Constructivism and Mathematics Teaching by Steve Atkins

Embed Size (px)

DESCRIPTION

An overview of the role of constructivism on shaping a philosophy for mathematics teaching and learning

Citation preview

  • 1

    Constructivism and Mathematics Teaching

    Dr. Steve Atkins

    The purpose of this article is to delineate a personal philosophy of mathematics teaching

    that is consistent with research and theoretic literature on radical and social constructivism. This

    eclectic approach to mathematics teaching places equal importance on students cognition and

    their socio-cultural contexts. Cobb et al. (1992) suggested that coordinating findings that derive

    from each perspective has led to advancements in the field of mathematics teaching and is

    necessary to understanding learning. Key components of an eclectic teaching philosophy based

    on combining radical and social constructivism have included: (a) Knowledge is actively

    constructed, not passively received; (b) teaching should focus on student thinking; (c) teaching

    should facilitate conceptual change; (d) teaching should be interactive; (e) teaching should

    support individual and collaborative learning arrangements; (f) teaching should involve

    authentic tasks; and (g) teaching should encourage multiple representations and connections.

    The article is organized into two parts: (a) an overview of radical and social

    constructivism; and (b) a detailed description and rationale for the key components of the

    teaching philosophy listed above based on theoretic and research findings in mathematics

    education.

    Constructivism

    Over the past two decades researchers have been developing a new view of what it means

    to teach mathematics. Constructivist philosophies have contributed to shaping these efforts by

    providing researchers and mathematics educators useful ways to understand learning.

    Constructivism encompasses a range of beliefs about learning. One extreme is radical

    constructivism, based on the views of Piaget (1978), which views knowledge as individually

    constructed; and the other extreme is social constructivism, consistent with the views of

  • 2

    Vygotsky (1962), which asserts that learning occurs solely through socio-cultural interactions.

    Simon (1995) claims that the difference depends on the focus of the observer. The radical

    constructivist emphasizes a cognitive orientation, focusing on the psychology of how an

    individual integrates knowledge; whereas social constructivists focus on social interactions and

    the individuals cultural context (Cobb, 1989).

    Many researchers see either extreme as too limiting (Cobb, 1995, Simon, 1995). Cobb

    (1994) states that each perspective, the socio-cultural and the cognitive, equally contributes

    toward understanding learning. He states that cognitive and social influences are complimentary

    in that social interactions implicitly assume an actively constructing child....cultural practice is

    the background against which self-organization comes to the fore. (p.17). Despite the

    differences in the locus of cognitive development, radical constructivists such as Piaget (1970)

    do not deny the importance of others in the development of cognition, nor did Vygotsky (1987)

    deny the primacy of the active construction of knowledge by the cognizing individual.

    Key Components of Constructivist Teaching Practices

    Knowledge is Constructed, Not Received

    Kilpatrick (1987) states that constructivist epistimologies consists of three major

    premises: (1) knowledge is actively constructed, not passively received or transmitted, (2)

    coming to know is an adaptive process of organizing ones experiences, and (3) learning does not

    involve discovering an independent pre-existing world outside the mind of the knower. These

    statements together form an epistemology that is in sharp contrast with the traditional view that

    sees learning as an encoding of an objective reality out there. Acceptance of these premises by

    the mathematics education community has consequently resulted in a shift from behaviorist and

  • 3

    positivist models of learning toward cognitive and interpretive models (Mathematical Sciences

    education Board [MSEB], 1989).

    Much of constructivism is based on the work of Piaget (1978) who viewed learners as

    builders of knowledge, not passive receptors or tabula rasas. From this perspective, learners

    take in knowledge and relate it to previously assimilated knowledge in order to fit it within

    their existing mental models (Simon, 1995). Hence, thinking is considered an interpretive act,

    which contrasts significantly with the more traditional view of learning that sees concepts as

    transferable and ready-made from teachers to students. Thus, constructivist theory suggests

    that, rather than simply accepting new information without question, students interpret

    everything they see and hear and then remember new information in relation to what they

    already know. Students enter the classroom with their own cognitive structures constructed from

    their own past experiences. This idiosyncratically constructed knowledge may be viable and

    useful, invalid, incomplete, or it may be hinder students subsequent learning. Simon (1995)

    states that the knowledge we have constructed helps us to make sense of, interpret, and predict

    events in our experiential world.

    The assumption that humans have no access to an objective reality independent of

    their way of knowing it seems counterintuitive to many mathematics educators (Wilson et al.,

    1995). Some educators believe that an acceptance of non-dualistic thought, where learners arrive

    at different but legitimate understandings, lead inevitably toward solipsism or an anything goes

    mentality (Simon, 1995). In response, Wilson (1995), a social constructivist, suggests that

    learners negotiate the meaning of their experiences by interacting with others to arrive at shared

    understandings. Von Glaserfeld (1987), states that student understanding of a concept derives its

    validity not from its accuracy in describing the real world, but whether or not it works. A concept

    works or is viable to the extent that it does what it needs to do: to make sense of our perceptions,

  • 4

    to make predictions accurately, or to solve a problem. Wilson et al. (1995) conjecture that the

    denial of dualism may result in classroom practices that are more sensitive toward personal

    meanings and multiple perspectives of the world.

    Social constructivists contend that the manner in which students learn is inseparable from

    the content (Schoenfeld, 1985). This is quite different from the traditional didactic education that

    assumes a separation between knowing and doing that treats knowledge as a self sufficient

    substance. A view that knowing mathematics is doing mathematics suggests that

    mathematics is not a fixed body of procedures, but rather a growing and changing science of

    quantity and pattern, whose practice involves experimentation, reasoning, and argumentation

    (MSEB, 1989). Wilson et al. (1995) suggest that instruction should recognize the dynamic nature

    of knowledge and encourage students to engage in a continuing search for improved

    understanding.

    Past and current research supports the claim that knowledge is constructed and not

    passively received. Research conducted by Brownell (1935) supports this view. At the beginning

    of the school year Brownell gave a group of third graders (n=57) a test in 100 simple addition

    combinations. They were taught these combinations in grades one and two by methods which

    agree with drill theory. Thirty-two children were chosen for interviews, ten students were

    selected that had the highest score, thirteen who had average scores, and nineteen who had low

    scores. Interviews attempted to discover how each child calculated sums and thought of the

    numbers and processes employed. The interview revealed that 22.7% of the combinations

    involved counting; that 14% were solved indirectly; that 23.8 % were incorrectly guessed; and

    that only 39.5% were known as memorized associations. The fact that students demonstrated

    strategies that were quite different from the learning intended by the teacher supports the notion

    that students do construct their own knowledge.

  • 5

    The study by Brownell demonstrates the inadequacy of teaching methods that are based

    on transmission models where teachers seek to transfer knowledge to passive students.

    Constructivists such as Blais have described how these methods effect the learner. Blais (1988)

    states that traditional teaching in mathematics that involves giving students a maximum of

    explanation implies the creation of a listener-follower role for students. Such a role, he

    conjectures, contributes to dependence, fosters the growth of learned helplessness, and

    encourages students to memorize information instead of construct robust knowledge structures.

    Blais contends that traditional mathematics courses focused too much on transmitting content at

    the expense of thought processes, and he felt that the focus should be on the development of

    thinking skills and conceptual understanding.

    That all knowledge is constructed differently and can interfere with subsequent learning

    has been an important focus in constructivist teaching. Luchens (1942) shows that students tend

    to solve problems using a single approach if the approach was used extensively prior to solving

    new problems, even though a different way would be more productive. For example, Luchens

    found that 64% of the college students in an experimental group were unable to solve the

    following problem: Measure 25 cups using jugs with 28, 76 and 3-cup capacities. The

    experimental group had worked a previous set of problems by applying a rule - the problems

    could all be solved by adding two of the first jug listed and subtracting one of each of the other

    two jugs. However, only 5% of a control group who were given no rules failed to solve this

    problem. This illustrates how (a) relying on algorithms for solving problems can inhibit students

    abilities to solve similar problems when the learned algorithm does not apply, and (b) giving

    students algorithms for solving problems does not necessarily produce viable knowledge.

    Norman and Pritchard (1994) have described how students prior

    conceptualization of functions as processes rather than objects causes difficulty for these

  • 6

    students in calculus. Being able to see a function as an object is essential in calculus for gaining a

    basic understanding of several concepts such as function composition, the chain rule, and

    integration (Ferrini-Mundy & Graham, 1991; Dubinsky, 1991). Norman and Pritchard state that

    students tendency to process is carried over into calculus. They offer evidence based on

    interviews with college students, that this limited intuition is often a cognitive obstacle toward

    students gaining a comprehensive understanding of the derivative and integral concept. During

    interviews students were asked to find F(a) when F(x) is defined by F(x) = ax f(t)dt. Most

    students responded that F(x) doesnt exist unless a formula for f(t) is known. Norman and

    Pritchard assert that students tendency to think of functions as processes impeded their ability to

    solve this and similar problems during interviews.

    Teaching Should Focus on How Students Think

    Taken together Brownell (1935), Luchins (1942), and Norman and Pritchard (1992)

    demonstrate that students do construct their own knowledge, and that students prior

    constructions influence subsequent learning. Knowing what mistakes students are likely to make

    and common errors in understanding will help teachers assist students in meaningful

    constructivist learning activities. For example, Carpenter et al. (1989) recruited 40 first-grade

    teachers to participate in a month long workshop on Cognitively Guided Instruction. Cognitively

    Guided Instruction (CGI) involves having teachers learn about research on childrens thinking

    processes. At the workshop Carpenter shared findings from a previous study, showing how five-

    and six-year-olds can solve many types of complex addition and subtraction word problems.

    Teachers then discussed and debated the meanings of these findings and also interviewed

    children to see for themselves whether children actually solved problems as reported by the

    researchers.

    One year following the workshop, workshop participants demonstrated stronger beliefs

  • 7

    that children construct, rather than receive mathematical knowledge; and, to facilitate this

    construction, instruction should not center around presentations but around childrens own

    knowledge and developing understandings. At the end of the school year assessments were made

    including a pretest and post-test of student achievement with various measures of problem-

    solving ability, measures of student confidence, and student interviews. Students in CGI teachers

    classes did better than students in control teachers classes on written and interview measures of

    both complex problem solving and number fact knowledge.

    Conceptual Change and Teaching

    It has been shown that research supports the view that knowledge is actively constructed,

    that these constructions effect new learning in desirable and sometimes undesirable ways, and

    that teachers knowledge of student constructions can impact student learning. Implications are

    that mathematics teachers must be knowledgeable not only in their content areas, but also in the

    research literature on how students learn and on instructional strategies that foster learning. A

    more detailed description of how conceptual growth occurs within individuals, from a cognitive

    and social perspective, will serve as a framework for discussing the research on what teachers

    might do to affect conceptual change.

    Simon (1995) states that students modify their understanding by directly confronting

    misconceptions. Piaget (1978) asserts that when misconceptions occur cognitive development

    takes place through the mechanism of equilibration. When presented with a new idea, something

    to learn, a learners cognitive equilibrium is disturbed. Either the student will ignore the new

    idea, and return to a state of equilibrium or modify ones cognitive structures so that the new idea

    is incorporated. Reflection on successful adaptive operations (reflective abstraction) leads to new

    or modified concepts. Piaget (1970) states that an unstimulating environment would result in

    infrequent states of disequilibrium providing the learner with little or no reason to learn.

  • 8

    Nussbaum and Novick (1982) state that constructivist researchers have focused on the

    identification of misconceptions in the cognitive structures of learners. They contend that

    standard methods such as lecture, demonstrations, and class tests are poor methods to identify

    and correct many kinds of misconceptions. Given the research findings that reveal how

    misconceptions hinder student ability to process information properly (Norman & Pritchard,

    1992), models are needed that support ways in which teachers can identify and assist students in

    affecting change in their conceptual frameworks, thereby eliminating misconceptions in favor of

    more viable schemas.

    According to Fischbein, (1990) beliefs are often robust, but can be addressed by inducing

    cognitive conflict during instruction. Research conducted by Skane (1993) investigated the

    efficacy of a conceptual change model in amending college students misconceptions about the

    distributive law - overgeneralizations of linearity that produce such errors as log (x+3) = log (x)

    + log(3). To amend this misconception in a community college precalculus course (n=68), the

    author implemented a method called a constructivist teaching sequence consisting of five stages

    of instruction: orientation, elicitation, restructuring, application, and review (reflection). During

    the elicitation phase students were given questionnaires containing problems that often involve

    student overgeneralization errors. Restructuring activities involved students discussing their

    answers to the questionnaires, negotiating reasons and answers until consensus was achieved,

    and noting in writing any changes in their understanding. This was followed by whole class

    discussion, with students presenting as many different reasons as possible and observing

    similarities and differences in items. Finally, the students reflected on the whole exercise and

    responded in writing to statements such as I used to think... with the least step being the most

    important one. Results indicate that the conceptual change model was effective immediately and

    over time as a teaching strategy to amend misconceptions. Pretest and posttest results

  • 9

    demonstrated positive amendment of distributive law misconceptions. Student comments were

    positive about the experience.

    In summary, research reveals that students have well-entrenched misconceptions. In order

    to change these misconceptions constructivists theorize that students must undergo conceptual

    change by restructuring their mental schemas. Skanes models contribute toward our

    understanding of how conceptual change can be facilitated within the context of whole class

    instruction. Other approaches such as cooperative learning (Yackel et al, 1991), discussed later in

    this paper, have also been used to facilitate amendment of misconceptions. Models of teaching

    have been designed to facilitate conceptual change through classroom discourse.

    Planning and Classroom Discourse

    The teachers role in the classroom from a constructivist teaching perspective is seen as

    an interactive one. It views communication in the classroom as a two-way street, where teachers

    ask probing questions to assess or to facilitate students understanding and where students are

    encouraged to question the teacher. Hence, the teacher is seen as a facilitator of learning rather

    than a transmitter of knowledge. Communication is vital to constructivist teaching in order to

    recognize misconceptions when they arise and to provide opportunities to challenge students

    misconceptions through discourse.

    Piaget (1980) and Vygotsky (1962) provide reasonable explanations for concept

    development occurring as a consequence of student-teacher interactions. Piaget emphasized that

    communication provides opportunities to take different points of view into account which, he

    claims, offer the potential to produce cognitive conflicts. Resolving the conflicts leads directly to

    cognitive development (Piaget & Inhelder, 1967). Vygotsky (1987), in contrast, asserts that

    learning develops between a learner and a more competent person who moves the learner from

    where he is to where he can be with help (zone of proximal development). By actively engaging

  • 10

    in discourse with teachers and more competent peers Vygotsky believed that learners internalize

    the methods of more expert problem solvers.

    Simon (1995) has developed a model for teaching consistent with a cognitive approach

    that elaborates on the teachers responsibilities in the mathematics classroom. His model was

    developed from analyzing data from a whole-class constructivist teaching experiment he

    conducted to explore the pedagogical implications of constructivism. Simons model of teaching

    involves three components (1) teachers learning goal, (2) teachers plan for learning activities,

    (3) and teachers hypothesis of students learning processes. The basis of the design is to

    hypothesize what the learner might learn and finding ways of fostering this learning. Class

    lessons consist of small-group problem solving and teacher led whole-class discussions. The goal

    is based on relating the teachers mathematical understanding to the teachers hypotheses about

    students learning.

    The hypothesis of student learning refers to the teachers prediction as to how learning

    might proceed. The generation of hypothetical learning is made prior to classroom instruction

    and is the process by which the teacher develops a plan for classroom activity. Simon states that

    although learning proceeds along idiosyncratic paths, mathematical activity often occurs in

    predictable ways, and that many students in the same class can benefit from the same

    mathematical task. The hypotheses of students learning processes is based on information from

    a variety of sources: experience with students in a related area, pretesting, past experience with

    similar groups, and research data on difficulties students have with particular concepts. Simon

    uses a rich set of problem situations that challenges students understanding and exposes their

    misconceptions and conceptual difficulties.

    During a teaching episode Simon poses questions to attempt to create disequilibrium with

    his students. He states that as he interacts, listens to and observes students, he constantly assesses

  • 11

    students thinking, which may in turn lead to a new or modified hypothetical trajectory of how

    learning will proceed. He continually engages in adjusting his hypothesis to better reflect his

    enhanced knowledge, and poses questions to help eliminate misconceptions that were exposed

    during the teaching process.

    In a similar study Tzur (1995) conducted a teaching experiment to account for possible

    relationships between classroom discourse and cognitive and social-emotional aspects of their

    fraction learning. Fifty-one teaching episodes were conducted with fourth and fifth grade

    children. The episodes were videotaped for later analysis.

    From his analysis of the data Tzur developed a constructivist model of teacher-learner

    interaction. The model stresses that during planning periods, the teacher infers the childrens

    fraction schemes and operations, and plans initial, reflective, and anticipatory tasks that are likely

    to activate the childrens schemes and engaging the whole class in mathematical inquiry. During

    interactive periods, the teacher poses the tasks and interacts with the children to facilitate child-

    initiated and child sustained iteration. According to the childrens work and responses the teacher

    modifies the tasks to help them in neutralizing perturbations by modifying their fraction

    schemes. Simons (1995) and Tzurs (1995) models of constructivist teaching and the

    contributions of other researchers in mathematics education provide a useful framework for

    considering the teachers role in constructivist teaching. Both models involve an initial

    assessment of students thinking prior to instruction. Lampert (1990) states that initial planning

    involves using a variety of methods that includes pre-testing, engaging in whole-class discussion,

    or using a task with the potential to reveal a wide variety of students thinking. After this initial

    assessment teachers plan tasks and situations to engage students in active learning. During the

    planning periods, the teacher infers students knowledge and operations from initial assessments;

    and then plans tasks that are likely to activate and challenge childrens current mental structures.

  • 12

    During interacting periods the teacher poses tasks, problems, or situations and interacts

    with learners to facilitate conceptual change (Simon, 1995). Teachers assist students in a

    supportive role as they explore concepts together instead of directing their learning autocratically

    (Brown, 1991). Hence, the teacher plays the role of facilitator, coach, or colleague (Cobb, et al.

    1992) who listens continually to students ideas as they are allowed and encouraged to explore,

    make conjectures, generalize and explain and justify their answers. Teachers show flexibility in

    class allowing for the teaching moment - where students thinking results in venues for

    subsequent dialogue or activity (NCTM, 1991).

    In a study which examined the role of discourse in the mathematics classroom

    Rosenblum (1992) employed a constructivist framework for classroom interaction consistent

    with Simons and Tzurs models. The role of communication was examined in helping children

    make connections between what they know and what they are trying to understand. A sample of

    20 classes were randomly selected and randomly assigned to treatment and control groups. The

    participating students were second and third graders. The treatment groups received a discourse

    intervention allowing students to solve problems many ways and to determine answers through

    negotiation. The control group received traditional instruction.

    The intervention took place for 30 minutes, three times a week for five months.

    Dependent variables included reasoning ability, self-esteem, computation and creativity.

    Multivariable analysis of covariance was performed on the four dependent variables using pretest

    scores as covariates. Computation and creativity contributed to a significant main effects for

    second and third graders respectively. No significant grade by group interaction was found.

    Benefits in the mathematical discourse according to reports from teachers included (a) better

    understanding and evaluation of student mathematical ability, and (b) improved student

    willingness to try difficult problems.

  • 13

    Theoretic and research literature on constructivism stress the importance of teachers

    interacting with the students in ways similar to the description provided by Simon (Steffe &

    Gale, 1995; Cobb et al., 1992; Tzur, 1995). While Simons and Tzurs models provide important

    insight into how constructivist theory can be concretized in the classroom, few models of

    mathematics teaching consistent with constructivism are located in the literature. Most offer little

    more than anecdotal evidence regarding their effectiveness. Much research remains to be done

    to understand the implications of models related to classroom discourse within the context of

    constructivism.

    Teaching Should Support Collaborative and Individual Constructions

    A growing body of research on collaborate or cooperative learning supports both

    Vygotskys and Piagets views of the advantages of social interactions for concept development.

    Yackel et al. (1991) conducted an experiment in which all instruction in a second grade

    mathematics classroom was replaced by small group problem-solving strategies for an entire

    school year. The goal was to foster collaborative learning and conflict resolutions skills. Children

    were grouped in pairs and spent half of each math period working together to solve math

    problems. The cooperative learning approach was used for all aspects of instruction including

    computation activities, and other traditional topics such as time and measurement, as well as

    instructional activities that were problematic situations. Work was not graded so that external

    reward systems were not a factor in the experiment. Pairs were formed that were homogeneous

    with respect to mathematical development and other pairs were heterogeneous in this regard.

    Whole group discussions followed paired activity. In groups where children were at different

    conceptual levels the teachers sometimes modified the obligation so that students could reach

    consensus about solution methods. It was acceptable for the most advanced child in a

    heterogeneous group to construct a solution method that the other could not understand provided

  • 14

    the weaker child had also constructed a solution.

    Yackel et al.(1991) concluded that the interactions provided students opportunities for

    learning that resulted directly from the interactions. Further, attempts to resolve conflicts led to

    opportunities for students to reconceptualize problems and construct other solution methods.

    Cobbs et al. study provides anecdotal evidence to support constructivist teaching models and

    provides a framework for implementing constructivist teaching.

    Qin (1993) examined the effectiveness of achieving higher-order learning tasks in

    cooperative learning compared with competitive learning through a meta-analysis. He looked at

    63 studies comparing the effectiveness of cooperative learning with competitive learning in

    different types of problem-solving. The major hypothesis of the study was that learners are more

    successful in problem-solving under cooperative learning than under competitive learning.

    Findings were: (a) overall learners who engaged in cooperative learning were more successful in

    problem-solving than students involved in competitive learning arrangements; and (b) there is a

    tendency for older learners to do better than younger learner in three types of problems:

    linguistic and well-defined problems, but not in ill-defined problem.

    Studies conducted by Yackel.et al. (1991) and Quin (1993) support constructivist teaching

    by providing evidence that social learning arrangements can provide contexts for constructing

    more viable schemas. They also provide evidence that cooperative learning may be differentially

    effective across learners.

    An important aspect of constructivist teaching requires that students become more

    reflective thinkers. von Glaserfeld (1987) discusses mathematical knowledge as a product of

    conscious reflection. Vygotsky (1962) recognized the importance of writing on reflective

    thought, he states, that in written speech, we are obliged to create the situation, to represent it to

    ourselves (p. 99). He believed that writing encouraged learners to analyze, compare facts, and

  • 15

    synthesize information. Moreover, he believed that language and thought were dialectic in

    nature, and were both transformed in the act of representation. Kennedy (1980) and Connelly

    (1989) state that writing requires students to think about a topic, focus on and construct

    important concepts, and make those concepts to some degree their own.

    A study conducted by Rose (1989) explored, the role of writing to support the learning of

    mathematics. In her study, students were encouraged to think on paper rather than communicate

    information or persuade an audience. The setting for the study was a calculus course for business

    majors at a small private college. As the researcher taught this course, she used autobiographical

    narratives, in-class focused writing, and spontaneous writing. In-class writing focused on specific

    mathematical content currently being covered. Typical writing assignments included: What is a

    composite function?; or What is the procedure for solving a related rate problem? Qualitative

    research methods were used on both what the students wrote and what they said about the

    writing experience to ascertain perceived benefits. An analysis of data revealed that (a) writing

    helped students retain concepts, (b) promoted independent learning, and (c) writing had transfer

    benefits to other courses. Students believed that throughout the dialogue created with the journal,

    a more supportive atmosphere was created in the classroom. Rose states that the activities

    fostered dialogue and helped produce a more personal, cooperative, and active learning

    environment.

    Powell (1989) used journal entries and free writing in a study of college freshmen

    enrolled in a developmental computations class. Powell asked students to write for five minutes

    during each class about what they have done, discovered, invented, or felt. Although students

    were free to write on any topic or issue, Powell offered questions to help stimulate thought and

    reflection. He found that journal writing enabled students to become more involved in actively

    constructing and making sense of mathematics; and that students benefited because they were

  • 16

    able to monitor their own learning. Powell concluded that writing caused students to reflect

    critically about their mathematics learning; and, that a high correlation existed between improved

    problem solving performance and the extent to which reflective and descriptive writing activities

    were used during the course.

    Students full use of language are vital aspects of constructivist teaching. Researchers

    provide evidence that collaborative learning and writing are ways in which teachers can utilize

    language to facilitate knowledge construction. The nature and quality of tasks need further

    elaboration.

    Authentic Tasks

    Tasks in the classroom from a constructivist teaching point of view should be authentic

    in the sense that students are allowed to solve meaningful problems in much the same manner as

    experts or mathematicians. That is students should be allowed to discuss problems with others, to

    conjecture, explore, invent, and to use all of the tools and resources that are common practice

    outside of school. (Brown, et al, 1991). The philosophy of constructivist teaching endorses the

    use of authentic tasks, viewing them through the lenses of both Piaget (1978) and Vygotsky

    (1962), as opportunities for students to modify their schemas and as opportunities to become

    enculturated into authentic practice through activity and social interaction.

    Authentic tasks are related to the idea of situated cognition. Collins (1991) states that

    situated cognition is based on the social constructivist claim that knowledge is coded by and

    connected to the activity and environment in which it was developed. The implications are that if

    students learning is embedded in a mathematics classroom which resembles the context of

    expert practitioners, then their cognition will be closer to that of an expert. Hence, constructivist

    teaching supports the use of authentic activities that reflect how problem solving is performed

    and how expertise is developed in out-of-school contexts. Schoenfeld (1987) states that

  • 17

    authentic practice implies the creation of a mathematics culture in the classroom that

    encourages students to think of mathematics as an integral part of their everyday lives.

    Brown et al. (1989) state that, to be truly authentic, tasks need to immerse students in all

    aspects of how problems are solved in out-of-school contexts. He states that real-world problem

    solving involves verbal and nonverbal interaction, technology including calculators and

    computers, writing, graphs, charts, tables, physical objects, learning strategies, computers, and

    calculators. The implications are that in-school mathematics that restrict students from using all

    of the tools and practices common to the community of expert practitioners causes students to

    become encultured into an unrealistic situation that will foster expertise only in in-school

    contexts (Schoenfeld, 1989). Vygotsky (1962) states that engaging in progressively more

    complex activities and tasks and internalizing social interaction and psychological tools bring

    about habits of the expert. The role of the teacher as participant and guide helps the student learn

    to solve problems and to eventually gain expertise (Vygotsky, 1987).

    Support for the situated cognition movement is found in Saxes (1991) analysis of the

    computational algorithms developed by child candy sellers in Brazil. Saxe reported that the

    candy sellers mathematical procedures were closely linked to their participation in the monetary

    system. He reported that the children could work with numerals only if they were those written

    on familiar bill or coins. Choi (1995) conducted a study that corroborated these findings. The

    purpose was to determine the effects of contextualization and complexity of a situation on

    achievement, transfer, and attitude toward mathematical problem solving. A total of 101 fifth

    grade students from an elementary school participated in the study. In general, students who

    studied complex, contextualized problems performed the best and students who studied complex,

    decontextualized problems performed the worst. Students who studied simple problems in

    decontextualized situations performed best on one-step questions (both context-rich and context-

  • 18

    poor), while students who studied complex problems in contextualized situations performed best

    on multi-step questions (both context-rich and context-poor). Results indicate that situated

    instruction can promote learning by providing meaningful and complex problems rather than

    decontextualized problems.

    An environment where students are allowed to explore real-world problems using

    cooperative learning and technology brings classroom practice closer to authentic math practice

    than traditional approaches that involve students working alone on textbook word problems.

    Johnson (1992) examined the impact of constructivist learning activities on students problem-

    solving skills and attitudes in a university-level applications mathematics course. Four sections

    were taught using a constructivist approach; and six sections were taught using traditional lecture

    methods. There were a total of 331 students. Constructivist activities included working in groups,

    having students generate problems, having students present problems and solutions to the class,

    and discussing concepts and the interpretation of problems and solutions. Control classes used

    the traditional lecture and discussion methods with nonroutine problem solving as time

    permitted. Non-routine problems were added to final exams to collect problem-solving skills

    data and math SAT scores were used as the control variable in an ANOVA.

    Results indicated that students in constructivist classes had significantly greater problem-

    solving achievement than students in the control classes. A t-test revealed that course grades

    were significantly higher for the treatment group, and the mean differences between pretest and

    posttest scores for an attitude measure, Aiken Revised Math Attitude Scale, demonstrated

    significantly improved attitudes for the treatment group.

    Brown et al. (1989) state that school mathematics disregards most of the inventive

    heuristics that students bring into the classroom. Hence, he claims that it implicitly devalues the

    whole process of inventive problem solving that is valued in out-of-school mathematics.

  • 19

    Anbanithi (1993) examined the effect of using a constructivist approach in the classroom that

    allowed students to discover, invent, and negotiate their own problem solving strategies. One

    hundred and six subjects were assigned to four experimental conditions, (1) constructivist

    teaching, (2) explicit strategy instruction, (3) explicit strategy teaching plus a constructivist

    environment, and (4) control. After an intervention of fourteen days, various measures were

    administered to assess students levels of conceptual understanding, beliefs about reasons for

    success in mathematics, use of deep processing strategies, ability to think and communicate

    mathematically, and perceived competence in mathematical problem solving.

    Four main findings emerged. First, students exposed to a constructivist environment

    performed superior to those in the explicit instruction group for transfer problems that measured

    deep conceptual understanding. Second, students exposed to a constructivist environment held

    stronger beliefs and more positive goals in math about the importance of understanding and

    collaborating. Third, constructivist students reported the use of deep processing strategies than

    students in the explicit instruction group. Fourth, constructivist students were able to

    communicate more effectively during mathematics problem solving than students in the direct

    explanation group.

    The studies conducted by Johnson (1992) and Anbanithi (1993) support the situated

    cognition approach for problem solving. They suggest that to help students develop

    mathematical understanding, they should engage in tasks of inquiry, reasoning, and problem

    solving that reflect real-world mathematical practice. They also reveal that constructivist

    approaches which emphasize communication, reasoning and problem solving have a positive

    effect on development of beliefs, problem solving ability, and promote conceptual understanding.

    Teaching Should Encourage Multiple Representations and Connections

    Both theoretic and research literature on constructivism suggest that teaching should

  • 20

    emphasize multiple representations and connections in the classroom. Bruner (1960) and Dienes

    (1977) suggest that concept learning is enhanced when a concept is presented in a variety of

    physical contexts. Bruner contends that students be allowed to construct different representations

    of concepts, and he claimed that representations the learner has constructed for himself will be

    more accessible for retrieval and more likely to be applied correctly in appropriate situations.

    Skemp (1977) states that students who are able to connect mathematical symbols to appropriate

    referents (representations) possess relational schemas that assist them in solving problems even

    when they forget the rules for conducting operations.

    Research on cognition has shown the importance of helping students develop a well-

    connected body of representations. Rosenshine (1995) states that the size of a persons mental

    structure, the number of connections between pieces of knowledge, the structure of the

    connections, and the organization and richness of the relationships are all important for

    processing information and problem solving. She contends that it is easier to assimilate new

    information and easier to use prior knowledge for problem solving when knowledge structures

    are large and well connected. Hiebart and Carpenter (1992) state that a major difference between

    an expert and novice is that an experts knowledge structures has a larger number of knowledge

    items, more connections between items, stronger links and connections, and a better organized

    knowledge structure. Hence, it is important to help students see the interrelations of topics in

    order to connect entities.

    The introduction of graphing calculators and computers has been a catalyst for research

    on representations and connections. Much of the research available indicates that students who

    use the graphing calculator and software to explore, propose, and build connections among the

    numeric, graphic, and algebraic representations of functions have a better understanding of

    functions and related concepts. Melin-Conjeros (1992) states that relationships that are unclear to

  • 21

    students in mathematical equations or tables of data become more distinct in graphical form.

    Melin-Conjeros investigated the effects of doing calculus homework assignments in a

    mathematics laboratory equipped with a Computer Algebra System (CAS) on students'

    achievement and attitude towards mathematics. Interviews revealed that students who had used

    the CAS for their homework had a better understanding of selected concepts - increasing and

    decreasing functions, asymptotes, concavity, limits and continuity - and that students taught with

    computer-generated representations were better able than students in a control group to recognize

    derivatives and integrals in different forms and to relate the forms to algebraic and graphical

    representations of functions. Goldenburg (1988) showed that the most successful students in

    advanced mathematics are those with the flexibility to work in more than one representation

    (graphic, numeric or symbolic) and that students who are limited to one representation are less

    likely to solve a wide range of problems. Research conducted by Browning (1990) corroborates

    the empirical studies which suggest that students in technology based courses gain a better

    understanding of connections between graphical, numerical, and algebraic representations than

    students in a traditional classroom.

    In summary, constructivist teaching offers a different view of teaching from the

    traditional positivist/behaviorist paradigm of teaching. The center of classroom teaching shifts

    from teacher-centered to a focus on student knowledge and experiences. The constructivist view

    requires a teacher to adjust his or her role as knowledge dispenser to a role of co-investigator,

    competent peer, or guide and interlocutor. They probe students understanding and help them

    resolve conflict between new concepts and previous mathematical knowledge. The most

    important attribute of the constructivist teacher is the ability to pose questions, tasks, and have a

    broad understanding of how students learn mathematics. Further, he or she must continually

    analyze plans and instructional approaches to fit students ways of knowing.

  • 22

    References

    Abel, J. & Abel, F. (1988). Writing in the mathematics classroom. The Clearing

    House, 62, 155-158.

    Anbanithi, M. (1993). Training mathematical reasoning: Direct explanation versus

    constructivist learning. Dissertation Abstracts International, 53/11, 3834..

    Beckman, C. (1989). Effect of computer graphics use on student understanding of

    calculus concepts. Dissertation Abstracts International, 50B.

    Blais, D. (1988). Constructivsim: A theoretical revolution for algebra. Mathematics

    Teacher, 81, 624-631.

    Britton, J. (1975). The development of writing abilities. London: Macmillan.

    Brownell, W. A. (1935). Psychological consideration in the learning and the teaching of

    arithmetic. The Teaching of Arithmetic, Tenth Yearbook of the National Council of Teachers of

    Mathematics, Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, New York, 1-31.

    Bruner, J. (1960). The process of education. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Brown, J. S., Collins, A. & Duguid, P. (1989). Situated cognition and the culture of

    learning. Educational Researcher, 18, 32-42.

    Browning, C. (1989). Characterizing levels of understanding for functions and their

    graphs. Dissertation Abstracts International, 49, 2957A.

    Carpenter, T., Fennema, E., Peterson, P., Chiang, C., & Loef, M. (1989). Using childrens

    mathematics thinking in classroom teaching: An experimental study. American Educational

    Research Journal, 26, 499-531.

    Choi, J. (1995). The effects of contextualization and complexity of situation on

    mathematics problem-solving and attitudes. Dissertation Abstracts International, 56-10A, 3884.

    Cobb P. (1989).Experimental, cognitive, and anthropological perspectives in mathematics

  • 23

    education. For the Learning of Mathematics, 9, 30-42.

    Cobb,P., Yackel, E., & Wood, T. (1992). A constructivist alternative to the

    representational view of mind in mathematics education. Journal for Research in Mathematics

    Education. 23, 2-33.

    Cobb, P. (1995). The relevance of Practice: A response to Orton. Journal for Research in

    Mathematics Education, 26, 230-253.

    Collins, A. (1991, September). The role of computer technology in restructuring schools.

    Phi Delta Kappan, 28-36.

    Conference Board for Mathematical Sciences (1983). The mathematical sciences

    curriculum K-12: What is still fundamental and what is not. In educating Americans for the 21st

    century.: Source materials, Washington, DC.: National Science Foundation, 1-23.

    Connolly, Paul. (1989). Writing and ecology of learning. In P. Connolly & T.

    Vilardi (Eds.), Writing to Learn Mathematics and Science. (pp. 56-69). New York: Teachers

    College Press.

    Crocker, D. (1991). Development of the concept of derivative in a calculus class using

    the computer algebra system Mathematical. Proceedings The Fourth Annual International

    Conference of Technology in Collegiate Mathematics: Conference Abstracts. University of

    Portland.

    Dewey (1973). The need for a recovery of philosophy. Chicago University of Chicago

    Press.

    Dienes, Z. (1977).On the understanding and use of mathematics. Literary Discussion, 8,

    101-120.

    Douglas, Ronald G., (Ed.). (1986). Toward a lean and lively calculus. MAA Notes, No.

    6. Washington, DC: Mathematical Association of America.

  • 24

    Dubinsky, E. (1991) Reflective abstraction. In D. Tall (Ed.), Advanced Mathematical

    Thinking. Boston: Kluser Academic publishers, 101-123.

    Emig, J. (1977). Writing: an act of cognition, in C.W. Griffin (ed.), New Direction for

    Teaching and Learning: Teaching Writing in All Disciplines, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco,15 -26.

    Ferrini-Mundy, J., & Graham, K (1991). Research in calculus learning. Understanding of

    limits, derivatives, and integrals. Paper presented at the Joint Mathematics Meetings, Special

    Session on Research in Undergraduate Mathematics Education, San Francisco.

    Fischbein, E. (1990). Introduction. In P. Nesher & kilpatrick (Eds.), Mathematics and

    Cognition: A research synthesis by the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics

    Education. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1-13.

    Frid, S. (1994). Three approaches to undergraduate calculus instruction: Their nature and

    potential impact on students language use and sources on conviction. In Ed Dubinsky, Alan

    Schoenfeld, & Jim Kaput (eds.), Research in Collegiate Mathematics Education. 1, Conference

    Board of the Mathematical Sciences. American Mathematical Society.

    Fulwiler, T. (1982). Writing: An act of cognition, in C. W. Griffin (ed.), New Direction

    for Teaching and Learning: Teaching Writing in All Disciplines, Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, 15-

    26.

    Goldenburg, P.E. (1988). Mathematical metaphors, and human factors: mathematical,

    technical, and pedagogical representations of function. Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 7,

    135-169.

    Heid, M. K. (1988). Resequencing skills and concepts in applied calculus using the

    computer as a tool. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 19, 3-25.

    Hiebert J., & Carpenter, T. P. (1992). Learning an teaching with understanding.

  • 25

    In D. A. Grouws (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 65-97).

    Reston, VA: National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; New York: Macmillan.

    Johnson, J. (1992). Using Symbolic calculators to facilitate constructivist learning in

    mathematics of finance. Dissertation Abstracts International, 53-02, 434.

    Judson, P. (1990). Using computer algebra systems in mathematics courses. Journal of

    Mathematical Behavior, 9, 153-158.

    Kilpatrick J. (1992). A history of Research in mathematics education. In D. A. Grouws

    (Ed.), Handbook of research on mathematics teaching and learning (pp. 3-23). Reston, VA:

    National Council of Teachers of Mathematics; New York: Macmillan.

    Knoblauch, C. D. & Brannon, L. (1983). Writing as learning through the

    curriculum, College English, 45, 465-74.

    Lampert, M. (1990). When the problem is not the and the solution is not the answer.:

    Mathematical knowing and teaching. American Education Research Journal, 27, 29-63.

    Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving. Psychological Monographs,

    546, 54.

    Mathematical Sciences Education Board and National Research Council, (1989).

    Everybody Counts: A report to the nation on the future of mathematics education. Washington,

    DC: National Academy press.

    Marwine, A. (1989). Reflections on the uses of informal writing. In P. Connolly

    & T. Vilardi (Eds.), Writing to learn mathematics and science (pp. 56-69). New York: teachers

    College Press.

    Mayher, J. S., Lester, N. B., & Pradl, G. M. (1983). Learning to Write/Writing to Learn.

    Boynton/Cook Publishers, Inc., Upper Montclair, NJ.

    Melin-Conjeros, J. (1992). The effect of using a computer algebra system in a

  • 26

    mathematics laboratory on the achievement and attitude of calculus students. Dissertation

    Abstracts International, 53, 2283A.

    Mett, C. L. (1989). Writing in mathematics: Evidence of learning through

    writing. The Clearing House, 62, 292-293.

    Monk, S. (1992). A framework for describing student understanding of functions. Paper

    presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San

    Francisco.

    Nahrgang, C.L. & Peterson, B.T.(1986). Using writing to learn mathematics.

    Mathematics Teacher, 79, 461-465.

    Norman, A. F. & Pritchard, M. K. (1994). Cognitive obstacles to the learning of

    calculus: a Kruketskiian perspective. In J. Kaput and E. Dubinsky (Eds.). Research Issues in

    Undergraduate Mathematics Learning: preliminary Analyses and Results. The Mathematical

    Association of America. Notes Number 33.(pp 65-74).

    National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1989). Curriculum and evaluation

    standards for school mathematics. Reston, V.A.: NCTM.

    National Council of Teachers of Mathematics (1991). Professional standards for teaching

    mathematics. Reston, V.A.: NCTM.

    Nussbaum, J. Novick, S. (1982). Alternative Frameworks, Conceptual conflict and

    accommodation: Toward a principled teaching strategy. Instructional Science,11, 183-200.

    Pearce, D. & Davidson, D. M. (1988). Teacher use of writing in junior high

    mathematics classrooms. School Science and Mathematics, 88, 6-15.

    Piaget, J. & Inhelder (1967). The childs conception of space. New York: W. W. Norton.

    Piaget (1970). Structuralism. New York: Basic Books.

    Piaget, J. (1980). Experiments in contradiction. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

  • 27

    Piaget, J (1978). The equilibration of cognitive structures. Chicago: University of

    Chicago Press.

    Powell, A. & Lopez, . (1989). Writing as a vehicle to learn mathematics: A case study. In

    P. Connolly & T. Vilardi (Eds.), Writing to learn mathematics and science (pp. 56-69). New

    York: teachers College Press.

    Porzio, D. (1994). The effects of differing technological approaches to calculus on

    students use and understanding of multiple representations. Dissertation Abstracts International,

    61/55. p.1087.

    Rose, B. (1989). Using expressive writing to support the learning of mathematics.

    Dissertation Abstracts International. 50/04, p. 867.

    Rosenblum, V. (1992). Mathematical Discourse in grades two and three: Effect on

    reasoning ability, self-esteem, computation, and creativity. Dissertation Abstracts International;

    DAI, 5304A, 1047.

    Saxe, G. (1988). Candy selling and math learning. Educational Researcher, 17, 14 -21.

    Schoenfeld, Alan H. (1985). Mathematical Problem Solving, Academic Press, Orlando.

    Schoenfeld, A.H. (1987). Cognitive science and mathematics education. Hillsdale, N.J.:

    Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Seldon, J., A., & Seldon, A. (1990, March). Constructivism in mathematics education: A

    view of how people learn. UME Trends, 8-9.

    Schoenfeld, A. H. (1991). On mathematics as sensemaking: An informal attack on the

    unfortunate divorce of formal an informal mathematics. In J. F. Voss, D. N. Perkins, & J. W.

    Segal (Eds.) , Informal reasoning and education (pp. 311-344) Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum.

    Simon (1995, May). Reconstructing mathematics pedagogy from a constructivist

    perspective. Paper presented at 9th Annual Research Institute Teacher Education. Center for

  • 28

    research in mathematics and science education. North Carolina State University.

    Schrock, C. (1990). Calculus and computing: An exploratory study ton examine the

    effectiveness of using a compute algebra system to develop increased conceptual understanding

    in a first-semester calculus course Dissertation Abstracts International, 50, 1926A.

    Skane M. (1993). Utilizing a conceptual change model with college precalculus studnts:

    Amending misconceptions about the distributive law. Dissertation Abstracts International-A

    54/05, 1715.

    Skemp R. (1976). Relational Understanding and Instrumental Understanding.

    Mathematics Teaching, 77; 20-26.

    Small, D.B., Hosack, J.M. (1986). Algebra systems tools for reforming calculus

    instruction. In R. G. Douglas (Ed.), Toward a Lean and Lively Calculus, (pp. 143). Washington,

    DC: The Mathematical Association of America.

    Steen, Lynn A.M (Ed). (1987). Calculus for a new century: A pump not a filter. (MAA

    Notes, No. 8). Washington DC: Mathematical Association of America.

    Steffe & Gale (1995). Constructivism in Education. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Stewart, C. B. (1992). Journal writing in mathematics classrooms: A practical

    inquiry (writing to learn). Dissertation Abstracts International, 53/12, p. 242.

    Tall, D. (1990). Inconsistencies in the learning of calculus and analysis. Focus on

    Learning Problems in Mathematics, 12, 49-63.

    Tzur R. (1995). Interaction and Childrens fraction learning. Dissertation Abstracts

    International, 56-10A, p. 3874.

    Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky, 1, Problems of general

    psychology. New York: Plenum Publishing.

  • 29

    Vygotsky, L.S. (1962). Thought and Language., MIT Press, Cambridge MA.

    Vygotsky, L. S. (1981). The instructional method in psychology. In J. V. Wertsch, (ed.),

    The concept of activity in Soviet psychology. Armonk, NY: M.E. Sharpe, pp. 134-143.

    Von Glasserfeld, E. (1987). Learning as a constructive activity. In C. Janvier (Ed.),

    Problems of representation in the teaching and learning of mathematics, (pp. 69-77). Hillsdale,

    NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Wilson B., Teslow, Osman, R. (1995). The impact of constructivism on ID Fundamentals.

    In B. Seels (Ed. ), Instructional Design Fundamentals: A Review and Reconsideration. (pp. 137-

    157) Englewood Cliffs NJ: Educational Technology Publications.

    Yackel, E, Cobb, P., & Wood, T. (1991) Small-group interaction as a source of learning

    opportunities in second-grade mathematics. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education,

    22(5), 390-408.