Kao EducAttARS

  • Upload
    gadowl

  • View
    244

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    1/28

    Annu. Rev. Sociol. 2003. 29:41742doi: 10.1146/annurev.soc.29.010202.100019

    Copyright c 2003 by Annual Reviews. All rights reservedFirst published online as a Review in Advance on June 4, 2003

    RACIAL AND ETHNIC STRATIFICATION INEDUCATIONAL ACHIEVEMENT AND ATTAINMENT

    GraceKao1andJennifer S.Thompson21Department of Sociology and Population Studies Center, University of Pennsylvania,

    Philadelphia 19104-6299; email: [email protected] Statistics Service Institute, American Institutes for Research, Washington,

    District of Columbia 20006; email: [email protected]

    Key Words immigrants, adolescents, aspirations

    s Abstract Understanding racial, ethnic, and immigrant variation in educationalachievement andattainment is more important than ever as theU.S. population becomesincreasingly diverse. The Census Bureau estimates that in 2000, 34% of all youth aged1519 were from minority groups; it estimates that by 2025, this will increase to 46%(U.S. Census Bureau 2000). In addition, approximately one in five school-age childrenreside in an immigrant family (Zhou 1997, Suarez-Orozco & Suarez-Orozco 2001).

    We provide an overview of recent empirical research on racial, ethnic, and immigrantdifferences in educational achievement and attainment, and we examine some cur-rent theories that attempt to explain these differences. We explore group differences ingrades, testscores, course taking, and tracking,especially throughoutsecondaryschool-ing, and then discuss variation in high school completion, transitions to college, andcollege completion. We also summarize key theoretical explanations used to explainpersistent differences net of variation in socioeconomic status, which focus on familyand cultural beliefs that stem from minority group and class experiences. Overall, thereare many signs of optimism. Racial and ethnic gaps in educational achievement andattainment have narrowed over the past three decades by every measure available tosocial scientists. Educational aspirations are universally high for all racial and ethnicgroups as most adolescents expect to go to college. However, substantial gaps remain,especially between less advantaged groups such as African Americans, Hispanics, andNative Americans and more advantaged groups such as whites and Asian Americans.The racial and ethnic hierarchy in educational achievement is apparent across varyingmeasures of the academic experience.

    INTRODUCTION

    In this review, we provide an overview of recent empirical research on racial, eth-

    nic, and immigrant differences in educational achievement and attainment, and

    we examine some current theories that attempt to explain these differences. The

    link between academic performance, educational attainment, and eventual labor

    market outcomes is well known and extensively documented (Coleman 1961,

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    2/28

    418 KAO THOMPSON

    Jencks 1972). Moreover, we know that some of the relative disadvantages and

    advantages faced by racial and ethnic minorities can be traced to their differen-

    tial educational achievement and attainment (Jencks 1972). Understanding race,

    ethnic, and immigrant variation in educational achievement and attainment ismore important than ever as the U.S. population becomes increasingly diverse.

    Because minority populations are younger than their white counterparts, the in-

    creasing diversity in the general population is even more apparent among youth.

    The Census Bureau estimates that in 2000, 34% of all youth aged 1519 were

    from minority groups; it estimates that by 2025, this will increase to 46% (U.S.

    Census Bureau 2000). Similarly, approximately one in five school-age children

    currently comes from an immigrant family (Zhou 1997, Suarez-Orozco & Suarez-

    Orozco 2001). For both Asian and Hispanic youth, understanding patterns of

    education among immigrants is especially vital. Approximately 50% of todaysHispanic youth are immigrants or children of immigrants; for Asian Americans,

    this proportion is closer to 90% (Zhou 1997). This proportion is especially high

    in urban school districts; for instance, in New York City, approximately 48%

    of children in school are children of immigrant parents (Suarez-Orozco &

    Suarez-Orozco 2001).

    Our review proceeds as follows: First, we provide a short overview of key the-

    oretical frameworks used to explain racial and ethnic differences in achievement.

    Second, we examine group differences in grades, test scores, educational aspi-

    rations, course taking, and tracking, especially throughout secondary schooling.Then we examine differences in high school completion, transitions to college, and

    college completion. Because parental socioeconomic background has a profound

    influence on educational achievement and attainment, we focus on race, ethnic,

    and immigrant group differences before and after taking into account differences

    in socioeconomic background. Finally, we review common themes and promi-

    nent theories that are used to account for these differences; although we do not

    offer an evaluation of these theories, we state some explanations that are more or

    less consistent with the findings that have persisted throughout myriad empirical

    analyses.We do not focus on the beginning school transition, as Entwisle & Alexander

    (1993) completed an excellent review of that literature, nor do we focus on the

    effects of the organization of schools on achievement outcomes (see Hallinan 1988,

    Arum 2000 for excellent reviews), although we do examine tracking. Moreover, we

    do not examine how race, ethnicity, and immigrant status affect youth in other areas

    of life (see Lichter 1997 for a review on poverty among children; see Zhou 1997

    for a review on immigrant children). Also, we do not examine the general literature

    on childhood and adolescent development, as superb reviews of these literatures

    were completed by Corsaro & Eder (1990) and Dornbusch (1989), respectively.We also do not fully explore the notion of social capital, which was thoughtfully

    reviewed by Portes (1998). There is relatively little research on how social capital

    differentially affects the educational achievement and attainment of race, ethnic,

    and immigrant groups.

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    3/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 419

    THEORETICAL OVERVIEW

    The achievement gap between blacks and whites is longstanding and has re-

    ceived considerable empirical attention and theoretical discussion (Jencks 1972,Miller 1995, Jencks & Phillips 1998). Much of the discussion of subsequent eth-

    nic groups academic achievement (for instance, Jews, Southern Italians, Greeks,

    and more recently, Hispanics and Asian Americans) follow similar types of ar-

    guments. Although the debates are complex, most contemporary theories about

    why ethnic groups differ in their educational achievement fall into two general

    categories. The first is about how cultural orientations of certain ethnic groups

    promote/discourage academic achievement, and the second is about how the struc-

    tural position of ethnic groups affects the childrens (parent, peer, and school)

    environments.The first argument credits ethnic group differences in cultural orientation, most

    specifically differences in orientation toward schooling (McClelland et al. 1953,

    Rosen 1959, Lewis 1966, Ogbu 1974, Caplan et al. 1991). Some of these arguments

    can be traced to Webers The Protestant Ethic (1976), where an ethnic-religious

    group was credited for having a cultural orientation conducive to the develop-

    ment of capitalism. Ethnic groups have cultural orientations, which can benefit or

    hurt their odds of economic (and in our case, educational) success relative to other

    groups. Following McClellands (McClelland et al. 1953) laboratory studies of the

    achievement motive, where he and his colleagues theorized that individuals havevarying degrees of the achievement motive and that those with higher achieve-

    ment motives perform better in achievement tests, Rosen (1959) argued for ethnic

    group differences in their orientation toward achievement. Rosen (1959) argued

    that Jews, Greeks, and Protestants shared greater motivation toward achievement;

    hence this explained their higher socioeconomic mobility. In contrast, Rosen found

    that blacks, Southern Italians, and French Canadians scored lower on items that

    measured their achievement motivation and vocational aspirations. It is important

    to note that minority status does not imply poor outcomes.

    The second argument traces the structural position of ethnic groups, creditingeither their time of arrival, the skills migrants brought with them at their time of

    arrival, the needs of the local economy, or the fit between their skills and their abil-

    ity to fill certain economic niches (or some combination thereof) (see Steinberg

    1989, Lieberson 1980, Wilson 1980 for examples). Again, because parental social

    class has a considerable influence on a childs educational outcome, structural

    arguments fault or credit differences in achievement and attainment on parental

    socioeconomic status (SES), which is associated with parental participation, qual-

    ity of instruction, school peers, teachers, and other influences (see Sewell & Shah

    1968, Sewell et al. 1969, Murnane et al. 1980, Baker & Stevenson 1986, Astone &McLanahan 1991, Bankston & Caldas 1998). In other words, class differences are

    manifested through varying parental practices and schooling opportunities, which

    in turn favor more advantaged students. We come back to these arguments in our

    overview of empirical findings.

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    4/28

    420 KAO THOMPSON

    Of course, some theoretical discussions fall in between, often crediting and

    faulting cultural orientations to ethnic groups economic positions or experiences

    in society (Lewis 1966, Ogbu 1974, Willis 1977, MacLeod 1995, Sue & Okazaki

    1990, Steele & Aronson 1995, Steele 1997). We expand on these explanations later,as most contemporary arguments about ethnic group differences in achievement

    fall along the intersection of cultural orientation and structural position.

    A third, but much less frequent, line of reasoning credits genetic differences

    between racial groups in their innate intelligence (Jensen 1969, Herrnstein &

    Murray 1994), but most sociologists today dismiss these arguments both for their

    inherently racist overtones and for the lack of empirical data to support such

    claims. Very simply, no gene has been identified as affecting test scores. What

    is more striking is that great improvements in IQ and achievement scores within

    racial groups and the shrinking gaps between groups have occurred in just a fewdecades; hence genetic differences cannot account for these considerable changes

    in a brief time span (Jencks & Phillips 1998).

    TEST SCORES

    The test scores of blacks have lagged behind those of whites in vocabulary, read-

    ing, and math for the past 30 years, although this gap is shrinking. The National

    Assessment of Educational Progress (NAEP) has been testing 17 year olds since

    1971; from 1971 to 1996, the black-white reading gap shrank by almost one halfand the math gap by almost one third (Jencks & Phillips 1998). Specifically, Miller

    (1995) documents that among 17 year olds, blacks scored an average of 239 points

    as opposed to 291 points among whites on the NAEP reading tests in 1971; by

    1990, blacks scored an average of 267 as opposed to 297 for whites (out of a

    possible 500 points total). On NAEP math tests in 1973, blacks scored an average

    of 270, whereas whites scored 310; by 1990, blacks average score was 289 com-

    pared with 310 for whites (Miller 1995, table 3.5, p. 53). Although parental SES

    accounts for some of the black-white test gap, it does not account for all of it (see,

    e.g., Kao et al. 1996).

    Similarly, for Hispanics, the average NAEP math score for 17 year olds in

    1973 was 277 (as opposed to 310 for whites); in 1990, the average score among

    Hispanics was 284 compared with 310 for whites (Miller 1995, table 3.5, p. 53).

    Due to their small population size in the 1970s, similar trend data are not available

    for Asian Americans, but their recent scores suggest that they perform at levels

    comparable or above that of whites. Data from the 1990 NAEP Mathematics

    Assessment Tests show that among twelfth graders, Asians scored an average of

    315 points compared with 301 points for whites, 270 for blacks, 278 for Hispanics,

    and 290 for Native Americans (Miller 1995, table 3.8, p. 58). Racial and ethnic

    differentiation is most apparent at the highest achievement levels; 13% of Asians

    performed at level of 350 points or higher, whereas only 6% of whites, less than

    1% of blacks, 1% of Hispanics, and less than 1% of Native Americans did so.

    Using standardized reading and math tests administered to a national sample of

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    5/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 421

    eighth graders, Kao et al. (1996) found that parental background characteristics

    accounted for differences between Asians and whites, but not between Hispanics

    and whites, nor between blacks and whites.

    Racial and ethnic variation in SAT scores are even more striking, although theyfollow a similar pattern. The average SAT math score in 1990 was 491 for whites,

    528 for Asians, 385 for blacks, 429 for Mexican Americans, and 437 for Native

    Americans (Miller 1995, table 3.9, p. 61). Again, racial and ethnic differentiation

    is most noticeable at the higher range of scores. For instance, 34% of Asians

    compared with 20% of whites, 3% of blacks, 7% of Mexican Americans, and 9%

    of Native Americans scored above a 600 on the SAT math section. On the SAT

    verbal section in 1990, whites scored an average of 442, compared with 410 for

    Asians, 352 for blacks, 380 for Mexican Americans, and 388 for Native Americans.

    Eight percent of whites, 10% of Asians, 2% of blacks, 3% of Mexican Americans,and 3% of Native Americans scored above 600 on the SAT verbal section in 1990

    (Miller 1995, table 3.9, p. 61).

    Overall, researchers have treated achievement test scores as an indicator, to

    varying degrees, of a combination of innate ability (intelligence) and learned in-

    formation. The underlying assumption is that achievement test scores, however

    imperfect, measure some degree of ability or intelligence. Hence, when racial and

    ethnic differences persist after taking parental socioeconomic background into ac-

    count, sociologists often fault unmeasured differences in quality of schooling or

    other unmeasured differences. Because recent studies suggest that the achieve-ment gap (at least for whites and blacks) begins at an early age (Dreeben &

    Gamoran 1986), increasingly researchers look at the growth in achievement test

    scores among a cohort of students as a way to examine how parents, schools, and

    peers affect changes in test scores (see, for example, Keith & Page 1985, Guo

    1998, Stevenson et al. 1994). Although change in test scores is, conceptually, a

    more precise measure, it is the absolute scores that affect attainment and eventual

    labor market outcomes.

    GRADES

    Another indicator of academic performance is grades. Although some researchers

    fault grades (especially self-reported grades) as being an imprecise measure that

    is affected by variation in biases of teachers and students as well as the quality

    of schools and the relative difficulty of particular classes, grades are an important

    outcome because students and parents regularly monitor student performances via

    grades (DiMaggio 1982, Fehrman et al. 1987, Farkas et al. 1990, Valenzuela &

    Dornbusch 1994, Fuligni 1997, Kao 1995, Kao et al. 1996). Grades are positively

    correlated with achievement tests, but they are more sensitive to student input,

    such as hours spent on homework and time watching television (see Fehrman

    et al. 1987, Rumbaut 1990, Kao et al. 1996, Fuligni 1997, Rumbaut, unpublished

    manuscript). Moreover, they are a more concrete measure of student orientation

    toward schooling than abstract attitudes or educational aspirations because most

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    6/28

    422 KAO THOMPSON

    students embrace proschooling attitudes and report extremely high educational

    aspirations in survey questionnaires (Alexander & Cook 1979). Moreover, grades

    signal to students, rightly or wrongly, their odds of success in school, which may

    affect their odds of attaining higher education (Fehrman et al. 1987).Racial and ethnic variation in grades parallel that of test scores; however, varia-

    tions in grades are more likely to be accounted for by parental background and stu-

    dent characteristics and behavior. Like achievement test scores, grades are highly

    correlated with parental SES. In some studies, parental SES accounts for much, if

    not all, of the variation. For example, in an earlier paper using a nationally represen-

    tative sample of eighth graders from the National Education Longitudinal Study of

    1988 (NELS), Kao et al. (1996) found that Asians had the highest GPA (3.24) ver-

    sus 2.96 for whites, 2.74 for Hispanics, and 2.73 for blacks. After taking parental

    education, income, household status, immigrant status, and prior experiences atschool into account, the mean GPA of Hispanics was no longer significantly differ-

    ent, whereas the mean GPA of Asians was still moderately significantly different

    from that of whites. The mean GPA of blacks, on the other hand, remained statisti-

    cally significantly lower than that of whites. It is likely that more precise measures

    of parental education would have better captured the extent to which Hispanic

    youth are disadvantaged because the lowest parental educational category was

    less than high school. Hence, more-detailed parental SES measures may have

    completely accounted for group differences between Hispanics and whites.

    EDUCATIONAL ASPIRATIONS

    Although educational aspirations were an important predictor of eventual educa-

    tion and occupational status, their position in recent social science literature is more

    problematic. Starting with the early studies of status attainment, researchers argued

    that educational aspirations were a good indicator of students plans for the future

    (Sewell et al. 1969, 1970; Campbell 1983). Hence, they provided an important clue

    to a students eventual educational and occupational attainment. More recently, the

    concept of educational aspirations has been more controversial. Although few re-

    searchers would cast doubt on the claim that high aspirations are an important

    precursor to high attainment, it is unclear that modern survey instruments actually

    capture the difference between students who are seriously and actively thinking

    about college and those who simply report lofty goals (Alexander & Cook 1979,

    Jencks et al. 1983, Kao & Tienda 1998). Moreover, some argue that aspirations are

    simply a report of students likelihood of attending college and not a motivating

    factor per se (Alexander & Cook 1979, Jencks et al. 1983). However, most youth

    report extremely high educational aspirations, with most surveys reporting that the

    vast majority of youth expect to complete college. Moreover, Asian, black, and

    Hispanic youth all report much higher aspirations than would be expected given

    their SES (Kao & Tienda 1998). The problem of almost uniformly high educational

    aspirations among youth (but much lower subsequent attainment) has been well

    documented by others, such as Hansons Lost Talent(1994, 1996), and Schneider

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    7/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 423

    & Stevensons The Ambitious Generation: Americas Teenagers, Motivated but

    Directionless (1999). Although aspirations are correlated with grades, test scores,

    and eventual attainment, it is unclear what having high educational aspirations

    actually implies for todays youth.

    TRACKING AND COURSE TAKING IN HIGH SCHOOL

    Students are stratified within schools according to ability groups or tracks. Nu-

    merous studies have shown that poor children and racial and ethnic minorities

    are disproportionately placed in low-ability groups early in their educational ca-

    reers and in non-college-bound groupings in junior high and high school (Joseph

    1998, Slavin & Braddock 1993, Oakes 1985). Likewise, research shows that low-

    income and minority students participate at higher rates in vocational curricula andat lower rates in academic curricula than do affluent and white students (Oakes &

    Guiton 1995, Ekstrom et al. 1988, Oakes 1985). Recent statistics from the Digest

    of Education Statistics (National Center for Education Statistics 1997) report the

    following patterns: the percentage of high school seniors who reported being in the

    college preparatory or academic track were 46% of whites, 36% of blacks, 31%

    of Hispanics, 51% of Asians, and 23% of Native Americans. Those reporting in

    the general track were 43% of whites, 49% of blacks, 56% of Hispanics, 40% of

    Asians, and 61% of Native Americans. Finally, the percentages of each race/ethnic

    group reporting to be in the vocational track were 11% of whites, 15% of blacks,13% of Hispanics, 9% of Asians, and 17% of Native Americans (National Center

    for Education Statistics 1997). These statistics show that half of Asians and almost

    half of whites report being in the highest track. Blacks, Hispanics, and Native

    Americans are more likely to be in the general track, and they also have the high-

    est numbers in the lowest track (vocational). Similarly, another study found that

    nonblacks were almost three times more likely than blacks to be in the honors or

    advanced track in English and math (Kubitschek & Hallinan 1996). Thus, patterns

    of racial and ethnic disadvantage in tracking continue.

    There is mixed evidence on whether these racial and ethnic effects on track

    placement remain once controls, such as ability, are added to analytic models

    (Oakes et al. 1992). An older study found race effects disappear (blacks versus

    nonblacks) once test scores, academic orientation, course selection, and grades

    were controlled for (Alexander & Cook 1982). However, a more recent study

    found that once academic achievement was controlled, racial and ethnic differences

    decreased but did not disappear (Hallinan 1994). School track placements may also

    be influenced by students measured English-language ability, so that, for instance,

    otherwise talented and capable Mexican-origin students are placed in remedial or

    vocational tracks (Donato et al. 1991).

    The effects of track placement have also been extensively studied. Because

    racial and ethnic minorities are disproportionately in lower tracks, the effects of

    tracks will lead to differential outcomes. The general conclusion on the effects of

    tracks is that tracking and ability groups have a negative effect on the achievement

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    8/28

    424 KAO THOMPSON

    of lower track students, a negligible effect on students in the middle groups, and a

    weak-to-modest positive effect on students in the high tracks (Hallinan 1988,

    Oakes 1985, Eder & Felmee 1984, Sorensen & Hallinan 1986, Alexander &

    McDill 1976, Hauser & Featherman 1976, Heyns 1974). Further disadvantagesfor the lower tracks include the development of negative attitudes and behaviors

    related to learning (Hallinan 1988). On the other hand, placement in the college

    preparatory track in high school produces positive effects such as high academic

    achievement (grades, test scores), measures of motivation, and educational aspi-

    rations and attainment, even after controlling for family background and ability

    differences (Rosenbaum 1976, Alexander et al. 1978, Hauser & Featherman 1976,

    Alexander & Cook 1982). Similarly, research has shown that upper-track students

    obtain higher grades, are more likely to complete college, have more positive self-

    concepts, and have lower rates of misconduct and truancy, even after controllingfor home background variables (Ansalone 2001).

    Just as there are racial and ethnic differences in student track and ability group

    placement, there are also differences in the courses students take in high school.

    Of course, patterns of course taking are related to placement in specific tracks.

    The Digest of Education Statistics (National Center for Education Statistics 1997)

    examined the average number of Carnegie units earned in various subjects and

    found that Asian students had the highest number in math, science, and foreign

    language. The Carnegie unit represents one credit for the completion of a 1-year

    course (National Center for Education Statistics 1997). Native Americans had thelowest total units in math and foreign language, whereas blacks had the lowest

    in science. Blacks and Native Americans had the highest vocational education

    units, whereas Asians had the lowest. The Digest (National Center for Education

    Statistics 1997) also reported the percentages of high school graduates earning

    various combinations of credits in different subjects. The highest level (4-English,

    3-science, 3-math, 0.5-computer science, 2-foreign language) was earned by 27%

    of whites, 20% of blacks, 28% of Hispanics, 36% of Asians, and 13% of Native

    Americans. Other statistics find similar patterns. Miller (1995) found that blacks

    and Hispanics lagged substantially behind whites in enrollment of all math andscience courses except Algebra I and Biology in 1982 and 1987. However, from

    the 1980s to the 1990s, both black and white high school graduates were following

    a more rigorous curriculum. Yet, black high school graduates were still less likely

    than white graduates to take advanced science and math courses or study a foreign

    language (U.S. Department of Education 1995, Epps 1995). Mare (1995) also

    found an increase over time in the total number of courses and basic academic

    courses taken, especially among blacks and Hispanics.

    Even within tracks, racial and ethnic differences in course taking persist. Within

    the vocational area, low-income and minority students disproportionately takeclasses related to low-skill jobs, whereas white and affluent students more of-

    ten take courses that teach general skills or include considerable academic con-

    tent (Oakes 1983). On the academic side of the curriculum, low-income and

    non-Asian minority students disproportionately take low-level and remedial

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    9/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 425

    courses, whereas whites and Asians tend to dominate enrollments in advanced

    and honors courses (Braddock 1990, Oakes 1990).

    One reason for the differential course taking in high school is that different

    schools offer different courses. Low-income, urban schools do not offer the samerange and level of courses as their more affluent suburban counterparts. Urban

    schools are less likely to offer advanced courses or gifted and talented programs

    (Garibaldi 1998). Predominantly white and wealthy schools offer more high-ability

    classestwo to three times as many advanced placement courses per student as

    low-income, predominantly minority schoolsand a larger share of their students

    take these advanced classes (Orfield et al. 1996). In addition, differences in course

    participation are due to educators perceptions about race and class differences in

    academic motivations and abilities. Students and parents also make choices about

    course taking (Oakes & Guiton 1995).Both tracking and course taking have effects on educational outcomes. Gamoran

    (1987) found that tracking and course taking together accounted for substantively

    significant differences in student achievement. Thus, racial and ethnic differences

    in the tracks students are placed in, and the courses they take in high school, can

    lead to further differentiation in educational achievement and attainment.

    EDUCATIONAL ATTAINMENT

    Whereas educational performance is crucial for eventual success in higher educa-tion, educational attainment is key in affecting eventual labor market outcomes. In

    general, Asians have the highest probability of school progression at each level of

    schooling, followed by whites, blacks and Hispanics, and Native Americans (Mare

    1995). For all groups except Asians, family background explains a large proportion

    of the differences in educational attainment between white and nonwhite ethnic-

    racial groups. In many cases, family background explains one half to two thirds

    of the difference (Mare & Winship 1988). However, remaining group differences

    suggest that other factors affect the collective performance of each group. Immi-

    gration policy also plays a role, as requirements of family reunification and occu-pational qualifications have resulted in the need for formal credentials, and account

    for the high educational levels among some Asian immigrant groups (Portes &

    Rumbaut 1996). The sections below review racial and ethnic differences in edu-

    cational attainment at the high school and college levels.

    HIGH SCHOOL:DROPOUTSAND COMPLETION

    DroppingOut

    Dropping out of school is not evenly distributed racially, economically, or geo-

    graphically (McLaren 1988). Researchers have noted that blacks, and especially

    Hispanics and Native Americans, are significantly more likely than white or other

    minority students to drop out of school (Velez 1989, Warren 1996, Teachman

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    10/28

    426 KAO THOMPSON

    et al. 1996, White & Kaufman 1997). For instance, researchers analyzing High

    School and Beyond (HS&B) data, which is a nationally representative sample of

    high school sophomores and seniors in 1980, found that Asians had the lowest

    percentage of drop outs (14%), followed by non-Hispanic whites (17%). NativeAmericans had the highest level (29%) followed by Mexican Americans (28%) and

    Puerto Ricans (26%). Slightly less than one quarter (24%) of blacks dropped out

    of high school (White & Kaufman 1997). More recent statistics (National Center

    for Education Statistics 1997) found that among persons 1624 years old in 1996,

    7% of non-Hispanic whites, 13% of non-Hispanic blacks, and 29% of Hispanics

    were high school drop outs. However, these figures include youth who were not

    educated in the United States, thus underestimating the educational outcomes of

    Hispanics in the U.S. educational system.

    For Mexican Americans, who make up more than 75% of all Hispanics in theUnited States, this categorization is especially problematic because the average

    educational attainment of Mexicans who migrate is very low. Combining those who

    are educated in the United States with those who are educated in Mexico, many

    who have left school in Mexico and then migrated to the United States to work

    underestimate the educational attainment of Mexicans who receive their education

    in the United States. Again, after controllingfor differences in parental background,

    some researchers have found Hispanics to have higher odds of graduating from

    high school (Hauser & Anderson 1991).

    Once factors such as generation, language, and social capital are controlled for,ethnicity does not appear to have much impact on dropping out (White & Kaufman

    1997). However, when students performance and expectations as sophomores are

    taken out of the analytic model, ethnicity increases in importance. This indicates

    that there are substantial ethnic differences in school performance and expectations

    that lead to differences in dropping out of high school (White & Kaufman 1997).

    Others find that the greater likelihood of blacks to drop out can be explained by

    the relative lack of financial, human, and social capital (Teachman et al. 1996).

    Researchers have also examined the factors that predict dropping out of school

    for various racial and ethnic groups. Using HS&B data, White & Kaufman (1997)found that immigrants are more likely to drop out than are native-born persons

    of native parentage, and that this is especially true of recent immigrants. The

    authors found that social capital is also very important in reducing the likelihood

    of dropping out, and that it can buffer negative effects associated with foreign

    birth and low socioeconomic origins. This runs somewhat counter to research using

    more recent data and larger samples of Asian Americans (see Kao & Tienda 1995),

    which suggest that the effects of immigrant status may vary substantially by ethnic

    group and by period effects. Moreover, recent research by Louie (2001) suggest

    that there are significant socioeconomic differences within immigrant ethnics, andthis affects their ability to transform uniformly high parental aspirations.

    Rumberger (1995) notes some differences between groups. He found that SES

    predicted drop-out rates for Hispanics and whites, but not blacks. Misbehavior,

    changing schools, and low grades all increased the odds of dropping out for blacks

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    11/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 427

    and whites, but not Hispanics. High school absenteeism predicted dropping out

    for all groups. Students who attend low-SES schools as well as schools that are

    predominantly black or Hispanic also experience higher drop-out rates, although

    for minority students, the effect of attending a predominantly black school islargely accounted for by the low mean SES of these schools (Mayer 1991). Velez

    (1989) examined the differences in drop-out predictors among Hispanics. Cutting

    classes, suspensions, dating, being older, and being female increased the odds of

    Chicano students dropping out. Others find that large families, Spanish-language

    dominance, foreign birth, urban environment, and lower-quality schools lessen

    the likelihood that the Mexican-American student will stay in school (Fligstein &

    Fernandez 1985). However, bilingualism has positive or neutral effects on edu-

    cational achievement (Fernandez & Nielsen 1986, Mouw & Xie 1999). Among

    Cuban students, suspensions increased the odds of dropping out, but having disci-plinary problems at school, high SES, and having two parents at home substantially

    decreased them. For Puerto Rican students, cutting classes, suspension, being older,

    and being female increased the odds, but having two parents at home decreased

    them. For non-Hispanic whites, dating, being older, and being female increased the

    odds of dropping out (Velez 1989). Immigration also had different effects. Cuban

    students who were relatively new arrivals in the United States were less likely to

    drop out, whereas their Puerto Rican and Chicano counterparts were more likely

    to drop out. Family background (high SES) reduced the odds that all students

    would drop out, although the effects of SES were particularly strong in the caseof Cubans (Velez 1989).

    HighSchool Completion

    The flip side of dropping out of high school is, of course, graduating from high

    school. All racial and ethnic groups have increased their average rates of school

    continuation and levels of educational attainment over time (Mare 1995). In 1990,

    among adults 25 years and older, approximately 78% of whites, 63% of blacks,

    50% of Hispanics, 78% of Asians, and 66% of Native Americans or Alaskan Na-tives had a high school diploma (National Center for Education Statistics 1997).

    This figure obscures those who were educated in the United States versus those

    who were not. Among most groups, a majority of members graduate from high

    school. When groups are further broken down into subgroups and by immigrant

    status, more variation is evident (Mare & Winship 1988, Mare 1995). For exam-

    ple, in 1980 (persons aged 2635), among foreign-born Mexican Americans, only

    29% of men and 27% of women had graduated high school. Native-born Mex-

    ican Americans also had relatively low rates of graduation (68% for men, 64%

    for women). Puerto Rican rates were even lower (54% for men, 53% for women).Groups with the highest high school graduation rates were native-born Japanese

    Americans (98% of men and women), native-born Chinese Americans (97% of

    men and 96% of women), foreign-born Japanese Americans (96% of men and 92%

    of women), and native and foreign-born Asian Indians (men have slightly higher

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    12/28

    428 KAO THOMPSON

    rates at 90%94% versus women, 78%87%). However, even among Asian Amer-

    icans, much variation exists as Japanese, Chinese, and Asian Indians have much

    higher graduation rates than Laotians, Hmong, and Vietnamese (Miller 1995).

    In general, Asian Americans have higher educational attainment levels than Pa-cific Islanders. Among Hispanics, Cubans have the highest high school graduation

    rates, followed by Central/South Americans, Puerto Ricans, and, lastly, Mexican

    Americans (Miller 1995).

    As in the research on dropping out of high school, researchers have simi-

    larly examined the predictors of completing high school. One area of interest has

    been the effects of family structure and other family background characteristics.

    Wojtkiewicz (1993) found that years spent in mother-only or stepparent families

    had a negative effect on high school graduation; however, a race difference was

    found, where mother-stepfather families were not negative for black students. Thiseffect of family structure on high school completion would have differential effects

    because blacks have the highest degree of experience with nonintact families, fol-

    lowed by Hispanics and whites (Wojtkiewicz 1993, Krein & Beller 1988). Duncan

    (1994), however, found that female family headship had a positive association with

    school completion for black females. Warren (1996) found that a great deal of the

    observed gap between whites and Mexican immigrant adolescents in the odds of

    completing high school and nearly all of the gap between whites and Chicano

    adolescents can be attributed to group differences in family background (parents

    education and occupation, family structure, siblings). However, even after numer-ous controls, Mexican-origin students are still less likely to complete twelfth grade,

    suggesting that issues such as discrimination, school segregation, or differences

    in aspirations play significant roles in creating and maintaining inequalities in ed-

    ucational attainment (Warren 1996). Wojtkiewicz (1993) also found that although

    family structure mattered (parental structure, number of siblings), family SES had

    a stronger effect on educational attainment. The effects do vary by group, how-

    ever. For example, background variables may be more powerful predictors of high

    school completion among whites than Mexican Americans (Fligstein & Fernandez

    1985). Generation also differentially affects high school completion. For Hispan-ics, high school completion rates increase with each generation, whereas for whites

    and Asians, they increase from the immigrant to second generation only, with little

    difference between the second generation and the native born of native parentage

    (Rong & Grant 1992). However, White & Glick (2000), using HS&B, found that

    immigrant youth who arrived as adolescents were more likely to persevere through

    high school, compared with both native-born and immigrant youth who arrived at

    an earlier age, despite having lower levels of parental SES and social capital.

    COLLEGE TRANSITION

    Although in recent years most high school seniors plan to go to college right

    after high school (in 1992, 77% of whites, 75% of blacks and Hispanics, 83% of

    Asians, and 66% of Native Americans), much fewer actually make the transition

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    13/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 429

    to college (National Center for Education Statistics 1997). College enrollment

    of blacks and Hispanics has increased over time, although these improvements

    slowed in the 1980s (Baker & Velez 1996). The enrollment rates of 1824 year

    olds in institutions of higher education in 1996 show racial and ethnic variation.Forty-five percent of non-Hispanic whites, 36% of non-Hispanic blacks, and 35%

    of Hispanics were enrolled (enrollment as a percent of high school graduates)

    in postsecondary institutions in 1996 (National Center for Education Statistics

    1997). Rates of college attendance for Asian groups are extremely high; except for

    Vietnamese, the rates of persons aged 2635 in 1980 with some college are close to

    or above 50% and in many cases as high as 7080% (Mare & Winship 1988). Some

    of the highest rates are among foreign-born Asians. Non-Hispanic blacks had rates

    of 33%34% (depending on gender), non-Hispanic whites were 44%54%, and

    Native Americans had lower rates of 30%36%. Among Hispanics, Cubans hadhigher rates than Mexican Americans and the foreign born were worse off than the

    native born. Lowest overall rates were among foreign-born Mexican women; only

    11% completed some college. Persistence in college also varied by group. Peng

    (1988) found that 86% of Asian Americans versus 64% of whites were found in

    some kind of higher education program 2 years after high school graduation. For

    those who entered a 4-year university, 86% of Asians stayed the following year

    compared with 75% of whites, 71% of blacks, and 66% of Hispanics.

    Hauser & Anderson (1991) found that the chances of college entry declined

    among blacks from the 1970s to the 1980s, and that this change could not beattributed to changing aspirations. In 1984, the odds that a black high school

    graduate would enter the first year of college within a year were less than half

    of the corresponding odds for a white high school graduate. Blacks are still less

    likely than whites to make an immediate transition from high school to college

    (U.S. Department of Education 1995). The decline in black college enrollments

    has been attributed in part to decreases in the amount and form of financial aid

    (Hauser 1992). During the 1970s, black high school graduates were more likely to

    enter college than were white high school graduates with the same family income.

    However, after 1980, differences in family income could no longer account forblacks lower odds of enrollment in college (Hauser & Anderson 1991). Older

    studies have also found that once background and aptitude are taken into account,

    traditionally disadvantaged minorities had equal or greater chances of attending

    college (Mare & Winship 1988, Rumberger 1982, Thomas et al. 1979). Further-

    more, for students with similar levels of academic aptitude, lower-class minority

    students had higher rates of college attendance than did lower-class white students

    (Alexander et al. 1987). Hallinan & Williams (1990) found that blacks had higher

    aspirations than whites but were less likely to be attending college 2 years after

    high school graduation. However, once SES and other variables were taken into ac-count, blacks had higher aspirations and higher outcomes (although the latter was

    not statistically significant). Interestingly, both black and white students who had

    cross-race friendships had higher educational aspirations and outcomes than did

    those with same-race friendships. This indicates a peer effect, although cross-race

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    14/28

    430 KAO THOMPSON

    friendships are rare (Hallinan & Williams 1990). The low college attendance rates

    of Hispanics, especially Mexican Americans, are due to low high school gradua-

    tion rates. When only the population of high school graduates is considered, some

    find that Mexican Americans attend college at a higher rate than whites (Fligstein& Fernandez 1985).

    Racial and ethnic groups vary in the kind of postsecondary institutions they

    attend. However, SES matters more than race and ethnicity for entry into selec-

    tive institutions (Hearn 1991). Racial and ethnic minorities are disproportionately

    represented in community colleges, which have poorer outcomes than other post-

    secondary schools (Brint & Karabel 1989, Dougherty 1994). The concentration

    is especially true of Hispanics (Lee & Frank 1990). In 1992, approximately 57%

    of Hispanics in higher education were enrolled in 2-year colleges, compared with

    39% of all students (U.S. Department of Education 1994). There is some evidencethat students of color have become more concentrated over time in the 2-year sec-

    tor than whites (Karen 2002). In addition, minorities were somewhat more likely

    than whites to attend school part-time (Rumberger 1982). Blacks were less likely

    to attend selective institutions, net of social background factors, such as parents

    education and income, and number of siblings (Karen 2002, Hearn 1991). The

    effect was reduced but still remained when academic controls, such as test scores,

    grades, expectations, and activities were added to the analytic model. Hispanics

    (controlling for various factors) attended schools that were as selective as the

    schools whites attended (Karen 2002). There appears to be some leveling off inthe past couple of decades in the movement of minorities into top-tier institutions

    (Karen 1991).

    COLLEGE COMPLETION

    Persistence through college and earning a bachelors degree are important markers

    that influence future labor market outcomes. Between the transition from some

    college to the attainment of the bachelors degree, some racial and ethnic differ-

    ences grew between 1980 and 1990 (Mare 1995). Progression probabilities grew

    for Asians and whites, whereas other groups remained unchanged. Mare (1995)

    explained this process where inequality in education attainment moves from ear-

    lier to later stages of schooling as average levels of attainment increase for all

    groups. In other words, inequalities persist or even increase at the postsecondary

    level as groups become more equal at lower levels of education. Recent statistics

    show that of persons aged 25 and older in 1990, 22% of whites, 11% of blacks, 9%

    of Hispanics, 37% of Asians and Pacific Islanders, and 9% of Native Americans

    or Alaskan Natives had earned a bachelors degree or higher (National Center for

    Education Statistics 1997). Asians are the most likely to complete college (exceed-

    ing the national average), followed by whites, blacks and Hispanics, and Native

    Americans. Variation exists within racial and ethnic groups. For instance, 26% of

    Japanese, 37% of Chinese, and 52% of Asian Indians had completed 4 or more

    years of college in 1980 (persons aged 2529), whereas only 6% of Laotians, 3%

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    15/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 431

    of Hmong, 13% of Vietnamese, 10% of Native Hawaiians, 11% of Melanesians,

    and 7% of Samoans did the same. Among Hispanics (aged 25 and over) in 1990,

    20% of Cubans, 16% of Central/South Americans, 10% of Puerto Ricans, and 5%

    of Mexican Americans had completed 4 or more years of college.Greater proportions of blacks, Hispanics, and Native Americans than whites

    drop out of college (Kalsner 1991). Camburn (1990) found that whites are con-

    siderably more likely than blacks and Hispanics to obtain bachelors degrees even

    when SES and college plans are controlled for. He found that on average, whites

    are about twice as likely as minorities to finish college (his sample included high

    school graduates in large metropolitan areas). High-SES students and those from

    high schools with higher percentages of white students were more likely to finish

    college (Camburn 1990). Controlling for high school grades and test scores negated

    the effect of race, indicating that racial differences in college completion may bedue to differences in academic preparation (Camburn 1990). Others have similarly

    seen race effects disappear in multivariate analyses (Velez 1985, Donovan 1984).

    In addition, blacks take longer than whites to complete college (U.S. Department

    of Education 1995). Just as some minority students attended less-selective schools,

    black and Hispanic students obtain their degrees at somewhat less prestigious in-

    stitutions than do whites, whereas Asian Americans are found in somewhat more

    prestigious universities (Jacobs 1996).

    EXPLANATIONSFORSECONDARY SCHOOLACHIEVEMENT DIFFERENCES

    Importanceof Parental SocioeconomicStatus

    Parental education and family income is probably the best predictor of even-

    tual academic outcomes among youth. These differences are substantial across

    race, ethnic, and immigrant groups, and help to explain a substantial proportion

    (although not all) of the variation in educational outcomes of youth. Hispanics

    (especially Mexican Americans, who make up the majority of the Hispanic popu-

    lation) are most disadvantaged in terms of parental education levels. After taking

    parental SES into account, most Hispanics perform at levels comparable to whites

    in statistical analyses (Kao et al. 1996, Warren 1996). Warren (1996) found that

    background differences accounted for most of the differences between native-born

    Mexican Americans and whites. Thus, although the lower academic achievement

    of Hispanics is problematic, their levels of achievement are comparable to that of

    whites from similar SES backgrounds.

    Asian American youth are extremely advantaged in terms of parental educa-

    tion levels. This advantage explains some of the relatively high performance and

    attainment of Asian American youth, but not all. In fact, several researchers have

    found that parental education usually does not explain any of the variation in grades

    within the Asian American population (Caplan et al. 1991, Kao 1995). In addi-

    tion, there is considerable heterogeneity among Asian American ethnic groups

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    16/28

    432 KAO THOMPSON

    (Kao 1995, Blair & Qian 1998). For instance, Kao (1995), using data from the

    National Education Longitudinal Study of 1988 (NELS), found that 81% of all

    South Asian youth had at least one parent with a college degree. In contrast, only

    20% of Southeast Asian youth had at least one parent with a college degree. (Thepercentage of white youth with one college-educated parent was 35%.)

    What gets missed when we use the large panethnic labels such as Asian and

    Hispanic is the great diversity of social class differences by immigrant groups. This

    difference is crucial as the vast majority of Asian American as well as a substantial

    portion of Hispanic parents are foreign born. So, whereas many Asian immigrants

    arrive in the United States with high levels of educational attainment and job skills,

    some Asian ethnic groups are extremely disadvantaged. For instance, only 25.5%

    of Mexican immigrants aged 25 and over in 1990 had a high school diploma, and

    only 3.5% had a college degree. These figures stand in stark contrast to those ofAsian ethnic groups64.9% of Indian immigrants aged 25 and over graduated

    from college, and 87.2% graduated from high school; 62.2% of Taiwanese immi-

    grants graduated from college, and 91.6% graduated from high school; 43% of

    Filipinos graduated from college, and 82.% graduated from high school; 34.4% of

    Koreans graduated from college, and 80.1% graduated from high school, and so

    forth (figures from table 4 in Portes & Rumbaut 1996).

    Although there is some rationale for the broad characterization of Asian Amer-

    icans as a model minority group, it is misleading and damaging to ethnic groups

    that are extremely disadvantaged but happen to be classified under the rubric ofAsian Americans. The term itself implies that minorities cannot have any posi-

    tive characteristics but that the model minority should be used as an example to

    all other minorities. Moreover, it suggests that the United States provides equal

    opportunities for all, because some minority groups have made it. For example,

    only 35% of Cambodian foreign-born adults (aged 25 or over) have a high school

    diploma, and only 5% have a 4-year college degree; similarly, 37% of foreign-born

    Laotians have a high school diploma, and only 4.6% have a 4-year college degree

    (figures from Table 4 in Portes & Rumbaut 1996).

    BeyondParental SocioeconomicStatus

    More recently, researchers have taken parental SES effects on academic achieve-

    ment as a given, and have added more complex measures of changing SES over

    time (Roscigno 1998, 2000). What motivates current debates is how to describe

    the remaining racial and ethnic variation in academic outcomes net of these ef-

    fects. Some researchers rely on other structural characteristics such as the quality

    of schools, peers, and neighbors; these characteristics, although correlated with

    parental SES, vary by ethnic group membership. In fact, some of the later statusattainment researchers examined these differences. Alwin & Otto (1977) examined

    school SES and school ability levels and found that they did not affect college plans

    and occupational aspirations but that they may affect other intervening characteris-

    tics, such as curriculum placement of students and the college plans of their peers.

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    17/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 433

    While a discussion of these studies is beyond the scope of this review, the average

    effects of school quality of individual performance is rather modest (Thornton &

    Eckland 1980, Hoffer et al. 1985, Keith & Page 1985, Bryk et al. 1993, Arum

    2000).Other studies look toward social capital of particular ethnic groups. For instance,

    Caplan and his colleagues (1991) argued that the high academic performance of

    Southeast Asian children from refugee families has to do with children tutoring

    each other on their homework. This was an effective way for older siblings to

    review school materials and for younger children to learn. Others have argued that

    perhaps the advantage of some Asian American groups comes from group-specific

    social capital, such as policing by other same-ethnic parents, which may prevent

    delinquent behavior (see Zhou & Bankston 1998). Similarly, one could argue that

    some Asian American groups (Chinese and Koreans, in particular) are advantagedfrom the many cram schools that offer advanced after-school training, or that

    South Asian youth are advantaged from their parental networks of mostly upper-

    middle-class friends. This is an important area that warrants further research.

    Finally, most of the understanding of the academic success of Asian Americans

    points to their positive cultural beliefs about the benefits of education. Although

    cultural deprivation models are out of favor among social scientists in explaining

    the lower performance of blacks, cultural models are popular for explaining the

    relatively higher performance of Asians (see Spencer 1990, Spencer et al. 1991,

    Caplan et al. 1991). Caplan and associates (1991) argue that Southeast Asians havea cultural understanding that prioritizes self-reliance and achievement. Fuligni

    (1997, 1998) found that immigrant youth (including Asians) were more likely to

    believe in education. Schneider & Lee (1990) argued that Asian youth felt a greater

    obligation to their immigrant parents and believed that it was their responsibility

    to the family to do well in school. Sue & Okazaki (1990) cite a number of studies

    that found Asian Americans to be more likely than whites to believe in the value

    of education for future socioeconomic mobility. Sue & Okazaki (1990) argued

    that anticipated discrimination leads Asian Americans to overperform in school.

    Interestingly, Ogbu (1991) argued that anticipated discrimination causes blacks towithdraw from academic activities.

    Despite the lower academic performance of Hispanics and their disadvantaged

    parental SES characteristics, they share some commonalities with Asian Ameri-

    cans. Valenzuela & Dornbusch (1994) argued that familism, or the valuation of

    close ties to family members, is an important form of social capital and was asso-

    ciated with higher academic achievement. Although it is outside the scope of this

    paper, there is a growing literature that argues that newer immigrant groups perform

    better in school because they are more likely to respect authority (Suarez-Orozco

    & Suarez-Orozco 1995), see their lives in the United States as an opportunityfor advancement, and come from families that are optimistic about their eventual

    attainment (Kao & Tienda 1995; see Zhou 1997 for an excellent review). Suarez-

    Orozco & Suarez-Orozco (1995) take their clue from McClellands studies of the

    achievement motivation and find that Mexican American youth born in Mexico

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    18/28

    434 KAO THOMPSON

    have higher achievement motivations than their native-born Mexican American or

    white counterparts.

    Perhaps the most influential theorist of minority school performance is anthro-

    pologist Ogbu (1974, 1991). He has argued that African American youth developan oppositional identity relative to whites. In other words, because the group iden-

    tity of blacks focuses on their collective experiences of discrimination, they define

    themselves in opposition to the dominant group (whites). Thus, they develop dis-

    tinct cultural and language norms to maintain their group identity (Gibson & Ogbu

    1991). Their parents past experiences with discrimination makes them distrust the

    dominant society, and makes them less likely to believe that schooling leads to

    socioeconomic mobility. Elsewhere, Fordham & Ogbu (1986) argued that notions

    of acting black and acting white become identified in opposition to one an-

    other. Hence, because acting white includes doing well in school, acting blacknecessarily implies not doing well in school (Fordham & Ogbu 1986).

    Another line of rationale, most notably posed by psychologist Steele (1997,

    Steele & Aronson 1995), is that stereotypes can shape how minorities perform in

    testing situations. For instance, he argues that in areas where groups are negatively

    stereotyped, the threat of that stereotype can dampen their test performance. Under

    experimental conditions where the stereotype is removed, these groups perform

    better than under the threat of negative stereotypes (Steele & Aronson 1995, Steele

    1997). Most of the empirical work has been conducted on blacks versus whites or

    women versus men in various experimental conditions.Recent attention to other minority groups has complicated these theoretical

    explanations. Gibson & Ogbu (1991) published a volume that examined ethnic

    differences in academic performance in comparative contexts, which led Ogbu

    to revise his theoretical model. Although blacks and some Hispanic groups fit

    his notion of involuntary minorities whose past experiences with discrimination

    makes them dubious of the fact that education leads to socioeconomic mobility

    for them, most Asian groups fit his notion of voluntary minorities who migrated

    to the United States (or other locales) by choice and are more likely to compare

    their current positions to their peers in their home country. Voluntary minoritiesare more likely to believe that they will be rewarded for their investments in ed-

    ucation. Although this typology is seductive, it leads to considerable confusion

    given the growing diversity of immigrants to the United States The confusion was

    even apparent in Ogbus own book (Gibson & Ogbu 1991), which at times treated

    refugees as voluntary migrants and at other times as involuntary migrants. Simi-

    larly, in direct opposition to Ogbus argument, many others have found that black

    youth have extremely high educational aspirations (Hauser & Anderson 1991, Kao

    & Tienda 1998, Ainsworth-Darnell & Downey 1998). Mickelson (1990) argued

    that black youth may have abstract attitudes that are congruent with achievementbut that their concrete attitudes regarding whether educational success will bring

    them socioeconomic mobility match more closely with Ogbus notion of an op-

    positional identity. She argues that although blacks are just as likely as whites

    to have positive abstract attitudes toward schooling, they are less likely to have

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    19/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 435

    positive concrete attitudes, which explains their lower achievement scores. Her

    study, however, does not resolve the issue of causalityit may be that lower-

    achieving youth are more likely to hold negative concrete attitudes about school-

    ing and not vice versa. More recently, Ainsworth-Darnell & Downey (1998) usedNELS to examine the key elements of Ogbus oppositional culture explanation and

    found that empirical analyses did not support Ogbus theory.

    Whereas Ogbu focuses on the mode of entry for immigrant groups, others have

    focused on the communities to which immigrants move. More recently, Portes,

    Zhou, and Rumbaut (Portes & Zhou 1993, Portes & Rumbaut 1996, Zhou 1997,

    Zhou & Bankston 1998) have argued for the concept of segmented assimilation in

    understanding differential outcomes of immigrant groups. They argue that because

    immigrants attempt to assimilate into their local communities, what assimilation

    means for immigrants varies a great deal. If the local majority population areinner-city African Americans, as was the case for Zhou & Bankstons (1998) study

    of Vietnamese American youth, then assimilation had a very negative implication

    to the educational and delinquent outcomes of youth. In contrast, if immigrants

    hope to assimilate to a high-SES suburban community, then that desire should lead

    to positive educational outcomes.

    CONCLUSION

    Overall, there are many signs of optimism. Racial and ethnic gaps in educationalachievement and attainment have narrowed over the past 3 decades by every mea-

    sure available to social scientists. Educational aspirations are universally high for

    all racial and ethnic groups (Hauser & Anderson 1991, Kao & Tienda 1998, Goyette

    & Xie 1999) as most adolescents expect to go to college. However, substantial gaps

    remain, especially between less-advantaged groups such as African Americans,

    Hispanics, and Native Americans and more advantaged groups such as whites and

    Asian Americans. The racial and ethnic hierarchy in educational achievement is

    apparent across varying measures of the academic experience.

    However, there is less consistency in what factors account for racial, ethnic,and immigrant differences in achievement and attainment (Gottfredson 1981).

    There is some evidence that whereas racial and ethnic gaps at the lowest levels

    have improved, some racial and ethnic patterns are more apparent at the highest

    levels of achievement. For instance, although black and Hispanic students are more

    likely to attend college than ever before, they are more likely than whites or Asians

    to attend a community college than a 4-year institution. Even among those who

    attend a 4-year college, they are more likely to attend less-prestigious institutions

    than whites or Asians (although this difference disappears for Hispanics once

    background characteristics are taken into account) (Karen 2002).The broad racial comparisons obscure considerable heterogeneity within the

    panethnic groups. High-achieving Asian American groups, such as South Asians,

    Chinese, and Koreans outperform whites on a number of measures, but

    low-achieving Asian American groups, such as Cambodians and Laotians, have

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    20/28

    436 KAO THOMPSON

    outcomes comparable to African Americans. Similarly, Hispanics, Cubans, and

    to a slightly less extent South and Central Americans have much higher educa-

    tional outcomes than Mexicans. Immigrants add to the complexity of the minority

    population of the United States, contributing youth from both high- and low-SESbackgrounds. Some evidence suggests that immigrant children outperform their

    same-ethnic counterparts with comparable parental backgrounds (Rumbaut 1990;

    Gibson & Ogbu 1991; Kao & Tienda 1995; Suarez-Orozco 1989; Zhou 1997; R.G.

    Rumbaut, unpublished manuscript; Goyette & Xie 1999). However, others find that

    immigrant children have poorer school outcomes than their native-born counter-

    parts (of native-born parents) (White & Kaufman 1997). In terms of absolute levels

    of achievement, recent immigrants often lag behind native-born minorities in their

    educational attainment.

    Given the greater cultural heterogeneity of students in the United States, re-searchers need to consider that a single model of achievement may not suffice

    (see Cooper 1990). Immigrant and minority families may work differently in

    translating aspirations into achievement and attainment. Although parental SES

    accounts for a substantial portion of the racial and ethnic gaps in achievement and

    attainment, there is clearly a place for explanations that do not rely solely on social

    class. Finally, researchers might consider how immigrants and minority families

    and youth contribute to our knowledge of how individuals succeed in light of great

    obstacles. Most of our studies consider minority and immigrant status as liabilities

    to overcome, but it is likely that there are benefits to minority and immigrant groupmembership.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    The authors wish to acknowledge support from the Spencer Foundation through a

    Spencer Small Grant awarded to the principal author. We thank Kim Goyette and

    Emily Hannum for their helpful comments.

    The Annual Review of Sociology is online at http://soc.annualreviews.org

    LITERATURECITED

    Ainsworth-Darnell JM, Downey DB. 1998. As-

    sessing the oppositional culture explanation

    for racial/ethnic differences in school perfor-

    mance. Am. Sociol. Rev. 63:53653

    Alexander KL, Cook MA. 1979. The motiva-

    tional relevance of educational plans: ques-

    tioning the conventional wisdom. Soc. Psy-

    chol. Q. 42:20213

    Alexander KL, Cook MA. 1982. Curricula and

    coursework: a surprise ending to a familiar

    story. Am. Sociol. Rev. 47:62640

    Alexander KL, Cook MA, McDill EL. 1978.

    Curriculum tracking and educational strati-

    fication: some further evidence. Am. Sociol.

    Rev. 43:4766

    Alexander KL, McDill EL. 1976. Selection and

    allocation within schools: some causes and

    consequence of curriculum placement. Am.

    Sociol. Rev. 41:4766

    Alexander KL, Pallas AM, Holupka S. 1987.

    Consistency and change in educational strat-

    ification: recent trends regarding social

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    21/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 437

    background and college access. In Research

    in Social Stratification and Mobility, ed. RV

    Robinson, pp. 16185. Greenwich, CT: JAI

    Alwin DF, Otto LB. 1977. High school context

    effects on aspirations. Sociol. Educ. 50:259

    73

    Ansalone G. 2001. Schooling, tracking, and in-

    equality. J. Child. Poverty 7:3347

    Arum R. 2000. Schools and communities: eco-

    logical and institutional dimensions. Annu.

    Rev. Sociol. 26:395418

    Astone NM, McLanahan SS. 1991. Family

    structure, parental practices and high school

    completion. Am. Sociol. Rev. 56:30920Baker DP, Stevenson DL. 1986. Mothers

    strategies for childrens school achievement:

    managing the transition to high school. So-

    ciol. Educ. 59:15666

    Baker TL, Velez W. 1996. Access to and op-

    portunity in postsecondary education in the

    United States: a review. Sociol. Educ. 69:82

    101

    Bankston CL III, Caldas SJ. 1998. Family

    structure, schoolmates, and racial inequali-ties in school achievement.J. Marriage Fam.

    60:71523

    Blair SL, Qian Z. 1998. Family and Asian stu-

    dents educational performance: a consider-

    ation of diversity.J. Family Issues 19:35574

    Braddock JH. 1990. Tracking: Implications for

    Student Race-Ethnic Subgroups. Tech. Rep.

    No. 1, Cent. Res. Eff. Sch. Disadv. Stud., Bal-

    timore, MD

    Brint S, Karabel J. 1989. The Diverted Dream:Community College and the Promise of Edu-

    cational Opportunity in America. New York:

    Oxford Univ. Press

    Bryk AS, Lee VE, Holland PB. 1993. Catholic

    Schools and the Common Good. Cambridge,

    MA: Harvard Univ. Press

    Camburn EM. 1990. College completion

    among students from high schools located

    in large metropolitan areas. Am. J. Educ.

    98:55169

    Campbell RT. 1983. Status attainment research:

    end of the beginning or beginning of the end?

    Sociol. Educ. 56:4762

    Caplan N, Choy MH, Whitmore JK. 1991. Chil-

    dren of the Boat People: A Study of Educa-

    tional Success. Ann Arbor: Univ. Mich. Press

    Coleman JS. 1961. The Adolescent Society: The

    Social Life of the Teenager and its Impact on

    Education. New York: Free Press

    Cooper AM. 1990. Fallacy of a single model for

    school achievement: considerations for eth-

    nicity. Sociol. Perspect. 33:15984

    Corsaro WA, Eder D. 1990. Childrens peer cul-

    tures. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 16:197220

    DiMaggio P. 1982. Cultural capital and school

    success: the impact of status culture partici-

    pation on the grades of U.S. high school stu-

    dents. Am. Sociol. Rev. 47:189201Donato R, Menchaca M, Valencia RR. 1991.

    Segregation, desegregation, and integration

    of Chicano students:problems and prospects.

    In Chicano School Failure and Success: Re-

    search and Policy Agendas for the 1990s,

    ed. RR Valencia, pp. 2763. London: Falmer

    Press

    Donovan R. 1984. Path analysis of a theoreti-

    cal model of persistence in higher education

    among low-income black youth. Res. High.Educ. 21:24352

    Dornbusch SM. 1989. The sociology of adoles-

    cence. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 15:23359

    Dougherty KJ. 1994. The Contradictory Col-

    lege: The Conflicting Origins, Impacts and

    Futures of the Community College. Albany:

    State Univ. NY Press

    Dreeben R, Gamoran A. 1986. Race, instruc-

    tion, and learning. Am. Sociol. Rev. 51:660

    69Duncan GJ. 1994. Families and neighbors as

    sources of disadvantage in the schooling de-

    cisions of white and black adolescents. Am.

    J. Educ. 103:2053

    Eder D, Felmiee D. 1984. The development of

    attention norms in ability groups. In The So-

    cial Context of Instruction: Group Organi-

    zation and Group Processes, ed. L Peterson,

    LC Wilkinson, MT Hallinan, pp. 189208.

    New York: Academic Press

    Ekstrom RB, Goertz ME, Rock DA. 1988. Ed-

    ucation and American Youth. Philadelphia:

    Falmer Press

    Entwisle DR, Alexander KL. 1993. Entry into

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    22/28

    438 KAO THOMPSON

    school: the beginning school transition and

    educational stratification in the United States.

    Annu. Rev. Sociol. 19:40123

    Epps EG. 1995. Race, class, and educational

    opportunity: trends in the sociology of edu-

    cation. Sociol. Forum. 10(4):593608

    Farkas G, Grobe RP, Sheehan D, Shuan Y.

    1990. Cultural resources and school success:

    gender, ethnicity, and poverty groups within

    an urban school district. Am. Sociol. Rev.

    55:12742

    Fehrman P, Keith TZ, Reimers TM. 1987.

    Home influence on school learning: direct

    and indirect effects of parental involve-ment on high school grades. J. Educ. Res.

    80(6):33037

    Fernandez RM, Nielsen F. 1986. Bilingualism

    and Hispanic scholastic achievement: some

    baseline results. Soc. Sci. Res. 15:4370

    Fligstein N, Fernandez RM. 1985. Educa-

    tional transitions of whites and Mexican-

    Americans. In Hispanics in the U.S. Econ-

    omy, ed. GJ Borjas, M Tienda, pp. 161192.

    Orlando: AcademicFordham S, Ogbu JU. 1986. Black students

    school success: coping with the burden of

    acting white. Urban Rev. 18:176206

    Fuligni AJ. 1997. The academic achievement

    of adolescents from immigrant families: the

    roles of family background, attitudes, and be-

    havior. Child Dev. 68:35163

    Fuligni AJ. 1998. Adolescents from immigrant

    families. In Studying Minority Adolescents:

    Conceptual, Methodological, and Theoreti-cal Issues, ed. VC McLoyd, L Steinberg, pp.

    12743. Mahwah, NJ: Earlbaum

    Gamoran A. 1987. The stratification of high

    school learning opportunities. Sociol. Educ.

    60:13555

    Garibaldi AM. 1998. Four decades of pro-

    cess . . . anddecline: an assessment of African

    American educational attainment. J. Negro

    Educ. 66(2):10520

    Gibson MA, Ogbu JU. 1991. Minority Sta-

    tus and Schooling: A Comparative Study of

    Immigrant and Involuntary Minorities. New

    York: Garland

    Gottfredson DC. 1981. Black-white differences

    in the educational attainment process: what

    have we learned?Am. Sociol. Rev. 46:54257

    Goyette K, Xie Y. 1999. Educational expec-

    tations of Asian American youths: determi-

    nants and ethnic differences. Sociol. Educ.

    72:2236

    Guo G. 1998. The timing of the influences of

    cumulative poverty on childrens cognitive

    ability and achievement. Soc. Forces 77:257

    88

    Hallinan MT. 1988. Equality of educational op-

    portunity. Annu. Rev. Sociol. 14:24968

    Hallinan MT. 1994. Tracking: from theory to

    practice. Sociol. Educ. 67(2):7984Hallinan MT, Williams RA. 1990. Students

    characteristics and the peer-influence pro-

    cess. Sociol. Educ. 63:12232

    Hanson SL. 1994. Lost talent: unrealized edu-

    cational aspirations and expectations among

    U.S. youths. Sociol. Educ. 67(3):15983

    Hanson SL. 1996. Lost Talent: Women in the

    Sciences. Philadelphia: Temple Univ. Press

    Hauser RM. 1992. The decline in college en-

    try among African-Americans: findings insearch of an explanation. In Prejudice, Poli-

    tics, and Race in America Today, eds. P Sni-

    derman, P Tetlock, E Carmines, pp. 271306.

    Stanford: Stanford Univ. Press

    Hauser RM, Anderson DK. 1991. Post-high

    school plans and aspirations of black and

    white high school seniors: 197686. Sociol.

    Educ. 64:26377

    Hauser RM, Featherman DL. 1976. Equality

    of schooling: trends and prospects. Sociol.Educ. 49:99120

    Hearn JC. 1991. Academic and nonacademic

    influences on the college destinationsof 1980

    high school graduates. Sociol. Educ. 64:158

    71

    Herrnstein RJ, Murray C. 1994. The Bell Curve:

    Intelligence and Class Structure in American

    Life. New York: Free Press

    Heyns B. 1974. Social selection and strat-

    ification within schools. Am. J. Sociol.

    79(6):143451

    Hoffer T, Greeley AM, Coleman JS. 1985.

    Achievement growth in public and Catholic

    schools. Sociol. Educ. 58:7497

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    23/28

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    24/28

    440 KAO THOMPSON

    Miller LS. 1995. An American Imperative: Ac-

    celerating Minority Educational Advance-

    ment. New Haven: Yale Univ. Press

    Mouw T, Xie Y. 1999. Bilingualism and the

    academic achievement of first- and second-

    generation Asian Americans: accommoda-

    tion with or without assimilation? Am. So-

    ciol. Rev. 64:23252

    Murnane RJ, Maynard RA, Ohls JC. 1980.

    Home resources and childrens achievement.

    Rev. Econ. Stat. 63(3):36976

    Natl. Cent. Educ. Stat. 1997.Digest of Educaion

    Statistics. U.S. Dep. Educ.: Off. Educ. Res.

    Improv.Oakes J. 1983. Limiting opportunity: student

    race and curricular differences in secondary

    vocational education. Am. J. Educ. 91:801

    20

    Oakes J. 1985. Keeping Track: How Schools

    Structure Inequality. New Haven: Yale Univ.

    Press

    Oakes J. 1990. Multiplying Inequalities: The

    Effects of Race, Social Class, and Ability

    Grouping on Opportunities to Learn Mathe-matics and Science. Santa Monica: RAND

    Oakes J, Gamoran A, Page RN. 1992. Curricu-

    lum differentiation: opportunities, outcomes,

    and meanings. In Handbook on Research

    in Curriculum, ed. P Jackson, pp. 570608.

    New York: Macillan.

    Oakes J, Guiton G. 1995. Matchmaking: the

    dynamics of high school tracking decisions.

    Am. Educ. Research J. 32:333

    Ogbu JU. 1974. The Next Generation; anEthnography of Education in an Urban

    Neighborhood. New York: Academic

    Ogbu JU. 1991. Immigrant and involuntary mi-

    norities in comparative perspective. In Mi-

    nority Status and Schooling, ed. MA Gibson,

    JU Ogbu, pp. 333. New York: Garland

    Orfield G, Eaton SE, Jones ER. 1996. Disman-

    tling Desegregation: The Quiet Reversal of

    Brown v. Board of Education. New York:

    New Press

    Peng S. 1988.Attainment status of Asian Amer-

    icans in higher education. Paper presented at

    Natl. Assoc. Asian Pacific Am. Educ. Con-

    fer., Denver, Colo.

    Portes A. 1998. Social capital: its origins and

    application in modern sociology. Annu. Rev.

    Sociol. 24(1)124

    Portes A, Rumbaut R. 1996. Immigrant Amer-

    ica: A Portrait. Berkeley: Univ. Calif.

    Press

    Portes A, Zhou M. 1993. The new second gen-

    eration: segmented assimilation and its vari-

    ants. Ann. Am. Acad. Polit. Soc. Sci. 530:74

    96

    Rong XL, Grant L. 1992. Ethnicity, generation,

    and school attainment of Asians, Hispanics,

    and non-Hispanic whites. Sociol. Q. 33:625

    36Roscigno VJ. 1998. Race and reproduction

    of educational disadvantage. Soc. Forces.

    76:103360

    Roscigno VJ. 2000. Family/school inequal-

    ity and African-American/Hispanic achieve-

    ment. Soc. Problems 47(2):26690

    Rosen BC. 1959. Race, ethnicity, and the

    achievement syndrome. Am. Sociol. Rev.

    24:4760

    Rosenbaum JE. 1976. Making Inequality: TheHidden Curriculum of High School Tracking.

    New York: Wiley

    Rumbaut RG. 1990. Immigrant students in Cal-

    ifornia public schools: a summary of current

    knowledge. Center Res. Effective Sch. Dis-

    adv. Stud. Rep. Assoc. Pap. Am. Sociol. As-

    soc. 11:130

    Rumberger RW. 1982. Recent high school

    and college experiences of youth: variations

    by race, sex, and social class. Youth Soc.13(4):44970

    Rumberger RW. 1995. Dropping out of middle

    school: a multilevel analysis of students and

    schools. Am. Educ. Res. J. 32:583625

    Schneider B, Stevenson D. 1999. The Ambi-

    tious Generation: Americas Teenagers, Mo-

    tivated but Directionless. New Haven: Yale

    Univ. Press

    Schneider B, Lee Y. 1990. A model for aca-

    demic success: the school and home envi-

    ronment of East Asian students. Anthropol.

    Educ. Q. 21:35877

    Sewell WH, Haller AO, Ohlendrof GW. 1970.

    The educational and early occupational

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    25/28

    RACE, ETHNICITY, AND EDUCATION 441

    attainment process: replication and revision.

    Am. Sociol. Rev. 35:101427

    Sewell WH, Haller AO, Portes A. 1969. The ed-

    ucational and early occupational attainment

    process. Am. Sociol. Rev. 34:8292

    Sewell WH, Shah VP. 1968. Social class,

    parental encouragement, and educational as-

    pirations. Am. J. Sociol. 73:55972

    Slavin RE, Braddock JH III. 1993. Ability

    grouping: on the wrong track. Coll. Board

    Rev. 168:1117

    Sorenson AB, Hallinan MT. 1986. Effects

    of ability grouping on growth in academic

    achievement. Am. Educ. Res. J. 23:51942

    Spencer MB. 1990. Development of minority

    children: an introduction. Child Dev. 61:267

    69

    Spencer MB, Swanson DP, Cunningham MC.

    1991. Ethnicity, ethnic identity, and compe-

    tence formation: adolescent transition and

    cultural transformation. J. Negro Educ. 60:

    36787

    Steele CM. 1997. A threat in the air: how stereo-types shape intellectual identity and perfor-

    mance. Am. Psychol. 52:61329

    Steele CM, Aronson J. 1995. Stereotype threat

    and the intellectual test performance of

    African Americans.J. Psychol. Soc. Psychol.

    69:797811

    Steinberg S. 1989. The Ethnic Myth: Race, Eth-

    nicity, and Class in America. Boston: Beacon

    Stevenson DL, Schiller KS, Schneider B. 1994.

    Sequences of opportunities for learning. So-ciol. Educ. 67:18498

    Suarez-Orozco MM. 1989. Central Amer-

    ican Refugees and U.S. High Schools:

    A Psychosocial Study of Motivation and

    Achievement. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ.

    Press

    Suarez-Orozco C, Suarez-Orozco MM. 1995.

    Transformations: Immigration, Family Life,

    and Achievement Motivation Among Latino

    Adolescents. Stanford, CA: Stanford Univ.

    Press

    Suarez-Orozco C, Suarez-Orozco MM. 2001.

    Children of Immigration. Cambridge, MA:

    Harvard Univ. Press

    Sue S, Okazaki S. 1990. Asian-American

    educational achievements: a phenomenon

    in search of an explanation. Am. Psychol.

    45:91320

    Teachman JD, Paasch K, Carver K. 1996. So-

    cial capital and dropping out of school early.

    J. Marriage Fam. 58:77383

    Thomas GE, Alexander KL, Eckland BK. 1979.

    Access to higher education: the importance

    of race, sex, social class, and academic cre-

    dentials. School Rev. 87:13356

    Thornton CH, Eckland B. 1980. High school

    contextual effects for black and white stu-

    dents: a research note. Sociol. Educ. 53:24752

    US Census Bureau. 2000. http://www.census.

    gov/population/www/projections/popproj.

    html

    US Dep. Educ. 1994. The Condition of Edu-

    cation. Washington, DC: Natl. Cent. Educ.

    Stat.

    US Dep. Educ. 1995. Findings from the Con-

    dition of Education 1994: The Educational

    Progress of Black Students. No. 2. Washing-ton, DC: Natl. Cent. Educ. Stat., Off. Educ.

    Res. Improv.

    Valenzuela A, Dornbusch S. 1994. Familism

    and social capital in the academic achieve-

    ment of Mexican origin and Anglo adoles-

    cents. Soc. Science Q. 75:1836

    Velez W. 1985. Finishing college: the ef-

    fects of college type. Sociol. Educ. 58:191

    200

    Velez W. 1989. High school attrition amongHispanic and non-Hispanic white youths. So-

    ciol. Educ. 62:11933

    Warren JR. 1996. Educational inequality

    among white and Mexican-origin adoles-

    cents in the American Southwest: 1990. So-

    ciol. Educ. 69:14258

    Weber M. 1976. The Protestant Ethic and the

    Spirit of Capitalism. Transl. T Parsons. New

    York: Scribners

    White MJ, Glick J. 2000. Generation, social

    captial and the routes out of high school. So-

    ciol. Forum. 15(4):67191

    White MJ, Kaufmann G. 1997. Language

    usage, social capital, and school completion

    b

    yHARVARDUNIVERSITYon

    07/27/07.Forpersonaluseonly.

  • 7/28/2019 Kao EducAttARS

    26/28

    442 KAO THOMPSON

    among immigrants and native born ethnic

    groups. Soc. Science Q. 78:38598

    Willis P. 1977. Learning to Labor: How Work-

    ing Class Kids Get Working Class Jobs. New

    York: Columbia Univ. Press

    Wilson WJ. 1980. The Declining Signifi-

    cance of Race: Blacks and Changing Amer-

    ican Institutions. Chicago: Univ. Chicago

    Press

    Wojtkiewicz RA. 1993. Duration in parental

    structures and high school graduation. So-

    ciol. Perspect. 36:393414

    Zhou M. 1997. Growing up American: the chal-

    lenge confronting immigrant children and

    children of immigrants. Annu. Rev. Sociol.

    23:6395

    Zhou M