1
ABSTRACT The purpose of this presentation is to evaluate the psychometric validity of a Spanish translated version of a family involvement questionnaire (FELP). Quantitative and qualitative analyses were conducted. Preliminary findings indicate the FELP is a reliable and valid measure for assessing family early literacy practices for Spanish-speaking families. Session attendees will learn about the validation of a self-report questionnaire. The benefits of analyzing item functioning will be discussed. INTRODUCTION -Research supports that family involvement in literacy activities is strongly related to child achievement (Evans et al., 2000). -Researchers have determined that indicators of a rich home environment include parent and guardiansresponsiveness to their childrens early literacy abilities, involvement with their children in language and literacy-based activities and routines, and the provision of literacy materials (Hart & Risley, 1995; Roberts et al., 2005; Sénéchal et al., 1998). -Much of the research on emergent literacy skills and parent involvement focus on English speaking and middle class families (see reviews: Fan & Chen, 2001; Manz et al., 2010, Mattingly et al., 2002) despite that the Spanish-speaking population is the fastest growing student population in United States schools (NCES, 2004). -Although this population is growing rapidly, there are few studies investigating the relationship between Spanish-speaking parents and their young childrens emergent literacy skills (see reviews: Fan & Chen, 2001; Manz et al., 2010, Mattingly et al., 2002). -The present study utilizes a mixed-methods design to evaluate the psychometric properties of this translated version of the FELP questionnaire in order to assess whether this is an appropriate measure of family practices and beliefs for a Hispanic and Spanish-speaking population. The aim of the study is to answer the following questions: 1 ∙ Does the original factor structure of the FELP remain consistent for a Spanish-speaking sample of parents of preschool and kindergarten age children? 2 ∙ Are the FELP factors/items relevant for a Spanish-speaking sample? 3 ∙ Does item functioning differ among diverse subgroups? DISCUSSION 1 ∙ Results indicate the FELP has good reliability and validity for Spanish-speaking parents of preschool age children. 2 ∙ Exploratory factor analysis of the FELP yielded support for a four-factor model, which did not align with the original theoretical construct of the FELP. 3 ∙ Internal consistency of the FELP was excellent. Cronbachs alpha ranged between .93 to .94. 4 ∙ This study also provides support for the construct validity of the FELP for parents of preschool age children. Rasch analysis revealed that the threshold for all four factors was in the appropriate order. This means the thresholds increased in a positive direction as the category label increased. 5 ∙ The people mean measure for Expectations was the highest at 4.35, which is almost 4 and a half a standard deviation above the mean of the items. This demonstrates that people typically endorsed many positive items for the Expectation factor, which ask parents about their beliefs about their childrens future education. The high mean measures for this scale suggest that most people positively endorsed those items. This finding is not surprising given that you might expect parents to believe positively about their childrens academic future. 6 ∙ Rasch analysis supported the reliability findings from the factor analysis. 7 ∙ Rasch analysis demonstrates that response patterns did not vary significantly (with the exception of one item) by primary language spoken in the home for the four factors. Limitations: Small sample size. All students recruited from preschool setting. Ratings can be subjective when using a Likert-scale. Future Research: Conduct evaluation again with a larger sample size. Include preschool age children who may not be attending a formal preschool setting in an effort to include a more diverse sample in future studies. Evaluate DIF between other subgroups (e.g., income, ethnicity). Compare the family measures to child outcome data with a diverse population. References: Andrich, D. (1978). A rating formulation for ordered response categories. Psychometrika, 43, 56173. Evans, M.A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 65-75. Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental Involvement and StudentsAcademic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 1-22. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing. Manz, P. H., Hughes, C., Barnabas, E., Bracaliello, C., & Ginsburg-Block, M. (2010). A descriptive review and meta-analysis of family-based emergent literacy interventions: To what extent is the research applicable to low-income, ethnic-minority or linguistically-diverse young children? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 409-431. Masters, G. N. (1982). A Rasch model for partial credit scoring. Psychometrika, 47, 149 74. Mattingly, D. J., Prislin, R., McKenzie, T. L., Rodriguez, J. L., & Kayzar, B. (2002). Evaluating evaluations: The case of parent involvement programs. Review of Educational Research, 72(4), 549-576. Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children's language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345-359. Sénéchal, M., Lefevre, J., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 96116. METHOD Participants 81 parents of preschool and kindergarten age children from preschool and kindergarten class in Orlando, Florida. Education: 51% Bachelors or higher, 30% HS or Some College, 19% No HS Diploma. Income: 68% under the poverty level, 32% above the poverty level. Ethnicity: 100% Hispanic (61% from Puerto Rico, 14% from Mexico, 14% from countries in the Caribbean Sea, and 11% from South American countries. Language: 56.% Spanish, 42% Spanish and English, and 2% English. Parents completed the FELP questionnaire. Instrumentation The FELP is a research-developed questionnaire (Likert Scale). There are 5 factors: Skill Building, Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment, Modeling and Monitoring, Expectations, and Child Skill. Parental Behaviors: Skill Building, Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment, and Modeling and Monitoring. Parental Beliefs: Expectations and Child Skill. Exploratory Factor Analysis used to obtain factors (majority Caucasian middle-class families) revealed the FELP had 5 robust factors with adequate alpha coefficients ranging from .77 to .87. Family Early Literacy Practices Questionnaire: A Validation Study for ELLs Data Analysis -Exploratory factor analysis Examine the factor structure of the FELP. -Reliability Internal consistency coefficients (Cronbachs alphas) and item-total correlations of the total scale and subscales of the FELP. -Rasch Analysis Examine the construct of the FELP. The Rating Scale Model (RSM), used to test the unidimensionality of each hypothesized factor (Andrich, 1978; Masters, 1982). DIF was used to evaluate the item response pattern among diverse subgroups. An acceptable range for the t value is between -2 to 2 (Bond & Fox, 2007). FELP Questionnaire 4 Dr. Laura Bailet and Dr. Cynthia Zettler-Greeley Nemours BrightStart! Dyslexia Initiative Jacksonville, FL Kandia Lewis and Dr. Marika Ginsburg-Block University of Delaware Newark, DE FELP Factor Structure Original F1-SB F2-IR F3-MM F4- Ex F5-CS Item Examples I teach my child letter sounds. I try to make reading a fun time for my child. I make sure my child sees me reading. I am confident that my child will receive a good education. Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at reading new words. I teach my child to read words. I let my child select the books when we read together. I read to myself. I am confident that my child will complete high school. Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at identifying the sounds of letters. Rasch Analysis Summary Statistics Item Response Pattern Person Factor Separation Reliability Real RMSE Model RMSE Mean Measure Skill Building 2.57 .87 .43 .38 1.13 Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment 3.70 .93 .32 .30 .29 Expectations 1.90 .78 1.10 1.00 4.35 Child Skill 2.72 .88 .69 .62 1.16 Item Factor Separation Reliability Real RMSE Model RMSE Mean Measure Skill Building 3.34 .92 .15 .14 .00 Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment 5.21 .96 .14 .13 .00 Expectations 6.45 .98 .31 .30 .00 Child Skill 3.80 .94 .19 .18 .00 RESULTS Factor Loadings Structure Matrix FELP ITEMS SB IR CS EX FELP18 I teach my child the names of letters. .880 FELP19 I teach my child letter sounds. .868 FELP21 I encourage my child to write. .832 FELP20 I teach my child to read words. .760 FELP28 I work with my child on skills that he/she learned in school. .724 FELP7 I help my child with writing. .713 FELP3 I sing songs with my child. .683 FELP5 I work on spelling words with my child. .669 FELP24 I correct my childs mistakes in reading. .637 FELP4 I show my child how to sound out words. .619 FELP29 I praise my child for his/her efforts in reading. .602 FELP12 I ask my child to read to me. .579 FELP32 I teach my child about words that start with the same sound (e.g. Peter Piper). .536 FELP31 I rhyme words with my child (e.g. cat, bat, sat). .451 FELP15 I read to my child. .801 FELP14 I let my child select the books when we read together. .772 FELP11 I try to make reading a fun time for my child. .745 FELP26 I have my child repeat a story to me in his/her own words. .738 FELP9 I tell my child stories. .732 FELP10 I encourage my child to read. .721 FELP25 I make sure my child sees me reading. .712 FELP22 I read the same books to my child over and over. .702 FELP16 I read to myself. .692 FELP13 I talk with my child about stories he/she has read or heard. .687 FELP23 I ask my child to try to read store signs and labels. .668 FELP6 I ask my child questions about the books that we read. .668 FELP30 My child asks an adult to read with him/her. .588 FELP17 My spouse or partner reads to himself/herself. .569 FELP27 I provide books for my child to read. .562 FELP2 I set aside quiet time for my child to read. .504 FELP8 I ask my child to write cards or letters to family members. .440 FELP1 I take my child to the library or bookstore. .366 FELP35 Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at identifying the sounds of letters. .890 FELPQ38 Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at spelling words. .855 FELPQ34 Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at naming the letters of the alphabet. .851 FELPQ37 Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at understanding and discussing a storybook that was read to him/her. .841 FELP36 Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at reading new words. .839 FELP33 Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at using and understanding words in conversation. .791 FELPQ43 I am confident that my child will continue education beyond high school. .962 FELPQ42 I am confident that my child will complete high school. .951 FELPQ45 I am confident that my child will have a steady job after finishing his/her schooling. .948 FELPQ41 I am confident that my child will succeed in school. .905 FELPQ44 I am confident that my child will finish college. .872 FELPQ40 I am confident that my child will receive a good education. .731 FELPQ39 I am confident that my child will have the motivation to complete schooling. .668 Reliability Statistics Factor Means, Std. Deviation, Cronbach’s Alpha, Item Correlation FELP M(SD) α Item- total r F1 Skill Building (14 items) 3.85 (.83) .931 .30-.83 F2 Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment (18 items) 3.21 (.88) .939 .16-.78 F3 Expectations (7 items) 4.70 (.59) .947 .52-.95 F4 Child Skill (6 items) 3.68 (.94) .938 .65-.81 -2 -1.5 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 1 FELP1 2 FELP2 6 FELP6 8 FELP8 9 FELP9 10 FELP10 11 FELP11 13 FELP13 14 FELP14 15 FELP15 16 FELP16 17 FELP17 22 FELP22 23 FELP23 25 FELP25 26 FELP26 27 FELP27 30 FELP30 t-value Item Interactive Reading/Enjoyment Language Spanish English Both -2 -1.5 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 3 FELP3 4 FELP4 5 FELP5 7 FELP7 12 FELP12 18 FELP18 19 FELP19 20 FELP20 21 FELP21 24 FELP24 28 FELP28 29 FELP29 31 FELP31 32 FELP32 t-value Item Skill Building Language Spanish English Both -2 -1.5 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 6 FELP6 33 FELP33 34 FELP34 35 FELP35 36 FELP36 37 FELP37 38 FELP38 t-value Item Child Skill Language Spanish English Both -2 -1.5 -1 -0.5 0 0.5 1 1.5 2 2.5 3 6 FELP6 39 FELP39 40 FELP40 41 FELP41 42 FELP42 43 FELP43 44 FELP44 45 FELP45 t-value Item Expectations Language Spanish English Both

Family Early Literacy Practices Questionnaire: A

  • Upload
    others

  • View
    10

  • Download
    0

Embed Size (px)

Citation preview

Page 1: Family Early Literacy Practices Questionnaire: A

ABSTRACT

The purpose of this presentation is to evaluate the psychometric validity of a Spanish translated version of a family involvement questionnaire (FELP). Quantitative and qualitative analyses were conducted. Preliminary findings indicate the FELP is a reliable and valid measure for assessing family early literacy practices for Spanish-speaking families. Session attendees will learn about the validation of a self-report questionnaire. The benefits of analyzing item functioning will be discussed.

INTRODUCTION

-Research supports that family involvement in literacy activities is strongly related to child achievement (Evans et al., 2000).

-Researchers have determined that indicators of a rich home environment include parent and guardians’ responsiveness to their children’s early literacy abilities, involvement with their children in language and literacy-based activities and routines, and the provision of literacy materials (Hart & Risley, 1995; Roberts et al., 2005; Sénéchal et al., 1998).

-Much of the research on emergent literacy skills and parent involvement focus on English speaking and middle class

families (see reviews: Fan & Chen, 2001; Manz et al., 2010, Mattingly et al., 2002) despite that the Spanish-speaking population is the fastest growing student population in United States schools (NCES, 2004).

-Although this population is growing rapidly, there are few studies investigating the relationship between Spanish-speaking

parents and their young children’s emergent literacy skills (see reviews: Fan & Chen, 2001; Manz et al., 2010, Mattingly et al., 2002).

-The present study utilizes a mixed-methods design to evaluate the psychometric properties of this translated version of

the FELP questionnaire in order to assess whether this is an appropriate measure of family practices and beliefs for a Hispanic and Spanish-speaking population. The aim of the study is to answer the following questions:

1 ∙ Does the original factor structure of the FELP remain consistent for a Spanish-speaking sample of parents of preschool and kindergarten age children? 2 ∙ Are the FELP factors/items relevant for a Spanish-speaking sample? 3 ∙ Does item functioning differ among diverse subgroups?

DISCUSSION 1 ∙ Results indicate the FELP has good reliability and validity for Spanish-speaking parents of preschool age children. 2 ∙ Exploratory factor analysis of the FELP yielded support for a four-factor model, which did not align with the original theoretical construct of the FELP. 3 ∙ Internal consistency of the FELP was excellent. Cronbach’s alpha ranged between .93 to .94. 4 ∙ This study also provides support for the construct validity of the FELP for parents of preschool age children. Rasch analysis revealed that the threshold for all four factors was in

the appropriate order. This means the thresholds increased in a positive direction as the category label increased. 5 ∙ The people mean measure for Expectations was the highest at 4.35, which is almost 4 and a half a standard deviation above the mean of the items. This demonstrates that

people typically endorsed many positive items for the Expectation factor, which ask parent’s about their beliefs about their children’s future education. The high mean measures for this scale suggest that most people positively endorsed those items. This finding is not surprising given that you might expect parents to believe positively about their children’s academic future.

6 ∙ Rasch analysis supported the reliability findings from the factor analysis. 7 ∙ Rasch analysis demonstrates that response patterns did not vary significantly (with the exception of one item) by primary language spoken in the home for the four factors.

Limitations: ◦ Small sample size. ◦ All students recruited from preschool setting. ◦ Ratings can be subjective when using a Likert-scale.

Future Research: ◦ Conduct evaluation again with a larger sample size. ◦ Include preschool age children who may not be attending a formal preschool setting in an effort to include a more diverse sample in future studies. ◦ Evaluate DIF between other subgroups (e.g., income, ethnicity). ◦ Compare the family measures to child outcome data with a diverse population.

References: Andrich, D. (1978). A rating formulation for ordered response categories. Psychometrika, 43, 561–73. Evans, M.A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 65-75. Fan, X., & Chen, M. (2001). Parental Involvement and Students’ Academic Achievement: A Meta-Analysis. Educational Psychology Review, 13(1), 1-22. Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing. Manz, P. H., Hughes, C., Barnabas, E., Bracaliello, C., & Ginsburg-Block, M. (2010). A descriptive review and meta-analysis of family-based emergent literacy interventions: To what extent is the research applicable

to low-income, ethnic-minority or linguistically-diverse young children? Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 409-431. Masters, G. N. (1982). A Rasch model for partial credit scoring. Psychometrika, 47, 149 –74. Mattingly, D. J., Prislin, R., McKenzie, T. L., Rodriguez, J. L., & Kayzar, B. (2002). Evaluating evaluations: The case of parent involvement programs. Review of Educational Research, 72(4), 549-576. Roberts, J., Jurgens, J., & Burchinal, M. (2005). The role of home literacy practices in preschool children's language and emergent literacy skills. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(2), 345-359. Sénéchal, M., Lefevre, J., Thomas, E. M., & Daley, K. E. (1998). Differential effects of home literacy experiences on the development of oral and written language. Reading Research Quarterly, 33(1), 96–116.

METHOD Participants

• 81 parents of preschool and kindergarten age children from preschool and kindergarten class in Orlando, Florida. • Education: 51% Bachelors or higher, 30% HS or Some College, 19% No HS Diploma. • Income: 68% under the poverty level, 32% above the poverty level. • Ethnicity: 100% Hispanic (61% from Puerto Rico, 14% from Mexico, 14% from countries in the Caribbean Sea, and 11% from South American countries. • Language: 56.% Spanish, 42% Spanish and

English, and 2% English. • Parents completed the FELP questionnaire. Instrumentation • The FELP is a research-developed questionnaire (Likert Scale).

• There are 5 factors: Skill Building, Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment, Modeling and Monitoring, Expectations, and Child Skill. • Parental Behaviors: Skill Building, Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment, and Modeling and Monitoring. • Parental Beliefs: Expectations and Child Skill.

• Exploratory Factor Analysis used to obtain factors (majority Caucasian middle-class families) revealed the FELP had 5 robust factors with adequate alpha coefficients ranging from .77 to .87.

Family Early Literacy Practices Questionnaire: A Validation Study for ELLs

Results

Data Entered Data Analyzed

Data Collected

Data Analysis -Exploratory factor analysis

• Examine the factor structure of the FELP.

-Reliability • Internal consistency

coefficients (Cronbach’s alphas) and item-total correlations of the total scale and subscales of the FELP.

-Rasch Analysis

• Examine the construct of the FELP.

• The Rating Scale Model (RSM), used to test the unidimensionality of each hypothesized factor (Andrich, 1978; Masters, 1982).

• DIF was used to evaluate the item response pattern among diverse subgroups. An acceptable range for the t value is between -2 to 2 (Bond & Fox, 2007).

FELP Questionnaire

Skill Building (n=10)

.92

.94 Expectations (n=7)

Child Skills (n=6)

Interactive Reading / Enjoyment (n=8)

.86

4

Dr. Laura Bailet and Dr. Cynthia Zettler-Greeley

Nemours BrightStart! Dyslexia Initiative

Jacksonville, FL

Kandia Lewis and Dr. Marika Ginsburg-Block

University of Delaware Newark, DE

FELP Factor Structure – Original

F1-SB F2-IR F3-MM F4- Ex F5-CS

Item Examples I teach my child letter sounds.

I try to make reading a fun time for my child.

I make sure my child sees me reading.

I am confident that my child will receive a good education.

Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at reading new words.

I teach my child to read words.

I let my child select the books when we read together.

I read to myself. I am confident that my child will complete high school.

Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at identifying the sounds of letters.

Rasch Analysis Summary Statistics

Item Response Pattern

Person Factor Separation Reliability Real RMSE Model RMSE Mean Measure

Skill Building 2.57 .87 .43 .38 1.13

Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment

3.70 .93 .32 .30 .29

Expectations 1.90 .78 1.10 1.00 4.35

Child Skill 2.72 .88 .69 .62 1.16

Item Factor Separation Reliability Real RMSE Model RMSE Mean Measure

Skill Building 3.34 .92 .15 .14 .00

Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment

5.21 .96 .14 .13 .00

Expectations 6.45 .98 .31 .30 .00

Child Skill 3.80 .94 .19 .18 .00

RESULTS Factor Loadings – Structure Matrix

FELP ITEMS SB IR CS EX

FELP18 – I teach my child the names of letters. .880

FELP19 – I teach my child letter sounds. .868

FELP21 – I encourage my child to write. .832

FELP20 – I teach my child to read words. .760

FELP28 – I work with my child on skills that he/she learned in school.

.724

FELP7 – I help my child with writing. .713

FELP3 – I sing songs with my child. .683

FELP5 – I work on spelling words with my child. .669

FELP24 – I correct my child’s mistakes in reading. .637

FELP4 – I show my child how to sound out words. .619

FELP29 – I praise my child for his/her efforts in reading. .602

FELP12 – I ask my child to read to me. .579

FELP32 – I teach my child about words that start with the same sound (e.g. Peter Piper).

.536

FELP31 – I rhyme words with my child (e.g. cat, bat, sat). .451

FELP15 – I read to my child. .801

FELP14 – I let my child select the books when we read together. .772

FELP11 – I try to make reading a fun time for my child. .745

FELP26 – I have my child repeat a story to me in his/her own words.

.738

FELP9 – I tell my child stories. .732

FELP10 – I encourage my child to read. .721

FELP25 – I make sure my child sees me reading. .712

FELP22 – I read the same books to my child over and over. .702

FELP16 – I read to myself. .692

FELP13 – I talk with my child about stories he/she has read or heard.

.687

FELP23 – I ask my child to try to read store signs and labels. .668

FELP6 – I ask my child questions about the books that we read. .668

FELP30 – My child asks an adult to read with him/her. .588

FELP17 – My spouse or partner reads to himself/herself. .569

FELP27 – I provide books for my child to read. .562

FELP2 – I set aside quiet time for my child to read. .504

FELP8 – I ask my child to write cards or letters to family members.

.440

FELP1 – I take my child to the library or bookstore. .366

FELP35 – Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at identifying the sounds of letters.

.890

FELPQ38 – Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at spelling words.

.855

FELPQ34 – Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at naming the letters of the alphabet.

.851

FELPQ37 – Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at understanding and discussing a storybook that was read to him/her.

.841

FELP36 – Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at reading new words.

.839

FELP33 – Compared to other children his/her age, my child is just as good or better at using and understanding words in conversation.

.791

FELPQ43 – I am confident that my child will continue education beyond high school.

.962

FELPQ42 – I am confident that my child will complete high school.

.951

FELPQ45 – I am confident that my child will have a steady job after finishing his/her schooling.

.948

FELPQ41 – I am confident that my child will succeed in school. .905

FELPQ44 – I am confident that my child will finish college. .872

FELPQ40 – I am confident that my child will receive a good education.

.731

FELPQ39 – I am confident that my child will have the motivation to complete schooling.

.668

Reliability Statistics

Factor Means, Std. Deviation, Cronbach’s Alpha, Item Correlation

FELP M(SD) α Item-total r

F1 – Skill Building (14 items)

3.85 (.83)

.931 .30-.83

F2 – Interactive Reading/ Enjoyment (18 items)

3.21

(.88)

.939 .16-.78

F3 –Expectations (7 items)

4.70 (.59)

.947 .52-.95

F4 – Child Skill (6 items)

3.68 (.94)

.938 .65-.81

-2

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

1 F

EL

P1

2 F

EL

P2

6 F

EL

P6

8 F

EL

P8

9 F

EL

P9

10

FE

LP

10

11

FE

LP

11

13

FE

LP

13

14

FE

LP

14

15

FE

LP

15

16

FE

LP

16

17

FE

LP

17

22

FE

LP

22

23

FE

LP

23

25

FE

LP

25

26

FE

LP

26

27

FE

LP

27

30

FE

LP

30

t-va

lue

Item

Interactive Reading/Enjoyment Language

Spanish

English

Both

-2

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

3 F

EL

P3

4 F

EL

P4

5 F

EL

P5

7 F

EL

P7

12

FE

LP

12

18

FE

LP

18

19

FE

LP

19

20

FE

LP

20

21

FE

LP

21

24

FE

LP

24

28

FE

LP

28

29

FE

LP

29

31

FE

LP

31

32

FE

LP

32

t-va

lue

Item

Skill Building Language

Spanish

English

Both

-2

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

6 F

EL

P6

33

FE

LP

33

34

FE

LP

34

35

FE

LP

35

36

FE

LP

36

37

FE

LP

37

38

FE

LP

38

t-va

lue

Item

Child Skill Language

Spanish

English

Both

-2

-1.5

-1

-0.5

0

0.5

1

1.5

2

2.5

3

6 F

EL

P6

39

FE

LP

39

40

FE

LP

40

41

FE

LP

41

42

FE

LP

42

43

FE

LP

43

44

FE

LP

44

45

FE

LP

45

t-va

lue

Item

Expectations Language

Spanish

English

Both