50
Learning how to think and learn Week 7 Psychology NJ Kang

Learning how to think and learn

  • Upload
    denali

  • View
    44

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

Learning how to think and learn. Week 7 Psychology NJ Kang. Attending, concentrating and remembering. Concentration. Better learning related to better concentration span Concentration span parallel to age Why?. Learning to remember: rehearsal and organization. - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Citation preview

Page 1: Learning how to think and learn

Learning how to think and learn

Week 7 Psychology NJ Kang

Page 2: Learning how to think and learn

Attending, concentrating and remembering

Page 3: Learning how to think and learn

Concentration• Better learning related to better con-

centration span• Concentration span parallel to age• Why?

Page 4: Learning how to think and learn

Learning to remember: rehearsal and organization

• 5 yrs: overestimate their own capaci-ties

• 11 yrs: remember more than the younger ones, realistic assessments of one’s own mental abilities.

• Memory is not the product of natural ability but involve learned activity

Page 5: Learning how to think and learn

A) Rehearsal • Is a powerful aid to deliberate memorization

and that the imposition of some structure or meaning on what we seek to memorize also determines the likelihood of successful re-call.

• Is continuous repetition, out loud or under the breath, of what one is trying to hold in mind.

• And younger ones have to learn how to do it (say out loud) and it leads them to success.

Page 6: Learning how to think and learn

Same to adult• Fast rehearsal seems to work well in

their long term memory• Learning rehearsal strategies would

not be maintained without being used.

Page 7: Learning how to think and learn

B) Categorical organization in mem-ory as a strategy

• 11 yr. know how to use this skill in memory strategy

• 5 yrs. Do not seemingly know how to use this skill in their memorizing af-ter even they rehearsed how to cate-gorize the objects.

Page 8: Learning how to think and learn

Memorizing : one activity or many?

• We are dealing with changes in the child’s cognitive skills and not simply with linguis-tic development.

• Learn how to remember needs to have a purpose and own strategy

• Preschooler will keep touching the appro-priate cup from time to time. The child uses his digits to ‘keep hold’ of the infor-mation he has to remember, literally ‘re-mind-ing ‘ himself of it by physical means

Page 9: Learning how to think and learn

Pre-schoolers’ natural history of memory development

• A camera is lost at the third post when they were told that they will take a picture later.

• They went to the third post to find the lost camera.

• They are making intelligent use of past experience to formulate a plan of action – evidence of both memory and reasoning. Piaget, physical action

thought formation

Page 10: Learning how to think and learn

Why? Are they so good?• Multiplicity of intellectual activities

that we commonly (and mistakenly) lump together under headings like memory.

• Not a deliberate memorization but natural and often incidental conse-quence of their activities.

Page 11: Learning how to think and learn

literally ‘re-mind-ing ‘ himself of it by physical means

• How do we use this in the teaching children English?

• Think about teaching parts of an Ele-phant.

Page 12: Learning how to think and learn

Memory and School-ing

Page 13: Learning how to think and learn

Asking vs interactions• They do not achieve when we simply ask

them to memorize.• They achieve more when they are led to

activities or interactions with the material destined to be remembered.

• The child’s active involvement with the material and his concentration on the task at hand helped him to encode or become consciously aware of the objects handled.

How can we use of this capacity to teaching chil-dren English phonics?

How can we link this understanding to Piagetian experiments or theory?

Page 14: Learning how to think and learn

Memory and schooling• Powers of concentration and memory are fostered in

social interaction? Then it should be different from cultures to culture.

• Rehearsal is not a feature of deliberate attempts at memorization by people in non-technological soci-eties.

• These cultures are childlike; non rational or in some other way less developed than in our own.

• It is due to schooling.• So children’s lack of memorization is due to learning

meaningfully by active involvement not by taught.

Page 15: Learning how to think and learn

Paying attention

Page 16: Learning how to think and learn

Paying attention• Children under eight are impulsive in or-

ganizing and interrogating and even anal-izing the unfamiliar and purposeless tasks.

• Paying more attention and complete more complicated tasks when these are more relevant to their own lives and interests and meaningful to them (identifying iden-tical and different pictures vs making puz-zles and model making by looking at the pictures)

Page 17: Learning how to think and learn

Wholes and parts

Page 18: Learning how to think and learn

Wholes and parts: theories of per-ception and understanding

• Relationship between perception and knowledge.

• Child aged below five see the parts not the whole. Lack in synthesizing ability.

• Under seven tend to centre upon either the overall configuration or its parts. Not the both.

• This age group seem not to see these both even though these are meaningful to them .

Page 19: Learning how to think and learn

Parallel to Piaget• Only simple can be traced (shape tracing in

the shape embedded shape in a simpler ver-sion only) but not the complicated one.

• Once the child perceives something in a par-ticular way, he seems unable to review it from a different perspective

• Pre-operational children cannot entertain the idea that an element of any task or situation, whatever its nature, can belong at the same time to two or more categories or classes.

Page 20: Learning how to think and learn

Are young children illog-ical, or limited proces-sors of information?

Page 21: Learning how to think and learn

Are young children illogical, or lim-ited processors of information?

• If the pre-operational children fail more than the older ones due to lack of experiences not of lack of logicali-ties. Then it could be possible to help them to learn how to think logically.

Page 22: Learning how to think and learn

Seriation task (put things into order from small to large)

• This demands intellectual co-ordina-tion of perceptual judgments which, as we have seen, pre-operational children cannot achieve. (understand put sticks in length and compare A>B B>C, so A>C)

• Piaget lack of co-ordination

Page 23: Learning how to think and learn

Bryant (1974) not due to the pre-logical

• Rather, their failure is due to the fact that these sorts of problems overload their mental capacities.

• Incapable of memorizing the relations be-tween the five sticks. is due to lack of con-centration time needed to solve the problem.

• His experiment, used coloured sticks and let them memorize the length of each stick com-pared to others. Then they were able to infer the order of length of the sticks.

Page 24: Learning how to think and learn

Children do not pass pre-operational stage

• In which they were unable to learn or be taught how to reason logically.

• Rather, the argument proceeds, the tasks used are so unfamiliar to chil-dren and make such unusual de-mands that failure is attributable to a lack of relevant experience, not to in-tellectual incompetence.

Page 25: Learning how to think and learn

What is effective instruc-tion?

Page 26: Learning how to think and learn

Question• How can we conceptualize and ana-

lyse the process of instruction in or-der to determine whether or not what is being taught is also being learned?

• It demands sufficient prior knowledge to memorize and recall what is shown and may prove too difficult to assimilate.

Page 27: Learning how to think and learn

ZPD• Gaps between unassisted and assisted

competence.• How can we determine whether or not

instruction is sensitive to a child’s zone of development

• When does it make demands beyond his potential level of comprehension?

• How can we sure that instruction does not underestimate his ability?

Page 28: Learning how to think and learn

Answers (Wood, Wood and Middle-ton, 1978)

What does it mean to be taught well to complete the task on his own?

What should be said or done first?

Page 29: Learning how to think and learn

Levels used to classify teaching in the pyramid task.

1. General verbal encouragement (why don’t you try to put some blocks together?)

2. Specific verbal instruction (can you find the four biggest blocks?)

3. Assists in choice of material (let’s try to put these big ones together)

4. Prepares material for assembly (selecting the blocks and lining them up)

5. Demonstrates an operation (how a set of four blocks fit together, doing several operations herself while the child looks on)

Page 30: Learning how to think and learn

• Too much specific instruction in-creases child’s successful completion of the task, but decreases child’s de-velopment and responsibility for completing the task.

• Too less specific instruction would not help child to complete the task but increases child’s responsibility for competing the task.

Page 31: Learning how to think and learn

Teach children contingently• Making any help given conditional

upon the child’s understanding of previous levels of instruction

• No left the child alone when he was overwhelmed by the task, and also guaranteed him greater scope for ini-tiative when he showed signs of suc-cess.

Page 32: Learning how to think and learn

Verbal instruction only• Failure

Page 33: Learning how to think and learn

Demonstration• Failure • Not understanding what they had

been shown.

Page 34: Learning how to think and learn

Contingent teaching = Scaffolding• Showing + verbal instruction• Get the four next ones’ because previous

instruction involved an element of showing which made such verbal instructions con-textually meaningful.

• by highlighting things they need to take ac-count of in each process.

• Breaks the task down into a sequence of smaller tasks which children can manage to construct the completed assembly.

Page 35: Learning how to think and learn

Piaget theory?• 4 yrs cannot do the tasks (seriation,

conservation) alone maybe

• 7 yrs can do the task alone

• Without being helped ? Then yes.

Page 36: Learning how to think and learn

Contingent teaching• Pacing the amount of help children

are given on the basis of their mo-ment to moment understanding.

• Child understand teacher step back.

• Child don’t understand teacher give more help.

• Nor is he left alone or too directive.

Page 37: Learning how to think and learn

In the school classroom

DIFFICULTBUT

POSSIBLE

Page 38: Learning how to think and learn

four – year olds• Can be taught to do tasks that alone

they will not master until around age seven or eight.

• For them, instruction must be geared to their level of competence.

• When this condition can be and is achieved, young children can be taught and do learn.

Page 39: Learning how to think and learn

Learning and generalization: first thoughts on a thorny issue

Page 40: Learning how to think and learn

Questions• How and why children develop the intelli-

gence to achieve understanding.• What knowledge and experience are they

drawing on and generalizing from in order to understand?

• For Piaget, the schemes of knowing that make possible the understanding and generalization of experience are rooted in actions on the world.

• For Vygotsky and Bruner: social interaction and largely informal teaching.

Page 41: Learning how to think and learn

Language of information processing• The mental activities that are believed to un-

derlie learning and thinking, • and in the experiences that lead to their ac-

quisition and perfection• Vygotsky (can do task by being taught contin-

gently properly) and Piaget (cannot do the task no matter what type of teaching is given) differ

• Vygotsky not only learn to complete the task but also learn the instructional process itself.

Page 42: Learning how to think and learn

Why do some children learn while others don’t.

• Piagetians: many failures to remem-ber and generalize --- premature teaching serves only to inculcate empty procedures or learned tricks.

• Preoperational children can be taught to complete the task but not to gen-eralize the knowledge.

Page 43: Learning how to think and learn

Is more complex issue• Learn one concept can be general-

ized to all the other concepts?• But if we don’t subscribe this issue

then we don’t have to expect them to do this.

Under-standing

One concept

An-other con-cept

Another concept

Another concept ?

Page 44: Learning how to think and learn

Piaget: Horizontal decalage• Gradual extension of competence from task

to task• moving from one to another stages de-

velopment can be done not by once but by gradual development

• this makes clear-cut predictions about when and how children will display logical understanding of different tasks difficult.

• it does not fit the developmental facts of Piaget

Page 45: Learning how to think and learn

Class inclusion• Piaget: do class inclusion understand-

ing number concept 4<6<8 and then, understand the grasp of number con-cepts.

• Lunzer, 1973: knowledge of number helps him to understand class inclusion problems

Page 46: Learning how to think and learn

Bruner’s thought• Teaching of procedures, facts, dates,

formulae and so for the will not en-gender understanding or facilitate generalization unless the child un-derstands the intentions and pur-poses that motivate both the disci-pline and the people who practise and teach it.

Page 47: Learning how to think and learn

The centrality of purposive activity in learning and development

• The roles they (V, B and P) portray for social interaction and instruction are quite different.

• P: cultural universals revealed by com-mon stages of development.

• V & B: find different ways of thinking and construing the world that arise out of cultural knowledge and different ways of socializing and educating children.

Page 48: Learning how to think and learn

Task • Listen to the story and try to teach

the sequencing the story to the 4 and 7 yrs olds using the theories of both Piaget and Vygotsky. And also contingent teaching.

Page 49: Learning how to think and learn

Midterm• Option 1• Compare theories of Piaget and Vy-

gotsky in terms of how young learners develop their cognitive skills.

• Develop a curriculum for teaching Eng-lish to two particular age groups (1 & 2, 3 & 4, 5, 6 & 7 of young learners) reflecting each theoriest’s views on learning.

Page 50: Learning how to think and learn

Midterm• Option 2• Select 3 Piaget’s experiments and

redo the research explaining under-pinning theories.

• Develop a curriculum and activities that reflect those experiments in teaching English to two particular age groups (1 & 2, 3 & 4, 5, 6 & 7 of young learners)