12
J Am Acad Audiol 10 : 14-25 (1999) Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children Patricia G. Stelmachowicz* Abstract The provision of appropriate amplification for a young hearing-impaired child is critical as the aided speech signal will be used for the development of speech and language . The purpose of this paper is to provide an overview of the complex issues surrounding the documenta- tion of hearing aid outcomes in the pediatric population . In the first two sections of the paper, the unique characteristics and needs of the pediatric population and factors complicating the measurement of outcome are described in detail . The third section provides a review of lit- erature on existing outcome measures for children and the fourth section is devoted to a discussion of alternative approaches . The final section is an overview of clinical and research needs in the area of hearing aid outcome measures for children . Key Words : Children, hearing aid, hearing loss, outcome Abbreviations : AAI = Aided Audibility Index; ABR = auditory brainstem response ; AI = Articulation Index ; APHAB = Abbreviated Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit ; CL = compression limiting ; COSI = Client-Oriented Scale of Improvement ; DSL= Desired Sensation Level ; HPIC = Hearing Performance Inventory for Children ; LDL = loudness discomfort level ; LIFE = Listening Instrument for Education ; PC = peak clipping ; SIFTER = Screening Instrument for Targeting Educational Risk ; WDRC = wide dynamic range compression N umerous approaches are available to evaluate the efficacy of a particular hearing aid fitting for adults . Objec- tive measures of benefit typically have been conducted in a structured clinical or laboratory setting and have focused on speech recognition under a variety of test conditions . It has been suggested that this type of approach may not pro- duce results that are highly correlated with the hearing aid benefit experienced in everyday lis- tening situations (Haggard et al, 1981 ; Berger and Hagberg, 1982 ; Oja and Schow, 1984) . This is not too surprising, given the wide range of lis- tening environments that a typical hearing aid user may experience during a day . It is also likely that the available clinical measures of speech recognition may not be sensitive enough to detect differences in performance produced by various types of signal processing . To circumvent the limitations of these objec- tive clinical measures, a wide variety of subjec- tive measures of hearing aid benefit have been *Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, Nebraska Reprint requests : Patricia G . Stelmachowicz, Boys Town National Research Hospital, 555 North 30th Street, Omaha, NE 68131 developed (see Weinstein [19971 for a review of this literature) . The vast majority of these are self-assessment inventories that attempt to quantify hearing aid benefit across a range of everyday listening conditions . This approach has high content validity and studies have shown good test-retest reliability for adults (Cox and Alexander, 1995) . In recent years, these types of metrics have gained increased use in clinical settings to document hearing aid benefit and to evaluate the efficacy of different types of signal processing . For adults and older children, a wide vari- ety of objective and subjective metrics are avail- able to document outcome . The various tests can be used in isolation or in combination with other tests in order to answer a particular clin- ical question . However, much of the methodol- ogy and many of the metrics typically used with adults in the assessment of hearing aid bene- fit are not appropriate for use with infants and young children . Advances in the early identifi- cation of hearing loss have increased the need to make decisions early in life that are depen- dent upon demonstrating or failing to demon- strate device or treatment efficacy (e.g ., sensory aid decisions, communication mode decisions) . Appropriate amplification will contribute to the speech and language acquisition process by 14

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

J Am Acad Audiol 10 : 14-25 (1999)

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children Patricia G. Stelmachowicz*

Abstract

The provision of appropriate amplification for a young hearing-impaired child is critical as the aided speech signal will be used for the development of speech and language . The purpose of this paper is to provide an overview of the complex issues surrounding the documenta-tion of hearing aid outcomes in the pediatric population . In the first two sections of the paper, the unique characteristics and needs of the pediatric population and factors complicating the measurement of outcome are described in detail . The third section provides a review of lit-erature on existing outcome measures for children and the fourth section is devoted to a discussion of alternative approaches . The final section is an overview of clinical and research needs in the area of hearing aid outcome measures for children .

Key Words : Children, hearing aid, hearing loss, outcome

Abbreviations : AAI = Aided Audibility Index; ABR = auditory brainstem response ; AI = Articulation Index; APHAB = Abbreviated Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit; CL = compression limiting ; COSI = Client-Oriented Scale of Improvement; DSL= Desired Sensation Level; HPIC = Hearing Performance Inventory for Children ; LDL = loudness discomfort level ; LIFE = Listening Instrument for Education ; PC = peak clipping ; SIFTER = Screening Instrument for Targeting Educational Risk ; WDRC = wide dynamic range compression

N

umerous approaches are available to evaluate the efficacy of a particular hearing aid fitting for adults . Objec-

tive measures of benefit typically have been conducted in a structured clinical or laboratory setting and have focused on speech recognition under a variety of test conditions . It has been suggested that this type of approach may not pro-duce results that are highly correlated with the hearing aid benefit experienced in everyday lis-tening situations (Haggard et al, 1981 ; Berger and Hagberg, 1982; Oja and Schow, 1984). This is not too surprising, given the wide range of lis-tening environments that a typical hearing aid user may experience during a day. It is also likely that the available clinical measures of speech recognition may not be sensitive enough to detect differences in performance produced by various types of signal processing .

To circumvent the limitations of these objec-tive clinical measures, a wide variety of subjec-tive measures of hearing aid benefit have been

*Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, Nebraska

Reprint requests : Patricia G . Stelmachowicz, Boys Town National Research Hospital, 555 North 30th Street, Omaha, NE 68131

developed (see Weinstein [19971 for a review of this literature) . The vast majority of these are self-assessment inventories that attempt to quantify hearing aid benefit across a range of everyday listening conditions . This approach has high content validity and studies have shown good test-retest reliability for adults (Cox and Alexander, 1995) . In recent years, these types of metrics have gained increased use in clinical settings to document hearing aid benefit and to evaluate the efficacy of different types of signal processing .

For adults and older children, a wide vari-ety of objective and subjective metrics are avail-able to document outcome. The various tests can be used in isolation or in combination with other tests in order to answer a particular clin-ical question . However, much of the methodol-ogy and many of the metrics typically used with adults in the assessment of hearing aid bene-fit are not appropriate for use with infants and young children . Advances in the early identifi-cation of hearing loss have increased the need to make decisions early in life that are depen-dent upon demonstrating or failing to demon-strate device or treatment efficacy (e .g ., sensory aid decisions, communication mode decisions) . Appropriate amplification will contribute to the speech and language acquisition process by

14

Page 2: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz

supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax. To provide the best pos-sible amplification device and take advantage of technological advances, there is a critical need for valid and reliable outcome measures for this population .

This paper is divided into five sections . In the first two sections, the unique needs of the pediatric population and factors complicating the measurement of outcome will be described in detail . The third section provides a review of lit-erature on existing outcome measures for chil-dren . The fourth section will be devoted to a discussion of alternative approaches to docu-ment outcome . The final section will provide an overview of clinical and research needs in this area .

PEDIATRIC POPUIATION: UNIQUE CHARACTERISTICS

AND UNIQUE NEEDS

T he process of fitting hearing aids to an infant or young child is qualitatively dif-

ferent than the adult case in a number of ways. Although newborn screening programs have enabled audiologists to identify hearing loss within the first few days of life, precise quan-tification of the degree and configuration of hearing loss may not be possible for many months . However, since recent data have shown that children with hearing loss who are involved in early intervention prior to 6 months of age out-perform children who are not involved in inter-vention until later (Moeller, in press ; Yoshinago-Itano et al, in press), it is not considered rea-sonable to delay amplification until the child is capable of providing a comprehensive behav-ioral audiogram (Pediatric Working Group, 1996) . For infants and older children with mul-tiple disabilities, it is often necessary to select amplification characteristics based largely upon tone-burst auditory brainstem response (ABR) thresholds . Because only a limited amount of audiologic information is available early on, hearing aid fitting schemes that require more than audiologic thresholds cannot be used .

Even when behavioral measures can be obtained, the results differ substantially from the adult case . Test reliability may be affected by developmental issues, such as level of cooperation, attentiveness, and/or middle ear disease. Chil-dren's speech perception skills need to be mea-sured within the confines of their receptive language abilities, which may limit the procedures that can be used and complicate interpretation of

results (Boothroyd, 1991) . Subjective feedback from the child will be minimal and informal obser-vations by parents or other professionals often can-not be easily interpreted in terms of cause and effect .

Numerous physical differences exist as well . Both the ear canal resonance (Kruger, 1987 ; Kruger and Ruben, 1987) and the real-ear-to-cou-pler difference (Nelson Barlow et al, 1988; Fei-gin et al, 1989; Moodie et al, 1994) have been shown to vary as a function of age within the first few years of life . It is also likely that the aver-age speech input level to the hearing aid micro-phone will be substantially greater for infants and toddlers than for adults due to proximity dif-ferences between parent and child in the first few years of life (Stelmachowicz et al, 1993) .

At the present time, there are differing opin-ions and philosophies regarding the electroa-coustic requirements for infants and young children . Some investigators have suggested that prescriptive hearing aid procedures devel-oped for adults can be applied to young chil-dren (Byrne and Ching, in press ; Jordt et al, in press) . These investigators argue that the revised National Acoustics Laboratory (NAL) approach (Byrne and Dillon, 1986), which is based on the amplification of all speech bands to equal loud-ness, should be appropriate for hearing-impaired individuals of all ages .

Other investigators maintain that the ampli-fication needs of infants and young children may differ from those of adults who generally acquire hearing loss in later life (Stelmachow-icz, 1991 ; Seewald et al, 1996). Specifically, the latter investigators feel that the goal of ampli-fication may be different for young children in that the hearing aids will be used for the devel-opment of language, speech perception, and speech production . Many of the available pre-scriptive procedures were based upon the pre-ferred hearing aid characteristics of adult users or were validated in this group. Young children with hearing loss have not yet developed the lin-guistic and world knowledge that supports speech perception for adults in the form of con-textual and redundancy cues .

Nittrouer and Boothroyd (1990) investigated the effect of context on the perception of four-word sentences in 4- and 5-year-old normal-hearing children and adults . The subject's task was to repeat both meaningful and nonmeaningful sen-tences presented at two different signal-to-noise (SIN) ratios . While the addition of context improved scores for both groups, the children showed significantly smaller improvements in

15

Page 3: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 1, January 1999

perception compared to the adults . These results suggest that, under adverse listening conditions, adults may be able to use contextual cues to "fill in the blanks" in a way that is not possible for young children. It follows that a less than opti-mal hearing aid fitting for an adult may not affect the perception of speech as much as would be the case for a child who is attempting to learn speech and language through audition .

In addition to these semantic differences, a number of investigators have shown that young children may differ from adults in their ability to use certain types of acoustic cues . Nozza et al (1991) compared the discrimination thresholds of two speech contrasts (/ba/ vs /da/ and /ba/ vs /ga/) between 7- to 11-month-old infants and adults . The infants required stimulus intensities of 25 to 28 dB more than the adults in order to reach a criterion level of performance. It also appears that children tend to use slowly chang-ing dynamic or temporal cues, whereas adults tend to rely more on static spectral cues (Mor-rongiello et al, 1984; Nittrouer and Studdert-Kennedy, 1987 ; Nittrouer, 1992).

Some investigators (Gatehouse, 1992, 1993) have shown that the speech perception perfor-mance of adult hearing-impaired listeners improves over time with repeated exposure to amplified speech . To date, there have been no sys-tematic studies of acclimatization in young chil-dren . The existence of this phenomenon in children could have a potential impact on the ability to compare different types of signal proces-sors during the hearing aid selection process . In addition, the use of vastly different types of pro-cessing schemes in multiple-memory hearing aids will alter the signal that is received by the child. Studies to determine the magnitude of acclimatization in the pediatric population and the clinical significance of the effects are needed .

The issue of which prescriptive procedure, if any, is optimal for young children has not been resolved . Ross and Levitt (1997) point out that, for many hearing-impaired individuals, virtually any type of amplification device will provide an improvement over unaided listening. As such, objective and/or subjective compar-isons of unaided and aided benefit in no way guarantee that a hearing aid fitting is optimal. For young children, it may be extremely difficult, if not impossible, to ensure that a particular set of hearing aid characteristics is substan-tially better than other alternatives . A small number of studies have been conducted with preschool and school-aged children in which use gain or preferred gain was compared to the gain

recommended by various prescriptive proce-dures with varying results (Snik and Homber-gen, 1993 ; Ching et al, 1994, 1996; Snik and Stollman, 1995 ; Snik et al, 1995). These types of studies are difficult to interpret because "use gain" will be highly dependent upon the ratio-nale and procedures used to fit the hearing aid initially. Similarly, "preferred gain" may be biased by the characteristics of the initial fitting and the effects of acclimatization; if the child is accustomed to listening with particular fre-quency/gain characteristics, this response may be preferred over other alternatives in a paired-comparison task . In the absence of a consensus on how to determine optimal hearing aid char-acteristics for children, the need for valid and reliable outcome measures that can be applied on an individual basis is critical .

PEDIATRIC POPULATION: FACTORS COMPLICATING THE MEASUREMENT OF OUTCOME

W hat should be used as an outcome measure? Potential outcome measures include audi- tory awareness, audibility of speech, speech intelligibility, accuracy of speech production, rate of language acquisition, loudness discom-fort, and social development . The metric selected will depend on both the age of the child and the degree of hearing loss and will be influenced by the age at which hearing loss identification occurs, concomitant middle ear problems, the existence of additional handicapping conditions, developmental factors, and the child's home and educational environments . The selected out-come measures also will depend upon the clin-ical question to be addressed . The question may be relatively broad, such as : What is the best hearing aid for a particular child? Or, given cur-rent hearing aid technology, more specific ques-tions may be asked : Is a binaural or monaural fitting most appropriate for a child with an asymmetric hearing loss? Should a directional or omnidirectional hearing aid be recommended? How much high-frequency gain should be rec-ommended for a child with a precipitous hear-ing loss? Should compression limiting (CL) or wide dynamic range compression (WDRC) be used for a child with a moderate-to-severe hear-ing loss? Under what conditions should a mul-timemory device be used and what hearing aid characteristics should be programmed into each memory? What real-ear saturation response will produce levels that are both safe and com-fortable? Obviously, each of these questions may

16

Page 4: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz

require a different kind of outcome measure or some combination of measures .

Over what time period should outcome mea-sures be made? With children, one would not expect to see immediate changes in speech pro-duction or language skills following the intro-duction of amplification or a change in hearing aid characteristics . Repeated auditory experi-ences or acclimatization (Gatehouse, 1993) also may be necessary in order to observe changes in speech perception . In most cases, longitudinal monitoring may be required to identify changes in performance that can be attributable to ampli-fication (e.g., improved speech production of a now audible sibilant) . The time frame for changes in behavior or performance is likely to be longer than is typical for an adult hearing aid user .

How can ongoing developmental changes be separated from changes related specifically to amplification? If it is necessary to use a longi-tudinal approach to document outcome, it may be difficult to separate the effects of normal development or the influence of other treat-ments (e.g., speech and language therapy) from the effects related to hearing aid use.

At what developmental age will outcome measures developed for adults be valid for use with children? For some subjective metrics it may be sufficient to alter the vocabulary, language level, and described situations to create an appropriate assessment tool . However, auditory goals and the child's ability to provide subjective feedback can be expected to change over time in a way that generally does not occur for adults . Thus, for most types of outcome measures devel-oped for the pediatric population, it will be nec-essary to develop a series of metrics to reflect changes in performance as the child matures .

CURRENT OUTCOME MEASURES

Objective Measures of Outcome

For adults and older children, the most com-monly used objective outcome measure is speech recognition . A wide variety of tests are available, measuring recognition of stimuli ranging from nonsense syllables to words and sentences . The materials, SIN ratio, and response format (closed set, open set) can be tailored to suit the specific objectives for each case .

Unfortunately, the situation is very differ-ent for young hearing-impaired children . Under the best of circumstances, it is not possible to obtain reliable measures of speech recognition for children less than 3 years of age. Beginning

at about 3 years of age, the most commonly used clinical tests require the child to select the appro-priate response from among a small set of pic-tures (e.g ., Ross and Lerman, 1970 ; Katz and Elliott, 1978) . While the words and pictures typ-ically are within the repertoire of children in the 3- to 5-year age range, for hearing-impaired children, it is often impossible to separate fac-tors such as auditory experience, participation in speech and language therapy, educational environment, and phonologic development from the true residual capacity of the damaged cochlea (Boothroyd, 1991 ; Boothroyd et al, 1996) . That is, a young child's previous experiences will influence performance on this type of test and are likely to complicate the interpretation of results . If the goal of a test is to determine how well a child is performing overall, these inter-actions may not be a problem . If the intent is to assess the efficacy of a particular signal pro-cessing scheme, however, a valid test of auditory speech perception capacity should be devoid of lexical, syntactic, and semantic constraints . While nonsense syllables meet this goal, the use of these stimuli with young children is prob-lematic in that it is difficult to provide an appro-priate visual reference (e .g ., written words, pictures) for each test item . An imitative test format has been developed for use in research studies with children as young as 3 years of age (Boothroyd, 1991; Boothroyd et al, 1996) . In its current form, this procedure requires post-test editing of speech samples and scoring by a group of normal-hearing listeners ; thus, it would not be applicable in a typical clinical setting .

Recently, Boothroyd (1991) has developed a PC-based video game (VIDSPAC) for evaluating the perception of various acoustic contrasts in children as young as 3 to 4 years of age . The pro-gram allows the child to select a colorful char-acter to "present" the test stimuli and a wide range of speech pattern contrasts can be evalu-ated (e.g., vowel height, final consonant voicing) . The child's task is to respond when a change in a string of stimuli is detected (/ta/, /ta/, /ta/, /da/, /ta/). Following each block of trials, an animated scene is displayed as a reward. In addition to the well-designed graphics, which are engaging for young children, the program has other features

that are important for this population. The inter-stimulus interval can be varied during the exper-iment according to the child's needs and summary statistics provide an account of both hits and false positive responses . This type of test should be extremely useful in extending the lower age limit at which valuable estimates of

17

Page 5: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 1, January 1999

speech perception can be obtained from hearing-impaired children .

For older children, a much wider range of objective tests is available. These include tools that measure the perception of nonsense sylla-bles, single words, sentences, and story com-prehension . Special purpose materials are available for children with varying degrees of hearing loss and for auditory-alone versus audi-tory/visual perception. (See The Pediatric Work-ing Group [19961 for a review of current speech tests for this population.) As previously men-tioned, it is important to note the limitation of speech recognition as an outcome measure for both children and adults . While these objective tests can provide a quantitative estimate of per-formance that can be compared to expected goals, it is unlikely that these measures will be sensitive enough to compare different hearing aid characteristics .

Subjective Measures of Outcome

To date, only a small number of subjective tests are available for children and most of these have been developed for use in educational set-tings exclusively. The Hearing Performance Inventory for Children (HPIC), developed by Kessler et al (1990), is a self-assessment instru-ment for children (ages 8-14 years) that samples a child's perceived communication difficulties in a variety of academic environments. The primary goal of this test is to develop a personal profile of difficult academic communication situations to aid in the design of an individualized man-agement program . The test consists of 31 test items depicting a variety of typical classroom lis-tening situations (e.g., hearing the teacher when his/her face is not visible) . For each test item, a picture and a verbal description are given and the child's task is to rate communication diffi-culty on a 5-point scale . In the development of the HPIC, test-retest measures were obtained on 15 normal-hearing and 7 hearing-impaired children, but no large-scale studies have been reported using this metric .

The Listening Inventories for Education (LIFE) were developed by Smaldino and Ander-son (1997), also for use in the classroom. The LIFE consists of three inventories, one for the student and two for the teacher. The Student Appraisal of Listening Difficulty is intended to be used in a pre- and post-test format in order to document changes following intervention . It has 15 test items that are picture cards depict-ing different common listening situations in the

classroom, chosen to be sensitive to the expected benefits of various forms of classroom amplifi-cation (e.g ., hearing aids, personal FM systems, soundfield FM systems) . The child's task is to report the level of difficulty associated with each of the 15 different situations . Children in grades 1 to 3 use a 3-point scale and older children use a 5-point scale. The Teacher Appraisal of Lis-tening Difficulty is designed to be given after intervention and consists of 16 questions related to specific areas of improvement in either behav-ior or communication. The Teacher Opinion and Observation List consists of four items in an open-ended format . Test-retest reliability of the student questionnaire was assessed with 19 normal-hearing third and fourth graders over a 1- to 3-week period . No significant differences were observed between the scores for any of the test items. These inventories are flexible and can be used in a variety of ways (e.g., document effi-cacy of amplification or classroom teaching meth-ods, inservicing of teachers, self-advocacy) .

The Screening Instrument for Targeting Educational Risk (SIFTER) (Anderson, 1989) was developed to identify school-aged children who are at risk for educational failure due to hearing loss and thus in need of further evalu-ation . This 15-item test is completed by the child's teacher and covers five content areas : academics, attention, communication, class par-ticipation, and school behavior. Teachers use a 5-point scale to evaluate a child using items such as "How distractible is the student in com-parison to his/her classmates?" The total score in each content area is categorized as either a pass, fail, or marginal outcome. A preschool ver-sion (3 years-kindergarten) of this test has also been developed (Anderson and Matkin, 1996) . While this test was designed as a screening tool, it can be used to assess the efficacy of various forms of intervention in the classroom (e.g., pref-erential seating, soundfield amplification, FM systems, personal amplification) .

Recently, Kopun and Stelmachowicz (1998) adapted the Abbreviated Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit (APHAB), developed by Cox and Alexan-der (1995), to be appropriate for children in the 10- to 15-year age range. Children with both moderate and severe hearing losses showed a pattern of responses across the four subtests of the APHAB that was similar to the aided adult data reported by Cox and Alexander and test-retest reliability over a 2- to 3-month period yielded correlation coefficients in the 0.83 to 0.95 range. Parents also were given the test in order to assess the parent's perception of the

18

Page 6: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz

child's communication problems . Interestingly, the correlations between the child and parental scores on all four subscales were quite low. That is, for this group of subjects, the parents and chil-dren did not agree with respect to communica-tion difficulties . No obvious trends were observed and it is unclear whose impression best reflects the degree of difficulty actually encountered . It is possible that the parents may have noticed communication difficulties that were not appar-ent to the child . It is also possible that parents of children in this age range may have had lim-ited opportunities to observe their child in the variety of communication situations described in this questionnaire . These results raise an important issue regarding the development of subjective measures for use with the pediatric population . Can parents, caregivers, or other individuals provide a valid estimate of commu-nication difficulties via observation?

The Meaningful Auditory Integration Scale (Robbins et al, 1991) is a parent report measure designed for use with profoundly hearing-impaired children . This tool consists of 10 ques-tions that include items on device acceptance and use, awareness of familiar environmental sounds, and the ability to distinguish speech from nonspeech stimuli . Parents use a 5-point scale in responding to each test item . This test is most appropriate for young children with pro-found hearing loss as ceiling effects are observed for children with greater residual hearing .

It may be possible to modify some existing subjective tests for use with young children . Moeller (1993) has suggested that modification of the Client-Oriented Scale of Improvement (COSI) described by Dillon et al (1991, 1997) might be appropriate for infants and toddlers . In its original form, the COSI is designed to allow the patient to select up to five situations in which communication is difficult . Following a trial period with amplification, the patient estimates the degree of change relative to the unaided situation using a 5-point scale . This is an efficient way to assess outcome because the questionnaire is individualized to focus on the specific goals of the patient . Because the goals differ from case to case, however, findings can-not be easily grouped across subjects . Poten-tially, this approach may prove to be very useful for young hearing-impaired children because individual situations and goals are likely to dif-fer substantially across children, even when they have similar hearing losses . Furthermore, goals are likely to change relatively often in the first few years of life as speech and language

skills develop . For an infant with a newly iden-tified hearing loss, initial goals may include auditory awareness and alerting to familiar environment signals . By 9 to 12 months of age, more specific goals may be appropriate such as differentiating certain sounds or words . This type of approach may require clear and struc-tured guidelines for parents and other care-givers to ensure that their observations are unbiased and focused appropriately on the spe-cific goal .

ALTERNATIVE APPROACHES

t should be obvious that the documentation of hearing aid efficacy in the pediatric popu-

lation is extremely complex . For a variety of reasons, it is unlikely that a single outcome measure or a single approach to the problem could provide a means by which to determine effi-cacy in all cases . The following alternative options are described in detail along with the assumptions and rationale for each approach .

Studies of Device Efficacy

For most hearing aid fittings, a wide range of options (e.g ., frequency response, gain, circuit type) are available and it would be impossible to entertain all options during the evaluation process . Studies of device efficacy with well-defined groups of subjects can help provide guidelines to indicate when certain types of technology may be most appropriate . For exam-ple, laboratory studies of adults with mild-to-moderate hearing loss have shown that directional microphones can improve the SIN ratio by 5 to 7 dB over omnidirectional micro-phones (Hawkins and Yacullo, 1984; Valente et al, 1995) . If a particular hearing aid circuit or characteristic is shown to be efficacious for a given group of hearing-impaired individuals, then it can be assumed that the feature would be efficacious in individual cases.

To date, only a few studies of this type have been conducted with children . Christensen and Thomas (1997) compared the performance of peak clipping (PC), CL, and WDRC in 9- to 14-year-old children with mild-to-moderate hear-ing loss and found significantly better speech recognition with WDRC and CL compared to the PC circuit. Significant differences in per-ception were not observed between the WDRC and CL circuits . In the second part of this study, a multiple-memory device was programmed with the three circuit types and the children

19

Page 7: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 1, January 1999

wore the hearing aids for 2 months, recording preferred memory settings in a diary. WDRC was preferred most often in noise and CL was pre-ferred most in quiet . The children reported sub-jective benefit from the use of multiple memories in different listening environments . Seewald et al (1997) measured speech recognition and aided loudness growth in a group of adolescents using both linear and WDRC hearing aids . They found better speech recognition over a wider range of input levels with WDRC compared to linear pro-cessing . In addition, WDRC produced loudness growth functions that closely approximated nor-mal loudness growth .

Gravel et al (in press) investigated the effi-cacy of dual-microphone technology for children in the 4- to 6- and 7- to 11-year age ranges . For the perception of single words, the directional microphone advantage was similar for both groups (an effective improvement in SIN ratio of -4.8 dB). For sentence materials, the younger group demonstrated an advantage of 4.3 dB and the older group showed an advantage of approx-imately 5.2 dB . These values are lower than those reported for adults (approximately 8.0 dB) using the same type of dual-microphone technology (Valente et al, 1995).

Laboratory studies of device efficacy in spe-cific populations can play an important role and have some advantages over individual measures of outcome. In these types of studies, the nec-essary variables can be controlled, thus facili-tating the interpretation of results.

Heal Ear SPL (dB)

14 Speech SPLogram

Audibility of Speech : Predictive Measures

While factors such as frequency or tempo-ral resolution and abnormal growth of loudness may play a role in speech perception, numerous studies have suggested that the primary factor responsible for speech recognition problems in individuals with hearing loss is the reduction in signal audibility (Pavlovic, 1984 ; Humes et al, 1987; Zurek and Delhorne, 1987 ; Humes, 1991). As such, measures of the audibility of speech under various aided conditions may provide a basis for determining efficacy of a particular hearing aid fitting. Both the Desired Sensation Level (DSL) fitting procedure for linear hearing aids (Seewald, 1992) and DSL i/o (Cornelisse et al, 1995) for nonlinear hearing aids provide a graphic display of the audibility of average speech across frequency. As shown in Figure 1, this display can provide a visual reference to aid

120-

100-

s0-

so-

40-

20-a

0 '-TTr--rT--I 125 _ 250 500 -1000 2000__ . ._4000 __8000

Frequency (Hz)

Figure 1 Graphic display from DSL i/o (Cornelisse et al, 1995). The open squares represent auditory thresh-olds, the filled triangles represent the predicted upper limit of comfort, and the three dashed curves show the target values for soft, average, and loud speech.

in the finetuning of a hearing aid fitting. In this figure, the open squares represent auditory thresholds, the filled triangles represent the predicted upper limit of comfort, and the three dashed curves show the target values for soft, average, and loud speech . If target values can be achieved, it can be assumed that speech will be audible, yet comfortable.

It is also possible to provide a quantitative estimate of audibility. The most commonly used method is the Articulation Index (AI) (American National Standards Institute, 1969), which pro-vides a number ranging from 0 (inaudibility) to 1.0 (audibility of the entire spectrum). A num-ber of investigators have suggested various adaptations of the AI to allow this approach to be applied to aided listeners under clinical con-ditions (Mueller and Killion, 1990; Hou and Thornton, 1994; Stelmachowicz et al, 1996). The Hou and Thornton (1994) and Stelmachowicz et al (1996) approaches provide aided AI values that incorporate a range of assumed input levels to the hearing aid. The Situational Hearing Aid Response Profile also provides a visual display of audibility and the aided dynamic range for var-ious listening conditions (Stelmachowicz et al, 1996). Figure 2 shows a display of the audibil-ity of speech under four listening conditions for a child with a moderate hearing loss using this approach . The Xs represent auditory thresh-olds, the asterisks show the hearing aid maxi-mum output. The cross-hatched region shows the portion of the speech spectrum that is audible

20

Page 8: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz

and the associated aided audibility index (AAI) is displayed . Such an approach can provide a quantitative way to compare audibility across

hearing aids or across situations . It is important to note that the aided audio-

gram or functional gain measures often cannot provide a valid representation of the audibility of speech at typical input levels (Macrae and Fra-zier, 1980; Macrae, 1982; Stelmachowicz and Lewis, 1988). This is particularly true for wide dynamic range hearing aids where the mea-sured gain varies as a function of input level.

Paired-Comparison Measures

A number of investigators have used a paired-comparison approach to evaluate differ-ences perceived in various hearing aid charac-

teristics by children . Eisenberg and Levitt (1991) investigated the feasibility of using a paired-com-parison task to select hearing aids for children .

Children with mild to moderately severe hear-ing loss judged the clarity of a children's story

that had been filtered with nine different fre-quency-gain shapes . They found the technique

to be feasible for hearing-impaired children as

young as 6.5 years of age . Ching et al (1994) used a paired-comparison procedure with a group of 21 children (ages 6-19 years) with severe to

profound hearing losses . The purpose of the

study was to evaluate the reliability and sensi-

tivity of intelligibility judgments under audio-

visual and auditory-alone conditions . Results showed that the method could be used reliably

with subjects as young as 6 years of age and that

the mode of presentation had little effect on the results . Recently, Stelmachowicz et al (in press)

used a paired-comparison procedure to assess

quality differences between speech processed

by either CL or PC . The frequency/gain and out-

put characteristics were selected to reflect a

typical state-of-the-art hearing aid fitting ; thus,

the acoustic differences across stimuli were sub-

tle compared to many earlier studies of this

nature . Normal and hearing-impaired subjects

in three age ranges (7-9 years, 10-12 years,

and adults) were tested. While the normal-hear-ing adults and the 10- to 12-year-old children

were able to distinguish between the stimuli, the

youngest normal-hearing group and both groups of hearing-impaired children were relatively

insensitive to these acoustic differences . These investigators suggested that the reliability and validity of paired-comparison measures may

depend upon the magnitude of the physical dif-

ferences between the stimuli to be compared . That is, it may only be possible to use this tech-nique with young children when the perceptual contrasts to be compared are relatively large .

Loudness Measures

While many prescriptive hearing aid meth-ods provide recommended real-ear saturation levels, these levels are based on average data . Hawkins et al (1987) have suggested that hear-ing aid rejection is often based on loudness dis-comfort. A small number of studies have been reported in which measures of loudness dis-comfort level (LDL) have been attempted in

young children . Kawell et al (1988) evaluated hearing-impaired children in the 7- to 14-year age range and reported across-session intra-subject standard deviations that ranged from a low of 3.0 dB at 500 Hz to 5.8 dB at 3000 Hz. Stu-art et al (1991) also evaluated 7- to 14-year-old hearing-impaired children and their within-ses-sion intrasubject standard deviations were slightly lower (3.4-4.9 dB). Compared to adult data, these values are within 1 dB of the stan-

dard deviations reported by Cox (1981) but are 4 dB greater than those reported by Hawkins et al (1987) . Macpherson et al (1991) evaluated LDLs in a younger group of normal-hearing children (3-5 years) and concluded that only children with mental ages > 5.0 years could per-

form this task reliably. Teghtsoonian (1980) applied a cross-modal-

ity matching technique to loudness scaling in 4-

to 12-year-old normal-hearing children and

adults . Children were asked to match the loud-

ness of a 1000-Hz tone to the length of a retractable tape . The slope of the function relat-

ing intensity to length was invariant across all ages, but the intercept differed significantly between the 4 and 6 year olds and the older

subjects . That is, at a given intensity, the younger

subjects selected a shorter length than did the

older subjects . By 8 years of age, both the slope

and the intercept approximated adult values .

Since developmental changes did not alter the

slope of the functions, the author concluded that

cross-modality matching can be used successfully

in children as young as 4 years of age . Despite the clinical use of loudness growth measures to

assist in the fitting of nonlinear hearing aids for adults, no systematic studies with young hear-ing-impaired children have been reported .

21

Page 9: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 1, January 1999

FREQUENCY (Hz) 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000

140

FREQUENCY (Hz) 125 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000

120

100

80

60

40

20

0

125 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000 125 250 500 1000 2000 4000 8000

Figure 2 Graphic display from the Situational Hearing Aid Response Profile (Stelmachowicz et al, 1996). Xs repre-sent auditory thresholds, asterisks show the maximum output, and the cross-hatched region shows the audible region of speech . The shaded region (lower left panel) denotes peak clipping . An aided audibility index (AAI) is displayed in each panel.

CLINICAL INNOVATION AND RESEARCH NEEDS

Speech Recognition Tests

There is a great need for more sophisticated approaches to the measurement and quantifi-cation of speech recognition abilities in young children . A developmentally appropriate bat-tery of measures that considers the symbolic and attentional demands of young children is needed . While interactive video technology may be promising with children > 3 years of age, the clinical measurement of speech recognition dur-

ing infancy is problematic. Laboratory studies using visual reinforcement techniques (Kuhl, 1983) have shown that normal-hearing infants are capable of recognizing a variety of acoustic differences . At present, these techniques have not been adapted for use in clinical settings .

Many of the current tests that are available for children > 3 years of age are somewhat out-dated both in terms of the pictorial representa-tions and vocabulary. Within this age range, there is also a need for tests that would focus on the type of speech recognition problems experi-enced by children with mild hearing loss or hearing loss restricted to the high frequencies.

22

Page 10: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz

Subjective Inventories

As noted earlier, a small number of inven-tories have been developed for use with school-aged children in academic settings . There is a

need for additional inventories that focus on specific listening situations outside the class-room . Studies are needed to determine the lower

age limit at which pictorial representation and/or simple descriptions of listening situations (e.g., listening to television with other children talk-

ing) can provide both valid and reliable data .

Parental Inventories

perceptual measures that can only be obtained reliably from adults and older children . For example, ABR thresholds have been shown to

agree well with behavioral results (Stapells et al, 1995), thus allowing hearing aids to be fitted

within the first few weeks of life . It may be pos-

sible to use electrophysiologic measures to pro-vide suprathreshold measures of auditory

function . Boothroyd (1991) has suggested that it may be possible to develop objective tests, using evoked potential techniques, to measure

differential responses to various acoustic con-trasts, thus providing an estimate of the audi-tory capacity for speech perception.

Parental inventories are widely used and

well accepted in a number of fields (e.g ., speech-language pathology, psychology, pediatrics) in order to document developmental milestones, abnormal behavior, and a variety of other fac-tors . Indeed, portions of many current develop-mental metrics include test items that may be

used to document auditory behaviors . Unfortu-nately, no single metric has been developed that focuses exclusively on the auditory behaviors of

infants and young children and on the expected benefits to be derived from amplification . These

types of measures may be the only alternative for use with infants and toddlers . There is a

need to develop clearly focused measures that will allow us to infer some probabilistic cause and effect relationship with amplification .

Modify Existing Subjective Procedures (e.g., Loudness Growth, Quality Judgments, Preferred Frequency Response)

While some progress has been made in this area, more studies are needed to determine if procedures designed for adults can be modified to provide valid and reliable data in young chil-dren . The degree of success for any particular procedure is likely to be influenced by the degree of hearing loss, cognitive development, and the child's attention span, as well as the design fea-tures of the task and the stimuli used .

Identify Objective Correlates

As the age at which the identification of hearing loss and the initiation of amplification decreases, the need for objective measures of per-formance increases . There is a need to identify objective measures that correlate highly with the

SUMMARY

T he documentation of hearing aid efficacy in young children is a complex issue con- founded by a variety of variables . While some progress has been made in recent years, there is a great need for additional work in this area .

It is likely that a battery of measures, adapted to certain age ranges, will be required . Parent report measures may have a significant role to

play with the youngest children with more reliance on direct measures as children mature .

In test development, both the symbolic and

attentional constraints of children must be con-

sidered . It also will be important to develop test items that focus on the specific benefits to be expected from various forms of amplification if

children are to take advantage of emerging tech-

nologies .

Acknowledgment. Portions of the work reported in this paper were supported by a grant from the National Institute on Deafness and other Communication Disorders

(NIH-NIDCD Grant P-60 DC00982) . The author wishes

to thank Kathryn Beauchaine, Brenda Hoover, Dawna Lewis, and Richard Seewald for their helpful comments on this paper.

REFERENCES

American National Standards Institute. (1969) . Methods for the Calculation of the Articulation Index. (ANSI 53.5-1969 .) New York : Acoustical Society of America.

Anderson K . (1989) . Screening Instrument for Targeting Education Risk (SIFTER) . Austin, TX : Pro-Ed .

Anderson K, Matkin N. (1996) . Screening Instrument for Targeting Educational Risk in Preschool Children (Age 3-Kindergarten) (Preschool S.I.FTE.R .) . Tampa, FL : Educational Audiology Association.

Berger KW, Hagberg EN . (1982) . Hearing aid users atti-tudes and hearing aid usage . Monographs in Contem-porary Audiology 3 .

23

Page 11: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Journal of the American Academy of Audiology/Volume 10, Number 1, January 1999

Boothroyd A. (1991). Speech perception measures and their role in the evaluation of hearing aid performance. In : Feigin J, Stelmachowicz P, eds. Proceedings of the 1991 National Conference of Pediatric Amplification. Boys Town National Research Hospital, Omaha, NE, 1991,77-91 .

Gravel J, Fausel N, Liskow C, Chobot J. (in press) . Children's speech recognition in noise using dual micro-phone hearing aid technology. Ear Hear.

Haggard MP, Foster JR, Iredale FE . (1981) . Use and ben-efit of postaural aids in sensory hearing loss. Scand Audiol 10:45-52 .

Boothroyd A, Eran O, Hanin L. (1996) . Speech percep-tion and production in children with hearing impairment . In : Bess FH, Gravel JS, Tharpe AM, eds. Amplification for Children with Auditory Deficits . Nashville, TN : Bill Wilkerson Center Press, 55-74.

Byrne D, Ching T. (1997) . Optimising amplification for hearing-impaired children: I. Issues and procedures. Aust J Educ Deaf 3:21-28 .

Byrne D, Dillon H. (1986) . The National Acoustic Laboratories' (NAL) new procedure for selecting the gain and frequency response of a hearing aid. Ear Hear 7:257-265 .

Ching T, Newall P, Wigney D. (1994). Audio-visual and auditory paired comparison judgments by severely and profoundly hearing impaired children : reliability and fre-quency response preferences . Aust J Audiol 16:99-106.

Ching T, Newall P, Wigney D . (1996) . Frequency response and gain requirements of severely and profoundly hear-ing impaired children . Aust JAudiol 18:99-101 .

Christensen LA, Thomas TE . (1997) . The Use of Multiple-Memory Hearing-Aid Technology in Children. Presented at the 2nd Biennial Hearing Aid Research and Development Conference, September 1997, Bethesda, MD.

Cornelisse LE, Seewald RC, Jamieson DG . (1995). The input/output formula: a theoretical approach to the fit-ting of personal amplification devices. JAcoust Soc Am 97:1854-1864 .

Cox RM. (1981). Using LDLs to establish hearing aid lim-iting levels . Hear Instr 32:16-20.

Cox RM, Alexander GC . (1995) . The Abbreviated Profile of Hearing Aid Benefit. Ear Hear 16:176-186 .

Dillon H, James A, Ginis J. (1997) . Client Oriented Scale of Improvement (COSI) and its relationship to several other measures of benefit and satisfaction provided by hearing aids . J Am Acad Audiol 8:27-43 .

Dillon H, Koritschoner E, Battaglia J, Lovegrove R, Ginis J, Mavrias G, Carnie L, Ray P, Forsythe L, Towers E, Goulias H, Macaskil F. (1991) . Rehabilitation effective-ness I: assessing the needs of clients of a national hearing rehabilitation program. Aust J Audiol 13:55-65 .

Eisenberg LS, Levitt H. (1991) . Paired comparison judg-ments for hearing aid selection in children . Ear Hear 12:417-430 .

Feigin JA, Kopun JG, Stelmachowicz PG, Gorga MP (1989). Probe-tube microphone measures of ear-canal sound pressure levels in infants and children . Ear Hear 10:254-258 .

Gatehouse S. (1992). The time course and magnitude of perceptual acclimatization to frequency responses: evi-dence from monaural fitting of hearing aids . J Acoust Soc Am 92 :1258-1268 .

Gatehouse S . (1993). Role of perceptual acclimatization in the selection of frequency responses for hearing aids . J Am Acad Audiol 4:296-306 .

Hawkins DB, Walden BE, Montgomery A, Prosek RA. (1987) . Description and validation of an LDL procedure designed to select SSPL90 . Ear Hear 8:162-169 .

Hawkins DB, Yacullo WS . (1984) . Signal-to-noise advan-tage of binaural hearing aids and directional microphones under different levels of reverberation. J Speech Hear Disord 49:278-286 .

Hou Z, Thornton AR . (1994) . A model to evaluate and maximize hearing aid performance by integrating the articulation index across listening conditions. Ear Hear 15:105-112 .

Humes LE . (1991) . Understanding the speech-under-standing problems of the hearing impaired. J Am Acad Audiol 2:59-69 .

Humes LE, Dirks DD, Bell TS, Kincaid GE . (1987) . Recognition of nonsense syllables by hearing-impaired listeners and by noise-masked normal hearers . JAcoust Soc Am 81:765-773 .

Jordt J, Ching T, Byrne D, Birtles G, Dillon H, Flora RD, Hill M, Battaglia J, Kirievsky L. (1997) . Optimising ampli-fication for hearing impaired children : II . Aided speech audiogram. Aust J Educ Deaf 3:29-33 .

Katz DR, Elliott LL . (1978) . Development of a New Children's Speech Discrimination Test. Paper presented at the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, November 1978, Chicago, IL .

Kawell M, Kopun J, Stelmachowicz PG. (1988) . Loudness discomfort levels in children . Ear Hear 9:133-136 .

Kessler AR, Giolas TG, Maxon AB. (1990) . The Hearing Performance Inventory for Children (HPIC): Reliability and Validity. Poster presented at American Speech-Language-Hearing Association Convention, November 1990, Seattle, WA .

Kopun JG, Stelmachowicz PG . (in press) . The perceived communication difficulties of children with hearing loss . JAm Acad Audiol.

Kruger B . (1987) . An update on the external ear reso-nance in infants and young children. Ear Hear 8:333-336.

Kruger B, Ruben AJ. (1987) . The acoustic properties of the infant ear. A preliminary report . Acta Otolaryngol 103:578-585 .

Kuhl PK (1983) . The perception of speech in early infancy: four phenomena. In : Gerger S, Mencher G, eds. The Development ofAuditory Behavior . New York : Grime and Stratton, 187-218.

Macpherson BJ, Elfenbein JL, Schum RL, Bentler RA. (1991) . Thresholds of discomfort in young children . Ear Hear 12:184-190 .

Macrae J . (1982) . The validity of aided thresholds . Aust JAudiol 4:48-54 .

Macrae J, Frazier G. (1980). An investigation of variables affecting aided thresholds . Aust JAudiol 2:56-62 .

Moeller MP. (1993) . Auditory Learning: Efficacy and Evaluation . Presented at Developments in Pediatric Audiology: Assessment and Amplification, September 1993, Omaha, NE .

24

Page 12: Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children - Audiology · Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz supporting the child in extracting the rules of phonology and syntax

Hearing Aid Outcome Measures for Children/Stelmachowicz

Moeller MP. (1998). Early intervention of hearing loss in children . In : Bess FH, ed . Fourth International Symposium on Childhood Deafness . Nashville, TN : Bill Wilkerson Center Press, 305-310.

Moodie KS, Seewald RC, Sinclair ST. (1994) . Procedure for predicting real-ear hearing aid performance in young children. Am JAudiol 3:23-31 .

Morrongiello BA, Robson RC, Best CT, Clifton RK . (1984) . Trading relations in the perception of speech by 5-year-old children . J Exp Child Psychol 37 :231-250 .

Children with Auditory Deficits . Nashville, TN : Bill Wilkerson Center Press, 161-191.

Smaldino J, Anderson K (1997) . Development of the Listening Inuentory for Education. Presented at the Second Biennial Hearing Aid Research and Development Conference, September 1997, Bethesda, MD.

Snik AF, Hombergen GC . (1993). Hearing aid fitting of preschool and primary school children. An evaluation using the insertion gain measurement. Scand Audiol 22:245-250 .

Mueller HG, Mllion MC . (1990). An easy method for cal-culating the articulation index. Hear J 43:14-17 .

Nelson Barlow NL, Auslander MC, Rines D, Stelmacho-wicz PG . (1988) . Probe-tube microphone measures in hearing-impaired children and adults . Ear Hear 9:243-247 .

Nittrouer S. (1992) . Age-related differences in percep-tual effects of formant transitions with syllables and across syllable boundaries . J Phon 20:1-32.

Nittrouer S, Boothroyd A. (1990) . Context effects in phoneme and word recognition by young children and older adults . JAcoust Soc Am 87:2705-2715 .

Nittrouer S, Studdert-Kennedy M. (1987) . The role of coarticulatory effects on the perception of fricatives by children and adults . JAcoust Soc Am 30:319-329 .

Nozza RJ, Rossman RNF, Bond LC. (1991) . Infant-adult differences in unmasked thresholds for the discrimina-tion of consonant-vowel syllable pairs . Audiology 30:102-112 .

Oja GL, Schow RL . (1984) . Hearing aid evaluation based on measures of benefit, use, and satisfaction . Ear Hear 5:77-86 .

Pavlovic CV (1984) . Use of the articulation index for assessing residual auditory function in listeners with sensorineural hearing impairment . J Acoust Soc Am 75 :1253-1258 .

Pediatric Working Group. (1996) . Amplification for infants and children with hearing loss . Am JAudiol 5 :53-68 .

Robbins AM, Renshaw J, Berry S. (1991) . Evaluating meaningful auditory integration in profoundly hearing-impaired children . Am J Otol 12 (Suppl):144-150 .

Ross M, Lerman J. (1970) . A picture identification test for hearing-impaired children . J Speech Hear Res 13 :44-53 .

Ross M, Levitt H. (1997) . Consumer satisfaction is not enough : hearing aids are still about hearing. Semin Hear 18 :7-11.

Seewald RC. (1992) . The Desired Sensation Level method for fitting children : version 3 .0 . Hear J 45:36-41 .

Seewald R, Cornelisse L, Jenstad L, Moodie S, Shantz J, Pumford J. (1997) . Rationale for WDRCApplications in Pediatric Fittings. Presented at Developments in Pediatric Audiology: Current Issues in Assessment and Management, October 1997, Omaha, NE .

Seewald RC, Moodie KS, Sinclair ST, Cornelisse LE . (1996) . Traditional and theoretical approaches to select-ing amplification for infants and young children . In : Bess FH, Gravel JS, Tharpe AM, eds . Amplification for

SnikAF, Stollman MH. (1995) . Measured and calculated insertion gains in young children . Br JAudiol 29:7-11.

Snik AF, van den Borne S, Brokx JP, Hoekstra C. (1995) . Hearing aid fitting in profoundly hearing-impaired chil-dren . Comparison of prescription rules. Scand Audiol 24:225-230 .

Stapells DR, Gravel JS, Martin BA . (1995) . Thresholds for auditory brain stem responses to tones in notched noise from infants and young children with normal hear-ing or sensorineural hearing loss . Ear Hear 16:361-371 .

Stelmachowicz PG . (1991). Current issues in pediatric amplification . In : Feigin J, Stelmachowicz PG, eds. Pediatric Amplification: Proceedings of the 1991 National Conference. Omaha, NE : Boys Town National Research Hospital, 1-17 .

Stelmachowicz PG, Kalberer A, Lewis DE . (1996) . Situational hearing aid response profile (SHARP). In : Bess FH, Gravel JS, Tharpe AM, eds. Amplification for Children with Auditory Deficits . Nashville, TN : Bill Wilkerson Center Press, 193-213 .

Stelmachowicz PG, Lewis DE . (1988) . Some theoretical considerations concerning the relation between func-tional gain and insertion gain . J Speech Hear Res 31:491-496 .

Stelmachowicz PG, Lewis DE, Hoover B, Keefe D. (in press) . Objective and subjective effects of peak clipping vs . compression limiting in normal and hearing-impaired children. J Acoust Soc Am.

Stelmachowicz PG, Mace AL, Kopun JG, Carney E. (1993) . Long-term and short-term characteristics of speech: impli-cations for hearing aid selection for young children . J Speech Hear Res 36:609-620 .

Stuart A, Durieux-Smith A, Stenstrom R. (1991). Probe tube microphone measures of loudness discomfort levels in children . Ear Hear 12:140-143 .

Teghtsoonian M . (1980) . Children's scales of length and loudness : a developmental application of cross-modal matching . J Exp Child Psychol 30:290-307 .

Valente M, Fabry DA, Potts LG . (1995) . Recognition of speech in noise with hearing aids using dual microphones. J Am Acad Audiol 6:440-449 .

Weinstein BE . (1997) . Outcome measures in the hearing aid fitting/selection process . Trends in Amplification 2:117-137 .

Yoshinago-Itano C, Sedey AL, Coulter DK, Mehl AL . (in press) . The impact of newborn hearing screening on lan-guage development. Pediatrics .

Zurek PM, Delhorne LA. (1987). Consonant reception in noise by listeners with mild and moderate sensorineural hearing impairment . J Acoust Soc Am 82:1548-1559 .