooms from two different schools. The data from the implementation of the

scaffolds were then analysed. Following this, refinements were made to not only

the scaffolds but also to the underlying initial conceptual framework. In Cycle 2,

the refined conceptual framework and set of scaffolds were applied in both

within- and between-group contexts in two classrooms from two different

schools. Following a process of planning, conducting, analysing and refinement

Page 18: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

5

similar to that utilised in Cycle 1, further refinements were made to the conceptual

framework. In Stage 2, the outcomes from Stage 1 (Cycles 1 and 2) were

synthesised into a group metacognition model that provides a unified conceptual

framework for the field of scaffolding group metacognition.

1.2 Discussion of terms

Throughout the course of this dissertation, reference often will be made to

terminology that has its genesis in the research literature. In order to provide an

advance organiser for the detailed review of the research literature that follows in

Chapter 2, a discussion of each of the following terms is now presented:

Groups and teams

Group problem solving and learning

Group computer work

Metacognition

Group metacognition

1.2.1 Groups and teams

A group is a number of people who communicate with one another over a

period of time (Benjamin, Bessant, & Watts, 1997). Shaw (1981) defined a group

as two or more individuals who interact with each other and are influenced by

each other. A group requires at least three people in order for certain group roles,

norms, and processes to emerge (Gottlieb, 2003; Samovar, Henman, & King,

Page 19: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

6

1996). Teams are groups of people that share a common purpose (Thomas, 1992).

All teams are groups; however, the reverse is not always true as teams focus on

both the group task and on the group relationships (Kormanski, 1999).

1.2.2 Group problem solving and learning

The use of problem-solving groups is increasing both in work and

education fields (Beatty & Barker, 2004; Dundis & Benson, 2003; Salas & Fiore,

2004). Therefore, in order to provide authentic learning opportunities for students,

it is important that they work in problem-solving groups within mathematics

classrooms (Lesh & Lamon, 1992; Light, 2004; Zawojewski, Lesh, & English,

2003). Group learning improves students’ mathematical understanding as well as

improving their communication and group skills (Haller, Gallagher, Weldon, &

Felder, 2000).

Groups can master mathematical problems too complex for individuals to

solve alone (Jonassen & Kwon, 2001). While problem solving in groups, students

have opportunities to ask questions, explain their reasoning, build upon their

knowledge, and discuss and develop problem-solving strategies (Curcio & Artzt,

1998; Gillies, & Ashman, 2000; Soller, 2001). The most effective learning

contexts for groups are problem-based (Jonassen & Kwon, 2001). Problem-based

learning increases motivation, develops critical thinking, and deepens

understanding of learning content.

Page 20: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

7

One type of complex and open-ended mathematical problem-solving tasks

that has been found to facilitate dynamic group discussion and knowledge

building is model-eliciting activities (Lesh & Doerr, 2003; Nason & Woodruff,

2003). Model-eliciting activities are mathematical-based tasks that present

realistic problem scenarios and often require students to work together as a group

to develop a model that can be used to solve the problem situation (Lesh & Harel,

2003).

1.2.3 Group problem solving with computers

The recognition of the educational importance of group problem-solving

has resulted in groups working together around computers (Hoyles & Healy,

1994; Neufeld & Haggerty, 2001; Seymour, 1994; Stahl et al., 2006). The

computer is a medium through which groups can communicate their

understanding and provides a way to represent and store shared knowledge

(Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1989; Crook, 1999; Reyna, Branerd, Effken, Bootzih, &

Lloyd, 2001; Sherman, 2001). Students collaboratively construct knowledge using

the shared interface that can be used to support group work by providing

scaffolding for them working together (Beamish & Au, 1995; Etheris & Tan,

2004; Lee, Chan, & van Aalst, 2006).

Small groups interacting around and through the computer promotes

productive collaborative learning (Littleton & Light, 1999). Crook (1996)

investigated how computers can facilitate collaborative learning and highlighted

Page 21: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

8

the difference between interacting through and around computers. Interacting

around computers refers to using the computer as a shared reference for a group

while interaction through computers refers to the use of a computer network.

The Knowledge Forum® software was used in this study as the CSCL

environment that allowed groups of students to engage with other groups through

a community database. Scardamalia and Bereiter (1994) stated that Knowledge

Forum© supports a student-centred open-ended learning environment where

students are actively engaged in knowledge building.

1.2.4 Metacognition

Metacognition involves students engaging in thinking about the nature of

the learning task and the social context in which learning takes place (Honess,

1986; Schoenfeld, 1983; Xiaodong, 2001). Metacognition is a form of cognitive

self-monitoring (Gama, 2000). It refers to self-knowledge about how one thinks

and includes the ability to self-regulate one’s learning, which is the degree that

students are active participants in their own learning process (Schunk &

Zimmerman, 1994).

1.2.5 Group metacognition

Group metacognition is part of a wider educational view of facilitating

self-regulation and is basically “what group members know about the way groups

process information” (Hinsz, Tindale, & Vollrath, 1997, p. 58). Groups of

Page 22: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

9

students need to develop a type of co-cognition in order to collaboratively develop

concepts and monitor their group performance (Costa & O'Leary, 1992).

Metacognition in groups includes the expectations group members have

about the way groups process and perform tasks (Hinsz, 2004). The focus on

shared group knowledge shifts the focus away from the individual learner and

groups need to set their own learning goals and monitor their progress towards the

goals (Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994).

1.3 Significance of study

There has been a shifting focus from the individual learner to a view of

learning as a social practice (Aldag & Fuller, 1993; Lave & Wenger, 1991;

Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1994; Stahl, 2006). Students need to be engaged in group

contexts where they can express their ideas, question each other, elaborate on

their thinking and receive feedback from their peers (Gillies & Ashman, 2003;

Kramarski & Mevarech, 2003).

The current understanding of students’ group metacognition is based on a

relatively narrow body of research (Hurme & Järvelä, 2001; Xiaodong, 2001).

The research has focused mainly on the development of strategies for problem

solving. Goos et al. (2002) suggested that the potential for group problem solving

to develop students’ group skills combined with their problem-solving skills has

remained largely unexplored. This study seeks to address this void by first

investigating students’ group metacognitive strategies during a complex model-

Page 23: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

10

eliciting problem-solving activity in a CSCL environment and second by

developing a theoretical framework to inform further research and practice in this

field.

In addition to its theoretical significance, the present research study also

has practical significance. A condition of a CSCL environment is that students are

metacognitive about their group work within the computer setting (Hurme &

Järvelä, 2001). Metacognitive strategies need to be taught and students need to be

supported in their group problem solving by providing metacognitive scaffolding

for the group process.

There is a need to bridge the cognitive skills associated with problem

solving and group work in order to develop a successful problem-solving group.

The joint construction of understanding concerned with the negotiation of

meaning in collaborative group work involves students thinking about their group

work. The combination of collaborative learning, problem solving, and

computers, leads to a learning community where metacognition is essential to

group learning.

1.4 Chapter Overviews

Chapter 1 provides information on the background of the research. The

significance of the study is examined and a summary of relevant literature is

given. Chapter 2 reviews the relevant literature and provides a foundation for the

study pertaining to group work, metacognition, and group metacognition. Chapter

Page 24: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

11

3 outlines the research methodology used in this study, including data collection

and analysis. The chapter examines the research design in detail. A description of

data collection is given including interviews and questionnaires used to obtain the

data, as well as details on the selection of the participants of the study. Chapter 4

presents the results from Cycle 1 of the study, which describes the effects of

introducing group metacognitive strategies to groups working around a computer.

Chapter 5 presents the results from Cycle 2, which describes the effects of

introducing group metacognitive strategies to groups working within a computer

supported collaborative learning (CSCL) environment. The findings from both

cycles of the research study are analysed and discussed in Chapter 6 in order to

develop a final unified conceptual model to inform the design of scaffolds to

facilitate group metacognition within CSCL environments. Finally in Chapter 7 a

conclusion to the study is provided, limitations are discussed, and

recommendations are made for future research.

1.5 Conclusion

This study investigates the development of group strategies by introducing

group metacognitive scaffolds within-groups of primary school students working

at the computer and between-groups working together on the Knowledge Forum®

CSCL environment. Both collaborative learning and metacognition have been

widely researched, (Blakey & Spence, 1990; Chizhik, 1998; Johnson & Johnson,

1999). However, there is limited research on the relationship between them. The

Page 25: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

12

research study reported in the following chapters addresses this void in research

on group learning and metacognition.

Page 26: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

13

CHAPTER 2: LITERATURE REVIEW

This chapter reviews the relevant literature from a number of research

disciplines on group work, metacognition, problem solving, and computer

supported collaborative learning (CSCL) environments and provides a foundation

for the study pertaining to group problem solving and learning (see Section 2.1)

and group metacognition (see Section 2.2). The results from the literature review

are accumulated in the conclusion of this chapter (see Section 2.3) and an initial

conceptual framework is provided (see Figure 2.7).

The major points from the literature review are synthesised into a

conceptual framework that will inform the research study. The group

metacognition framework introduces the idea of group members building a shared

model or representation in order to develop a shared understanding during group

problem solving.

The shared group understanding is facilitated by CSCL environments,

such as the Knowledge Forum® software used in this study, which scaffolds the

problem-solving process. This review will focus on how to scaffold group

metacognition in order to develop successful problem solving within-groups

working around a computer and between-groups working together on the

Knowledge Forum® CSCL environment.

Page 27: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

14

2.1 Group problem solving and learning

Within the research literature, group problem solving and learning is often

referred to as either collaborative or co-operative learning (Gut, 2000; Mueller &

Fleming, 1994; O’Neil, Chuang, & Chung, 2003; Oxford, 1997). Collaborative

learning is distinguished from cooperative learning in that cooperative learning is

described as the sharing of a task where each person is responsible for a portion of

the task, whereas collaboration involves the coordinated effort of group members

to achieve the task together (Dillenbourg, Baker, Blaye, & O' Malley, 1996).

Collaborative learning is a coordinated activity resulting from a continual

attempt to construct and maintain a shared understanding (Teasley & Roschelle,

1993). It occurs when a group of students work together to accomplish a shared

learning goal and is linked to a wide range of positive learning outcomes

(Chizhik, 1998; Teasley & Roschelle, 1993). Cooperative learning is the use of

small groups in order to accomplish a group task (Johnson & Johnson, 1999).

Cooperative learning involves the sharing and exchange of ideas among students

and is effective for achieving intellectual as well as social learning goals (Cohen,

1994; Haller et al., 2000).

The corpus of knowledge about group problem solving and learning,

derived from research into collaborative and co-operative learning over the last

thirty years, indicates that students’ learning in successful groups can achieve

higher cognitive levels than working alone (Brown & Palincsar, 1989; Johnson &

Johnson, 1999; Vygotsky, 1978). When students interact with their peers during

Page 28: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

15

problem-solving activities, cognitive restructuring can occur (King, 1989).

Working together on a common goal, students encourage one another’s learning,

leading to a co-construction of ideas that could not be achieved individually

(Slavin, 1995). Groups can share ideas, develop common goals, as well as learn

from and support each other’s learning (Benjamin et al., 1997). Students share

understanding and encourage each other to complete the group task and learning

occurs through this collaborative interaction with peers (Barron, 2000).

According to Crook (1996) computers can support collaboration by

providing students with a point of shared reference that supports a group

understanding. Group learning tasks incorporating the computer tend to encourage

interactions amongst students (King, 1989). The computer focuses the group

attention on the mutually shared object (Crook, 1999; O’Malley, 1995;

Puntambekar, 2006; Stahl et al., 2006). Students also engage in more task-related

interaction when they work on computer tasks in cooperative groups (Jonassen &

Kwon, 2001; Poole & Zhang, 2005).

In recent years, a new dimension has been added to collaborative learning

with computers in the form of Computer Supported Collaborative Learning

(CSCL) environments. CSCL environments support and scaffold collaborative

problem-solving and knowledge building and students can collaboratively

construct knowledge using a shared interface that supports group work and

scaffolds their working together (Jonassen & Kwon, 2001; O'Malley , 1995;

Wang, Hinn, & Kanfer, 2001).

Page 29: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

16

While numerous studies suggest that group problem solving is more

productive than individual problem solving, merely organising students into

groups around the computer and telling them to work together does not guarantee

they will cooperate and learn as a group (Fiore & Schooler, 2004; Gillies, 2003;

Johnson & Johnson, 1999; McWhaw, Schnackenberg, Sclater, & Abrami, 2003;

Shah, Dirks, & Chervany, 2006; Stahl, 2006). As West (2004) pointed out, ‘good’

groups do not occur naturally. A review of the research literature indicates that

most prior research and practice into collaborative group learning tended to focus

on either organisational or cognitive factors. Both the organisational and cognitive

factors need to be considered to provide the conditions necessary for successful

group problem solving and learning.

2.1.1 Organisational factors

Organisational factors influence how students successfully work as a

group to solve problems. In most classes, when students are assigned to group

work, they tend to seek information from each other and work “in” groups rather

than “as” a group (Johnson & Johnson, 1999; Ogden, 2000). Working in groups

involves students working individually on the same task while working as a group

involves students working together to complete the one task. If students do not

know how to work as a group, they will need constant supervision (Cohen, 1994).

Learning how to work successfully as a group is not a simple process

(Cohen, 1994; Johnson & Johnson, 1999; Tuckman & Jensen, 1977). To become

Page 30: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

17

a successful problem solving and learning group often entails students proceeding

through a series of stages. Many researchers have studied how groups develop

and have identified definable stages of group development (Bales, 1970; Bales &

Cohen, 1979; Mennecke, Hoffer, & Wynee, 1992; Tubbs, 1995; Tuckman &

Jensen, 1977).

Bales (1970) analysed group statements and formed a model of the

structure of group discussion which included three stages of group development:

orientation, evaluation, and control (Bales & Cohen, 1979). Mennecke et al.

(1992) identified five group development stages: orientation, exploration,

normalisation, production, and termination. Tubbs’ (1995) stages included:

orientation, conflict, consensus, and closure. While Wheelan’s (1990) stages of

dependency and inclusion, counterdependency and flight, trust and structure,

working productively and finalisation, are similar to the stages proposed by

Tuckman and Jensen (1977). All of these models have highlighted a progression

through the stages of group development from the initial orientation, or inclusion

stage to the final control, closure, or finalisation stage.

Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) group development stages are the most

widely used group development model. Tuckman and Jensen identified five

stages through which groups typically develop: forming, storming, norming,

performing, and adjourning. During the group forming stage, group members

form a group, either online or face-to-face. Group members seek acceptance by

the group and conflict is avoided as members get to know one another. The major

Page 31: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

18

group orientation concerns the task and relationships begin to form among group

members.

Groups enter the storming stage when members begin to risk conflict as

they deal with issues such as who is responsible for what, and conflicts occur over

leadership, structure, and authority (Tuckman & Jensen, 1977). Conflict is often

an inevitable part of group work and can enhance the group decision-making

process (Goos et al., 2002; Korsgaard, Brodt, & Sapienza, 2003; West, 2004).

Students can gain a shared understanding of the task by engaging in task-related

conflict (Crook, 1996; Rentsch & Zelno, 2003). However, West (2004) cautioned

that conflict can also be destructive and lead to poor team performance.

Group conflict can be based on either task or team issues. Task conflicts

tend to be related with positive group outcomes and usually pertain to group

procedures, roles, and resources. Team based conflict involves socio-emotional

conflict that is associated negatively with group work (Rentsch & Zelno, 2003;

Wheelan, 2005). West (2004) also highlighted three types of conflict: task, team,

and interpersonal. Task conflict leads to differences of opinion and is desirable for

effective teamwork while team and interpersonal conflict can be destructive to the

team. The major group task during the storming phase is the development of an

ability to listen and seek productive resolutions to conflict.

As groups begin dealing with conflicts, the third stage of development,

norming, occurs as groups achieve group cohesion and are able to work

productively. Leadership is shared and members share both task and socio-

Page 32: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

19

emotional information. Members begin to experience a sense of belonging to the

group and have positive feelings regarding the resolution of group conflicts.

Membership of the group is maintained by groups reinforcing acceptable group

behaviours (Benjamin et al., 1997). When students work toward a group goal they

tend to form behaviour norms in order for the group to succeed (Benjamin et al.,

1997; Slavin, 1995). Norms tend to regulate group learning. Each group defines

its own norms for their group and depends on how group members accept and

apply norms in order to achieve a balance between task work and team work

(Beatty & Baker, 2004). Movement to the next stage requires evolving group

interdependence.

The fourth stage, performing, involves members working interdependently

as a group. Group members are task and team oriented. This productive phase of

group development is one that is achieved by only a relatively small percentage of

groups (Tuckman & Jensen, 1977). The final stage of group development,

adjourning, involves groups completing the group task and involves giving

members opportunity for reflection and encouraging an open discussion of

feelings.

Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) model of team development suggested a

progression through the stages. However, many teams can waver between stages

(Langan-Fox, 2003). Tuckman and Jensen (1977) also suggested that most groups

fail to achieve and move past the third stage of development, norming, which

occurs as groups achieve group cohesion and are able to work productively

Page 33: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

20

together. The fourth stage, performing, which involves members working together

interdependently as a group, occurs in only a small percentage of groups (Langan-

Fox, 2003).

Johnson, Johnson, and Johnson-Holubec (1993) suggested that in order for

students to work together productively, five elements must be incorporated into

learning activities.

These five elements are:

1. Face-to-face interaction

2. Social skills

3. Individual accountability

4. Positive interdependence

5. Group processing

Face-to-face interaction enables learners to encourage and assist each

other’s learning. Research in face-to-face education contexts confirms the benefits

of group learning (Archer-Kath & Johnson, 1994). However, some researchers

have questioned this assumption and have noted benefits of group learning

mediated by computer networks rather than by face-to-face interaction (Hiltz,

1994; Hron & Friedrich, 2003). Jonassen and Kwon (2001) suggested that groups

communicating face-to-face are more personal, while using computers group

communications are more task-orientated. In both face-to-face and online groups,

Page 34: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

21

students can share resources and support and encourage each other’s learning, as

well as maintain an awareness of what other group members are doing.

Group skills such as social skills, communication skills, and conflict

resolution skills are important components of achieving a successful group.

Dishon and O’Leary (1984) divided group skills into task and maintenance skills.

Task skills are associated with the specific problem-solving task and maintenance

skills are the social skills used in order to maintain the group in working order.

According to Armstrong and Priola (2001), groups must perform two kinds of

processes: one concerned with completing the task, and the other with

maintaining the group. Hayes (2002) suggested that groups require regular

maintenance to be effective and that groups need scaffolds in order to adopt group

maintenance behaviours. Group maintenance behaviours include contributing

ideas, expressing feelings, active listening skills, expressing support, encouraging

others, checking for understanding, and performing various group roles (Cohen,

1994; Dishon & O’Leary, 1984).

Individual accountability is where each group member is accountable for

the group goal and leads to a situation where individual group member’s learning

maximises the group’s learning (Johnson & Johnson, 1999). Archer-Kath and

Johnson (1994) suggested one way to structure individual accountability is to

provide feedback on the extent to which group members are engaging in targeted

group roles or skills.

Page 35: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

22

Positive interdependence is a group state where each group member must

depend on other group members to accomplish the shared task. Members share

common goals and encourage each other’s efforts to reach the group goals.

Interdependence combines goal interdependence, task interdependence, resource

interdependence, role interdependence, and reward interdependence (Johnson &

Johnson, 1999). Johnson and Johnson (1987) stated that when positive

interdependence is present students will be sitting close together, talking about the

task work, sharing ideas, and encouraging each other to learn.

Finally, group processing allows a general assessment of how groups are

functioning to achieve their goals (Benjamin et al., 1997). Group processing

involves groups reflecting on how they are functioning and adjusting behaviours

and strategies in order to have a successful group outcome (Johnson et al., 1993).

The purpose of group processing is to constantly improve the effectiveness of

group learning (Johnson & Johnson, 1999).

Most of the organisational factors identified and discussed in this section

of the literature review do not operate in isolation; they operate in an interrelated

way such as indicated in Figure 2.1. In this figure, the interrelationship between

key seminal ideas from Cohen (1994), Johnson and Johnson (1999) and Tuckman

and Jensen (1977) have been integrated into a model of organisational factors that

influence group problem solving and learning. This model highlights that the six

elements of interaction, constructive conflict, social skills, individual

accountability, positive interdependence, and group processing, need to be

Page 36: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

23

incorporated in order for a group to progress through the group development

stages.

Organisational factors

Forming

Storming

Norming

Performing

Adjourning

Interaction face-to-face and on-line

Constructive conflict

Social skills

Individual accountability

Positive interdependence

Group processing

Figure 2.1. Model representing organisational factors.

2.1.1.1 Strategies to address organisational factors

The following section discusses specific strategies that address the

organisational factors relevant to group problem solving and learning, including

problem-solving skills, group skills, conflict management skills, observation and

feedback, group roles, computer-support for group problem solving and learning,

and problem-solving tasks.

Page 37: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

24

Problem-solving skills: Problem solving is a multi-step procedure where

the group needs to develop a plan to reach a solution (Hoover, 2002). Groups

need to develop a range of skills for solving problems (Dishon & O’Leary, 1984;

Jonassen & Kwon, 2001). There is inconsistency within the literature regarding

the teaching of problem-solving skills. Some propose explicit teaching of skills

(e.g., Hoek, Terwel, & van den Eeden, 1997; Malouff, 2006). Others suggest that

choosing appropriate skills is learnt by solving a variety of problems and

reflecting on the effective skills used (e.g., Delclos & Harrington, 1991; De Corte,

Greer, & Verschaffel, 1996). However, there is general agreement in the literature

that groups need to learn to monitor and adjust the problem-solving skills they are

using as they solve a problem (Garofalo & Lester, 1985).

Problem-solving skills can be classified into two categories; skills to help

represent the problem and skills to help solve the problem. Skills to help represent

the problem include restating the problem, stating the goal of the problem,

simplifying the problem, drawing a diagram, making a table, making a list, and

acting the problem out (Berardi-Coletta, Buyer, Dominowski, & Rellinger, 1995;

De Corte et al., 1996; Dominowski, 1998; Malouff, 2006). While skills to help

solve the problem include solving a simpler problem, working backwards,

guessing and checking, and looking for patterns (Bransford & Stein, 1993;

Nickerson, 1994).

Structuring mathematics lessons so that students work in groups to discuss

the problem and explain their use of problem-solving skills helps students engage

Page 38: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

25

in the components of problem solving (Johnson & Johnson, 1990; Yackel, Cobb,

& Wood, 1991). Talking through mathematical problems in groups helps students

understand how to solve the problems correctly together (Johnson & Johnson;

Puntambekar, 1999).

Group skills: Students need to be taught specific group skills in order for

them to work successfully in groups. Cohen (1994) noted that some students have

no group strategies other than physical or verbal assault. Students need to be

taught how to work together and specific teaching should deal with the

cooperative behaviours that are required by group work. Therefore, the first step

in preparing students for working together is to teach specific group skills (Cohen,

1994).

Group skills such as social skills, communication skills, and conflict

resolution skills are important components of achieving a successful group.

Groups must perform two kinds of processes: those concerned with completing

the task, and others concerned with maintaining the group (Armstrong & Priola,

2001). Group skills can be divided into task skills and maintenance skills (Dishon

& O’Leary, 1984). Task skills are associated with the specific problem-solving

task and maintenance skills are the social skills used in order to maintain the

group in working order.

Task skills included checking group understanding (Johnson et al., 1993),

giving ideas (Farivar & Webb, 1994), sharing information (Barron, 2000), talking

Page 39: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

26

about the work, getting the group back to work (Tjosvold, West, & Smith, 2003),

repeating what has been said (Dillenbourg et al., 1996), and asking questions

(Johnson, & Johnson, 1990). Maintenance skills included encouraging, checking

for agreement, encouraging other members to talk, sharing feelings, keeping

things calm (Johnson et al., 1993), responding to ideas (Dickson & McIntyre,

1997), using eye contact (Gillies & Ashman, 2000), and saying ‘thank you’

(Farivar & Webb, 1994).

Students need to learn what group skills are available and when they

should be used appropriately (Hayes, 2002; Johnson et al., 1993). One way to

improve the effectiveness of a group is to improve the interaction skills of its

members (Barker, Abrams, Tiyaamornwong, & Seibold, 2000). Group skills need

to be explicit and involve basic social skills such as sharing responsibility,

discussing group goals, active listening, as well as negotiating conflicts (Cohen,

1994; West, 2004).

Conflict management skills: Skills for managing conflict constructively

are important for the success of the group (Johnson et al., 1993). Therefore,

groups need to be taught conflict management skills just the same as academic

subjects. Furthermore, students need to be involved in working out reasons for

conflict and trying to solve them within their group (Benjamin et al., 1997).

Page 40: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

27

Students need to be taught two sets of skills for dealing with group

conflicts (Johnson et al., 1993). First, they need to know how to manage conflicts

that occur in their group. Second, they must be taught how to negotiate a

constructive resolution to any group conflict. Johnson et al. (1993) caution not to

intervene more than necessary during group conflict as group members should be

involved in working out solutions to their own conflicts. When intervention is

necessary, the group should be asked to create three possible solutions to the

conflict and then decide as a group which solution to try first.

West (2004) recommended four stages for conflict mediation: Step One

involves exploring the feelings of the team members involved in the conflict; Step

Two involves exploring the facts from the perception of each member; during

Step Three, group members agree to goals for avoiding a reoccurrence of the

conflict; and finally, in Step Four, the group members agree on an action plan. In

order to discourage destructive conflict, group member roles and responsibilities

should also be made clear to all group members (West, 2004).

Observation and feedback: Observations should be made of interactions

among group members engaged in group problem solving and group members

should receive feedback from these observations about their group performance

(Johnson et al., 1993). The feedback should include how the group is performing

and how the group is completing the task. Observations can be made about the

Page 41: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

28

task progress as well as the use of group skills and students can also be trained to

be observers (Johnson et al., 1993).

Observations based on group maintenance skills need to be shared with

students in order for them to monitor and evaluate their own group performance

(Dishon & O’Leary, 1984). To facilitate this process, Dishon and O’Leary (1984)

compiled a checklist of indicators to observe group behaviour (see Appendix A, p.

261). Feedback is also important to discover students’ ideas of the group problem-

solving process (Dishon & O’Leary, 1984; Goos et al., 2002). In order to reflect

on their problem-solving processes and increase their use of cooperative skills,

students need to receive feedback on their group’s problem-solving performance

(Chizhik, 1998; Mevarech, Siber, & Fine, 1991; Xiaondong, 2001).

Group roles: One problem that affects teamwork is the lack of clarity

about group roles (West, 2004). Therefore, assigning roles during group problem

solving is seen as an effective method for students to learn the specific social

skills needed for group learning (Cohen, 1994). Cohen suggested that in order to

make the roles public and give students authority to act in the group role, teachers

should set up a chart for role assignments. The chart also helps to clarify the role

to other group members.

Many different classifications of group roles have been produced. Group

roles in most of these classifications can be classified into two distinct categories:

Page 42: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

29

task roles and team roles (Bales, 1970; Bales & Cohen, 1979; Hoover, 2002).

Task roles relate to the focus of the group toward a solution, while team roles

focus on building and maintaining the group (Bales & Cohen; Gottlieb, 2003).

Some of the most cited group role classifications, found in the literature, are

presented in Table 2.1.

Table 2.1

Group Roles

Task roles

Checker (Cohen, 1994; Dishon & O'Leary, 1984;

Hayes, 2002) Coordinator (Gottlieb, 2003; Tyson, 1989) Elaborator (Gottlieb, 2003) Follower (Gottlieb, 2003) Information and opinion giver (Bales & Cohen,

1979; Gottlieb, 2003; Tyson, 1989) Information and opinion seeker (Gottlieb, 2003;

Tyson, 1989) Keyboarder (Cohen, 1994) Recorder (Dishon & O'Leary, 1984; Gottlieb,

2003; Johnson et al., 1993; Tyson, 1989) Summariser (Johnson et al., 1993; Dishon &

O'Leary, 1984; Tyson, 1989)

Team roles Conflict manager (Bales & Cohen, 1979; Gottlieb, 2003; Tyson, 1989)

Encourager (Bales & Cohen. 1979; Cohen, 1994; Dishon & O'Leary, 1984; Gottlieb, 2003; Hayes, 2002; Johnson et al., 1993; Tyson, 1989).

Moderator (Johnson et al., 1993) Spokesperson (Tyson, 1989) Supporter (Tyson, 1989)

Page 43: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

30

Computers: Computers have the potential to support group problem

solving and learning (Light, Littleton, Messer, & Joiner, 1994). However, in order

for this potential to be realised, there is a need for students to apply group skills

when engaged in problem solving and learning around a computer (Cohen, 1994;

Dishon & O’Leary, 1984). Assigning group roles is an effective way to develop

group skills for groups working around a computer (Cohen, 1994). The group

roles need to be rotated so all group members participate and have equal access to

the computer (Cohen, 1994).

CSCL is increasingly being used to support group decision making by

scaffolding online groups’ communication. However, Kreijns and Kirschner

(2001) pointed out that CSCL environments do not go far enough with regard to

supporting group problem solving and learning. CSCL environments need to

encourage group maintenance as well as task orientated discussion. According to

Kreijns et al. (2002), within the field of CSCL research, most of the focus has

been on the development of cognitive-technological scaffolds.

The cognitive scaffolds provided within Knowledge Forum® are typical

examples of technology scaffolds. However, as Bielaczyck and Collins (1999)

pointed out, cognitive/technology scaffolds by themselves are not sufficient to

ensure that the engagement and interaction necessary for knowledge building

discourse to occur within CSCL environments; they, like Kreijns et al. (2002),

contend that social interactions also need to be planned for within CSCL

environments. In both computer and non-computer contexts, the task also

Page 44: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

31

influences how well the group interacts and works together (Crook, 1999;

Jonassen & Kwon, 2001; Light & Littleton, 1999; Underwood & Underwood,

1999).

Problem-solving task: The problem-solving task administered to learners

can influence how well groups will work together (Jonassen & Kwon, 2001; Light

& Littleton, 1999; Underwood & Underwood, 1999). Problem tasks that are

closed, or only require one answer tend to not facilitate cooperative discourse and

interactions with other students in a group (Cohen, 1994; Lesh & Doerr, 2003).

Closed problems require low levels of cooperation as students do not need to

discuss how to proceed; nor do they need to restructure their own ideas taking into

account other members’ perspectives (Cohen, 1994).

However, according to Cohen (1994), groups are more likely to be

productive if tasks are complex, ill-defined, ill-structured and open-ended. Such

problems have vague or unclear goals, multiple solutions, multiple solution paths,

multiple criteria, and provide opportunities for students to engage in collective

meaning making (Cathcart, Samovar, & Henman, 1996; Jonassen & Carr, 2000;

Jonassen & Kwon, 2001). When problem tasks are complex and ill-defined,

students need to be involved in high levels of cooperation, as they work together

(Lesh & Kelly, 2002; Puntambekar, 1999). Students need to work as a group in

order to solve the problem and need to discuss how to proceed and reconstruct

ideas taking into account other members’ perspectives (Puntambekar, 1999).

Page 45: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

32

Research regarding model-eliciting activities confirms that the use of

realistic ill-defined problems allows students to engage in collective meaning

making (English, Fox, & Watters, 2005; Lesh & Lamon, 1992; Zawojewski et al.,

2003). Model-eliciting activities are mathematical-based tasks that present

realistic problem scenarios and often require students to work together as a group

to develop a shared model that can be used to solve the problem situation (Lesh &

Doerr, 2003; Lesh & Harel, 2003). The activities are designed to encourage

students to build mathematical models in order to solve complex problems.

Model-eliciting activities are based on six specific principles (Lesh, Hoover,

Hole, Kelly, & Post, 2000):

1. Model construction principle: problems must be designed to allow for the

creation of a model dealing with elements, relationships and operations

between these elements and patterns and rules governing these

relationships.

2. The reality principle: problems must be meaningful and relevant to the

students.

3. Self-assessment principle: students must be able to self-assess or measure

the usefulness of their solutions.

4. Construct documentation principle: students must be able to reveal and

document their thinking processes within their solution.

Page 46: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

33

5. Construct shareability and reusability principle: solutions created by

students should be generalisable or easily adapted to other situations.

6. Effective prototype principle: others should easily be able to interpret

solutions.

2.1.2 Cognitive factors

Cognitive factors influence how group members develop a shared

knowledge and understanding about the problem (Lesh et al., 2000). A degree of

shared knowledge about the team is necessary for teams to work effectively

(Cannon-Bowers & Salas, 1998; Lim & Klein, 2006). This knowledge contributes

to the group’s ability to accomplish their task work (Canon-Bowers & Salas,

2001) and ensures that problem solving becomes a co-construction of ideas by

group members (Chizhik, 1998; Light & Littleton, 1999).

The development of shared knowledge about a problem is predicated on

the construction of a shared understanding of the problem (Cohen & Gibson,

2003). As Barron (2000) pointed out, group members must establish a shared

understanding to make sense of the problem as a group. The following sections

highlight that groups are more likely to construct shared internal knowledge and

understandings if they develop shared external representations about the

problem(s) being investigated.

Page 47: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

34

Shared external representation: In order to develop a shared

understanding of the problem, group members must first negotiate a shared

external representation or model of the problem (Fiore & Schooler, 2004). Groups

of students need to develop an external representation of the problem in order to

articulate their thinking, making their group understanding explicit and visible in

order to collaborate with group members (Lewis, 1997; Mohammed & Dumville,

2001). By sharing their ideas, students are able to gain a joint understanding of

not only of how their group works but also of the problem-solving task they are

collaboratively completing (Antaki & Lewis, 1986; Cathcart et al., 1996; King,

1989). External representations facilitate the process of articulating students’

thinking and allow group members to formulate accurate shared internal

representations of both their team-work and task-work (Cannon-Bowers & Salas,

2001; Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994; Webber, Chen, Payne, Marsh, & Zaccaro,

2000).

Shared internal representations: The development of shared knowledge

by a group is facilitated by groups developing shared internal representations

regarding both task- and team-related information (Rentsch & Klimoski, 2001).

The two dimensions of group functioning, that is, the task a group is required to

complete and the group as a social unit, need to be focused on in order for groups

to achieve a shared understanding of the task and how to work successfully as a

team (West, 2004). Group efforts need to be made to help all group members

Page 48: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

35

understand the task and the team requirements (Johnson et al., 1993). The

development of shared knowledge by a group can be facilitated by the

development of schema similarity among group members (Rentsch & Klimoski,

2001; Woehr & Rentsch, 2003).

In order to evaluate schema similarity, Rentsch and Klimoski (2001)

created the construct of Team Member Schema Similarity (TMSS). TMSS is the

degree of similar or overlapping team knowledge that members hold of teamwork

and task work (Langan-Fox, Anglim, & Wilson, 2004). According to Woehr and

Rentsch (2003), teamwork schema similarity leads to improved team processes,

while task work schema similarity leads to improved task performance. Each

group member’s understanding, of what they are working on, needs to merge into

a similar cognitive or mental model during collaboration (Cannon-Bowers &

Salas, 2001; Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994).

In a construct similar to Rentsch and Klimoski’s (2001) TMSS, Cannon-

Bowers and Salas (1998) focused on groups constructing shared mental models. A

shared mental model is a mental representation of shared knowledge (Halford,

1993; Mathieu, Heffner, Goodwin, Cannon-Bowers, & Salas, 2005). This shared

knowledge combines knowledge about the task and the team, including

declarative knowledge (knowledge about) and procedural knowledge (knowledge

how) (Cooke, Salas, Kiekel, & Bell, 2004; Mohammed & Dumville, 2001).

In order to build an effective shared group mental model during group

problem solving, group members must also hold a similar shared and accurate

Page 49: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

36

knowledge about the components of successful groups and the problem-solving

task (Cooke et al., 2004; Fiore & Schooler, 2004; Mathieu, Goodwin, Heffner,

Salas, & Cannon-Bowers, 2000; Rentsch & Klimoski, 2001; Smith-Jentsch,

Campbell, & Milanovich, 2001). Groups are more likely to work effectively when

members develop a shared model together (Woehr & Rentsch, 2003).

The cognitive factors discussed in this section of the literature review and

represented in Figure 2.2 highlight that groups are more likely to develop a shared

knowledge and understanding if they develop a shared internal representation as

well as a shared external representation.

Cognitivefactors

Shared knowledge and understanding

Shared external representation

Shared internalrepresentation

Figure 2.2. Model representing cognitive factors.

2.1.2.1 Strategies to address cognitive factors

In order to develop a shared knowledge and understanding, group

members need to articulate their problem-solving plans and group strategies

(Fiore & Schooler, 2004). Group members develop shared understandings of the

group process and the problem-solving task by asking other group members to

Page 50: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

37

justify and clarify ideas they do not understand (Fiore & Schooler, 2004; Goos et

al., 2002; Klimoski, & Mohammed, 1994). Groups also need to reflect on the

solution process, as well as the group collaboration process (Klimoski &

Mohammed, 1994; Puntambekar, 1999).

In order to improve the effectiveness of group problem solving and

learning, group members need to reflect on how they are accomplishing the

shared task and how their group is working as a team. Reflecting on group

problem solving helps students understand how the team goals of the group are

related to the task goals (Beamish & Au, 1995). Scaffolds need to be provided to

help students reflect on their problem solving (Puntambekar, 1999). The

following sections discuss specific strategies that help students address the

cognitive factors relevant to group problem solving and learning, including the

use of scaffolds, strategic questions, and CSCL environments.

Scaffolds: Scaffolds are an important aspect of students’ learning during

group problem solving and learning (Hamilton, 1986; Hartman, 2001;

Puntambekar, 1999). Scaffolds support, guide, and cue thinking (Vygotsky, 1978)

and are needed to help students develop a shared model as well as reflect on their

group problem-solving process (Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994).

Scaffolding can be initially provided to groups in order for them to

understand what the problem is asking, plan how the group will go about solving

Page 51: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

38

it, monitor the group’s progress towards a solution and finally evaluate the

effectiveness of their group problem-solving process. Beamish and Au (1995)

suggested that this process can be facilitated by encouraging students to question

what they know about a specific problem, what they want to know, and what they

must learn to solve the problem.

Strategic questions: The use of strategic questions such as those utilised

by Gama (2000), Johnson et al. (1993) and King (1991) scaffold the problem

solving as well as the group process (see Table 2.2). The series of structured

questions assists students with their problem solving. According to King (1991),

students also need to be trained to ask scaffolding questions of each other during

group problem solving. King (1991) categorised these scaffolding questions into

planning, monitoring, and evaluating questions. Johnson et al. (1993) also

suggested that groups need checklists and questions to structure the group

learning process. Strategic questions and scaffolds can also be incorporated with

CSCL to promote reflection and positive interaction (Puntambekar, 1999).

Page 52: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

39

Table 2.2

Strategic Questions

Types of questions Examples

Planning:

Gama (2000)

What is our plan?

What is the problem?

What do we know about the problem?

King (1991) What is the nature of the task?

What is the goal?

What information is needed?

What strategies can be used?

How much time and what resources are needed?

Monitoring:

King (1991)

Do you understand what to do?

Does the task make sense?

Are the goals being reached?

Do changes need to made?

Johnson et al. (1993) What are three things your group is doing well and

one thing that needs to improve?

Evaluating:

King (1991)

Have the goals been reached?

What worked?

What didn’t work?

What would be done differently next time?

Johnson et al. (1993) How frequently did each member:

Explain how to solve the problem?

Correct or clarify other member’s explanations?

Page 53: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

40

CSCL: Scaffolding group problem solving in the Knowledge Forum®

CSCL environment requires supporting students with their problem solving and

group work around the computer, as well as supporting problem solving between

groups working online. The Knowledge Forum® software can help support group

online problem solving as students collaboratively construct knowledge using the

shared interface (Hakkarainen, Lipponen, Jarvela, & Niemivirta, 1999; O’Malley,

1995; Wang et al., 2001).

Knowledge Forum© supports a student-centred open-ended learning

environment where students are actively engaged in knowledge building

(Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1994). The database software supports groups of

students in constructing “notes” about a problem through structures such as model

building and model critiquing scaffolds. Other groups can view the database,

adding text, graphics, questions, and comments on each other’s work.

However, Cannon-Bowers and Salas (2001) warned that, if not enough

support is given to online teams, they will fail. They indicated that in order to

scaffold effective online problem-solving groups, there is a need to identify

successful group skills that combine task-related knowledge and skills with team-

related knowledge and skills.

Page 54: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

41

2.1.3 Summary

Participating in successful group problem solving and learning, within the

CSCL Knowledge Forum® environment, entails students co-constructing a

shared understanding by combining task- and team-related knowledge and skills.

This shared understanding is also essential for groups working together on non-

computer tasks. The shared understanding is facilitated by groups developing a

shared mental model or external representation of their group problem-solving

process (Fiore & Schooler, 2004; Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994; Mohammed &

Dumville, 2001).

Students cannot form a shared understanding or interact successfully as a

group, if they have had no preparation for group work (Cohen, 1994; Johnson et

al., 1993; Ogden, 2000). A number of factors influence how groups work

effectively and form a shared understanding, including organisational factors such

as how the group develops and resolves conflicts, and cognitive factors such as

how the group forms a shared understanding (see Figure 2.3).

Page 55: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

42

Organisational factors

Forming

Storming

Norming

Performing

Adjourning

Interaction face-to-face and on-line

Constructive conflict

Social skills

Individual accountability

Positive interdependence

Group processing

Cognitivefactors

Shared knowledge and understanding

Shared external representation

Shared internalrepresentation

Figure 2.3. Factors influencing efficiency of group problem solving and learning.

Page 56: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

43

However, incorporating organisational and cognitive strategies to help

scaffold group problem solving is insufficient. Group problem solving and

learning activities also need students to metacognitively reflect on the

organisational and cognitive factors influencing their group work (see Figure 2.4).

When students reflect on their group problem solving, both their problem solving

and their group work is said to improve (Cohen, 1994; Johnson et al., 1993).

Cognitivefactors

Organisationalfactors

Metacognitivefactors

Group problem solving and learning

Figure 2.4. Group problem solving and learning factors.

2.2 Group metacognitive factors

This section first highlights the varied definitions of metacognition, as

well as setting the stage for the establishment of group metacognition. It then sets

out to combine theories on group work and metacognition in order to focus on

how to scaffold group metacognition within groups working around a computer

Page 57: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

44

and between groups working together on the online Knowledge Forum® CSCL

environment.

There are many varied definitions of metacognition (Antaki & Lewis,

1986). Flavell, Friedrichs, and Hoyt (1970) first introduced the concept of

metacognition as an individual’s awareness, choice, and control of their cognitive

processes. Metacognition is defined as “one’s knowledge concerning one’s own

cognitive processes and products or anything related to them” (Flavell, 1976, p.

232). Schoenfeld (1987) focused on metacognition as students’ beliefs based on

past experiences, knowledge of own thinking processes and self-awareness of the

process of problem solving. Metacognition is a conscious effort to identify

learning strategies and being able to apply strategies across various knowledge

domains (Beamish & Au, 1995).

Metacognitive research has focused on three main components of

metacognition:

1. Metacognitive beliefs and attitudes (Brown & Palincsar, 1989;

Schoenfeld, 1992).

2. Metacognitive control (Beamish & Au, 1995; Blakey & Spence, 1990)

3. Metacognitive knowledge (Flavell, 1976; Schoenfeld, 1987)

Metacognitive belief is based on previous experience, skills, and

knowledge and affects motivation to direct metacognitive knowledge and use

metacognitive strategies (Brown & Palincsar, 1989; Desoete, Roeyers, & Buysse,

Page 58: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

45

2001; Desoete, Roeyers, & De Clercq, 2003). Metacognitive control is the use of

strategies including planning, monitoring, and evaluating and the strategies are

essential for the self-regulation of thought processes (Beamish & Au, 1995;

Garofalo, & Lester, 1985). Metacognitive knowledge consists of knowledge about

thought processes (Flavell, 1976; Schoenfeld, 1987).

Metacognitive knowledge can be manifested either as declarative or

procedural knowledge (Azevedo & Hadwin, 2005; Desoete et al., 2001).

Declarative metacognitive knowledge focuses on what students know about

knowledge and learning where procedural knowledge is what students know

about the process of using their declarative knowledge (Efklides, 2006;

Schoenfeld, 1992). Procedural metacognitive knowledge is necessary in order for

students to apply declarative knowledge and is important for successful problem

solving (Berardi-Coletta et al., 1995).

Metacognitive knowledge

Metacognitive belief

ProceduralMetacognitive

controlDeclarative

Metacognitive strategies

Planning

Monitoring

Evaluating

Metacognitivefactors

Figure 2.5. Model representing metacognitive factors.

Page 59: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

46

Figure 2.5 highlights the main metacognitive factors identified in the

research literature. As the figure shows, metacognitive knowledge is distinguished

from metacognitive control. Palincsar and Brown (1984) believed it was

important to distinguish between the two as metacognitive knowledge deals with

students’ knowledge of thinking whereas metacognitive control, also called

executive control, is concerned with learners actively planning, monitoring,

revising, and evaluating their own learning.

Most research on metacognition has considered the role of metacognition

as an individual learning process (Hoyles & Healy, 1994; Hurme & Järvelä,

2001). By focusing solely on the individual student, researchers have failed to

address the dynamics required for progressive knowledge building by

collaborative learning groups (Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1994). It is in this context

that Hurme and Järvelä (2001) called for more research on the metacognition

process as a socially shared practice.

The shared understanding and shared mental representations group

members have about the way their group performs the task and works as a team

are examples of group metacognition (Hinsz, 2004). According to Hinsz (1995,

2004), the understanding group members share about the way groups operate and

function represents how group members think about the way groups process

information and perform cognitive tasks.

Group metacognition is facilitated by interaction between group members

(McNeese, 2000). The use of metacognition facilitates successful group problem

Page 60: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

47

solving, as learners reflect on their learning processes more readily in groups

(Antaki & Lewis, 1986; Hamilton, 1986; Murphey & Jacobs, 2000). Group

members can discuss and compare their thoughts and behaviour with other group

members. Metacognition of groups also requires group members to understand

how effective groups process information and perform tasks (Hinsz, 2004).

Planning to achieve the group task goal, as well as monitoring the group’s

progress required to achieve the group goal, are group metacognitive processes.

Effective learners need to know how to plan, monitor, and regulate their

learning processes, including those involved in cooperative learning contexts

(Antaki & Lewis, 1986). Metacognitive individuals plan, monitor, and evaluate

their learning processes (Schraw, 2001). These abilities are also essential for

groups building a shared knowledge (Bereiter & Scardamalia, 1989). Groups of

students need to develop co-cognition in order to collaboratively develop concepts

and monitor their own group performance (Costa & O’Leary, 1992). Co-cognition

requires the cooperative development of strategies used to plan, monitor, and

evaluate group behaviour.

2.2.1 Group metacognitive strategies

Studies have shown that students do not engage in metacognitive thinking

unless they are encouraged to do so (Gillies, 2000). The three main metacognitive

strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating are identified in the

Page 61: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

48

metacognitive literature for individual learners (Flavell, 1976; Gourgey, 2001).

These strategies can also be used in a group context where students can note any

difficulty with working in their groups and solve the problem as a group (Johnson

et al., 1993).

The use of strategy training in metacognitive research has been frequently

used and includes students planning how to approach a given task, monitoring

their progress in the task and finally, evaluating their learning process (Flavell,

1976). However, students also need instruction on strategies including how, when,

and why to use each strategy (King, 1991; Palincsar, 1986). Metacognitive

strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating are needed in problem-solving

groups in order to attain specific learning goals (Blakey & Spence, 1990; Flavell,

1976; Lesh et al., 2000; McNesse, 2000; Tombari & Borich, 1999).

Group metacognition facilitates the use of appropriate metacognitive

strategies during group problem solving and learning. The strategies assist groups

to progress towards a solution and reflect on their group learning process. For

groups to be successful depends on the members knowing how and when to use

specific problem-solving and group strategies (Johnson et al., 1993). According to

Desoete et al. (2001), strategies can be selected to solve the group problem

situation in order to improve the group learning process. Groups can use

metacognitive strategies to determine whether their group problem-solving is

successful and what remedial action needs to be taken to make the group

problem-solving process more effective (McNeese, 2000). Students can develop a

Page 62: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

49

shared understanding of specific group problem-solving behaviours and strategies

helpful for their group (Cohen, 1994). Using the student-generated criteria group

members can plan, monitor, and evaluate their own group performance (Davidson

& Worsham, 1992; Gillies, 2000).

As noted previously, research studies have shown that students do not

engage in self-regulated or metacognitive thinking unless they are encouraged to

do so (Flavell, 1976; Gillies, 2000; Tombari & Borich, 1999; Xiaodong, 2001).

While there are several approaches to metacognitive instruction, the most

effective involves providing learners with: knowledge of problem-solving and

group learning strategies (Wilson & Johnson, 2000); practice in using the

strategies (Flavell, 1976); and opportunities to plan, monitor, and evaluate the

strategies used (Xiaodong, 2001).

2.2.2 Group metacognitive scaffolds

As students do not engage in metacognitive thinking unless it is structured

in learning activities, it is important to include metacognitive scaffolds for the

group learning process (Gillies, 2000). Metacognitive scaffolds can be provided

so students are able to regulate their own learning. The metacognitive scaffolds

encourage students to gradually take responsibility for group problem solving by

developing a shared understanding regarding effective group work and problem

solving (Hinsz, 2004). Group metacognitive scaffolds can be gradually withdrawn

Page 63: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

50

as students generate their own strategies and develop a shared knowledge

(Chizhik, 1998).

One framework for scaffolding problem solving is the metacognitive

questionnaires developed by Fortunato, Hecht, Tittle, and Alvarez, (1991) (see

Appendix B, p. 262) and King (1991). The questionnaire developed used

metacognitive strategies to scaffold questions based around the problem-solving

process. The three main metacognitive scaffolds used in King’s study were

planning, monitoring, and evaluating. Puntambekar (1999) also devised a

cognitive/metacognitive framework for group problem solving, involving four

categories: orientation, organisation, execution, and verification. Orientation is

concerned with assessing and understanding the requirements of the problem;

organisation involves monitoring behaviours, planning actions and choosing

strategies; execution involves monitoring progress towards a solution; verification

consists of evaluating decisions made.

The group learning context requires that groups have knowledge of

specific group problem-solving strategies that emphasise both the task and the

team work (McNeese, 2000). Figure 2.6 is compiled from a review of the

literature concerning group problem solving, learning, and metacognition, and

highlights that group metacognition needs to be scaffolded in the following ways:

explicit instruction of problem-solving and group strategies (Wilson & Johnson,

2000; Xiaondong, 2001); provision of a supportive environment, where students

can practise their group problem-solving strategies (Gourgey, 2001); mediation of

Page 64: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

51

metacognitive strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating by using diaries

and checklists (Blakey & Spence, 1990; Wilson & Johnson, 2000).

Wilson and Johnson (2000) stated that students need to be provided with

knowledge of group problem-solving strategies, practice in using the strategies

and opportunity to evaluate the outcomes of the strategies (Flavell, 1976;

Xiaodong, 2001). The importance of a supportive environment in developing

students’ metacognition was emphasised by Brown and Palincsar (1989).

Fostering a learning environment that encourages metacognition requires

developing students’ awareness of their own thinking (Blakey & Spence, 1990).

Blakey and Spence (1990) stated that class discussion should focus on the

learning and thinking processes used, in order to develop students’ awareness of

learning strategies.

It is important to encourage groups to reflect on their relationships and

achievement. This requires a continuous analysis of how group effectiveness can

be enhanced (Beatty & Barker, 2004). Groups need to spend time discussing how

well they work together and planning how to improve future group work

(Murphey & Jacobs, 2000). West (2004) stated that ‘task reflexivity’ and ‘social

reflexivity’ are when teams reflect on the social climate of the group and how

they achieved their group goal. Students need to be encouraged to communicate

with each other, thinking aloud while working together and reflecting on their

learning processes, in order to develop a shared understanding (Murphey &

Jacobs, 2000).

Page 65: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

52

Instruction on problem solving skills

and group skills

Provision of a supportive

environment to practise skills

Metacognition scaffolded by diaries

and checklists

Scaffolds

Metacognitive factors

Cognitivefactors

Organisationalfactors

Figure 2.6. Scaffolding group metacognition.

Page 66: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

53

One way to encourage self-reflection and the use of metacognitive

strategies is through the use of scaffolded questions in a journal or learning diary

(Blakey & Spence, 1990; Walker, 1985). Students, who write about and monitor

their learning process through the use of a diary, or journal, have been shown to

be more efficient learners (Gourgey, 2001). Wilson and Johnson (2000) used four

scaffolds to develop primary students’ metacognitive thinking: written reflections

in a journal; self assessment orally with peers and team leaders as well as in the

students’ journals; cooperative group work in order for students to reflect on their

small group practices; and concept mapping both as a reflective and a self-

assessment activity.

Diaries and journals are used as a means to develop metacognition as

students can write and reflect upon their thinking while making note of

inconsistencies and progressively commenting on any difficulties (Blakey &

Spence, 1990). Reflective writing is related to the development of metacognitive

skills and writing in mathematics helps students reflect on their work and helps to

clarify and deepen understanding (Pugalee, 2001). Writing helps to develop the

vocabulary students need for thinking and talking about their learning (Blakey &

Spence, 1990).

Including a checklist in the diary, in which students can monitor their

learning, assists students to be reflective learners and scaffolds their

metacognitive processes (Blakey & Spence, 1990; Mueller & Fleming, 1994;

Wilson & Johnson, 2000). An effective way to develop metacognition is for

Page 67: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

54

students to answer a series of metacognitive questions that focuses on the

planning of the problem task, monitoring progress towards completion, and

evaluating the learning process (Kramarski & Mevarech, 2003). Schraw (2001)

proposed a checklist for improving students’ thinking about their learning (see

Appendix C, p. 263). The checklist includes questions students could ask

themselves during the planning, monitoring, and evaluating stages of their

problem solving.

2.2.3 Summary

The review of the literature indicates that metacognitive strategies are

important for developing group metacognition; applying metacognitive strategies

for group problem solving allows students to focus on the organisational and

cognitive factors that influence how groups perform their problem-solving task

and work together as a team. Also, the literature clearly points out that students do

not engage in metacognitive strategies unless they are structured within the

learning activity (Gillies, 2000). Scaffolds such as checklists, diaries, and a

supportive environment need to be provided in order to develop group

metacognitive strategies. Groups can plan, monitor, and evaluate group strategies

specific to their team and the group problem-solving task. They can also apply

these strategies to develop a shared group understanding.

Page 68: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

55

2.3 Conclusion

There is a definite relationship between positive group work outcomes and

group metacognition. However, the majority of research in metacognition has

tended to look at individual student’s learning process (Gourgey, 2001; Hartman,

2001). A number of factors influence how groups work effectively, including

organisational factors such as how the group develops, works together, and

resolves conflicts, and cognitive factors such as how the group forms a shared

understanding. Group metacognition depends on the degree to which the

cognitive representations that group members have for the task and learning

situation are shared (Hinsz, 2004).

Results from research literature have resulted in an initial conceptual

framework (see Figure 2.7) to inform the research study. The framework

highlights the fact that effective group problem solving requires groups to think

about organisational, cognitive, and metacognitive factors. These factors

influence how groups work together and develop a shared understanding. This

shared understanding is facilitated by groups developing a shared internal

understanding and a shared external representation of their group problem-solving

process (Fiore & Schooler, 2004; Klimoski & Mohammed, 1994; Mohammed &

Dumville, 2001).

Page 69: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

56

Organisational factors

Forming

Storming

Norming

Performing

Adjourning

Interaction face-to-face and on-line

Constructive conflict

Social skills

Individual accountability

Positive interdependence

Group processing

Cognitivefactors

Metacognitivefactors

Metacognitive Knowledge

Metacognitive belief

Metacognitive control

Procedural

Declarative

Metacognitive strategies

Planning Monitoring Evaluating

Shared knowledge and understanding

Shared external representation

Effective group problem solving and

learning

Shared internalrepresentation

Figure 2.7. Conceptual framework.

Page 70: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

57

CHAPTER 3: RESEARCH DESIGN AND METHOD

The aim of this research study was to develop a conceptual model that

provides guidelines for scaffolding within- and between-group metacognition in

Computer Supported Collaborative Learning (CSCL) environments. This chapter

describes the research design used in this study, the methods of data collection

and analysis, as well as how the study proceeded to achieve the research aim.

3.1 Research design

In order to meet the aim of this study, a design research methodology was

utilised. Design research methodology is increasingly being utilised and is

regarded as having a key role in advancing both practical and theoretical

knowledge in a variety of educational contexts (Bereiter, 2002; Edelson, 2002).

Design research, also called development research or design experiment,

is a research methodology used in a range of research contexts concerned with

refining theory and developing instructional materials (Barab, 2006; Collins,

Joseph, & Bielaczyc, 2004; O’Donnell, 2004). Design research looks at the key

elements of the research setting and the intervention and how they work together

toward an educational goal (Collins et al., 2004). The methodology allows for the

collecting and analysing of large amounts of data and analysing in terms of key

elements in order to understand the context in detail. Woodruff and Nirula (2005)

Page 71: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

58

stated that this allows the whole learning context to be taken into account when it

is difficult to study one aspect independently.

Design research methodology includes cyclical phases of preliminary

design, teaching the activity, also called the teaching experiment, and data

analysis (Shavelson, Phillips, Towne, & Feuer, 2003). Teaching experiments are

used in order to develop activities and instructional tools or for studying the

influence of instruction upon students (Romberg, 1992). The cyclical nature of

the design research methodology ensures that the teaching experiment is revised

for each successive research cycle (Collins et al., 2004; Woodruff & Nirula,

2005).

A design research methodology thus was adopted for this study because,

unlike other methodologies, it allows for multiple cycles of design, experiment,

and analysis in order to not only develop instructional materials but also to refine

theory (Bereiter, 2002; Collins et al., 2004; Shavelson et al., 2003; Woodruff &

Nirula, 2005). The multiple cycles of design, experiment, and analysis also

enabled the researcher to progressively test and refine the educational designs

based on theoretical principles utilised during the course of the study (Collins et

al., 2004).

However, design research is not a fully defined methodology and other

methods may also be employed to understand the research context (Bereiter,

2002; Confrey, 2006). Therefore, a descriptive case study method was also used

for data collection to incorporate multiple sources of data and in order to bring out

Page 72: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

59

the viewpoint of the group members involved in the study (Stake, 1995; Tellis,

1997; Yin, 1993). A descriptive case study research method facilitates the

development of 'thick descriptions' (Huberman & Miles, 2002; Stake, 1995).

3.2 Data Collection and Analysis

Data collection was conducted in an ongoing hermeneutic cycle (Guba &

Lincoln, 1989). Collected data were thus were analysed as they were collected in

order to inform further collection (Huberman & Miles, 2002). Yin (1994) also

suggested three principles of data collection for case studies: first, use multiple

sources for the triangulation of data; second, create a case study database; and

finally, maintain a chain of evidence. Stake (1995) suggested using multiple

sources of data to bring out the details of the study. Following the principles of

data collection proposed by Yin (2003) and Stake (1995), multiple data were

collected from transcripts of classroom interaction, participant observation,

interviews, and classroom artefacts such as the computer printouts, diaries, and

checklists (see Table 3.1).

Page 73: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

60

Table 3.1

Data Collection Methods

Source of data Nature of data collected Time/frequency of data

collection

Observations Participant observation

Videotapes

Audio (MP3) transcriptions

Throughout study

Throughout study

Throughout study

Group interviews Focus group interviews After each cycle

Classroom

artefacts

Diaries

Checklists

Questionnaires

Knowledge Forum notes

Mathematical ranking

models

Throughout study

Throughout study

Prior to and after each cycle

Throughout study

After each cycle

3.2.1 Observations

Observations involved the collection of a variety of data including the use

of a video camera to record groups as they worked together, the use of MP3

digital recorders to record what group members were saying to each other, as well

as participant observations of the learning situation.

Johnson et al. (1993) suggest observing the interactions among group

members in order to assess students’ use of group skills and behaviours.

Page 74: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

61

Observations can lead researchers toward a greater understanding of the case

(Stake, 1995). Observation is the recording and describing of behaviour as it

occurs (Johnson & Johnson, 2004).

A video camera and MP3 digital recorders were used to supplement the

direct observation by the researcher. Transcripts of the recordings provided a

record of group interaction and tally sheets were used to mark when students and

groups engaged in one of the targeted actions or behaviours.

3.2.1.1 Participant observation

Participant observation is a valuable research tool as it involves observing

as well as participating in the research context (Bositis, 1988). The participant-

observer role enabled the researcher to gain insights from the students as they

were involved in the study activity. Thus, information derived from the analysis

of the participant observation data enabled the researcher to constantly develop

and refine the classroom instructional activities utilised during the course of the

study.

Bositis (1988) stated that participant observation allows researchers to

both observe certain behaviours as well as being able to provoke certain

behaviours to be observed. During design research the researcher is directly

involved in the situation and can participate as well as observe (Bereiter, 2002).

The researcher was a participant observer with all classes involved in this

research, introducing the activity to the students, and interacting with the students

Page 75: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

62

as they completed the activity. During observation a record was kept of certain

events for further analysis and reporting. Video and MP3 digital recordings were

also transcribed for analysis.

3.2.1.2 Video recordings

Groups of students were videotaped to capture the discourse within their

groups. One camera was used and one group from each class was videotaped from

each learning activity phase. A video camera was used with one group from each

class in order to capture the group work around the computer and in order to

follow the group’s development.

Video recordings preserve aspects of interaction including talking,

gestures, and eye gaze. Walker (1985) warned that no recording is a full account

of what actually happens in a context. However, group work is complex and the

use of videotapes is an extension of participant observation and allows for

numerous revisits in order to observe the full extent of the learning context. The

weekly video recordings enabled the researcher to replay sequences of interaction

repeatedly.

3.2.1.3 Audio (MP3) recordings

MP3 recorders were also used in order to capture the discourse of all the

groups. These provided an excellent record of the group’s interactions as students

adapted to them quickly and the majority of students tended to forget they were

Page 76: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

63

being recorded. Each group had one MP3 recorder and one student from each

group wore the MP3 recorder around their neck on a lanyard. The wearing of the

MP3 recorders was rotated weekly to make sure that recordings were clearly

captured from each student.

The MP3 audio recordings were transcribed for further analysis. The

transcripts were saved into the Nvivo® software and categories were coded

according to the factors identified in the initial conceptual framework (see Figure

2.7).

3.2.2 Focus group interview

At the end of each research cycle, a focus group interview was used to

record students’ reactions towards the group work, as well as to ascertain

students’ perceptions, feelings, attitudes and ideas regarding the group work and

the group processes. Eder and Fingerson (2003) recommend interviewing children

as a group to make the context more natural and less intimidating than individual

interviews.

Interviews are considered interactive encounters as the social interaction

can shape the nature of the knowledge gained (Fontana & Frey, 2000). However,

data from a focus group is often richer than data from individual interviews as

participants can interact amongst themselves leading to an open participant-led

discussion (Maykut & Morehouse, 1994; Vaughn, Schumm, & Sinagub, 1996).

Page 77: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

64

During the interview, students were encouraged to discuss their experience

in their own words. They were asked what they thought about the activity and

what they had learnt about working together. Interviews were conducted using a

format designed to elicit responses about thinking about cooperative group work.

Charmaz (2003) listed examples of questions to use during interviews (see

Appendix D, p. 264). Charmaz’s questions were used as starting points for the

group interviews. For example: What advice would you give to someone who has

discovered they will be working in a group?

The interviews were conducted in the week following the last computer

session. Students were invited to attend group meetings where they could discus

the research project. All interviews were recorded and transcribed in order to have

a detailed record for analysis. Pertinent comments were noted for further analysis.

3.2.3 Classroom artefacts

Five types of classroom artefacts were collected and analysed during the

course of the study: diaries, checklists, questionnaires, Knowledge Forum notes,

and mathematical ranking models.

3.2.3.1 Diaries

Students were encouraged to maintain a group diary to document issues

the group encountered in the process of working together (Hare & O’Neill, 2000;

Walker, 1985). Diaries can be used as a means to develop metacognition as

Page 78: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

65

students reflect upon their group thinking, make note of their group

inconsistencies, and progressively comment on how the group deals with any

difficulties (Blakey & Spence, 1990).

The students made all of their notes regarding the task and their team work

in their group diaries. The groups of students were also asked to plan, monitor,

and evaluate their team work in their diaries using the checklists provided.

3.2.3.2 Checklists

Group checklists are a good way to monitor group issues and can be used

to provide immediate feedback to groups regarding their use of cooperative skills

(Reid, Forrestal, & Cook, 1989). Checklists were used to monitor the

effectiveness of instruction and groups were asked to complete a group-

processing checklist in their group diary during the computer session.

The checklists included metacognitive questions based on the studies by

King (1991) and Schraw (2001) that focused on planning, monitoring, and

evaluating. King (1991) suggested using strategic questions to guide students’

cognitive and metacognitive activity during problem solving. Schraw (2001) also

used metacognitive questioning to help students to monitor their performance.

Students were asked to complete the planning checklist in the first

computer session prior to commencing the problem-solving task. The planning

checklist asked the groups to describe both the problem and their plan to solve it.

The initial planning checklist included the following questions:

Page 79: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

66

What do we know about the problem?

What is the goal?

What is our plan to solve the problem and reach the goal?

What group roles will we use?

What group skills will we use?

In the following three computer sessions, the students completed

monitoring checklists in which they were asked questions regarding how their

group was completing the problem-solving task and how their group was

functioning as a team. These questions were based on King’s (1991) study

regarding the effects of training students in strategic questioning on children's

problem-solving performance. The initial monitoring checklist asked groups to

complete the following questions:

Are we following the group plan?

Do we need to make changes?

What group roles will we use?

What group skills will we use?

Johnson and Johnson (1999) suggested that groups also need opportunity

to describe what group actions are helpful and unhelpful and make decisions

about what behaviours to continue or change. In order to focus group members on

the positive aspects of their team as well as what improvements could be made to

the team problem solving, the checklists also asked groups:

Page 80: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

67

What are two things our group is doing well and one thing that

needs to improve?

The second monitoring checklist asked the students three questions to help

them plan their computer session and to focus members on the positive aspects of

their group. The following questions were asked:

What group roles will we use?

What group skills will we use?

What is our group is doing well?

The third and final monitoring checklist from Cycle 2 also included a

further question in order for students to plan their Knowledge Forum team work

and included:

What group skills will we use on Knowledge Forum?

The final evaluation checklist, based on questions used by King (1991)

and Schraw (2001), was completed at the final computer session and asked groups

to comment on the following:

Have we reached our goal?

What worked?

What didn’t work?

What could we do differently next time?

The groups were also asked to comment on how the diary and Knowledge

Forum could be improved. This question is consistent with Woodruff and Nirula’s

Page 81: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

68

(2005) view of student participants as co-investigators in order to discover how

technology could be used better within specific activities.

3.2.3.3 Questionnaires

Students were also asked to individually complete questionnaires prior to

and after the learning activity. The initial questionnaire (see Appendix E, p. 265)

contained five questions that elicited students’ initial thoughts on group work. The

first two questions asked were:

What is a group?

What do you like about working in a group?

These two questions are based on questions from Carley’s (1997) study on

information systems. The first question was designed to elicit students’ shared

declarative knowledge, while the second question combined both declarative and

procedural knowledge. The following two questions asked were derived from

questions used in a study by Whicker, Nunnery, and Bol (1997) in which students

were interviewed on their perception of cooperative group work. These questions

focused on the student’s prior experiences with group work and included:

What do you not like about working in a group?

Where did you learn to work with other people?

The final question added was taken from Charmaz’s (2003) guide on questions to

use during interviews (see Appendix F, p266). The final question asked students:

Page 82: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

69

What advice would you give to someone who has just discovered

that they will be working in a group?

A final questionnaire (see Appendix F, p. 266) was also completed at the

conclusion of each research cycle. The questionnaire incorporated questions asked

in the initial questionnaire in order to ascertain any changes in students’

perceptions about group work. The questionnaire also included three questions

designed to elicit students’ feelings about their group learning:

What do you feel was easier to understand or learn in your group?

What do you feel would have been easier to understand or learn on

your own?

What would you change about the group?

An individual Likert scale questionnaire was used to measure group

cohesiveness (see Appendix G, p. 267). The group cohesiveness questionnaire

focused on students’ perceptions of their group and other group members. Group

cohesiveness is important for group performance as cohesive groups perform

better than less cohesive groups (Brannick & Prince, 1997; McIntyre & Salas,

1995).

Groups were also asked to complete a metacognitive questionnaire at the

final computer session. This questionnaire was based on Fortunato et al. (1991)

metacognitive questionnaire (see Appendix B, p. 262) and focused on planning,

monitoring, and evaluating strategies. Previous studies have demonstrated the

reliability and validity of the metacognitive questionnaire (e.g., Schwartz,

Page 83: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

70

Andersen, Hong, Howard, & McGee, 2004; Sperling, Howard, Miller, & Murphy,

2002.). Students were asked to respond to 21 statements that reflect their thinking

while solving a problem. The questionnaires were used in conjunction with

participant observations, group interviews and other classroom artefacts such as

the Knowledge Forum group notes.

3.2.3.4 Knowledge Forum notes

Knowledge Forum® was used in this study in order for groups to engage

with other groups online. Scardamalia and Bereiter (2006) stated that on

Knowledge Forum® the knowledge is publicly produced by students as they

collectively build knowledge using the posted discussion site. Knowledge

Forum® supported the group’s learning by representing the group’s model and

groups could also construct ‘notes’ about their problem. Other groups could view

the database adding text, questions, and comments on each group’s work. The

Knowledge Forum notes were collected from the computer sessions following

students constructing their mathematical ranking models using Excel®.

3.2.3.5 Mathematical ranking models

The mathematical model ranking activity was developed taking into

account previous literature on mathematical modeling activities (Doerr & English,

2001; English et al., 2005; Lesh et al., 2000; Lesh & Lamon, 1992; Nason &

Woodruff, 2003) and the initial conceptual framework (see Figure 2.7). The

Page 84: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

71

framework informed the group metacognitive scaffolds of planning, monitoring,

and evaluating the group skills and roles that the groups needed to successfully

work together and develop a shared understanding. This shared understanding

was facilitated by the groups developing a shared external representation of the

mathematical problem using Excel®.

Each team was required to complete an Excel® spreadsheet ranking model

prior to posting it onto the Knowledge Forum® database. Jonassen and Carr

(2000) state that spreadsheets are a ‘mindtool’ that assists with knowledge

representation. Spreadsheets are frequently used as a tool for developing

mathematical problem solving (Abramovich, 2003). For example, teams used the

Excel® spreadsheet to rank the major Australian cities according to various

categories in order to ascertain the ‘best’ city in Australia.

3.3 Procedure

In order to achieve the research aim, the study proceeded in two stages:

Stage 1: Cycles of design experiment (Research Objectives 1 and 2)

Stage 2: Design of unified conceptual model (Research Objective 3)

3.3.1 Stage 1: Cycles of design experiment

Stage 1 involved two cycles investigating within- and between-group

metacognition in order to develop a unified conceptual model in Stage 2. Cycle 1

Page 85: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

72

involved the design of within-group metacognitive scaffolds during mathematical

problem solving for groups working around a computer (Research Objective 1).

Cycle 2 involved the design of within- and between-group metacognitive

scaffolds for groups building collective knowledge within the Knowledge

Forum® learning environment (Research Objectives 1 and 2). Knowledge

Forum® is a computer supported collaborative learning environment that was

used in this study in order for groups to engage with other groups through a

database (Stein, 1998).

Each of the two cycles consisted of three successive phases of planning,

conducting, analysing, and refinement, which enabled the outcomes of Cycle 1 to

be fed into the next cycle (see Figure 3.1).

Figure 3.1. Phases of design cycles.

The first phase of each cycle involved preparing for the research setting by

planning the learning activity. Relevant literature was reviewed and a conceptual

Page 86: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

73

framework was developed in order to inform the development of the learning

activity (see Figure 2.7). The second phase involved conducting the teaching

experiment, or learning activity, in the classroom setting. Aspects of a teaching

experiment approach were incorporated in order to study the influence of group

metacognition instruction on the co-operative groups (Cohen, Manion, &

Morrison, 2000). The learning activity was constantly under development during

the design experiment and data were collected throughout the learning activity to

inform future activities. The third and final phase of each cycle involved the

analysis of data from the teaching experiment in order to refine the problem, the

group roles, and group skills.

Cycle 1 was conducted in 2005 in two classrooms in different primary

schools. Cycle 2 was conducted in 2006 in the same schools used in the first

cycle. The school principals and class teachers volunteered their students to be

part of the research study. Ethical clearance was obtained from the educational

authorities involved, and students and their parents from both cycles of the study

signed a consent form in order to be involved in the study. Cycle 1 resulted in a

preliminary theoretical framework informing strategies for scaffolding within-

group metacognition as well as a preliminary instructional activity. Cycle 2 was

informed by the analysis of the previous cycle as well as insights gained from an

updated literature review. The design was constantly modified throughout the

design cycles.

Page 87: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

74

3.3.1.1 Cycle 1: Within-group metacognition

Cycle 1 involved the design of within-group metacognitive scaffolds

during mathematical problem solving for groups working around a computer

(Research Objective 1).

Participants

The participants selected for Cycle 1 consisted of 47 students, 27 girls and

20 boys, from two classes from different primary schools. One class was a

combined Year 4-7 class while the other class was a Year 4 class. The Year 4

teacher formed seven groups of three students and one group of four students of

mixed ability and mixed gender, while the Year 4-7 students were allowed to

form six friendship groups of three students and one group of four students. The

Year 4 teacher chose to mix students of different gender and ability in order for

students to learn to work with students other than their close group of friends.

Groups were made up of three members to allow certain group processes

to emerge, as a three person group is considered the minimum size for a group

and is a small enough number for groups to work comfortably around a single

computer (Samovar et al., 1996). The group of four students was formed in one

class due to computer availability. In the second class one group of four students

was formed to include one student new to the class.

The classroom teachers assigned the students to the groups from the same

school and the students remained in the same groups throughout the study. Each

Page 88: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

75

group worked together on their mathematical ranking model which was placed on

the Knowledge Forum® database. All groups, from both schools, could view the

database and comment on any group’s model.

Procedure for Cycle 1

This cycle proceeded in three phases: Phase 1: Planning the teaching

experiment; Phase 2: Conducting the teaching experiment; Phase 3: Analysis of

data from the teaching experiment.

Phase 1: Planning the teaching experiment

The first phase of this cycle involved preparing for the research setting by

planning the learning activity (see Figure 3.1). The learning activity was

developed from the conceptual framework developed from the literature review in

Chapter 2 (see Figure 2.7). The framework identified organisational factors,

cognitive factors, and group metacognition factors. Strategies for addressing these

factors were incorporated into this phase of Cycle 1.

In order to address the organisational factors (see Section 2.1.1), this first

cycle focused mainly on the face-to-face interaction that occurred amongst the

group. Strategies such as group skills, team roles, and checklists to monitor the

use of the skills and to provide scaffolds for feedback, were included as support

for the organisational factors identified in the theoretical framework from Chapter

2.

Page 89: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

76

The type of problem-solving task used was also identified in Chapter 2 as

being an important strategy to ensure that students used teamwork as well as task

work in order to work together (Dishon & O’Leary, 1984). An open-ended model-

eliciting problem-solving task, ‘Best City’, incorporating core learning outcomes

from the Queensland Mathematics Syllabus: Years 1-10 (Queensland Studies

Authority, 2004) was planned for the group work (see Appendix H, p. 268). The

mathematical problem-solving task, ‘Best City’, involved students working

together around the computer to rank information about the capital cities in

Australia (see Appendix I, p. 274). The design of this mathematical problem-

solving task was informed by Lesh et al. (2000) principles for model-eliciting

activities. The ‘Best City’ problem-solving task met all of Lesh et al.’s six

principles for model-eliciting activities.

Groups were to externally represent their knowledge by producing a

mathematical ranking model on Excel®. The catalyst of this problem-solving task

was a newspaper article published in the Herald Sun (see Appendix J, p. 282), in

October 2005, ranking the livability of cities throughout the world. The groups

involved in this cycle needed to come up with an overall ranking system in order

to find the ‘best city’ in Australia.

Specific skills and group roles, were identified from the literature review,

and were used for the groups working around the computer (see Appendix K, p.

283) (Cohen, 1994; Dishon & O’Leary, 1984). These group skills and roles were

included in a group diary in order for the students to choose group skills

Page 90: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

77

appropriate for their group and in order to allocate the group roles to each member

of the group (see Appendix K, p. 283).

In order to address the cognitive factors (see Section 2.1.2) of group

problem solving, the groups needed to form a shared understanding of the task

they were completing and how to work effectively together. Strategic questions

and scaffolds were identified from the literature review and from the conceptual

framework in Chapter 2 (see Figure 2.7). The scaffolds were included to

encourage students to question what they knew about the specific problem, what

they wanted to know, and what they needed to learn to solve the problem. The

strategic questions were included in order to scaffold the group processes as well

as the problem-solving process. The questions were used to develop

questionnaires and checklists to scaffold a shared understanding by each group of

how they were working together as a group to solve the problem (see Appendix L,

p.284). Incorporating the use of Excel® sheets and the Knowledge Forum®

database also enabled students to articulate their problem solving in order to build

a shared understanding about the problem-solving task.

In order to address the metacognitive factors (see Section 2.2) identified in

the literature review, group problem-solving checklists were also used in the

group diary to encourage groups to plan, monitor, and evaluate their problem

solving and their group work. The checklists were included in the group diaries in

order for the groups to plan, monitor, and evaluate their problem solving and how

they worked together (see Appendix L, p. 284).

Page 91: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

78

Phase 2: Conducting the teaching experiment

This phase of the cycle involved conducting a teaching experiment, or

learning activity, in the classroom setting (see Figure 3.1). This phase ran for six

weeks with one computer lesson per week. The computer sessions were of one

hour duration. Prior to the first computer session, the students were given the

initial individual questionnaire (see Appendix E, p.265) regarding their

perceptions on group work and what problems they had encountered previously

when working in a group. A final individual questionnaire was completed at the

end of the study (see Appendix F, p. 266). A focus group interview was also

conducted, and recorded, at each school where students were asked about the

group task and their use of Excel® and Knowledge Forum® and what they

thought worked well and what did not work well. This is similar to Nirula and

Woodruff’s (2005) design study in which students were viewed as co-

investigators in order to discover how technology and pedagogy could be used in

a specific activity.

The mathematical model building activity was introduced to the groups of

students from the same class, working around the computer (Jonassen & Kwon,

2001; Lesh & Harel, 2003; Light & Littleton, 1999; Underwood & Underwood,

1999). The model building activity required groups to work together to develop a

mathematical ranking model that could be used to solve the problem situation (see

Appendix H, p. 268).

Page 92: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

79

During the first computer session students were shown the newspaper

article (see Appendix J, p. 282) ranking the cities of the world and asked their

opinion on whether they thought that the Australian cities were ranked correctly.

Students from both classes decided that they would rank the cities differently and

it was decided that an overall ranking would be compiled from an amalgamation

of each group’s mathematical ranking model (see Appendix M, p. 288).

Cannon-Bowers and Salas (2001) and Klimoski and Mohammed (1994)

suggested that external representations facilitate the process of articulating

students’ thinking and allow group members to formulate an accurate shared

understanding of both teamwork and task work. The group skills, roles and

problem-solving strategies, derived from relevant literature were included in the

group diary (see Appendix K, p. 283), as well as the metacognitive checklists of

planning, monitoring, and evaluating.

Students were also asked to complete the initial planning checklist in the

group diary (Appendix L, p. 284). The planning was based on the listed problem-

solving skills and team skills included in the group diary (see Appendix K, p.283).

Each group was asked to answer the following questions on their planning sheet:

What do we know about the problem?

What is the goal?

What is our plan to solve the problem and reach the goal?

What group roles will we use?

Page 93: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

80

What group skills will we use?

Each group skill was introduced using a T-chart (see Appendix N, p. 290),

with one column titled ‘looks like’ and the other column titled ‘sounds like’,

where students were asked to describe behaviours that exhibited the skill. For

example, students suggested that for the group skill of ‘encouraging other

members to talk’ they could smile at the group member and look at them as they

spoke. They also suggested they could ask students who were usually quiet what

they wanted to do. Groups were asked to nominate one task skill and one

maintenance, or team skill, their group could concentrate on during the following

computer session.

Groups also assigned roles to each group member including: keyboarder,

checker, elaborator, researcher, recorder, encourager, and observer (Dishon &

O'Leary, 1984; Johnson et al., 1993). Assigning roles for groups working on the

computer ensures equal keyboard access for all members, as well as being an

effective method for students to build social skills (Cohen, 1994; Johnson &

Johnson, 1993).

In the second computer session, groups chose the categories on which they

were going to rank the major cities from sheets containing information about the

major cities (see Appendix I, p.274). Groups were also asked to monitor their use

of group skills and what they were doing well and needed to improve (see

Appendix L, p. 284). The groups reallocated group roles so each group member

Page 94: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

81

would have a turn at the keyboard and each group was asked to answer the

following questions on their monitoring sheet:

Are we following the group plan?

Do we need to make changes?

What group roles will we use?

What group skills will we use?

What are 2 things our group is doing well and 1 thing that needs to

improve?

Groups were also asked to complete monitoring sheets after the third and

fourth computer sessions where they were asked to nominate the group roles and

skills they were going to use within their groups and what group skills they were

going to use on Knowledge Forum® (see Appendix L, p. 284). Students were also

asked to complete the monitoring checklist in the group diary regarding how their

group was completing the problem-solving task and how their group was

functioning as a team.

A brief introduction to the Knowledge Forum® was provided at the

beginning of the third session in which groups were shown how to post a note and

how to reply to others’ notes. A guide to the Knowledge Forum® database was

also given to the groups (see Appendix O, p. 291). The groups became familiar

with the Knowledge Forum® database as they wrote welcome notes to each other.

Page 95: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

82

The groups were also required to place the categories they were going to rank

their cities with onto their monitoring sheet and the Knowledge Forum ® database

(see Appendix P, p. 293). The categories chosen by the groups were placed on a

CD (see Appendix Q, p. 294) and information was included from the various

websites so that students could obtain further details about their chosen categories

(see Appendix R, p.295).

The fourth computer session involved groups completing a further

monitoring sheet, as well as replying to the welcome notes posted onto

Knowledge Forum ® during the previous session. Groups also developed their

Excel® rankings, using the categories they chose in the previous session. A 10-

minute lesson was given to students on how to add columns and create formulas

in Excel®. A guide was also given to the groups to help them use the Excel®

spreadsheets (see Appendix S, p. 296).

Only one of the classes had prior experience with Excel®. However, this

experience was limited to having received a brief introduction on spreadsheets

from a parent helper. While the class had experience in reading and filling out

columns in Excel®, they had no experience with adding the columns or using

formulas. The other class had no prior experience with Excel® spreadsheets.

During the fourth computer session, following the development of their

initial Excel® ranking model, each group of students was required to post their

model to Knowledge Forum®. The groups also posted “notes” regarding their

Page 96: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

83

ranking model in order to explain their ranking and to help develop online

communication. Other groups could view the Knowledge Forum® database and

include their own “notes” adding questions and comments on each other’s

spreadsheet.

In the fifth computer session, the groups revised their ranking systems

based on comments received from other groups and from further categories

chosen. The groups also added comments to their Excel® sheet to explain how

they had ranked the cities according to the categories they had chosen (see

Appendix Q, p.294). During this fifth computer session, groups added all the

ranked categories from the Knowledge Forum® database onto their Excel®

spreadsheet. The groups then sorted the combined rankings to find the best city in

Australia. The final ranking model included the categories from all the groups’

spreadsheets (see Appendix M, p. 288). The final model was added to the Best

City CD and a comment was added by one of the classes involved in the study

(see Appendix T, p. 297).

The sixth and final computer session involved groups completing an

evaluation checklist in their group diary (see Appendix L, p. 284). Groups were

asked to comment on what worked with their group, what didn’t work and what

they might do differently next time they worked together. During the final session

groups also wrote farewell messages to the other groups on the Knowledge

Forum® database. For example, one group wrote:

Page 97: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

84

Merry x-mas and a happy new year thats (sic) for the last week of the

school (Group M)

Phase 3: Analysis of data from the teaching experiment

This third and final phase of Cycle 1 involved the analysis of data from the

teaching experiment conducted in Phase 2 (see Figure 3.1). Triangulation

occurred in this study with the use of multiple sets of data and the data were

analysed using several different coding methods. Following the principles of data

collection and analysis proposed by Yin (1994), the data collected and method of

data analysis are presented in Table 3.2. Yin’s three principles proposed were:

first, use multiple sources for the triangulation of data; second, create a database;

and third, maintain a chain of evidence. A sample of the data coding was checked

by a senior researcher (R. Nason, personal communications, 2007) until all

categories were agreed upon.

As the study focused on the group metacognition that is shared amongst

team members, the unit of analysis focused on the group. This approach is

consistent with an approach adopted by Stahl (2006) who suggested that learning

can take place at the group level as well as with the individual student. It is for

this reason that Bales’ Interactive Process Analysis (IPA) has been included in the

data analysis (Bales, 1970; Bales & Cohen, 1979; Miller, 1991). The data analysis

also included categories based on the conceptual framework from Chapter 2 (see

Page 98: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

85

Figure 2.7). The categories included group development stages and group

metacognitive strategies. The analysis also incorporated a constant comparison

method as recommended by Strauss and Corbin (1998).

Table 3.2

Cycle 1 Data Collected and Method of Analysis

Source of data Type of data collected Method of data analysis

Observations Videos Transcriptions of audio

(MP3) recordings

Interactive Process Analysis (IPA)

Identification of group development stages (derived from conceptual framework)

Identification of metacognitive strategies (derived from conceptual framework)

Classroom artefacts Group diaries Checklists Metacognitive

questionnaires Individual questionnaires

Identification of task and team skills (derived from literature review)

Constant comparison Identification of

metacognitive strategies (derived from conceptual framework)

Constant comparison

Group interview Transcription of focus

group interview

Constant comparison

Page 99: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

86

The transcriptions of the MP3 recordings of the groups working together

were analysed using Bales’ IPA system which has been used previously in group

research (Hare & Hare, 1996). Armstrong and Priola (2001) stated that the IPA

system is one of the most widely employed schemes for assessing group

interaction. Previous studies have demonstrated the reliability and validity of the

IPA method of analysis (e.g., Underwood & Underwood, 1999).

The IPA consists of a content analysis that looks at the communication

patterns of the groups involved in the study. The IPA divides group

communication into acts, or sentences, where each member’s speech can be

composed of one or more acts (Bales & Cohen, 1979). The ‘act’ of

communicating which is directed at other group members, is coded. The content

is not recorded, rather the behaviour it represents is recorded (Bales, 1970).

Coding consists of making a tally when certain group communication acts occur.

Bales’ (1970) IPA coding system identifies a set of twelve categories that

identify group communication behaviours or acts (see Appendix U, p. 298). The

behaviours are divided into two main domains: those behaviours concerned with

the group task and those focusing on the maintenance of group relationships

(Hoover, 2002). Each group was recorded during each computer session and the

recordings were transcribed. The transcriptions were coded using the IPA coding

scheme.

Transcribed interviews, group interactions, diary checklists, and group

questionnaires were also analysed according to the existing conceptual framework

Page 100: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

87

from Chapter 2. The initial coding system, using concepts from the literature, was

based on the metacognitive strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating

identified in Chapter 2. The metacognitive strategies were used as a basic

framework in which to begin to categorise the group data. The coded planning,

monitoring, and evaluating stages of group metacognition, were further

categorised using Schraw’s (2001) regulatory checklist for improving students’

thinking about their learning processes (see Appendix C, p. 263). The final coding

system (see Appendix V, p. 299) reflects the use of group metacognitive strategies

by the groups involved in the study.

The aim of using previously identified concepts from the literature was to

find any emerging patterns common to a number of groups. Miles and Huberman

(1994) suggested researchers initially use themes derived from the literature. An

advantage of using the concepts from literature is that they are already loaded

with analytical meaning. Collected data in the form of observations, individual

interviews, and diary inserts also informed further data collection during the

group interviews (Strauss & Corbin, 1998; Walker, 1985).

A constant comparison approach, where theory is derived from the data,

was also adopted in this study in order to extend the analysis from ‘coding and

counting’ to ‘exploring and understanding’ (Stahl, 2006). Coding occurred in two

main stages. The first stage involved a process of open coding in which

transcripts, interviews, individual questionnaires, and classroom artifacts were

coded in a process of ‘in vivo coding’ (Strauss & Corbin, 1990). ‘In vivo coding’

Page 101: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

88

consists of a set of procedures for analysing qualitative data in order to build a

theoretical framework (Strauss & Corbin, 1998). As the study already consisted of

an initial conceptual framework the analysis focused on discovering themes and

refining concepts identified in the existing framework.

Categories were identified from the data collected using the constant

comparison method. Constant comparison relies on the emergence of theoretical

categories from an on-going data collection and analysis (Huberman & Miles,

2002). The continual analysis of the questionnaires, and group diaries followed

the constant comparison method and included comparing:

1. Data from the groups at different points in time

2. Data within categories

3. Data across categories (Charmaz, 2000; Denzin & Lincoln, 1998; Ryan

& Bernard, 2003; Silverman, 2001; Strauss & Corbin, 1998).

Huberman and Miles (2002) stated that the key to comparison making is

looking at the data in divergent ways and to define categories before identifying

within-group and between-group similarities and differences (Strauss & Corbin,

1998). Categories were identified from the data as they emerged in order to

inform the final conceptual model (see Figure 6.4).

Data analysis was conducted in an ongoing hermeneutic cycle (Guba &

Lincoln, 1989). Collected data was entered into NVivo® software allowing data

to be organised into codes and categories. As patterns were identified, they were

Page 102: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

89

compared with existing theory and a coding scheme was created. Further

categories were added as data were collected and analysed.

All data from this cycle were analysed in order to inform the design of a

conceptual model that would scaffold within-group metacognition while building

collective knowledge with groups working around computers. Data were also

analysed in order to ascertain where modifications were to be made to the initial

conceptual framework and instructional activity to be used in Cycle 2 of the

study.

3.3.1.2 Cycle 2: Within- and between-group metacognition

Cycle 2 involved the design of within- and between-group metacognitive

scaffolds for groups building collective knowledge within the Knowledge

Forum® learning environment (Research Objectives 1 and 2).

Participants

The participants in the final cycle of this study were chosen from one Year

4 class and one combined Year 4-7 class from the same schools used in the first

cycle of this study. All students from both classes were involved in the study. The

students from the Year 4 class were not involved previously with the study while

some students in the Year 4-7 class had been involved in the first cycle of the

study. The same students were included in the second cycle in order to ascertain if

Page 103: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

90

students who had previous experience with the task and with the structured group

activity could transfer their knowledge to the second cycle.

The class teachers formed the groups with the Year 4 teacher forming

mixed-ability groups of mixed gender and the Year 4-7 teacher allowing students

to form their own groups. Gillies and Ashman (2000) found the effect of different

ability and gender composition in trained co-operative groups was minimal, so the

teachers chose the group composition based on their preferred method.

Three groups from each class were chosen for an in-depth study based on

teacher observations of the students involved. These six groups (18 students),

were selected by means of theoretical sampling, a purposive sampling technique

in which maximum diversity is sought in characteristics considered salient to the

research question (Case & Gunstone, 2002; Glaser & Strauss, 2004; Lincoln &

Guba, 1985). Theoretical sampling involves choosing groups which are likely to

extend the emerging theory on group metacognition, while maximum diversity or

variation involves selecting a wide range of participants in order to achieve the

maximum variation possible (Guba & Lincoln, 1989; Huberman & Miles, 2002).

The comparative method of data analysis also requires deviant cases to be

identified in order to account for all of the data collected in the study (Silverman,

2001).

One group from each class was chosen as they were nominated as being

able to work well in groups, while one group was chosen due to the students being

identified as not working well with other students due to their disruptive

Page 104: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

91

behaviour. A final mixed-gender group was also selected from each class in order

to achieve a wide variation of groups. The six groups formed two online teams

consisting of groups from both schools (see Table 3.3).

Table 3.3

Formation of Online Teams

School 1 School 2

Team One Group A

Group C

Group B

Team Two Group F Group D

Group E

Procedure for Cycle 2

Cycle 2 proceeded in three phases: Phase 1: Planning the teaching

experiment, which involved refining the experiment from Cycle 1; Phase 2:

Conducting the teaching experiment; Phase 3: Analysis of data from the teaching

experiment.

Phase 1: Planning the teaching experiment

Phase 1 of this second cycle involved refining the teaching experiment, or

learning activity, based on the results from Cycle 1. The problem-solving task

Page 105: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

92

involved extending the activity from Cycle 1 by having groups form online

Knowledge Forum® teams with other groups (see Table 3.3). Each group created

their own mathematical model and then combined their model with the other

groups in their team to find the best city in Australia (see Appendix W, p. 300).

This task differed from Cycle 1 in which groups made a combined ranking model

between the two classes (see Appendix M, p. 288). The CD incorporating the

categories from Cycle 1 was used in this cycle (see Appendix T, p. 297). A

number of new websites were also included on the CD for students to further

research their chosen categories in Cycle 2.

The results from Cycle 1 were used to inform the design of within- and

between-group metacognitive scaffolds for groups building collective knowledge

within the Knowledge Forum® learning environment. Specific group roles and

skills that students from Cycle 1 had highlighted as being necessary for effective

groups were placed on posters and displayed in each class (see Appendix X, p.

302). The use of social skills, such as those used in the poster, influences the

effectiveness of cooperative groups (Johnson & Johnson, 1993) and the posters

were based on Dishon and O’Leary’s (1984) task- and team-skills as well as

group roles identified in the first cycle. Conflict management skills were also

placed on a poster and displayed, due to students in Cycle 1 identifying conflicts

and disagreements as the things they liked least about working in a group.

Page 106: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

93

Phase 2: Conducting the teaching experiment

The second phase of this study focused mainly on the online interaction

that occurred amongst the groups. This phase of the study ran for six weeks with

one computer lesson per week. The computer sessions were of one hour duration.

Prior to the first computer session, individual students were asked to complete an

initial questionnaire regarding their previous experience with group work. Follow-

up questionnaires were also completed by the students at the completion of the

computer sessions.

The group diary was introduced during the first lesson to scaffold

between-group metacognition. The students were asked to use the group diaries to

plan, monitor, and evaluate the introduced group roles and skills. Ten minutes of

instruction was also given at the start of the computer sessions where the groups

were shown the posters and introduced to the specific skills needed for effective

co-operative groups (see Appendix X, p.302). All groups also completed the

group planning checklist during the first computer session (Johnson & Johnson,

1993). Students were also made aware of the group roles of Keyboarder, Checker,

and Encourager, as these were the roles that groups in Cycle 1 had frequently

chosen. The group roles were written in the group diary in order to publicly

clarify the roles (see Appendix K, p. 283).

Instructions were given at the beginning of the first two computer sessions

where groups were introduced to Excel® spreadsheets and to the Knowledge

Forum® environment and shown the CD compiled from various websites

Page 107: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

94

detailing information about each major city (see Appendix T, p. 297). In order to

find the “best city”, in Australia, students were asked to evaluate information

taken from the various websites in order to rank the major cities in Australia.

During this phase of Cycle 2, each group of students formed an online

Knowledge Forum® team with two other groups and each online team included at

least one group from each school (see Table 3.3). The three groups in each team

initially developed their own ranking models using information they gained from

the CD database and from an Internet search. Incorporating the use the

Knowledge Forum® database enabled the groups to share their models and co-

construct a shared team model (see Appendix W, p. 300).

The new online team needed to collaborate online in order to create a

combined mathematical ranking model by providing feedback (such as comments

and questions) to each other about their ranking models. They also simultaneously

engaged in the process of revising and improving their ranking models based on

feedback they received from other online teams. Groups were asked to rank two

categories each and then to work out an overall ranking for their online team

compiled from an amalgamation of each group’s categories. The groups ranked

the categories they had chosen by using the information they gained from the CD

database (see Appendix T, p. 297) and from additional websites included on the

CD. The ranked categories from each group were then combined into an Excel®

Page 108: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

95

spreadsheet for each online team. The teams then sorted the combined rankings to

find the best city in Australia (see Appendix W, p. 300).

In order to scaffold within-group metacognition, the metacognitive

scaffolds of planning, monitoring and evaluating were introduced, by the use of a

learning diary, to the groups working around the computer (see Appendix L, p.

284). Groups were required to reflect on how their group was functioning using

the planning, monitoring, and evaluating scaffolds: planning how to approach the

given learning task, monitoring progress of the task, and evaluating their progress

toward the completion of the task.

Students also completed a questionnaire after the activity on how they

thought the group had functioned; focusing on both the task and team aspects of

group work (see Appendix F, p.266). The questions were similar to the questions

used in a study by Whicker et al. (1997), in which students were interviewed on

their perception of co-operative group work.

Prior to the first computer session, students from the Year 4 class were

asked to complete the initial group work questionnaire regarding their previous

experience with group work (see Appendix E, p. 265). The Year 4-7 class had

been asked the same questions during the first cycle, a year earlier. A final

individual group-work questionnaire was completed at the end of the computer

sessions by all students involved in this second cycle of the study. The final

questionnaire encouraged students to comment on their group work during the

Page 109: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

96

study and what they thought worked well and what did not work (see Appendix F,

p. 266).

While most CSCL studies have looked at the frequency and length of

messages, they rarely indicated why the messages happened (Henri, 1991; Mason,

1991). For this reason, groups were interviewed regarding their use of the

Knowledge Forum® database. Silverman (2001) advocates using open-ended

interview questions in order to gain a more effective understanding of students’

group experiences (see Section 3.2.2).

The focus-group interviews focused on the problem solving and the team

aspects of the teaching experiment (Hoyles & Healy, 1994). During the interview,

students were also asked questions based on those used by Charmaz (2003) (see

Appendix D, p. 264), and based on answers received in the first cycle. Students

were asked to elaborate on their answers and further questions were asked

depending on the answers given during the interview (Morse, 1998). Students

were also asked to complete a group questionnaire on their metacognition during

problem solving (Appendix B, p. 262).

Phase 3: Analysis of data from the teaching experiment

The final phase of Cycle 2 focused mainly on the online interaction that

occurred between the groups. The focus of the data analysis in this phase was on

data that reflected students’ knowledge and understanding of group context. A

qualitative descriptive analysis approach was used for bringing out the details

Page 110: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

97

from the viewpoint of the group members and provided information about the

context, the participants, and the activities involved (Johnson & Christensen,

2004).

Data for this stage of the study were derived from classroom artefacts (see

Section 3.2.3), including group diaries, checklists, questionnaires, and Knowledge

Forum® notes, that showed the groups working together. A focus-group interview

was also conducted. The data collected and method of data analysis are presented

in Table 3.4.

The data analysis focused on how teams form a shared-team monitoring of

their group process. Categories coded included the task and team skills selected

by the groups in Cycle 1 (see Section 4.1.2) and categories based on the

conceptual framework from Chapter 2 (see Figure 2.7). Tindale, Kameda, and

Hinsz (2003) stated that metacognition in groups can be considered to be how

group members think about the ways they process and share knowledge in an

attempt to reach group decisions. Therefore, the Knowledge Forum® notes were

coded according to the metacognitive and group strategies taken from the

conceptual framework (See Figure 2.7).

The Knowledge Forum® notes were also coded using the Bales’ IPA

system that was used in Cycle 1 to code the MP3 audio transcripts of the groups

working together. The Knowledge Forum® notes were divided into two main

domains: those concerned with the task and those concerned with the team. These

Page 111: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

98

two domains were compared to the domains coded in Cycle 1 from the MP3

transcriptions.

Table 3.4

Cycle 2 Data Collected and Method of Analysis

Source of data Type of data collected Method of data analysis

Classroom

artefacts

Group diaries (Within-group metacognition)

Checklists and metacognitive questionnaires (Within-group metacognition)

Individual questionnaires

Knowledge Forum® notes (Between-group metacognition)

Identification of task and team skills (derived from Cycle 1)

Identification of metacognitive strategies (derived from conceptual framework)

Constant comparison

IPA Identification of group

development stages (derived from conceptual framework)

Identification of metacognitive strategies (derived from conceptual framework)

Group interview Transcription of focus group interview

Constant comparison

Page 112: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

99

Mohammed, Klimoski, and Rentsch (2000) stated that the most common

methodologies used to study shared-team mental models are Likert-scale

questions to measure the degree the team shared their mental models. A final

Likert-scale questionnaire was issued to group members where they were asked

questions based on Schraw’s (2001) metacognitive checklist (see Appendix C, p.

263). For example, ‘What is our goal?’ was one of the planning questions asked.

After groups completed the questionnaire, the answers were analysed in order to

discover the degree to which each team shared an understanding of the group

metacognitive process.

Students were also asked in their final interview if they would be willing

to work in the same groups in future activities. Tindale et al. (2003) state the team

members need to have a similar understanding of the team processes groups need

to use in order to work effectively. The shared teamwork schema leads to team

members being more willing to work together in future activities. The answers

given by students in this cycle were compared to the answers given by students in

Cycle 1.

3.3.2 Stage 2: Development of a unified conceptual model

During this stage, the findings from Cycles 1 and 2 were cumulated into a

unified conceptual model to inform the design of scaffolds for within- and

between-group metacognition within CSCL environments. The unified conceptual

model generated in this stage had its genesis in the initial conceptual framework

Page 113: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

100

presented in the summary of Chapter 2 (see Figure 2.7). This initial framework

was developed from an analysis and synthesis of the relevant research literature.

During each of the two cycles in Stage 1, various aspects of the initial framework

were evaluated. The outcomes of these cyclic evaluations were able to inform on-

going modifications of the initial framework and also inform the design of the

final unified conceptual model (see Figure 6.4).

3.4 Conclusion

This study proceeded in two stages. Stage 1 included the two cycles of the

design experiment (see Figure 3.1). Stage 2 involved cumulating the results from

both cycles into a unified conceptual model to inform the design of scaffolds for

within- and between-group metacognition within CSCL environments.

Each cycle in Stage 1 included three phases of planning the experiment,

teaching the experiment, and analysing the results, in order to study the influence

of group metacognitive scaffolds on primary school groups. The focus of the

design experiment was to develop scaffolds for group metacognition that could be

used for instructional tools and to inform theory. In each cycle, a case study was

presented and data was collected and analysed to inform the development of

scaffolds for within- and between-groups working in CSCL environments. The

problem as well as the group roles and skills were refined following the analysis

of each cycle.

Page 114: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

101

Analysis involved a content analysis, categories from the literature review

including group-development stages and metacognitive strategies, and constant

comparison. The analysis allowed for the development of theory, from the data

collected, on how to establish and maintain shared metacognitive group thinking

within- and between- cooperative groups working around and through the

computer. All data from Cycle 1 and Cycle 2 were analysed, in order to inform

the design of a conceptual model in Stage 2 of the study that would scaffold

within- and between-group metacognition while building collective knowledge in

CSCL environments (see Figure 6.4).

The following chapter presents the results from Cycle 1 of the study which

introduced group-metacognitive scaffolds for groups working around the

computer. Chapter 5 presents the results from Cycle 2 of the study which

introduced group-metacognitive scaffolds for groups working within a computer

supported collaborative (CSCL) environment. Chapter 6 combines the results

from Cycles 1 and 2 into a unified conceptual model that can be used to scaffold

within- and between-group metacognition within CSCL environments.

Page 115: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

102

Page 116: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

103

CHAPTER 4: RESULTS FROM CYCLE 1

Within-group metacognition

The aim of this study was to develop a conceptual model to inform the use

of scaffolds to facilitate group metacognition during mathematical problem

solving in CSCL environments. In order to achieve the study aim, a design

research methodology incorporating two cycles was used. Cycle 1 focused on

within-group metacognition for groups working around the computer (Research

Objective 1); Cycle 2 focused on both within- and between-group metacognition

for groups working within a computer supported collaborative learning (CSCL)

environment (Research Objectives 1 and 2).

This chapter presents the results from Cycle 1 which focused on within-

group metacognition. Data sources (see Table 3.2) included observations (see

Section 3.2.1); a focus group interview (see Section 3.2.2); and classroom

artefacts (see Section 3.2.3) including group diaries, checklists, metacognitive

questionnaires, and individual questionnaires.

Three categories of themes that mirror the three main factors for

successful problem solving and learning identified in the conceptual framework

presented in Chapter 2 were derived from the analysis of data: (1) organisational,

(2) cognitive, and (3) metacognitive.

Page 117: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

104

4.1 Organisational themes

Six organisational themes were derived from the analysis of the data:

1. The groups engaged in a higher number of task-related than team-related

behaviours.

2. The groups adopted task- and team-orientated behaviours in order to work

together efficiently.

3. Task-related disagreements increased during the course of the study for

some groups.

4. The groups went through a development process which was less linear and

more convoluted than that proposed by Tuckman and Jensen’s model

(1977).

5. Students identified five aspects important for group work: having fun,

working together, listening, helping, and sharing ideas.

6. Students identified disagreements and conflicts as aspects they liked least

about working in a group.

4.1.1 Theme 1

The groups engaged in a higher number of task-related than team-related

behaviours.

Theme 1 was derived from the analysis based on Bales’ Interactive

Process Analysis (IPA) (Bales, 1970, Bales & Cohen, 1979; Hare & Hare, 1996;

Miller, 1991) of the video and MP3 recordings of the groups working together.

The IPA method employs a set of 12 categories divided into two domains: task-

related and team-(or socio-emotional) related behaviours.

Page 118: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

105

An example of task-related interaction can be seen in this conversation

from Group A when the group members were ranking the cities according to

rainfall.

Student 1: Who has the most rain?

Student 2: Um Darwin, Melbourne

Student 1: Which one, which one has the most rain?

An example of team-related interaction can be seen in the following

conversation from Group C during the third computer session when they were

discussing how well their group was working together:

Student 1: It is fun working with all these people it's great

Student 2: We could all write something

Student 3: Yeah but it’s fun working with…because it's really nice

Student 1: We're good friends and good to gets along with

Student 3: It's quite nice

The IPA coding of the transcriptions of the recordings presented in Table

4.1 indicates that the groups had a much higher number of task-related behaviours

(77.8%) than team-related behaviours (22.2%).

Page 119: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

106

Table 4.1

IPA Domain Frequency

Domains Number of behaviours Percentage of total

1. Task-related 2567 77.8%

2. Team-related 734 22.2%

Total 3301 100%

A further analysis of the video and MP3 recordings found that a major reason

for why there were more task-related behaviours than team-related behaviours

was due to the open-ended model-eliciting nature of the problem task

administered to the students. This finding is consistent with findings from

previous research studies that indicate that the task affects the type of interaction

that groups engage in while problem solving (Cohen, 1994; Dishon & O’Leary,

1984; Jonassen & Kwon, 2001; Puntambekar, 1999). Complex, open-ended

problems such as the model-eliciting mathematical ranking activity utilised in this

study require a high level of task-related interaction.

4.1.2 Theme 2

The groups adopted task- and team-orientated behaviours in order to work

together efficiently.

Theme 2 was also derived from the analysis based on Bales’ Interactive

Process Analysis (IPA). With the IPA, the task and team domains are divided into

Page 120: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

107

twelve categories. Categories 1-3 (seems friendly, dramatises, agrees) relate to

positive team behaviours, categories 4-9 (gives suggestion, gives opinion, gives

information, asks for information, asks for opinion, and asks for suggestion) are

task-related, and categories 10-12 (disagrees, shows tension, and seems

unfriendly) relate to negative team behaviours.

The IPA categories that had high frequency counts were the task-related

categories of: gives information (29.8%), gives opinion (15%), and gives

suggestion (14.9%) and the positive team-related category of agrees (15.4%).

An example of gives information (29.8%) can be seen in this exchange by

members in Group E during the first computer session when the groups were

working on their ranking models:

Student 1: Our plan is to get information to use to find the top eight which

is the best

Student 2: And also, And also use information to find the best of them all

An example of gives opinion (15%) can be seen in this conversation by

Group F during the second computer session when group members were deciding

on how they wanted to rank the size of the city:

Student 1: I think big cities are best though

Student 2: I hate big cities

Page 121: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

108

The category gives suggestion (14.9%) was evident in the discussion from

Group P during the third computer session when they were working out how to

rank movie theatres:

Student 1: Let’s take a vote

Student 2: Let’s count

The category of agrees (15.4%) was the positive team-related behaviour

most frequently coded for this cycle. The category agrees was coded for

behaviours or acts that show agreement, passive acceptance, understanding,

concurring or compliment. An example of when group members showed this

category was when Group H was working together to rank the top cities:

Student 1: Then we'll do pollution (Gives suggestion)

Student 2: OK (Agrees)

The categories with the lowest frequency counts were the negative team-

related categories of: seems unfriendly (0.3%) and shows tension (0.5%). Seems

unfriendly was coded in the first computer session for Group L when the group

members were posting their first note to the Knowledge Forum ® database:

Student 1: I don’t like Evan (Student 2) as much I like you Larry (Student

3)

Page 122: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

109

An example of shows tension was evident from Student 1 in Group E,

during the second computer session, when the group was choosing the group

skills to focus on in the following computer session:

Student 1: Come on I’ve already told you

Student 2: So we got to pick one?

Student 1: We all have to agree on this one alright

Table 4.2 displays the IPA category frequency for this cycle of the study.

The IPA data coding was discussed with a senior researcher (R. Nason, personal

communications, 2007) and all observed behaviours were able to be categorised

under the IPA categories. The results from the IPA category count showed all

groups exhibited group behaviours that focused mainly on the task-related

behaviours of gives information (29.8%), gives opinion (15%), and gives

suggestion (14.9%) and the positive team-related category of agrees (15.4%).

This finding is consistent with previous studies that suggested that groups

working on open-ended problems need a high level of discussion to clarify the

goal and work together efficiently (Cohen, 1994; Gillies, 2000; Jonassen & Carr,

2000).

Page 123: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

110

Table 4.2

IPA Category Frequency

Categories Number of behaviours Percentage of total

Positive team behaviours

1. Seems friendly

24

0.7%

2. Dramatises 77 2.3%

3. Agrees

Task-related behaviours

508 15.4%

4. Gives suggestion 491 14.9%

5. Gives opinion 494 15%

6. Gives information 983 29.8%

7. Asks for information 280 8.5%

8. Asks for opinion 153 4.6%

9. Asks for suggestion

Negative team behaviours

166 5%

10. Disagrees 99 3%

11. Shows tension 16 0.5%

12. Seems unfriendly 10 0.3%

Total 3301 100%

4.1.3 Theme 3

Task-related disagreements increased during the course of the study for some

groups.

Theme 3 was derived from the analysis based on Bales’ Interactive

Process Analysis (IPA). IPA frequency count tables were completed for each of

the 16 groups (see Appendix Y, p. 306). The percentage of behaviours is

Page 124: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

111

presented in Appendix Y (p. 306) for each group as well as a brief description of

each group’s activities, including examples of group behaviours, in order to

highlight why certain ‘acts’ or ‘behaviours’ occurred.

Group conflict can be classified into either task or team issues (Rentsch &

Zelno, 2003). Task conflict leads to differences of opinion and is desirable for

effective teamwork while team conflict can be destructive to the team. The

majority of the disagreements in the first computer session tended to be team-

related. The following example, from the first session, shows Group B involved in

a team-related conflict regarding what member was doing which group role:

Student 1: You’re not the recorder, I am (disagrees)

Student 2: It's recording (gives information)

Student 1: You’re not allowed to record (disagrees)

Eight groups had behaviours coded for the category disagrees (4.3%).

However, in terms of knowledge building, most of the disagreements towards the

end of the study were task-based in nature and tended to be resolved quickly. The

category disagree was observed and coded during the last session, from Group E,

when the group was discussing how to rank their final categories. The following

example shows the task-related disagreement with Group E was minor and

resolved quickly:

Student1: Brisbane doesn't have a lot of hospitals.

Page 125: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

112

Student 2: It does but

Student 3: Canberra doesn't

Student 1: What about we start from number eight. What about number

eight should be….

Student 2: Darwin doesn't have that many

Student 1: This isn't how many hospitals they have

Student 2: Oh

Student 1: I was going to say cause Brisbane that's going to be

Student 2: Melbourne

Student 3: No Melbourne you've got

Student 2: Ok do all them Melbourne being number 8

Student 1: No we all have to agree on them

Student 2: That's number one

Student 3: Yep

Student 1: We have to agree on every single one

Student 2: So let's pick them together

4.1.4 Theme 4

The groups went through a development process which was less linear and more

convoluted than that proposed by Tuckman and Jensen’s model (1977).

Theme 4 was derived from an analysis of the transcripts of the videos and

MP3 recordings. The transcripts were coded using constructs derived from

Page 126: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

113

Tuckman’s model of group development (Tuckman & Jensen, 1977). Tuckman

and Jensen’s model suggests that groups progress through five linear stages of

development: forming, storming, norming, performing, and adjourning. The

analysis of data found that the groups went through a development process much

less linear and more convoluted than that proposed by Tuckman and Jensen’s

(1977) model. For example, in this study, eight of the 16 groups in the initial

computer session when they were supposedly going through the forming stage of

team development engaged in conflict.

According to Tuckman and Jensen (1977), during the forming stage,

conflict is generally avoided as members get to know one another. However, in

these eight teams, the students engaged in team-related conflict about who was

going to take up each group role during the initial session. Unlike many of the

group formation studies reported by Tuckman and Jensen, the members of each

group in this study came from the same classroom. Therefore, the conflict

observed in the first session with these eight groups may have been related more

to past events than the forming of the teams in this study.

Kormanski (1999) stated that the forming stage involves groups getting

orientated to the task and getting to know other members. The groups showed no

behaviours related to getting to know each other, as all 16 groups had prior

knowledge of their group members. However, a brief forming stage, where groups

were orientated to the task, occurred during the first computer session when

groups were involved in planning the task.

Page 127: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

114

Two basic tenets of Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) model are that all

groups go through the stage of storming and that storming types of behaviour

occur early on during group development immediately after the forming stage.

The analysis of the observation data in this study revealed that only nine of the 16

groups engaged in storming types of behaviour during the course of the study.

Furthermore, storming types of behaviour in these nine groups occurred

throughout the study rather than just after the forming stage where Tuckman and

Jensen’s (1977) model predicts it will occur. The major group task during

storming is the development of an ability to listen and seek productive resolutions

to conflict (Tuckman & Jensen). The nine groups engaged in resolving a number

of conflicts. The following example shows how Group K resolved their team

conflict during the fourth computer session, as they reached an agreement about

who was doing what role and what each group role entailed:

Student 1: Then I get the MP3 player

Student 2: No you don't. Student 2 gets the MP3 player you get to be

keyboarder. You’re keyboarder, you get to type

Student 3: And I help him

Student 2: No, he doesn't need help

Student 1: Yeah the encourager helps

Student 2: No the encourager just goes your do you need help

Student 3: Yeah I'll ask that and help

Page 128: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

115

Student 1: Yeah and I'll help too

Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) model also posits that the process of group

norming (establishing the norms of behaviour) predominantly occurs during the

third stage of group development. From the transcripts (of the audio MP3

recordings), it was evident that the norming process took place at the beginning of

each session as the groups allocated the group roles and worked out which group

skills their group needed to focus on during the following session. For example,

during the first session as Group C was allocating member roles, Student 1

defined what they needed to do when they were allocated the group role of

encourager:

Student 1: And I'm the encourager and, and I'm the encourager that keep

(sic) people happy and that

Group P also discussed what the encourager role entailed:

Student 1: You’re the encourager so you've got to say things like say “Oh

wow!”

Student 2: Encourage

Student 1: Say like “You’re doing really good, really good”

Norming negotiations occurred amongst group members about who was

doing which role. For example, during the second session the group members

Page 129: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

116

from Group E worked out who was doing the group roles of keyboarder and

recorder.

Student 1: You can be the keyboarder and I'll be the recorder

Student 2: I'm the keyboarder and I get it (the recorder role) next week

Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) model also includes a fourth development

stage, performing, which involves members working interdependently as a group.

Langan-Fox (2003) stated that the performing stage occurs in only a small

percentage of groups. However, the analysis of data in this study found that the

groups were working interdependently throughout the study incorporating

effective task- and team-oriented behaviours, and not just throughout the later

phases of the study.

Adjourning is also an important stage identified by Tuckman and Jensen

(1977). Kormanski (1999) stated that the adjourning stage involves the

disengagement of the group and the finalising of the task. All 16 groups finalised

the task by posting their final mathematical ranking model on to the Knowledge

Form® database.

Page 130: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

117

4.1.5 Theme 5

Students identified five aspects important for group work; having fun, working

together, listening, helping, and sharing ideas.

Theme 5 was derived from an analysis of the individual questionnaires.

The questionnaires were coded according to a constant comparison method of

data analysis (Charmaz, 2000; Denzin & Lincoln, 1998).

Question two on the initial questionnaire asked students what they liked

about working in a group. The responses tended to fall within four categories,

having fun (10%), sharing ideas (15%), helping (20%), and working together

(25%). Table 4.3 presents the questions asked, results from a constant comparison

method of analysis, and examples of the responses in order to highlight why

certain concepts emerged.

Question one on the final individual questionnaire also asked students

what they liked about the learning groups. The responses fell into similar

categories to the responses to the same question on the initial questionnaire. Three

categories were similar including working together (21%), fun (14%), and sharing

ideas (12%).

Page 131: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

118

Table 4.3

Responses to Question Two on Initial Individual Questionnaire

Question Responses For example

What do you like about

working in a group?

Having fun

(10%)

Other poeple (sic) can

help one enouther (sic)

and it’s rather fun

(Student 1, Group H).

Share ideas

(15%)

You can share you (sic)

ideas with other people

(Student 3, Group O).

Helping (20%) You can help each other

(Student 3, Group K).

Working

together (25%)

That you can work

together and get to know

people better (Student 1,

Group I).

Question five on the initial individual questionnaire asked students what

advice they would give to someone who has just discovered they will be working

in a group. The fun aspect of working with others was highlighted by students

(50%). This response supported the fun aspect highlighted from the third question

which asked students what they liked about working in groups (10%).

Page 132: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

119

Question seven on the final individual questionnaire also asked students

what advice they would give to someone who has just discovered they would be

working in a group (Table 4.4). The responses that were made included comments

that fell into four main categories including have fun (17%), listen (12%), work

together (12%), and share ideas (7%), as well as a variety of other comments

(26%). These findings differed from the responses to the same question on the

initial individual questionnaire.

Table 4.4

Responses to Question Seven on Final Individual Questionnaire

Question Responses For example

What advice would you give to someone who has just discovered they would be working in a group?

Share ideas (7%) I would encourage them to have a go at sharing their ideas and if that doesn’t work out have another go (Student 1, Group I).

Listen (12%)

Listen to everyone's ideas (Student 3, Group N).

Work together

(12%) Get your group to work

together (Student 1, Group M).

Have fun (17%)

That it is really fun and just ask if you need help plus that everyone is nice (Student 2, Group H).

Other comments

(26%) To relaxe (sic) because

nothing is rough with working with other people (Student 3, Group A).

Page 133: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

120

Responses to the initial questionnaire mainly focused on the fun aspect

(50%) while the responses to the final questionnaire went beyond having fun to

include knowledge building behaviours such as sharing ideas, listening, and

working together. The perceptions of students regarding this aspect of group work

is important for understanding what students consider important for effective

group work.

4.1.6 Theme 6

Students identified disagreements and conflicts as aspects they liked least about

working in a group.

Theme 6 was also derived from an analysis of the individual

questionnaires. On the questionnaires students were encouraged to comment on

their group work and what they thought worked well and what did not work well.

Question three on the initial questionnaire asked students what they did

not like about working in groups. Students commented that disagreements and

conflicts (55%) were the things they liked least about working in groups (Table

4.5). Students also included comments about dominant group members (20%) and

others not being listened to (10%).

Page 134: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

121

Table 4.5

Responses to Question Three on Initial Individual Questionnaire

Question Responses For example

What don’t you like

about working in

groups?

Not listened to (10%) Sometimes you don’t

get to have a say

(Student 2, Group

O).

Dominant group

members (20%)

When there is a

bossey (sic) peson

(sic) (Student 1,

Group P).

Disagreements and

conflicts (55%)

That sometimes you

can disagree and

things get into a

fight (Student 2,

Group I).

Question two on the final questionnaire asked students what they did not

like about working in the group. On the initial questionnaire 55% of students had

commented on disagreements and conflicts while on this questionnaire, only 29%

made comments about disagreements and conflicts. One student commented:

There was a little bit of a fight but we got over it and went on with

our job (Student 2, Group J).

Page 135: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

122

The comments on the initial questionnaires focused on the fights that

occurred with previous group work. The comments regarding conflicts decreased

in the final questionnaire and focused on the disagreements that had occurred

during the activity and how the groups had worked through them.

4.1.7 Organisational themes summary

Groups engaged in a higher number of task-related than team-related

behaviours. Task-related behaviours such as gives information, gives opinions,

and gives suggestions were the most prevalent behaviours shown throughout the

course of the study. The high number of task-related knowledge building

behaviours could be attributed to the nature of the task in which the students were

engaged. This assertion is consistent with the literature that indicates that groups

involved in solving complex open ended problems tend to engage in task-related

knowledge building interactions (Dishon & O’Leary, 1984; Jonassen & Kwon,

2001).

The groups adopted effective task- and team-orientated behaviours in

order to work effectively together. The number of positive team behaviours

increased over the study, while the negative team behaviours of seems unfriendly

and shows tension decreased from the first to the last session. These two

behaviours were disruptive and unproductive to the work of the group. Cathcart et

Page 136: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

123

al. (1996) stated that such unproductive behaviours can lead to a feeling among

group members that they are not accepted by other group members.

Task-related disagreements increased during the course of the study for

some groups. Disagreements increased from the first session. However, the

disagreements were more task-related (as opposed to team-related) in the final

sessions. This finding is consistent with reports in the literature that indicates that

task-related conflicts tend to be related with positive group outcomes (Rentsch &

Zelno, 2003).

The groups performed well but not in the linear group development

process suggested by Tuckman and Jensen (1977). The group forming stage only

occurred briefly at the beginning of the activity, as groups were involved in

planning the task. Storming and norming stages occurred throughout the activity

rather than in a linear pattern or at certain ‘stages’. The storming stage occurred

throughout the study as groups sought productive resolutions to conflicts.

Norming occurred throughout the study as the groups allocated group roles and

chose group skills to focus on at the beginning of each session. Most groups

worked interdependently during the study and developed their norms of behaviour

at the beginning of each computer session. Performing occurred throughout the

study as groups incorporated effective task- and team-oriented behaviours. The

groups were all involved in the final adjourning phase as the task was finalised

and the groups were disbanded.

Page 137: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

124

The results showed the groups organised their task- and team-orientated

behaviours in order to work efficiently together. Students identified a number of

aspects important for group work including having fun, working together,

listening, helping, and sharing ideas. The students also identified arguments and

fighting as the things they liked least about working in a group.

4.2 Cognitive themes

Two cognitive themes were derived from the analysis of the data:

7. The groups developed a shared knowledge of the task and how they

wanted to perform as a team.

8. The groups developed and focused on their own task- and team-skills.

4.2.1 Theme 7

The groups developed a shared knowledge of the task and how they wanted to

perform as a team.

Theme 7 was derived from the analysis of the group diaries. The group

diaries were used by the groups to organise their task- and team-skills and to

delegate group roles while they completed their mathematical ranking models.

The task- and team-skills adopted and utilised by the groups were initially

identified from the literature review in Chapter 2. Further team- and task-skills

then were identified using a constant comparison method of analysis.

Page 138: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

125

Task-skills included check group understanding, give ideas, share

information, talk about the work, get the group back to work, repeat what has

been said, and ask questions. Team-skills included encourage, check for

agreement, encourage other members to talk, respond to ideas, using eye contact,

say ‘thank you’, share feelings, and keep things calm. The skills chosen were used

by the groups during the following computer session.

In Session 3, Group C planned to focus on the task-skills of check group

understanding and share information; the group used these skills in the following

computer session where groups were ranking the capital cities in Australia:

Student 1: Today we’re going to be ranking, ranking the city (Check group

understanding)

Student 2: What do you think would make a good city? (Check group

understanding)

Student 3: Not leaving rubbish behind and riding (Share information)

Student 2: And what do you think would make a good city? (Check group

understanding)

Student 1: Less pollution, good hospitals (Share information)

Student 2: I think a good city would have lots of good schools and parks,

good committees and groups, good theatres umm beautiful

parks that's what I think would make a good city (Share

information)

Page 139: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

126

As shown in Table 4.6, the group task-skills of talk about the work (9),

check group understanding (8), and ask questions (8), were the task-skills most

chosen by the groups to focus on in the computer sessions.

Table 4.6

Frequency of Task-Skills Chosen

Check group

understanding

Give ideas Share

information

Talk about

the work

Ask questions

8 5 5 9 8

Table 4.7 shows the group team-skill of encourage was the team-skill

most chosen (12) from the list of group skills for the groups to focus on in the

computer sessions, followed by check for agreement (8), use eye contact (7), and

keep things calm (7).

By planning which team-skills would be focused on, group members were

able to identify skills needed to be improved. For example, in session 3, Group A

planned to focus on the team skill of encourage, the use of this skill was evident

during the following session.

Student 1: You've done a good draft (encourage)

Student 2: Does that make you satisfied? (Check for agreement)

Student 1: You've done well (Encourage)

Page 140: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

127

Table 4.7

Frequency of Team-Skills Chosen

Encourage Check for

agreement

Use eye

contact

Say ‘Thank

you’

Keep things

calm

12 8 7 4 7

There was an overlap of skills nominated by the groups as task- or team-

skills. Some groups nominated the same skill as the task- and the team-skill they

were going to focus on in the next computer session. For example, Group D chose

the skill of be positive for their task- and team-skill during the third computer

session. The overlap of task- and team-skills chosen by the groups shows that the

group members were developing a shared understanding about what their group

needed to be efficient.

4.2.2 Theme 8

The groups developed and focused on their own task- and team-skills.

Theme 8 was also derived from the analysis of the group diaries. The

analysis showed that seven groups chose skills to focus on that were not on the

list of group skills included in the diary. The seven groups nominated group skills

relevant to the specific needs of their group, combining both task- and team-skills.

The main task-skills nominated by the groups included, doing the work,

seek opinions and information, working together, and contributing ideas. The

Page 141: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

128

main team-skills nominated by the groups included: listening to each other,

communicating, be positive, and being quiet. Benjamin et al. (1997) and Cathcart

et al. (1996) stated that students should be encouraged to create their own list of

group skills appropriate for their group to be effective. The findings show that

from the second computer session, the seven groups began to nominate their own

group skills relevant to their group. Group members took a shared responsibility

for nominating skills relevant to their group, rather than just copying from the list

of group skills.

4.2.3 Cognitive themes summary

During this cycle, the diaries helped the groups to organise their

knowledge of the task- and team-skills into a shared understanding of how they

wanted to perform as a team. Group members showed a shared-group

understanding by nominating their own group skills, adopting what Scardamalia

(2002) describes as a collective cognitive responsibility. Scardamalia stated that

this is a necessary condition for knowledge building behaviour.

The development of an external representation regarding team work and

task work enabled groups to develop a shared understanding of how their group

was performing. Researchers such as Cannon-Bowers and Salas (2001) and

Klimoski and Mohammed (1994) have stated that external representations, such

as the group diary used in this research study, help groups formulate a shared

Page 142: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

129

understanding of both their team-work and their task-work as well as allowing

them to articulate their thinking. During this study, group members were able to

use the diary to organise their knowledge of the group task, roles, and skills into a

shared team model of how their group needed to perform.

It was noted that the groups incorporated the nominated group skills into

their co-operative group behaviour. The task- and team-skills nominated by the

groups, from the group skills listed in the group diaries, were used in the

following computer sessions. This is consistent with Gourgey’s study (2001)

which found that students who monitor their own learning process, through the

use of diaries or learning journals, become more proficient group learners.

Benjamin et al. (1997) and Cathcart et al. (1996) stated that students

should also be encouraged to create their own list of group skills. Groups in this

study also nominated group skills to use that were not listed in the group diary.

The group diary enhanced the co-construction of a shared understanding about

what skills the groups needed to be effective and helped the groups develop a

consensus of skills relevant to their group.

4.3 Metacognitive themes

Two metacognitive themes were derived from the analysis of the data:

9. The groups reflected on the strategies specific to the problem-solving task.

10. The groups used metacognitive scaffolds to plan, monitor, and evaluate

their task- and team- work.

Page 143: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

130

4.2.1 Theme 9

The groups reflected on the strategies specific to the problem-solving task.

Theme 9 was derived from the analysis of the answers from the group

metacognitive questionnaire that the groups were asked to complete (see

Appendix B, p. 262). The metacognitive questionnaire was completed at the end

of the study to scaffold group reflection on the three main metacognitive

strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating (Fortunato et al., 1991). The

questionnaire asked the groups to respond to 21 statements which described

metacognitive behaviour relating to the task that the groups had engaged in during

their problem solving. The 21 statements were categorised under four main

questions. The responses to the four questions are presented in this section as well

as separate examples of responses from the 21 statements (see Table 4.8). The

examples highlight the specific strategies that groups adopted for the problem-

solving task.

Page 144: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

131

Table 4.8

Metacognitive Questionnaire

Yes No Maybe BEFORE YOUR GROUP BEGAN TO SOLVE THE PROBLEM WHAT DID YOUR GROUP DO?

1 We read the problem more than once. 7 1 8

2 We understood what the problem was asking us 7 2 7

3 We tried to put the problem into our own words 11 2 3

4 We tried to remember if we had worked a problem like this before 2 12 2

5 We thought about what information we needed to solve this

problem 6 1 9

6 We asked ourselves, is there information in this problem that we

don’t need 2 4 3

AS YOUR GROUP WORKED ON THE PROBLEM WHAT DID YOUR GROUP DO?

7 We thought about the steps as we worked on the problem 9 0 7

8 We kept looking back at the problem after we did a step 6 8 2

9 We had to stop and rethink a step we had already done 5 7 4

10 We checked our work step by step as we worked the problem 7 4 5

11 We did something wrong and had to redo our step(s) 10 4 1

AFTER YOUR GROUP FINISHED WORKING THE PROBLEM WHAT DID YOUR GROUP

DO?

12 We looked back to see if we did the correct procedures 6 1 8

13 We checked to see if our calculations were correct 6 3 7

14 We went back and checked our work again 6 8 2

15 We looked back at the problem to see if our answer made sense 6 4 6

16 We thought about a different way to solve the problem 7 4 4

DID YOUR GROUP USE ANY OF THESE WAYS OF WORKING?

17 We drew a picture to help us understand the problem 1 15 0

18 We guessed and checked 2 9 5

19 We picked out the operations we needed to do this problem 4 4 7

20 We felt confused and could not decide what to do 4 6 6

21 We wrote down important information 6 3 5

Page 145: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

132

The first question (Before your group began to solve the problem what did

your group do?) contained six statements regarding what the group had done prior

to solving the problem (Table 4.8). Eleven groups (69%) indicated that in the

early stages of the problem solving, they had tried to put the problem into their

own words. The response to Statement 3 relates to the findings from the group

diary checklists where groups had to state what the problem was and how they

were going to solve it. All groups had used the diary to restate the problem.

Dominowski (1998) stated that restating and explaining the problem promotes

metacognitive processing. The groups used the group diaries to explain the

problem in their own words and to describe the categories they had considered.

The response to the metacognitive statement confirmed that groups had restated

the problem in their own words and showed that groups were reflecting on the

processes they had used when solving the task.

The second question (As your group worked on the problem what did your

group do?) related to how the groups had monitored their problem solving. Ten

groups (66%) stated that they had to redo their step(s) if they did something

wrong (Statement 11). Fortunato et al. (1991) stated that metacognition is the

awareness and reflection of cognitive activities engaged in during problem

solving. This metacognitive activity showed students’ awareness of their task

performance as they monitored their problem solving and had to redo steps

(Schraw, 2001).

Page 146: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

133

The third question (After your group finished working on the problem

what did your group do?) contained five statements regarding how the groups had

evaluated their problem solving. Seven groups (47%) indicated that they thought

of a different way to solve the problem (Statement 16). This was evident in the

focus group interview where students discussed how they could vote on the

different cities.

The final question (Did your group use any of these ways of working?)

was designed to prompt students to consider the strategies they had used to solve

the problem. Six groups (43%) stated that they did write down important

information (Statement 21). This was evident in the groups’ use of the group

diaries that helped scaffold the team- and the task-work of the group. All 16

groups used the diaries to plan, monitor, and evaluate strategies for both the

problem-solving task and their team-work.

The group metacognitive questionnaires, completed by the groups at the

end of the study, helped the groups reflect on their problem solving. These

reflections indicated that the groups had adopted strategies specific to the

problem-solving task as they tried to put the problem into their own words, redo

their step(s) if they did something wrong, wrote down important information, and

thought of a different way to solve the problem.

Page 147: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

134

4.3.2 Theme 10

The groups used metacognitive scaffolds to plan, monitor, and evaluate their task-

and team- work.

Theme 10 was derived from an analysis based on the three metacognitive

strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating. These strategies were used as a

basic framework with which to begin the metacognitive analysis of the audio

transcripts and the metacognitive checklists. The transcriptions and metacognitive

checklists were also coded according to Schraw’s (2001) regulatory checklist for

improving students’ thinking about their learning processes (see Appendix C, p.

263). Schraw’s checklist included questions that focused on the planning,

monitoring, and evaluating strategies.

The coding of the transcripts found that the groups were involved in

planning, monitoring, and evaluating discussions as they completed the group

metacognitive checklists in their group diaries. There were 24 planning, 23

monitoring, and 23 evaluating episodes coded from the 16 groups. For example,

planning from Group L occurred when the group was working out their plan to

find the best city in Australia:

Student1: Our goal is to rank cities

Student 2: Yes we've got ranking cities

Student1: Ok what's our plan?

Student 2: What's our plan?

Page 148: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

135

Student 3: Rank the top cities in Australia

Student 1: Ask questions

Student 2: What else?

Student 3: Encourage

Student 1: Rank the 8 cities

Students also monitored and evaluated their progress with the task and

their team work. An example of monitoring occurred with Group M occurred

during the second session when they were completing the monitoring sheet in

their group diary:

Student 1: Are we following the group plan? Yes. Do we need to make

changes?

Student 2: I don't think we need to make changes

An example of evaluation occurred from Group D when the group was

completing the evaluation sheet in their group diary. During the final computer

session Group D discussed what their group did well and what they could

improve:

Student 1: What worked? What do you think what worked or you know

still do we have to do?

Page 149: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

136

Student 2: What didn’t work?

Student 3: No nothing we just had fun

Student 1: What do we do?

Student 2: Finish work quicker. Well we could all finish work quicker

couldn’t we?

The literature, reviewed in Chapter 2, suggested that metacognition is a

linear process where planning is the first group metacognitive process and

evaluating is the last. The coding of the transcripts, using Schraw’s (2001)

checklist, shows that the group metacognitive process was not necessarily linear;

the groups were involved in a constant iterative process of planning, monitoring,

and evaluating within each of the sessions throughout the study (see Table 4.9 &

Table 4.10).

The groups combined the strategies of planning, monitoring, and

evaluating within each computer session in order to facilitate their group work.

Table 4.9 shows Group E’s metacognitive processes during the first computer

session and Table 4.10 shows Group C’s metacognitive processes during the final

computer session.

Page 150: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

137

Table 4.9

Group Metacognition Coding for Group C

Coded Checklist Text

Monitoring 1. Do I have a clear understanding of what I am doing?

Student 1: You're the encourager

which means that you have this and

I'm the coordinator

Student 2: Which also means I

encourage people

Planning

1. What is the nature of the task? 3. What kind of information and strategies will I need?

Student 1: We're just going to go and

try Knowledge Forum

Student 2: We could put how we're

going to reply to someone

Student 3: That's great

Planning

1. What is the nature of the task?

Student 1: We can write about that

Evaluation

1. Have I reached my goal? 2. What worked?

Student 1: What's 1 Melbourne, Sydney Student 2: Darwin I like Darwin Student 3: I like Brisbane Student 2: Yeah Student 1: We had fun today

Page 151: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

138

Table 4.10

Group Metacognition Coding for Group E

Coded Checklist Text

Planning

1. What is the nature of the task?

Student 1: Right, OK so we haven't started filling that in yet which we're about to do

Evaluating 3. Am I reaching my goals?

Student 1: We got a fair bit done on our one actually

Planning

2. What is my goal? 3. What kind of information and strategies will I need?

Student 1: And we have for the goal finding the top cities in Australia Student 2: The top do we have to find the top eight and the best of the top eight? Student 1: We have a plan We have a plan Student 2: Our plan is to get information to use to find the top eight which is the best Student 1: And also And also use information to find the best of them all

Monitoring

1. Do I have a clear understanding of what I am doing? 2. Does the task make sense?

Student1: We all have to agree on this one alright Student 1: This is the task what are we going to do?

Planning

3. What kind of information and strategies will I need?

Student 1: Get group back to work Student 2: Ask questions Student 1: Share information Student 2: Yeah share information

Evaluating 2. What worked?

Student 1: We are doing that, that's what we are doing well

Page 152: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

139

The group metacognitive checklists were completed by the groups at the

beginning of each computer session. The group checklist included questions

based on Schraw’s (2001) regulatory checklist (see Appendix C, p.263).

The initial planning checklist included the following questions:

What do we know about the problem?

What is the goal?

What is our plan to solve the problem and reach the goal?

The analysis of data revealed that these three questions successfully

scaffolded the groups’ planning of their model-building activity in this study. For

example, Group N indicated that they needed to rate all the cities in Australia

while all the other groups indicated that they knew they had to rank the cities in

Australia. When stating the goal, Group B responded that their goal was to learn

and have fun, while the other groups’ responses indicated the goal was to either

find the top city or the best city in Australia.

When stating what their plan was to solve the problem and reach the goal,

the groups provided a variety of responses. Group C stated that they would draw

a chart, Group E stated they would get information, use to find top eight and

which is best, while Group M stated they would use computer. The remaining

thirteen groups listed the categories they would use to rank the cities (see Table

4.11).

Page 153: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

140

Table 4.11

Group Plan to Solve the Problem and Reach the Goal

A Clean environment, nice parks, good education

B We like to go skateboarding and chilling out. Laser skirmish (sic)

C Draw a chart part

D Theme parks, sports, public health system, infrastructure (sic).

E Get information, use to find top 8, which is best

F Health, neighbours, pets, education, community,

shopping, beauty, weather, parks, government, food, water.

G Shopping, movies, theme parks, food court, park, sports

H The population, environment, wild life, food, theme parks, buildings,

education

I food, movies

J Adventure, parks, shops.

K Sport, trees, shop, rivers, school, buildings, food, culture, money, weather

L sports

M Use computer

N Theme parks, transport, safety, lifestyle, shopping, real estate, education,

population, hospitals

O Wild life, buildings, food and drink, and population

P Sport Art Showers gardens, trees, water, food.

The monitoring checklist included the following questions that focused on

monitoring and evaluating:

Are we following the group plan?

Do we need to make changes?

Page 154: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

141

What are two things our group is doing well and 1 thing that needs

to improve?

The analysis of data indicated that these three questions successfully

scaffolded monitoring actions by the groups as they were engaged in the process

of co-constructing their ranking models. All groups responded that they were

following the group plan and did not need to make any changes to the plan.

When asked to nominate two things the group was doing well 30

responses were given by the 16 groups, including 15 task-related responses and

15 team-related responses. Five groups included the task-related behaviour of

working together; the remaining 11 groups included a variety of task behaviours.

For example:

Share information

Figuring

Follow ideas

Efert (sic)

Four groups included the team-related behaviour of listening to each other; the

remaining 12 groups included a variety of team behaviours. For example:

Cooperating

Encouraging

Being patient

Keeping things calm

Page 155: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

142

When the 16 groups were asked what things their group needed to

improve, one group responded there was nothing they needed to improve; one

group stated that they needed to “reech (sic) to the top”. Three groups responded

with three task- related behaviours and 11 groups responded with a variety of

team-related behaviours. The task-related behaviours included:

Talk about the work

Check group understanding

Contribute ideas

The 11 team-related responses included:

Communicating

Be positive

Be nice

Include everyone

The final evaluation checklist asked groups to comment on the following topics:

Have we reached our goal?

What worked?

What didn’t work?

What could we do differently next time?

The analysis of data revealed that these four questions scaffolded the

groups’ evaluation of their task- and team-work during the final computer session.

All groups responded that they had reached the group goal nominated in their

Page 156: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

143

planning checklist. A variety of positive responses were given to the question

what worked including: eight groups responding with comments such as

everything or most things worked, five groups responding with task-related

comments such as the CD or Knowledge Forum, and three groups responding

with team-related comments such as cooperating or communicating.

When responding to the question what didn’t work in their view, the 11

groups stated nothing or left the question blank. Five groups included a variety of

comments including:

Because people were better at something (sic) so others didn't do

anything

Listening

One group member kept on hitting keys

Finally, when the 16 groups responded to the question what could we do

differently next time, ten groups responded with either nothing or left the question

blank, five groups responded with comments relating specifically to their own

group such as try to be nice about their decisions, listen, and not talk that much.

One group responded with a comment on the organisation of the groups: choose

different groups and mix girls and boys.

4.3.3 Metacognitive themes summary

The metacognitive checklists and questionnaires used in this study

incorporated the metacognitive strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating.

Page 157: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

144

The groups applied the metacognitive strategies to constantly improve on how

their group was completing the problem-solving task and working together as a

team. The metacognitive strategies were adopted by the groups in order to work

effectively together.

The group metacognitive questionnaires, completed by the groups at the

end of the study, helped the groups reflect on their problem solving. These

reflections indicated that the groups had used the three main metacognitive

strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating during their co-construction of

the mathematical ranking model.

The analysis of the audio transcripts showed that the groups were involved

in discussing their team- and task-work, as they completed the metacognitive

checklists in their group diaries. The results also showed that the metacognitive

process was not always linear as groups were involved in planning, monitoring,

and evaluating throughout the study.

4.4 Focus-group interview

A focus-group interview involving ten students was conducted in the

concluding phase of the study. The interview focused on eliciting the students’

thoughts on the problem-solving task used in the study. Students were encouraged

to give their opinions on what they thought worked well and what did not work

well.

Page 158: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

145

When students were asked if there was anything they would change about

the task, the following discussion took place:

Student 1: I don’t think it was good how we took from every group cause

we thought Melbourne should have won but Brisbane won. It’s

probably because everyone lives in Brisbane made Brisbane

best city.

Student 2: By movie theatres we ranked by most movie theatres

Student 1: I don’t like it how they make Brisbane -there’s lots of others

Student 3: Better cities

Student 2: Good cities

Student 4: Cities bigger than Brisbane

Student 3: The reason I like Adelaide it’s peaceful. Darwin is good.

Perth…

Student 2: Same here

Student 1: I don’t really agree with that

Student 3: And Melbourne I think should have won because it’s got lots of

entertainment and sport.

This discussion concerning the task showed that the students involved in

the focus-group interview were not satisfied with the solution to the city ranking

problem. The students discussed the task amongst themselves and concluded that

other cities could also be ranked number one depending on the categories used to

Page 159: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

146

rank the city. Lesh and Lamon (1992) stated that the construction of alternative

mathematical models is acceptable as the goal for students and students should be

involved in judging the usefulness of their model. The discussion from the

interview showed that students were judging their model and suggesting

alternatives.

During the focus-group interview, students were also asked if there was

anything they would change about the group diary. The students indicated that,

apart from a few minor changes such as having pictures included in the diary,

they enjoyed using them. One student commented:

Well, I don’t think we would change anything cause they’re pretty

good the way they are

Students indicated that they had fun and enjoyed the group task. When

asked if there was anything they learnt during the activity that they could take to

another group the following comment was made:

I will take the different strategies of working together

4.5 Summary and conclusion

This chapter focused on the first cycle of this study involving within-

group metacognitive scaffolds during mathematical problem solving for groups

working around a computer. The analysis of data in this cycle identified three

categories of themes that mirrored the three main components of successful

Page 160: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

147

problem solving and learning that were presented in the conceptual framework in

Chapter 2: (1) organisational factors, (2) cognitive factors, and (3) metacognitive

factors.

Results from Cycle 1 of this study have highlighted that effective group

problem solving requires groups to think about organisational, cognitive, and

metacognitive factors. These factors influence how groups work together and

develop a shared understanding. This shared understanding is facilitated by

groups forming an external representation of how they want their group to

perform as a team and solve the problem-solving task.

The study highlighted that while organisational and cognitive factors are

important to help groups develop and form a shared understanding, metacognitive

factors are also important for groups to reflect on and improve their problem

solving and their group work.

4.6 Implications for Cycle 2

A design research methodology was adopted for this study as it allows for

multiple cycles of design, experiment, and analysis (Shavelson et al., 2003). The

design research methodology seeks to find solutions from the analysis of data and

identifies new goals for each successive iteration (Bereiter, 2002). The cyclical

nature of the design research methodology allowed for changes to be made to the

second cycle based on the results from the first cycle of the study (Collins et al.,

2004; Woodruff & Nirula, 2005).

Page 161: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

148

The findings from Cycle 1 showed that group members need to be aware

of successful group roles and skills, including conflict management skills. They

need to choose skills and roles relevant to their own group in order to build a

shared understanding of how their group is performing. The groups also need to

apply metacognitive strategies to constantly improve how their group is

completing the task and working together as a team.

A number of aspects from this cycle helped inform Cycle 2 of the study

(see Chapter 5). First, specific group roles and skills that students from this cycle

highlighted as being necessary for effective groups were placed on posters and

displayed for the second cycle of the study. Conflict management strategies were

also displayed on a poster in the second cycle as students identified arguments

and fighting as the aspect that they liked least about working in a group.

Second, the group diaries were included in Cycle 2 as all students

indicated that they had enjoyed using them. The diaries were used to organise the

group roles and skills in order to form a shared understanding of both the team

work and the task work.

Third, the metacognitive checklists successfully scaffolded groups

planning, monitoring, and evaluating of their task- and team-work in the first

cycle. The scaffolds were included in the group diaries for the second cycle for

groups to reflect on how their group was performing, using the planning,

monitoring, and evaluating strategies.

Page 162: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

149

Fourth, a group cohesiveness questionnaire (see Appendix G, p. 267) was

included in Cycle 2 in order to ascertain if the group cohesiveness observed in the

first cycle was also perceived by the students in Cycle 2. The questionnaire

focused on students’ perceptions of their group and group members.

Finally, the group metacognition study was extended in Cycle 2 to include

between-group metacognition for groups working in online teams. In order to

extend the emerging theory on within- and between-group metacognition, six

groups of students in the second cycle were chosen to form two online teams. The

mathematical ranking model-eliciting activity was also changed for the online

teams as some students indicated in the focus group interview that they were not

satisfied with the combined solution to the city ranking problem. Each online

team in Cycle 2 was asked to collaboratively create a combined mathematical

ranking model in order to find the best city in Australia.

Page 163: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

150

Page 164: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

151

CHAPTER 5: RESULTS FROM CYCLE 2

Within- and between-group metacognition

The aim of this study was to develop a conceptual model to inform the use

of scaffolds to facilitate group metacognition during mathematical problem

solving in CSCL environments. This chapter presents the results of Cycle 2 which

focused on both within- and between-group metacognition for groups working

around the computer and working with other groups in the Knowledge Forum®

CSCL environment (Research Objectives 1 and 2). Three groups from each of the

two classes involved in the study formed two online teams. Each online team

included at least one group from each class (see Table 3.3). Data sources (see

Table 3.4) included classroom artefacts (see Section 3.2.3); including the group

diaries, checklists, metacognitive questionnaires, and individual questionnaires;

and a focus group interview (see Section 3.2.2).

The three categories of themes (organisational, cognitive, and

metacognitive) derived from the analysis of data from the first cycle of this study

were replicated in the second cycle. The results of the analysis of the data from

this second cycle also highlighted five themes not revealed during the first cycle.

There were four new organisational themes relating to the online environment and

one new cognitive theme relating to the group cohesiveness questionnaire used in

this cycle.

Page 165: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

152

5.1 Organisational themes

Four new organisational themes, relating specifically to the online

environment, were derived from the analysis of the data in this second cycle:

1. The groups posted a higher number of task-related than team-related

messages.

2. There were a higher proportion of team-related messages demonstrated

with the online teams than with the face-to-face groups in the first cycle.

3. The groups posted effective task- and team-related messages in order to

work efficiently together.

4. The online teams went through a three stage development process of

forming, performing, and adjourning rather than the five stages proposed

by Tuckman’s model.

Two of the organisational themes derived from the analysis of the data in

the first cycle of this study were replicated in this second cycle:

5. Students identified five aspects important for group work: having fun,

working together, listening, helping, and sharing ideas.

6. Students identified disagreements and conflicts as aspects they liked least

about working in a group.

Page 166: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

153

5.1.1 Theme 1

The groups posted a higher number of task-related than team-related messages.

Theme 1 was derived from the analysis based on Bales’ Interactive

Process Analysis (IPA) (Bales, 1970; Bales & Cohen, 1979; Miller, 1991) of the

Knowledge Forum® notes. The Knowledge Forum® notes were coded according

to the task- and team-domains from Bale’s IPA method. There were 12 team-

related notes, 16 task-related notes and 15 notes that contained messages relating

to both the team and the task.

The majority of team-related notes were posted as greetings when groups

formed their online Knowledge Forum® teams. For example, Group E posted the

following team-related note:

Hi where (sic) Student 1 and Student 2 (team)

The online groups sent the majority of the task-related notes whilst in the

process of co-constructing the ranking system with the other two groups in their

team. For example, one of Group B’s notes to their online team was:

We think in the best city the Government should NOT let old ladies water

their gardens whenever they want!!! We also want a large range of sports

an AFL stadium etc etc etc...

Also lots of different cultures like Aboriginal, Chinese etc etc etc...

Page 167: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

154

The Knowledge Forum® notes that related to both the team and the task

were mainly sent while the teams were engaged in compiling their categories for

their team ranking model. The following note was sent from Group A:

Cant (sic) wait to work with you [team]

We will do two categories each [task]

The 43 Knowledge Forum® notes were further coded into 60 messages.

There could be more than one message for each note. For example, Group A sent

the following note that contained two messages, one that was task-related and one

that was team-related.

We can’t wait to work wit u 2 (sic) (team)

U (sic) have good categories, but maybe we need more of them (task)

As can be seen in Table 5.1, the total domain frequency shows that the

groups had a slightly higher number of task-related messages (57%) than team-

related messages (43%).

Table 5.1

IPA Domain Frequency

Domains Number of KF notes Percentage of total 1. Task-related 34 57%

2. Team-related 26 43%

Total 60 100%

Page 168: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

155

The coding of the Knowledge Forum® notes showed all groups together

posted a higher number of task-related than team-related messages to the

Knowledge Forum® database. As in the first cycle, the high number of task-

related knowledge building behaviours could be attributed to the nature of the task

in which the students were engaged (Dishon & O’Leary, 1984; Jonassen & Kwon,

2001).

5.1.2 Theme 2

There were a higher proportion of team-related messages demonstrated with the

online teams than with the face-to-face groups in the first cycle.

Theme 2 was derived from the analysis of the Knowledge Forum® notes

based on Bales’ IPA compared to the IPA analysis of the transcripts of the face-

to-face groups working together from Cycle 1. Table 5.2 shows the task- and

team-related domain count from both cycles.

Table 5.2 IPA Domain Frequency for Both Cycles

Domains Face-to-face (Cycle 1)

Knowledge Forum (Cycle 2)

1. Task-related 77.8% 57%

2. Team-related 22.2% 43%

Total 100% 100%

Page 169: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

156

The results from this study showed that the groups working face-to-face in

the first cycle engaged in a higher percentage of task-related behaviours, or acts,

(77.8%) than the online teams in the second cycle (57%). This is counter to

Jonassen and Kwon (2001) who stated that students working with computer

conferencing are more task-directed compared to students working face-to-face.

The higher percentage of team-related (43%) messages in Cycle 2 than the

team-related (22.2%) behaviours observed with the face-to-face groups in Cycle 1

were mainly due to groups getting to know each other. Palloff and Pratt (2005)

stated that when virtual teams are formed in work environments, it is important to

spend time to get to know one another before group work is attempted. This

aspect of team forming was reflected in the Knowledge Forum® notes related to

the team. As groups formed their online Knowledge Forum® team, each group

posted their welcome notes. For example, Group B posted the following note to

their online team:

Were (sic) Group B! What grade are you in? We are in year 5!

By the way were (sic) Student 1, Student 2, Student 3! What are your

hobbies?? Ours are AFL & other sport!

Page 170: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

157

5.1.3 Theme 3

The groups posted effective task- and team-related messages in order to work

efficiently together.

Theme 3 was also derived from the analysis based on Bales’ Interactive

Process Analysis (IPA). With the IPA method, the task and team domains are

divided into twelve categories. Table 5.3 shows that the IPA categories with the

higher frequency counts are the team-related category of seems friendly (40%)

and the task-related category of gives information (35%). The groups had no

messages in the negative team-related categories of disagrees, seems unfriendly,

and shows tension.

An example of gives information from Cycle 2 of this study can be seen in

this note from Group C when they where posting their ranking model for their

online team.

Melbourne is our best city because it had great adventures.

An example of seems friendly can be seen in the welcome note posted by

Group C to their online team:

Hi were (sic) Group C. We will be happy to be working with you on

Monday.

Page 171: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

158

Table 5.3

IPA Category Frequency

Categories Number of messages Percentage of total

1. Seems friendly 24 40%

2. Dramatises 2 3%

3. Agrees 0 0%

4. Gives suggestion 7 12%

5. Gives opinion 3 5%

6. Gives information 21 35%

7. Asks for information 2 3%

8. Asks for opinion 1 2%

9. Asks for suggestion 0 0%

10. Disagrees 0 0%

11. Shows tension 0 0%

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0%

Total 60 100%

The results from the category count showed that group messages focused

mainly on either the team-related behaviour of seems friendly (40%) or the task-

related behaviour of gives information (35%). The results from the category count

in Cycle 1 also showed that group behaviours focused mainly on the task-related

Page 172: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

159

behaviour of gives information (29.8%) as group members clarified or confirmed

what the task involved.

5.1.4 Theme 4

The online teams went through a three stage development process of forming,

performing, and adjourning rather than the five stages proposed by Tuckman’s

model.

Theme 4 was derived from an analysis of the Knowledge Forum® notes

that were coded using constructs derived from Tuckman’s model of group

development (Tuckman & Jensen, 1977). Tuckman and Jensen suggested that

groups proceed through five linear stages of development, forming, storming,

norming, performing, and adjourning.

The analysis of data from Cycle 1 found that the groups went through a

development process in a much less linear and more convoluted than that

proposed by Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) model. The analysis of the Knowledge

Forum® notes from this cycle also found that while the online teams posted

effective task- and team-related messages, they did not progress through all of the

five stages of team development.

The online teams engaged in the forming stage as each group sent notes

introducing themselves and giving information about the task. This team

formation phase has been identified by Tuckman and Jensen (1977) as an

important step for effective development. As groups formed their online

Page 173: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

160

Knowledge Forum® team, each group posted their welcome notes and gave

information about the categories they were going to use. For example, Group C

posted the following note to their online team:

Hi were (sic) Group C. We will be happy to be working with you on

Monday. For our 2 categories are Movies & Adventure.

According to Tuckman and Jensen (1977), during the forming stage,

conflict is generally avoided as members get to know one another. There was no

conflict observed from the Knowledge Forum® notes. This differs from Cycle 1

where eight of the sixteen groups in the initial computer session engaged in

conflict.

The analysis of the Knowledge Forum® notes also suggests that the online

teams did not engage in the storming phase of group development suggested by

Tuckman and Jensen (1977). By contrast, nine of the sixteen face-to-face groups

engaged in “storming” types of behaviour throughout the first cycle. Palloff and

Pratt (2005) stated that conflict is critical to the development of the group.

However, the finding in this cycle of the study is consistent with that of Johnson,

Suriya, Won Yoon, Berrett and La Fleur (2002) who found that the rapid

movement between the stages with an online group resulted in no evidence of the

storming stage.

The third stage of group development, norming, predominantly follows

the storming stage. The groups did not post notes with norming messages.

Page 174: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

161

However, the groups posted friendly messages to their online teams. The groups

completed their group checklists regarding the team and task skills they wanted to

focus on during the following computer session. The groups also used these skills

on the Knowledge Forum® database. For example, Group B stated they would be

positive and sent the following note to their online team:

Hi!!!

This is Student 1, Student 2, & Student 3! We are Group B! We are

looking forward to helping you guys!

Tuckman and Jensen’s (1977) model also includes a fourth development

stage, performing, which involves members working interdependently. The

groups working face-to-face incorporated effective team- and task-related

behaviours. This was also evident with the online teams. For example, Group A

received this note incorporating team-and task-related messages.

Yo we totally cant (sic) wait to talk to u more

Do you need help with the websites

While some groups may go through general stages, it does not mean that

all groups do (Benjamin et al., 1997). There were three stages common to both

cycles of the study; the beginning (forming stage), which refers to the bringing

together of the group members to the groups and the groups to the online teams;

the performance (performing stage), which refers to the management of both the

Page 175: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

162

task- and team-skills in order to perform successfully; and the ending (adjourning

stage), which refers to how the task- and team-work was finalised.

5.1.5 Theme 5

Students identified six aspects important for group work: working with friends,

having fun, working together, listening, helping, and sharing ideas.

Theme 5 was derived from an analysis of the individual questionnaires.

The initial questionnaires focused on eliciting individual student’s thoughts on

group work prior to commencing the study. The final questionnaires repeated

questions asked in the initial questionnaire in order to ascertain any changes in

students’ perceptions about group work.

The questionnaires were coded according to a constant comparison

method of data analysis (Charmaz, 2000; Denzin & Lincoln, 1998). Responses

from the initial and final questionnaire about group work were compared. The

responses from this cycle were also compared with responses from students in the

first cycle of this study.

The third question on the initial questionnaire asked students what they

liked about working in a group or a team. Table 5.4 shows the main category that

emerged was friends (43%). The categories identified in Cycle 1 of the study

included working together (25%), helping (20%), sharing ideas (15%) and having

fun (10%).

Page 176: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

163

Table 5.4

Responses to Question Three on Initial Individual Questionnaire

Question Responses For example

What do you like

about working in a

group?

Friends (43%) I like to be with my

friends and working

togethe (sic) (Student 1,

Group A).

Other comments (57%) We can help other people

when there (sic) confused

(Student 3, Group A).

I like evry (sic) ones (sic)

ideas and finding out the

best one out of evry (sic)

one (Student 1, Group F).

The first question on the final questionnaire asked students what they liked

about the learning groups. Fun was the major focus of the responses from students

(50%) from the second cycle who commented that they had fun learning in their

groups. Fun (14%) was also focused on by students on the final questionnaire in

the first cycle. The importance of being able to help and share ideas was identified

in both cycles.

The third question on the final questionnaire asked students what they felt

was easier to understand or learn in their group. All students (100%) indicated

Page 177: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

164

that everything was easier to understand in the group. One student responded that

discussing the work was easier within the group. Students in the first cycle had

made a variety of comments including everything was easier to understand. They

also stated that “working together as a group was easier to learn within their

group”.

The final question asked students to give advice to someone who has

discovered that they will be working in a group. Have fun (29%) was the advice

given by students in this second cycle of the study. Responses from students in

Cycle 1 included have fun (17%), listen (12%), work together (12%), and share

ideas (7%). One student (7%) in Cycle 2 left this question blank. Some students

(64%) included advice on working together. Their comments included:

To work hard and enjoy (Student 2, Group D).

Listen to other people and be kind (Student 1, Group D).

They will do well (Student 3, Group C).

5.1.6 Theme 6

Students identified disagreements and conflicts as aspects they liked least about

working in a group.

Theme 6 was also derived from an analysis of the individual

questionnaires. The questionnaires related to students previous experiences with

group work and what they had learnt about group work during the study.

Page 178: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

165

The fourth question on the initial questionnaire asked students what they

did not like about working in groups. As can be seen from Table 5.5 students

(57%) commented on the group members not agreeing. The students in Cycle 1

(55%) had indicated that disagreements and conflicts were the things they liked

least about working in a group. Students from Cycle 2 (43%) also made other

general comments regarding what they did not like about working in groups.

Table 5.5

Responses to Question Four on Initial Individual Questionnaire

Question Responses For example

What don’t you like

about working in a

group?

Not agreeing (57%) Sometimes we don’t always

agree with each other

(Student 3, Group A).

Sometomes (sic) it’s a

chaleng (sic) to agry (sic)

(Student 1, Group F).

Other comments (43%) Some people take to (sic)

long! (Student 1, Group F).

When people don’t let you

help (Student 1, Group A).

The second question on the final questionnaire asked students to indicate

what they did not like about team work. Students (64%) indicated that there was

nothing they did not like about the team work. This differs to the responses for

Page 179: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

166

this question from the first cycle where some students (29%) made comments

about disagreements and conflicts. In the second cycle, two students (14%) also

left the question blank and three students (21%) commented negatively, with only

one student commenting that sometimes the group did not agree:

Getting upset with the person thats (sic) not doing what were told

to (Student 2, Group E).

The students indicated in Cycle 1 that they disliked disagreements and

conflicts that occurred within groups. Students in Cycle 2 also indicated on the

initial questionnaire that they disliked not agreeing when they work in groups.

This response was only given by one student in the second cycle on the final

questionnaire.

5.1.7 Organisational themes summary

The findings from this study showed that the Knowledge Forum®

database functioned as a shared knowledge base and the mathematical ranking

models provided a basis for discussion amongst the groups. The analysis of the

Knowledge Forum® notes showed the groups engaged in a higher number of

task-related than team-related behaviours. The task-related messages of gives

information and the team-related messages of seems friendly were the most

prevalent messages posted.

Page 180: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

167

As in the first cycle, the high number of task-related knowledge building

behaviours could be attributed to the open-ended mathematical ranking activity in

which the students were engaged (Dishon & O’Leary, 1984; Jonassen & Kwon,

2001). The online teams went through a three stage development process of

orientation to the task and to the group, management of both task- and team-skills,

and finalisation of the task and the team work. The advice that students gave in

both cycles, on how to work in a group, included have fun, listen, work together,

and share ideas.

5.2 Cognitive themes

Three cognitive themes were derived from the analysis of the data. Two of

the themes in this second cycle were also identified in the analysis of the data in

the first cycle of this study:

7. The groups developed a shared knowledge of the task and how they

wanted to perform as a team.

8. The groups developed their own task- and team-skills to focus on.

The third theme relates specifically to the group cohesiveness

questionnaires completed at the end of Cycle 2:

9. Group members held positive attitudes towards their groups and perceived

that the groups worked well together.

Page 181: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

168

5.2.1 Theme 7

The groups developed a shared knowledge of the task and how they wanted to

perform as a team.

Theme 7 was derived from the analysis of the group diaries. The groups

were asked to use the group diaries to organise their task- and team-skills and to

delegate group roles. The group roles and the task- and team-skills adopted and

utilised by the groups were initially identified from the group roles used in Cycle

1. Further team- and task-skills then were identified using a constant comparison

method of analysis.

The group roles were chosen by the groups from the team role poster (see

Appendix X, p. 302). Team roles included encourager (encourage others to talk

and encourage others to listen); manager (be positive, manage conflict, and share

positive feelings); checker (check for agreement, manage conflict, and keep things

calm). Task roles included keyboarder (give ideas, repeat ideas, and respond to

ideas); coordinator (seek ideas and search for information); and recorder (check

for understanding, ask questions, and talk about the work).

Only the three groups from the Year 4 class included the group roles in

their group diary. As shown in Table 5.6, the main group roles chosen by these

three groups from the group roles poster were the task roles of

keyboarder/computer (100%) and recorder/MP3 (67%). One group chose the

team role of encourager. The three groups from the Year 4 class also chose the

task role of writer (100%), a role not included in the group diary or team poster.

Page 182: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

169

Table 5.6

Frequency of Group Roles Chosen

Writer Recorder/MP3 Keyboarder/computer Encourager

3 2 3 1

The groups from the Year 4-7 did not include the group roles in the group

diary but they still changed task roles each week, so one student was at the

keyboard and one student was recorder. All the groups from the Year 4-7 class

had participated in the first cycle of this study and had clearly developed a shared

understanding of the task roles without the need for the role scaffolds included in

the diary. Because the Year 4-7 students had, in Cycle 1, co-constructed a shared

understanding of what the group roles entailed, the scaffolding provided by the

group diaries had become redundant, a finding that is consistent with the corpus

of knowledge about cognitive scaffolding (e.g., Gourgey, 2001; Hartman, 2001;

Vygotsky, 1978).

Groups were asked to choose team- and task-skills from the poster of

group skills derived from Cycle 1 (see Appendix X, p. 302). Task-skills included

share ideas (give ideas, repeat ideas, and respond to ideas); share information

(seek ideas and search for new information); and check understanding (ask

questions and talk about the work). Team-skills included encourage (encourage

others to talk and encourage others to listen); be positive (say thank you, use eye

Page 183: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

170

contact, and say positive things); and check for agreement (manage conflict and

keep things calm).

The groups chose sixteen team-skills and nine task-skills. As shown in

Table 5.7, the most common task-skills chosen by the groups from the task-skills

poster were work together, three groups (50%) chose this skill to focus on, and

two groups (33%) chose share ideas and share information. One group included

draw a picture or make a list from the problem-solving poster.

Table 5.7

Frequency of Task Skills Chosen

Share ideas Share information

Draw a picture or make a list

Work together

2 2 1 3

As shown in Table 5.8, four groups (67%) chose the team-skill of

encourage, 5 groups (83%) chose be positive, and all six groups (100%) chose

check for agreement, as the team-skills they would focus on in the following

computer sessions.

Table 5.8

Frequency of Team Skills Chosen

Encourage Be positive Check for agreement

4 5 6

Page 184: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

171

The analysis of the diaries showed that the group diaries helped the groups

formulate a shared understanding of both their team-work and their task-work.

The diaries provided a space for groups to represent their shared understanding

and reflect on how they wanted their group to perform. This result is consistent

with Blakey and Spence (1990) who stated that diaries help students to monitor

and reflect upon their learning performance.

While the results from the second cycle are similar to the results of the

analysis of the group diaries from the first cycle, there were some important

differences. There were more task-roles than team-roles chosen and the groups

from the Year 4-7 allocated their group roles without the use of the scaffolds

included in the diaries. The Year 4-7 groups had developed an understanding of

what the group roles entailed and mainly used the task roles of recorder and

keyboarder. The finding that the groups also decided to use group skills not listed

in the group diary replicates what was found in Cycle 1. This finding shows that

groups were incorporating skills relevant to the needs of their group.

5.2.2 Theme 8

The groups developed and focused on their own task- and team-skills.

Theme 8 was also derived from an analysis of the group diaries. The

analysis was based on the task- and team-skills identified in Cycle 1. These skills

were included on the group skills poster used in Cycle 2.

Page 185: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

172

The results showed that groups included skills not identified in the posters,

including the task-skill of complete task and the team-skills of work together and

try our best. Even though work together was listed on the group skills poster as a

task skill three groups (50%) included work together as a team-skill. This

highlighted the importance the groups placed on this skill.

A number of researchers (e.g., Benjamin et al., 1997; Cathcart et al., 1996)

have stated that for groups to be effective, students should be encouraged to

incorporate appropriate group skills. The results show that groups were

nominating skills relevant to the needs of their group. This finding was also

evident in the first cycle of this study where seven groups nominated skills not

included in the list of group skills in the group diary.

5.2.3 Theme 9

Group members held positive attitudes towards their groups and perceived that

the groups worked well together

Theme 9 was derived from the analysis of the answers from the group

cohesiveness questionnaires. Eleven students were also asked to complete the

questionnaires during the final computer session. The questionnaire was based on

Gillies (2003) study on group cohesiveness. The questionnaire was added to this

cycle of the study as there was an observed solidarity built amongst the group

members in the first cycle. In order to establish if students also perceived if they

Page 186: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

173

were cohesive, the group members were asked to complete the questionnaire in

this second cycle. This questionnaire focused on students’ perceptions of their

group and other group members.

Group cohesiveness occurs when group members begin to identify with

the group as they solve the problem (Gillies, 2003; Wheelan, 2005). As shown in

Table 5.9 the majority of students indicated that they were glad they belonged to

the group and they thought that the group worked well together. All but one

student thought the group was important. However, all students agreed or strongly

agreed that their group had worked well together.

Two positive results were identified from the questionnaires regarding the

students’ thoughts about group work. First, group members held positive attitudes

towards their groups. Second, group members perceived that the groups worked

well together. This group cohesion was developed by groups in both cycles of the

study as the group members decided how they would work together and

incorporated group behaviours necessary for their group to perform productively.

The results from the group cohesiveness questionnaire showed that group

members felt comfortable with their groups, were glad they belonged to their

group, and had developed an identity as being part of a group. This finding is

consistent with Gillies (2003) who found that cohesive groups develop when

group members begin to identify with the group as they solve the problem. All

group members indicated that they thought their group had worked well together.

This positive attitude that group members had towards each other and their group

Page 187: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

174

was important for their group effectiveness and group cohesiveness (cf. Dickson

& McIntyre, 1997).

Table 5.9

Responses to Group Cohesiveness Questionnaire

Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree

Strongly Disagree

a. I'm glad I belong to this

group 6 4 1

b. I feel held back by this

group 2 6 3

c. I am an important part of

this group 4 5 2

d. I don't fit in with other

kids in this group 1 6 4

e. I feel strongly tied to this

group 3 5 3

f. I don't think the group is

that important 1 5 5

g. I think this group worked

well together 9 2

h. I don't feel comfortable

with the other kids in this

group 1 4 6

Page 188: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

175

5.2.4 Cognitive themes summary

The group diary (see Appendix L, p. 284) scaffolded the construction of a

shared understanding in both classrooms about what relevant group skills, group

roles, conflict management skills, and problem-solving strategies needed to be

incorporated. The group diary enabled groups to form a shared understanding of

both their team-work and their task-work. The groups used the diaries to organise

the group roles, skills, and strategies they felt were important to their group

performing effectively.

The results from the group cohesiveness questionnaire showed that the

group members held a positive attitude towards their group work and the other

members of their group. Cohesive teams were developed as groups formed a

shared understanding of what team and task skills they needed to incorporate into

their group. All groups in this cycle also formed cohesive online teams with two

other groups using the Knowledge Forum® database.

5.3 Metacognitive themes

Two metacognitive themes were derived from the analysis of the data.

These two themes were also identified in the analysis of the data in the first cycle

of this study.

10. The groups reflected on the strategies specific to the problem-solving task.

11. The groups used metacognitive scaffolds to plan, monitor, and evaluate

their task- and team- work.

Page 189: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

176

5.3.1 Theme 10

The groups reflected on the strategies specific to the problem-solving task.

Theme 10 was derived from the analysis of the answers from the group

metacognitive questionnaire that the groups were asked to complete at the end of

the study (see Appendix B, p. 262). The questionnaire asked students to respond

to 21 statements which described metacognitive behaviour relating to the task that

the groups had engaged in during problem solving. The responses to this

questionnaire are shown in Table 5.10.

The responses to the metacognitive questionnaire highlight the specific

strategies that groups adopted for the problem-solving task. This first question

(Before your group began to solve the problem what did your group do?)

contained six statements regarding what the groups had done prior to solving the

problem (see Table 5.10). Four of the six groups indicated that they had read the

problem more than once (Statement 1), while two groups indicated that they

understood what the problem was asking them (Statement 2). When responding to

Statement 4: We tried to remember if we had worked a problem like this before,

only one group indicated that they had tried to remember if they had worked on a

problem like this before. This response was despite the fact that students from the

Year 4-7 class had worked on the same problem the previous year.

Page 190: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

177

Table 5.10

Metacognitive Questionnaire

Yes No Maybe

BEFORE YOUR GROUP BEGAN TO SOLVE THE PROBLEM WHAT DID YOUR GROUP DO? 1 We read the problem more than once. 4 1 1

2 We understood what the problem was asking us 2 0 4

3 We tried to put the problem into our own words 1 1 4

4 We tried to remember if we had worked a problem like this before 1 2 3

5 We thought about what information we needed to solve this

problem 3 0 3

6 We asked ourselves, is there information in this problem that we

don’t need 0 5 1

AS YOUR GROUP WORKED ON THE PROBLEM WHAT DID YOUR GROUP DO? 7 We thought about the steps as we worked on the problem 2 0 4

8 We kept looking back at the problem after we did a step 2 3 1

9 We had to stop and rethink a step we had already done 2 2 2

10 We checked our work step by step as we worked the problem 1 0 5

11 We did something wrong and had to redo our step(s) 4 2 0

AFTER YOUR GROUP FINISHED WORKING ON THE PROBLEM WHAT DID YOUR GROUP DO? 12 We looked back to see if we did the correct procedures 2 0 4

13 We checked to see if our calculations were correct 1 1 4

14 We went back and checked our work again 6 0 0

15 We looked back at the problem to see if our answer made sense 3 2 1

16 We thought about a different way to solve the problem 3 1 2

DID YOUR GROUP USE ANY OF THESE WAYS OF WORKING?

17 We drew a picture to help us understand the problem 0 5 1

18 We guessed and checked 0 0 6

19 We picked out the operations we needed to do this problem 1 2 3

20 We felt confused and could not decide what to do 0 2 4

21 We wrote down important information 6 0 0

Page 191: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

178

Groups were also asked to respond to the following question: As your

group worked on the problem what did your group do? Four of the six groups

(67%) stated that they redid their step(s) if they did something wrong (Statement

11). This is consistent with findings from the first cycle of this study. Four groups

in Cycle 1 also indicated that they had to redo their steps.

Groups were also asked to respond to the question: After your group

finished working on the problem, what did your group do? All six groups

indicated that they had gone back and checked their work again after they had

finished working on the problem (Statement 14).

Table 5.10 also shows groups’ responses to the question regarding the

question: Did your group use any of these ways of working? All groups stated that

they wrote down important information (Statement 21). Six groups in Cycle 1

also stated they wrote down important information. This was evident in the

metacognitive checklist where groups restated the problem in their own words

and listed the categories they wanted to use to rank the cities.

The group metacognitive questionnaires, completed by the groups at the

end of the study, helped the groups reflect on their problem solving. The groups

used the three main metacognitive strategies of planning, monitoring, and

evaluating during the co-construction of their mathematical ranking model. The

reflections also indicated that on average groups adopted strategies specific to the

problem-solving task as they read the problem more than once, had to redo their

Page 192: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

179

step(s) if they did something wrong, went back and checked the work, and wrote

down important information.

5.3.2 Theme 11

The groups used metacognitive scaffolds to plan, monitor, and evaluate their task-

and team- work.

Theme 11 was derived from an analysis of the Knowledge Forum® notes

and the metacognitive checklists. The metacognitive checklists were coded

according to Schraw’s (2001) regulatory checklist (see Appendix C, p. 263). The

43 Knowledge Forum® notes were placed in the initial categories of

metacognitive or not-metacognitive.

There were 25 notes coded as not-metacognitive and 18 notes coded as

metacognitive. The 18 metacognitive notes were further separated into the

categories of planning, monitoring, and evaluating. There were six planning notes,

six monitoring notes and six evaluation notes.

The six planning notes included messages regarding what groups were

going to do. Each group chose two categories to rank and then added the

categories together to form a ranking system for the team. For example, the initial

Knowledge Forum ® notes from team one included messages planning what

categories they were going to rank.

Group A: Hi! we are Group A. Cant (sic) wait to work with you. We will

do 2 categories each.

Page 193: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

180

Group C: Hi were (sic) Group C. We will be happy to be working with you

on Monday. For our 2 categories are Movies & Adventure.

The six monitoring notes included messages from groups making sure that

their team was using enough categories to rank the cities. For example, Group C

sent a note to their online team that included a message regarding the categories

they were looking at:

U have good categories, but maybe we need more of them

The monitoring notes also included messages ensuring that all the

categories the team needed for their ranking system were incorporated. For

example, Group B sent a note to their online team asking them to look at water

restrictions in order to get the city they had chosen to number one on their ranking

system:

Yo!!

Hey can u do like water restrictions & all that type of stuff to make Sydney

no 1?? Thanx!!

The six evaluation notes all included messages regarding what the groups

had done on the task. Each group incorporated the categories from their online

team and their final Knowledge Forum® notes included their final Excel®

ranking sheet. The groups used Excel® formulas to work out which was the best

Page 194: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

181

city. The final evaluation note from Group B included the formula they had used

to work out that Brisbane was the best city in Australia:

We gave extra points for Culture (*9) and Art (*5). Sydney and Melbourne

are also top cities in Australia. Melbourne has great adventures.

(=B2+C2*9+D2+E2+F2+G2+H2+I2*5) (Formula used in Excel® to

rank the cities).

The teams engaged in a constant process of planning, monitoring, and

evaluating during the online co-construction of their teams’ mathematical ranking

models. The coding of Knowledge Forum® notes showed that the metacognitive

strategies were adopted by the online teams in order to solve the task together.

The groups were involved in discussing the planning, monitoring, and evaluating

of their team and task work as they completed the metacognitive checklists in

their group diaries.

In a procedure similar to that used in Cycle 1, the group metacognitive

checklists were completed by the groups at the beginning of each computer

session. The metacognitive scaffolds of planning, monitoring, and evaluating

were included in the checklists and groups were asked to use the scaffolds with

the introduced group roles and skills.

Page 195: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

182

All groups completed the group planning checklist during the first

computer session (cf. Johnson & Johnson, 1993). The initial planning checklist

included the following questions:

What do we know about the problem?

What is our plan to solve the problem and reach the goal?

The analysis of data showed that these questions successfully scaffolded

the groups’ planning of their mathematical model-building activity. When asked

to state what they knew about the problem, two groups indicated information

about countries or information about cities. While other groups indicated either

that they were looking for the best city in Australia or had to decide the best city

first. When asked what their plan was to solve the problem and reach the goal,

four groups included group roles as part of their plan. Group skills were also

mentioned by the three of the groups including:

Don't argue! Be positive! Share ideas (Group B).

Say thank you (Group C).

Well (sic) first share ideas and find the best idea to use and then

figure it out (Group E).

Groups were also asked what their group thought would make a good city.

Two of the groups focused on one city and why they thought that city would

make the best city. The responses included:

Page 196: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

183

Brisbane is good because it has the best theame (sic) parks, the

tempriture (sic) is excellent and It (sic) is a clean place and great

movies! (Group C).

We like Sydney because of the Sydney Harbour Bridge (Group F).

Other groups focused on what they considered to be important for any

city. These responses included:

A city with enough people to support a wealthy economy but small

enough to have a rich culture in sport and food and culture (Group

D).

Good and fun, Theme parks, Good amount of rain, Atractev (sic)

(Group A).

It has to have a nice environment lots of yummy and healthey (sic)

food that isn't really expensive and a city that has peace and nice

people (Group E).

Groups were given the CD (see Appendix T, p. 297) compiled from

various websites, detailing information about each major city. The CD

incorporated the categories from Cycle 1 and students were asked to nominate

other categories they would like to rank the cities. The categories of Sport,

Movies, and Adventure were chosen by three groups. Two groups chose Culture,

Food, and Art. Other categories chosen by the groups were Heath system, Rain

Page 197: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

184

fall, and Theme parks. The additional categories were placed on the CD along

with a number of websites so students could research their chosen categories (see

Appendix T, p. 297).

The groups also completed the monitoring checklists regarding how their

group was completing the problem-solving task and functioning as a team. The

checklists helped groups develop a shared understanding of what they were doing

well and what team-skills they still needed to incorporate in order to perform as a

team. The monitoring checklists included the following questions:

Are we following the team plan?

What are we doing well and what do we need to improve?

The analysis of data indicated that these questions successfully scaffolded

monitoring actions by the groups as they were engaged in the process of co-

constructing their ranking models. All groups indicated that they were following

the group plan and did not need to make any changes. When asked what they

were doing well, comments from groups included:

Working together, being positive, completing tasks (Group B).

Saying thank you (Group C).

We are sharing information Group F).

All groups completed the final evaluation checklist during the final

computer session (cf. Johnson & Johnson, 1993). The evaluation checklist asked

groups to comment on the following:

Page 198: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

185

Have we reached the team goal?

How did we go?

What did our team do well and what do we still need to improve?

The analysis of data indicated that these three questions successfully

scaffolded the groups’ evaluation during the final computer session. All groups

responded that they had reached the group goal. A variety of responses was given

to the question how did we go including:

We think that encoareging (sic) worked and we went well (Group

A).

We did good at working together! (Group B).

Good! (Group C).

We will work as a team hard and we had fun (Group D).

Are(sic) teamwork worked (Group E).

Very good (Group F).

When responding to the question about what their team did well, three

groups stated that they did everything well, while other comments included:

We listend (sic) to other peoples (sic) comments (Group C).

We tryied (sic) to get all of our work done (Group E).

Page 199: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

186

Finally when responding to the question what they still needed to do to

improve, three groups indicated that they did not need to improve while the

remaining three groups’ comments included:

We need to improve on working as a team (Group D).

We need to work together more (Group C).

We need to improve understanding (Group E).

The analysis of data showed that the checklists scaffolded the

metacognitive strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating. King (1991)

found that when students were taught specific guided questioning strategies

designed to scaffold students during problem solving, it assisted them to become

aware of their own problem-solving and metacognitive skills. The planning

checklist involved groups restating the problem and showing how they were

going to go about solving it. The monitoring checklist involved groups developing

a shared understanding of what they were doing well and what team-skills they

still needed to incorporate in order to perform effectively as a team. The

evaluation checklist involved groups reflecting on the team goal, what task- and

team-skills worked well or did not work, and what still could be improved.

5.3.3 Metacognitive themes summary

The study highlighted that the metacognitive strategies of planning,

monitoring, and evaluating are essential for groups building a shared knowledge.

Page 200: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

187

Providing groups with group metacognitive checklists resulted in groups

planning, monitoring, and evaluating the task and team aspects of their group

work. The metacognitive strategies were also adopted by the online teams. The

teams engaged in a constant process of planning, monitoring, and evaluating

during the online co-construction of their teams’ mathematical ranking models.

The metacognitive strategies allowed students to build a shared understanding of

how their group was completing the problem-solving task and working together

as a team.

5.4 Focus group interview

This section focuses on the students’ perceptions with regards to the group

diaries and the posters. Group interviews were conducted in order to obtain

students’ perceptions of the scaffolds and skills included in the diaries and posters

(see Appendix D, p. 264).

The students stated that the group diaries were effective and did not need

to be changed. Students also indicated that they used the following team posters:

Team skills, Problem solving; and Conflict management (see Appendix X, p.

302). The students stated that the team skills of being positive and sharing

information were the two most important team skills included on the posters. This

is consistent with the findings from this study where giving information and seems

Page 201: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

188

friendly were the highest categories coded from the Knowledge Forum® notes

posted.

5.5 Summary and conclusion

The scaffolds used in this study affected how group members in this cycle

interacted and developed, including the problem-solving task; the posters of group

skills, roles, conflict management skills, and problem-solving strategies; the use

of group diaries to build a shared understanding of the requirements of the task

and the team, and incorporating metacognitive checklists to scaffold the planning,

monitoring, and evaluating of the groups shared understanding. The Knowledge

Forum® database and Excel® mathematical ranking models also helped groups

develop a shared understanding of the task they were collaboratively completing.

The following chapter discusses the results from Cycles 1 and 2 and

combines the findings from both cycles into a unified conceptual model that can

be used to scaffold within- and between-group metacognition within CSCL

environments. Chapter 7 concludes the study by overviewing the findings and

declaring the limitations of the study. Recommendations are made for future

research and implications for practical application of the group metacognition

model are also discussed.

Page 202: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

189

CHAPTER 6: DEVELOPMENT OF UNIFIED CONEPTUAL MODEL

The aim of this study was to develop a conceptual model to inform the use

of scaffolds to facilitate within- and between-group metacognition during

mathematical problem solving in computer supported collaborative learning

(CSCL) environments. To address the aim, the study proceeded in two stages.

Stage 1 focused on Research Objectives 1 and 2. It consisted of two cycles of

design experiments (Cycles 1 and 2). The results from these two cycles were

presented and discussed in Chapter 4 and 5. This chapter reports on Stage 2 of the

study. Stage 2 focuses on Research Objective 3. It thus discusses and cumulates

the results from Stage 1 to develop a unified conceptual model (see Figure 6.4)

that can be used to inform the use of scaffolds for within- and between-group

metacognition in CSCL environments.

This chapter begins with an overview of results from Stage 1 (see Section

6.1). The findings from Cycles 1 and 2 are further analysed and synthesised in

order to identify the elements and form the structure of the unified conceptual

model (see Section 6.2). The application of the unified conceptual model is then

discussed in Section 6.3. A summary and conclusion is presented in Section 6.4.

6.1 Overview of results

The results from both cycles in Stage 1 of the research study showed that

the problem-solving context played a crucial role in determining how the students

Page 203: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

190

worked together both within-groups and between-groups. Many of the

organisational factors identified in the initial conceptual framework (see Figure

2.7) also were found to help groups to develop into successful problem-solving

teams. The findings clearly indicate that having appropriate problem-solving

activities, and incorporating categories of organisational factors such as problem-

solving strategies, group roles, group skills, and conflict management skills in

CSCL environments, to help students during group problem solving, are

necessary but not sufficient conditions for successfully scaffolding engagement in

knowledge building within- and between-groups in CSCL environments.

Groups also need to engage in cognitive activity and develop shared

external representations of both team knowledge and task knowledge. It is only

then that they begin to internalise their shared understanding of the problem-

solving task and how to work together as a successful team. Furthermore, the

findings from Stage 1 also indicate that in order for groups to develop and

advance shared understandings of the problem-solving task and how to work

together as a successful team, they also need to metacognitively reflect overtly on

the organisational and cognitive strategies while completing the problem-solving

task. Each of these four elements will now be discussed in detail.

6.1.1 Problem-solving task

Groups from both cycles of the study were engaged in the ‘Best city’

model-eliciting problem-solving task. In this task, the groups were required to

Page 204: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

191

construct a mathematical model to rank the cities in order to find the best city in

Australia. This task was quite different from the types of mathematical tasks that

the students normally did in their classrooms. In addition to creating, sharing,

critiquing, and improving mathematical models, this task was conducted over a

period of six one hour lessons rather than a period of just ten to fifteen minutes.

The groups in Cycle 1 developed their ranking models within their group

and then the models were combined in a class model. The students from Cycle 1

indicated that they had found the model-eliciting activity interesting but they were

not completely satisfied with the combined model developed. Many of the

students expressed doubts about the validity of the combined class model and

some of the assumptions underlying this model. They felt that other assumptions

should have been adopted and that other cities should have been ranked first.

Thus, changes occurred in the students’ conceptions about the nature and

discourse of mathematics; they began to realise that mathematics was not

objective in nature but that it was a discipline in which they could engage in

discourse about the validity of the assumptions underlying, and the solutions of,

mathematical problems.

In Cycle 2, the groups initially developed their ranking model and then

worked in an online team with two other groups to form a combined team model.

Thus, the teams in Cycle 2 were provided more freedom than the groups in Cycle

1 to manipulate many assumptions during the development of their ranking model

for identifying the top city. The online teams were able to share, discuss, and

Page 205: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

192

negotiate many aspects of their ranking model such as the underlying

assumptions, what data to include or exclude from their model, what criteria

should be used to rank the cities, and what weighting should be attributed to each

criteria used in their model (see Appendix W, p. 300).

6.1.2 Organisational factors

The organisational factors that were identified in the initial conceptual

framework influenced how the groups in this study developed into successful

problem-solving teams (see Figure 2.7). While the majority of the groups did not

progress neatly through the development stages highlighted by Tuckman and

Jensen (1977), the results from this study showed that the groups performed well

and incorporated effective team- and task-behaviours. Tuckman and Jensen’s

(1977) framework was useful in identifying the different development stages that

the groups were involved in during this study. However, the findings from the

study showed that the groups and the online teams went through a three stage

development process of forming, performing, and adjourning, rather than the five

stages proposed by Tuckman and Jensen’s model (Tuckman & Jensen. 1977).

By scaffolding the norming and storming stages of group development, all

groups involved achieved the performing stage and were working

interdependently as a team incorporating both task- and team-oriented behaviours.

This performing stage of group development usually only occurs in only a small

percentage of groups (Langan-Fox, 2003; Tuckman & Jensen, 1977).

Page 206: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

193

The group roles, group skills, problem-solving strategies, and conflict

management skills scaffolded in this study helped the groups adopt group norms

that focused on the task as well as their team performance. Each group defined

their own group norms as they were involved in choosing group roles and skills to

use each computer session and were continually evaluating the use of the group

skills throughout the study. The expectations for the group were made explicit to

all group members as the chosen problem-solving strategies, group skills and

roles, and conflict management skills were written in the group diaries. All groups

showed high levels of cooperation as they discussed how they were progressing as

a group and used the group diaries to self regulate their own group processes.

During Cycle 1, group roles and skills were placed in the group diary and

the groups chose which skills they wanted to concentrate on in the following

computer session. The group skills, problem-solving strategies, and group roles

used in Cycle 1, were placed on classroom posters for Cycle 2. Conflict-

management strategies were also placed on a poster during Cycle 2 as the students

in Cycle 1 had identified arguments and fighting as the aspect that they liked least

about working together.

While Tuckman and Jensen (1977) suggest that conflict is an important

stage of group development, only six groups from the sixteen groups (37.5%) in

Cycle 1 were engaged in storming behaviours. During the initial stages of the first

cycle, the disagreements were more team- than task-related. Task-related conflict

Page 207: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

194

occurred throughout the cycle. However, by the end of the first cycle, group

disagreements tended to be more task-related than team-related and were resolved

quickly by the group members. By engaging in task-related rather than team-

based conflict, groups were able to engage in knowledge building. This is

consistent with previous research studies that showed that students can gain a

shared understanding of the task by engaging in task-related conflict (Crook,

1996; Rentsch & Zelno, 2003).

While Poole and Zhang (2005) have stated that virtual teams are also

likely to experience conflict, the groups involved in the online teams in the second

cycle of the study tended to be involved in negotiation over task issues rather than

becoming involved in conflict. Negotiation is seen by Puntambekar (2006) as the

process by which groups arrive at group decisions. The groups in Cycle 2 were

also involved in more positive negotiation about group roles and skills as group

members worked out what roles and skills would be focused on in each computer

session.

The main difference between the two cycles was that, during the second

cycle groups were overtly made aware of conflict management strategies, via

classroom posters. Group members were also made aware that disagreements are

part of developing into a productive team and that there were strategies that could

be used to work through conflicts (see Appendix X, p. 302).

Langan-Fox (2003) and Tuckman and Jensen (1977) suggested that the

performing stage of group development occurs in only a small percentage of

Page 208: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

195

groups. The results from both cycles in this study show that all groups involved

achieved the performing stage and were working interdependently as a team

incorporating both task- and team-oriented behaviours. The groups adopted the

group norms and focused on the task as well as their group performance. The

problem-solving skills, group roles, group skills, and conflict management skills

scaffolded in this study helped create interdependence among group members as

the groups involved encouraged and helped each other to reach the task goal.

6.1.2.1 Summary

The findings from this study show that successful groups are achievable

with primary school students when task- and team-related knowledge and skills

are made explicit and groups are able to plan and reflect on the skills used.

Students need to be made aware of problem-solving strategies, group roles and

skills, and conflict management skills, in order for them to gain knowledge about

the strategies used in successful groups. Groups also need to be involved in

choosing the appropriate skills and group roles relevant to their own group.

While the findings in this study were informed by Tuckman and Jensen’s

(1977) model, group development was not always a linear process. The storming

stage was not evident in the second cycle of the study. Norming was evident in all

stages of the study as groups defined their own norms by choosing and reflecting

on problem-solving strategies, group roles and skills, and conflict management

skills, throughout the study.

Page 209: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

196

6.1.3 Cognitive factors

Cognitive factors also influenced how the groups in this study developed

into successful teams and helped them form shared external understandings and

shared internal understandings, or mental models, of the task and of how their

group needed to perform. Druskat and Pescosolido (2002) stated that shared

mental models emerge as team members interact and develop shared beliefs about

the task and how they should work together. This occurred in both cycles of this

study; the collective internal models helped the team members to determine

appropriate actions and helped contribute to a sense of ownership and control of

their team work.

The groups, in both cycles, developed a shared understanding about the

problem task they were solving together. They also developed a shared

knowledge about their team work as they self-managed their team work by

allocating group roles appropriate to the needs of their team and planning what

skills they would use to work as a team. By focusing on both the task and team

aspects of their group, the groups formed a shared internal knowledge about how

successful groups perform. This is consistent with Webber et al. (2000) who

stated that groups must focus on both the task and the team work and share

knowledge of both in order to perform as a successful group. This shared

knowledge helped the groups focus on their problem solving as well as their

group processes. The shared understanding of the problem-solving strategies,

Page 210: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

197

group skills and roles, and conflict-management skills, allowed group members to

reflect on their group performance and constantly improve their problem solving.

Barron (2000) suggested that in order to establish shared understanding,

group members must first negotiate a shared external representation. Students

need to articulate their thinking and make their group understanding explicit in

order to collaborate with group members (Lewis, 1997; Mohammed & Dumville,

2001). The shared external representations in this study were facilitated by group

diaries, checklists, the mathematical ranking models, and the Knowledge Forum®

database, which all helped scaffold the group and the problem-solving process.

Explicit knowledge of effective problem-solving strategies, group roles

and group skills were shared with the students in both cycles of the study which

helped the groups to form their own shared understanding of both their task- and

team-work. Explicit knowledge about conflict management skills was also shared

with the students in Cycle 2 of this study. The shared external goals helped

students develop a sense of being part of the 'group' and the group processing

checklists, from the group diaries, allowed groups to assess how well they were

working together.

During Cycle 2, the mathematical ranking models and the Knowledge

Forum® notes helped students develop an external representation of the task they

were collaboratively completing. Groups shared their ranking models with their

online teams by posting the models onto the Knowledge Forum® database; the

Page 211: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

198

groups also posted notes that related to both the task and the team. As predicted

by Scardamalia and Bereiter (1994), the Knowledge Forum® database allowed

the task knowledge to be visible to all groups and provided a record of the

problem-solving process (Hurme & Järvelä, 2001; Jonassen & Carr, 2000;

Ngeow, 1998; Scardamalia & Bereiter, 1994; Stein, 1998).

By explicitly sharing their ideas, the students were able to gain a shared

understanding of how their group needed to work together to solve the problem-

solving task they were completing. The shared explicit understanding allowed

groups to work effectively and to build a shared internal understanding for solving

the problem and working together as a team.

6.1.3.1 Summary

The findings from this study highlight the importance of groups

developing shared external representations of problem-solving strategies, group

roles and skills, and conflict management skills, in order to develop shared

understandings of both their task-work and their team-work. The shared internal

understandings that groups formed from the shared external knowledge ensured

that the groups had common expectations of the task and the team, which allowed

them to be able to coordinate their actions to perform successfully. The findings

are consistent with Cannon-Bowers and Salas (2001) concept of shared mental

model construction for effective work teams. The findings emphasise that the

Page 212: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

199

concept of shared mental models are also important for groups developing a

shared understanding in educational settings.

This shared understanding of both their task and their team helped the

groups coordinate their actions towards achieving the solution to the task as well

as working together successfully as a team. Groups need to have a shared internal

model of how their group is developing and how effectively their group is

performing. Groups also need to reflect on and develop a shared understanding of

both their task- and their team-work.

6.1.4 Group metacognition

In this study, engagement in group metacognitive strategies of planning,

monitoring, and evaluating their shared understandings of both their task- and

team-work was scaffolded by the group diaries. By engaging in these three

metacognitive strategies, the groups identified the skills and strategies that they

could use to improve their group problem solving. Group members were able to

develop a shared understanding of their problem-solving task and their team by

planning, monitoring and evaluating their group’s success and by discarding

inappropriate team- and task-skills.

Because of these metacognitive strategies, the groups in this study

developed and advanced their knowledge about successful problem-solving

strategies, group roles and skills, and conflict management strategies, and

developed a belief that their group processes could be improved by using the

Page 213: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

200

metacognitive strategies of planning, monitoring, and evaluating. This knowledge

and belief helped members develop a positive attitude towards their group and an

understanding that they could control their own group processes.

6.1.4.1 Planning

Ward and Morris (2005) proposed that the first stage of planning is to

form a mental representation of the problem. According to Ward and Morris, the

representation needs to include the initial state as well as a range of possible

actions that need to be taken to reach the goal state. In both cycles of this study,

this was achieved by the groups using their diaries to state what the problem was

as well as what their plan was to solve the problem. The Knowledge Forum®

notes in Cycle 2 also contained planning messages regarding what categories each

group was going to use to rank the best city.

The planning phase helped build the groups’ knowledge about effective

group work and they developed a shared understanding of what needed to be done

in order to work effectively together and to achieve a solution to the problem-

solving task. The groups used the planning page of the diary, at the start of each

cycle, to focus on their group roles, group skills, and problem-solving skills.

Group members planned together which roles and skills were important for their

group to develop into an effective performing team. By planning which skills

would be focused on, group members were able to identify the skills that they

needed to improve.

Page 214: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

201

6.1.4.2 Monitoring

Students often fail to monitor their problem solving or their group work

(Carr & Biddlecomb, 1998; White & Frederiksen, 2005). In this study, this

dilemma was addressed by the including of a monitoring checklist in the group

diary. This helped the groups reflect on their learning, and scaffolded the

monitoring processes (c.f., Blakey & Spence, 1990; Mueller & Fleming, 1994;

Wilson & Johnson, 2000). The groups used the monitoring checklists to decide if

they needed to change the skills they had chosen in the planning stage. The

groups also determined what other skills needed to be included to make the group

problem-solving process more effective.

Transcripts of the audio (MP3) recordings of the groups working together

in Cycle 1 showed that groups were monitoring their shared understanding of

their team and their task work throughout the study as they used the group diaries

in each computer session to reassign group roles and identify skills that they

needed to improve their problem solving and their group work. The Knowledge

Forum® notes, from Cycle 2, showed that the groups were also monitoring their

shared understanding of their online team’s progress with the problem-solving

task (see Section 5.3.2). The monitoring notes included messages ensuring that all

the categories the team needed for their ranking system were incorporated.

Page 215: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

202

6.1.4.3 Evaluating

Each group in this study utilised an evaluation checklist to develop shared

knowledge of how to evaluate their understanding of the task and the team. The

evaluation checklist in the group diary asked groups to reflect on what had

worked well with their groups and what they could do differently next time they

worked as a group. The checklist helped the groups reflect on what task- and

team-skills they used and what skills they need to incorporate in the future. The

groups evaluated their own skill selection throughout the study reflecting on the

effectiveness of their team and their task work. This is consistent with Johnson et

al. (1993) who stated that groups need to reflect on how their group is functioning

to achieve their goals and adjust the skills needed in order to develop into a

successful performing group. By evaluating the skills used group members were

able to a shared develop understanding of how they could effectively work

together and of their problem-solving task.

At the end of each cycle, the groups were also asked to evaluate how the

activity including the group diaries, posters, and Knowledge Forum® database

could be improved. The students all agreed that the group diaries were effective

and did not need to be changed. In Cycle 2 students indicated that they found the

team skills, problem solving, and conflict management posters useful (see

Appendix X, p. 302). The students also stated that the team skills of being positive

and sharing information were the two most important team skills included on the

posters.

Page 216: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

203

6.1.4.4 Summary

This study showed that applying metacognitive strategies for group

problem solving allows students to focus on the organisational and cognitive

factors that influence how groups perform their problem-solving task and work

together as a team. Groups need to plan, monitor, and evaluate group skills and

strategies specific to the task and their team. They also need to be able to apply

these strategies to develop a shared group understanding

Groups in this study assumed responsibility for planning, monitoring, and

evaluating their group learning through the use of the metacognitive checklists.

The group metacognition led to shared understandings of team- and task-work

which helped the groups take responsibility for their own group performance.

Group members also developed knowledge about metacognitive strategies as they

answered the group metacognitive questionnaire and completed the planning,

monitoring, and evaluating checklists in the group diary.

6.2 Group metacognitive model

The results from Cycles 1 and 2 have been cumulated into a unified

conceptual model that can be used to inform the scaffolding of within- and

between-group metacognition during mathematical problem solving within CSCL

environments (see Figure 6.4). The following sections construct the group

metacognition model and describe how groups can develop a shared

Page 217: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

204

understanding of how to work successfully as a team while engaged in group

mathematical problem solving.

6.2.1 Problem-solving context

The complex problem-solving context forms the initial component of the

group metacognition model emanating from this study (See Figure 6.1).

Complex problem-s olving context

Figure 6.1. Complex problem-solving context.

Previous studies have found that the type of mathematical problem

administered to students during group problem solving has much impact on the

quantity and quality of students’ face-to-face and online discussions (Cho &

Page 218: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

205

Jonassen, 2002; Etheris & Tan, 2004; Jonassen & Kwon, 2001). When groups of

students are involved in problem tasks which are ill-defined such as the model-

eliciting problems (Lesh & Kelly 2002), groups need to be involved in high levels

of co-operation, as they work together. The outcomes of this study confirmed this.

The ‘Best city’ model-eliciting problem required the students to use task and

team-skills to work together to solve the problem (Dishon & O'Leary 1984).

As was noted in Chapter 3, the design of the ‘Best city’ model eliciting

activity was informed by Lesh et al.’s (2000) six principles for the construction of

model-eliciting activities (see Section 2.1.1.1). Based on the analysis of

observation and focus group interview data, the reality and construct shareability

principles were found to be most crucial. That is, the problem needs to be:

1. Interesting, meaningful, and relevant to students and to enable them to

engage in the problem for a longer period of time than traditional

classroom mathematical problems and exercises (Reality principle).

2. Designed to allow for the creation of mathematical model(s) that can be

shared and advanced through discourse with other groups of students

(Construct shareability principle)

A key outcome of the analysis of observation, focus group interviews, and

Knowledge Forum data was that most students came to the realisation that

mathematics was not a one-right-answer one-right-procedure type of endeavour.

In addition to helping the students gain this more advanced conceptualisation

Page 219: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

206

about the nature and discourse of mathematics, this realisation was found to be a

very motivating factor for the students. Thus, in addition to Lesh et al.’s (2000)

principles, the outcomes of this study indicate an additional principle that focuses

on the design of problem tasks that helps students to gain insights about the

subjectivity of mathematics was needed, that is a Subjectivity of Mathematics

Principle such as:

The design of the tasks should facilitate the creation of multiple and

sharable models that are open to modification, improvement, and

validation.

Problem tasks informed by these three principles would provide students

with contexts that would facilitate the development and utilisation of group

metacognitive behaviour.

6.2.2 Organisational factors

The initial conceptual framework developed from the literature review

suggested that organisational factors influenced group problem solving and

learning (see Figure 2.7). The results from this study showed that the

organisational factors could be aggregated into four categories: problem-solving

strategies, group roles, group skills, and conflict management skills, in order for

groups to gain knowledge about the strategies used by successful groups. Posters

of these skills and strategies should be made available to the groups. Groups also

Page 220: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

207

need to be involved in choosing the skills and group roles relevant to their own

group.

The findings from Cycle 1 highlighted that making the skills explicit helps

students understand the importance of using the skills. Analysis of Cycle 2

highlighted that it is also important that students are aware of group processes and

how group skills will benefit their group work. Groups must learn how to control

their own group’s learning in order to perform successfully and in order to learn

the task- and team-skills for future group work. Therefore, the following four

organisational factors were added to the model: problem-solving strategies, group

roles, group skills, and conflict management strategies (see Figure 6.2).

The process of constructing explicit knowledge about these four

organisational factors can be scaffolded by providing posters that make the

problem-solving strategies, group roles, group skills and conflict management

strategies public and shared knowledge that groups can constantly refer to and

build on when engaged in collaborative group problem solving.

Page 221: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

208

Complex problem-s olving context

Group ski lls

Problem-solving

strategies

Group roles

Confl ict management

ski l ls

Organisational factors

Figure 6.2. Organisational factors.

6.2.3 Cognitive factors

Groups who engage in ill-defined tasks, such as model-eliciting tasks need

to develop a shared understanding of the task, as they work out what they need to

achieve and how they will share relevant information to arrive at the answer

(Zawojewski et al., 2003). By explicitly sharing their ideas, groups are able to

gain a shared understanding of how their group needs to work together to solve

the problem-solving task they are completing. A shared understanding of the task

also helps groups reflect on how their group can perform and coordinate their

actions to achieve a solution. The results from this study confirmed that in order

Page 222: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

209

to successfully work together, group members also need to have shared

knowledge of effective problem-solving strategies, group roles and skills, and

conflict management skills.

The findings from this study also highlight the importance of groups

developing a shared external representation of their task- and team-work in order

to develop a shared internal understanding. The development of the external

representation of their team-work and task-work enabled the groups to develop a

shared internal understanding of the problem-solving task and how their group

was performing. Therefore, within the cognitive section of the conceptual model,

the importance of developing shared team-knowledge and shared task-knowledge

is highlighted (See Figure 6.3).

Shared task understanding

Shared team understanding

Group ski ll s

Problem-solving

strategiesGroup roles

Confl ict management

ski l ls

Organisationalfactors

Cognitive factors

Complex problem-s olving context

Figure 6.3. Cognitive factors.

Page 223: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

210

6.2.4 Metacognitive factors

The findings in this study clearly indicate that successful problem-solving

groups critically reflect on the skills used in order to constantly improve their

shared understanding of their team work and their problem-solving task.

Therefore, the conceptual model posits the need for planning, monitoring, and

evaluating strategies that help groups to reflect on their developing shared

knowledge about team- and task-work within the complex problem-solving

context (see Figure 6.4).

Applying metacognitive strategies for group problem solving allows

groups to focus on the organisational and cognitive factors that influence how

they perform their problem-solving activity and work together as a team. Groups

need to plan, monitor, and evaluate group skills, roles, and strategies specific to

their group and the group problem-solving task. They also need to apply these to

develop a shared group understanding of the team and task aspects of the group

problem-solving activity.

Page 224: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

211

Shared task understanding

Shared team understanding

Planning Monitoring Evaluating

Group ski ll s

Problem-solving

strategiesGroup roles

Confl ict management

ski l ls

Organisational factors

Cognitive factors

Metacognitive factors

Complex problem-s olving context

Figure 6.4. Group metacognition model.

6.2.5 Discussion

In this section, the interrelationships between the four components of the

group metacognition model (the complex problem-solving context, organisational

factors, cognitive factors, and metacognitive factors) will be analysed and

discussed. The analysis and discussion will highlight not only the importance of

each component of the model but also the consequences of students not acquiring

specific elements of each of the components.

The first component of the model, the context provided by complex

mathematical problems, was based on the three modifications or design principles

Page 225: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

212

(see Section 6.2.1). Therefore, the groups in both cycles were able to engage in

discourse that facilitated the advancement of both task- and team-knowledge.

If, however, problem tasks that do not adequately meet these three

principles are used, it is unlikely that the students will have opportunities to

engage in the mathematical knowledge building discourse necessary for the

adoption of the organisational, cognitive, and group metacognitive factors

identified in the group metacognition model. As Lesh et al. (2000) pointed out,

most mathematical problems generally found in mathematics textbooks and

worksheets fall into this category and do not meet the three design principles.

They usually fail to motivate groups of students to engage in sustained task-

related discourse. Furthermore, they often only have one answer and one ‘correct’

solution procedure. These two characteristics of “classroom” mathematical

problems make it highly unlikely for groups to engage in the task-related

discussion that is essential for effective mathematical knowledge building.

The second component of the model, the four categories of organisational

factors: problem-solving strategies, group roles, group skills, and conflict

management skills, enable groups to gain knowledge about the skills and

strategies used in successful problem-solving teams. These four categories are

complementary and all must be performed for groups to work together effectively.

The problem-solving strategies can be used by the groups to identify

possible solution paths specific to the problem being solved. The strategies also

provide a language of discourse for effectively allowing groups to discuss their

Page 226: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

213

solution paths as they reflect on how they solved the problem. If the students are

not conversant with the problem-solving strategies, they may not be able to start

the problem-solving process. However, the main consequence of not being

conversant with these strategies is that it could limit the task-related discourse that

is so crucial for advancement of the mathematical models by both face-to-face

and online teams.

By scaffolding group skills, groups are able to incorporate both successful

task- and team-oriented behaviours. Task-skills include sharing ideas, sharing

information, and checking for understanding (see Appendix X, p. 302). Team-

skills include encouraging others, being positive, and checking for agreement (see

Appendix X, p.302). If these group skills are not incorporated by the groups, then

they almost certainly will not focus on their team- as well as their task-

performance.

The group roles help the groups self-organise and allow each student an

opportunity to use the computer. As reported by Cohen (1994), it is important to

make these roles public and give students authority to act in the group role

allocated. This helps clarify the group role for the student and for other group

members. If the group roles are not incorporated by the groups, the groups

probably will lack direction and engage in non-productive team-conflict.

Conflict management skills are necessary as disagreements and conflicts

are often the reason why students do not like to work together in groups and/or do

not work effectively as teams. Giving the groups the tools to resolve their own

Page 227: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

214

conflicts enables conflict to be a positive rather than a negative factor influencing

group problem solving. If groups do not acquire these conflict management skills,

most conflict will be team-based rather than task-based. As was noted in the

literature review, knowledge-building behaviour is enhanced when conflict is

task-based rather than being team-based (see Section 2.1.1).

The third component of the model, the cognitive factors are important in

helping groups develop and form a shared understanding of their team- and task-

work during a complex problem-solving task. Without this shared understanding,

groups may not advance their shared knowledge of the organisational factors (i.e.,

problem-solving strategies, group skills, group roles, and conflict management

skills) that the group needs to incorporate in order to improve their group problem

solving and learning. If the groups do not acquire and utilise these cognitive

factors, then it is highly unlikely that the groups’ shared understanding of the

organisational factors will advance beyond what has been directly taught to them

by their teacher(s). That is, they will not learn how to expand their repertoire of

problem-solving strategies, group skills, group roles, and conflict management

skills beyond what the teacher has introduced to them. Without this ability,

limited growth will occur in the groups’ collaborative group learning.

While the organisational and cognitive factors are important, the fourth

component of the model, the metacognitive factors, is probably the most crucial

component of the group metacognition model. These factors are crucial for the

development and improvement of the groups’ shared understanding of their team-

Page 228: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

215

and their task-work. By planning, monitoring, and evaluating their shared team

and task understanding, groups can identify the skills and strategies that they can

use to improve their problem solving and their team work. Groups are able to

advance their shared team and task understandings by using the metacognitive

scaffolds as they plan, monitor, and evaluate their problem-solving strategies,

group skills, group roles, and conflict management skills. Without these group

metacognitive factors, advances on the groups’ shared understandings of their

task- and team-work are unlikely to occur. At best, there will be only limited

advances in these shared understandings.

6.3 Application of group metacognition model

The aim of this study was to develop a conceptual model to inform the use

of scaffolds to facilitate within- and between-group metacognition during

mathematical problem solving in computer supported collaborative learning

(CSCL) environments.

The model in the form presented in Figure 6.4 can be applied in both

within- and between-group contexts. However, the findings from this study

indicate that before the between-group online teams engage in CSCL, it is

important that they have had within-group face-to-face experience in applying the

organisational, cognitive, and metacognitive factors subsumed in the group

metacognition model. All groups worked together in their face-to-face groups in

order to plan, monitor, and evaluate their team and task work. The groups also

Page 229: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

216

worked with other groups and formed online teams. The team and task skills that

the groups used face-to-face were transferred to the online environment.

Therefore, it is a recommendation of this study that groups have extensive face-

to-face experience in applying the group metacognition model prior to engaging

in group work in an online environment.

This recommendation is consistent with prior CSCL research (e.g., Brett,

Nason & Woodruff, 2002; Brett, Woodruff & Nason, 1999, 2002; Nason, Brett, &

Woodruff, 1996) that indicates that group collaboration skills need to be learnt

and consolidated at face-to-face levels prior to their implementation within CSCL

contexts. If this is not done, group members will not be able to transfer their

collaboration skills to the online environment and will not be able to form

effective online problem-solving teams.

The findings from this study also indicate that the mathematical problem-

solving task administered to the groups enabled them to share and discuss the

mathematical models they had constructed with the other two groups contained in

their online CSCL team. This enabled them to immediately engage in online

CSCL knowledge building. Without these mathematical models to discuss,

evaluate, and advance, it is highly unlikely that this CSCL knowledge building

discourse would have occurred.

Page 230: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

217

6.4 Conclusion

In this chapter, the findings from Stage 1 of the study were cumulated into

a unified conceptual model to inform research and practice about the development

and maintenance of within- and between-group metacognition during

mathematical problem solving with primary school students. The conceptual

model posits that in order to promote group metacognition during mathematical

problem solving, the following four components need to be addressed: 1. problem

solving-context, 2. organisational factors, 3. cognitive factors, and 4.

metacognitive factors. The model highlights that organisational and cognitive

factors are important to help groups develop and form a shared understanding of

their team- and task-work during complex mathematical problem solving in a

CSCL environment. The model also highlights that metacognitive factors are

crucial as groups need to continually reflect on and improve their shared

understandings.

Page 231: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

218

Page 232: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

219

CHAPTER 7: CONCLUSION

This chapter begins with a brief overview of the study in Section 7.1. The

theoretical and practical significance of the study is then discussed in Section 7.2.

Following this, the limitations of the study are identified in Section 7.3. The

chapter concludes with recommendations for future research in Section 7.4.

7.1 Overview of the study

The aim of this study was to develop a conceptual model to inform the use

of scaffolds to facilitate within- and between-group metacognition during

mathematical problem solving in computer supported collaborative learning

(CSCL) environments. In order to meet this aim, a design research methodology

was adopted. The design research methodology involved two cycles of design,

experiment, and analysis in order to generate outcomes that have application both

within and beyond the context of the present study and to build theory (Bereiter,

2002; Shavelson et al., 2003). A case study method was also used for data

collection to incorporate multiple sources of data and in order to bring out the

viewpoint of the group members involved in the study (Tellis, 1997).

The study proceeded in two cycles. Cycle 1, which focused on Research

Objective 1, involved the design, evaluation, and advancement of within-group

metacognitive scaffolds during mathematical problem solving for groups working

around a computer. Cycle 2, which focused on both Research Objectives 1 and 2,

Page 233: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

220

involved the design, evaluation, and advancement of within- and between-group

metacognitive scaffolds for groups building collective mathematical knowledge in

CSCL environments. The results from Cycles 1 and 2 were synthesised into a

unified model (Research Objective 3) that could be used to inform the design of

within- and between-group metacognitive scaffolds in CSCL environments (see

Figure 7.1).

Shared task understanding

Shared team understanding

Planning Monitoring Evaluating

Group ski ll s

Problem-solving

strategiesGroup roles

Confl ict management

ski l ls

Organisational factors

Cognitive factors

Metacognitive factors

Complex problem-s olving context

Figure 7.1. Final model.

The final model identified four categories of factors that influence how

groups work effectively within CSCL environments. These four categories are:

the problem-solving context, organisational factors, cognitive factors, and

Page 234: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

221

metacognitive factors. Without an appropriate problem-solving context, it is

highly unlikely that students will engage in knowledge-building discourse that

leads to the advancement of their mathematical knowledge. The model also

clearly identified that the scaffolding of organisational and cognitive knowledge

about the team and the task are necessary but not sufficient conditions for

facilitating effective group problem solving and collaborative learning. The model

highlighted that the metacognitive factors are crucial for groups to develop into

effective collaborative knowledge building teams; without the scaffolding of

group metacognition, it is highly unlikely that groups will learn how to advance

their organisational and cognitive strategies and skills.

7.2 Significance

The literature review presented in Chapter 2 noted that because most prior

research into collaborative group learning inside and outside of CSCL

environments had tended to focus on organisational or cognitive factors only,

relatively few recent significant theoretical and practical advances had been made

in the field. The group metacognition model (see Figure 6.4) with its focus on the

problem-solving context, organisational, cognitive, and metacognitive factors and

the symbiotic relationships between these four categories of factors thus provides

a conceptual framework to advance the field of collaborative group learning

within CSCL environments both theoretically and practically.

Page 235: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

222

The group metacognition model has what Yin (1994) refers to as

analytical generalisability but not statistical generalisability. Findings from case

studies are generalisable to theoretical propositions but not to populations (Yin,

1994). The theoretical propositions subsumed within the group metacognition

model thus can be generalised to other group contexts (Miles & Huberman, 1994)

and can be utilised to provide a conceptual framework to advance the fields of

collaborative learning and metacognition both theoretically and practically.

7.2.1 Theoretical significance

The theoretical significance of the study is predominately derived from the

four categories of factors, the structure, and the interaction between the four

categories subsumed within the group metacognition model which is presented in

Figure 6.4. This model thus provides a holistic framework for not only enabling

researchers to investigate each of these four categories of factors in isolation but

also a framework for investigating the relationship between these categories.

Most of the research focusing on collaborative group learning within

CSCL environments has not considered the influence of the type of task that

students are engaged in has on the development of effective group learning. Three

principles were used in this study to inform the design of the mathematical

problem-solving task to provide the optimal context for facilitating within- and

between-group metacognition in a CSCL environments. The set of three

principles provides researchers with starting points for research in the fields of

Page 236: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

223

CSCL, group metacognition, and mathematics. The set of principles to inform the

design/selection/modification of mathematical problems subsumed within the

group metacognition model also provides researchers with principles that can be

either supported or refuted by future research.

The group metacognition model clearly identified close relationships

between team and task organisational strategies and skills. The model thus

provides researchers with a new lens for investigating organisational factors

within CSCL environments. In particular, the model provides researchers with a

framework for proceeding beyond the investigation of the scaffolding of team and

task strategies and skills in isolation that has characterised most past research in

this area.

The shared cognitive understandings of team and task that form an

essential component in the group metacognition model had their genesis in

organisational research. The inclusion of cognitive factors in the model thus

provides conceptual artefacts that hitherto have seldomly been utilised in

educational research in general and in the field of CSCL in particular. The lenses

offered by these artefacts enable researchers to investigate how students can

advance their knowledge about team and task beyond what has been introduced to

them by their teachers.

The inclusion of metacognitive factors within the model extends theory

and research about metacognition beyond the study of individuals to the study of

metacognition within collaborative learning groups. As Costa and O'Leary (1992)

Page 237: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

224

and Stahl (2006) pointed out, this is an area within CSCL research that has been

under-researched. Prior research into group metacognition, in the main, has not

focused on the relationship between group metacognition and the three other

factors identified in the group metacognition model. Therefore, in addition to

providing a conceptual framework for extending research into metacognition

beyond the individual, the model provides a framework to inform future research

into the relationship between group metacognition and the other three factors.

The four categories of factors subsumed within the group metacognition

model provide future researchers with conceptual artefacts that can be confirmed

or refuted. This is consistent with Popper’s (1979) notion that in order to facilitate

the advancement of knowledge, researchers should present their findings in forms

that enable future researchers to confirm or refute their findings.

7.2.2 Practical significance

In addition to theoretical significance, the outcomes of the present

research study also have practical significance. The group metacognition model

presented in Figure 6.4 can provide teachers with a framework to inform the

implementation of effective CSCL groups within their classrooms.

The set of principles to inform the design of mathematical problem-

solving tasks (subsumed within the group metacognition model) can be used by

teachers to design appropriate mathematical problems to facilitate the building of

mathematical models that form the basis for knowledge building discourse by

Page 238: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

225

groups of students. The principles can also be used by teachers to select

appropriate existing mathematical problems or to modify inappropriate existing

problems into forms which will facilitate knowledge building discourse by groups

of students.

Problems need to interesting, meaningful, and relevant to students and be

able to engage students for a longer period of time than traditional classroom

problems. Problems needed to be designed to allow for the creation of

mathematical model(s) that can be shared and advanced through discourse with

other groups of students. During the solution of the problems, students should

gain an understanding about the subjectivity of mathematical models (i.e.,

mathematical models are open to criticism and revision).

The team and task organisational scaffolds subsumed within the group

metacognition model can be utilised by teachers to introduce and maintain

effective group roles, group skills, conflict management skills, and problem-

solving strategies (see Appendix X, p. 302) within collaborative learning groups.

The list of team- and task-organisational scaffolds subsumed within the group

metacognition model can also be used by teachers as a checklist for evaluating

their current applications of collaborative group learning within and between their

classrooms.

Teachers can use group diaries and the structure of CSCL interfaces

(subsumed within the group metacognition model) to scaffold the cognitive

Page 239: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

226

strategies and skills necessary for effective collaborative learning groups. Group

diaries facilitate the building of shared external representations necessary for

effective collaborative group learning. The structure of the CSCL interfaces can

facilitate online discourse that goes beyond socialisation to cognition.

Teachers can also use metacognitive scaffolds, such as those identified in

the final group metacognition model, to enable groups to focus on how their

group is completing the problem-solving task and working together as a team.

The planning, monitoring, and evaluating checklists incorporating the use of

metacognitive strategies subsumed within the group metacognition model can be

used to initiate, maintain, and advance group metacognition. In addition, teachers

can utilise the planning, monitoring, and evaluating checklists to enhance

metacognitive behaviour by individual students within their classrooms.

7.3 Limitations

During the course of the study, the following possible limitations were

noted:

1. Generalisability of the outcomes of the study

2. Researcher as participant

3. Withdrawal of the group metacognitive scaffolds

4. Design of scaffolding tools

Page 240: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

227

Because of the case-study methodology utilised in the study, the group

metacognition model generated during the study does not have statistical

generalisability (Yin, 1994). However, as was noted earlier in Section 7.2, the

theoretical propositions subsumed within the model have analytical

generalisability. Thus, it can be argued that the outcomes of the study have

analytical generalisability.

There are also limitations concerning the role of the researcher as

participant. The nature of the design research methodology used in this study is

that the design experiment is set up and implemented by the researcher. To

overcome this limitation Bereiter (2002) suggested that the research design

methodology requires ongoing collaboration with the teachers involved. A good

rapport was established between the researcher, the classroom teachers, and the

students. The classroom teachers in this study approved the activity, provided

information about the class, and helped form the groups.

A further limitation to this study was the failure to withdraw scaffolds

once the group metacognitive processes involved had been internalised. Scaffolds

are temporary supports which should be gradually decreased as the groups

competence increases (King, 1989; Vygotsky, 1978). Due to the limited duration

of the study, the scaffolds were not withdrawn as the groups were still

internalising the process. However, the groups involved in the study showed that

they were developing a shared internal understanding of both the task and team

aspects of their group work. The development of the within-group metacognition

Page 241: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

228

also negated the need for between-group online metacognitive scaffolds as the

primary school groups working face-to-face were able to transfer their shared

understandings to the online context.

Another limitation of the research study was that the study did not

generate principles to inform the advancement of the design of tools to scaffold

the development of organisational, cognitive, and group metacognitive strategies

and skills. The tools utilised in this study were derived from previous research

studies. During the course of the study, the researcher noted limitations in many

of these scaffolding tools. Many of these limitations can probably be traced back

to the limitations of the theories on which these tools were based. Research that

advances theory underlying many of these scaffolding tools is needed.

7.4 Recommendations for further research

Further research needs to address group metacognition as a process of

planning, monitoring, and evaluating shared group understandings of the task and

team aspects of effective groups. While this study addresses this issue to a certain

extent, group metacognition requires further study and has implications for a

range of educational settings. Further research could focus on different

educational levels, including secondary and tertiary, in order to demonstrate if the

results presented in this research study can be replicated.

Aspects of the group metacognition model have implications for the

design of CSCL environments. The organisational, cognitive, and metacognitive

Page 242: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

229

factors identified in the final model need to be further investigated. They also

need to be tested and refined in other group-based situations. Future research

should also focus on the development of sets of principles to inform

advancements in the design of scaffolding tools such as the posters, group diaries,

and metacognitive checklists utilised in this study.

7.5 Conclusion

Within the research literature, there is a general consensus that group

metacognitive activities such as planning how to approach a given learning task,

monitoring progress, and evaluating progress toward the completion of the task

play critical roles in successful group learning and problem solving. There is also

a general consensus within the research literature that metacognitive activities of

planning, monitoring, and evaluating need to be scaffolded. However, prior to this

research study, there were limitations in the corpus of knowledge about the

scaffolding of group metacognition; most of the current knowledge in this field

tended to be fragmented, seemingly inconsistent, and compartmentalised in

nature. For example, knowledge about planning, monitoring, and evaluating team

aspects of group problem solving rarely has been integrated with knowledge

about planning, monitoring, and evaluating task aspects of group problem solving.

Furthermore, integration with respect to problem contexts and organisational,

cognitive, and metacognitive factors is not evident in the current corpus of

research literature.

Page 243: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

230

The major outcome of this study, the group metacognition model,

integrated key components of the field, namely complex problem-solving

contexts, organisational factors, cognitive factors, and metacognitive factors, into

a unified conceptual model. This model advances the field in at least two ways.

First, the unified model provides researchers and practitioners with a framework

to gain a holistic and better understanding of the corpus of previous research

knowledge in the field of scaffolding group metacognition. Second, the model

also provides researchers and practitioners with a conceptual framework and four

sets of conceptual artefacts to inform future research and practice, and thus

advance the corpus of knowledge about scaffolding within- and between-group

metacognition in CSCL environments.

Page 244: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

231

REFERENCES

Abramovich, S. (2003). Spreadsheet-enhanced problem solving in context as

modeling. Spreadsheets in Education, 1(1), 1-17.

Ackerman, M. (2000). The intellectual challenge of CSCW: The gap between social

requirements and technical feasibility. Human-Computer Interaction, 15(2-3),

179-203.

Aldag, R. J., & Fuller, S. R. (1993). Beyond fiasco: A reappraisal of the group think

phenomenon and a new model of group decision processes. Psychological

Bulletin, 113(3), 533-552.

Antaki, C., & Lewis, A. (1986). Mental mirrors: Metacognition in social knowledge

and communication. London: Sage.

Archer-Kath, J., & Johnson, D. (1994). Group work in education: Psychological

aspects. Journal of Social Psychology, 134(5), 681-694.

Armstrong, S., & Priola, V. (2001). Individual differences in cognitive style and their

effects on task and social orientations of self-managed work teams. Small

Group Research, 32(4), 283-312.

Azevedo, R., & Hadwin, A. F. (2005). Scaffolding self-regulated learning and

metacognition: Implications for the design of computer-based scaffolds.

Instructional Science, 33(5-6), 367–379.

Bales, R. (1970). Personality and interpersonal behaviour. New York: Holt,

Rhinehart and Winston.

Bales, R., & Cohen, S. (1979). SYMLOG: A system for the multiple level observation

of groups. New York: Free Press.

Page 245: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

232

Barab, S. (2006). Design based research: A methodological toolkit for the learning

scientist. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning

sciences (pp. 153-169). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Barker, V., Abrams, J., Tiyaamornwong, V., & Seibold, D. (2000). New contexts for

relational communication in groups. Small Group Research, 31(4), 470-503.

Barron, B. (2000). Achieving coordination in collaborative problem-solving groups.

The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 9(4), 403-436.

Beamish, P., & Au, W. (1995). Learning with computers and instructional strategies.

Australian Computers in Education. University of Newcastle, Perth, Western

Australia. Retrieved February 1, 2005 from:

http://www.educationau.edu.au/archives/CP/REFS/wing.htm

Beatty, C., & Barker, S. (2004). Building smart teams: A roadmap to high

performance. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Benjamin, J., Bessant, J., & Watts, R. (1997). Making groups work: Rethinking

practice. St. Leonards, New South Wales: Allen & Unwin.

Berardi-Coletta, B., Buyer, L., Dominowski, R., & Rellinger, E. (1995).

Metacognition and problem solving: A process-oriented approach. Journal of

Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21(1), 201-233.

Bereiter, C. (2002). Design research for sustained innovation. Cognitive Studies,

Bulletin of the Japanese Cognitive Science Society, 9(3), 321-327.

Bereiter, C., & Scardamalia, M. (1989). Intentional learning as a goal of instruction.

In L. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of

Robert Glaser (pp. 361-392). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 246: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

233

Bielaczyc, K., & Collins, A. (1999). Learning communities in classrooms: Advancing

knowledge for a lifetime. National Association of Secondary School

Principals, 83(604), 4-10.

Blakey, E., & Spence, S. (1990). Developing metacognition. Eric Reproduction

Services No. ED327218. Retrieved February 1, 2005 from EDRS Online:

http://www.thememoryhole.org/edu/eric/ed327218.html.

Bositis, D. A. (1988). Some observations on the participant method. Political

Behavior, 10(4), 333-348.

Brannick, M. T., & Prince, C. (1997). An overview of team performance

measurement. In M. T. Brannick, E. Salas, & C. Prince (Eds.), Team

performance and measurement: Theory, methods, and applications (3-16).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Bransford, J., & Stein, B. (1993). The IDEAL problem solver: Improve critical

thinking, improve learning and memory skills, solve problems creatively (2nd

ed.). New York: W.H. Freeman.

Brett, C., Nason, R.A., & Woodruff, E. (2002). Communities of inquiry among pre-

service teachers investigating mathematics. THEMES in Education 3(1), 39-62

Brett, C., Woodruff, E., & Nason, R.A. (1999). Online community and preservice

teachers’ conceptions of learning mathematics. In the Proceedings of the

Computer Support for Collaborative Learning ‘99 Conference (pp.57-65),

Stanford University, December 1999. http://kn.cilt.org/cscl99/A06/A06.HTM

Brett, C., Woodruff, E. & Nason, R. (2002). Developing Identity as Pre-service

Elementary Mathematics Teachers: the contribution of online community.

Page 247: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

234

Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational

Association, New Orleans, April, 2002.

Bromme, R., Hesse, F. W., & Spada, H. (2005). Barriers, biases and opportunities of

communication and cooperation with computers: Introduction and overview.

In R. Bromme, F. W. Hesse, & H. Spada (Eds.), Barriers and biases in

computer-mediated knowledge communication: And how they may be

overcome. (pp. 1-14). New York: Springer.

Brown. A., & Palincsar, A. (1989). Guided, co-operative learning and individual

knowledge acquisition. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.), Knowing and learning: Essays

in honour of Robert Glaser (pp. 393-451). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

Cannon-Bowers, J., & Salas, E. (Eds.). (1998). Making decisions under stress:

Implications for individual and team training. Washington, WA: American

Psychological Association.

Cannon-Bowers, J., & Salas, E. (2001). Reflections on shared cognition. Journal of

Organizational Behaviour, 22(2), 195-202.

Carley, K. (1997). Extracting team mental models through textual analysis. Journal of

Organizational Behavior, 18 (S1), 533-558.

Carr, M., & Biddlecomb, B. (1998). Metacognition in mathematics: From a

constructivist perspective. In J. Hayes, J. Dunlosky, & A. Graesser (Eds.),

Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 69-91). Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Case, J., & Gunstone, R. (2002). Metacognitive development as a shift in approach to

learning: An in-depth study. Studies in Higher Education, 27(4), 459-470.

Page 248: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

235

Cathcart, R., Samovar, L., & Henman, L. (1996). Small group communication:

Theory and practice. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

Charmaz, K. (2000). Grounded theory: Objectivist and constructivist methods. In N.

Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (pp. 509-535).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Charmaz, K. (2003). Qualitative interviewing and grounded theory analysis. In J.

Holstien & J. Gubrium (Eds.), Inside interviewing: New lenses, new concerns

(pp. 311-330). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Chizhik, A. (1998). Collaborative learning through high-level verbal interaction:

From theory to practice. Eric Clearing House, 72(1), 58-62.

Cho, K., & Jonassen, D. (2002). The effects of argumentation scaffolds on

argumentation and problem solving. Educational Technology, Research and

Development, 50(3), 5-18.

Cohen, E. (1994). Designing groupwork: Strategies for the heterogeneous classroom

(2nd ed.). New York: Teachers College Press.

Cohen, L., Manion, L., & Morrison, K. (2000), Research methods in education (5th

ed.). London: Routledge.

Cohen, S., & Gibson, C. (2003). In the beginning: Introduction and framework. In C.

Gibson & S. Cohen (Eds.), Virtual teams that work: Creating conditions for

virtual team effectiveness (pp. 1-13). San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Collins, A., Joseph, D., & Bielaczyc, K. (2004). Design research: Theoretical and

methodological issues. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 13(1), 15-42.

Page 249: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

236

Confrey, J. (2006). The evolution of design studies as methodology. In R. K. Sawyer

(Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences (pp. 135-151). New

York: NY: Cambridge University Press.

Cooke, N., Salas, E., Kiekel, P., & Bell, B. (2004). Advance in measuring team

cognition. In E. Salas & S. Fiore (Eds.), Team cognition: Understanding the

factors that drive process and performance (pp. 83-106). Washington, WA:

American Psychological Association.

Costa, A., & O'Leary, P. (1992). Co-cognition: The co-operative development of the

intellect. In N. Davidson & T. Worsham (Eds.), Enhancing thinking through

co-operative learning (pp. 41-65). New York: Teachers College Press.

Crook, C. (1996). Computers and the collaborative experience of learning. New

York: Routledge.

Crook, C. (1999). Computers in the community of classrooms. In K. Littleton & P.

Light (Eds.), Learning with computers: Analysing productive interaction (pp.

102-117). London: Routledge.

Cullen, J. (1999). Socially constructed learning: A commentary of the concept of the

learning organisation. The Learning Organization, 6(1), 45-53.

Curcio, F., & Artzt, A. (1998). Students communicating in small groups: Making

sense of data in graphical form. In H. Steinbring, M. Bartolini Bussi, & A.

Sierpinska (Eds.), Language and communication in the mathematics

classroom (pp. 179-189). Reston, VA: National Council of Mathematics

Teachers.

Page 250: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

237

Davidson, N., & Worsham, T. (1992). Enhancing thinking through cooperative

learning. New York: Teachers College Press.

De Corte, E., Greer, B., & Verschaffel, L. (1996). Mathematics teaching and learning.

In D. Berliner & R. Calfee (Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology (pp.

491-549). New York: Simon & Schuster Macmillan.

Delclos, V. R., & Harrington, C. (1991). Effects of strategy monitoring and proactive

instruction on children's problem-solving performance. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 83(1), 35-42.

Denzin, N., & Lincoln, Y. (1998). Strategies of qualitative inquiry. Thousand Oaks,

CA: Sage.

Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & Buysse, A. (2001). Metacognition and mathematical

problem solving in grade 3. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34(2), 435-449.

Desoete, A., Roeyers, H., & De Clercq, A. (2003). Can offline metacognition enhance

mathematical problem solving? Journal of Educational Psychology, 95(1),

188-200.

Dickson, T. L., & McIntyre, R. M. (1997). A conceptual framework for teamwork

measurement. In M. T. Brannick, E. Salas, & C. Prince (Eds.), Team

performance and measurement: Theory, methods, and applications (pp. 19-

43). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Dillenbourg, P., Baker, M., Blaye, A., & O'Malley, C. (1996). The evolution of

research on collaborative learning. In E. Spada & P. Reiman (Eds.), Learning

in humans and machine: Towards an interdisciplinary learning science (pp.

189-211). Oxford: Elsevier.

Page 251: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

238

Dishon, D., & O'Leary, P. (1984). A guidebook for co-operative learning: A technique

for creating more effective schools. Holmes Beach, FL: Learning Publications.

Doerr, H. M., & English, L. D. (2001). A modeling perspective on students' learning

through data analysis. In M. Heuvel-Panhuizen (Ed.), Proceeding of the

Twenty-fifth annual meeting of the International Group for the Psychology of

Mathematics Education. (Vol. 2, pp. 361-368). Utrecht, Netherlands:

Freudenthal Institute.

Dominowski, R. (1998). Verbalization and problem solving. In D. Hacker, J.

Dunlosky & A. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and

practice. (pp. 25-45). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Druskat, V. U., & Pescosolido, A. T. (2002). The content of effective teamwork

mental models in self-managing teams: Ownership, learning, and heedful

interrelating. Human Relations, 55(3), 283-314.

Dundis, S., & Benson, S. (2003). Building more effective virtual teams: An

examination of the task variable in online group problem-solving.

International Journal on E-Learning, 2(4), 24-38.

Edelson, D. (2002). Design research: What we learn when we engage in design.

Journal of the Learning Sciences, 11(1), 105-121.

Eder, D., & Fingerson, L. (2003). Interviewing children and adolescents. In J.

Holstien & J. Gubrium (Eds.), Inside interviewing: New lenses, new concerns

(pp. 33-53). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Efklides, A. (2006). Metacognition and affect: What can metacognitive experiences

tell us about the learning process? Educational Research Review, 1(1), 3-14.

Page 252: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

239

English, L., Fox, J. L., & Watters, J. J. (2005). Problem posing and solving with

mathematical modeling. Teaching Children Mathematics, 12(3), 156-163.

Etheris, A., & Tan, S. (2004). Computer-supported collaborative problem solving and

anchored instruction in a mathematics classroom: An exploratory study.

International Journal of Learning Technology, 1(1), 16-39.

Farivar, S., & Webb, N. M. (1994). Helping and getting help: Essential skills for

effective group problem solving. Arithmetic Teacher, 41(9), 521-525.

Fiore, S., & Schooler, J. (2004). Process mapping and shared cognition: Teamwork

and the development of shared problem models. In E. Salas & S. Fiore. (Eds.),

Team cognition: Understanding the factors that drive process and

performance (pp. 133-152). Washington, WA: American Psychological

Association.

Flavell, J. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. B. Resnick (Ed.),

The nature of intelligence (pp. 231-235). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Flavell, J., & Friedrichs, A., & Hoyt, J. (1970). Developmental changes in

memorization processes. Cognitive Psychology, 1(4), 323-340.

Fontana, A., & Frey, J. (2000). The interview: From structured questions to negotiated

text. In N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), Handbook of qualitative research (2nd

ed., pp. 645-672). Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.

Fortunato, I., Hecht, D., Tittle, C., & Alvarez, L. (1991). Metacognition and problem

solving. The Arithmetic Teacher, 39(4), 38-40.

Gama, C. (2000). The role of metacognition in problem solving: Promoting reflection

in interactive learning systems. Sussex, England: University of Sussex.

Page 253: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

240

Garofalo, J., & Lester, F. (1985). Metacognition, cognitive monitoring, and

mathematical performance. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,

16(3), 163-76.

Gillies, R. M. (2000). The maintenance of co-operative and helping behaviours in co-

operative groups. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(1), 97-

111.

Gillies, R. M. (2003). The behaviors, interactions, and perceptions of junior high

school students during small-group learning. Journal of Educational

Psychology, 95(1), 137-147.

Gillies, R. M., & Ashman, A. F. (2000). The effects of cooperative learning on

students with learning difficulties in the lower elementary school. Journal of

Special Education, 34(1), 19-27.

Gillies, R. M., & Ashman, A. F. (2003). An historical review of the use of groups to

promote socialisation and learning. In R. M. Gillies & A. F. Ashman (Eds.),

Co-operative learning: The social and intellectual outcomes of learning in

groups (pp. 1-18). London: RoutledgeFalmer.

Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (2004). Theoretical sampling. In C. Seale (Ed.), Social

research methods: A reader (pp. 226-231). London: Routledge.

Goos, M., Galbraith, P., & Renshaw, P. (2002). Socially mediated metacognition:

Creating collaborative zones of proximal development in small group problem

solving. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 49(2), 193-223.

Gottlieb, M. (2003). Managing group process. Westpoint, CT: Praeger.

Page 254: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

241

Gourgey, A. (2001). Metacognition in basic skills instruction. In H. Hartman (Ed.),

Metacognition in learning and instruction: Theory, research and practice (pp.

17-32). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Guba, E., & Lincoln, Y. (1989). Fourth generation evaluation. Newbury Park, CA:

Sage.

Gut, D. (2000). We are social beings learning how to learn cooperatively. Teaching

Exceptional Children, 32(5), 46-53.

Hakkarainen, K., Lipponen, L., Jarvela, S., & Niemivirta, M. (1999). The interaction

of motivational orientation and knowledge-seeking inquiry in computer-

supported collaborative learning. Journal of Educational Computing

Research, 21(3), 263-284.

Halford, G. (1993). Children's understanding: The development of mental models.

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Haller, C., Gallagher, V., Weldon, T., & Felder, R. (2000). Dynamics of peer

education in cooperative learning workgroups. Journal of Engineering

Education, 89(3), 285-293.

Hamilton, M. (1986). Knowledge and communication in children. In C. Antaki & A.

Lewis (Eds.), Mental mirrors (pp. 78-96). London: Sage.

Hare, A., & Hare, S. (1996). Implicit personality theory, social desirability, and

reflected appraisal of self in the context. Small Group Research, 27(4), 504-

532.

Hare, L., & O'Neill, K. (2000). Effectiveness and efficiency in small academic peer

groups. Small Group Research, 31(1), 24-54.

Page 255: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

242

Hartman, H. (Ed.). (2001). Metacognition in learning and instruction: Theory,

research and practice. Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic

Publications.

Hayes, J. (2002). Interpersonal skills at work. Sussex, England: Routledge.

Henri, F. (1991). Computer conferencing and content analysis. In A. Kaye (Ed.),

Collaborative learning through computer conferencing: The Najaden papers

(pp. 117-136). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Hiltz, R. (1994). The virtual classroom: Learning without limits via computer

networks. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.

Hinsz, V. B. (1995). Mental models of groups as social systems: Considerations of

specification and assessment. Small Group Research, 26 (2), 200-233.

Hinsz, V. B. (2004). Metacognition and mental models in groups: An illustration with

metamemory of group recognition memory. In E. Salas & S. Fiore (Eds.),

Team cognition: Understanding the factors that drive process and

performance (pp. 33-58). Washington, WA: American Psychological

Association.

Hinsz, V., Tindale, R. S., & Vollrath, D. A. (1997). The emerging conceptualization

of groups as information processors. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 43-64.

Hoek, D., Terwel, J., & van den Eeden, P. (1997). Effects of training in the use of

social and cognitive strategies: An intervention study in secondary

mathematics in co-operative groups. Educational Research and Evaluation,

3(4), 364-389.

Page 256: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

243

Honess, T. (1986). Mirroring and social metacognition. In C. Antaki & A. Lewis

(Eds.), Mental mirrors: Metacognition in social knowledge and

communication. (pp. 58-67). London: Sage.

Hoover, J. (2002). Effective small group and team communication. Orlando, FL:

Harcourt.

Hoyles, C., & Healy, L. (1994). Learning mathematics in groups with computers:

Reflections on a research study. British Educational Research Journal, 20(4),

465-484.

Hron, A., & Friedrich, H. (2003). A review of web-based collaborative learning:

Factors beyond technology. Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 19(1),

70-79.

Huberman, A., & Miles, M. (2002). The qualitative researcher's companion.

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Hurme, T., & Järvelä, S. (2001). Metacognitive processes in problem solving with

CSCL in mathematics. Retrieved June 21, 2004 from:

http://www.mmi.unimaas.nl/euro-cscl/Papers/70.doc.

Hurme, T., Palonen, T., & Järvelä, S. (2006). Metacognition in joint discussions: An

analysis of the patterns of interaction and the metacognitive content of the

networked discussions in mathematics. Metacognition and Learning, 1(2),

181-200.

Johnson, B., & Christensen, L. (2004). Educational Research: Quantitative,

qualitative, and mixed approaches (2nd ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Page 257: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

244

Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (1987) Learning together and alone (2nd ed.). Englewood

Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall.

Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (1990). Using cooperative learning in math. In N.

Davidson (Ed.), Cooperative learning in mathematics: A handbook for

teachers (pp. 103-125). Menlo Park, CA: Addison-Wesley.

Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (1999). Making co-operative learning work. Theory into

practice, 38(2), 67-73.

Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (2003). Training for cooperative group work. In M. A.

West, D. Tjosvold, & K. G. Smith (Eds.), International handbook of

organizational teamwork and cooperative working (pp. 167-183). West

Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons.

Johnson, D., & Johnson, R. (Eds.). (2004). Assessing students in groups: Promoting

group responsibility and individual accountability. CA, California: Corwin

Press.

Johnson, D., Johnson, R., & Johnson-Holubec, E. (1993). Co-operation in the

classroom (6th ed.). Edina, MN: Interaction Book Company.

Johnson, S. D., Suriya, C., Won Yoon, S. W., Berrett, J. V., & La Fleur, J. (2002).

Team development and group processes of virtual learning teams. Computers

and Education, 39(4), 379-393.

Jonassen, D., & Carr, C. (2000). Mindtools: Affording multiple knowledge

representations for learning. In S. Lajoie (Ed.), Computers as cognitive tools:

No more walls (pp. 165-195). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 258: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

245

Jonassen, D., & Kwon, H. (2001). Communication patterns in computer mediated

versus face-to-face group problem solving. Educational Technology Research

and Development, 49(1), 35-51.

King, A. (1989). Verbal interaction and problem solving within computer-assisted co-

operative learning groups. Educational Computing Research, 5(1), 1-15.

King, A. (1991). Effects of training in strategic questioning on children's problem-

solving performance. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(3), 307-317.

Klimoski, R., & Mohammed, S. (1994). Team mental model: Construct or metaphor?

Journal of Management, 20(2), 403-437.

Kormanski, C. (1999). The team: Explorations in group process. Colorado, CO:

Love.

Korsgaard, M., Brodt, E., & Sapienza, H. (2003). Trust, identity, and attachment:

Promoting individuals' cooperation in groups. In M. A. West, D. Tjosvold, &

K. G. Smith (Eds.), International handbook of organizational teamwork and

cooperative working (pp. 113-130). West Sussex, England: John Wiley &

Sons.

Kramarski, B., & Mevarech, Z. (2003). Enhancing mathematical reasoning in the

classroom: The effects of cooperative learning and metacognitive training.

American Educational Research Journal, 40(1), 281-310.

Kreijns, K., & Kirschner, P. (2001, January). The social affordances of computer-

supported collaborative learning environments. Paper presented at the 31th

ASEE/IEEE Frontiers in Education conference, Reno, NV. Retrieved June 21,

2004 from: http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/iel5/7628/20809/00963856.pdf.

Page 259: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

246

Kreijns, K., Kirschner, P., & Jochems, W. (2002). The sociability of computer-

supported collaborative learning environments. Educational Technology &

Society, 5(1), 8-22.

Langan-Fox, J. (2003). Skill acquisition and development of a team metal model: An

integrative approach to analysing organizational teams, task, and content. In

M. A. West, D. Tjosvold, & K. G. Smith (Eds.), International handbook of

organizational teamwork and cooperative working (pp. 321-360). West

Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons.

Langan-Fox, J., Anglim, J., & Wilson, J. (2004). Mental models, Team mental

models, and performance: Process, development, and future directions. Human

Factors and Ergonomics in Manufacturing, 14(4) 331-352.

Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991). Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral participation.

Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

Lee, E. Y., Chan, C. K., & van Aalst, J. (2006). Students assessing their own

collaborative knowledge building. International Journal of Computer-

Supported Collaborative Learning, 1(2), 277-307.

Lesh, R. & Doerr, H. (2003). Beyond Constructivism: A models and modeling

perspective on mathematics problem solving, learning, and teaching.

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lesh, R. & Harel, G. (2003). Problem solving, modeling, and local conceptual

development. Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal,

5(2/3), 157-190.

Page 260: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

247

Lesh, R., Hoover, M., Hole, B., Kelly, A., & Post, T. (2000). Principles for

developing thought-revealing activities for students and teachers. In A. Kelly

& R. Lesh (Eds.), Research Design in Mathematics and Science Education

(pp. 591-646). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lesh, R., & Kelly, A. (2002). Multitiered teaching experiments. In A. Kelly & R.

Lesh (Eds.), Research design in mathematics and science education (pp. 197-

230). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Lesh, R., & Lamon, S. (1992). Assessment of authentic performance in school

mathematics. Washington, WA: American Association for the Advancement

of Science.

Lewis, R., (1997). An activity theory framework to explore distributed communities.

Journal of Computer Assisted Learning, 13(4), 210-218.

Light, P. (2004). Collaborative learning with computers. In H. Daniels & A. Edwards

(Eds.), The RoutledgeFalmer reader in psychology of education (pp. 294-306).

London: Routledge.

Light, P., & Littleton, K. (1999). Social processes in children's learning. New York:

Cambridge University Press.

Light, P., Littleton, K., Messer, D., & Joiner, R. (1994) Social communicative

processes in computer based problem solving, European Journal of

Psychology of Education, 7(4), 1-14.

Lim, B., & Klein, K. J. (2006). Team mental models and team performance: A field

study of the effects of team mental model similarity and accuracy. Journal of

Organizational Behaviour, 27(4), 403-418.

Page 261: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

248

Lincoln, Y., & Guba, E. (1985). Naturalistic Inquiry. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE.

Littleton, K., & Light, P. (1999). Learning with computers: Analysing productive

interaction. London: Routledge.

Malouff, J. (2006). Fifty problem solving strategies explained.

Retrieved November 7, 2006 from:

http://www.une.edu.au/psychology/staff/malouffproblemsolving.php.

Mason, R. (1991). Evaluation methodologies for computer conferencing applications.

In A. Kaye. Collaborative Learning Through Computer Conferencing: The

Najaden Papers (pp. 90-105). New York: Springer-Verlag.

Mathieu, J. E., Goodwin, G. F., Heffner, T. S., Salas, E., & Cannon-Bowers, J. A.

(2000). The influence of shared mental models on team process and

performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(2), 273-283.

Mathieu, J. E, Heffner, T. S, Goodwin, G., Cannon-Bowers, J. A., & Salas, E. (2005).

Scaling the quality of teammates’ mental models: Equifinality and normative

comparisons. Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 26(1), 37-56.

Maykut, P., & Morehouse, R. (1994). Beginning qualitative research: A philosophic

and practical guide. Philadelphia, PA: Falmer Press.

McIntyre, R. M., & Salas, E. (1995). Measuring and managing for team performance:

Emerging principles from complex environments. In R. A. Guzzo & E. Salas

(Eds.), Team effectiveness and decision making in organizations (pp. 9-45).

San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

McNeese, M. (2000). Socio-cognitive factors in the acquisition and transfer of

knowledge. Cognition, Technology & Work, 2(3), 164-177.

Page 262: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

249

McWhaw, K., Schnackenberg, H., Sclater, J., & Abrami, P. C. (2003). From co-

operation to collaboration: Helping students become collaborative learners. In

R. M. Gillies & A. F. Ashman (Eds.), Co-operative learning: The social and

intellectual outcomes of learning in groups. (pp. 69-86). London:

RoutledgeFalmer.

Mennecke, B. E., Hoffer, J. A., & Wynee, B. E. (1992). The implications of group

development and history for group support system theory and practice. Small

Group Research, 23(4), 524-572.

Mevarech, Z., Silber, O., & Fine, D. (1991). Learning with computers in small

groups: Cognitive and affective outcomes. Journal of Educational Computing

Research, 7(2), 233-243.

Miles, M., & Huberman, A. (1994). Qualitative data analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

Miller, D. (1991). Handbook of research design and social measurement. Newbury

Park, CA: Sage.

Mohammed, S., & Dumville, B. (2001). Team mental models in a team knowledge

framework: Expanding theory and measurement across disciplinary

boundaries. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22(2), 189-106.

Mohammed, S., Klimoski, R., & Rentsch, J. R. (2000). The measurement of team

mental models: We have no shared schema. Organizational Research

Methods, 3(2), 123-165.

Page 263: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

250

Morse, J. (1998). Designing funded qualitative research. In N. Denzin & Y. Lincoln

(Eds.), Strategies of qualitative inquiry (pp. 56-85). Thousand Oaks, CA:

Sage.

Mueller, A., & Fleming, T. (1994). Cooperative learning: Listening to how children

work at school. Journal of Educational Research, 94(5), 259-265.

Murphey, T., & Jacobs, G. (2000). Encouraging critical collaborative autonomy.

JALT Journal, 22(2), 228-244.

Nason, R.A., Brett, C., & Woodruff, E. (1996). Creating and maintaining knowledge-

building communities of practice during mathematical investigations. In P.

Clarkson (Ed.), Technology in mathematics education (pp. 20-29). Melbourne,

Australia: Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia.

Nason, R., & Woodruff, E. (2003). Fostering authentic, sustained, and progressive

mathematical knowledge-building activity in computer supported

collaborative learning (CSCL) communities. Journal of Computers in

Mathematics and Science Teaching, 22(4), 345-363.

Neufeld, N., & Haggerty, N. (2001). Collaborative team learning in information

systems: A pedagogy for developing team skills and high performance. The

Journal of Computer Information Systems, 42(1), 37-43.

Ngeow, K. (1998). Enhancing student thinking through collaborative learning.

ERICDigest. Eric Reproduction Services No. ED422586. Retrieved February

1, 2005 from: http://www.ericdigests.org/1999-2/learning.htm.

Page 264: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

251

Nickerson, R. (1994). The teaching of thinking and problem solving. In R. Sternberg

(Ed.), Thinking and problem solving (pp. 409-449). San Diego, CA: Academic

Press.

O’Donnell, A. M. (2004). A commentary on design research. Educational

Psychologist, 39(4), 255-260.

Ogden, L. (2000). Collaborative tasks, collaborative children: An analysis of

reciprocity during peer interaction at Key Stage 1. British Educational

Research Journal, 26(2), 211-226.

O'Malley, C. (Ed.). (1995). Computer supported collaborative learning. Berlin, GDR:

Springer.

O’Neil, H. F., Chuang, S., & Chung, G. K. (2003). Issues in the computer-based

assessment of collaborative problem solving. Assessment in Education, 10(3),

361-373.

Oxford, R. (1997). Cooperative learning, collaborative learning, and interaction:

Three communicative strands in the language classroom. Modern Language

Journal, 81(4), 443-456.

Palincsar, A. S. (1986). Reciprocal teaching. In A. S. Palincsar, D. S. Ogle, & B. F.

Jones (Eds.), Teaching reading as thinking: Specific research-based strategies

for teaching reading comprehension (pp. 5-7). Alexandria, VA: Association of

Supervision and Curriculum Development.

Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A.L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-

fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognitive Instruction,

1(2), 117-175.

Page 265: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

252

Palloff, R., & Pratt, K. (2005). Collaborating online: Learning together in community.

San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.

Poole, M., & Zhang, H. (2005). Virtual teams. In S. Wheelan (Ed.), The handbook of

group research and practice (pp. 363-384). Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.

Popper, K. R. (1979). Objective knowledge: An evolutionary approach. Oxford:

Clarendon Press.

Pugalee, D. (2001). Writing, mathematics, and metacognition: Looking for

connections through students' work in mathematical problem solving. School

Science and Mathematics, 101(5), 236-246.

Puntambekar, S. (1999). An integrated approach to individual and collaborative

learning in a web-based learning environment. Computer Support for

Collaborative Learning (CSCL) 1999 Conference, Stanford University, Palo

Alto, California, Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Puntambekar, S. (2006). Analyzing collaborative interactions: Divergence, shared

understanding and construction of knowledge. Computers & Education, 47(3),

332–351.

Queensland Studies Authority. (2004). Mathematics: Years 1 to 10 syllabus. Brisbane,

Queensland, Australia: Queensland Studies Authority.

Reid, J., Forrestal, P., & Cook, J. (1989) Small group learning in the classroom.

Scarborough, WA: Chalkface Press.

Rentsch, J., &. Klimoski, R. (2001). Why do 'great minds' think alike? Antecedents of

team member schema agreement. Journal of Organizational Behaviour, 22(2),

107-120.

Page 266: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

253

Rentsch, J., & Zelno, J. (2003). The role of cognition in managing conflict to

maximise team effectiveness: A team member schema similarity approach. In

M. A. West, D. Tjosvold & K. G. Smith (Eds.), International handbook of

organizational teamwork and cooperative working (pp. 131-150). West

Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons.

Reyna, V., Brainerd, C., Effken, J., Bootzin, R., & Lloyd, F. (2001). Human-computer

mismatches: A framework for assessment. In C. Wolfe (Ed.). Learning and

teaching on the world wide web (pp. 23-44). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Romberg, T. A. (1992). Perspectives on scholarship and research methods. In D. A.

Grouws (Ed.), Handbook on research on mathematics teaching and learning

(pp. 49-64). New York: MacMillan.

Ryan, G., & Bernard, H. (2003). Data management and analysis methods. In N.

Denzin & Y. Lincoln (Eds.), Collecting and interpreting qualitative materials

(pp. 259-309). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Salas, E., & Fiore, S. (Eds.). (2004). Team cognition: Understanding the factors that

drive process and performance. Washington, WA: American Psychological

Association.

Samovar, L., Henman, L., & King, S. (1996). Small group process. In R. Cathcart, L.

Samovar & L. Henman (Eds.), Small group communication: Theory and

practice (pp. 7-11). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

Scardamalia, M. (2002). Collective cognitive responsibility for the advancement of

knowledge. In B. Smith (Ed.), Liberal education in a knowledge society (pp.

67-98). Chicago, IL: Open Court.

Page 267: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

254

Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (1994). Computer support for knowledge-building

communities. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 3(3), 265-283.

Scardamalia, M., & Bereiter, C. (2006). Knowledge building: Theory, pedagogy and

technology. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning

sciences (pp. 97-115). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Schoenfeld, A. (1983). Beyond the purely cognitive: Belief systems, social

cognitions, and metacognitions as driving forces in intellectual performance.

Cognitive Science, 7(4), 329-363.

Schoenfeld, A. (1987). What's all the fuss about metacognition? In A. Schoenfeld

(Ed.) Cognitive science and mathematics education, (pp. 189-215). Hillsdale,

NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Schoenfeld, A. (1992). Learning to think mathematically: Problem solving,

metacognition, and sense-making in mathematics. In D. Grouws (Ed.),

Handbook for Research on Mathematics (pp. 334-370). New York:

Macmillan.

Schraw, G. (2001). Promoting general metacognitive awareness. In H. Hartman (Ed.),

Metacognition in learning and instruction: Theory, research and practice (pp.

33-68). Dordrecht, The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Schunk, D., & Zimmerman, B. (1994) (Eds.). Self-regulation of learning and

performance: Issues and educational applications. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence

Erlbaum.

Page 268: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

255

Schwartz, N. H., Andersen, C., Hong, N., Howard, B., & McGee, S. (2004). The

influence of metacognitive skills on learners' memory of information in a

hypermedia environment. Educational computing research, 31(1), 77-93.

Seymour, S. (1994). Operative computer learning with cooperative task and reward

structures. Journal of Technology Education, 5(2). Retrieved February, 2004

from http://scholar.lib.vt.edu/ejournals/JTE/v5n2/seymour.jte-v5n2.html.

Shah, P. P., Dirks, K. T., & Chervany, N. (2006). The multiple pathways of high

performing groups: The interaction of social networks and group processes.

Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27(3), 299-317.

Shavelson, R., Phillips, D., Towne, L., & Feuer, M. (2003). On the science of

education design studies. Educational Researcher, 32(1), 25-28.

Shaw, M. (1981). Group dynamics: The psychology of small group behaviour. New

York: McGraw-Hill.

Sherman, L. (2001). Cooperative learning and computer supported intentional

learning experiences. In C. Wolfe (Ed.), Learning and teaching on the world

wide web (pp. 113-130). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

Silverman, D. (2001). Interpreting qualitative data: Methods for analysing talk, text,

and interaction. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Sims, D., Salas, E., & Burke, C. (2005). Promoting effective team performance

through team training. In S. Wheelan (Ed.), The handbook of group research

and practice (pp. 407-425). Thousands Oaks, CA: Sage.

Slavin, R. (1995). Co-operative learning (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

Page 269: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

256

Smith-Jentsch, K., Campbell, G., & Milanovich, D. (2001). Measuring teamwork

mental models to support training needs assessment, development and

evaluation: Two empirical studies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 22(2),

179-190.

Soller, A. (2001). Supporting social interaction in an intelligent collaborative learning

system. International Journal of Artificial Intelligence in Education, 12(1),

40-62.

Sperling, R. A., Howard, B. C., Miller, L. A., & Murphy, C. (2002). Measures of

children's knowledge and regulation of cognition. Contemporary Educational

Psychology, 27(1), 51-79.

Stahl, G. (2006). Group cognition: Computer support for building collaborative

knowledge. Cambridge, MA: The MIT press.

Stahl, G., Koschmann, T., & Suthers, D. D. (2006). Computer supported collaborative

learning. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.), The Cambridge handbook of the learning

sciences (pp. 409-425). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Stake, R. (1995). The art of case study research. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Stein, D. (1998). Situated learning in adult education. Educational Resource

Information Center Digest No. 195. ERIC NO: ED418250.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1990). Basics of qualitative research: Grounded theory

procedures and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Strauss, A., & Corbin, J. (1998). Grounded theory methodology. In N. Denzin & Y.

Lincoln (Eds.), Strategies of Qualitative Inquiry (pp. 158-183). Thousand

Oaks, CA: Sage.

Page 270: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

257

Teasley, S., & Roschelle, J. (1993). Constructing a joint problem space: The computer

as a tool for sharing knowledge. In S. P. Lajoie & S. J. Derry (Eds.),

Computers as cognitive tools (pp. 229-258). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

Tellis, W. (1997). Application of a case study methodology. The Qualitative Report.

3(3). Retrieved February 1, 2004 from http://www.nova.edu/ssss/QR/QR3-

3/tellis2.html.

Thomas, G. (1992). Effective classroom teamwork: Support or intrusion? London:

Routledge.

Tindale, R. S., Kameda, T., & Hinsz, V. B. (2003). Group decision making. In M. A.

Hogg & J. Cooper (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of social psychology (pp. 381-

403). London: Sage.

Tjosvold, D., West, M. A., & Smith, K. G. (2003). Teamwork and cooperation:

Fundamentals of organizational effectiveness. In M. A. West, D. Tjosvold &

K. G. Smith (Eds.), International handbook of organizational teamwork and

cooperative working (pp. 3-8). West Sussex, England: John Wiley & Sons.

Tombari, M., & Borich, G. (1999). Authentic assessment in the classroom:

Applications and practice. New Jersey, NJ, Prentice-Hall.

Tubbs, S. (1995). A systems approach to small group interaction. New York:

McGraw-Hill.

Tuckman, B.W., & Jensen, M.A. (1977). Stages of small group development

revisited. Group & Organization Studies 2(4), 419-427.

Tyson, T. (1989). Working with groups. Melbourne, Victoria, Australia: MacMillian.

Page 271: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

258

Underwood, J., & Underwood, G. (1999). Task effects on co-operative and

collaborative learning with computers. In K. Littleton and P. Light (Eds.),

Learning with computers: Analysing productive interaction (pp. 10-23).

London: Routledge.

Vaughn, S., Schumm, J., & Sinagub, J. (1996). Focus group interviews in education

and psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Vygotsky, L. (1978). Mind in society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Walker, R. (1985). Doing research: A handbook for teachers. London: Routledge.

Wang, X., Hinn, D., Kanfer, A. (2001). Potential of computer-supported collaborative

learning for learners with different learning styles. Journal of Research on

Technology in Education, 34(1), 75-85.

Ward, G., & Morris, R. (2005). Introduction to the psychology of planning. In R.

Morris & G. Ward (Eds.), The cognitive psychology of planning (pp. 1-34).

New York: Psychology Press.

Webber, S. S., Chen, G., Payne, S. C., Marsh, S. M., & Zaccaro, S. J. (2000).

Enhancing team mental model measurement with performance appraisal

practices. Organizational Research Methods, 4(4), 307-322.

West, M. (2004). Effective teamwork: Practical lessons from organisational research.

Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Wheelan, S. A. (1990). Facilitating training groups: A guide to leadership and verbal

intervention skills. New York: Praeger.

Wheelan, S. (2005). Group process: A developmental perspective (2nd ed.). Boston:

Allyn and Bacon.

Page 272: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

259

Whicker, K., Nunnery, J., & Bol, L. (1997). Cooperative learning in the secondary

mathematics classroom. The Journal of Educational Research, 91(1), 42-48.

White, B., & Frederiksen, J. (2005). A theoretical framework and approach for

fostering metacognitive development. Educational Psychologist, 40(4), 211–

223.

Wilson, J., & Johnson, P. (2000). Students thinking about their learning: Assessment

to improve learning. Educational Research Quarterly, 24(2), 10-20.

Woehr, D., & Rentsch, J. (2003). Elaborating team member schema similarity: A

social relations modeling approach. 18th annual meeting of the Society for

Industrial/Organizational Psychology, Orlando, FL.

Woodruff, E., & Nirula, L. (2005). Design research in the elementary school

classroom. In C. Howard, J. Boettcher, L. Justice, & K. Schenk (Eds.),

Encyclopedia of Distance Learning (V2) (pp. 510-517). Hershey, PA.: Idea

Group.

Xiaodong, L. (2001). Designing metacognitive activities. Educational Technology,

Research and Development, 49(2), 23-40.

Yackel, E., Cobb, P., & Wood, T. (1991). Small-group interactions as a source of

learning opportunities in second-grade mathematics. Journal for Research in

Mathematics Education, 22(5), 390-408.

Yin, R. K. (1993). Applications of case study research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage.

Yin, R. K, (1994). Case study research, design and methods, (2nd ed.) Newbury Park,

CA: Sage.

Page 273: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

260

Yin, R. K, (2003). Case study research, design and methods, (3rd ed.) Newbury Park,

CA: Sage.

Zawojewski, J., Lesh, R., & English, L. (2003). A models and modeling perspective

on the role of small group learning activities. In R. Lesh & H. Doerr (Eds.),

Beyond Constructivism: A models and modeling perspective on mathematics

problem solving, learning, and teaching (pp. 337-358): Mahwah, NJ:

Lawrence Erlbaum.

Page 274: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

261

APPENDICES

APPENDIX A: Checklist to observe group behaviour

Group Members Group Skills

Checks group understanding

Gives ideas

Shares information

Talks about the work

Gets group back to work

Repeats what has been said

Asks questions

Encourages

Checks for agreement

Encourages others to talk

Responds to ideas

Uses eye contact

Says ‘Thank you’

Shares feelings

Disagrees in a nice way

Keeps things calm

Total

Page 275: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

262

APPENDIX B: Metacognitive questionnaire

NO –No, I didn’t do this. MAYBE-I may have done this. YES-Yes, I did do this. NO MAYBE YES BEFORE YOU BEGAN TO SOLVE THE PROBLEM-WHAT DID YOU DO?

1. I read the problem more than once. 2. I thought to myself, do I understand what the

problem is asking me?

3. I tried to put the problem into my own words. 4. I tried to remember if I had worked a problem like

this before.

5. I thought about what information I needed to solve this problem.

6. I asked myself, is there information in this problem that I don’t need?

AS YOU WORKED ON THE PROBLEM-WHAT DID YOU DO?

7. I thought about the steps as I worked the problem. 8. I kept looking back at the problem after I did a step. 9. I had to stop and rethink a step I had already done. 10. I checked my work step by step as I worked the

problem.

11. I did something wrong and had to redo my step(s). AFTER YOU FINISHED WORKING THE PROBLEM-WHAT DID YOU DO?

12. I looked back to see if I did the correct procedures. 13. I checked to see if my calculations were correct. 14. I went back and checked my work again. 15. I looked back at the problem to see if my answer

made sense.

16. I thought about a different way to solve the problem.

DID YOU USE ANY OF THESE WAYS OF WORKING?

17. I drew a picture to help me understand the problem. 18. I guessed and checked. 19. I picked out the operations I needed to do this

problem.

20. I felt confused and could not decide what to do. 21. I wrote down important information.

Fortunato et al., (1991)

Page 276: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

263

APPENDIX C: Self regulatory checklist

Planning 1. What is the nature of the task?

2. What is my goal?

3. What kind of information and strategies will I need?

4. How much time and resources will I need?

Monitoring 1. Do I have a clear understanding of what I am doing?

2. Does the task make sense?

3. Am I reaching my goals?

4. Do I need to make changes?

Evaluation 1. Have I reached my goal?

2. What worked?

3. What didn't work?

4. Would I do things differently next time?

Schraw (2001)

Page 277: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

264

APPENDIX D: Interview questions

Initial questions Intermediate questions Ending Questions

1. Tell me about what happened 2. When, if at all, did you first experience/notice…? 3. (If so,) What was it like? What did you think then? How did you happen to? Who, if anyone, influenced your actions? Tell me about how he/she or they influenced you. 4. Could you describe the events that led up to … 5. What contributed to..? 6. How would you describe how you viewed…before… happened? How, if at all, has your view of … changed? 7. How would you describe the person you were then?

1. What, if anything did you know about...? 2. Tell me about your thoughts and feelings when you learned about… 3. What happened next? 4. Who, if anyone, was involved? When was that? How were they involved? 5, Tell me about how you learned to handle… 6. How, if at all, have your thoughts and feelings about … changed since? 7. What positive changes have occurred since…? 9. Tell me how you go about… What do you do? 10. Could you describe the most important lessons you learned about … through experiencing…? 11. What helps you to manage…? What problems might you encounter? Tell me the sources of these problems. 12. Who has been most helpful to you during this time? How has he/she been helpful?

1. What do you think are the most important ways to…? How did you discover (or create) them? How has your experience before affected how you handled…? 2. Tell me about how your views may have changed since you have…? 3. Tell me about your strengths that you discovered or developed through… 4. After having these experiences, what advice would you give to someone who has just discovered that he or she…? 5. Is there anything that you might not have thought about before that occurred to you during this interview? 6. Is there anything you would like to ask me?

Charmaz (2003)

Page 278: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

265

APPENDIX E: Initial individual questionnaire

1. What is a group?

2. What do you like about working in a group?

3. What do you not like about working in a group?

4. Where did you learn to work with other people?

5. What advice would you give to someone who has just discovered that they will be working in a group?

Page 279: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

266

APPENDIX F: Final individual questionnaire

1. What did you like about the group work?

2. What did you not like about the group work?

3. What do you feel was easier to understand or learn in your group?

4. What do you feel would have been easier to understand or learn on your own?

5. What would you change about the group?

6. What advice would you give to someone who has just discovered that they will be working in a group?

Page 280: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

267

APPENDIX G: Group cohesiveness questionnaire

NAME

Strongly Agree Agree Neutral Disagree

Strongly Disagree

I'm glad I belong to this group I feel held back be this group I am an important part of this group I don't fit in with other kids in this group I feel strongly tied to this group I don't think the group is that important I think this group worked well together I don't feel comfortable with the other kids in this group

Page 281: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

268

APPENDIX H: Lesson plan

Learning outcomes

Data CD 3.2 Students design and trial a variety of data collection methods and use existing sources of data to investigate their own and others’ questions, organise data and create suitable displays, identifying and interpreting elements of the displays.

CD 4.2 Students plan and carry out data collections using their own data record templates; choose and construct appropriate displays and make comparisons about the data based on the displays and measures of location.

CD 5.2 Students plan investigations involving discrete and continuous data, produce and compare data displays involving grouping, and compare measures of location.

Investigation Last week, (5 October, 2005) the London-based Economic Intelligence Unit released their

ranking of the top cities to live. Melbourne was the top Australian city, then, Sydney, Perth –

Brisbane was rated number 11. However, the Economic Intelligence Unit didn’t include

categories that children may think are important.

Students need to decide what information is important in ranking cities in Australia according

to the liveability and how they can collect and present the information. Throughout this

activity students will be working in cooperative groups of 3 students. Using the Internet, they

will learn gather data about Australian cities. To organise the data, the students will use

EXCEL. The groups will use the Knowledge Forum database to share information as well as

discuss and revise their solution. They will also give feedback to other groups on the online

database. The final solution will be presented a word document –Newspaper format.

Page 282: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

269

Identifying and describing

Overview of activity Students: discuss the requirements of investigation identify the data to collect determine how data will be collected design data collection template Level Three Level Four Level Five Students: know that data collection is

used to investigate questions design data collection

methods to investigate questions

create suitable data displays know the elements of data

displays interpret data displays using

elements of displays.

Students: know data collection is

planned to investigate particular situations

plan and carry out data collections

design data record templates compare data based on

displays

Students: know data collection is

planned to investigate particular situations

plan investigations compare grouped data

Understanding and applying

Students: carry out data collections using

data record templates record data construct data display using

excel

Communicating and justifying

Students:

generate, discuss the data, and compile a report

report findings to the class

Thinking, reasoning and

working mathematically

Page 283: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

270

Lesson sequence Anticipated evidence

Introducing the investigation Students discuss the requirements of the investigation

and determine that data will need to be collected. Students form groups. Groups brainstorm the types of data they need to fill

their categories: - the climate –temperature, rainfall,

humidity - recreational activities of the students –

parks, sports, scouts, guides, holiday clubs.

- tourist attractions in the local area –Beaches, fun parks.

Students will:

decide what data they will need to collect (they may decide to collect data about recreational activities and the temperature)

determine how they will collect the data. Will it be from existing data sources, or will they need to collect the data themselves?

identify categories for data, and design data record templates.

know data collection is planned to investigate particular situations (Level 3/4/5) plan and carry out data collections design data record templates

Page 284: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

271

Lesson sequence Anticipated evidence

Exploring data Students carry out data collections using data record

templates. Students will: enter data onto a spreadsheet and compare and analyse

data construct data displays using the data they have

gathered and the ways it has been classified.

compare data based on displays know ways of displaying data for comparison compare grouped data

Lesson sequence Anticipated evidence

Making judgments using data using the data that has been collected throughout the

course of the activity – students interpret data and compile a report.

students use data collected to inform about most liveable city.

the report to will be presented for editing, and will be presented to the class.

compare data based on displays (Level 4)

Page 285: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

272

Contributions to the valued attributes of a lifelong learner Through engagement with activities in this module, students develop the following attributes:

Knowledgeable person with deep understanding understands the purpose for collecting data makes judgements on data collected Complex thinker analyses and organises information Active investigator uses data collection to investigate questions accesses information from a variety of sources Effective communicator presents data collections to others uses data as a means of communicating information Participant in an interdependent world works in groups and acknowledges the ideas of others Reflective and self directed learner looks for and recognises ways of “working mathematically” in everyday life

Procedure:

Day 1

Introduction Talk about research and what I do. Introduce the idea of working in groups or teams. Introduce Rainbow Book with group roles and skills. Group roles –how they will change weekly –not always on the computer-everyone should get a chance to do each role –one person today can be the recorder. Go through skills, strategies, problem-solving, and group skills from book. Discuss what other strategies/skills students have used on problems. Present problem Read Newspaper article Sun/Herald. Introduce CNN site and their ranking system Students will brainstorm –categories can use –Write up a few ideas on the board. Brainstorm in groups. Write all ideas down –on red pages in Rainbow book. Introduce Planning sheet in Rainbow Book –on orange pages in Rainbow book. In group work out who will be doing each role (remember they will be changed next week). Choose a recorder and help the recorder complete planning sheet in the rainbow book. Come up with team name and password. Write in Rainbow book. Collect books. Discuss other categories groups thought were important and why –asking the encourager/checker. Ext: Demonstrate how to set up Excel to organise data –Cities, ranking. Ext: Demonstrate Knowledge Forum.

Page 286: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

273

Day 2

Distribute rainbow books and remind students to change group roles next time on computer and to record in book so that no arguments about who has next turn. Introduce Knowledge Forum -Written guidelines for using Knowledge Forum provided for the students to refer to. Demonstrate how to use Excel to organise data –Cities, ranking –need to decide what ranking you give each category eg. Out of 10 –Lots of parks 10, no parks 0 -written guidelines for using EXCEL will be provided for the students to refer to. Model how to evaluate some sites with students. Discussion will include the validity of websites and the need to find information by using appropriate websites to find data. Students will then review at least three sites – demonstrate how to gather data and complete information sheet Web Resources The students will select those websites from this list which are relevant to their project: (This will depend on students’ rankings from day 1). Groups also need to read other groups Knowledge Forum notes and write back to welcome note. Complete yellow monitoring sheet in rainbow book –how well group working together, what needs to change?

Day 3 & 4

Complete ranking system and review and model how to import Excel onto Knowledge Forum –also show how to add comments to Excel sheet in order to justify reasons why cities are ranked accordingly.

Model Feedback –Praise, comment, suggestion.

Day 5 & 6

Read Knowledge Forum note from groups and finalise overall ranking system.

What is the best city in Australia to live? Explain your system of ranking the cities, why it is a good system and how your ranking system could be used to rank other cities in the world in terms of liveability.

Follow-up

In discussion and writing, students will reflect on project in Rainbow Book -Complete group evaluation and reflection. Complete final individual questionnaires and group metacognitive questionnaire.

Page 287: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

274

APPENDIX I: City information

Adelaide Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 1,100,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 21.4 °c

Minimum average temperature 11.2 °c

Average rainfall per year 450 mm

Crime Murder per 100,000 persons 5.9

Economy Major industries Manufacturing, refining

Cost of living Not rated

Attractions

Natural

Kilometers of beaches, Lots of parks City is surrounded by parkland

Tourist Coastal beaches and the Mount Lofty Ranges

Stadiums

AAMI -51,515 (sc), Entertainment Centre- 11,000 (sc), Adelaide Oval- 33,597 (sc), Prospect Oval- 20,000 (sc)

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 288: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

275

Brisbane Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 1,627,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 25.4°c

Minimum average temperature 15.75°c

Average rainfall per year 1152

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 5.1

Economy Major industries Mining, tourism, agriculture

Cost of living 84/100

Attractions

Natural Morteon Bay, islands, parks

Tourist Southbank Gold Coast in the south and the Sunshine Coast in the north

Stadiums

Ballymore -24,000 (sc), Entertainment Centre – 13,500 (sc), Exhibition ground -25,490, Gabba -37,600, QE11 Stadium -49,000 (sc), Suncorp Stadium -52,579 (sc)

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 289: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

276

Canberra Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 304,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 19.5°c

Minimum average temperature 6.5°c

Average rainfall per year 630 mm

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 1.9

Economy

Major industries Federal government, public administration, manufacturing, education, tourism, IT

Cost of living 45/100 (45th out of the 100 most expensive cities to live).

Attractions

Natural No beaches, Lots of parks.

Tourist Parliament House, War Memorial, National Gallery, Old Parliament House.

Stadiums AIS -5050 seating capacity, Canberra Stadium -24,647 (sc), Manuka Oval -15,000.

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 290: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

277

Darwin Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 80,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 32.5 °c

Minimum average temperature 23.6 °c

Average rainfall per year 1570 mm

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 8.5

Economy Major industries Rural, hospitality, tourism

Cost of living Not rated

Attractions

Natural Harbor twice the size of Sydney 20 parks and nature reserves

Tourist

Stadiums Marrara Hockey -10,000 ( sc), Marrara Stadium - !5, 000 (sc)

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 291: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

278

Hobart Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 193,500

Climate

Maximum average temperature 16.7 °c

Minimum average temperature 8.2 °c

Average rainfall per year 626 mm

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 3.7

Economy

Major industries

Tourism, meatpacking, food processing, and the making of textiles, chemicals, and glass

Cost of living 99/100

Attractions

Natural

Tourist

Stadiums

Bellerive Oval- 16,000 (sc), North Hobart-18,000, Queenborough Oval-8,000 (sc), TCA-8,000 (sc)

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 292: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

279

Melbourne Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 3,200,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 19.8 °c

Minimum average temperature 10. °c

Average rainfall per year 660 mm

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 3.1

Economy

Major industries

Tourism, mining, food processing, chemicals, steel, industrial and transport

Cost of living 68/100

Attractions

Natural

Melbourne is on the bank of the Yarra River and is five kilometres from Phillip Bay

Tourist Trams,

Stadiums

Raceway- 44,000(sc), McHale Stadium- 27,000, Melbourne Cricket Ground- 100,000, Optus Oval- 35,000, Telstra Dome- 56,347

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 293: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

280

Perth Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population 1,223,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 23.6 °c

Minimum average temperature 13.3 °c

Average rainfall per year 869 mm

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 3.8

Economy Major industries

Tourism, agriculture, mining, horticulture

Cost of living 93/100

Attractions

Natural Fabulous beaches, National Parks

Tourist

Stadiums

East Freemantle Oval 25,00 (sc), Perry Lakes Stadium- 30,000 (sc), Freemantle Oval- 17,500(sc), Entertainment Centre- 8,200 (sc), Subiaco Oval- 42,922 (sc)

Cost of living rating

Rating of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 294: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

281

Sydney Categories Indicators Information Rating

Population Number of people 4,600,000

Climate

Maximum average temperature 21.6 °c

Minimum average temperature 13.5 °c

Average rainfall per year 1226 mm

Crime murder per 100,000 persons 3.4

Economy Major industries

Agriculture, manufacturing, mining

Cost of living 20/100

Attractions

Natural National Parks, Sydney Harbor

Tourist Botanic Gardens Harbor Bridge, Opera House.

Stadiums

Aussie Stadium- 40,792 (sc), Hensen Park- 30,000 (sc), Sydney Cricket Ground- 44,000, Entertainment Centre- 10,500, Showground -21,000, Telstra Stadium- 83,500

Cost of living rating

Rating out of 100 most expensive cities to live in the world.

Stadiums (sc)-seating capacity.

Page 295: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

282

APPENDIX J: Newspaper article

Melbourne rated only the second best Jane Metlikovec 05oct05

MELBOURNE is still the best city in Australia, but has lost its world title after two years at the top. Vancouver has pipped Melbourne as the world's most liveable city by only 1 per cent, according to an annual survey of 127 cities. The London-based Economic Intelligence Unit based its rankings on stability, health care, culture and environment, education and infrastructure.

A low threat of terrorism and good health and education systems saw Melbourne rank

second to Vancouver, beating rival Sydney, which polled equal fifth with Adelaide and Perth.

Brisbane was the only major Australian city to miss out on the Top 10, coming eleventh.

Editor of the survey Jon Copestake said it was not surprising Western cities dominated the top rankings.

"In the current global political climate, it is no surprise that the most desirable destinations are those with a lower perceived threat of terrorism," Mr Copestake said.

Port Moresby, the crime-ridden capital of Papua New Guinea, ranked as the worst city in the world for its high level of corruption, crime rates and low availability of entertainment.

Metlikovec, Jane, Melbourne rated only the second best. Herald and Weekly Times, http://www.heraldsun.news.com.au/common/story_page/0,5478,16817534%255E2862,00.html Accessed: 7/10/05.

Page 296: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

283

APPENDIX K: Group roles, skills, and problem-solving strategies

Group skills Task (What) Team (How)

Check group

understanding. Give ideas. Share information. Talk about the work. Get group back to work. Repeat what has been

said. Ask questions.

Encourage. Check for agreement. Encourage others to talk Respond to ideas. Use eye contact. Say ‘Thank you’. Share feelings. Disagree in a nice way. Keep things calm.

Group roles

Task roles Team roles

Keyboarder/Checker - seek opinions and information, summarise ideas.

Coordinator/Recorder - organise, give information and opinions, elaborate on ideas, follow group plan.

Encourager/Moderator - encourage participation, manage conflict, encourage harmonious discussion, support decisions, spokesperson for group.

Problem-solving strategies

Strategy How

Draw Solve by drawing model/diagram.

Small Simplify the problem using small numbers.

Parts Solve part(s) of the problem first.

Before List information given before and after the action; compare the information to the unknown.

Backwards Solve by working backwards.

Page 297: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

284

APPENDIX L: Group diary checklists

PLANNING What do we know about the problem?

(What information is needed?)

What is the goal?

What is our plan to solve the problem and reach the goal? (What strategies can be used?)

What group roles will we use? Name: Group role: Name: Group role: Name: Group role:

What group skills will we use?

Task (What) Team (How)

Page 298: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

285

MONITORING

Are we following the group plan?

Do we need to make changes?

What group roles will we use? Name: Group role: Name: Group role: Name: Group role:

What group skills will we use? Task (What) Team (How)

Looks Like Sounds Like Looks Like

Sounds Like

What are 2 things your group is doing well and 1 thing that needs

to improve? (Eg. contributing ideas, encouraging participation, checking for understanding, and keeping things calm) The group is doing well …

The group is doing well …

The group is doing well …

The group needs to improve …

Page 299: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

286

MONITORING 2 Group Roles

Group Role Name

Keyboarder/Checker

Coordinator/Recorder

Encourager/Moderator

Group Skills

Task Team

Best City

What group skills will we use on Knowledge Forum? (Eg. give ideas, share information, encourage participation, check for agreement, and respond to ideas.)

Category:

City Ranking

Adelaide

Brisbane

Canberra

Darwin

Hobart

Melbourne

Perth

Sydney

We ranked this city as number 1 because:

Page 300: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

287

EVALUATING

Have we reached our goal?

What worked?

What didn't work?

What would we do differently next time?

How can we improve: o The Rainbow book?

o Knowledge Forum?

Page 301: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

288

APPENDIX M: Final overall ranking system: Cycle 1

Page 302: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

289

Page 303: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

290

APPENDIX N: T-chart

Group skill

Looks Like

Sounds Like

Page 304: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

291

APPENDIX O: Knowledge Forum guide

Sign on to Knowledge Forum

-Select the PAKSTEM database.

-Enter team User Name

-Enter team Password

-Click Sign On

Build-on notes

-Click on the note you want to build on.

-Click the button.

-A new note will open.

-Type your new note.

-Click on Close and Contribute.

(If you just click Close the note will not be added).

Page 305: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

292

Create a new note -Click the new note button. -Maximise note window. Title the note

-Type a heading for your note. Adding a scaffold

-Select a scaffold. -Click on the Add button. Adding ideas

-Place the cursor in between the > < signs. -Type your note. Contribute ideas -Click the Close and Contribute button.

Page 306: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

293

APPENDIX P: Group categories

Group Group diaries Knowledge Forum A clean ivienment (environment) nice

parks good egecashon (education) Clean environment, nice parks, and food education, health.

B skateboarding and chilling out, laser skirmish

Skate boarding and chilling out laser skirmish

C draw a chart part Education, environment and pollution. D Theme parks, sports, public health

system, infastructure. Sport, public health system and education, Facilities

E Get information -Use to find top 8 -Which is best

Shopping, showgrounds, food, transport, main attractions, theme parks, education, sports, health, facilities

F health, neighbours, pets, egication (education), comunity, shopping, beauty, weather, parks, goverment, food, water.

Shopping beauty neighbours food water parks school community pets health weather and government

G Shopping, movies, theme parks, foodcourt, park, sports

Shopping movies theme parks food court park sports and computers

H The population, environment, wild life, food, theame parks, buildings, edgucation.

Population, environment, wild life, food and drinks, theme parks, buildings and education.

I Sports, education, health, wildlife, foods and drinks

J Adresher (Adventure) parks, shops. Adventure parks, shops to rank K Sport, trees, shop, rivers, school,

buildings, food, culture, money, weather

Sports, population, pollution, schools, culture, buildings and shops

L 8 citys sports Sport’s M Use computer Sports, main attractions, health,

environment, energy saving cities, wildlife

N Theme parks, transport, safety, lifestyle, shopping, real estate, education, population, hospitals

Theme Parks, Transport, Safety, Lifestyle, Shopping, Real Estate, Education and Population.

O Wild life, buildings, food and drink, and population

Wildlife, buildings, food and water ,parks and population

P sport Art Showers gardens, trees, water, food.

Sports, art, gardens, food and water

Page 307: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

294

APPENDIX Q: List of categories

Adventure parks Art Beauty Community Culture Education Environment Facilities Food Food courts Gardens Government Health Hospitals Infrastructure Lifestyle Main attractions Money Movies Neighbours Parks Pets Pollution Population Public health system Rain Real estate Rivers Safety Shopping Show grounds Sports Theme parks Transport Weather Wildlife

Page 308: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

295

APPENDIX R: CD: Australia’s best city

Australia's best city Adelaide Brisbane Canberra Darwin Hobart Melbourne Perth Sydney

Our Categories

Adventure Movies

Art Pollution

Attractions Population

Beauty and Health Rain

Culture Real estate

Economy Safety

Food Shopping

Gardens and Parks Sports

Government Theme parks

Health System Transport

Hospitals Weather

Page 309: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

296

APPENDIX S: Excel guide

Entering Data Click on a cell to make it active. Type in your data and press Enter.

Selecting Data Click on a cell and drag the pointer so all data is selected.

Sorting Data

Select the columns to be sorted. Click on Data on the menu bar, and select Sort.

Sort by

Sort by column to be ranked (e.g. population) Click OK.

Writing Formulas

Click on a cell and type = + Addition -B2+C2+D2 - Subtraction; e.g. A5–B5 * Multiplication; e.g. D1*D2 / Division; e.g. B2/A3

Clicking in cell B2 displays B2 in the equation.

Using the Fill Handle

Position the pointer in lower-right corner of cell until it changes into a small +

Drag + to select cells

The formula is copied to the cells selected.

Page 310: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

297

APPENDIX T: Final CD: Cycle 1

Brisbane is Australia's best city Adelaide Brisbane Canberra Darwin Hobart Melbourne Perth Sydney

We found out the best city was........... Brisbane!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!

Our best city is Brisbane because we have found that Brisbane has the best stuff and because it has more places than other cities. We think Brisbane is the cleanest, it has a nice mix of culture, beautiful attractions, and the best theme parks!

Our Categories

Adventure Movies

Art Pollution

Attractions Population

Beauty and Health Rain

Culture Real estate

Economy Safety

Food Shopping

Gardens and Parks Sports

Government Theme parks

Health System Transport

Hospitals Weather

Our Ranking system

Links

Page 311: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

298

APPENDIX U: Bales’ Interaction Process Analysis

(IPA) Behaviours Row Total 1. Seems friendly, raises other’s status, gives help, reward

2. Dramatises, jokes, laughs, shows satisfaction

3. Agrees, shows passive acceptance, understands, concurs, complies

4. Gives suggestion, direction, implying autonomy for other

5. Gives opinion, evaluation, analysis, expresses feeling, wish

6. Gives information, repeats, clarifies, confirms

7. Asks for information, repetition, confirmation

8. Asks for opinion, evaluation, analysis, expresses feeling

9. Asks for suggestion, direction, possible ways of action

10. Disagrees, shows passive rejection, formality, withholds help

11. Shows tension, asks for help, withdraws

12. Seems unfriendly, deflates other’s status, defends or asserts self

Column Total

Page 312: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

299

APPENDIX V: Group metacognition coding

Coded Checklist Text

Planning 1

Planning 2

Planning 3

What is the nature of the task?

What is the goal?

What kinds of strategies are

needed?

Coded Checklist Text

Monitoring 1

Monitoring 2

Monitoring 3

Clear understanding of what we

are dong

Does the task make sense?

Are we reaching our goals?

Coded Checklist Text

Evaluation 1

Evaluation 2

Evaluation 3

Evaluation 4

Have we reached our goal?

What worked?

What didn’t work?

Would we do things differently

next time?

Page 313: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

300

APPENDIX W: Mathematical model for Team One

Team One (Group A, B, C).

Group A (Team One)

Page 314: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

301

Group B (Team One)

Group C (Team One)

Page 315: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

302

APPENDIX X: Posters

TEAM SKILLS POSTER

The team The work Encourage encourage

others to talk encourage

others to listen

Ideas give ideas repeat ideas respond to

ideas

Be positive say ‘thank you’ use eye contact say positive

things

Share information seek ideas search for new

information

Check for agreement manage conflict keep things

calm

Check understanding ask questions talk about the

work

Page 316: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

303

GROUP ROLES POSTER

Team roles Work roles Encourager encourage some

to talk encourage

others to listen

Keyboarder give ideas repeat ideas respond to

ideas

Manager be positive manage conflict share positive

feelings

Coordinator seek ideas search for new

information

Checker check for

agreement manage conflict keep things

calm

Recorder check for

understanding ask questions talk about the

work

Page 317: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

304

PROBLEM-SOLVING POSTER

Make a list Draw a picture

Guess and check Work backward

Make a table Choose an

operation

Find a pattern Check solution

Page 318: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

305

CONFLICT MANAGEMENT POSTER

Listen to each other

Try to understand

everyone’s point of view

Give everyone a

chance to speak

Discuss solution paths

Get help if needed

Page 319: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

306

APPENDIX Y: IPA Coding for each group

Group A

Figure Y1 shows that Group A has a higher number of task-related

communication (74.7%) than team-related communication (25.3%). The task-related

communication domain includes the categories, gives suggestion, gives opinion, gives

information, asks for information, asks for opinion, and asks for suggestion. While the

team-related domain includes the categories, seems friendly, dramatises, agrees,

disagrees, shows tension, and seems unfriendly.

24.9

57.8

16.9

0.40

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1.- 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

quen

cy (%

)

Figure Y1. IPA domain frequencies for Group A.

The IPA category frequencies for Group A are shown in Table Y1. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (38.8%), agrees (21.1%), and gives suggestion (11.8%). The category

gives suggestions is coded for behaviour or acts that include giving suggestions,

giving direction or implying autonomy for other students. The category gives

information is coded for behaviours or acts that include giving information, repeating

what has been said, and clarifying or confirming what has been said. The categories

Page 320: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

307

with the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded, are

dramatises, disagrees, and seems unfriendly.

Gives suggestions is the highest category in session 4 while the category gives

information is the highest in all the other sessions. An example of gives information

from session 1 was when groups were selecting a category to rank the major cities:

That's when you, when people are healthy and be healthy (Student 3).

The category agrees is higher in sessions 2 and 3 than the gives suggestions

category. The category agrees is coded for behaviours or acts that show agreement,

passive acceptance, understanding, concurring or compliment. An example of agrees

from session 2 was when groups were asked to place the categories they were going

to rank their cities on to the Knowledge Forum ® database:

OK (Student 1)

The behaviour of shows tension (0.4%) was coded in session 3. Behaviours in

this category include asking for help or withdrawing from the group. An example of

shows tension from session 3 was when groups were writing in their diary:

I need your help (Student 2).

Page 321: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

308

Table Y1 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group A

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 2 5 0 0 7 3

2. Dramatises 0 2 0 0 2 0.8

3. Agrees 6 10 31 3 50 21.1

4. Gives suggestion 7 8 6 7 28 11.8

5. Gives opinion 2 7 8 0 17 7.2

6. Gives information 14 38 36 4 92 38.8

7. Asks for information 5 6 4 1 16 6.8

8. Asks for opinion 5 3 5 0 13 5.5

9. Asks for suggestion 5 5 1 0 11 4.6

10. Disagrees 0 0 0 0 0 0

11. Shows tension 0 0 1 0 1 0.4

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 46 84 92 15 237 100

Page 322: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

309

Group B Figure Y2 shows that Group B has a higher number of task-related

communication (77.1%) than team-related communication (22.9%).

16.3

55.4

21.7

6.6

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y2. IPA domain frequencies for Group B.

The IPA category frequencies for Group B are shown in Table Y2. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

suggestion (27.1%), gives information (17.4%), and asks for suggestion (12.8%). The

categories with the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded, is

seems friendly.

Gives suggestion is the highest category in session 2 and 4, while gives

information is the highest category in session 3. An example of gives suggestion from

session 2 was when groups were introducing themselves to other groups on

Knowledge Forum ®:

Yeah I want to say something like I heard something about your school

I heard it is pretty good, I have (Student 1)

Page 323: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

310

An example of gives information from session 4 was when groups were

selecting a category to rank the major cities:

You can only choose one (Student 2).

The behaviour of seems unfriendly (2.7%) was coded in sessions 2 and 3.

Behaviours in this category include seeming unfriendly, deflating other’s status, and

defending or asserting oneself. An example of seems unfriendly was when groups

were developing their ranking models:

Are you doing any of the work? (Student 2).

Page 324: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

311

Table Y2

IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group B

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 2 6 2 10 3.9

3. Agrees 10 11 11 32 12.4

4. Gives suggestion 22 16 31 70 27.1

5. Gives opinion 5 16 7 28 10.9

6. Gives information 12 22 11 45 17.4

7. Asks for information 5 4 4 13 5.0

8. Asks for opinion 3 5 2 10 3.9

9. Asks for suggestion 6 7 20 33 12.8

10. Disagrees 5 3 2 10 3.9

11. Shows tension 1 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 4 3 0 5 2.7

Total 75 93 90 258 100

Page 325: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

312

Group C Figure Y3 shows that Group C has a higher number of task-related

communication (78.8%) than team-related communication (21.2%).

19.5

59.8

19.0

1.7

0.0

10.0

20.0

30.0

40.0

50.0

60.0

70.0

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y3. IPA domain frequencies for Group C.

The IPA category frequencies for Group C are shown in Table Y3. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

opinion (25.9%), gives information (19%), and gives suggestion (14.9%). Gives

opinion was coded for behaviours that include evaluation, analysis, or an expression

of feeling or a wish. The categories with the lowest frequency counts, with no

behaviours observed or coded, are seems friendly and seems unfriendly.

Gives opinion is the highest category in session 3 and 4, while gives

suggestion is the highest category in session 1 and 2. An example of gives opinion

from session 3 was when groups were developing their ranking models of the top

cities in Australia:

Page 326: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

313

I think a good city would have lots of good schools and parks, good

committees and groups, good theatres umm beautiful parks that's what

I think would make a good city (Student 2).

The behaviour of shows tension (0.6%) was coded in session 1. An example of

shows tension was when groups were sending their welcome notes to Knowledge

Forum:

Do you have anything to say? Gave no answer [this was said aloud]

(Student 1)

Page 327: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

314

Table Y3 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group C

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 0 0 11 4 15 8.6

3. Agrees 1 1 15 2 19 10.9

4. Gives suggestion 9 4 6 7 26 14.9

5. Gives opinion 4 0 33 8 45 25.9

6. Gives information 6 1 18 8 33 19.0

7. Asks for information 3 2 6 1 12 6.9

8. Asks for opinion 1 1 10 3 15 8.6

9. Asks for suggestion 2 1 3 0 6 3.4

10. Disagrees 1 0 1 0 2 1.1

11. Shows tension 1 0 0 0 1 0.6

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 28 10 103 33 174 100

Page 328: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

315

Group D Figure Y4 shows that Group D has a higher number of task-related

communication (77.2%) than team-related communication (22.8%).

21.6

51.5

25.7

1.20

10

20

30

40

50

60

1.- 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. -12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y4. IPA domain frequencies for Group D.

The IPA category frequencies for Group D are shown in Table Y4. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (21.6%), gives opinion (19.9%), and agrees (14%). The category with the

lowest frequency count, with no behaviours observed or coded, is shows tension.

Gives information is the highest category in session 3 and 4, while gives

opinion is the highest category in session 2. An example of gives information

suggestion from session 4 was when groups were working on their ranking models:

Adelaide got first in the food (Student 2).

The behaviours of seems unfriendly (0.6%) and disagrees (0.6%) were coded

in sessions 2 and 3. The category disagrees was coded for behaviours or acts that

show disagreement, passive rejection, formality or withholding help. An example of

Page 329: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

316

disagrees from session 3 was when groups were working on their ranking system for

the top cities in Australia:

Hobart's third (Student 1) (Gives opinion)

I just said Adelaide was third (Student 2) (Disagrees).

Table Y4 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group D

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 3 0 3 1.8

2. Dramatises 0 2 8 10 5.8

3. Agrees 1 18 5 24 14.0

4. Gives suggestion 4 13 0 17 9.9

5. Gives opinion 10 15 9 34 19.9

6. Gives information 3 19 15 37 21.6

7. Asks for information 1 13 6 20 11.7

8. Asks for opinion 1 3 7 11 6.4

9. Asks for suggestion 5 4 4 13 7.6

10. Disagrees 0 1 0 1 0.6

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 1 0 0 1 0.6

Total 26 91 54 171 100

Page 330: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

317

Group E

Figure Y5 shows that Group E has a higher number of task-related

communication (78.3%) than team-related communication (21.7%).

18.4

56.6

21.7

3.3

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

1.- 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y5. IPA domain frequencies for Group E.

The IPA category frequencies for Group E are shown in Table Y5. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (23.2%), gives suggestion (17.4%), gives opinion (15.9%), and agrees

(15.7%). The category with the lowest frequency count, with no behaviours observed

or coded, is seems unfriendly.

Gives information is the highest category in session 2 and 4, while agrees is

the highest category in session 3. An example of gives information from session 2 was

when groups were working on their ranking models:

Our plan is to get information to use to find the top eight which is the

best (Student 1)

Page 331: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

318

An example of agrees from session 4 was when groups were placing the

categories they had ranked on to the Knowledge Forum ® database:

Yeah, that definitely needs a hospital (Student 2).

The behaviour of shows tension (1%) was coded in sessions 2 and 4. An

example of shows tension from session 4 was when groups were allocating group

roles to each member of the group:

We're trying to tell you but you weren't listening to us (Student 2).

Page 332: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

319

Table Y5 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group E

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 2 4 0 6 1.5

2. Dramatises 3 0 2 5 1.3

3. Agrees 15 12 35 62 15.7

4. Gives suggestion 31 11 27 69 17.4

5. Gives opinion 13 11 39 63 15.9

6. Gives information 38 11 43 92 23.2

7. Asks for information 18 8 7 33 8.3

8. Asks for opinion 11 1 16 28 7.1

9. Asks for suggestion 12 6 7 25 6.3

10. Disagrees 1 2 6 9 2.3

11. Shows tension 2 0 2 4 1.0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0

Total 146 66 184 396 100

Page 333: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

320

Group F

Figure Y6 shows that Group F has a higher number of task-related

communication (74.9%) than team-related communication (25.2%).

20.4

61.1

13.8

4.8

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y6. IPA domain frequencies for Group F.

The IPA category frequencies for Group F are shown in Table Y6. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (32%), gives opinion (18.6%), and agrees (18.6%). The category with the

lowest frequency count, with no behaviours observed or coded, is seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in session 1, 3, and 5, while gives

opinion was the highest category in session 4. The categories with the highest number

in session 2 includes agrees, give suggestion, and asks for suggestion. An example of

gives information from session 5 was when groups were finishing placing their

ranking system on to the Knowledge Forum ® database:

We don't need the web site (Student 2).

Page 334: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

321

The behaviours of seems friendly (0.9%), dramatises (0.9%), and shows

tension (0.6%), were coded in sessions 2 and 3. The category seems friendly was

coded for behaviours or acts that seemed friendly, raised other’s status, gave help or

reward. The category dramatises was coded for behaviours or acts that showed

dramatising, joking, laughing, or showing satisfaction. An example of dramatises

from session 2 was when groups were working on their ranking system for the top

cities in Australia:

This is cool. This is cool (Student 1)

Page 335: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

322

Table Y6 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group F

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Session

5

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 1 0 1 0 1 3 0.9

2. Dramatises 0 2 1 0 0 3 0.9

3. Agrees 9 4 18 22 9 62 18.6

4. Gives suggestion 6 4 10 7 8 35 10.5

5. Gives opinion 7 1 10 32 12 62 18.6

6. Gives information 14 3 21 20 49 107 32.0

7. Asks for information 2 0 2 2 23 29 8.7

8. Asks for opinion 0 2 3 1 0 6 1.8

9. Asks for suggestion 0 4 1 3 3 11 3.3

10. Disagrees 1 0 5 6 1 13 3.9

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 2 1 3 0.9

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 40 20 72 95 107 334 100

Page 336: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

323

Group G

Figure Y7 shows that Group G has a higher number of task-related

communication (79%) than team-related communication (21%).

16.4

50.7

28.3

4.6

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. -12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y7. IPA domain frequencies for Group G.

The IPA category frequencies for Group G are shown in Table Y7. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (23%) and gives opinion (17.8%). The categories with the lowest

frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded, are seems friendly, shows

tension, and seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in session 2 and 3, while asks for

opinion was the highest category in session 4, followed by gives opinion. The

category asks for opinion is coded for behaviours or acts that ask for opinion,

evaluation, analysis, or an expression of feelings. An example of asks for opinion

from session 4 was when groups were writing in their group diary:

Page 337: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

324

You reckon that's neat writing (Student 1)

The behaviour of dramatises (2%) was coded in sessions 2 and 3. An example

of dramatises was when groups were developing their ranking models:

Rock on (Student 1)

Table Y7 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group G

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 1 2 0 3 2.0

3. Agrees 11 10 1 22 14.5

4. Gives suggestion 10 3 2 15 9.9

5. Gives opinion 13 8 6 27 17.8

6. Gives information 13 19 3 35 23.0

7. Asks for information 4 8 0 12 7.9

8. Asks for opinion 3 8 7 18 11.8

9. Asks for suggestion 3 8 2 13 8.6

10. Disagrees 0 6 1 7 4.6

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0

Total 58 72 22 152 100

Page 338: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

325

Group H

Figure Y8 shows that Group H has a higher number of task-related

communication (76.3%) than team-related communication (23.7%).

23.7

60.2

16.1

00

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y8. IPA domain frequencies for Group H.

The IPA category frequencies for Group H are shown in Table Y8. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are agrees

(23.7%), and gives information (21.5%). The categories with the lowest frequency

counts, with no behaviours observed or coded, are seems friendly, dramatises,

disagrees, shows tension, and seems unfriendly.

Agrees was the highest category in session 4, and was the highest in session 2

along with the categories of gives suggestion and gives information. An example of

agrees from session 3 was when groups were ranking the top cities in Australia:

Student 1: Then we'll do pollution (Student 2) (Gives suggestion)

Student 2: OK (Student 3) (Agrees)

Page 339: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

326

The behaviour of asks for suggestion (3.2%) was coded in sessions 3 and 4.

Behaviours in this category include behaviours, or acts, that ask for suggestions,

direction, or possible ways of action. An example of asks for suggestion from session

3 was when groups were developing their ranking models:

Ok, so which city should go first? (Student 2).

Table Y8

IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group H

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 0 0 0 0 0

3. Agrees 4 8 10 22 23.7

4. Gives suggestion 4 9 5 18 19.4

5. Gives opinion 3 8 7 18 19.4

6. Gives information 4 8 8 20 21.5

7. Asks for information 0 4 2 6 6.5

8. Asks for opinion 0 1 5 6 6.5

9. Asks for suggestion 0 2 1 3 3.2

10. Disagrees 0 0 0 0 0

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0

Total 15 40 38 93 100

Page 340: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

327

Group I

Figure Y9 shows that Group I has a higher number of task-related

communication (23.1%) than team-related communication (76.8%).

20.5

60.0

16.8

2.6

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y9. IPA domain frequencies for Group I.

The IPA category frequencies for Group I are shown in Table Y9. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (32.6%), agrees (15.8%), and gives opinion (14.2%). The categories with

the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded are seems

friendly, shows tension, and seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in session 1, 2, and 4, while gives

opinion was the highest category in session 3. An example of gives information from

session 2 was when groups were allocating group roles to their group members:

I'm going to have to be recorder for first half (Student 2).

Page 341: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

328

The behaviour of disagrees (2.6%) was coded in sessions 1, 3, and 4. An

example of disagrees from was when groups were placing their ranking models on to

the Knowledge Forum ® database:

No, you're going to put equals (Student 1)

Table Y9 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group I

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 6 0 2 1 9 4.7

3. Agrees 12 0 17 1 30 15.8

4. Gives suggestion 16 2 5 2 25 13.2

5. Gives opinion 5 0 21 1 27 14.2

6. Gives information 31 2 13 16 62 32.6

7. Asks for information 6 0 3 8 17 8.9

8. Asks for opinion 3 0 1 3 7 3.7

9. Asks for suggestion 3 1 3 1 8 4.2

10. Disagrees 3 0 1 1 5 2.6

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 85 5 66 34 190 100

Page 342: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

329

Group J Figure Y10 shows that Group J has a higher number of task-related

communication (78.8%) than team-related communication (21.2%).

15.7

66.8

12.05.5

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 8. 9. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y10. IPA domain frequencies for Group J.

The IPA category frequencies for Group J are shown in Table Y10. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (43.3%), gives suggestions (15.7%), and agrees (14.3%). The categories

with the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded, are shows

tension and seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in all sessions. An example of

gives information from session 3 was when groups were placing their ranking models

on to the Knowledge Forum ® database:

It's double click down there (Student 3).

Page 343: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

330

The behaviour of seems friendly (0.5%) was coded in session 3. An example

of seems friendly was when groups were writing their group and task skills in their

group diary:

You got nicer handwriting than me (Student 1)

Table Y10 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group J

Categories Session 2

Session 3

Total % of total

1. Seems friendly 0 1 1 0.5

2. Dramatises 0 2 2 0.9

3. Agrees 0 31 31 14.3

4. Gives suggestion 0 34 34 15.7

5. Gives opinion 2 15 17 7.8

6. Gives information 3 91 94 43.3

7. Asks for information 3 12 15 6.9

8. Asks for opinion 0 5 5 2.3

9. Asks for suggestion 0 6 6 2.8

10. Disagrees 0 12 12 5.5

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0

Total 8 209 217 100

Page 344: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

331

Group K Figure Y11 shows that Group K has a higher number of task-related

communication (70%) than team-related communication (30%).

20.0

51.1

18.9

10.0

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y11. IPA domain frequencies for Group K.

The IPA category frequencies for Group K are shown in Table Y11. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (27.9%), and agrees (16.8%). The category with the lowest frequency

count, with no behaviours observed or coded is seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in session 2, 3, 4, and 5, while

gives suggestion was the highest category in session 1. An example of gives

information from session 4 was when groups were combining their ranking to find the

best city:

Why did we rank Melbourne number one? (Student 1) (Asks for information)

Because that had the most facilities, it had the most stadiums (Student 2)

(Gives information)

Page 345: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

332

The behaviours of seems friendly (1.1%) and shows tension (1.1%) were

coded. An example of seems friendly was when groups were working out their group

roles:

Yeah and I'll help too (Student 1)

Table Y11

IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group K

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Session

5

Total % of

total

1. Seems friendly 0 1 0 1 0 2 1.1

2. Dramatises 0 3 0 0 1 4 2.1

3. Agrees 2 6 1 22 1 32 16.8

4. Gives suggestion 3 6 0 14 3 26 13.7

5. Gives opinion 1 3 4 9 1 18 9.5

6. Gives information 2 12 6 30 3 53 27.9

7. Asks for

information

2 9 1 13 1 26 13.7

8. Asks for opinion 0 0 0 5 0 5 2.6

9. Asks for suggestion 1 0 1 3 0 5 2.6

10. Disagrees 1 1 0 15 0 17 8.9

11. Shows tension 2 0 0 0 0 2 1.1

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 14 41 13 112 10 190 100

Page 346: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

333

Group L

Figure Y12 shows that Group L has a higher number of task-related

communication (82.2%) than team-related communication (17.8%).

13.5

64.8

17.4

4.3

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y12. IPA domain frequencies for Group L.

The IPA category frequencies for Group L are shown in Table Y12. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (41.3%), gives suggestion (14.8%), and asks for information (13%). The

category with the lowest frequency count, with no behaviours observed or coded are

asks for opinion.

Gives information was the highest category in all sessions. An example of

gives information from session 1 was when groups were filling in the monitoring

checklist in their group diary:

Our goal is to rank cities (Student 1)

Page 347: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

334

The behaviour of seems friendly (0.9%) was coded in session 1 and 2. An

example of seems friendly was when groups were placing a note on to the Knowledge

Forum ® database:

Yeah, I want to say something like I heard of your school, I heard it's

pretty good (Student 1)

Table Y12 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group L

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 1 1 0 2 0.9

2. Dramatises 1 2 2 5 2.2

3. Agrees 2 6 16 24 10.4

4. Gives suggestion 17 11 6 34 14.8

5. Gives opinion 8 3 9 20 8.7

6. Gives information 19 20 56 95 41.3

7. Asks for information 4 6 20 30 13.0

8. Asks for opinion 0 0 0 0 0

9. Asks for suggestion 6 3 1 10 4.3

10. Disagrees 0 3 0 3 1.3

11. Shows tension 0 4 0 4 1.7

12. Seems unfriendly 1 2 0 3 1.3

Total 59 61 110 230 100

Page 348: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

335

Group M

Figure Y13 shows that Group M has a higher number of task-related

communication (86%) than team-related communication (14%).

8.6

71.8

14.1

5.5

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y13. IPA domain frequencies for Group M.

The IPA category frequencies for Group M are shown in Table Y13. As can

be seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (40.3%) and gives suggestion (20.1%). The categories with the lowest

frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded are seems friendly,

dramatises, and seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in all sessions. An example of

gives information was when groups were reading the notes that were placed on to the

Knowledge Forum database®:

We got a note, they said great idea (Student 1)

Page 349: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

336

The behaviour of shows tension (0.6%) was coded in session 5. An example of

shows tension was when groups were typing a reply into the Knowledge Forum®

database:

I, I know how to spell it (Student 2).

Table Y13 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group M

Categories Session

1

Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Session

5

Total %

of

total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

3. Agrees 3 3 7 1 0 14 8.6

4. Gives suggestion 4 4 20 0 4 32 19.6

5. Gives opinion 8 5 3 0 2 18 11.0

6. Gives information 8 20 24 3 12 67 41.1

7. Asks for

information

1 5 5 0 1 12 7.4

8. Asks for opinion 0 2 2 0 0 4 2.5

9. Asks for suggestion 1 4 2 0 0 7 4.3

10. Disagrees 3 0 0 0 5 8 4.9

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 1 1 0.6

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 28 43 63 4 25 163 100

Page 350: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

337

Group N Figure Y14 shows that Group N has a higher number of task-related

communication (81.6%) than team-related communication (18.4%).

17.0

61.0

20.6

1.40

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y14. IPA domain frequencies for Group N.

The IPA category frequencies for Group N are shown in Table Y14. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (30.5%), gives opinion (19.1%), and agrees (16.3%). The categories

with the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded are seems

friendly, shows tension, and seems unfriendly

Gives information was the highest category in all sessions. An example of

gives information from session 3 was when groups were placing their ranking models

on to the Knowledge Forum ® database:

Brisbane's the best cause (sic) it had the most hospitals (Student 2).

Page 351: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

338

The behaviour of dramatises (0.7%) was coded in session 2. An example of

dramatises was when groups were developing their ranking models:

Oh cool (Student 1)

Table Y14 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group N

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 1 0 0 1 0.7

3. Agrees 8 14 1 23 16.3

4. Gives suggestion 7 5 4 16 11.3

5. Gives opinion 7 17 3 27 19.1

6. Gives information 16 17 10 43 30.5

7. Asks for information 2 3 8 13 9.2

8. Asks for opinion 2 11 1 14 9.9

9. Asks for suggestion 0 1 1 2 1.4

10. Disagrees 0 2 0 2 1.4

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0

Total 43 70 28 141 100

Page 352: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

339

Group O

Figure Y15 shows that Group O has a higher number of task-related

communication (78.2%) than team-related communication (21.8%).

16.2

62.0

16.2

5.6

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1. - 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. -12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y15. IPA domain frequencies for Group O.

The IPA category frequencies for Group O are shown in Table Y15. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (35.9%), agrees (15.5%), and gives opinion (14.8%). The categories with

the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded are seems

friendly, shows tension, and seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in session 1, 3, and 5, while asks

for suggestion was the highest category in session 2, and asks for opinion was the

highest category in session 4. An example of gives information from session 1 was

when groups were deciding how to rank the capital cities in Australia:

We are ranking cities with sports and environment (Student 1)

Page 353: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

340

The behaviour of dramatises (0.7%) was coded in session 1. An example of

dramatises was when groups were writing welcome notes and placing them on the

Knowledge Forum® database:

I don't know [laughs] (Student 2).

Table Y15 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group O

Categories Session 1 Session 2 Session

3

Session

4

Session

5

Total % of

total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 1 0 0 0 0 1 0.7

3. Agrees 9 1 7 2 3 22 15.5

4. Gives suggestion 3 2 5 1 5 16 11.3

5. Gives opinion 6 3 5 2 5 21 14.8

6. Gives

information

29 0 13 0 9 51 35.9

7. Asks for

information

2 0 3 1 1 7 4.9

8. Asks for opinion 2 0 1 3 1 7 4.9

9. Asks for

suggestion

0 4 3 2 0 9 6.3

10. Disagrees 1 0 2 0 5 8 5.6

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems

unfriendly

0 0 0 0 0 0 0

Total 53 10 9 11 29 142 100

Page 354: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

341

Group P Figure Y16 shows that Group P has a higher number of task-related

communication (77%) than team-related communication (23%).

22.3

65.0

12.1

0.50

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1.- 3. 4. - 6. 7. - 9. 10. - 12.

IPA Categories

Fre

qu

ency

(%

)

Figure Y16. IPA domain frequencies for Group P.

The IPA category frequencies for Group P are shown in Table Y16. As can be

seen in this table the categories with the highest total frequency counts are gives

information (25.7%), gives opinion (25.2%), and agrees (18.9%). The categories with

the lowest frequency counts, with no behaviours observed or coded are seems

friendly, shows tension, and seems unfriendly.

Gives information was the highest category in all sessions. An example of

gives information from session 4 was when groups were working on their ranking

models to find the ‘best’ city in Australia

And the top city is Brisbane again (Student 1)

The behaviour of disagrees (0.5%) was coded in session 3. An example of

disagrees was when the group was discussing a new name for their group:

Page 355: Primary students’ group metacognitive …eprints.qut.edu.au/29819/1/Christina_Chalmers_Thesis.pdfPrimary students’ group metacognitive processes in a computer supported collaborative

342

The jungle girls (Student 2) (Gives suggestion)

No (Student 1) (Disagrees)

Table Y16 IPA Frequency Count for Each Category for Group P

Categories Session

2

Session

3

Session

4

Total %

of total

1. Seems friendly 0 0 0 0 0

2. Dramatises 1 2 4 7 3.4

3. Agrees 3 10 26 39 18.9

4. Gives suggestion 7 12 10 29 14.1

5. Gives opinion 8 12 32 52 25.2

6. Gives information 12 13 28 53 25.7

7. Asks for information 4 7 7 18 8.7

8. Asks for opinion 0 1 2 3 1.5

9. Asks for suggestion 0 0 4 4 1.9

10. Disagrees 0 1 0 1 0.5

11. Shows tension 0 0 0 0 0

12. Seems unfriendly 0 0 0 0 0

Total 35 58 113 206 100